Abstract
Background
Impulsivity is common in people with Parkinson’s disease (PD), with many developing impulsive compulsive behavior disorders (ICB). Its pathophysiological basis remains unclear.
Objectives
We aimed to investigate local field potential (LFP) markers of trait impulsivity in PD and their relationship to ICB.
Methods
We recorded subthalamic nucleus (STN) LFPs in 23 PD patients undergoing deep brain stimulation implantation. Presence and severity of ICB were assessed by clinical interview and the Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in PD-Rating Scale (QUIP-RS), whereas trait impulsivity was estimated with the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS-11). Recordings were obtained during the off dopaminergic states and the power spectrum of the subthalamic activity was analyzed using Fourier transform-based techniques. Assessment of each electrode contact localization was done to determine the topography of the oscillatory activity recorded.
Results
Patients with (n = 6) and without (n = 17) ICB had similar LFP spectra. A multiple regression model including QUIP-RS, BIS-11, and Unified PD Rating Scale-III scores as regressors showed a significant positive correlation between 8–13 Hz power and BIS-11 score. The correlation was mainly driven by the motor factor of the BIS-11, and was irrespective of the presence or absence of active ICB. Electrode contact pairs with the highest α power, which also correlated most strongly with BIS-11, tended to be more ventral than contact pairs with the highest beta power, which localize to the dorsolateral motor STN.
Conclusions
Our data suggest a link between α power and trait impulsivity in PD, irrespective of the presence and severity of ICB.
Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, impulsivity, impulsive compulsive behavior disorders, deep brain stimulation, subthalamic nucleus
Cognitive and neuropsychiatric disorders are common in people with Parkinson’s disease (PD) and have a major impact on patients’ and caregivers’ quality of life. The high prevalence of cognitive and neuropsychiatric disorders has led to the suggestion that PD should more accurately be conceptualized as a neuropsychiatric disorder.1
Among the neuropsychiatric symptoms of PD, impulsive compulsive behaviors (ICB) represent a frequent source of disability with a 5-year cumulative incidence of 46.1%.2 ICB comprise impulse control disorders such as pathological gambling, compulsive shopping, hypersexual behavior, and compulsive eating, and related disorders such as punding/hobbyism and dopamine dysregulation syndrome. Interplay between exposure to dopaminergic medications and endogenous characteristics in people with PD seem to underlie the generation of these behavioral disorders.3 In particular, certain personality traits have been suggested to predispose people with PD to develop ICB, namely a novelty seeking profile and high level of impulsivity.4,5 Impulsivity has been conceptualized as a multifactorial construct accounting for at least three dimensions: trait impulsiveness, impulsive action, and impulsive choices.6
Clinical and experimental evidence suggests that the subthalamic nucleus (STN) plays a key role in the inhibitory control of both action and behavior.7 The STN is also the main target of deep brain stimulation (DBS), a well-recognized effective treatment for people with advanced PD. STN-DBS can provide the opportunity to record STN local field potentials (LFPs) and such recordings have provided important information on the pathophysiology of motor features of PD and the effects of treatment with levodopa. However, relatively little is known about the LFP correlates of non-motor symptoms in PD and in particular ICB. An influential resting state study reported greater theta band activity in the ventral STN in PD with active ICB in the “on medication” condition,8 but the extent to which this might reflect trait differences as opposed to a particular neural circuit response to levodopa medication remains unclear. Indeed, to date, no studies have investigated the electrophysiological markers of trait impulsivity, which might predispose patients to ICB. Accordingly, here, we evaluate the LFP correlates of trait impulsivity in a cohort of PD patients with and without ICB, assessing trait impulsivity with the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS-11), a validated and widely used scale.
Methods
Patients and Clinical Assessments
Consecutive PD patients undergoing bilateral STN-DBS who gave their written informed consent to participate in the experiment (IRAS project ID: 75154) were recruited at St. George’s University Hospital, London, United Kingdom (UK).
Motor symptoms were assessed before DBS (205 + 61 days before) and included a levodopa challenge test as per Core Assessment Program for Surgical Interventional Therapies in PD (CAPSIT-PD) protocol with rating of Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III (UPDRS-III) in the practically defined off and on medication state.9 Presence and severity of dyskinesia were evaluated with the UPDRS-IV (total score and sum of sub-items 32–35 for dyskinesia) and the Rush Dyskinesia Rating Scale (RDRS). The diagnosis of ICB relied on a semi-structured interview based on ICB diagnostic criteria,10 which was performed on the same day as the levodopa challenge test and confirmed at the time of the study. ICB was defined as “Active” if it was ongoing at the time of study entry. “Past” ICB were defined as patients who had ICB at any point during their disease, but were in remission at the time of the study. The Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in PD-Rating Scale (QUIP-RS)11 and BIS-1112 were used to measure severity of ICB and trait impulsivity, respectively. The BIS-11 scale is a 30-item questionnaire measuring trait impulsivity, comprising the following factors: attentional (attention and cognitive instability), motor (motor and perseverance), and non-planning (self-control and cognitive complexity). Presence and severity of depression was evaluated with the Hamilton depression rating scale (HDRS).
