Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jul 18.
Published in final edited form as: J Psychopharmacol. 2019 May 21;33(6):655–659. doi: 10.1177/0269881119845799

Antidepressants, withdrawal, and addiction; where are we now?

Sameer Jauhar 1,, Joseph Hayes 2, Guy M Goodwin 3, David S Baldwin 4, Philip J Cowen 3, David J Nutt 5
PMCID: PMC7613097  EMSID: EMS150073  PMID: 31111764

Abstract

Controversy continues with regard to antidepressants and withdrawal. Recent debates have focused on the prevalence and length of withdrawal, and some continue to state that withdrawal from these compounds constitutes ‘addiction’. In this editorial we examine the evidence underlying these recent debates. We acknowledge gaps in knowledge, and make suggestions for how the field can progress.

Keywords: Antidepressive agents, addiction, serotonin uptake inhibitors, paroxetine


Despite acknowledgement of antidepressant withdrawal syndromes dating back to the use of the first tricyclic antidepressant, imipramine (Mann and Macpherson, 1959), the topic remains controversial. A recent review, published in Addictive Behaviours, commissioned by the United Kingdom All Party Parliamentary Group for Prescribed Drug Dependence, has suggested that symptoms of antidepressant withdrawal are similar to those seen with drugs of dependence; that the prevalence is significantly higher than originally proposed (around 50%); that withdrawal is severe in nearly half of cases, and that symptoms can persist for years (Davies and Read, 2018). This review has been submitted to the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE), and Public Health England (PHE), who will appraise the evidence. When bold assertions (with potentially significant repercussions at societal level) are made, it is vital that they are correctly scrutinised on the basis of existing literature.

We therefore examine the conceptual basis of proposing antidepressants as ‘addictive’, withdrawal phenomena associated with antidepressants, how evidence has accumulated, gaps that exist in the literature and pharmacological considerations. We then use the example of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), paroxetine, to illustrate these points.

Are antidepressants addictive?

Conceptualising antidepressant withdrawal as ‘addiction’ is not new and has been debated before (Medawar, 1997; Nielsen et al., 2012; Nutt, 2003; Tyrer, 1999). The main arguments proposed for addiction include the presence of withdrawal symptoms and the evolution of the concept of benzodiazepine dependence (and the similarity of some symptoms of antidepressant and benzodiazepine withdrawal).

Putting aside the fact that antidepressants are a very heterogeneous pharmacological group of compounds (see below), with the possible exception of tranylcypromine and amineptine it is difficult to make a case for antidepressants to be considered ‘addictive (Haddad, 1999)’. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) has taken a broader conceptualisation of addiction, and adapted the DSM-IV concept of substance-related disorders to substance-related and addictive disorders. Antidepressants are not included within the 10 substance classes put forward within DSM-5 criteria (unlike sedatives, hypnotics and anxiolytics). Looking at the features of substance-use disorders, it is difficult to see how these could be applied to antidepressants. DSM-5 is careful to state that occurrence of symptoms such as withdrawal during medical treatment are not to be considered as criteria for substance-misuse disorders, though allowances are made for opioid addiction, where substances are used inappropriately and symptoms of compulsive drug-seeking are seen (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Compulsion is fundamental here – the disease model of addiction is built on this concept, where ‘initially, drug use is a voluntary behavio[u]r, but when that (metaphorical) switch is thrown, the individual moves into the state of addiction, characterized by compulsive drug seeking and use’ (parentheses added) (Leshner, 1997; 46). This contrasts with benzodiazepine dependence, where compulsion may be seen in those who abuse them. It is also worth noting that not all those exposed to benzodiazepines at a dose and period sufficient to cause dependence will develop it. Repeated studies have indicated that around 35% of people will develop dependence, and these people appear more likely to have been taking benzodiazepines for longer periods (>5 years) and have dependence-prone personalities (Murphy and Tyrer, 1991). The duration of prior use of antidepressants, seems to have effects on antidepressant withdrawal symptoms, though this depends on the drug. Specifically, the Committee of Safety of Medicines expert working group report analysis of published and unpublished clinical trial data, found treatment duration to increase incidence of withdrawal from paroxetine, escitalopram, with some evidence for venlafaxine, though not clearly so for mirtazapine, sertraline and fluoxetine (Weller et al., 2005).

How have withdrawal effects been studied?

