Abstract
Children who experience adversities have an elevated risk of mental health problems. However, the extent to which adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) cause mental health problems remains unclear, as previous associations may partly reflect genetic confounding. In this Registered Report, we used DNA from 11,407 children from the UK and USA to investigate gene-environment correlations and genetic confounding of the associations between ACEs and mental health. Regarding gene-environment correlations, children with higher polygenic scores for mental health problems had a small increase in odds for ACEs. Regarding genetic confounding, elevated risk of mental health problems in children exposed to ACEs was at least partially due to pre-existing genetic risk. However, some ACEs (e.g., childhood maltreatment, parental mental illness) remained associated with mental health problems independent of genetic confounding. These findings suggest that interventions addressing heritable psychiatric vulnerabilities in children exposed to ACEs may help to reduce their risk of mental health problems.
Introduction
Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) are well established risk factors for mental health problems. For example, a wealth of research has shown that children exposed to abuse, neglect, and dysfunctional home environments (such as domestic violence, parental separation, parental mental illness, criminal behaviour, or parental substance abuse) have a higher risk of developing internalising disorders such as depression and anxiety1–4, and externalising disorders such as conduct disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)5–7. However, as highlighted recently by policy makers8, charities9, and scientists10,11, the extent to which ACEs cause mental health problems is not known. This is because ACEs are not randomly distributed in the population, and children exposed to ACEs are likely to have other risk factors for mental health problems. In addition to wider environmental risks, one key potential vulnerability is genetic liability to mental health problems12.
There are at least two reasons why children exposed to ACEs might have an elevated genetic liability to mental health problems. First, parents with mental health problems may pass on genetic variants conferring psychopathology risk to their child and provide them with an adverse rearing environment. This represents a ‘passive gene-environment correlation’13,14, and is plausible as parental mental illness is considered to be an ACE, and other ACEs often occur in families where parents have mental health difficulties15. Second, a child with early phenotypic expressions of genetic liability to mental health problems might be more likely to elicit harsh parenting or stress responses in their parents (e.g., depressive symptoms). This represents an ‘evocative gene-environment correlation’13,14 and has been evidenced in adoption studies, where children at genetic risk of externalising problems were more likely to experience negative parenting from adoptive parents16,17. Importantly, if children with increased genetic liability to mental health problems have an elevated risk of experiencing ACEs, the association between ACEs and mental health problems may partly reflect genetic confounding.
It is important to investigate the extent to which genetic influences contribute to associations between ACEs and mental health to provide insights into causality and interventions. For example, if the associations are partly confounded by genetic influences, then the causal contribution of ACEs to mental health is likely to be lower than estimated in non-genetically informative studies. If this is the case, then even if we succeeded in implementing effective primary prevention of ACEs, this would only partly reduce children’s risk of mental health problems. In addition, secondary preventative strategies that support exposed children and address heritable vulnerabilities to psychopathology would be needed to reduce their risk of developing mental health problems. For example, this could include skills building components to manage negative emotions and behaviours as part of trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy9. Of course, there is a moral imperative to reduce the likelihood that children will experience ACEs, regardless of the degree to which they impact mental health. However, this research can improve our mechanistic understanding of the relationship between ACEs and mental health in ways that can help optimise approaches to prevention and intervention.
To examine the extent to which genetic influences contribute to associations between ACEs and mental health, particular genetically informed methods are needed. Twin methods (which have traditionally been used to test for genetic confounding)18,19 can be limited because many ACEs affect all children in a family, and thus, twins typically do not differ for the exposure. In addition, the adoption design (which can rule out genetic confounding due to passive gene-environment correlation) has limited utility because ACEs are rare in adoptive families20. Fortunately, recent advances in genome-wide association studies have allowed us to assess genetic influences in samples of unrelated individuals though polygenic scores. Polygenic scores capture common genetic influences by summing the effects of many genetic variants (known as single nucleotide polymorphisms; SNPs) on a trait into a single individual-level score. Through using polygenic scores, we can test whether (1) children with increased genetic liability to mental health problems are more likely to be exposed to ACEs (i.e., gene-environment correlation), and (2) such genetic influences contribute to the associations between ACEs and mental health (i.e., genetic confounding).
To examine gene-environment correlation, we can test whether a child’s polygenic score for a mental health problem (e.g., depression) predicts their exposure to ACEs. Three prospective studies employing this method have suggested that children with genetic liability to mental health problems may be more likely to experience ACEs. First, Sallis and colleagues21 found that children with higher polygenic scores for schizophrenia, ADHD, bipolar disorder, depression, and neuroticism had greater risk of exposure to broadly defined childhood trauma (including maltreatment, bullying, and domestic violence), with each standard deviation increase in the polygenic score predicting childhood trauma with odds ratios ranging between 1.07 (bipolar disorder) to 1.16 (depression). Second, Zwicker and colleages22 found that young people exposed to higher levels of broadly defined childhood adversity (including maltreatment, bullying and domestic violence) had higher polygenic scores for ADHD (standardised β=0.24), but not schizophrenia. Third, Schoeler and colleagues23 found that polygenic scores for depression, ADHD and risk taking (as well as body mass index and intelligence) independently predicted exposure to bullying victimisation in a multi-polygenic score model (with standardised βs ranging from 0.04 [risk taking] to 0.07 [depression]). These findings are also consistent with evidence from retrospective studies showing that adults reporting childhood maltreatment had higher polygenic scores for depression, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder (with odds ratios ranging from 1.03 [bipolar disorder] to 1.20 [depression])24,25 as well as autism (standardised β=0.03)26. However, no study has systematically tested whether polygenic scores for a range of mental health problems predict a range of different ACEs, including indicators of household dysfunction (e.g., domestic violence, parental separation, parental mental illness, criminal behaviour, or parental substance abuse) as well as maltreatment. As such, it is not known whether some ACEs are more strongly linked to genetic risk of mental health problems than others, and whether certain genetic liabilities are particularly important in risk of exposure to ACEs.
To examine genetic confounding, we can test the extent to which the associations between ACEs and mental health are reduced when accounting for children’s polygenic scores for mental health problems27. To date, no study has examined whether this is the case for the associations between ACEs and mental health. However, studies have examined whether this is the case for related environmental experiences, such as adoption and parenting. With regard to adoption, Lehto and colleagues28 found that the associations between adoption and mental health-related outcomes in adulthood (depressive symptoms, bipolar disorder, neuroticism, and life satisfaction) were attenuated by between 3% (for bipolar disorder) to 18% (for life satisfaction) when controlling for the respective polygenic scores. With regard to parenting, Wertz and colleagues29 found that the associations between cognitive stimulation, warm, sensitive parenting, household chaos, and a safe, tidy home environment with child educational attainment were reduced by approximately 8% when controlling for the child’s polygenic score for education. Furthermore, Krapohl and colleagues30 found that the associations between parental slapping/smacking with ADHD and conduct problems were attenuated by 6% and 7%, respectively, when controlling for the child’s polygenic score for educational attainment.
Controlling for polygenic scores for mental health problems in this manner can indicate whether there is likely to be a genetic contribution to the association between ACEs and mental health. However, one limitation of this methodological approach is that polygenic scores only capture a small proportion of heritability, and thus do not fully account for genetic confounding. This can be addressed by a newly developed genetic sensitivity analysis27 which estimates shared genetic effects under scenarios in which the polygenic score captures additional genetic variance in the outcome (i.e., SNP- and/or twin-based heritability; see ‘Analysis plan’ section in the Methods for a detailed description of this method). A recent application of this genetic sensitivity analysis found that the associations between maternal education with offspring ADHD, educational achievement, and body mass index (BMI) were moderately explained by shared genetic effects27, consistent with findings from Children of Twins studies and adoption designs31. For example a latent polygenic score that captured SNP-based heritability in educational achievement (i.e., 31%32) explained 50% of the association between maternal education and child educational achievement27. However, this approach has never been applied to assess the extent to which genetic influences contribute to the associations between ACEs and mental health.
In this study, we systematically investigated the role of genetic liability in the associations between ACEs and mental health problems. To do so, we used data from more than 11,000 genotyped children from two cohorts in the United Kingdom (the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children [ALSPAC]) and the United States (the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development [ABCD] Study), with prospective measures of ACEs and mental health. (Note that the ABCD Study was not originally included in the Stage 1 pre-registration, but we used it because the original dataset, the Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden [CATSS], was not accessible after Stage 1 acceptance [detailed in “Methods”]). We addressed the following aims and hypotheses (summarised in Table 1).
To examine gene-environment correlations, we investigated whether children with genetic liability to mental health problems are more likely to be exposed to ACEs (Aim 1). We addressed this by testing three hypotheses. First, we tested whether polygenic scores for mental health problems (e.g., depression, ADHD, schizophrenia, and others) are associated with exposure to ACEs. We hypothesised that polygenic scores for mental health problems would be associated with an increased risk of exposure to ACEs (Hypothesis 1a). Second, we tested whether polygenic scores for certain mental health problems are more strongly associated with ACEs than other polygenic scores. We hypothesised that there would not be evidence for differential associations between polygenic scores for different mental health problems with ACEs (Hypothesis 1b), given that previous research has identified similar size bivariate associations between a range of polygenic scores and ACEs21. Third, we tested whether certain ACEs are linked to greater polygenic risk for mental health problems than other ACEs. We hypothesised that parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, and parental criminality would be associated with higher polygenic risk for mental health problems relative to maltreatment, domestic violence, and parental separation (Hypothesis 1c), because the former exposures are most likely to be linked to intergenerational transmission of genetic risk for psychopathology.
To examine genetic confounding, we investigated the extent to which genetic liability to mental health problems contributes to the associations between ACEs and mental health (Aim 2). We addressed this by testing two hypotheses. First, we examined the proportions of the associations between ACEs and internalising and externalising problems that are explained by observed polygenic scores for mental health problems. We hypothesised that observed polygenic scores would explain a small proportion (between 5% to 20%) of the associations between ACEs and internalising and externalising problems (Hypothesis 2a) , given that a similar proportion of covariation between other early environments (adoption and parental discipline) and psychopathology were captured by polygenic scores29,30. Second, we estimated the proportions of the associations between ACEs and internalising and externalising problems that would be explained by latent polygenic scores which capture additional heritability in mental health problems. We hypothesised that polygenic scores that capture SNP heritability in internalising and externalising problems would explain a moderate proportion (between 20% to 40%) of the associations between ACEs and these outcomes (Hypothesis 2b). This is based on evidence showing that accounting for SNP heritability in an outcome can increase the covariance captured in an association by more than double, relative to a standard polygenic score27.
Table 1. Design table summarising the study’s research questions, hypotheses, power calculations, analyses, and conditions for interpretation.
