Skip to main content
MIT Press Open Journals logoLink to MIT Press Open Journals
. 2022 Oct 1;34(11):2065–2081. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01896

Shared Responsibility Decreases the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain

Marwa El Zein 1,2, Ray J Dolan 2, Bahador Bahrami 2,3
PMCID: PMC7613691  EMSID: EMS155147  PMID: 35900857

Abstract

Sharing responsibility in social decision-making helps individuals use the flexibility of the collective context to benefit themselves by claiming credit for good outcomes or avoiding the blame for bad outcomes. Using magnetoencephalography, we examined the neuronal basis of the impact that social context has on this flexible sense of responsibility. Participants performed a gambling task in various social contexts and reported feeling less responsibility when playing as a member of a team. A reduced magnetoencephalography outcome processing effect was observed as a function of decreasing responsibility at 200 msec post outcome onset and was centered over parietal, central, and frontal brain regions. Before outcome revelation in socially made decisions, an attenuated motor preparation signature at 500 msec after stimulus onset was found. A boost in reported responsibility for positive outcomes in social contexts was associated with increased activity in regions related to social and reward processing. Together, these results show that sharing responsibility with others reduces agency, influencing pre-outcome motor preparation and post-outcome processing, and provides opportunities to flexibly claim credit for positive outcomes.

INTRODUCTION

In collective decision-making, we have less control over the choices and outcomes than when we are making decisions alone. We are, however, not completely bound by constraints or instructions as to what to do. In return for this partial concession of control to the collective, we benefit from a sharing of responsibility for our choices (El Zein, Bahrami, & Hertwig, 2019). Indeed, when people assign credit to contributors in a team, they tend to overestimate their own contribution and over attribute a team's success to their own abilities, effort, and merit (Farwell & Wohlwend-Lloyd, 1998). Conversely, when outcomes are poor, the collective context allows us to distance ourselves from regret (El Zein & Bahrami, 2020) and offload blame onto others (El Zein, Seikus, De-Wit, & Bahrami, 2019). Teams are more likely to violate rules than individuals (Lohse & Simon, 2021), and, correspondingly, people find it harder to punish groups (vs. individuals) that have violated a social norm (El Zein, Seikus, et al., 2019). The advantages of such flexibility are not restricted to the psychology laboratory and can be observed in everyday life. When weapons of mass destruction were not found in Iraq in 2003 or the years that followed, intelligence agencies whose reports had justified the catastrophic invasion of Iraq defended themselves by claiming that “everyone had agreed at the time.”

The brain mechanisms that underlie our sense of responsibility for the outcomes of our actions have generally been investigated by comparing active versus forced (or passive) choices (Caspar, Beyer, Cleeremans, & Haggard, 2021; Caspar, Christensen, Cleeremans, & Haggard, 2016; Kool, Getz, & Botvinick, 2013; Desmurget et al., 2009; Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). The subjective experience of a coerced (or instructed) action is similar to that of a passive action and is associated with reduced neural processing of an action's outcome (Caspar et al., 2016). These studies have invariably focused on the context of isolated individuals making private decisions. However, in social contexts, for example, voting, we do not operate in the extremes of free versus coerced choice. As explained above, the collective context affords a level of cognitive flexibility that helps individuals favorably serve themselves by claiming credit or avoiding blame. As such, examining the neurobiological basis of shared responsibility and agency in the collective context opens a unique and novel door to the flexibility of human cognition that goes beyond earlier studies on the neurobiology of agency in private decision-making.

In this study, we investigated the neurobiological substrates of this flexible sense of responsibility in a social collective context. Operationally, we defined responsibility as a participant's subjective judgment on the causal attribution between a decision and its outcome. In this sense, we followed the lead of earlier literature that proposes a strong connection between responsibility, the “sense of agency” and feeling of control (El Zein, Bahrami, et al., 2019; Haggard, 2017; Caspar et al., 2016; Frith, 2014). We replicated previous investigations that focused on the evaluation of outcomes in free versus instructed decision-making by individuals (Beyer, Sidarus, Bonicalzi, & Haggard, 2017; Caspar et al., 2016; Li, Han, Lei, Holroyd, & Li, 2011; Li et al., 2010) and went beyond those studies to examine the impact of various collective group sizes on the processing of positive and negative outcomes.

Moreover, it has been suggested that examining the brain's responses to outcomes has provided a very convenient methodological approach to the complicated concept of responsibility (Haggard, 2017). Our experimental design permitted us to go one important conceptual step further and ask if the neurobiological substrates of responsibility in the human brain emerge during deliberation, thus before a choice is made and the outcome is known.

In a choice-based gambling task, participants made decisions that led to positive or negative outcomes, whereas responsibility was parametrically modulated by the impact of different social contexts. We constructed four different contexts: (1) Private, in which the individual participant assumed full responsibility; (2) Dyadic, and (3) Group, in which the individual decided together with one or four other people, respectively, and shared the responsibility with them; and (4) Forced, where another person decided on behalf of the participant, thus absolving the participant of all responsibility. Critically, the statistical frequencies of various outcomes were kept constant across all conditions, thus controlling for the expected value of options and choices. Another important issue that distinguishes our design from previous works is the distinction between actions and decisions. A number of previous studies showed that performing actions together with others reduces subjective ratings of responsibility and control (Dewey, Pacherie, & Knoblich, 2014; Nicolle, Bach, Frith, & Dolan, 2011; Li et al., 2010). However, our study is the first to investigate joint responsibility for collective decisions. We expected responsibility to be highest in the Private context and progressively decrease from Dyadic to Group, and then to its lowest level in the Forced context (behavioral Hypothesis 1a).

We used magnetoencephalography (MEG), which provides a high temporal resolution neural signal, to unravel the dynamics in the neural processes that underlie responsibility in social contexts at various stages of the task. Our design allowed us to conduct trial-by-trial regressions of responsibility contexts with MEG signals (El Zein, Wyart, & Grèzes, 2015; Wyart, de Gardelle, Scholl, & Summerfield, 2012), instead of comparing grand-average, event-related fields under high versus low responsibility contexts as previously done (Beyer et al., 2017; Caspar et al., 2016; Li et al., 2010, 2011). Previous studies found a decrease in the neural signatures of outcome processing that resulted in reduced responsibility, for example, when people are coerced to perform an action (vs. willingly performing the same action)—an auditory tone signaling the outcome of the action evoked a lower, auditory, evoked potential (N1; Caspar et al., 2016). Moreover, the feedback-related negativity—an evoked potential that appears around 200–300 msec after outcome onset—is also attenuated when an outcome resulted from an action performed in the presence of another agent (Beyer et al., 2017), with others (Li et al., 2010), and during a task where participants' control over outcomes was modulated (Li et al., 2011). A frontoparietal brain network—including the inferior parietal lobule, the angular gyrus, and the premotor and motor cortices—is implicated in the subjective feeling of control (Chambon, Wenke, Fleming, Prinz, & Haggard, 2013; Desmurget & Sirigu, 2012; Desmurget et al., 2009; Haggard, 2009). Based on these findings, we predicted attenuated outcome processing as a function of decreased responsibility, in particular at 200–300 msec after outcome onset with localization in a frontoparietal network (Hypothesis 2).

To identify neural signatures of outcome-independent, prospective responsibility, we examined a MEG signal for a sense of agency in the time period before the outcome. It has been suggested that when deliberating on an action, our sense of agency stems from a mental simulation of that action (Haggard, 2017; Jeannerod, 2001; Gallagher, 2000). Prospective responsibility, in this sense, corresponds to the mental simulation of the likely outcomes of imagined actions. One study, in which participants underwent fMRI while reading vignette scenarios and imagining themselves or others as the protagonist, showed that the contemplation of the consequences of imagined actions recruited premotor cortex (Blackwood et al., 2003) consistent with a mental simulation account. Thus, we predicted that before an outcome, the preparation of motor activity would be modulated by the responsibility context. Specifically, we hypothesized that lateralized motor preparation signals (El Zein et al., 2015; Donner, Siegel, Fries, & Engel, 2009) would be attenuated in social contexts where responsibility is reduced (Hypothesis 3).