Using SSRI withdrawal as an example, the first descriptions were case reports, identifying increased reporting of withdrawal symptoms with paroxetine (Price et al., 1996). This was accompanied by randomised controlled trials (RCTs), which included randomised double-blind studies with drug interruption using unstructured and, subsequently, standardised assessment measures such as the Discontinuation-Emergent Signs and Symptoms (DESS) checklist (Oehrberg et al., 1995; Rosenbaum et al., 1998). This was followed by a number of randomised placebo-controlled studies across a range of disorders, an example being a summary of studies reported by Baldwin et al. who found withdrawal incidence (change in DESS⩾4 in the ranges of 1.9– 12.2% (continuation of placebo), 6.9–27.3% (escitalopram to placebo), 28.4–32.7% (paroxetine to placebo) and a figure of 31.5% for venlafaxine to placebo (Baldwin et al., 2007). It is worth noting the presence of a ‘nocebo’ response to dummy withdrawal, which in some studies was numerically higher than after withdrawal of active drug (Montgomery et al., 2004; Zajecka et al., 1998), a fact omitted from the Davies and Read review (Jauhar and Hayes, 2019).

Much of the evidence provided in Davies and Read’s review comes from online surveys, many of which are their own. Though undoubtedly rich in qualitative data, these convenience sample surveys are not generally accepted as quantitative analyses with clear criteria set by journals for inclusion of quantitative data (Bethlehem, 2010; Cook et al., 2000). This makes it impossible to extrapolate data from such surveys to the general population of people receiving antidepressants. The estimates of withdrawal severity by Davies and Read were only taken from such surveys. Two of the four surveys were from people self-identifying as experiencing withdrawal requiring treatment (people using tapering kits (Groot and Van Os, 2018) and people contacted through withdrawal websites (Davies and Read, 2018)). By the very nature of the sampling method, this population is likely to report severe symptoms. Despite this, the authors did not measure study quality or comment on potential selection bias in these surveys. Their interpretation of any observed effects as being directly attributable to the drug ignores a host of potential ascription and other confounding factors which contribute to the reporting of withdrawal symptoms under non-blinded conditions and are seen in the placebo condition in well-designed discontinuation studies. This, and other troublesome methodological errors (such as no clear inclusion or exclusion criteria), makes not only interpretation but also replication of their review very challenging (Jauhar and Hayes, 2019).

Differentiating illness relapse from discontinuation symptoms

Most published literature relates to people with defined mental illness and therefore it must be considered whether new symptoms emerging on antidepressant cessation represent a withdrawal syndrome or a return of illness (Jha et al., 2018). The question of whether withdrawal symptoms constitute a relapse of illness has been addressed in controlled studies examining people with depression who have responded to antidepressant treatment, where responders can develop withdrawal symptoms on discontinuation. One example is self-limiting somnolence and dizziness in a fluoxetine discontinuation RCT (Zajecka et al., 1998), and another example concerns a number of psychiatric and somatic symptoms after successful treatment with paroxetine or sertraline (Michelson et al., 2000). Increased depressive symptoms and adverse effects on social functioning were noted in the paroxetine group in a similarly designed RCT in people with successfully treated depression, receiving either fluoxetine or paroxetine. When DESS symptoms were examined by category, differences were seen in the ‘body as a whole’, and in the digestive and nervous systems, suggesting symptoms secondary to discontinuation (Judge et al., 2002).

However, these trials are not necessarily representative of antidepressant use in clinical practice (Wiles et al., 2013). A large number of patients will be partial responders to antidepressants, with just over one-third achieving full remission with their first prescribed antidepressant (Rush et al., 2006). Therefore, the risk of interpreting illness relapse as withdrawal is particularly high in these populations and may go some way to explain the high rates identified by surveys. Exclusion of relapse symptoms from drug withdrawal can only be properly done in studies of healthy volunteers receiving antidepressants or by identifying DESS symptoms which have no depression or anxiety symptom analogue.

How best to measure withdrawal effects?

Given these considerations, how would one best examine the nature, incidence, severity and duration of antidepressant withdrawal?

In terms of study design, we should rely on the evidencebased hierarchy as follows;

  • (a)

    Meta-analysis of randomised double-blind placebo-controlled withdrawal studies (in healthy controls, and in patients)

  • (b)

    Placebo-controlled RCTs

  • (c)

    Controlled studies

  • (d)

    Case series

  • (e)

    Questionnaire studies

  • (f)

    Self-report

What are the gaps in the literature?