Neurophysiological Testing
DBS implantation was a staged procedure. LFP recordings were made between 3 and 5 days postoperatively, while electrode leads were still externalized and before implantation of the subcutaneous pulse generator. Patients were recorded OFF medication.
LFP recordings were made for a minimum of 3 minutes (maximum 10 minutes) while patients were seated comfortably at rest in an armchair with their eyes open. Signals were recorded with a TMSi-Porti amplifier (TMS International, Oldenzaal, the Netherlands). The ground electrode was placed on a forearm. LFP signals were sampled at 2048 Hz and common average referenced. LFPs from quadrapolar electrodes were offline reconfigured to give a bipolar contact arrangement between the four levels so that each electrode afforded three bipolar signals on the left (from bottom to top: L01, L12, L23) and right (R01, R12, R23). In 17 patients, directional leads (15 Boston Scientific and 2 Abbott Neuromodulation) were implanted and directional contacts of one level were connected together to form one “ring” contact, and then bipolar contacts were constructed offline as before. Note that the contributions of the three segmented contacts to the ring contact average may not be equal considering that impedances may have differed between the segmented contacts, although the latter have similar dimensions.
In one patient (case 16) the lead was a non-directional octopolar one and we selected the four most ventral electrodes per side, after checking that they were located adjacent to or in the STN. In one patient (case 7) we excluded one channel (R23) because of the presence of artifacts. Bipolar montages between adjacent contact pairs were used because they limit the effects of volume conduction from distant sources.13
The data were first visually inspected using Spike2 Software (CED Cambridge Electronic design limited, UK). Sections contaminated by artifacts (eg, signal saturation or movement artifact) were removed from the data set. The average duration of analyzed artifact-free data was 262 ± 161 seconds. For the main analysis LFP activity in the STN region was computed by averaging across all bipolar channels of each electrode to avoid any channel selection bias. Spectral analysis based on the fast Fourier transform was performed in Spike2. Non-overlapping windows of 2048 data points were analyzed, affording a spectral resolution of 1 Hz. LFP power at frequencies ≤4 Hz was excluded from further analysis, because this is subject to movement artifacts and heavily influenced by the 1/f nature of the signal rather than by oscillations. We considered power between 5–35 Hz as our range of interest because it includes the θ-α band previously implicated in ICB8 and the β band associated with motor impairment, but also implicated in response inhibition in some studies. Power in this range of interest was normalized by dividing it by the sum of the power over 5–395 Hz (excluding line noise peaks at 50 ± 5 Hz and its harmonics) and multiplying by 100 to give a percentage. This step is necessary to limit the variation in spectral power between sides and subjects because of variance in targeting and impedance.
Additional information is in the Supporting Data.
Statistical Analysis
In our core analysis, we used a general linear model (GLM) to investigate if STN power averaged across all contacts and the left and right hemispheres was significantly related to ICB severity (QUIP-RS) and trait impulsivity (BIS-11). To assess if motor symptoms accounted for variations in the LFP we also added the UPDRS-III scores to the GLM. The predictor variables were z-transformed to enable a better comparison of the regression coefficients. The model was fitted using the MATLAB function fitlm with a bisquare weighting function to compute a robust fit (v. 2019b, The MathWorks, Natick, MA). Regression coefficients were computed separately for frequencies ranging from 5 to 35 Hz and corrected for multiple comparisons with a cluster-based permutation procedure described in the Supporting Data.
In subsequent exploratory analyses we took the average power over the 8–13 Hz frequency range found to be significant using the above approach and determined its relationship with the three components of the BIS-11 captured by the attentional, motor and non-planning sub-scores, and with the HDRS score. Spearman’s correlation coefficients were computed to compare the size of correlations. STN power was averaged across the left and right hemispheres in each subject before the correlation analysis unless otherwise stated. Finally, we determined the relationship between α power and BIS-11 scores when extracting power from the bipolar contact configuration that had the highest α power and contrasted this with that from the contact pair with the highest β power.
Results
We evaluated 23 PD patients undergoing STN-DBS (five women, mean age 59.0 ± 6.2 years, mean disease duration 9.8 ± 3.7; Table 1). According to the semistructured clinical interview, six patients had active ICB and 17 had no-active ICB. Demographic and clinical differences between these subgroups are summarized in Table 2. Within the no-active ICB group seven patients had ICB in the past, whereas 10 patients had never had any ICB.
Table 1. Demographical and clinical features of the study cohort.