Finally, we addressed the influence of outcome valence and its interaction with responsibility. Previous research has shown that people tend to have a self-serving bias, whereby they take more credit for positive outcomes as compared with negative outcomes—an effect evident in both group and individual decision-making (Jaquiery & El Zein, 2021; Yoshie & Haggard, 2013; Leary & Forsyth, 1987; Caine & Schlenker, 1979; Forsyth & Schlenker, 1977). Based on these data, we expected to observe increased responsibility ratings for positive versus negative outcomes irrespective of whether participants made decisions alone or with others, but not when they had no responsibility for the decision, that is, in the Forced context (Hypothesis 1b). In addition, we hypothesized that, because social contexts offer a possibility to share responsibility, this would enable participants to cherry pick credit for positive outcomes and offload blame for negative outcomes onto others (El Zein, Bahrami, et al., 2019), thereby exaggerating a self-serving bias in the Dyadic and Group contexts versus the Private context. Importantly, our experimental design allowed us to examine two distinct possible mechanisms: a concurrent increase of credit and decrease in blame, or a specific modulation of either credit or blame, respectively (Hypothesis 1c).

METHODS

Participants

Previous electroencephalography studies in relation to high versus low control over outcomes had sample sizes of 16–22 participants (Caspar et al., 2016; Li et al., 2010, 2011). As we had four control/responsibility conditions instead of two, we aimed to double this number for our study. To allow for exclusions, we tested 46 healthy adults (24 women, mean age = 24.13 ± 4.42) in our MEG study. Two participants were excluded because of technical errors in saving the data or in the triggers' information. Four participants were excluded because of noisy MEG data noted in the preprocessing phase (their data had more than 10 noisy channels and/or more than 15% of trials removed after visual inspection). This left 40 participants (21 women, age = 24.00 ± 4.46). The study was conducted at the Wellcome Centre for Neuroimaging, London. Participants were recruited by e-mail via the University College London (UCL) and the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience participant pool. All participants were aged 18–36 years, right-handed, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had no neurological or psychiatric history. They provided written, informed consent according to the regulations approved by the UCL research ethics committee (Project ID Number 9923/002). Participants were informed that they would receive £25 for their participation and a bonus of up to £5 based on their gains during the experiment. All participants were given the bonus and compensated with £30.

Experimental Design

Stimuli were generated using Cogent 2000 and Cogent Graphics toolboxes running in MATLAB (MathWorks, Inc.). The task was presented as a learning game where participants had to choose between two different gambles that supposedly had different probabilities of getting a positive or negative outcome. The two gambles corresponded to two different images of hand-drawn, realistic gambling devices from among 40 total drawings (an example of two drawings is shown in Figure 1B). Two gambles were pseudorandomly presented for each trial from among a set of 10 gambles for each block: We controlled that the gambles were presented for approximately the same amount of repetition within each context (contexts described below Figure 1A) and each block, and not more than 3 times in a row. Unbeknownst to participants, the gambles were not associated with different probabilities that yielded positive or negative outcomes, as the frequency of positive and negative outcomes was entirely controlled for and was equal to 50% positive and 50% negative.

Figure 1. .

Figure 1. 

Experimental design. (A) Participants performed a decision-making task in four contexts: (1) Private: playing alone; (2) Dyadic: together with another participant; (3) Group: together in a group of five participants; and (4) Forced: another participant played for them. Dyadic and Group contexts are henceforth referred to as social contexts. (B) Each trial (384 trials in eight blocks), began with participants having 1 sec to see the context (as described in a) in which they would be playing. Next, they chose between two gambling options that were displayed on the screen for a maximum of 2 sec. After making a choice, the selected gamble remained on the screen for 200 msec. In the social contexts, the selected gamble may or may not coincide with the gamble chosen by the group. Finally, the outcome (positive or negative) was displayed for 1 sec. The trials of each block were randomly assigned into three groups. One third of participants rated how responsible they felt for the upcoming outcome on a continuous scale immediately after the gamble selection (here marked by T1). In another third of the trials, participants rated how responsible they felt for the obtained outcome on a continuous scale after the outcome (T2). In the remaining third of the trials, no rating was required.

In each trial, following a fixation cross displayed for 700–900 msec, participants first saw a cue (duration 1000 msec) indicating which context they would be playing in. There were four possible contexts (Figure 1A): (1) Private; (2) Dyadic: A participant plays with one other player, so that both players make a decision, but only one of their decisions is selected to determine the outcome; (3) Group: A participant plays with four other people where the selected gamble is based on a majority vote (the gamble picked by three or more people)—(2) and (3) are referred to as Social contexts; and (4) Forced: Another player chooses on behalf of the participant (participants did not have the choice to not accept the other player's selection). After the cue, a fixation cross was displayed for a variable period of 1000–1200 msec. Then, two gambles appeared on the screen—one on the left and one on the right side of the cross—and participants had to select between them by pressing the respective button on two external devices that they held in their right (right gamble) and left (left gamble) hands (Figure 1B). The devices are 932 USB Keypads recorded directly into the MEG sending triggers on their own channel via a parallel port.

In the Forced context, participants were instructed to always press the same button after seeing the gambles. They were informed whether to press the left or the right button (constant throughout the experiment) in this context at the beginning of the experiment, and this was counterbalanced across participants. This button press was included to ensure that action requirements for all trial types were identical, but does not indicate an actual choice, which allowed us to maintain identical stimulus–response mappings. Participants were told that different gambles had different probabilities of winning and losing, and that they should try and pick the one that had a higher chance of winning. In all trials, the response window was 2 sec, otherwise the trial was classified as missed (even in the Forced context). The selected gamble was then displayed for 200 msec followed by a variable blank period of 650–850 msec. The trial ended with an outcome (positive/negative) presentation (1000 msec). Participants were told that one trial would subsequently be picked from each block and that they would earn a cumulative bonus based on whether the outcomes of selected trials were positive or negative, with a missed trial counting as a negative outcome. Note that positive outcomes allowed them to win a bonus, whereas negative outcomes were similar to “neutral” outcomes as they could not “lose” any money.

In one third of the trials, immediately after having made a decision, but before an outcome was shown (i.e., T1; see Figure 1B), participants rated how responsible they felt for the upcoming outcome (from “not at all” to “partially” to “very much” on a continuous scale). In another third of the trials, they made a similar rating immediately after the outcome (i.e., T2). In one third of the trials, no ratings were required.

Participants completed 384 trials in eight blocks of 48 trials each. Each block was composed of a balanced number of trials: 2 contexts (Alone, Forced) × 2 outcomes (Positive/Negative) × 3 scales (T1, T2 or none) × 2 repetitions; and in the Social contexts: 2 contexts (Dyadic, Group) × 2 outcomes (Positive/Negative) × 3 scales (T1, T2, or none) × 2 feedback (whether the participant's vote was selected or not).

Behavioral Analyses

Responsibility ratings, based on a continuous scale, were z-scored for each participant before subject and group-level statistical analyses were made and are reported in arbitrary units. A general linear model (GLM) was performed for each participant's responsibility ratings with the parametric responsibility contexts as a regressor (1, 2, 3, 4): 1 = Private, 2 = Dyadic, 3 = Group, 4 = Forced. The betas of these regressions were tested against zero with a t test for the group-level statistics. Paired student t tests were used to compare ratings in each context from one another and to assess the differences between ratings for positive and negative outcomes as well as differences between ratings both before and after an outcome. RTs for the gamble decision were also compared between contexts using paired student t tests. The mean of the effect μ across participants (responsibility ratings, RTs, or the parameter estimate of the regression computed for each participant), confidence intervals of the effects, and effect size (Cohen's d for one-sample t tests or standardized Cohen's dz for matched-pairs t tests) are provided.

MEG Acquisition and Preprocessing

MEG data were recorded using a 275-channel tr Omega system whole-head gradiometer (VSM MedTech) that uses axial gradiometers. Neuromagnetic signal was continuously recorded at a 600-Hz sampling rate with a low-pass filter at ¼ of the sampling rate at 150 Hz. After participants were comfortably seated in the MEG, head localizer coils were attached to the nasion and 1-cm anterior to the left and right tragus to monitor their head movement during recording. Because of technical issues, three gradiometers were disabled from the system: MLO42, MRC12, and MLT54, leaving 272 instead of 275 channels. Three additional channels recorded eye movements (x, y movement, and pupil diameter) using an eye-tracker (SR Research EyeLink nonferrous infrared eye tracking system).

Stimuli were projected at a 60-Hz frequency on a screen of 42 × 32 cm, with a distance of about 60 cm between the screen and the eyes. During piloting, a photodiode placed on the screen allowed us to measure a delay of about 33 msec between the trigger signal and the projection of stimuli. The appearance of stimuli on the screen was therefore monitored with a photodiode attached at the lower edge of the screen. A black square was presented there for 50 msec at the time when the stimulus was presented. The signal from the photodiodes was recorded in parallel to the other MEG channels to provide a precise temporal marker for the appearance of stimuli. All triggers were matched to the signal of the photodiode associated with each stimulus of interest.

We used FieldTrip version 2018 (Oostenveld, Fries, Maris, & Schoffelen, 2011) and MATLAB 2017a to process the data. The raw MEG data were notched for the 50-, 100-, and 150-Hz power line noise before visual inspection (combined with an automatic detection of artifacts) to reject trials with jumps and strong muscular activity. The data were low-pass filtered at 35 Hz and epoched using the photodiode signal locked to gamble onset, response onset, and outcome onset. Independent component analysis was performed on the data to correct for eye blinks, saccades, and heartbeat components thanks to the visual inspection of the components. The mean proportion of rejected, artifacted trials was 7.7% ± 3.1 for context-locked data, 6.5% ± 3.4 for gamble-locked data, 6.4% ± 3.4 for response-locked data, and 5.9% ± 2.8 for outcome-locked data.

Time–frequency decompositions were performed by computing the spectral power of the 8- to 32-Hz frequency bands using multitapering transform (Slepian tapers, 8–32 Hz, four cycles) centered on gamble presentation and response onset, using the FieldTrip function ft_freqanalysis. The power spectrum was extracted for the main analysis, and the complex Fourier spectrum was extracted for the source reconstruction analyses.

MEG Analyses

Regression Analyses of Outcome-locked MEG Signals

We performed single-trial regressions of MEG signals low-pass filtered at 8 Hz (note that all the results are the same whether this filter is applied or not on the MEG data) against variables of interest:

  • Responsibility context (1, 2, 3, 4) as for the behavioral analyses: 1 = Private, 2 = Dyadic, 3 = Group, 4 = Forced. Please note that negative parameter estimates indicate higher activity for more responsibility.

    We included all the other experimental factors as additional regressors:

    -Feedback: _ + 1 vote selected, −1 vote not selected. In the private context +1 as the participants vote was always selected. In the Forced context, −1 as the participant's choice was never selected.

    -Outcome: +1 positive outcome, −1 negative outcome

  • Social (+1 for the Dyadic and Group contexts) versus Private (−1) contexts. This regression thus used a centered regressor. This analysis was a post hoc analysis based on behavioral analyses and was done in one specific condition: when the outcome was positive and the participant's choice was selected, and therefore did not include additional regressors.

These regressors were entered in addition to the always present intercept term in the regression for each participant. The effects shown are thus independent from the model intercept.

As our hypothesis predicted that outcome processing would be modulated by responsibility at 200–300 msec, we performed regressions on the mean activity of MEG signals at 200–300 msec after outcome onset at each of the 272 electrodes. Beta coefficients of regressions for each participant were tested against zero for the group-level statistics. Multiple comparison corrections were performed in the electrode space using ft_prepare_neighbours in FieldTrip, coupled with nonparametric Monte Carlo statistics to determine the clusters of electrodes where these regressions were significant with a p value < .05 (cluster corrections, cluster alpha = .05, test statistic set as the maximum level of the cluster-level statistic, alpha = .05, 10000 randomizations, two minimal neighboring channels).

We report μ, the overall mean of the parameter estimate (computed for each participant) across participants, as well as confidence intervals of the effects and effect size (Cohen's d for one-sample t tests or standardized Cohen's dz for matched-pairs t tests).

Motor Preparation Measures in the Time–Frequency Domain

Motor lateralization.

The suppression of 8- to 32-Hz frequency bands in the hemisphere contralateral to the hand used in a motor press response provides a neural marker for motor preparation (Donner et al., 2009). For each participant, the power of this frequency band when participants responded with their right hand was subtracted from the power when they responded with their left hand, at 100 msec before a response press. When averaged across participants, this allowed us to identify the central electrodes with maximal suppression (Figure 4A). We note that doing the same analysis on the time window from −100 to −50 msec (50-msec time window) or −100 msec to response (100-msec time window) results in similar topographies and electrodes with maximal activity. Motor lateralization for responses with the right and left hand was obtained by subtracting power activity in the central electrodes contralateral to the utilized hand from power activity in the central electrodes ipsilateral to the hand used, thus resulting in positive motor preparation as shown in Figure 4B (top).

Right electrodes: MRC13; MRC14; MRC22; MRC31; MRC32; MRC41; MRC42; MRC53; MRC54; MRC55.

Left electrodes: MLC13; MLC14; MLC22; MLC31; MLC22; MLC41; MLC42; MLC53; MLC54; MLC55.

We performed regressions on this motor lateralization measure at each time point, locked to the gamble and to the response, using the following regressor:

  • Responsibility context (1, 2, 3) 1 = Private, 2 = Dyadic, 3 = Group

  • Social context (+1 pooling both Dyadic and Group) versus Private (−1)

We added RTs (z scored) as an additional regressor, to account for effects over and above differences in RTs for motor press. The parameter estimates were tested for significance against zero at each time point from 0 to 600 msec after gamble onset, with multiple comparison corrections across time points implemented using nonparametric cluster-level statistics (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). The pairing between experimental conditions and MEG signals was shuffled 1000 times, and the maximum cluster-level statistics (the sum of one-tailed t values across contiguously significant time points at a threshold level of 0.05) was extracted for each shuffle to compute a null distribution of the size of the effect across a time window of (0, +600) msec locked to stimulus presentation, or (−600, 0) msec locked to response onset. For significant clusters in the original (nonshuffled) data, we computed the proportion of clusters in the null distribution where statistics exceeded that of the one obtained for the cluster in question, as it corresponds to its “cluster-corrected” p value.

Figure 4. .

Figure 4. 

Motor preparation signals are modulated by the responsibility context. (A) Topography showing the mean power of 8–32 Hz frequencies at 100 msec before the motor response for conditions where participants answered with the left hand minus the right hand and the associated estimated sources. Bottom: response-locked motor lateralization: motor preparation from 0.8 sec before and up to response measured with the power of 8–32 Hz frequencies in ipsilateral minus contralateral electrodes relative to the hand pressed, locked to the motor response in all four different contexts. (B) Stimulus-locked motor lateralization. Top: mean parameter estimate across participants of the regression of motor preparation signal against the Social versus Private regressor done at each time point after stimulus onset. Negative parameter estimates indicate lower motor preparation in the Social versus Private context, which is significant around 500 msec after gamble onset. The black line indicates time points where the parameter estimate is significant against zero (cluster, one-tailed, pcorr < .05). The blue (Private) and red (Social) bars indicate the time when the button was pressed based on mean RTs and their standard errors: note that the means and standard errors are overlapping for the two contexts. Bottom: motor preparation signal at the peak of the effect at 516 msec for the Private versus the Social context. **p < .01.

Source Reconstruction

Minimum-norm source estimates were performed using Brainstorm (Tadel, Baillet, Mosher, Pantazis, & Leahy, 2011). We computed kernel inversion matrices for each subject and for each of the eight blocks (recommended because of differences in participants' head movements in different blocks), using all trials consisting of nonoverlapping time windows locked to context, gamble, and outcome. We used a generic brain model taken from the default anatomy in Brainstorm: ICBM 152 Nonlinear atlas Version 2009 (Fonov et al., 2011; Fonov, Evans, McKinstry, Almli, & Collins, 2009). The head model was computed with an overlapping spheres method. The noise covariance was computed based on a 900-msec baseline before the context onset (i.e., the baseline of the whole trial). Sources were computed with minimum norm imaging and the current density map method. Following recommendations for when there is no individual anatomical MRI available, we chose unconstrained solutions to source estimation. In unconstrained source maps, there are three dipoles with orthogonal orientations at each cortex location (15,002 vertices × 3 orientations = 45,006 × 272 electrodes, 8 inversion matrices—for each of the 8 blocks—for each participant). To display these as an activity map and perform statistics, the norm of the vectorial sum of the three orientations at each vertex is computed as follows:

S=Sx2+Sy2+Sz2
GLM in the source space.