Question | Hypothesis | Sampling plan (e.g.
power analysis) |
Analysis plan | Interpretation given to different outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|
Do children with genetic liability to mental health problems have an increased risk of ACEs? | 1a) Polygenic scores for mental health problems will be associated with an increased risk of exposure to ACEs. | N=4,700 gives 0.96 power to detect an average odds ratio of 1.04 for the association between polygenic scores and ACEs using the ‘agg’ function in the MAd package114 (accounting for dependent effect sizes). |
|
|
Are polygenic scores for certain mental health problems more strongly associated with ACEs than other polygenic scores? | 1b) Polygenic scores for different mental health problems equally predict exposure to ACEs. | N=4,700 gives 1.00 power to detect a significant difference of 0.11 in odds ratios reflecting the average association between different polygenic scores and ACEs, using a Wald test. |
|
|
Are some ACEs linked to greater polygenic risk for mental health problems than other ACEs? | 1c) Parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, and parental criminality will be associated with higher polygenic risk for mental health problems relative to maltreatment, domestic violence, and parental separation. | N=4,700 gives 1.00 power to detect a significant difference of 0.10 in odds ratios reflecting the average association between polygenic scores and different ACEs, using a Wald test. |
|
|
What proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising and externalising problems are explained by observed polygenic scores for mental health problems? | 2a) Observed polygenic scores will explain a small proportion (between 5% to 20%) of the associations between ACEs and internalising and externalising problems. | N=4,700 gives 0.95 power to detect the proportion of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by observed polygenic scores in a SEM. For the aggregate model, N=4,700 will give power of 1.00 to detect an average proportion of 5% (of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by polygenic scores). |
|
|
What proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising and externalising problems are explained by polygenic scores which capture additional heritability in mental health problems? | 2b) Polygenic scores that capture SNP heritability in internalising and externalising problems will explain a moderate proportion (between 20% to 40%) of the associations between ACEs and these outcomes. | N=4,700 gives 1.00 power to detect the proportion of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by increasingly powerful polygenic scores in a SEM. |
|
|
Table legend: If findings differ between ALSPAC and the ABCD Study, we proposed to interpret this as reflecting: (1) differences between countries (the UK [ALSPAC] versus the USA [ABCD]), or (2) differences in historical time periods (as ALSPAC participants were born in 1991-1992 and ABCD participants were born in 2006-2008). Differences in results between cohorts are less likely to be due to polygenic scores (as the same GWAS summary statistics will be used for both cohorts), ACE measures (as both cohorts used similar questionnaires reported by parents and children), mental health measures (as both cohorts used similar parent-reported questionnaires) and timing of assessments (as ACEs were assessed between birth to age 9/10 in both cohorts, and mental health was assessed at age 10 in ALSPAC and age 9/10 in ABCD. Note that the ABCD Study was not originally included in the Stage 1 pre-registration, but we used it because the original replication cohort (CATSS) was not accessible after Stage 1 acceptance (detailed in “Methods – Change in replication cohort”).
Results
Sample description
After imputation, the samples included 6,411 participants from ALSPAC and 4,996 participants from the ABCD Study. (Note that the ABCD Study was not originally included in the Stage 1 pre-registration, but we used it because the original dataset, the Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden [CATSS], was not accessible after Stage 1 acceptance. Further information on the change in sample from CATSS to ABCD is reported in “Methods – Change in replication cohort”). Descriptive statistics are shown in Supplementary Table 1. Below we report results for the imputed samples, before testing whether findings replicate in the complete case samples (n=4,106 in ALSPAC and n=4,662 in ABCD).
1a). Do children with genetic liability to mental health problems have an increased risk of ACEs?
ALSPAC
We first tested the associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems (depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder, autism, ADHD, antisocial behaviour, alcohol use disorder, and schizophrenia) and individual ACEs (maltreatment, domestic violence, parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, parental separation, and parental criminality). To obtain a single effect size reflecting the average association between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs, we pooled the results across all individual associations. On average, we found that children from ALSPAC with higher polygenic scores for mental health problems had a small increase in odds of ACEs (pooled OR=1.05, 95% CI=1.01-1.10, p=0.0081; Figure 1A). To examine whether this effect size was trivially small, we performed equivalence tests, which assess whether the 90% confidence intervals for the effect size lie entirely within pre-specified equivalence bounds of OR=0.94-1.06 (indexing the smallest effect size of interest; see Methods, “Analysis plan”). The 90% CIs for the pooled association between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs (1.02-1.09) did not fall completely within the equivalence bounds, suggesting the association was of meaningful magnitude. In contrast, negative control polygenic scores for handedness and cataracts were not associated with ACEs (pooled OR=0.98, 95% CI=0.94-1.02, p=0.39; Figure 1B).
Figure 1. Associations between polygenic scores and ACEs in ALSPAC.
Note. Data are presented as odds ratios +/- 95% CIs, obtained from logistic regression models. Panel A shows associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs, Panel B shows associations between negative control polygenic scores and ACEs. P-values for individual associations between polygenic scores and ACEs are from two-sided tests and are false discovery rate (FDR) corrected. The sample size for ALSPAC analyses was n=6,411.
ABCD
Similar to the ALSPAC Study, children in the ABCD cohort with greater polygenic scores for mental health problems had a small increase in odds of ACEs (pooled OR=1.09, 95% CI=1.03-1.15, p=0.0021, Figure 2A), and the 90% CIs (1.04-1.14) did not fall completely within the equivalence bounds (0.94-1.06). Conversely, negative control polygenic scores were not associated with ACEs (pooled OR=1.02, 95% CI=0.97-1.07, p=0.52; Figure 2B).
Figure 2. Associations between polygenic scores and ACEs in ABCD.
Note. Data are presented as odds ratios +/- 95% CIs, obtained from logistic regression models. Panel A shows associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs, Panel B shows associations between negative control polygenic scores and ACEs. P-values for individual associations between polygenic scores and ACEs are from two-sided tests and are FDR corrected. The sample size for ABCD analyses was n=4,996.
Taken together, findings from both cohorts pooled across ACEs support the hypothesis that polygenic scores for mental health problems are associated with an increased risk of exposure to ACEs.
1b). Are polygenic scores for certain mental health problems more strongly associated with ACEs than other polygenic scores?
ALSPAC
Next, we examined whether polygenic scores for mental health problems differed in their average associations with ACEs. In ALSPAC, we found that polygenic scores for various mental health problems were differentially associated with ACEs (Wald-test F (7, 16,573)=2.62, p=0.011). Pairwise comparisons showed that the polygenic scores for depression, ADHD and schizophrenia predicted average risk of ACEs more strongly than various other polygenic scores (particularly for autism and alcohol dependence; Figure 3A). The 90% CIs for these differences did not fall entirely within the pre-specified equivalence bounds (-0.10 to 0.10 on the log odds scale; Figure 3A), suggesting that the differences were of a meaningful size.
Figure 3. Pairwise differences between polygenic scores in their association with ACEs.
Note: Data are presented as log odds differences +/- 90% CIs. Positive effect sizes reflect the first labelled polygenic score having a stronger positive average association with ACEs than the second polygenic score. Red dashed lines show the pre-specified equivalence bounds. 90% confidence intervals are presented and p-values are for the difference in log odds ratio between polygenic scores (two-sided tests). n=6,411 in ALSPAC and n=4,996 in ABCD.
ABCD
In the ABCD Study, polygenic scores for various mental health problems also showed different associations with ACEs (Wald-test F (7, 436,521)=7.68, p=2.60x10-9). Consistent with the ALSPAC findings, polygenic scores for depression, ADHD, and schizophrenia showed stronger average associations with ACEs than various other polygenic scores (particularly for autism and alcohol dependence; Figure 3B). However, in contrast to ALSPAC, polygenic scores for antisocial behaviour and bipolar disorder were more strongly associated with ACEs than some other polygenic scores (particularly autism and alcohol dependence). The 90% CIs for these differences did not fall within the equivalence bounds. Therefore, findings from both cohorts do not support the hypothesis that polygenic scores for different mental health problems would be equally associated with ACEs.
1c). Are some ACEs linked to greater polygenic risk for mental health problems than other ACEs?
ALSPAC
We next examined whether the associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs differed across ACEs. There was no evidence to suggest that average polygenic risk for mental health problems differed across ACEs (Wald-test F (5, 5,319)=1.07, p=0.37). Furthermore, equivalence tests suggested that the majority of ACEs were associated with similar polygenic risk of mental health problems, as the 90% CIs for the differences between most ACEs fell inside the equivalence bounds (-0.05 to 0.05 on the log odds ratio scale; Figure 4A).
Figure 4. Pairwise differences between ACEs in their association with polygenic risk for mental health problems.
Note: Data are presented as log odds differences +/- 90% CIs (two-sided tests). Positive effect sizes reflect the first labelled ACE having a stronger positive association with pooled polygenic risk for mental health problems; negative effect sizes reflect the second labelled ACE having a stronger positive association with pooled polygenic risk for mental health problems. The red dashed lines show the p re-specified equivalence bounds. n=6,411 in ALSPAC and n=4,996 in ABCD.
ABCD
Similar to ALSPAC, in the ABCD cohort, average polygenic risk for mental health problems was not significantly different across ACEs (Wald-test F (5, 246,200)=2.00, p=0.08). Equivalence tests also suggested that the majority of ACEs were associated with equal polygenic risk of mental health problems, as the 90% CIs for most differences between ACEs fell inside the equivalence bounds (Figure 4B). Therefore, findings from both cohorts did not support the hypothesis that parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, and parental criminality would be associated with higher polygenic risk for mental health problems than other ACEs.
2a). What proportions of the associations between ACEs with internalising and externalising problems are explained by observed polygenic scores for mental health problems?
ALSPAC
To test genetic confounding, we next examined the proportion of the associations between ACEs and childhood mental health problems that were explained by polygenic scores for mental health problems (depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder, autism, ADHD, antisocial behaviour, alcohol use disorder, and schizophrenia) using a structural equation model (Figure 5C). In ALSPAC, polygenic scores for mental health problems explained a very small average proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising problems at age 10 (4.4%, 95% CI=1.9-6.8%, p=0.0004). These polygenic scores also explained a small average proportion of the associations between ACEs and externalising problems at age 10 (5.8%, 95% CI=3.4-8.2%, p=3.18x10-6). Results for associations between specific ACEs with internalising and externalising problems are shown in Figure 6A-B (red points for adjusted associations) and Supplementary Table 2A. In contrast, negative control polygenic scores for handedness and cataracts did not explain any part of the associations between ACEs with internalising problems (average proportion=0.0%, 95% CI= -0.6;0.5, p=0.91) or externalising problems (average proportion= -0.1%, 95% CI= -0.5;0.4, p=0.77).
Figure 5. Diagrams showing structural equation models to estimate the genetic contribution to the associations between ACEs and mental health.