ID | Age (y) | Disease duration (y) | Type of electrode | Most affected side | Total LEDD (mg) | D-ag LEDD (mg) | UPDRS-III off | UPDRS-III on | ICB state | Type of active ICB |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
01 | 60 | 8 | Medtronic 3389 | L | 970 | 320 | 28 | 8 | Active ICB | Compulsive shopping, hypersexuality |
02 | 60 | 15 | DB-2202 | R | 1563 | 315 | 50 | 30 | Past ICB | |
03 | 60 | 6 | Medtronic 3389 | L | 800 | 0 | 48 | 14 | Past ICB | |
04 | 60 | 13 | DB-2202 | L | 1403 | 280 | 77 | 27 | past ICB | |
05 | 47 | 16 | DB-2202 | R | 865 | 0 | 71 | 37 | past ICB | |
06 | 53 | 7 | DB-2202 | L | 635 | 160 | 38 | 25 | Active ICB | Hypersexuality, compulsive eating, punding |
07 | 55 | 16 | Medtronic 3389 | R | 1810 | 360 | 51 | 19 | Never ICB | |
08 | 65 | 15 | Medtronic 3389 | L | 666 | 0 | 57 | 34 | Never ICB | |
09 | 52 | 7 | DB-2202 | L | 1152 | 180 | 18 | 1 | Active ICB | Hypersexuality, compulsive shopping |
10 | 66 | 9 | DB-2202 | R | 1124 | 360 | 52 | 30 | Never ICB | |
11 | 65 | 5 | Medtronic 3389 | R | 995 | 320 | 34 | 16 | Never ICB | |
12 | 48 | 7 | DB-2202 | R | 1180 | 0 | 45 | 34 | Active ICB | Pathological gambling |
13 | 61 | 10 | DB-2202 | R | 500 | 0 | 33 | 12 | Past ICB | |
14 | 51 | 12 | DB-2202 | L | 1020 | 320 | 27 | 13 | Active ICB | Hypersexuality |
15 | 56 | 6 | DB-2202 | R | 1380 | 480 | 48 | 19 | Past ICB | |
16 | 66 | 6 | Bsci octode | L | 580 | 0 | 17 | 11 | Never ICB | |
17 | 67 | 7 | AB-6170 | L | 820 | 320 | 17 | 5 | Never ICB | |
18 | 62 | 6 | DB-2202 | R | 1082 | 300 | 42 | 13 | Never ICB | |
19 | 64 | 12 | DB-2202 | L | 160 | 160 | 52 | 21 | Active ICB | Compulsive shopping |
20 | 63 | 10 | DB-2202 | R | 780 | 80 | 40 | 17 | Never ICB | |
21 | 63 | 10 | AB-6170 | R | 1800 | 375 | 41 | 11 | Past ICB | |
22 | 52 | 11 | DB-2202 | L | 1470 | 160 | 54 | 27 | Never ICB | |
23 | 63 | 10 | DB-2202 | R | 1275 | 300 | 29 | 10 | Never ICB |
Abbreviations: D-Ag, dopamine-agonists; F, female; ICB, impulsive compulsive behavior disorders; L, left; LEDD, levodopa equivalent daily dose; M, male; R, right; y, years; UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale; Implanted leads, Medtronic 3389, (Medtronic, Minneapolis, USA) four 0.5-mm spaced contacts of 1.5-mm length with platinum-iridium cylindrical surfaces; DB-2202, directional leads from Boston Scientific with three segmented contacts on the middle levels (Boston Scientific, USA); Bsci octode, octopolar non directional leads from Boston Scientific; AB-6170, directional leads from St. Jude Medical with three segmented contacts on the middle levels (model 6170, Abbott Neuromodulation, Austin, TX, USA).
Table 2. Comparison between PD ICB active and PD-no-active ICB for demographic and clinical data.
PD noICB | PD ICB | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (y) | 60.6 ± 5.4 | 54.6 ± 6.0 | 0.05 |
Disease duration (y) | 10.0 ± 3.8 | 8.8 ± 2.5 | 0.7 |
Total LEDD (mg) | 1112.5 ± 410.8 | 852.8 ± 391.3 | 0.3 |
Dopamine-agonists LEDD (mg) | 214.7 ± 166.0 | 190 ± 119.8 | 0.7 |
UPDRS-III off med | 44.7 ± 16.1 | 34.6 ± 12.6 | 0.1 |
UPDRS-III on med | 19.5 ± 9.4 | 17 ± 12.0 | 0.6 |
UPDRS-IV total | 5.5 ± 2.0 | 4.8 ± 3.3 | 0.3 |
UPDRS-IV dyskinesia sub-score | 1.6 ± 1.4 | 1.8 ± 2.2 | 0.8 |
RDRS | 3.4 ± 2.4 | 4.4 ± 4.3 | 0.8 |
QUIP-RS total score | 11.3 ± 15.0 | 37 ± 17.4 | a0.003 |
QUIP-RS ICD score | 5.8 ± 7.8 | 20.1 ± 8.6 | a0.004 |
BIS-11 total score | 57.1 ± 10.3 | 63.3 ± 10.1 | 0.2 |
BIS-11 attention | 13.2 ± 3.1 | 17 ± 3.8 | a0.04 |
BIS-11 motor | 21.2 ± 4.1 | 22.5 ± 1.9 | 0.3 |
BIS-11 nonplanning | 22.6 ± 6.9 | 23.8 ± 5.3 | 0.5 |
HDRS | 6.0 ± 5.1 | 6.5 ± 6.2 | 0.8 |
Values are means ± SD.
Significant at P < 0.05 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons).
Abbreviations: BIS-11, Barratt impulsivity scale; F, female; ICB, impulsive compulsive behavior disorders; ICD, impulse control disorders; HDRS, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; LEDD, levodopa equivalent daily dose; M, male; QUIP-RS, Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in PD-Rating Scale; RDRS, Rush dyskinesia rating scale; UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale-motor part; UPDRS-IV, UPDRS complications of therapy part; UPDRS-IV dyskinesia sub-score, sum of items 32–35.
Dependencies between LFP Power and Symptom Complexes
To explore statistical dependencies between LFP power over the 5–35 Hz range and behavioral features, we computed a multiple regression model including the QUIP-RS score, BIS-11 score, and UPDRS-III score as predictors and LFP power as outcome variable. We found significant positive regression coefficients for LFP power estimates between 8–13 Hz and BIS-11 score after correcting for multiple comparisons over the whole 5–35 Hz frequency range with the cluster-based permutation procedure (Fig. 1A). Although the active ICB group had a power peak in the low β frequency range, which was not present in the no-active ICB group (Fig. 2), the regression analysis did not result in a significant cluster in this frequency band.
Neither the coefficients for the QUIP-RS or UPDRS-III score were significant in this model after correction for multiple comparisons, although those for the UPDRS-III did show a peak in the low β range.
This dependency between α power and BIS-11 score was confirmed in a model with power averaged across 8–13 Hz as outcome variable (F-statistic = 4.21, P = 0.019; BIS-11: t-statistic [t-stat] = 3.17, P = 0.005; QUIP-RS: t-stat = –0.07, P = 0.942; UPDRS: t-stat = –0.55, P = 0.589). Therefore there was a link between STN oscillatory activity in the α range and impulsivity, independent of motor symptoms and the severity of ICB. To visualize this relationship, we correlated 8–13 Hz power with the BIS-11 scores across patients (Spearman’s ρ = 0.67; Fig. 1B). To test if the diagnosis of active ICB played a role in influencing the correlation, we computed another regression model with “active ICB”/”no-active ICB” as categorical variable, age, and the total BIS-11 score as predictors. Again, only the coefficient for the BIS-11 scores was significant (BIS-11: t-stat = 2.81, P = 0.011; active/no-active ICB: t-stat = –0.59, P = 0.562, age: t-stat = 0.15, P = 0.884, interaction BIS-11*active/no-active: ICB t-stat = 0.65, P = 0.525).
The lack of significance of the active ICB diagnosis variable (t-stat = 0.587, P = 0.564) and lack of interaction (t-stat = 0.723, P = 0.5), together with the significant BIS-11 t-statistic (t-stat = 3.04, P = 0.007) highlights that the correlation between BIS-11 and α power was present irrespective of the presence of active ICB. The three PD subgroups (Never ICB, Past ICB, and Active ICB) are highlighted in red, blue, and black in Fig. 1B. It shows that a positive relationship was present within each subgroup (Never ICB: ρ = 0.48, P = 0.160 [n = 10]; Past ICB: ρ = 0.79, P = 0.048 [n = 7]; Active ICB: ρ = 0.83, P = 0.058 [n = 6]). The relationship was only significant in the Past ICB group, but the number of patients in each subgroup was small. Example spectra of two patients, one with a low and one with a high BIS score, are shown in Figure 1C.
Then we performed a series of exploratory correlations aimed at refining the link between α activity and the BIS-11 score. Impulsivity is thought to comprise three components dissociable as the attentional, motor, and non-planning second-order factors in the BIS-11. Accordingly, we correlated each of these BIS-11 sub-scores with the normalized LFP power averaged over the 8–13 Hz frequency bins. Alpha power correlated most strongly with the motor component sub-score (ρ = 0.61, P = 0.002, unadjusted and significant after Holm-Bonferroni correcting for multiple comparisons), followed by the attentional sub-score (ρ = 0.53, P = 0.010), although the latter was also correlated with the motor component sub-score (ρ = 0.67, P < 0.001, significant after multiple comparison correction). The non-planning component was the most weakly correlated with α power (ρ = 0.42, P = 0.047), and also did not correlate significantly with the motor subcomponent (ρ = 0.14, P = 0.518). Given the correlation between the motor and attentional subcomponents, we performed partial correlations between the BIS-11 motor sub-score and α power while controlling for attention (ρ = 0.39, P = 0.072) and, conversely, between the BIS-11 attention sub-score and α power, controlling for the motor sub-score (ρ = 0.19, P = 0.400).
We also performed partial correlations between α power and BIS-11 controlling for: depression, dopaminergic therapy (total levodopa equivalent daily doses [LEDD] and LEDD dopamine-agonists) and dyskinesia severity. The correlation remained high (HDRS: ρ = 0.70, P = 0.001, n = 21 instead of 23, because for two patients the depression scores were not recorded; total LEDD: ρ = 0.67, P = 0.001; LEDD dopamine-agonists: ρ = 0.67, P = 0.001; UPDRS-IV dyskinesia sub-scores: ρ = 0.65, P = 0.001; RDRS scores: ρ = 0.67, P = 0.001). Together these results suggest that the correlation between BIS-11 and α power is dominated by the correlation between the motor impulsivity component and α power.
Source of the LFP Activity Correlating with Trait Impulsivity
We found no evidence of lateralization when correlating the BIS-11 score with the average normalized 8–13 Hz LFP power obtained from two subsets (left and right) of contacts obtained in the two hemispheres (Supporting data). Averaging these two subsets again confirmed the significant relationship between alpha power and BIS-11 scores (averaged between hemispheres [n = 23]: ρ = 0.61, P = 0.002).
Contacts were located predominantly in the posterolateral “motor STN,” as shown by the distribution of α power within the recorded region of the STN (Fig. 3). A hotspot is seen that is inset from the postero-lateral border of the STN by approximately a quarter of the distance between the nucleus’ poles. Another way of determining whether the α band signal correlating with the BIS-11 comes primarily from the motor STN or not, is to contrast the correlations established above with correlations based on α power from the bipolar contacts that had the highest β (rather than alpha) power per electrode. This is because the bipolar contact with the highest β power tends to localize to the dorsal posterolateral “motor” STN.14 Use of the highest β contact pair led to weakening of the relationship between α power and BIS-11 scores (averaged between hemispheres [n = 23]: ρ = 0.37, P = 0.080; compared to ρ = 0.61, P = 0.002 for peak α contacts). This suggested that the source of the α power that correlates best with BIS-11 scores may be slightly different to the β power hotspot in the motor region of the STN. Indeed, the relationship between the different sets of contact pairs supported a slightly more ventral source for the peak α activity that maximally correlates with the BIS-11 score. The bipolar contact with the highest α power (and the stronger α-BIS correlations) was ventral in 21 of 46 (46%) instances, overlapping in 17 of 46 (37%) instances and dorsal only in 8 of 46 (17%) instances when compared with the bipolar contact with the highest β power.
Discussion
This is the first study in people with PD to explore STN LFP correlates of trait impulsivity per se. We recorded LFPs at rest in the “off medication” condition and found a positive correlation between the oscillatory activity in the α band (8–13 Hz) and the degree of trait impulsivity in PD patients, irrespective of the presence and severity of active ICB. STN LFP power spectra showed no significant differences between patients with and without active ICB.
Trait Impulsivity Is Separate from ICB
The relationship between impulsivity and ICB is complex and still not very clear. Indeed, impulsivity is common in PD, even in the absence of ICB. It is a psychological construct characterized by poor control of thoughts and actions with a tendency to rapidly respond to impulses and environmental cues despite potential negative consequences. Therefore impulsivity itself has obvious aspects in common with ICB, and indeed previous studies have shown high trait impulsivity (ie, a high score on the Barrat-11 scale) in PD patients with ICB. However, there is no evidence on whether this is a specific component of ICB or a predisposing factor that might be present in a number of these patients.4,15 Moreover, clinical studies looking at differences in impulsivity between PD patients with and without ICB have reported inconsistent results with higher4,15,16 or comparable levels of trait impulsivity measured by BIS-11.17 Behavioral aspects of impulsivity, such as action and choice impulsivity have also been studied in relation to ICB, again with inconsistent findings. Indeed, on behavioral testing, the presence of ICB in PD have been associated with increased impulsivity,5,18,19 less impulsivity,20,21 or no difference compared to PD without ICB.22,23 Our data expand this previous literature and suggest that ICB, encompassing compulsive components, do not necessarily share the same alterations of neural activity as underlie trait impulsivity. Hence, patients may exhibit the two behavioral phenomena to different degrees, and trait impulsivity can be present even in the absence ofICB. Finally, the demonstration of a physio-marker for trait impulsivity described here would provide the opportunity to assess its modulation by short and long-acting dopaminergic medications and correlation to ICB change using novel sensing DBS devices.
Relationship to Other LFP Studies
Previous LFP studies in PD patients have reported a modulation of low frequency activity in the α-θ band during tasks assessing inhibitory control of action as a proxy of impulsivity, such as the stop-signal, Stroop and Flanker tasks.24 These results support the role of STN and low frequencies in modulating action and choice inhibition in PD. However, whether impairments in these tasks are relevant in the generation of behavioral and affective symptomatology is unclear.
Only two studies have specifically carried out LFP recordings in the STN in PD patients focusing on ICB. Rodriguez-Oroz et al8 reported θ activity in the ventral region of STN in ICB in the on medication condition. In addition, Rosa et al25 demonstrated that PD patients with pathological gambling, a specific type of ICB, adopted a risky strategy during decision making and showed a greater change in low-frequency power in the STN when evaluating trials with conflict compared to those without. Like Rodriguez-Oroz et al,8 we found no difference between PD with and without ICB in LFP patterns recorded in the “off medication” condition, suggesting perhaps that the differences they identified on medication might reflect a specific neural circuit response to dopaminergic stimulation in ICB.
Impulsivity is a multi-faceted construct and this multi-dimensionality is reflected in the BIS-11 factor structure. Our data suggest that the positive correlation between α power and BIS-11 scores was mostly driven by the second order motor factor rather than by the non-planning or attentional factors of the BIS-11. Both mapping of the α power distribution in space across subjects and along the electrode within subjects raised the possibility that α power correlating with the BIS-11 tends to arise from an area slightly ventral to the dorsolateral motor area. This is consistent with higher resolution microelectrode recordings, which identify a region more ventral to the motor area that is characterized by 7–10 Hz activity.26
The dominant correlation between α power and the BIS-11 motor factor is also consistent with a recent structural connectivity study that associates impulsivity in PD to STN connectivity, with the response inhibition or “stopping” network linking left supplementary area and right inferior frontal gyrus.27 This network is thought to involve hyperdirect pathway input to the STN,28 with the strength of hyperdirect connections correlating with the efficacy of stopping.29,30
Impulsivity is a key component in a number of other neuropsychiatric disorders, and it is therefore interesting that, among these, elevated α band power at rest has been reported in the ventralis oralis complex of the thalamus of patients with Tourette’s syndrome,31 in the bed nucleus of stria terminalis or in subgenual cingulate area in patients with major depression,32 and in the nucleus accumbens in patients with addiction or substance use disorders.33 This raises the possibility that α activity might serve as a biomarker for behavioral impulsivity across neuropsychiatric disorders.
Study Limitations
First, our sample included six patients with active ICB; hence, the absence of state-dependent differences in STN LFPs may have been a type 2 error, particularly because group differences might have been attenuated by a post-operative stun effect. Second, although our data demonstrate that α power and trait impulsivity are correlated, they cannot ascertain whether this reflects a primarily pathological or a secondary, possibly compensatory, process. Third, the precision of our neurosurgical targeting meant that we could not sample directly from the ventromedial limbic and associative parts of the STN. Fourth, previous LFP studies in PD have shown a correlation between the reduction in α power in the STN in response to emotionally charged stimuli and depression.34,35 However, we controlled for the role of depression when assessing the relationship between resting α power and trait impulsivity and showed that the relationship was independent from the severity of depression. We also controlled for total LEDD and LEDD dopamine-agonists considering that dopaminergic therapy can enhance impulsivity, and controlled for the severity of dyskinesia as previous studies found an association between dyskinesia and increased α-θ power in the on medication condition.8,36 We showed that the relationship between BIS-11 and α activity remained strong even after controlling for these factors, perhaps because our recordings were only carried out in the off-medication condition. However, because dopaminergic therapy has a key role in triggering ICB in PD patients with predisposing factors, future studies with LFP recordings on and off medication are warranted to further explore the complex relationship between trait impulsivity, ICB, and dopaminergic medication.
Moreover, we acknowledge that our recordings were carried out only in the off-medication condition, because dopaminergic therapy has a key role in triggering ICB in PD patients with predisposing factors, future studies with LFP recording on and off medications are encouraged to further shade light on the complex relationship between trait impulsivity, ICB, and dopaminergic medications.
We normalized the spectra relative to a very broad frequency range spanning 5–395 Hz to minimize the contributions of other physiological or pathological peaks such as those in the β range. In addition, it is possible that our frequency range of interest, 8–13 Hz, was influenced by the tail of any peaks in the low-β power range. On the other hand, this frequency range was objectively defined as that showing significant correlation with impulsivity in a sweep of all frequencies from 5–35 Hz.
Finally, clinical assessments predated LFP recordings, which might have weakened the behavioral correlations and might, together with the use of the total UPDRS III score, help explain the LFP correlations with motor impairment compared to those previously reported.37,38
In conclusion, we show a positive correlation between α power and trait impulsivity in PD patients with and without ICB. Our findings motivate further investigation of α power in PD and other conditions characterized by impulsivity and suggest that this spectral feature might serve as a neural biomarker that relates to impulsive behavior.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Andrew Clouter and Shenghong He for their help in recording some of the patients; Andrea De Angelis and Marianna Sarchioto for their help in the clinical assessment of some of the patients; Fotios Bourlogiannis for his contribution in electrode position, data collection, and measurement.
Funding agencies
The MedicalResearch Council(MC_UU_12024/1), the NationalInstitute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) and Rosetrees Trust. G.T. received project funding from the Swiss Parkinson Association and Baasch-Medicus Foundation, Switzerland.
Financial Disclosures
L.R. has received research support from MRC. M.E. has received speaking honoraria from Merz and Boehringer Ingelheim; research support from NIHR and MRC; and royalties from Oxford University Press. M.E. is also the Associate Editor of European Journal of Neurology. E.A.C.P. has received teaching honoraria from Abbott, Boston Scientific, and Medtronic; has received travel grants from Abbott and Boston Scientific; and royalties from Elsevier and Oxford University Press. F.M. has received speaking honoraria from Abbvie, Medtronic, Zambon, Bial, and Merz; travel grants from the International Parkinson’s disease and Movement Disorder Society; advisory board fees from Boston Scientific, Merz; consultancy fees from Boston Scientific, Merz and Bial; research support from Boston Scientific, Merz, and Global Kynetic; royalties from Springer; and is a member of the editorial board of Movement Disorders, Movement Disorders Clinical Practice, European Journal of Neurology. P.B. has received consultancy fees from Medtronic; is a member of the editorial board of Neurobiology of Disease.
Footnotes
Relevant conflicts of interest/financial disclosures: None.
Author Roles
(1) Research project: A. Conception, B. Organization, C. Execution; (2) Statistical Analysis: A. Design, B. Execution, C. Review and Critique; (3) Manuscript Preparation: A. Writing of the first draft, B. Review and Critique.
L. R.: 1A, 1B, 2A, 2B, 3A
P.F.: 2A, 2B, 3A
A.M.: 1C, 3B
G.T.: 3B
F.T.: 3B
F.B.: 1C
M. J.E.: 1A, 3B
E. A.C.P.: 1B, 3B
F. M.: 1A, 2C 3B
P.B.: 1A, 1B, 2A, 2C, 3B
References
- 1.Weintraub D, Burn DJ. Parkinson’s disease: the quintessential neuropsychiatric disorder. Mov Disord. 2011;26:1022–1031. doi: 10.1002/mds.23664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Corvol JC, Artaud F, Cormier-Dequaire F, et al. Longitudinal analysis of impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2018;91:e189–e201. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kelly MJ, Baig F, Hu MT, Okai D. Spectrum of impulse control behaviours in Parkinson’s disease: pathophysiology and management. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2020;91:703–711. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2019-322453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Isaias IU, Siri C, Cilia R, de Gaspari D, Pezzoli G, Antonini A. The relationship between impulsivity and impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2008;23:411–415. doi: 10.1002/mds.21872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Voon V, Reynolds B, Brezing C, et al. Impulsive choice and response in dopamine agonist-related impulse control behaviors. Psychopharmacology. 2010;207:645–659. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1697-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Evenden J. Impulsivity: a discussion of clinical and experimental findings. J Psychopharmacol. 1999;13:180–192. doi: 10.1177/026988119901300211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jahanshahi M, Obeso I, Baunez C, Alegre M, Krack P. Parkinson’s disease, the subthalamic nucleus, inhibition, and impulsivity. Mov Disord. 2015;30:128–140. doi: 10.1002/mds.26049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lopez-Azcarate J, Garcia-Garcia D, et al. Involvement of the subthalamic nucleus in impulse control disorders associated with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2011;134:36–49. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Defer GL, Widner H, Marie RM, Remy P, Levivier M. Core assessment program for surgical interventional therapies in Parkinson’s disease (CAPSIT-PD) Mov Disord. 1999;14:572–584. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(199907)14:4<572::aid-mds1005>3.0.co;2-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Weintraub D, Hoops S, Shea JA, et al. Validation of the questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24:1461–1467. doi: 10.1002/mds.22571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Weintraub D, Mamikonyan E, Papay K, Shea JA, Xie SX, Siderowf A. Questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease-rating scale. Mov Disord. 2012;27:242–247. doi: 10.1002/mds.24023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Marmor O, Valsky D, Joshua M, et al. Local vs. volume conductance activity of field potentials in the human subthalamic nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 2017;117:2140–2151. doi: 10.1152/jn.00756.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Horn A, Neumann WJ, Degen K, Schneider GH, Kuhn AA. Toward an electrophysiological “sweet spot” for deep brain stimulation in the subthalamic nucleus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:3377–3390. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Voon V, Sohr M, Lang AE, et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a multicenter case-control study. Ann Neurol. 2011;69(6):986–996. doi: 10.1002/ana.22356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hlavata P, Linhartova P, Sumec R, et al. Behavioral and neuroanatomical account of impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurol. 2020;10:1338. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Marin-Lahoz J, Pagonabarraga J, Martinez-Horta S, et al. Parkinson’s disease: impulsivity does not cause impulse control disorders but boosts their severity. Front Psych. 2018;9:465. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Housden CR, O’Sullivan SS, Joyce EM, Lees AJ, Roiser JP. Intact reward learning but elevated delay discounting in Parkinson’s disease patients with impulsive-compulsive spectrum behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:2155–2164. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Claassen DO, van den Wildenberg WP, Ridderinkhof KR, et al. The risky business of dopamine agonists in Parkinson disease and impulse control disorders. Behav Neurosci. 2011;125:492–500. doi: 10.1037/a0023795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Claassen DO, van den Wildenberg WP, Harrison MB, et al. Proficient motor impulse control in Parkinson disease patients with impulsive and compulsive behaviors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2015;129:19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wylie SA, Claassen DO, Huizenga HM, et al. Dopamine agonists and the suppression of impulsive motor actions in Parkinson disease. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24:1709–1724. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Djamshidian A, O’Sullivan SS, Lees A, Averbeck BB. Stroop test performance in impulsive and non impulsive patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2011;17:212–214. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ricciardi L, Haggard P, de Boer L, et al. Acting without being in control: exploring volition in Parkinson’s disease with impulsive compulsive behaviours. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2017;40:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Drummond NM, Chen R. Deep brain stimulation and recordings: insights into the contributions of subthalamic nucleus in cognition. Neuroimage. 2020;222:117300. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rosa M, Fumagalli M, Giannicola G, et al. Pathological gambling in Parkinson’s disease: subthalamic oscillations during economics decisions. Mov Disord. 2013;28:1644–1652. doi: 10.1002/mds.25427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rappel P, Grosberg S, Arkadir D, et al. Theta-alpha oscillations characterize emotional subregion in the human ventral subthalamic nucleus. Mov Disord. 2020;35:337–343. doi: 10.1002/mds.27910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mosley PE, Paliwal S, Robinson K, et al. The structural connectivity of subthalamic deep brain stimulation correlates with impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2020;143:2235–2254. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Neumann WJ, Schroll H, de Almeida Marcelino AL, et al. Functional segregation of basal ganglia pathways in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2018;141:2655–2669. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rae CL, Hughes LE, Anderson MC, Rowe JB. The prefrontal cortex achieves inhibitory control by facilitating subcortical motor pathway connectivity. J Neurosci. 2015;35:786–794. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3093-13.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Forstmann BU, Keuken MC, Jahfari S, et al. Cortico-subthalamic white matter tract strength predicts interindividual efficacy in stopping a motor response. Neuroimage. 2012;60:370–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Marceglia S, Servello D, Foffani G, et al. Thalamic single-unit and local field potential activity in Tourette syndrome. Mov Disord. 2010;25:300–308. doi: 10.1002/mds.22982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Neumann WJ, Huebl J, Brucke C, et al. Different patterns of local field potentials from limbic DBS targets in patients with major depressive and obsessive compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:1186–1192. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ge S, Geng X, Wang X, et al. Oscillatory local field potentials of the nucleus accumbens and the anterior limb of the internal capsule in heroin addicts. Clin Neurophysiol. 2018;129:1242–1253. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kuhn AA, Hariz MI, Silberstein P, et al. Activation of the subthalamic region during emotional processing in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;65:707–713. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000174438.78399.bc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Huebl J, Spitzer B, Brucke C, et al. Oscillatory subthalamic nucleus activity is modulated by dopamine during emotional processing in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex. 2014;60:69–81. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alegre M, López-Azcárate J, Alonso-Frech F, et al. Subthalamic activity during diphasic dyskinesias in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2012 Aug;27(9):1178–1181. doi: 10.1002/mds.25090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Beudel M, Oswal A, Jha A, et al. Oscillatory Beta power correlates with Akinesia-rigidity in the Parkinsonian subthalamic nucleus. Mov Disord. 2017;32:174–175. doi: 10.1002/mds.26860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Neumann WJ, Kuhn AA. Subthalamic beta power-unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale III correlations require akinetic symptoms. Mov Disord. 2017;32:175–176. doi: 10.1002/mds.26858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.