At 200–300 msec after outcome onset for each subject, the mean MEG signals were multiplied by individual kernel matrices for each block, and the norm was computed before implementing the GLMs at each vertex at the source level. The averaging in time was performed before computing the norm of the triplets of dipoles. Finally, the betas at each vertex were averaged across blocks, resulting in one map of parameter estimates for the effect of interest for each participant. Then, for group-level analyses, t tests against zero were done at each vertex, and only the vertices with mean parameter estimates significant across participants at p value < .01 are shown. To assess the anatomical location of the significant vertices, we first reported regions based on the Destrieux Atlas (Destrieux, Fischl, Dale, & Halgren, 2010) that is provided in Brainstorm. Second, we extracted Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates of regions and projected them onto the human Brainnetome Atlas (BNA; Fan et al., 2016; through MRIcron where MNI coordinates can be matched to the anatomical location of the chosen atlas).

Motor lateralization.

At 100 msec before response press for each participant, the MEG Fourier transforms from the time–frequency decomposition analysis were multiplied by individual kernel matrices for each frequency and trial. Then, after taking the power of the magnitude of the complex Fourier spectrum, a mean was performed on the frequencies (8–32 Hz) and the conditions where the participant responded with the right hand and the conditions where the participant responded with the left hand. The sum of the power for each orientation was computed for each block, before doing a mean on the eight blocks to obtain one activity map per condition and per subject. Left press versus right press were then contrasted by performing a t test in the source level after a z score of the activity map per participant, and keeping only significant vertices at p < .01 for the figure. The mean difference across all participants at the vertices with this significant effect is shown (Figure 4A, bottom). Note that we only did the source reconstruction at response time and did not do it for the effects locked to the stimulus: we followed previous studies (El Zein et al., 2015; Wyart et al., 2012; Donner et al., 2009) by extracting the maximum activity central sensors when the motor response is considered to be the strongest just before response. We source localized this activity to confirm its motor sources. Then, we examined how this motor preparation is modulated since stimulus onset.

RESULTS

Behavioral Results

Participants performed an apparent learning task where they had to choose between two gambles that could yield positive or negative outcomes (at chance level, 50% positive 50% negative, although participants believed some gambles had higher probabilities of winning or losing). They did so in four different contexts: alone (Private context), with one other player (Dyadic context), in a group of five players (Group context), and where another player decided for them (Forced context; Figure 1A). In the last context, Forced, participants were requested to press the right or left button although it did not count as their choice. As they already knew which button to press, participants were faster to respond in a Forced context compared with all the other contexts (all T39 > 6.4, all p < .001). Participants were also faster at making a decision in the Dyadic context as compared with the Group context (T39 = −2.63, p = .01, μ = −15.94, CI [−28.18, −3.71], dz = 0.41), but no other differences were observed. After a decision was made between the two gambles, participants used a scale to report how responsible they felt for the outcome of the trial. These responsibility ratings represent our main behavioral variable of interest (Figure 1B).

Parametric Responsibility Reporting

To test Hypothesis 1a, we regressed z-scored, continuous responsibility ratings against responsibility contexts (1 = Private, 2 = Dyadic, 3 = Group, 4 = Forced) for each participant. All participants except for one (p > .2) showed a significant negative slope (37 participants p < .001, one participant p < .005, one participant p < .02), that is, they reported a linearly decreasing sense of responsibility, starting from the Private context and moving down to the Dyadic, Group, and then Forced contexts (t test of the betas computed for each participant against zero, T39 = −19.72, p < .001, μ = −0.61, CI [−0.67, −0.55], d = −3.11; Figure 2A). t-Test comparisons between contexts confirmed this linear change in responsibility: Responsibility ratings were higher in the Private context as compared with the Dyadic context (T39 = 12.14, p < .001, μ = 0.76, CI [0.63, 0.89], dz = 1.95). Dyadic context ratings were higher than Group context ratings (T39 = 6.08, p < .001, μ = 0.24, CI [0.16, 0.33], dz = 0.96). Finally, Group context ratings were higher than those in the Forced context (T39 = 13.19, p < .001, μ = 0.96, CI [0.81, 1.10], dz = 2.08).

Figure 2. .

Figure 2. 

Behavioral results. Responsibility ratings were reported on a continuous scale from “not at all responsible” to “very much responsible” (see Figure 1B). The ratings were z scored for each participant for the analyses and are therefore reported in arbitrary units in all graphs. (A) Parametric decrease of reported responsibility from Private to Dyadic to Group to Forced contexts. Circles represent the mean values across participants for each context with the associated standard errors. The line is the mean general linear model fit (fit for each participant, then averaged across all 40 participants). (B) The impact of outcome valence on reported responsibility. The y axis shows the differences in responsibility claimed for positive (R+) and negative (R) outcomes. In all cases where the participant had some choice in the selection of the gamble, they claimed more responsibility for positive (vs. negative) outcomes. The Forced context, where the subject had no say in gamble selection did not show a similar bias. (C) When compared with reported responsibility before the outcome, we observed that outcome valence modulated responsibility in both positive and negative directions. Data from the Private context alone: Participants both claimed more credit after positive outcomes and disavowed a negative outcome. The y axis shows the difference between responsibility ratings after versus before the outcome. The x axis shows outcome valence. (D) When compared with pre-outcome ratings, people claimed more credit for a positive outcome in the social contexts when their vote was selected compared with the Private context. Error bars represent standard errors. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05, ns: nonsignificant.

These behavioral results confirm a reduced sense of responsibility in Social contexts, and furthermore show that this reduction depends on group size. Moreover, the reports of decreased responsibility validate our experimental paradigm, which was designed to address the neural processes involved in decision-making in different responsibility contexts.

Self-serving Bias

Our behavioral Hypothesis 1b stated that responsibility ratings would reveal a self-serving bias, with participants attributing more responsibility to themselves for positive (vs. negative) outcomes. We observed that participants indeed provided higher responsibility ratings for positive (vs. negative) outcomes in the three active contexts (Private context: T39 = 5.18, p < .001, μ = 0.28, CI [0.17, 0.39], dz = 0.82; Dyadic context: T39 = 3.92, p < .001, μ = 0.25, CI [0.12, 0.39], dz = 0.61; and Group context: T39 = 4.89, p < .001, μ = 0.29, CI [0.16, 0.40], dz = 0.77), but not in the Forced context (T39 = 1.76, p > .08, μ = 0.09, CI [−0.01, 0.20], dz = 0.27; Figure 2B). The magnitude of this bias did not differ across the three active contexts (all p > .48, all T > 0.69). Contrary to our Hypothesis 1c, we found no evidence for an increase in self-serving bias in the Social (vs. Private) contexts.

Claiming Credit for Success or Disavowing Blame for Failure?

Next, we examined if a self-serving bias was observed through the attribution of credit after positive outcomes or disavowal of responsibility after negative outcomes. In different trials, we asked participants to report their responsibility ratings before and after an outcome. Taking the before ratings as a baseline, we then assessed whether the self-serving bias consisted of higher ratings after positive outcomes and/or lower ratings after negative outcomes. For each outcome valence, we subtracted the before ratings from the after ratings.

In the Private context, a self-serving bias was demonstrated in both components: more responsibility was claimed after (vs. before) positive outcomes (T39 = 4.41, p < .001, μ = 0.16, CI [0.09, 0.23], dz = 0.72) and less responsibility was accepted after (vs. before) negative outcomes (T39 = −2.96, p < .01, μ = −0.12, CI [−0.20, 0.03], dz = −0.46; Figure 2C).

In the Social contexts, a more complex analysis was required to accommodate the relationship between a participant's decision, the collective choice, and the outcome. When the collective choice matched the participant's decision, responsibility ratings were boosted after (vs. before) an outcome (T39 > 4.26, p < .001). It is important to note that participants claimed more credit in Social contexts (vs. the Private context) for positive outcomes (Private vs. Dyadic: T39 = −2.51, p < .02, μ = −0.15, CI [−0.27, −0.03], dz = −0.39; Private vs. Group: T39 = −4.39, p < .001, μ = −0.23, CI [−0.33, −0.12], dz = −0.70; Figure 2D). This finding is partly consistent with Hypothesis 1c in revealing how Social contexts offered a “cover” for claiming more credit for a positive outcome. Participants, however, did not disavow responsibility for negative outcomes (T39 > 0.59, p < .55), which led to a conclusion that was opposite to our prediction: Participants disavowed negative outcomes more in Private versus Social contexts (Private vs. Dyadic: T39 = −3.05, p = .004, μ = −0.15, CI [−0.25, −0.05], dz = −0.48; Private vs. Group: T39 = −2.79, p = .007, μ = −0.14, CI [−0.25, −0.04], dz = −0.42). In trials where the collective choice was different from the participant's decision, responsibility ratings were generally lower after (vs. before) an outcome (T39 > 5.3, p < .001).

MEG Results

Outcome Processing Is Parametrically Modulated by Shared Responsibility

Our key neural hypothesis stated that neural signatures for shared responsibility would be common to those identified for a sense of agency. Hypothesis 2 stated that outcome processing within 200–300 msec after outcome onset would vary linearly with responsibility levels. This signal would locate to a frontoparietal brain network that is associated with a sense of agency and includes the inferior parietal lobule, angular gyrus, and the premotor and motor cortices. We isolated preprocessed MEG signals in this time window and performed whole-brain regressions of those signals against the responsibility contexts (see Methods section). One cluster of electrodes showed significant activity that survived correction for multiple comparisons (cluster alpha < .05; Figure 3A, right panel central electrodes: MZC01, MZF02, MZF03, MLC11 to MLC16, MLC21 to MLC23, MLC51, MRC11; Frontal electrodes: MLF21 to MLF23, MLF25, MLF31 to MLF35, MLF41 to MLF46, MLF51 to MLF56, MLF61 to MLF67, MRF21, MRF41; temporal electrodes MLT11 to MLT13, MLT21, MLT22, MLT32. The mean parameter estimate of the regression for the significant electrodes is shown through time in Figure 3A, left panel (statistics of the effect at its peak in the 200- to 300-msec window at 266 msec (T39 = 3.20, p = .002, μ = 2.14, CI [0.79, 3.50], d = 0.50). With these same electrodes, we performed an additional descriptive analysis, computing event-related fields locked to outcome, separately for each context. The largest event-related field was observed for the Private context and then decreased linearly through the Forced context at ∼200 msec after outcome onset (Figure 3B). This is in line with the trial-by-trial GLM results. Finally, the same GLM regressions in the source space (see Methods section) revealed that the parametric encoding of responsibility at 200–300 msec after outcome onset is associated with frontal, parietal, and central sources (Figure 3C), which is described in detail in Table 1.

Figure 3. .

Figure 3. 

Agency-related neural correlates of responsibility at 200–300 msec after outcome onset. (A) Parameter estimate of responsibility regression. Right panel: scalp topography of the parameter estimate of responsibility regression (1 Private, 2 Dyadic, 3 Group of Five, 4 Forced) on the mean MEG activity at 200–300 msec after outcome. White dots represent the significant electrodes where the MEG signal linearly co-varies with the level of responsibility using cluster corrections of the effect against zero at an alpha cluster level < .05. Left panel: parameter estimate of responsibility regression at the significant cluster locked to outcome onset. (B) Associated event-related fields (ERF) at the same cluster show how the amplitude of the ERF locked to outcome increases with responsibility. (C) Estimated cortical sources of the responsibility parameter estimate at 200–300 msec. Parameter estimates of the responsibility regression at the source level that are significant at p < .05 are shown. R = right hemisphere and L = left hemisphere, and the associated numbers refer to the different brain regions reported and anatomically localized in Table 1.

Table 1. .

Anatomical Sources of Parametric Responsibility Encoding Locked to Outcome

Regions Destrieux Atlas MNI Coordinates Human Brainnetome Atlas (BNA)
Index Name Localization x y z Label ID Gyrus Anatomy Lobe
Left 1 (L1) 14 Triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus Inferior frontal gyrus −56 35 3        
Left 2 (L2) 16 Superior frontal gyrus Medial aspect of the frontal lobe −13 8 66 7 Superior frontal gyrus Dorsolateral Area 6 Frontal
Left 3 (L3) 67 Postcentral sulcus Main sulci of the lateral aspect of the parietal lobe −34 −36 45 159 Postcentral gyrus Area 2 Parietal
Left 4 (L4) 56 Intraparietal sulcus and transverse parietal sulci Main sulci of the lateral aspect of the parietal lobe −34 −46 45 129 Superior parietal lobule Lateral Area 5 Parietal
Left 5 (L5) 41 Posterior ramus of the lateral sulcus Insula −47 −39 25 145 Inferior parietal lobule Rostroventral Area 40 Parietal
Left 6 (L6) 73 Superior temporal sulcus Lateral aspect of the temporal and occipital lobes −47 −51 15 123 Posterior STS Caudoposterior Superior temporal sulcus Temporal
Left 7 (L7) 2 Inferior occipital gyrus and sulcus Ventral aspect of the temporal and occipital lobes −21 −84 18 209 Lateral occipital cortex Lateral superior occipital gyrus Occipital
Right 1 (R1) 16 Superior frontal gyrus Medial aspect of the frontal lobe 14 67 15 14 Superior frontal gyrus Medial Area 10 Frontal
Right 2 (R2) 15 Middle frontal gyrus Lateral aspect of the frontal lobe 33 35 50        
Right 3 (R3) 41 Posterior ramus of the lateral sulcus Insula 42 −33 19 146 Inferior parietal lobule Rostroventral Area 40 Parietal
Right 4 (R4) 34 Lateral aspect of the superior temporal gyrus Lateral aspect of the temporal and occipital lobes 60 10 −4.5 74 Superior temporal gyrus TE1.0 and TE1.2 Temporal
Right 5 (R5) 37 Inferior temporal gyrus Ventral aspect of the temporal and occipital lobes 64 −37 −22 102 Inferior temporal gyrus Caudoventral of Area 20 Temporal
Right 6 (R6) 25 Angular gyrus Inferior parietal lobule 56 −67 32 144 Inferior parietal lobule Rostroventral Area 39 Parietal
Right 7 (R7) 19 Middle occipital gyrus Lateral aspect of the temporal and occipital lobes 55 −70 24 136 Inferior parietal lobule Caudal Area 39 Parietal

Regions were determined based on the Destrieux Atlas and implemented in Brainstorm. Then, MNI coordinates were extracted from Brainstorm and projected onto the BNA atlas.

Neural Correlates of a Prospective (Outcome-independent) Sense of Responsibility

Having established a neural signature of responsibility in outcome processing, we then investigated a neuronal expression of pre-outcome responsibility under Private and Social contexts. Here, we refer to responsibility experienced before choice and outcome, which we hypothesized would be related to motor preparation activity and subject to modulation by the responsibility contexts. Specifically, we tested whether lateralized motor preparation signals to select a gamble with the left or right hand (Donner et al., 2009) decreased in the Social contexts where responsibility is reduced. We first computed motor preparation signals at 100 msec before the response button was pressed by subtracting the power in the 8- to 32-Hz frequency band when participants responded with the right hand from when they responded with the left hand, which allowed us to identify the central electrodes with maximal suppression (Figure 4A, top). Then, we subtracted the power in the 8- to 32-Hz frequency band in ipsilateral minus contralateral maximal central electrodes relative to the hand pressed (Figure 4A, bottom). We found that motor preparation signals increase gradually until response in all four contexts. It should be noted that motor preparation signals in the Forced context, where the choice of button press was already known and the RTs were fastest (Private: 605.78 msec ± 29.82, Dyadic: 601.00 msec ± 28.46, Group: 615.20 msec ± 26.81, Forced: 430.54 msec ± 16.02), diverged radically from the other conditions and were excluded from further hypothesis testing.

To test Hypothesis 3, we first asked whether motor preparation signals locked to gamble onset varied parametrically with responsibility. This first regression revealed a weak effect, peaking at 516 msec (T39 = −1.90, p = .03 one-tailed), which did not survive cluster multiple comparison corrections (p > .22). As our hypothesis involves social contexts where responsibility was shared, we conducted a new regression that pooled the Dyadic and Group contexts (i.e., Social contexts), allowing a comparison of the Private and Social contexts. The parameter estimate of this regression was significant at ∼500 msec after gamble stimulus onset (peak of parameter estimate at 516 msec, T39 = −2.81, p = .007 two-tailed, μ = −0.068, CI [−0.11, −0.02], d = 0.44, cluster from 500 to 566 msec two-tailed pcorr = .09, cluster from 466 to 566 msec, one-tailed pcorr < .05; Figure 4B, top), where a stronger motor preparation signal was evident for Private compared with Social contexts (direct two-tailed t test at the peak 516 msec between Private and Social contexts, T39 = 2.78, p = .008, μ = 0.14, CI [0.03, 0.24], dz = 0.45; Figure 4B, bottom). No significant cluster was observed for the same analyses locked to motor response rather than gamble onset (p > .1), suggesting that the effect is related to the motor intention locked to the stimulus, rather than the motor action itself.

Neural Correlates of Increased Claims of Credit in Social Contexts

Earlier, we provided behavioral analyses that showed that a Social (vs. Private) context boosted the credit claimed for positive outcomes. In an exploratory analysis, we examined the neural correlates of this specific positive boost, focusing on Private and Social contexts for trials where the collective choice matched a participant's vote. Concentrating on a post-outcome, 200- to 300-msec time window, we identified MEG signals locked to positive outcomes and then ran a GLM with Social versus Private as the regressor. This revealed a significant cluster (cluster alpha < .05) that included frontal, temporal, and central electrodes (Figure 5A; frontal: MLF12 to MLF14, MLF22 to MLF25, MLF32 to MLF35, MLF43 to MLF46, MLF53 to MLF56, MLG63 to MLF67, temporal: MLT12 MLT13, MLT21 to MLT23 and central: MLC12 to MLC16, MLC21 to MLC24, MLC31 MLC41 MLC51 MLC52). No significant clusters appeared for the same regression analysis for MEG signals locked to negative outcomes. The same regression conducted at MEG source level for signals locked to positive outcomes revealed source estimates in the OFC and temporal lobe, including the STS (see details of brain regions in Table 2, Figure 5B).

Figure 5. .

Figure 5. 

The processing of positive outcomes in Social versus Private contexts. (A) Scalp topography of the parameter estimate of the regression Social versus Private on the mean MEG activity at 200–300 msec after positive outcomes following decisions that matched the participant's vote. White dots represent the significant cluster of electrodes differentiating between Social and Private outcome processing using cluster corrections of the effect against zero (cluster alpha < .05). (B) Estimated cortical sources of the Social versus Private parameter estimate at 200–300 msec. Parameter estimates that are significant at p < .01 are shown. R = right hemisphere and L = left hemisphere, and the associated numbers refer to the different brain regions reported and anatomically localized in Table 2.

Table 2. .

Anatomical Sources of Social versus Private Processing of Positive Outcomes

Regions Destrieux Atlas MNI Coordinates Human Brainnetome Atlas (BNA)
Index Name Localization x y z Label ID Gyrus Anatomy Lobe
Left 1 (L1) 24 Orbital gyri Ventral aspect of the frontal lobe −21 14 −27        
63 Medial orbital sulcus Ventral aspect of the frontal lobe −14 17 −15 49 Orbital gyrus Area 13 Frontal
  Gyrus rectus Medial aspect of the frontal lobe −3 11 −23        
  Subcallosal gyrus Limbic gyrus −4 6 16        
Left 2 (L2)   Temporal pole Superior aspect of the temporal lobe −22 13 −43        
Left 3 (L3)   Triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus Main frontal gyri −51 45 5        
  Inferior frontal sulcus −40 44 2.0 21 Middle frontal gyrus Ventral Area 9/46 Frontal
15 Middle frontal gyrus −49 49 3        
Left 4 (L4) 28 Postcentral gyrus Lateral aspect of the parietal lobe −24 −31 77 161 Postcentral gyrus Area 1/2/3 Parietal
Left 5 (L5) 27 Superior parietal lobule Superior parietal lobule −37 −52 66        
Right 1 (R1) 64 Orbital sulcus Ventral aspect of the frontal lobe 23 39 −22 46 Orbital gyrus Lateral Area 11 Frontal
Right 2 (R2)   Orbital gyri 43 25 −18 44 Orbital gyrus Orbital Area 12/47
Right 3 (R3)   Orbital gyri 43 57 −11        
15 Middle frontal gyrus Main frontal gyri 39 63 −1 28 Middle frontal gyrus Lateral Area 10 Frontal
Right 4 (R4) 72 Superior temporal sulcus Lateral aspect of temporal lobe 67 −46 15 144 Inferior parietal lobule Rostroventral Area 39 Parietal
Right 5 (R5) 38 Middle temporal gyrus 64 −54 15 144
25 Angular gyrus Inferior parietal lobule 63 −56 19 144
Right 6 69 Superior part of the precentral sulcus Main frontal sulci 30 −4.5 64 8 Superior frontal gyrus Dorsolateral area Frontal

Regions were determined based on the Destrieux Atlas and implemented in Brainstorm. Then, MNI coordinates were extracted from Brainstorm and projected onto the BNA atlas. Gray areas represent MNI coordinates that could not be matched to the BNA atlas.

DISCUSSION

We examined the behavioral and neuronal signatures of shared responsibility in collective decision-making. Behaviorally, we showed that responsibility was reduced in collective contexts compared with private, individual decision-making, and this decrease was dependent on the size of the collective. Previous neurobiological findings on responsibility for socially executed actions have consistently shown decreased outcome processing under coercion (Caspar et al., 2016), cooperative gambling (Li et al., 2010), and in the presence of another person (Beyer et al., 2017). Our work goes beyond those studies in several important aspects. First, our study examined collective choice rather than action. Second, we developed a systematic, parametric design with four levels of responsibility that produced a highly reliable empirical framework for studying the subtle concept of responsibility. Third, we studied the neuronal mechanisms underlying the flexible interaction between social context and outcome valence that permitted participants to cherry-pick the level of responsibility that best served them with regard to claiming more credit for positive outcomes. Finally, whereas previous neural studies on responsibility had focused exclusively on outcome evaluation (Beyer et al., 2017; Caspar et al., 2016; Kool et al., 2013; Li et al., 2010, 2011), we could examine the neural substrates of responsibility during deliberation and action preparation before a choice was made or outcomes were known.

Our findings showed that people's subjective reports of responsibility varied according to social context, with greater responsibility reported when making decisions privately compared with when making decisions with others. This is in line with previous studies that show that people feel less control, responsibility, and regret when acting with others (El Zein & Bahrami, 2020; Dewey et al., 2014; Nicolle et al., 2011; Li et al., 2010). Reported responsibility varied with group size, with more responsibility reported in Dyads versus in a Group of five. This finding builds on previous results showing that people take less credit when they are problem-solving in a larger group (Forsyth, Zyzniewski, & Giammanco, 2002).

We showed that at 200–300 msec after an outcome, MEG-recorded activity of bilateral frontoparietal brain regions decreased linearly, from its highest at full responsibility in the Private context, to shared responsibility in Dyads, to shared responsibility in Groups, to no responsibility in the Forced context. This complements previous findings of decreased outcome processing associated with low responsibility contexts (Beyer et al., 2017; Caspar et al., 2016; Li et al., 2010), but goes beyond those earlier works by showing a parametric—and not categorical—relationship to an incremental manipulation of responsibility. Second, using source estimation, we confirmed our key neural hypothesis that a marker of responsibility is localized to brain regions previously associated with a sense of agency in frontal and central cortices, and superior and inferior parietal lobules (Chambon et al., 2013; Desmurget & Sirigu, 2012; Desmurget et al., 2009; Haggard, 2009; Chaminade & Decety, 2002). Our findings are also consistent with previous work that identified correlates of motor intentions in the parietal cortex (Desmurget et al., 2009) and motor control in inferior parietal lobule (Desmurget & Sirigu, 2012).

It is important to note that the outcome-dependent neural signature of agency identified here (Figure 3) was not influenced by outcome valence. In our design, we were mindful to ensure that the probability of winning did not depend on the gambling choice. The probability of winning was also independent of whether a participant decided privately, did not decide at all, or when the participant's choice matched that of the group or not. Furthermore, to minimize incidental learning, we used a large set of visual stimuli (i.e., 40 illustrations of various gambling devices) and had the participants choose between randomly sampled pairs.

Nevertheless, although no learning was possible in our experiment, it was framed to participants as a learning task, and therefore, we cannot exclude that the observed effects of responsibility are not in part also because of changes in learning mechanisms. In fact, similar brain signatures of outcome processing are observed in apparent learning tasks (Yeung, Holroyd, & Cohen, 2005) as for learning, and when participants are aware they do not control their choice (Li et al., 2011). Changes in responsibility in social contexts may be expected to change learning as agency plays a role in how people update prediction error in learning tasks (Chambon et al., 2020; Cockburn, Collins, & Frank, 2014). Moreover, making a decision in groups where agency and responsibility are decreased reduces the consideration of decision outcomes, which may suggest reduced learning from these outcomes (El Zein & Bahrami, 2020).