Some of the concerns of critics of previous RCTs of withdrawal symptoms include length of follow-up and length of antidepressant treatment before cessation. Data suggest withdrawal phenomena can be non-normally distributed (Price et al., 1996) and most treatment trials only last 12 weeks or less. Survey data suggest that some people develop withdrawal after prolonged use of SSRIs, as well as longer-term discontinuation symptoms. However, this has not been studied in a systematic fashion, or in naturalistic settings, as noted above.

There is also a dearth of animal studies examining discontinuation effects and possible brain mechanisms in animal models, and studies that have been conducted are predominantly with fluoxetine within a short time-frame (Renoir, 2013).

The ideal studies would include blinded RCTs in healthy volunteers with a full range of antidepressants, treatment durations and prolonged follow-up, which would be necessary to fully understand withdrawal phenomena: these are unlikely to be feasible. However, other RCTs are underway. An example is the ANTidepressants to prevent reLapse in dEpRession (ANTLER) trial (https://ukctg.nihr.ac.uk/trials/trial-details/trial-details?trialNumber=ISRCTN15969819) which will examine cessation effects for up to 52 weeks, in patients who have taken medication for >9 months, thereby overcoming some of the previous RCT limitations.

Pharmacological considerations; the example of paroxetine

The concept of an ‘antidepressant’ medication is an old one, borrowing from the serendipitous way in which these compounds were first discovered. Modern, neuroscience-based nomenclature (NbN) suggests classification according to pharmacological properties (Zohar, 2014). This makes it easier to understand the therapeutic effects and side-effects.

In the case of antidepressant withdrawal, it helps to differentiate compounds such as agomelatine (a melatonin receptor agonist with antagonist effects on 5HT2B and 5HT2C receptors) which has no reported withdrawal syndrome (Montgomery et al., 2004) from, for example, paroxetine. As well as being the most potent SSRI, paroxetine has the greatest affinity for the muscarinic M1 receptor, is also the most potent inhibitor of cytochrome P450 2D6 (which means it slows its own metabolism) and has a relatively short half-life of less than 24 h (Nevels et al., 2016; Tonks, 2002). For these reasons, paroxetine is one of the antidepressants with a particularly high propensity for withdrawal symptoms. It is therefore worth examining data on paroxetine withdrawal symptoms in more detail.

Paroxetine is an effective antidepressant that also has established efficacy in a range of anxiety disorders. However, the story of paroxetine has been marred with controversy; it has been the focus of negative documentaries, and trial data have needed to be reanalysed because of the potential downplaying of adverse events (Le Noury et al., 2015). Many clinicians report not prescribing paroxetine because of potential side effects and withdrawal problems (Martin et al., 2006). We identified three randomised placebo-controlled trials which examined withdrawal effects associated with paroxetine under the somewhat disadvantageous condition of sudden withdrawal (Figure 1). Participants in the paroxetine arm underwent abrupt cessation from daily doses of 20–60 mg after 12 weeks treatment for panic disorder (Oehrberg et al., 1995), 20 mg after 12 weeks for generalised anxiety disorder (Baldwin et al., 2006) and 20 mg after 24 weeks for social anxiety disorder (Lader et al., 2004) The withdrawal data for Baldwin et al., 2006 and Lader et al., 2004 are reported in Baldwin et al., 2007.) The first trial defined withdrawal as ‘any withdrawal symptoms’ (i.e. a low threshold definition for withdrawal) and the latter two trials defined withdrawal as an increase in DESS>3. In addition, these two trials were industry-sponsored and possibly designed to favour the comparator drug, escitalopram. Withdrawal symptoms were observed in both placebo and paroxetine arms. The difference in risk of withdrawal symptoms (i.e. the risk attributable to the active drug versus placebo) was consistent across trials at 23% (95% confidence interval (CI) 17–29%). Therefore, under unfavourable conditions (abrupt withdrawal), one in four people experienced some form of withdrawal symptoms. These trials do not cover long-term use of paroxetine, though, as noted above, longer use of paroxetine is associated with increased risk of withdrawal (Weller et al., 2005).

Figure 1. Withdrawal symptoms in placebo-controlled randomised controlled trials (RCTs) of abrupt paroxetine cessation.

Figure 1

CI: confidence interval. Withdrawal data for Baldwin et al., 2006 and Lader et al., 2004 are included in Baldwin et al., 2007.

Data from published and unpublished clinical trials, employing mandatory taper of 10mg weekly, with at least one week at 20mg, published by the Committee of Safety of Medicines in 2005, gave adverse event rates of 29.9% for people taking paroxetine (n=2794) and 20.1% for those taking placebo (n=18920 on withdrawal from treatment. Approximately 85% of events in the paroxetine group were mild to moderate in intensity (Weller et al., 2005).