Note. In all diagrams, ACE represents the adverse childhood experience, MH represents the mental health outcome (e.g., internalising problems or externalising problems) and PGS represents the polygenic score, with one polygenic score shown in panels A and B, and all 8 polygenic scores (PGS_1-PGS_8) shown in panel C. Panel A depicts the underlying conceptual model, in which the polygenic score is treated as a confou nder, whereas panel B depicts the statistical model to calculate the genetic confounding effect, in which the polygenic score is treated as a mediator. Note that conceptually, the polygenic score cannot be a mediator in the association between ACEs and mental health because genetic variants are set at conception and do not change throughout the lifespan. However, statistically, we can estimate the genetic confounding effect by treating the polygenic score as a mediator and calculating the indirect effect of ACEs on mental health through the polygenic score. Panel C represents the statistical model in which all 8 polygenic scores are included as mediators. Though not depicted in the figure to aid clarity, we will account for correlations between polygenic scores in the model.
Figure 6. Genetic confounding of the associations between ACEs with internalising and externalising problems.
Note. Data are presented as standardised beta coefficients +/- 95% CIs for associations between ACEs and mental health outcomes, before accounting for polygenic scores (yellow circles), and after accounting for (i) observed polygenic scores for mental health problems (red points), and (ii) a latent polygenic score capturing SNP heritability in the outcome (blue points). Panel A shows the associations between ACEs and internalising problems in ALSPAC; Panel B shows the associations between ACEs and externalising problems in ALSPAC; Panel C shows the associations between ACEs and internalising problems in ABCD; Panel D shows the associations between ACEs and externalising problems in ABCD. Tests were two-sided. Confidence intervals could not be reliably computed for associations attenuated to zero and therefore these estimates should be interpreted with caution. n=6,411 in ALSPAC and n=4,996 in ABCD.
ABCD
Similar to ALSPAC, in the ABCD Study, polygenic scores for mental health problems explained a very small average proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising problems at age 9/10 (3.0%, 95% CI=1.0-4.9%, p=0.003), and a small average proportion of the associations between ACEs and externalising problems at age 9/10 (5.0%, 95% CI=3.3-6.7%, p=6.38x10-9). Results for associations between specific ACEs with internalising and externalising problems are shown in Figure 6C-D (red points for adjusted associations) and Supplementary Table 2B. Negative control polygenic scores did not explain any of the associations between ACEs with internalising problems (average proportion=0.0%, 95% CI= -0.3%;0.4%, p=0.90) or externalising problems (average proportion=0.1%, 95% CI= -0.3%;0.5%, p=0.56). Taken together, these findings broadly support the hypothesis that observed polygenic scores account for a small proportion (defined as 5-20%) of the average association between ACEs and mental health problems, although the proportion captured for internalising problems was slightly smaller (<5%) than hypothesised.
2b). What proportions of the associations between ACEs with internalising and externalising problems are explained by latent polygenic scores capturing additional heritability in mental health problems?
Because polygenic scores for mental health problems only captured a very small proportion of variance in internalising problems (<1%) and externalising problems (1.6%; Supplementary Table 3), the previous analyses likely underestimated the magnitude of genetic confounding. To address this, we conducted a genetic sensitivity analysis27, which estimates genetic confounding using latent polygenic scores capturing SNP heritability in outcomes (6% and 9% for internalising and externalising problems, respectively33,34).
ALSPAC
In ALSPAC, the genetic sensitivity analysis suggested that a large average proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising problems was explained by genetic confounding (90.3%, 95% CI=80.1-100%, p=1.76x10-68), with proportions ranging from 56.9% for parental mental illness to 100% for domestic violence, parental substance abuse, criminality, and separation (Supplementary Table 4A; Figure 6A [blue points for adjusted associations]). Similarly, a large average proportion of the associations between ACEs and externalising problems was accounted for by genetic confounding (76.5%, 95% CI=59.5%-93.6%, p=1.43x10-18), with proportions ranging from 49.4% for child maltreatment to 100% for parental substance abuse (Supplementary Table 4A; Figure 6B [blue points]). However, confidence intervals could not be reliably computed for some individual estimates (where the genetic confounding effect explained 100% of the associations; Supplementary Table 4; Figure 6A-B) and therefore such estimates should be interpreted with caution.
ABCD
In the ABCD Study, the genetic sensitivity analysis suggested that genetic confounding accounted for a large average proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising problems (68.6%, 95% CI=55.5%-81.7%, p=1.07x10-24), with proportions ranging from 22% for parental mental illness to 100% for parental criminality and separation (Supplementary Table 4B; Figure 6C [blue points for adjusted associations]). Similarly, a large average proportion of the associations between ACEs and externalising problems was captured by genetic confounding (60.3%, 95% CI=48.7%-71.9%, p=2.22x10-24), with proportions ranging from 30.2% for parental mental illness to 100% for parental criminality (Supplementary Table 4B; Figure 6D [blue points]). These results indicate that the proportion of the associations between ACEs and mental health explained by genetic confounding is greater than the moderate amount (between 20% to 40%) hypothesised.
Robustness analyses
To assess the robustness of our results, we conducted three sets of analyses. First, because reliable confidence intervals could not be computed for some results in the genetic sensitivity analysis (Supplementary Table 4), we were concerned that these results might have biased the pooled estimates of genetic confounding. We therefore re-estimated the average proportions of genetic confounding after excluding these results with unreliable confidence intervals. The average proportion of the associations between ACEs and internalising problems explained by genetic confounding was attenuated but still large (ALSPAC: 70.8%, 95% CI= 40.4-100%, p=4.88x10-6; ABCD: 52.9%, 95% CI=33.2-72.6%, p=1.33x10-7). This was also the case for the associations between ACEs and externalising problems (average proportion genetically confounded: 71.8% in ALSPAC [95% CI=51.4-92.3%, p=6.01x10-12] and 52.4% in ABCD [95% CI=38.5-66.3%, p=1.66x10-13]).
Second, we repeated all analyses in the complete case samples from ALSPAC and ABCD (N=4,106 and N=4,662, respectively) and observed largely consistent results (Supplementary Results 1).
Third, because we constructed the polygenic scores for bipolar disorder from an updated GWAS35 that differed from the older GWAS36 that we proposed to use in the Stage 1 pre-registration (Supplementary Table 5), we repeated the analyses with polygenic scores for bipolar disorder derived from the pre-registered GWAS. The results were consistent with the main findings (Supplementary Results 2).
Discussion
This Registered Report examined the genetic contribution to the associations between adverse childhood experiences and mental health, in two prospective cohorts of over 11,000 children from the UK and US. Our findings provide insight into gene-environment correlations and genetic confounding of the relationship between ACEs and mental health.
With regard to gene-environment correlations, there are three key findings. First, children with higher polygenic scores for mental health problems had an elevated risk of ACEs. This gene-environment correlation was small but robust (replicating across cohorts) and negative control polygenic scores were not associated with ACEs. This supports our hypothesis and other (largely non-pre-registered) research showing that polygenic scores for mental health problems are associated with greater risk of exposure to childhood adversities21–25,37,38. Importantly, this does not suggest that exposure to ACEs is determined by genes, is the fault of the child, or is not preventable. Rather, the findings suggest that children with higher genetic liability to mental health problems are on average, slightly more likely to experience ACEs. However, ACEs are influenced by many factors (including social and environmental risks39) and can be effectively prevented through social interventions40,41.
Second, in both cohorts, polygenic scores for ADHD, depression, and schizophrenia were more strongly associated with risk of exposure to ACEs than some other polygenic scores (particularly alcohol use and autism). In the ABCD Study, polygenic scores for antisocial behaviour and bipolar disorder also showed stronger associations with ACEs. These results do not support our hypothesis that there would be no differences between polygenic scores, but broadly align with evidence showing that polygenic scores for ADHD, depression, and schizophrenia are independently associated with child maltreatment37 and bullying victimisation23, while polygenic scores for other psychiatric disorders are not. This finding should be interpreted with caution as it may reflect differences in predictive power of polygenic scores, given that the most predictive polygenic scores tended to be based on large GWAS samples and have higher SNP heritability (Supplementary Table 5). Alternatively, such differences might be because genetic liabilities to ADHD, depression, and schizophrenia have greater causal effects on exposure to ACEs than other genetic liabilities (e.g., because of stronger passive or evocative gene-environment correlations).
Third, different ACEs were associated with similar genetic risk of mental health problems in both cohorts. This was contrary to our hypothesis that parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, and parental criminality would be associated with greater (child) genetic risk of psychopathology than other ACEs, due to intergenerational genetic transmission. While these ACEs (originating in the parents) are likely to be linked to child genetic risk of psychopathology largely via passive gene-environment correlation, other ACEs might be related to genetic risk of psychopathology in part via evocative gene-environment correlation. Indeed, evidence suggests that children at genetic risk for externalising problems are more likely to experience negative parenting via evocative gene-environment correlation16,17, and evocative gene-environment correlations were found to partly underlie risk of maltreatment42. Importantly, evidence of such evocative gene-environment correlation does not mean children are to blame for ACEs – rather, parents are responsible for protecting them and reacting to their behaviour in an appropriate way42. Evidence of evocative gene-environment correlation would therefore highlight the importance of family-based interventions to help parents respond effectively to their child’s behaviour, and support children with vulnerabilities.
With regard to genetic confounding, we first found that observed polygenic scores for mental health problems explained on average, 3-5% of the associations between ACEs and internalising problems and 5-6% of the associations between ACEs and externalising problems. In contrast, negative control polygenic scores did not account for any of the associations between ACEs and mental health problems. These results broadly support our hypothesis that a small proportion (defined as 5-20%) of the associations between ACEs and mental health would be captured by polygenic scores for psychopathology. However, these results likely under-estimate the magnitude of genetic confounding as the polygenic scores for mental health problems only captured a very small amount of variation (<1% and <1.6%, respectively) in internalising and externalising outcomes.
To address this, we conducted a genetic sensitivity analysis27 using latent polygenic scores capturing SNP heritability in internalising and externalising problems (6% and 9%, respectively). This analysis suggested that genetic confounding accounted for a large average proportion of the associations between ACEs with internalising and externalising problems, in both cohorts. However, we caution against drawing strong conclusions based on the specific proportions of genetic confounding, for three reasons. First, the precise magnitude of genetic confounding varied between cohorts, and point estimates were greater in ALSPAC than in the ABCD Study. This is likely to be because ACEs had weaker associations with mental health problems in ALSPAC (Figure 5), increasing the likelihood that genetic confounding could account for the association. In contrast, the magnitude of associations between polygenic scores and ACEs did not differ between both cohorts (Supplementary Table 3). Second, confidence intervals could not be reliably estimated for some specific estimates of genetic confounding, in particular for proportions of 100% (largely observed for internalising outcomes in ALSPAC), suggesting that these proportions may not be reliable. Third, the genetic sensitivity analysis is best suited for scenarios in which the polygenic score strongly and specifically predicts the outcome27. Given the lack of available GWASs for both child internalising and externalising problems, we used polygenic scores for adult psychiatric disorders, which showed similar or stronger magnitude associations with ACEs as with child internalising and externalising problems (Supplementary Table 3). The use of a polygenic score that is not specific to the outcome may result in overestimated genetic confounding (discussed in detail in the Supplementary Discussion). Therefore, it will be important to repeat the genetic sensitivity analysis with future GWASs of child internalising and externalising problems, when available.
Despite our cautious interpretation surrounding specific estimates of genetic confounding, the overall pattern of results supports findings from other genetically informed designs, with different assumptions and sources of bias. For example, we found that child maltreatment was largely associated with internalising and externalising problems, independent of genetic confounding. This is consistent with evidence of causal effects of maltreatment on psychopathology from Mendelian Randomisation42, co-twin control43, and other quasi-experimental studies44. We also found that parental mental illness was associated with internalising and externalising problems independent of genetic confounding, which supports evidence from Children-of-Twins (CoT) and adoption studies45–47. In contrast, we found that parental substance abuse, parental criminality, and parental separation were predominantly associated with internalising and externalising problems via genetic confounding. Notably, similar genetically confounded associations with psychopathology have also been reported for parental substance abuse in CoT48,49 and adoption studies50, for parental criminality in an adoption study51, and for parental separation in some52 (though not all53) CoT studies.
We acknowledge some limitations. First, it is possible that observed associations might be inflated by reporting bias, as parents with genetic liability to psychopathology might be more likely to perceive ACEs54 and child psychopathology, as well as transmit genetic liability to their children. Future studies using different informants to measure ACEs and psychopathology (e.g., from objective records to more subjective self-reports) are needed to map the impact of reporting biases on observed gene-environment correlations38,55 and estimates of genetic confounding. Second, ALSPAC and the ABCD Study differed in various ways, such as the country of origin (UK vs USA), historical context (born in 1991-1992 vs 2006-2008), and prevalence of ACEs (e.g., higher rates of maltreatment and parental criminality in ALSPAC, perhaps due to repeated assessments [vs a single assessment in ABCD]). The ABCD analysis is therefore not a direct replication of the ALSPAC findings, and any differences in findings might be attributable to these cohort differences. However, the overall pattern of results was consistent across both cohorts, indicating that the findings are robust. Third, as discussed, it was not possible to infer whether differential associations between polygenic scores for psychiatric disorders and ACEs reflected specific genetic liabilities underlying risk of ACEs, or differences in the predictive power of polygenic scores (e.g., due to different GWAS discovery sample sizes). Fourth, our analysis was limited to individuals of European descent to match the ancestry of the GWAS discovery samples56. Once large-scale trans-ancestry GWASs become available, it will be important to replicate our findings in ancestrally diverse samples, to ensure greater representation in research57. Finally, these findings reflect average population effects, and do not preclude the existence of causal effects of certain ACEs (e.g., parental substance abuse, parental criminality, and parental separation) on child psychopathology in subpopulations.
Our findings have implications for future research. First, to understand the extent to which the observed gene-environment correlations are passive or evocative in nature, future studies should integrate polygenic scores into family-based designs (e.g., parent-offspring trios)30. Second, to the extent that ACEs are causal risk factors for psychiatric disorders, genetic variants influencing exposure to ACEs (i.e. gene environment correlations) might be captured in GWASs of those disorders55,58. If GWASs of ACEs were to become available, future genetically informed studies could test whether this reflects one of the origins of the observed associations between polygenic scores for psychiatric disorders and ACEs. Third, the gene-environment correlations observed here challenge the assumption in gene-environment interaction (GxE) studies that genetic influences on psychopathology and ACEs are independent13,59. Future GxE studies on childhood adversity and psychopathology should adopt methods that account for such gene-environment correlations to mitigate bias13,59. Lastly, this study suggests that non-genetically informative studies are likely to have overestimate the causal contribution of ACEs to mental health problems. To provide accurate estimates on the causal effects of ACEs, future studies should employ methods that account for genetic confounding, and triangulate evidence across methods with different assumptions and sources of potential bias60,61. More broadly, combining genetically informed methods with open science practices (e.g., pre-registration / Registered Reports) will help to address multiple sources of bias (e.g., genetic confounding, researcher bias62, and publication bias63) to enable rigorous evidence on the effects of ACEs on health.
Our findings also have implications for interventions. Because child maltreatment and parental mental illness were largely associated with child psychopathology independent of genetic influences, preventing these ACEs may not only improve child welfare and family functioning, but may also help to prevent child psychopathology in the population. Such interventions could include parenting support programmes to prevent maltreatment40, and more accessible psychiatric treatment for parents with mental health problems. In contrast, preventing ACEs with entirely genetically confounded effects is unlikely to substantially impact child psychopathology at the population level, although such interventions are likely to have other important positive outcomes (e.g., for child welfare, family functioning, and potentially physical health64–67). Furthermore, because polygenic scores for mental health problems accounted for at least part of the associations between all ACEs and psychopathology, strategies that address heritable psychiatric vulnerabilities in children exposed to ACEs (e.g., through skills building68 or fostering positive family interaction) should reduce their risk of developing psychopathology.
Methods
Change in replication cohort
As stated in our Stage 1 protocol (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13580777.v1), this Registered Report originally proposed to replicate findings from ALSPAC in the Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS) dataset, and not the ABCD Study. However, after receiving Stage 1 in-principle acceptance, we experienced two unforeseen issues which meant that we could not use the CATSS dataset: (1) data could not be accessed in a timely manner because of covid-related travel restrictions for Sweden, and (2) data access restrictions from the Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare meant that we could not use national registry data to measure ACEs, as originally proposed. We therefore proposed and received permission to use the ABCD Study as an alternative replication sample to CATSS (after peer review of the protocol for analysis on ABCD). Importantly, we had not accessed data from either CATSS or the ABCD Study at the time in which we proposed to use the ABCD Study, so we were blind to the results in these cohorts (though we had undertaken analysis in ALSPAC). To provide transparency about what we intended to do in the Stage 1 protocol, we report all details about the CATSS dataset in Supplementary Methods 1.
Ethics information
Ethics approval for ALSPAC was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Informed consent for the use of data collected via questionnaires and clinics was obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time. Consent for biological samples has been collected in accordance with the Human Tissue Act (2004). Ethics approval for the ABCD Study was given by a central Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of California, San Diego, and in some cases by individual site IRBs (e.g. Washington University in St. Louis)69. Parents or guardians provided written informed consent after the procedures had been fully explained and children assented before participation in the study70.
Design
ALSPAC and the ABCD Study are prospective longitudinal cohort studies. A description of these datasets and their measures is below.
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children
Sample
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a longitudinal study of children born in the United Kingdom in 1991-1992. ALSPAC sought to recruit all pregnant women in the former county of Avon, United Kingdom, with an expected due date between April 1, 1991 and December 31, 1992. The initial sample consisted of children of 14,541 mothers. Children have been followed-up and assessed repeatedly across development through questionnaires, face-to-face interviews and physical and psychological assessments (including biological assays)71–73. The study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/). 49% of the analytic sample was female.
Measures
Adverse childhood experiences
We examined six ACEs: maltreatment, domestic violence, parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, parental separation, and parental criminality. These experiences all involve adversity in the family context, and were included in the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Adverse Childhood Experiences Study3,74 and the World Health Organization ACE international questionnaire75. In ALSPAC, these ACEs were assessed prospectively through parent and child reports via questionnaires at multiple assessment phases from birth to age 9 years (115 months). Details of these assessments are provided in Supplementary Table 6. We derived binary measures reflecting exposure to each ACE according to definitions shown in Supplementary Table 6 and recommended by a previous ALSPAC Data Note on ACE measures76. Note that sub-types of maltreatment (physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, and neglect) were combined into a single measure due to low individual prevalence and high co-occurrence76,77. Measures of each ACE were derived for participants with responses to ≥50% of the questions assessing that ACE between birth to age 9 years. We used multiple imputation to estimate ACE exposure in participants with responses to <50% but ≥10% of questions assessing the ACE (see Supplementary Methods 2 for further details of the multiple imputation procedure).
Mental health problems
Internalising problems and externalising problems were assessed through parent reports on the Development and Wellbeing Assessment (DAWBA)78 at age 10 years. The DAWBA is a semi-structured interview assessing multiple domains of child psychopathology with good validity78 and reliability79. Items from the DAWBA used to derive the mental health measures are presented in Supplementary Table 7.
Internalising problems were assessed through modules on separation anxiety (11 items, scale from 0-20), social anxiety (6 items, scale from 0-12), generalised anxiety (15 items, scale from 0-28), and major depression (15 items, scale from 0-15). We derived one overall measure of internalising problems through the following steps. First, we calculated the mean for each of the four modules (separation anxiety, social anxiety, general anxiety, and major depression) for participants with data for ≥50% of the items, before standardising the scores. Next, we summed the scores across the anxiety sub-scores and standardised the measure, so we have one overall measure of anxiety, and one for major depression. Last, we summed these anxiety and depression scores, before standardising the overall single measure.
Externalising problems were assessed through modules on hyperkinesis/ADHD (18 items, scale from 0-36) and conduct/oppositional disorders (17 items, scale from 0-34). To derive one overall measure of externalising problems, we first calculated the mean for each of the two modules for participants with data for ≥50% of the items. We then standardised the two scores and summed them, before standardising the overall single measure.
Genotyping and quality control
ALSPAC children have been genotyped using the Illumina HumanHap550 quad chip genotyping platforms by 23andme subcontracting the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute, Cambridge, UK and the Laboratory Corporation of America, Burlington, NC, US. Quality control (QC) was carried out in PLINK80, adhering to standard guidelines81,82 which have been previously used effectively for analysis of genetic data in ALSPAC21,23,83. Specifically, samples were removed on the basis of (1) low call rate (poor DNA quality), (2) outlying heterozygosity across autosomes, (3) relatedness (based on identity-by-state), (4) gender mismatches, and (5) non-European population ancestry. SNPs were removed on the basis of (1) low call rate, (2) extreme deviation from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, and (3) low minor allele frequency. Further details are provided in Supplementary Table 8.
Polygenic scores
We derived polygenic scores for mental health problems; namely, major depressive disorder, anxiety disorder, bipolar disorder, autism, ADHD, antisocial behaviour, alcohol use disorder, and schizophrenia. We selected these polygenic scores because they (i) index genetic liability to a range of mental health problems, and (ii) have been found to be associated with ACEs21–23,26 and/or psychopathology in young people84–87. We also derived negative control polygenic scores for traits with no known association with ACEs or mental health (namely, handedness and cataracts). All polygenic scores were generated using GWAS summary statistics which (i) were derived from European samples that did not include ALSPAC and ABCD participants (to avoid sample overlap), and (ii) had N > 16,000 in the discovery sample (to ensure adequate power). Supplementary Table 5 provides details of the GWAS summary statistics which were used to derive polygenic scores.
In our Stage 1 protocol, we specified that if new, larger GWASs were published after submission, we would use the updated summary statistics to benefit from greater power (and report any such changes in the Stage 2 submission). Since the Stage 1 submission, new, larger GWASs were published for bipolar disorder35 (N=413,466 versus N=51,710 in the original GWAS36) and antisocial behaviour problems88 (N=83,674 versus N=16,400 in the original GWAS87), and so we derived polygenic scores from these updated summary statistics for our main analyses. For transparency, we also report the results using the originally pre-registered GWAS summary statistics36 to derive the polygenic score for bipolar disorder. We did not do this using the older GWAS for antisocial behaviour87, as we realised that there was sample overlap for ALSPAC, which could have led to biased estimates89.
Polygenic scores were derived in PRSice software90,91, using the following method: First, SNPs from participants were matched with SNPs reported in the GWAS summary statistics for each phenotype (e.g., each mental health problem). Clumping was conducted to remove SNPs in linkage disequilibrium (r2>0.1 within a 250–base pair window). Next, we summed the alleles associated with the phenotype and weighted them by their effect sizes reporting the corresponding GWAS, to compute polygenic scores. We included all matched SNPs regardless of the nominal significance for their association with ACEs. To control for population stratification, we residualised polygenic scores for the first 10 principal components estimated from the genome-wide SNP data. To facilitate interpretability, all polygenic scores were standardised to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1.
The Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study
Sample
The Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study is a prospective cohort of 11,878 children born during the period 2006-2008, and their parents from 21 sites in the United States. The 21 geographic locations of the ABCD research sites are nationally distributed and generally represent the range of demographic and socio-economic diversity of the U.S. birth cohorts comprising the ABCD study population92. Full details on the recruitment strategy are available elsewhere93. Briefly, children aged 9-10 years were recruited through probability sampling of public and private elementary schools within the catchment areas of the 21 research sites. School selection was based on gender, race and ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and urbanicity. Inclusion criteria were the child’s age and attending a public or private elementary school within the catchment areas. Exclusion criteria for children were limited to not being fluent in English, having a parent not fluent in English or Spanish, major medical or neurological conditions, gestational age <28 weeks or birthweight <1200 g, contraindications to MRI scanning, a history of traumatic brain injury, a current diagnosis of moderate/severe autism spectrum disorder, intellectual disability, schizophrenia, or alcohol/substance use disorder94. Assessments were made through in-person visits. This study used data from the baseline assessment (ages 9-10) and 1-year follow-up (ages 10-11), from ABCD Data Release 3.0. 47% of the analytic sample was female.
Measures
Adverse childhood experiences
Consistent with the ALSPAC cohort, we assessed six ACEs (maltreatment, domestic violence, parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, parental separation, and parental criminality) between birth and age 9-10 years. These ACEs have been assessed through parent and child reports from validated questionnaires at the baseline and 1-year follow-up assessment95. Details of these assessments are reported in Supplementary Table 9. In brief, maltreatment was assessed using the parent-reported Kiddie-Structured Assessment for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia module for post-traumatic stress disorder96,97 (KSADS-PTSD; with 8 items for physical, sexual, and emotional abuse) and the Children’s Report of Parental Behavioral Inventory98 (with 5 items for neglect), consistent with previous studies42. Domestic violence was assessed using parent reports on the KSADS-PTSD, and parent and child reports on the Family Environment Scale – Family Conflict Subscale99,100. Parental mental illness and substance abuse were assessed via parent reports on the Family History Assessment Module101 and the Adult Self Report102,103. Parental criminality was assessed through parent reports on the Adverse Life Events Scale104, and parental separation was assessed through parent reports on the Demographic Survey. Measures of each ACE were derived for participants with responses to ≥50% of the questions assessing that ACE between birth to age 9-10 years.
Mental health problems
Internalising problems and externalising problems were assessed using parent reports on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)105 from the baseline assessment at age 9/10. The CBCL is a 119-item, 3-point scale questionnaire which measures problems occurring in the past 6 months, with excellent reliability and validity106. Items from the CBCL used to derive the mental health measures are presented in Supplementary Table 10.
Internalising problems were assessed through the anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, and somatic complaints subscales (32 items), as recommended107. Externalising problems were assessed through the rule-breaking behaviour, aggressive behaviour, and attention problems subscales (45 items). These subscales broadly map onto the DAWBA subscales used to assess internalising and externalising problems in ALSPAC, maximising consistency between the samples. To derive composite scores of internalising and externalising problems, we summed scores across the relevant items (for participants with data for >50% of the items) before standardising the summary measures.
Genotyping and QC
Children from the ABCD Study have been genotyped from blood and saliva samples using the Affymetrix NIDA SmokeScreen Array108. Sample preparation and genotyping was performed by Rutgers RUCDR. Initial QC was performed by the ABCD Data Analysis, Informatics & Resource Center following the Ricopili pipeline109 (see Supplementary Table 8 for details). Imputation was then performed on genotype data using the TOPMed imputation server, following pre-imputation steps instructed at: https://topmedimpute.readthedocs.io/en/latest/prepare-your-data/. In line with previous ABCD studies42,110,111, we performed additional QC on the imputed genetic data (Supplementary Table 8), including removing samples with high relatedness and non-European population ancestry, and removing SNPs which deviate from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, have a low minor allele frequency, and poor imputation quality.
Polygenic scores for mental health problems
We derived polygenic scores for mental health problems and negative controls using the same procedure as described for ALSPAC participants. We also residualised polygenic scores for genotyping batch as ABCD participants have been genotyped in multiple batches.
Analysis plan
We conducted all statistical analyses in R Version 3.6.2112, focusing first on the ALSPAC cohort before testing whether the findings replicate in the ABCD Study (originally planned to be the CATSS dataset). Below we describe the statistical analyses that we will use to test each of our aims and hypotheses (summarised in Table 1). The multiple imputation procedure for ALSPAC and ABCD data is described in the Supplementary Methods 2-3.
Aim 1: Investigate whether children with genetic liability to mental health problems are more likely to be exposed to ACEs
Hypothesis 1a
We first tested the associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs through logistic regression models. We ran separate models for each ACE and each polygenic score (including negative controls). Log odds coefficients were exponentiated to obtain odds ratios reflecting odds of exposure to each ACE per one standard deviation increase in the polygenic score. These models (and all further analyses) controlled for sex and were two-sided. To account for multiple testing, we computed false discovery rate corrected p-values113.
In order to obtain a single effect size reflecting the average association between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs, we pooled the results across all logistic regression models within each cohort. This procedure was performed using the ‘agg’ function in the MAd package114, which accounts for correlations across effect sizes (as a function of the same sample). We pooled two sets of results: 1) for associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs, and 2) for associations between negative control polygenic scores and ACEs.
Because null hypothesis significance testing cannot enable substantive interpretation of statistically non-significant findings, we conducted an equivalence test115 to quantify support for the null hypothesis. This involves assessing whether the 90% confidence intervals for the effect size lie entirely inside pre-specified equivalence bounds indexing the smallest effect size of interest. If the confidence intervals lie inside the equivalence bounds, the effect size can be said to be no more than trivially small. If the confidence intervals are not inside the equivalence bounds, the effect size can be said to be of meaningful magnitude. Note that the 90% (rather than 95%) confidence intervals are used, corresponding to (1–2α) × 100%, because the effect size is tested against two equivalence bounds separately (i.e., the upper and lower bound).
To select equivalence bounds, we followed guidance to use the lower confidence interval of a meta-analytic estimate of the effect of interest115,116. Because no such meta-analysis exists, we conducted a meta-analysis of all studies21–26 that to our knowledge, have tested the association between polygenic scores for mental health problems (see https://osf.io/2uc4p/?view_only=2d9afc1b072b4507ba11ba8771aaab62 for code and results). The pooled association between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs was OR=1.10 (95% CI=1.06-1.14). We thus selected equivalence bounds of 0.94-1.06 on the odds ratio scale, because 1.06 was the lower confidence interval of the meta-analytic effect and 0.94 is the equal delta of 1.06 in the opposite direction on the log odds ratio scale.
We proposed to infer support for Hypothesis 1a (that children with greater genetic liability to mental health problems would have a higher risk of experiencing ACEs) if 1) the pooled odds ratio for the association between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs was greater than 1 and statistically significant, 2) the 90% confidence interval for this effect was not within the equivalence bounds, and 3) the pooled odds ratio for the association between negative control polygenic scores and ACEs was non-significant. The interpretation of alternative patterns of results is shown in Table 1.
Hypothesis 1b
We next tested whether polygenic scores for certain mental health problems are more strongly associated with ACEs than other polygenic scores. To do so, we first used a structural equation model to estimate the associations between each polygenic score and each ACE (Supplementary Fig. 1). This model accounted for correlations between polygenic scores, allowing us to estimate the independent effect of each polygenic score on each ACE. From the model, we calculated the average effect of each polygenic score across all ACEs, estimated as: for the first polygenic score (“PGS_1” in Supplementary Fig. 1) for the second polygenic score (“PGS_2” in Supplementary Fig. 1), and so forth for each polygenic score. These analyses were conducted using the lavaan package117, using the WLSMV estimator with robust standard errors, and the ‘ordered’ argument (for the binary ACE endogenous variables). To aid interpretation, we converted the resulting probit coefficients into odds ratios using the formula: exp(probit × 1.8)118,119. We then conducted a Wald test (using the “lavTestWald” function) to test whether the average effect of each polygenic score on all ACEs varied across polygenic scores. If the Wald test was statistically significant (p<0.05), we conducted pairwise comparisons to assess which polygenic scores differ in prediction of ACEs.
Lastly, we tested for statistical equivalence between different polygenic scores in their average association with ACEs by (1) calculating differences in the average effects of polygenic scores, expressed as (log) odds ratios120, and (2) assessing whether 90% confidence intervals for these differences fall within equivalence bounds of -0.10 to 0.10. We selected these equivalence bounds by identifying the smallest effect size that we have 95% power to detect (log odds difference = 0.10, 95% CI=0.07-0.13). This approach is recommended in the absence of a strong theoretical justification for equivalence bounds115, which was the case as no previous study has formally tested differences between polygenic scores in the association with ACEs.
We proposed to infer support for Hypothesis 1b (that polygenic scores for different mental health problems would equally predict exposure to ACEs) if the Wald test was statistically non-significant (p>0.05) and the 90% confidence intervals for the differences between polygenic scores (in their associations with ACEs) fell within the equivalence bounds. The interpretation of alternative patterns of results is shown in Table 1.
Hypothesis 1c
Next, we tested whether some ACEs were associated with higher polygenic risk of mental health problems than other ACEs. To do so, we used the same structural equation model as estimated for Hypothesis 1b (shown in Supplementary Fig. 1), and calculated the average effect of all polygenic scores for mental health problems on each ACE, estimated as: for the first ACE (“ACE_1”), for the second ACE (“ACE_2”), and so forth for each ACE. We converted results to odds ratios using the formula: exp(probit × 1.8)118,119. We then used a Wald test to test whether the average effect of all polygenic scores for mental health problems on each ACE varies across ACEs. Lastly, we tested for statistical equivalence between different ACEs in their association with polygenic scores by (1) calculating differences in (log) odds ratios between ACEs, and (2) assessing whether 90% confidence intervals for these differences fall within equivalence bounds of -0.05 to 0.05. We selected these equivalence bounds because 0.05 is the smallest effect size that we have 95% power to detect (log odds difference = 0.05, 95% CI=0.03-0.07). We adopted this approach in the absence of theoretical justification for equivalence bounds115, as no previous study has tested for differences between ACEs in their association with polygenic scores for psychopathology.
We proposed to infer support for Hypothesis 1c (that parental mental illness and parental substance abuse would be associated with higher polygenic risk for mental health problems) if 1) the Wald test was significant (p<0.05) and further pairwise comparisons (between parental mental illness, parental substance abuse, and parental criminality with all other ACEs) showed that these ACEs were associated with higher polygenic risk than other ACEs, and 2) the 90% confidence intervals for these differences were not within the equivalence bounds. Interpretation of alternative patterns of results is shown in Table 1.
Aim 2: Investigate the extent to which genetic liability explains the associations between ACEs and mental health
Hypothesis 2a
To test the proportion of the associations between ACEs and mental health (internalising and externalising problems) explained by observed polygenic scores, we used structural equation models in the lavaan117 package. Fig. 5 depicts these models, with panel A showing the underlying conceptual model, panel B showing the statistical model with one polygenic score, and panel C showing the statistical model with multiple polygenic scores. As shown in panels B and C, polygenic scores were treated as mediators, as mediation and confounding are statistically equivalent121. The genetic confounding effect was therefore calculated as the indirect effect of the ACE on mental health through the polygenic scores: (a1 * b1) + (a2 * b2) + … + (a8 * b8), based on Fig. 5C. Notably, this estimate does not conflate genetic confounding with genetic effects on mental health mediated via exposure to ACEs (see 27 and https://osf.io/2uc4p/?view_only=2d9afc1b072b4507ba11ba8771aaab62 for further explanation and simulations demonstrating this). In turn, the proportion of the association between the ACE and mental health outcome explained by the polygenic scores was calculated as:
For this analysis, we included all polygenic scores (i.e., 8 mediators) and estimated separate models for each ACE and each mental health outcome (internalising and externalising problems). As a quality control check, we estimated a separate model including only negative control polygenic scores (Supplementary Figure 1).
To obtain a single estimate reflecting the proportion of the associations between ACEs and mental health outcomes captured by observed polygenic scores, we averaged the results across 6 models for all ACEs (for internalising and externalising problems, separately). This was performed using the ‘agg’ function from the MAd package114. Prior to aggregating the results, we planned to transform proportions using the Freeman-Tukey double arcsine transformation122 to normalise and stabilise the variance of the sampling distribution. However, it was not possible to apply this transformation across the results as several proportions were less than zero – which can arise when the direct and indirect effects are in different directions. We therefore used the raw proportions for consistency across all models. We pooled two sets of results, reflecting proportions of the associations between ACEs and mental health captured by: 1) polygenic scores for mental health problems, and 2) negative control polygenic scores.
We proposed to infer support for Hypothesis 2a (that a small proportion of the associations between ACEs and mental health problems would be explained by polygenic scores) if 1) polygenic scores for mental health problems explained, on average, between 5% to 20% of the associations, and 2) the average proportion of the association explained by negative control polygenic scores was not significantly different from zero. We proposed to interpret alternative proportions of less than 5% as “very small”, proportions between 20% and 40% as “moderate”, and proportions of more than 40% as “large”, broadly in line with guidance for interpreting effect sizes123.
Hypothesis 2b
Lastly, we estimated the proportion of the associations between ACEs and mental health problems explained by a latent polygenic score which captures SNP heritability in the mental health outcome. This genetic sensitivity analysis27,124 involves estimating the structural equation model shown in Fig. 5B from a correlation matrix. This matrix includes correlations between the polygenic score and the ACE (a path), the polygenic score and the mental health outcome (b path), and the ACE and the mental health outcome (cp path). Critically, this correlation matrix can be modified to reflect additional genetic variance captured in the outcome. For example, as the SNP-based heritability of parent-reported childhood internalising problems is 6%33, the correlation coefficient from the polygenic score to internalising problems (b path) can be changed to r = 0.24 (calculated by taking the square-root of 0.06). The correlation coefficient for the a path between the polygenic score and the ACE (a path) will also increase to , where k reflects the ratio between the path from the polygenic score to the ACE, and the path from the polygenic score to internalising problems (k = a / b). Note that the SNP heritability estimate for childhood externalising problems that was used for this analysis is 9%33 (hence, r = 0.30). Supplementary Table 11 shows the method for estimating each of the original paths included in the correlation matrix.
To obtain a single estimate reflecting the proportion of the associations between ACEs and mental health outcomes captured by polygenic scores capturing SNP-based heritability, we averaged the results across 6 models for all ACEs (for internalising and externalising problems, separately). As described above for Hypothesis 2a, this was performed using the MAd package125.
We proposed to infer support for Hypothesis 2b (that a moderate proportion of the association is explained by polygenic scores) if polygenic scores capturing SNP-based heritability explained between 20% to 40% of the associations between ACEs and mental health outcomes on average. We planned to interpret alternative proportions of less than 5% as “very small”, proportions between 5% and 20% as “small”, and proportions of more than 40% as “large”.
Sampling plan
Inclusion criteria and sample size
ALSPAC
We planned to include ALSPAC children if they had data on genotype that passed QC (see QC exclusions in Supplementary Table 8), ACEs (defined as responses to ≥ 50% of the questions in the assessments between birth and age 9 years for each ACE), internalising problems at age 10 (defined as responses to ≥ 50% of items assessing separation anxiety, social anxiety, general anxiety, and major depression on the Development and Wellbeing Assessment [DAWBA]) and externalising problems at age 10 (defined as responses to ≥ 50% of items assessing hyperkinesis/ADHD and conduct/oppositional disorders on the DAWBA). Based on a previous ALSPAC study using data on genotype and the DAWBA at age 10126, we expected the sample of complete cases to be N~5,900. However, to maximise sample size and reduce selection bias due to attrition, we proposed to use multiple imputation to impute missing values in the ACEs and internalising and externalising problems measures (see Supplementary Methods 2 for details of the inclusion criteria for imputation).
ABCD
We planned to include children from the ABCD Study if they had data on genotype that passed QC (see QC exclusions in Supplementary Table 8), ACEs (defined as responses to ≥ 50% to items assessing each ACE), internalising problems, and externalising problems at age 9/10 (defined as responses to ≥ 50% of relevant items on the CBCL). Based on previous ABCD studies using genotype data and ACEs/CBCL data, we expected the sample size to be between 4,700-5,40042,127. However, because we anticipated that the sample size may vary across different assessments (used to derive measures of ACEs and mental health), we proposed to use multiple imputation to maximise the sample size by imputing missing values in measures of ACEs and mental health (see Supplementary Methods 3 for details of the inclusion criteria for imputation).
Power calculations
We calculated power to test each of our hypotheses assuming a conservative minimum sample size of N=4,700, as the minimum expected sample sizes were 4,700 for the ABCD Study and 5,900 for ALSPAC. (Note that the ABCD Study was not originally included in the Stage 1 pre-registration, but we used it because the original dataset, the Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden [CATSS], was not accessible after Stage 1 acceptance. The expected sample size for CATSS was 11,000). We conducted each power analysis using simulation (1,000 simulated datasets) in the MASS128 and stats112 packages, and set the alpha level for statistical significance to 0.05. As described below, power to test each hypothesis was ≥0.95.
Hypothesis 1a
We calculated power to obtain a single effect size reflecting the average association between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs across 48 logistic regression models (i.e., 8 polygenic scores x 6 ACEs). This analysis showed that power will be 0.96 to detect an average odds ratio of 1.04 for the effect of polygenic scores on ACEs using the ‘agg’ function in the MAd package114 (accounting for dependent effect sizes). An odds ratio of 1.04 is a conservative estimate as the average odds ratio for the associations between polygenic scores for mental health problems and ACEs in previous research21–26 was 1.10 (see https://osf.io/2uc4p/?view_only=2d9afc1b072b4507ba11ba8771aaab62 for details).
Hypothesis 1b
We calculated power to detect a significant difference in the associations between polygenic scores and ACEs according to the type of polygenic score, using a Wald test in lavaan117. This analysis showed that we will have 1.00 power to detect a difference across 8 effect sizes (reflecting the average effect of each polygenic score on ACEs), when the smallest and largest odds ratios differ by 0.11 (e.g., odds ratio=1.05 versus 1.16), with other effect sizes taking intermediate values. A simulation using a structural equation model (shown Supplementary Fig. 1) showed that these odds ratios are plausible assuming previously observed effects of polygenic scores on ACEs (odds ratios of between 1.03 and 1.1621), and average correlations of r = 0.06 between polygenic scores23 and r = 0.30 between ACEs in ALSPAC77.
Hypothesis 1c
Similarly to Hypothesis 1b, we calculated power to detect a significant difference in the associations between polygenic scores and ACEs according to the type of ACE, using a Wald test in lavaan117. This analysis showed that we will have 1.00 power to detect a difference across 6 effect sizes (reflecting the average effect of all polygenic scores on each ACE), when the smallest and largest odds ratios differ by 0.10 (e.g., odds ratio = 1.05 versus 1.15), with other effect sizes taking intermediate values. As described above, these effect sizes were found to be plausible in a simulation based on the structural equation model in Supplementary Fig. 1, assuming previously observed odds ratios for the effects of polygenic scores on different ACEs varying between 1.03 to 1.1621 and average correlations of r = 0.06 between polygenic scores23 and r = 0.30 between ACEs in ALSPAC77.
Hypothesis 2a
We calculated power for two analyses: (i) a structural equation model to estimate the proportion of the association between (individual) ACEs and mental health outcomes explained by polygenic scores, and (ii) an aggregate model to average the results across individual structural equation models. For the structural equation model (shown in Fig. 5C), power was 0.95 to detect the proportion of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by observed polygenic scores. This is assuming previously observed small independent effects of polygenic scores for mental health problems on ACEs (r = 0.03-0.07)23 and internalising and externalising problems (r = 0.01-0.05)84, small effects of individual ACEs on internalising and externalising problems (r=0.06)129, and average correlations between polygenic scores of r = 0.0623. For the aggregate model, power was 1.00 to detect an average proportion of 5% (of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by polygenic scores), assuming correlations of r = 0.30 between effect sizes. We consider 5% to be a conservative estimate of the likely proportion of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by multiple polygenic scores, given that prior studies have found that a single polygenic score can account for larger proportions of the associations between environmental exposures and mental health (e.g., 6%30 and 18%28).
Hypothesis 2b
We calculated power for a structural equation model with a single mediator (i.e., a polygenic score capturing additional genetic variance in the outcome), as shown in Fig. 5B. Power was 1.00 to detect the proportion of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by a polygenic score that captures SNP heritability in the outcome. This is assuming a path from the polygenic score to internalising problems of r = 0.24 (i.e., the square root of 0.06, as the SNP-based heritability of internalising problems is 6%33), a path from the polygenic score to the ACE of r = 0.07 (assuming that k=0.33, i.e., that the effect of the observed polygenic score on the ACE is a third of the size as the effect of the observed polygenic score on internalising problems), and a path from the ACE to internalising problems of r = 0.06 (as observed previously129). Note that power will be equally high for analyses on externalising problems because the SNP-based heritability of externalising problems is slightly higher than for internalising problems (9% versus 6%33). Furthermore, note that power will be ≥0.96 to aggregate these results to obtain an average proportion across models, assuming that the proportion will be 5% or greater (as tested above for Hypothesis 2a). This is because as the strength of the association between polygenic scores and mental health outcomes increases, the proportion of the association between ACEs and mental health explained by polygenic scores will increase27.
Protocol registration
The Stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on 4 January 2021. The protocol, as accepted by the journal, can be found at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13580777.v1
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
ALSPAC. We are extremely grateful to all the ALSPAC families who took part in the study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant Ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC data collection. A comprehensive list of grant funding is available on the ALSPAC website. GWAS data was generated by Sample Logistics and Genotyping Facilities at Wellcome Sanger Institute and LabCorp (Laboratory Corporation of America) using support from 23andMe.
ABCD. Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9-10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. We are extremely grateful to Dr Gustavo Sudre for his support with the quality control of the ABCD genetic data.
Authors’ funding
This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust [grant 215917/Z/19/Z]. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. L.D.H is supported by a Career Development Award fellowship from the UK Medical Research Council (MR/M020894/1). H.M.S, A.S.F.K, M..M and L.D.H work in a unit that receives funding from the University of Bristol and the UK Medical Research Council (MC_UU_00011/5, MC_UU_00011/7). H.M.S and M.R.M are supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at the University Hospitals Bristol National Health Service Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol. M.R.M and H.M.S are members of the UK Centre for Tobacco and Alcohol Studies, a UKCRC Public Health Research: Centre of Excellence. A.D was funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC; grant no. P005918) and by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. The funders have/had no role in study design, data analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Author contributions
Author contributions are presented according to the CRediT (Contributor Roles Taxonomy). J.R.B: Conceptualisation, methodology, formal analysis, data curation, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing, project administration, funding acquisition. H.M.S: Software, data curation, writing - review & editing. T.S: Software, data curation, writing - review & editing. M.J.T: Software, data curation, writing - review & editing. A.S.F.K: Writing - review & editing. J.T: Software, resources, writing - review & editing. W.B: Software, resources, data curation, writing - review & editing. V.W: Software, writing - review & editing. L.D.H: Software, data curation, writing - review & editing. A.D: Conceptualisation, writing - review & editing. E.M: Writing - review & editing. F.R: Methodology. H.L: Investigation, writing - review & editing. S.L: Investigation, writing - review & editing. R.K: Software, data curation. P.L: Conceptualisation, investigation, writing - review & editing. M.M: Conceptualisation, writing - review & editing, supervision. J-B.P: Conceptualisation, methodology, formal analysis, data curation, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing, supervision.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Data availability
The ABCD Study anonymized data, including all assessment domains, are released annually to the research community. Information on how to access ABCD data through the NDA is available on the ABCD Study data-sharing webpage: https://abcdstudy.org/scientists_data_sharing.html. Instructions on how to create an NDA study are available at https://nda.nih.gov/training/modules/study.html. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ALSPAC data are not publicly available as informed consent for public data-sharing, and ethical approval for public data-sharing were not obtained from participants. Researchers can find details of how to apply for access to the ALSPAC dataset here: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/.
Code availability
Analysis code can be found on https://github.com/jr-baldwin/ACEs_mental_health_RR.
References
- 1.Chapman DP, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. J Affect Disord. 2004;82:217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hughes K, et al. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2017;2:e356–e366. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Felitti VJ, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Baldwin JR, et al. Population vs individual prediction of poor health from results of Adverse Childhood Experiences screening. JAMA Pediatrics. 2021;175:385–393. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.5602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McLaughlin KA, et al. Childhood adversities and first onset of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of US adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:1151–1160. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brown NM, et al. Associations between adverse childhood experiences and ADHD diagnosis and severity. Acad Pediatr. 2017;17:349–355. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2016.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hunt TK, Slack KS, Berger LM. Adverse childhood experiences and behavioral problems in middle childhood. Child Abuse Neglect. 2017;67:391–402. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.House of Commons Science and Technology Committee. Evidence-based early years intervention. 2018. https://publications.parliament.uk/pa/cm201719/cmselect/cmsctech/
- 9.Early Intervention Foundation. Adverse childhood experiences: What we know, what we don’t know, and what should happen next. London: 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Danese A. Annual Research Review: Rethinking childhood trauma‐new research directions for measurement, study design and analytical strategies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;61:236–250. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jaffee SR. Child maltreatment and risk for psychopathology in childhood and adulthood. Annual review of clinical psychology. 2017;13:525–551. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Polderman TJ, et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet. 2015;47:702–709. doi: 10.1038/ng.3285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jaffee SR, Price TS. Gene–environment correlations: A review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:432–442. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1977;84:309–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sidebotham P, Golding J, Team AS. Child maltreatment in the “Children of the Nineties”: A longitudinal study of parental risk factors. Child Abuse Neglect. 2001;25:1177–1200. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(01)00261-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.O’Connor TG, Deater-Deckard K, Fulker D, Rutter M, Plomin R. Genotype– environment correlations in late childhood and early adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems and coercive parenting. Developmental psychology. 1998;34:970. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Marceau K, et al. Gene–environment correlation underlying the association between parental negativity and adolescent externalizing problems. Child development. 2013;84:2031–2046. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baldwin JR, et al. Adolescent victimization and self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: A genetically sensitive cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58:506–513. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.07.903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Baldwin JR, Ayorech Z, Rijsdijk FV, Schoeler T, Pingault J-B. Cyber-victimisation and mental health in young people: a co-twin control study. Psychol Med. 2021;51:2620–2630. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Van IJzendoorn MH, Euser EM, Prinzie P, Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Elevated risk of child maltreatment in families with stepparents but not with adoptive parents. Child Maltreat. 2009;14:369–375. doi: 10.1177/1077559509342125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sallis HM, et al. Genetic liability to schizophrenia is associated with exposure to traumatic events in childhood. Psychol Med. 2020;51:1814–1821. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Zwicker A, et al. Neurodevelopmental and genetic determinants of exposure to adversity among youth at risk for mental illness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;61:536–544. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schoeler T, et al. Multi–polygenic score approach to identifying individual vulnerabilities aassociated with the risk of exposure to bullying. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:730–738. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Coleman JR, et al. Genome-wide gene-environment analyses of major depressive disorder and reported lifetime traumatic experiences in UK Biobank. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25:1–17. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0546-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Peyrot WJ, et al. Does childhood trauma moderate polygenic risk for depression? A meta-analysis of 5765 subjects from the psychiatric genomics consortium. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84:138–147. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Warrier V, Baron-Cohen S. Childhood trauma, life-time self-harm, and suicidal behaviour and ideation are associated with polygenic scores for autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;26:1670–1684. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0550-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pingault J-B, et al. Genetic sensitivity analysis: adjusting for genetic confounding in epidemiological associations. PLoS Genet. 2021;17:e1009590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1009590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lehto K, et al. Childhood Adoption and Mental Health in Adulthood: The Role of Gene-Environment Correlations and Interactions in the UK Biobank. Biol Psychiatry. 2020;87:708–716. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wertz J, et al. Using DNA from mothers and children to study parental investment in children’s educational attainment. Child development. 2019;91:1745–1761. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Krapohl E, et al. Widespread covariation of early environmental exposures and trait-associated polygenic variation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017;114:11727–11732. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1707178114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Holmlund H, Lindahl M, Plug E. The causal effect of parents’ schooling on children’s schooling: A comparison of estimation methods. J Econ Lit. 2011;49:615–651. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Krapohl E, Plomin R. Genetic link between family socioeconomic status and children’s educational achievement estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:437–443. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cheesman R, et al. Childhood behaviour problems show the greatest gap between DNA-based and twin heritability. Translational Psychiatry. 2017;7:1284. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0046-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cheesman R, et al. Extracting stability increases the SNP heritability of emotional problems in young people. Translational Psychiatry. 2018;8:223. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0269-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mullins N, et al. Genome-wide association study of more than 40,000 bipolar disorder cases provides new insights into the underlying biology. Nat Genet. 2021;53:817–829. doi: 10.1038/s41588-021-00857-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Stahl EA, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 30 loci associated with bipolar disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51:793–803. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0397-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ratanatharathorn A, et al. Polygenic risk for autism, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, schizophrenia, major depressive disorder, and neuroticism is associated with the experience of childhood abuse. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:1696–1705. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-00996-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Peel AJ, et al. Genetic and early environmental predictors of adulthood self-reports of trauma. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2022:1–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2021.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Walsh D, McCartney G, Smith M, Armour G. Relationship between childhood socioeconomic position and adverse childhood experiences (ACEs): a systematic review. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2019;73:1087–1093. doi: 10.1136/jech-2019-212738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Olds DL, et al. Long-term effects of home visitation on maternal life course and child abuse and neglect: Fifteen-year follow-up of a randomized trial. JAMA. 1997;278:637–643. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Feinberg ME, et al. Couple-focused prevention at the transition to parenthood, a randomized trial: Effects on coparenting, parenting, family violence, and parent and child adjustment. Prev Sci. 2016;17:751–764. doi: 10.1007/s11121-016-0674-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Warrier V, et al. Gene–environment correlations and causal effects of childhood maltreatment on physical and mental health: a genetically informed approach. Lancet Psychiatry. 2021;8:373–386. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30569-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lecei A, et al. Evidence that the association of childhood trauma with psychosis and related psychopathology is not explained by gene-environment correlation: A monozygotic twin differences approach. Schizophr Res. 2019;205:58–62. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Baldwin JR, Wang B, Karwatowska L, Schoeler T, Tsaligopoulou A, Munafò MR, Pingault JB. Childhood maltreatment and mental health problems: A systematic review and meta-analysis of quasi-experimental studies. Am J Psychiatry. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.20220174. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Eley TC, et al. The intergenerational transmission of anxiety: a children-of-twins study. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172:630–637. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14070818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Silberg JL, Maes H, Eaves LJ. Genetic and environmental influences on the transmission of parental depression to children’s depression and conduct disturbance: an extended Children of Twins study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51:734–744. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02205.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.McAdams T, et al. The relationship between parental depressive symptoms and offspring psychopathology: evidence from a children-of-twins study and an adoption study. Psychol Med. 2015;45:2583–2594. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Haber JR, Jacob T, Heath AC. Paternal alcoholism and offspring conduct disorder: evidence for the ‘common genes’ hypothesis. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2005;8:120–131. doi: 10.1375/1832427053738782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Waldron M, Martin NG, Heath AC. Parental alcoholism and offspring behavior problems: Findings in Australian children of twins. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2009;12:433–440. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.5.433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kendler K, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, Sundquist J. Cross-generational transmission from drug abuse in parents to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Psychol Med. 2016;46:1301–1309. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bornovalova MA, et al. Understanding the relative contributions of direct environmental effects and passive genotype–environment correlations in the association between familial risk factors and child disruptive behavior disorders. Psychol Med. 2014;44:831–844. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.D’Onofrio BM, et al. A children of twins study of parental divorce and offspring psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:667–675. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01741.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.D’Onofrio BM, et al. A genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:570. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Baldwin JR, Degli Esposti M. Triangulating evidence on the role of perceived versus objective experiences of childhood adversity in psychopathology. JCPP Advances. 2021;1:e12010. doi: 10.1111/jcv2.12010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Pingault JB, Allegrini AG, Odigie T, Frach L, Baldwin JR, Rijsdijk F, Dudbridge F. Research Review: How to interpret associations between polygenic scores, environmental risks, and phenotypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022 doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Duncan L, et al. Analysis of polygenic risk score usage and performance in diverse human populations. Nature communications. 2019;10:1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11112-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Martin AR, et al. Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nat Genet. 2019;51:584–591. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0379-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gage SH, Davey Smith G, Ware JJ, Flint J, Munafo MR. G= E: What GWAS can tell us about the environment. PLoS Genet. 2016;12:e1005765. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1005765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Dunn EC, et al. Research review: Gene–environment interaction research in youth depression–a systematic review with recommendations for future research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:1223–1238. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02466.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Schoeler T, Duncan L, Cecil CM, Ploubidis GB, Pingault J-B. Quasi-experimental evidence on short-and long-term consequences of bullying victimization: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2018;144:1229. doi: 10.1037/bul0000171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ahmadzadeh YI, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of genetically informed research: associations between parent anxiety and offspring internalizing problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60:823–840. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.12.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Baldwin JR, Pingault J-B, Schoeler T, Sallis HM, Munafò MR. Protecting against researcher bias in secondary data analysis: challenges and potential solutions. Eur J Epidemiol. 2022;37:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10654-021-00839-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Franco A, Malhotra N, Simonovits G. Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer. Science. 2014;345:1502–1505. doi: 10.1126/science.1255484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Holmes MV, et al. Association between alcohol and cardiovascular disease: Mendelian randomisation analysis based on individual participant data. BMJ. 2014;349:g4164. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g4164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ. Tests of causal links between alcohol abuse or dependence and major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:260–266. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, Sundquist J. Drug abuse-associated mortality across the lifespan: a population-based longitudinal cohort and co-relative analysis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52:877–886. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1398-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Murray J, Blokland A, Farrington DP, Theobald D. Labeling Theory. Routledge; 2017. pp. 209–235. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Early Intervention Foundation. Trauma-Focused Cognitive Behavioural Therapy. 2020. < https://guidebook.eif.org.uk/programme/trauma-focused-cognitive-behavioural-therapy#about-the-programme>.