We predict that if differences in learning exist in the different responsibility contexts, they would be driven by the changes in responsibility rather than the other way around. Previous studies indeed support this idea as they have shown similar modulation of brain signals by responsibility as in our experiment in nonlearning contexts (Beyer et al., 2017; Caspar et al., 2016; Li et al., 2010). Future studies using more sophisticated designs to decorrelate changes in learning and responsibility could address the important question of specific and shared mechanisms involved in changes in responsibility and learning in social contexts.

With regard to collective decision-making, several nonmonetary motivational factors also came into play in our paradigm. The first factor was autonomy and control. Rewards have a higher salience when we are instrumental in obtaining them compared with when they are merely thrust upon us. Our key neural findings (Figure 3) are consistent with this, showing that the participant's level of involvement in an outcome modulates an outcome-dependent neural signal. Thus, brain responses to outcomes were stronger when participants decided privately and parametrically decreased as responsibility decreased. The second factor was the approval of others. Previous work shows that others' approval is, inherently, capable of driving the brain's motivational reward network even when no monetary reward is at stake (Campbell-Meiklejohn, Bach, Roepstorff, Dolan, & Frith, 2010). In our paradigm, participants' choices were sometimes agreed with and other times overruled by the collective. Accordingly, we found that approval by the collective was associated with increased neural activity in the OFC, the STS, and the temporal pole, brain regions variously associated with reward, social processing, and mentalizing (Schurz, Radua, Aichhorn, Richlan, & Perner, 2014; Frith & Frith, 2007). In addition to being associated with monetary reward and value processing (Lopez-Persem et al., 2020; Padoa-Schioppa & Conen, 2017), the human OFC is implicated in individual differences in conformity and reaction to other people's opinions (Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2012). As outcome processing of a social decision involves considering other people's responsibility, the involvement of the STS is in line with the finding that this region is involved when participants consider others' responsibility (Blackwood et al., 2003). Note that differences between social and private contexts in our experimental design can only be related to confirmation of choice by others and not to a change in the value of the outcome because it belongs to both the participant and other members of the group. Indeed, even in the social contexts, no rewards were given to other group members, their role was only to contribute to the decision—and only the participants received the rewards.

Previous studies of the neural basis of responsibility have invariably focused on the outcomes of decisions and actions. Our study breaks with this tradition and examines neural substrates of responsibility before an outcome, when deliberation and action preparation are taking place. We show that the motor preparation (to pick the right or left gamble) around 500 msec after the onset of a visual display of a gambling option was reduced under shared (Social) as compared with full responsibility (Private) conditions. We acknowledge that the effect is weak because only the one-tailed, and not the two-tailed, significant time cluster survived multiple comparisons, but we believe that the result is novel and should be taken seriously and discussed. This result was observed only when the analysis was stimulus-locked, but not locked to the motor response, indicating that the decreased motor preparation in Social versus Private contexts most likely reflects a deliberative rather than motor process. This is also in line with findings that self-responsibility, as compared with shared responsibility, recruits brain areas associated with action simulation, including the premotor context, suggesting that higher-order social processes may relate to simple goal-directed action (Blackwood et al., 2003). This effect did not follow a parametric pattern, as it did not decrease based on group size and thus seems to be related to a more general social context where responsibility is shared with others. A recent theoretical model proposed that a decreased sense of agency in social contexts relates to mentalizing processes, as people need to take into account the perspectives of others (Sidarus, Travers, Haggard, & Beyer, 2020; Beyer et al., 2017). This model posits that through mentalizing, social contexts increase decision disfluency and action planning. Here, we provide the neural evidence that action planning is indeed reduced when people make decisions with others in social contexts.

The higher responsibility ratings for positive versus negative outcomes (Figure 2B) replicates the “self-serving bias” effect, whereby participants take more credit for positive (vs. negative) outcomes (Yoshie & Haggard, 2013; Leary & Forsyth, 1987; Caine & Schlenker, 1979). It is also in line with an increased sense of agency for positive versus negative outcomes (Barlas, Hockley, & Obhi, 2017; Yoshie & Haggard, 2013; although the opposite effect was found in an unpredictable environment; Tanaka & Kawabata, 2021). Our experimental design included a subset of trials where we probed responsibility before an outcome was declared (Figure 2CD). This allowed us to treat these trials as a baseline and evaluate if the observed self-serving bias came from claiming more credit for positive outcomes and/or offloading blame for the negative ones. In addition, our design also distinguished between trials in which the group decision was in line with that of the participant and those when they were not. Consequently, collective decisions in which the group and participant agreed offered a particularly informative situation, as an individual retained some control but still shared responsibility with others. In these situations, participants were particularly inclined to claim disproportionately more credit for positive outcomes. These findings point to a potential motivation to join groups, particularly as claiming credit for success has been shown to increase self-esteem (Leary & Forsyth, 1987). Interestingly, in these trials, we did not observe any offloading of blame onto others for negative outcomes. This observation is consistent with a similar recent report (Sidarus et al., 2020).

Our novel experimental design manipulates levels of responsibility for the outcomes of decisions and shows that responsibility influences how these outcomes are processed at 200 msec in brain regions that are related to a sense of agency. Our results are also the first to provide an outcome-independent neural signature of responsibility evident in the reduction in pre-outcome motor preparation signatures at 500 msec in shared responsibility contexts, that is, social contexts. The finding that prospective and retrospective responsibility in social contexts involves neural mechanisms common to a sense of agency can potentially advance our understanding of the complex notion of societal responsibility and is relevant to a wide range of societal domains, including the legal and medical sectors as well as ethical issues related to artificial intelligence.

Acknowledgments

This project has received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (PI: B. B. grant agreement No. 819040 - acronym: rid-O) and from the Wellcome Trust (PI: R. J. D. grant number 098362/Z/12/Z). M. E. Z. was funded by the Wellcome Trust, grant number 204702, and is currently supported by the European Union's Horizon 2020 Marie Curie Individual fellowship, grant number 882936 acronym SIND. B. B. was supported by the Humboldt Foundation and by the NOMIS foundation. R. J. D. is in receipt of a Wellcome Trust Investigator Award.

We would like to thank Daniel Bates for his valuable help with data acquisition, the MEG team at the Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging for their feedback on the project, David Wurzer for his help with data preprocessing, Alizee Lopez for her help with source analyses, and Stephanie Don for editing the article.

Reprint requests should be sent to Marwa El Zein, Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Center for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany, or via e-mail: marwaelzein@gmail.com.

Data Availability Statement

MATLAB code for the analyses of the behavioral and neural data and the behavioral data will be shared on the Open Science Framework upon publication. Further requests can be addressed to M. E. Z.

Author Contributions

Marwa El Zein: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Project administration; Visualization; Writing—Original draft. Ray J. Dolan: Funding acquisition; Resources; Writing—Review & editing. Bahador Bahrami: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Methodology; Supervision; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review & editing.

Funding Information

Bahador Bahrami, European Commission (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000780), grant number: grant agreement No. 819040 - acronym: rid-O. Marwa El Zein, European Commission (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000780), grant number: grant number 882936 acronym SIND. Ray J. Dolan, Wellcome Trust (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100010269), grant number: grant number 098362/Z/12/Z. Marwa El Zein, Wellcome Trust (https://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100010269), grant number: grant number 204702. For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

Diversity in Citation Practices

Retrospective analysis of the citations in every article published in this journal from 2010 to 2021 reveals a persistent pattern of gender imbalance: Although the proportions of authorship teams (categorized by estimated gender identification of first author/last author) publishing in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (JoCN) during this period were M(an)/M = .407, W(oman)/M = .32, M/W = .115, and W/W = .159, the comparable proportions for the articles that these authorship teams cited were M/M = .549, W/M = .257, M/W = .109, and W/W = .085 (Postle and Fulvio, JoCN, 34:1, pp. 1–3). Consequently, JoCN encourages all authors to consider gender balance explicitly when selecting which articles to cite and gives them the opportunity to report their article's gender citation balance.

REFERENCES

  1. Barlas, Z., Hockley, W. E., & Obhi, S. S. (2017). Effects of free choice and outcome valence on the sense of agency: Evidence from measures of intentional binding and feelings of control. Experimental Brain Research, 236, 129–139. 10.1007/s00221-017-5112-3, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beyer, F., Sidarus, N., Bonicalzi, S., & Haggard, P. (2017). Beyond self-serving bias: Diffusion of responsibility reduces sense of agency and outcome monitoring. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 138–145. 10.1093/scan/nsw160, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blackwood, N. J., Bentall, R. P., ffytche, D. H., Simmons, A., Murray, R. M., & Howard, R. J. (2003). Self-responsibility and the self-serving bias: An fMRI investigation of causal attributions. Neuroimage, 20, 1076–1085. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00331-8, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Caine, B. T., & Schlenker, B. R. (1979). Role position and group performance as determinants of egotistical perceptions in cooperative groups. Journal of Psychology, 101, 149–156. 10.1080/00223980.1979.9915066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Bach, D. R., Roepstorff, A., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2010). How the opinion of others affects our valuation of objects. Current Biology, 20, 1165–1170. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.055, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Kanai, R., Bahrami, B., Bach, D. R., Dolan, R. J., Roepstorff, A., et al. (2012). Structure of orbitofrontal cortex predicts social influence. Current Biology, 22, R123–R124. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.012, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Caspar, E. A., Beyer, F., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2021). The obedient mind and the volitional brain: A neural basis for preserved sense of agency and sense of responsibility under coercion. PLoS One, 16, e0258884. 10.1371/journal.pone.0258884, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caspar, E. A., Christensen, J. F., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2016). Coercion changes the sense of agency in the human brain. Current Biology, 26, 585–592. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chambon, V., Théro, H., Vidal, M., Vandendriessche, H., Haggard, P., & Palminteri, S. (2020). Information about action outcomes differentially affects learning from self-determined versus imposed choices. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 1067–1079. 10.1038/s41562-020-0919-5, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chambon, V., Wenke, D., Fleming, S. M., Prinz, W., & Haggard, P. (2013). An online neural substrate for sense of agency. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1031–1037. 10.1093/cercor/bhs059, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chaminade, T., & Decety, J. (2002). Leader or follower? Involvement of the inferior parietal lobule in agency. NeuroReport, 13, 1975–1978. 10.1097/00001756-200210280-00029, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cockburn, J., Collins, A. G. E., & Frank, M. J. (2014). A reinforcement learning mechanism responsible for the valuation of free choice. Neuron, 83, 551–557. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.035, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Desmurget, M., Reilly, K. T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese, C., & Sirigu, A. (2009). Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science, 324, 811–813. 10.1126/science.1169896, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Desmurget, M., & Sirigu, A. (2012). Conscious motor intention emerges in the inferior parietal lobule. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22, 1004–1011. 10.1016/j.conb.2012.06.006, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A., & Halgren, E. (2010). Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage, 53, 1–15. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dewey, J. A., Pacherie, E., & Knoblich, G. (2014). The phenomenology of controlling a moving object with another person. Cognition, 132, 383–397. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.002, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Donner, T. H., Siegel, M., Fries, P., & Engel, A. K. (2009). Buildup of choice-predictive activity in human motor cortex during perceptual decision making. Current Biology, 19, 1581–1585. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.066, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. El Zein, M., & Bahrami, B. (2020). Joining a group diverts regret and responsibility away from the individual. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 287, 20192251. 10.1098/rspb.2019.2251, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. El Zein, M., Bahrami, B., & Hertwig, R. (2019). Shared responsibility in collective decisions. Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 554–559. 10.1038/s41562-019-0596-4, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. El Zein, M., Seikus, C., De-Wit, L., & Bahrami, B. (2019). Punishing the individual or the group for norm violation. Wellcome Open Research, 4, 139. 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15474.2, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. El Zein, M., Wyart, V., & Grèzes, J. (2015). Anxiety dissociates the adaptive functions of sensory and motor response enhancements to social threats. eLife, 4, e10274. 10.7554/eLife.10274, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fan, L., Li, H., Zhuo, J., Zhang, Y., Wang, J., Chen, L., et al. (2016). The human Brainnetome Atlas: A new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 3508–3526. 10.1093/cercor/bhw157, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Farwell, L., & Wohlwend-Lloyd, R. (1998). Narcissistic processes: Optimistic expectations, favorable self-evaluations, and self-enhancing attributions. Journal of Personality, 66, 65–83. 10.1111/1467-6494.00003, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fonov, V., Evans, A. C., Botteron, K., Almli, C. R., McKinstry, R. C., & Collins, D. L. (2011). Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage, 54, 313–327. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fonov, V., Evans, A., McKinstry, R., Almli, C., & Collins, D. (2009). Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage, 47, S102. 10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70884-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Forsyth, D. R., & Schlenker, B. R. (1977). Attributing the causes of group performance: Effects of performance quality, task importance, and future testing. Journal of Personality, 45, 220–236. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1977.tb00148.x, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Forsyth, D. R., Zyzniewski, L. E., & Giammanco, C. A. (2002). Responsibility diffusion in cooperative collectives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 54–65. 10.1177/0146167202281005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Frith, C. D. (2014). Action, agency and responsibility. Neuropsychologia, 55, 137–142. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.007, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2007). Social cognition in humans. Current Biology, 17, R724–R732. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.068, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 14–21. 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Haggard, P. (2009). Neuroscience. The sources of human volition. Science, 324, 731–733. 10.1126/science.1173827, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 196–207. 10.1038/nrn.2017.14, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382–385. 10.1038/nn827, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jaquiery, M., & El Zein, M. (2021). Stage 1 registered report: How responsibility attributions to self and others relate to outcome ownership in group decisions. Wellcome Open Research, 6, 24. 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16480.1, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14, S103–S109. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kool, W., Getz, S. J., & Botvinick, M. M. (2013). Neural representation of reward probability: Evidence from the illusion of control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 852–861. 10.1162/jocn_a_00369, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Leary, M. R., & Forsyth, D. R. (1987). Attributions of responsibility for collective endeavors. In Hendrick C. (Ed.), Group processes (pp. 167–188). Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  38. Li, P., Han, C., Lei, Y., Holroyd, C. B., & Li, H. (2011). Responsibility modulates neural mechanisms of outcome processing: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 48, 1129–1133. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01182.x, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Li, P., Jia, S., Feng, T., Liu, Q., Suo, T., & Li, H. (2010). The influence of the diffusion of responsibility effect on outcome evaluations: Electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Neuroimage, 52, 1727–1733. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.275, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lohse, T., & Simon, S. (2021). Compliance in teams—Implications of joint decisions and shared consequences. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 94, 101745. 10.1016/j.socec.2021.101745 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lopez-Persem, A., Bastin, J., Petton, M., Abitbol, R., Lehongre, K., Adam, C., et al. (2020). Four core properties of the human brain valuation system demonstrated in intracranial signals. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 664–675. 10.1038/s41593-020-0615-9, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164, 177–190. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nicolle, A., Bach, D. R., Frith, C., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Amygdala involvement in self-blame regret. Social Neuroscience, 6, 178–189. 10.1080/17470919.2010.506128, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J.-M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 156869. 10.1155/2011/156869, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Padoa-Schioppa, C., & Conen, K. E. (2017). Orbitofrontal cortex: A neural circuit for economic decisions. Neuron, 96, 736–754. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.031, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9–34. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sidarus, N., Travers, E., Haggard, P., & Beyer, F. (2020). How social contexts affect cognition: Mentalizing interferes with sense of agency during voluntary action. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 89, 103994. 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103994 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Tadel, F., Baillet, S., Mosher, J. C., Pantazis, D., & Leahy, R. M. (2011). Brainstorm: A user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 879716. 10.1155/2011/879716, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tanaka, T., & Kawabata, H. (2021). Sense of agency is modulated by interactions between action choice, outcome valence, and predictability. Current Psychology, 40, 1795–1806. 10.1007/s12144-018-0121-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wyart, V., de Gardelle, V., Scholl, J., & Summerfield, C. (2012). Rhythmic fluctuations in evidence accumulation during decision making in the human brain. Neuron, 76, 847–858. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.015, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Yeung, N., Holroyd, C. B., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 535–544. 10.1093/cercor/bhh153, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Yoshie, M., & Haggard, P. (2013). Negative emotional outcomes attenuate sense of agency over voluntary actions. Current Biology, 23, 2028–2032. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.034, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

MATLAB code for the analyses of the behavioral and neural data and the behavioral data will be shared on the Open Science Framework upon publication. Further requests can be addressed to M. E. Z.


Articles from Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of MIT Press

RESOURCES