Conclusions and future directions

There is minimal evidence, using established classification systems and concepts, that antidepressants should be classified as addictive substances. Whilst withdrawal effects do exist, the available literature suggests modest, albeit heterogeneous, effects of antidepressant drug discontinuation from randomised placebo-controlled trials, which can be contrasted with the survey reports of severe symptoms related to open-label discontinuation. What the survey data do indicate is that there are a number of people who have been taking antidepressants with longer-term symptoms of withdrawal which affect their functioning. The existing evidence is unable to address this population and though current trials (such as ANTLER) may address this to a degree, it is difficult to know if studies will be adequately powered to adequately address these concerns.

What would seem most appropriate for those concerned with policy is to consider balanced scientific evidence, and potential intervention, for those people who may experience longer-term withdrawal problems, rather than accepting uncritically a partisan narrative.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: SJ is supported by a John, Margaret, Alfred and Stewart (JMAS) Sim Fellowship form the Royal College of Physicians (Edinburgh) and National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. JH is supported by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Career Development Fellowship and the University College London Hospitals (UCLH) NIHR Biomedical Research Centre. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Footnotes

Declaration of conflicting interests

The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: SJ and JH declare no conflicting interests. PJC has no conflicting interests during the last three years. GMG holds shares in P1Vital and has served as consultant, advisor or Continuing Medical Education (CME) speaker for Allergan, Angelini, Compass Pathways, MSD, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Takeda, Medscape, Minervra, P1Vital, Pfizer, Servier, Shire and Sun Pharma. DSB has attracted research funding (between 1994–2018) from the following pharmaceutical and biotechnology companies: AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Eli Lilly Ltd, Glaxo-SmithKline, H. Lundbeck A/S, Pharmacia, Pierre Fabre, Pfizer Ltd, Servier, Vernalis Ltd and Wyeth Ltd. He has attended advisory boards organised by Grunenthal, Liva Nova, Mundipharma, Roche and Sumitomo; and has received personal honoraria for lecture engagements organised by AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly Ltd, Glaxo-SmithKline, Janssen, H. Lundbeck A/S, Pharmacia, Pierre Fabre, Pfizer Ltd, Servier and Wyeth Ltd. DJN declares the following: advisor – British National Formulary, Medical Research Council (MRC); past president – British Neuroscience Association - European Brain Council; past president – European College of Neuropsychopharmacology; chair – DrugScience (UK); member International Centre for Science in Drug Policy; editor of the Journal of Psychopharmacology; advisory boards – Nalpharm, Mundipharma, Ranvier, Indivior, Opiant; speaking honoraria (in addition to above) Lundbeck BMS/Otsuka, Janssen, Martindale; member of the Lundbeck International Neuroscience Foundation; chair campus editorial board; grants or clinical trial payments: Wellcome Trust, MRC; share options – P1vital, Alcarelle; director Equasy Enterprises; expert witness in a number of legal cases relating to psychotropic drugs; edited/written >32 books – some purchased by pharma companies.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®) 5th ed. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; Arlington, VA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baldwin DS, Huusom AKT, Maehlum E. Escitalopram and paroxetine in the treatment of generalised anxiety disorder: Randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind study. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:264–272. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.012799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baldwin DS, Montgomery SA, Nil R, et al. Discontinuation symptoms in depression and anxiety disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;10:73–84. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705006358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bethlehem J. Selection bias in web surveys. Int Stat Rev. 2010;78:161–188. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cook C, Heath F, Thompson RL. A meta-analysis of response rates in web- or internet-based surveys. Educ Psychol Meas. 2000;60:821–836. [Google Scholar]
  6. Davies J, Read J. A systematic review into the incidence, severity and duration of antidepressant withdrawal effects: Are guidelines evidence-based? Addict Behav. 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.08.027. Epub ahead of print 4 September 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Groot PC, van Os J. Antidepressant tapering strips to help people come off medication more safely. Psychosis. 2018;10:142–145. [Google Scholar]
  8. Haddad P. Do antidepressants have any potential to cause addiction? Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 1999;13(3):300–307. doi: 10.1177/026988119901300321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Jauhar S, Hayes J. The war on antidepressants: What we can, and can’t conclude, from the systematic review of antidepressant withdrawal effects by Davies and Read. Addict Behav. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.01.025. Epub ahead of print 23 January 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Jha MK, Rush AJ, Trivedi MH. When discontinuing SSRI antidepressants is a challenge: Management tips. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175:1176–1184. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18060692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Judge R, Parry MG, Quail D, et al. Discontinuation symptoms: Comparison of brief interruption in fluoxetine and paroxetine treatment. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;17(5):217–225. doi: 10.1097/00004850-200209000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Lader M, Stender K, Bürger V, et al. Efficacy and tolerability of escitalopram in 12-and 24-week treatment of social anxiety disorder: Randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, fixed-dose study. Depression and Anxiety. 