- 69.Auchter AM, et al. A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Clark DB, et al. Biomedical ethics and clinical oversight in multisite observational neuroimaging studies with children and adolescents: The ABCD experience. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:143–154. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Boyd A, et al. Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Fraser A, et al. Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;42:97–110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Northstone K, et al. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC): an update on the enrolled sample of index children in 2019. Wellcome Open Research. 2019;4 doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15132.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Dong M, et al. The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse Neglect. 2004;28:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.World Health Organisation. Adverse Childhood Experiences International Questionnaire (ACE-IQ) 2018 [Google Scholar]
- 76.Houtepen L, Heron J, Suderman M, Tilling K, Howe L. Adverse childhood experiences in the children of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) Wellcome Open Research. 2018;3 doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.14716.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Russell AE, et al. Pathways between early‐life adversity and adolescent self‐harm: the mediating role of inflammation in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;60:1094–1103. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. The development and well‐being assessment: Description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:645–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Aebi M, et al. The use of the development and well-being assessment (DAWBA) in clinical practice: a randomized trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:559–567. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0293-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Purcell S, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Anderson CA, et al. Data quality control in genetic case-control association studies. Nat Protoc. 2010;5:1564. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2010.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Marees AT, et al. A tutorial on conducting genome‐wide association studies: Quality control and statistical analysis. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2018;27:e1608. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.University of Bristol. GWAS data generation Details as of 26/11/2012. 2012. < http://www.bristol.ac.uk/media-library/sites/alspac/migrated/documents/gwas-data-generation.pdf?u07022013>.
- 84.Akingbuwa WA, et al. Genetic associations between childhood psychopathology and adult depression and associated traits in 42?998 individuals: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77:715–728. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Taylor MJ, et al. Association of genetic risk factors for psychiatric disorders and traits of these disorders in a Swedish population twin sample. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:280–289. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kandaswamy R, Allegrini AG, Cave SN, Plomin R, von Stumm S. Predicting alcohol use from genome-wide polygenic scores, environmental factors, and their interactions in young adulthood. Psychosom Med. 2022;84:244–250. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000001005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Tielbeek JJ, et al. Genome-wide association studies of a broad spectrum of antisocial behavior. JAMA psychiatry. 2017;74:1242–1250. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Tielbeek JJ, et al. Uncovering the Genetic Architecture of Broad Antisocial Behavior through a Genome-Wide Association Study Meta-analysis. bioRxiv. 2021:2021.2010.2019.462578. doi: 10.1101/2021.10.19.462578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Choi SW, Mak TSH, Hoggart CJ, O’Reilly PF. EraSOR: Erase Sample Overlap in polygenic score analyses. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1093/gigascience/giad043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Euesden J, Lewis CM, O’Reilly PF. PRSice: polygenic risk score software. Bioinformatics. 2014;31:1466–1468. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Choi SW, Mak TS-H, O’Reilly PF. Tutorial: a guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nat Protoc. 2020;15:2759–2772. doi: 10.1038/s41596-020-0353-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Heeringa SG, Berglund PA. A guide for population-based analysis of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study baseline data. BioRxiv. 2020 [Google Scholar]
- 93.Garavan H, et al. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Michelini G, et al. Delineating and validating higher-order dimensions of psychopathology in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Translational psychiatry. 2019;9:1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0593-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Hoffman EA, et al. Stress exposures, neurodevelopment and health measures in the ABCD study. Neurobiology of stress. 2019;10:100157. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Geller B, et al. Reliability of the Washington University in St. Louis Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (WASH-U-KSADS) mania and rapid cycling sections. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:450–455. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200104000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Kaufman J, et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): Initial Reliability and Validity Data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Schaefer ES. Children’s reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child development. 1965:413–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Moos RH. Family environment scale manual: Development, applications, research. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 100.Moos RH. Conceptual and empirical approaches to developing family‐based assessment procedures: Resolving the case of the Family Environment Scale. Fam Process. 1990;29:199–208. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1990.00199.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Rice JP, et al. Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1995;19:1018–1023. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb00983.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Achenbach TM. Manual for the young adult self-report and young adult behavior checklist. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rescorla LA. The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) or Ages 18 to 90+ Years. The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment: Volume 3: Instruments for adults. 2004;115 [Google Scholar]
- 104.Tiet QQ, et al. Relationship between specific adverse life events and psychiatric disorders. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2001;29:153–164. doi: 10.1023/a:1005288130494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Achenbach T. Manual for the Child Behaviour Checklist and 1991 profile. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 106.Achenbach T, Rescorla L. Reliability, internal consistency, cross-informant agreement, and stability. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. 2001:99–135. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Achenbach TM. In: Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Kreutzer Jeffrey S, DeLuca John, Caplan Bruce., editors. Springer; New York: 2011. pp. 546–552. [Google Scholar]
- 108.Baurley JW, Edlund CK, Pardamean CI, Conti DV, Bergen AW. Smokescreen: a targeted genotyping array for addiction research. BMC Genomics. 2016;17:1–12. doi: 10.1186/s12864-016-2495-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Lam M, et al. RICOPILI: Rapid Imputation for COnsortias PIpeLIne. Bioinformatics. 2020;36:930–933. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btz633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Ohi K, et al. Polygenic risk scores for major psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders contribute to sleep disturbance in childhood: Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Translational psychiatry. 2021;11:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01308-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Hatoum AS, et al. Polygenic risk scores for alcohol involvement relate to brain structure in substance‐naïve children: Results from the ABCD study. Genes, Brain and Behav. 2021;20:e12756. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 113.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 114.Del Re A. A practical tutorial on conducting meta-analysis in R. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology. 2015;11:37–50. [Google Scholar]
- 115.Lakens D. Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2017;8:355–362. doi: 10.1177/1948550617697177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Lakens D, Scheel AM, Isager PM. Equivalence testing for psychological research: A tutorial. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2018;1:259–269. [Google Scholar]
- 117.Rosseel Y. Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5–12 (BETA) Journal of Statistical Software. 2012;48:1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 118.Muthén LK, Muthén B. Regression analysis, exploratory factor analysis, confirmatory factor analysis, and structural equation modeling for categorical, censored, and count outcomes. 2009. < http://statmodel.com/download/Topic%202-v20%20%5BCompatibility%20Mode%5D1.pdf>.
- 119.Agresti A. An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 120.Genome Toolbox. Test for a Difference in Two Odds Ratios. 2014 < http://genometoolbox.blogspot.com/2014/06/test-for-difference-in-two-odds-ratios.html>. [Google Scholar]
- 121.MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000;1:173–181. doi: 10.1023/a:1026595011371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Freeman MF, Tukey JW. Transformations related to the angular and the square root. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics. 1950;21:607–611. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Funder DC, Ozer DJ. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2:156–168. [Google Scholar]
- 124.Pingault J-B, et al. Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2018;19:566. doi: 10.1038/s41576-018-0020-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Del Re A, Hoyt WT. MAd-package: Meta-Analysis with Mean Differences. 2014 [Google Scholar]
- 126.Nivard MG, et al. Genetic Overlap Between Schizophrenia and Developmental Psychopathology: Longitudinal and Multivariate Polygenic Risk Prediction of Common Psychiatric Traits During Development. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:1197–1207. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Waszczuk MA, et al. General v. specific vulnerabilities: Polygenic risk scores and higher-order psychopathology dimensions in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Psychol Med. 2021:1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721003639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Venables WN, Ripley BD. Modern applied statistics with S-PLUS. Springer Science & Business Media; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 129.Cecil CA, Viding E, Fearon P, Glaser D, McCrory EJ. Disentangling the mental health impact of childhood abuse and neglect. Child Abuse Neglect. 2017;63:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The ABCD Study anonymized data, including all assessment domains, are released annually to the research community. Information on how to access ABCD data through the NDA is available on the ABCD Study data-sharing webpage: https://abcdstudy.org/scientists_data_sharing.html. Instructions on how to create an NDA study are available at https://nda.nih.gov/training/modules/study.html. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ALSPAC data are not publicly available as informed consent for public data-sharing, and ethical approval for public data-sharing were not obtained from participants. Researchers can find details of how to apply for access to the ALSPAC dataset here: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/.
Analysis code can be found on https://github.com/jr-baldwin/ACEs_mental_health_RR.