2004;19(4):241–248. doi: 10.1002/da.20014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Le Noury J, Nardo JM, Healy D, et al. Restoring Study 329: Efficacy and harms of paroxetine and imipramine in treatment of major depression in adolescence. BMJ. 2015;351:h4320. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h4320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Leshner AI. Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science. 1997;278:45–47. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Mann AM, Macpherson AS. Clinical experience with imipramine (G22355) in the treatment of depression. Can Psychiatr Assoc J. 1959;4:38–47. doi: 10.1177/070674375900400111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Martin RM, May M, Gunnell D. Did intense adverse media publicity impact on prescribing of paroxetine and the notification of suspected adverse drug reactions? Analysis of routine databases, 2001-2004. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2006;61:224–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2005.02527.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Medawar C. The Antidepressant web: Marketing depression and making medicines work. The Int J Risk Saf Med. 1997;10:75–126. doi: 10.3233/JRS-1997-10203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Michelson D, Fava M, Amsterdam J, et al. Interruption of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment: Double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;176:363–368. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.4.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Montgomery SA, Kennedy SH, Burrows GD, et al. Absence of discontinuation symptoms with agomelatine and occurrence of discontinuation symptoms with paroxetine: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled discontinuation study. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2004;19:271–280. doi: 10.1097/01.yic.0000137184.64610.c8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Murphy SM, Tyrer P. A double-blind comparison of the effects of gradual withdrawal of lorazepam, diazepam and bromazepam in benzodiazepine dependence. Br J Psychiatry. 1991;158:511–516. doi: 10.1192/bjp.158.4.511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Nevels RM, Gontkovsky ST, Williams BE. Paroxetine-the antidepressant from hell? Probably not, but caution required. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2016;46:77–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Nielsen M, Hansen EH, Gøtzsche PC. What is the difference between dependence and withdrawal reactions? A comparison of benzodiazepines and selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors. Addiction. 2012;107:900–908. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03686.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Nutt DJ. Death and dependence: Current controversies over the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. J Psychopharmacol. 2003;17:355–364. doi: 10.1177/0269881103174019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Oehrberg S, Christiansen PE, Behnke K, et al. Paroxetine in the treatment of panic disorder. A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167:374–379. doi: 10.1192/bjp.167.3.374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Price JS, Waller PC, Wood SM, et al. A comparison of the post-marketing safety of four selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors including the investigation of symptoms occurring on withdrawal. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1996;42:757–763. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2125.1996.00498.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Renoir T. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressant treatment discontinuation syndrome: A review of the clinical evidence and the possible mechanisms involved. Front Pharmacol. 2013;4:45. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2013.00045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Rosenbaum JF, Fava M, Hoog SL, et al. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor discontinuation syndrome: A randomized clinical trial. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:77–87. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00126-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, et al. Acute and longer-term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: A STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1905–1917. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.11.1905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Tonks A. Withdrawal from paroxetine can be severe, warns FDA. BMJ. 2002;324:260. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7332.260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Tyrer P. Stress diathesis and pharmacological dependence. J Psychopharmacol. 1999;13:294–295. doi: 10.1177/026988119901300316. discussion 299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Weller IVD, Ashby D, Brook R, et al. Report of the CSM Expert Working Group on the Safety of Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors. Medicines and Healthcare Products Regulatory Agency; London: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  32. Wiles N, Thomas L, Abel A, et al. Cognitive behavioural therapy as an adjunct to pharmacotherapy for primary care based patients with treatment resistant depression: Results of the CoBalT randomised controlled trial. Lancet (London, England) 2013;381:375–384. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61552-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Zajecka J, Fawcett J, Amsterdam J, et al. Safety of abrupt discontinuation of fluoxetine: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1998;18:193. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199806000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Zohar J, Nutt DJ, Kupfer DJ, et al. A proposal for an updated neuropsychopharmacological nomenclature. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol: The Journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;24:1005–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES