Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Dec 9.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Biol. 2023 Jun 28;33(14):2925–2940.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.06.022

Sleep-like changes in neural processing emerge during sleep deprivation in early auditory cortex

Amit Marmelshtein 1,2, Anabel Eckerling 1,3, Barak Hadad 4, Shamgar Ben-Eliyahu 1,3, Yuval Nir 1,2,5,6,*
PMCID: PMC7617130  EMSID: EMS193476  PMID: 37385257

Summary

Insufficient sleep is commonplace in modern lifestyle and can lead to grave outcomes, yet the changes in neuronal activity accumulating over hours of extended wakefulness remain poorly understood. Specifically, which aspects of cortical processing are affected by sleep deprivation (SD), and whether they also affect early sensory regions, remains unclear. Here, we recorded spiking activity in rat auditory cortex along with polysomnography while presenting sounds during SD followed by recovery sleep. We found that frequency tuning, onset responses, and spontaneous firing rates were largely unaffected by SD. By contrast, SD decreased entrainment to rapid (≥20 Hz) click-trains, increased population synchrony, and increased the prevalence of sleep-like stimulus-induced silent periods, even when ongoing activity was similar. Recovery NREM sleep was associated with similar effects as SD with even greater magnitude, while auditory processing during REM sleep was similar to vigilant wakefulness. Our results show that processes akin to those in NREM sleep invade the activity of cortical circuits during SD, already in early sensory cortex.

Keywords: NREM, REM, A1, frequency tuning, rat, click-trains, OFF periods, state-dependent, sensory

Introduction

Sleep deprivation (SD) is inherent to modern daily life and entails considerable social and health-related costs1. During SD, homeostatic and circadian processes interact to build up sleep pressure2 that impairs cognitive performance3, and can lead to serious consequences such as car accidents and medical errors1. Cognitive functions particularly affected by SD include psychomotor and cognitive speed, vigilant and executive attention, working memory, emotional regulation, and higher cognitive abilities4 associated with activity in attentional thalamic and fronto-parietal circuits513.

Previous non-invasive studies examined the effect of insufficient sleep on neurophysiological activity4,1417, yet only few studies examined the effects of SD on spiking activities in local neuronal populations. In the rat frontal cortex, robust changes in spontaneous cortical activity gradually emerge during merely a few hours of SD18. One study examined the effects of extended wakefulness on sensory responses in high-order human temporal lobe neurons, reporting attenuated, prolonged and delayed responses associated with behavioral lapses19. However, it remains largely unknown whether such effects are restricted to high-order multi-modal regions, or may also affect neuronal activities along specific sensory pathways. Studying the effects of SD on early sensory processing can help shed light on the fundamental processes by which the slow buildup of sleep pressure alters neural processing.

A parallel, equally important, motivation for studying the effects of SD on sensory processing is that it serves as a unique and powerful model for assessing the effects of brain state and arousal on sensory processing at the neuronal level20,21. A rich body of literature reports the effects of behavioral state and arousal on sensory processing, particularly in the auditory domain. Such studies typically employ one of the following three strategies; One approach is studying how sensory processing differs with respect to behavioral performance on specific tasks2226. A second approach focuses on momentary changes in arousal, indexed by pupil size, EEG or locomotor activity during wakefulness2731. The third strategy contrasts sensory processing in wakefulness with those during anesthesia or natural sleep3239. In this context, SD affords an additional unique window to examine how brain states affect sensory processing by offering a ‘middle-tier’ alternative - a state where subjects are awake and responsive but already show behavioral deficits4,40. It remains unexplored whether slow accumulation of sleep pressure over hours of SD and extended wakefulness may cause state-dependent changes in sensory processing similar to those associated with momentary arousal changes, on one hand, and to what extent such changes are reminiscent of changes observed during actual sleep, on the other.

Here, we set out to address these issues and examine to what extent SD constitutes an intermediate state between vigilant wakefulness and sleep. We compared neuronal spiking activity in the auditory cortex of freely behaving rats in response to a wide array of sounds including click trains and tones (dynamic random chords41). We separately examined how SD affects different aspects of auditory processing including spontaneous activity, frequency tuning, population synchrony, onset vs. sustained responses, and entrainment to slow- vs. fast-varying inputs. Previous research established that some “motifs” of cortical auditory processing are relatively invariant to momentary changes in arousal (e.g. onset responses) whereas other motifs are sensitive to behavioral state (e.g. noise correlations, late sustained responses)35,42. Therefore, we hypothesized that cumulative changes over several hours of experimentally-induced SD will lead to changes in specific aspects of sensory-evoked activity and that such changes will be detected already in early auditory cortex22,23,25,29. In line with this hypothesis, our results show that frequency tuning, onset responses, and spontaneous firing rates were unaffected by SD. By contrast, SD decreased neuronal entrainment to rapid (≥20 Hz) click-trains, increased population synchrony, and increased the prevalence of sleep-like stimulus-induced silent intervals. The changes brought about by SD were qualitatively similar to those observed during recovery NREM sleep, but not during REM sleep where auditory processing was similar to vigilant wakefulness. Thus, our results show that processes akin to those in NREM sleep invade the activity of cortical circuits during SD, already in early sensory cortex.

Results

To study how sleep deprivation and sleep states affect sensory processing and compare auditory responses across Vigilant, Tired, and sleep conditions, adult male Wister rats (n=7) were implanted with microwire arrays targeting the auditory cortex (AC), as well as EEG and EMG electrodes. After recovery and habituation, rats were placed inside a computer-controlled motorized running wheel within an acoustic chamber for 10 hours starting at light onset (Fig. 1A). We confirmed successful targeting of AC (either A1, AAF or dorsal AC) with histology (Fig. 1B), and by examining the response latency of neuronal units to clicks. 69.2% of recorded units showed an excitatory onset response, and 93.3% of these units (320/343) responded within <20ms (Fig. 1C) attesting to successful targeting of early AC.

Figure 1. Experimental Setup.

Figure 1

A) Experimental setup (left) – Wistar rats were confined to a motorized running wheel operated intermittently (right, Lafayette Instrument), placed inside an acoustic chamber with an ultrasonic speaker for auditory stimulation and video synchronized with continuous EEG/EMG/intracranial electrophysiology. B) Histology of microwire traces targeting the auditory cortex C) Distribution of peak response latencies to click stimuli across all units with an excitatory onset response (n=343) attesting to successful micro-electrode targeting to early auditory cortex. Red line represents the median latency (13.5ms). D) Top: Representative hypnogram (time-course of sleep/wake states, top) along with dynamics of slow wave activity (SWA, EEG power < 4Hz) normalized to session mean in 100s time bins. Bottom: Schematic description of experimental paradigm. Rats were sleep deprived for five hours (zeitgeber time [ZT] 0-5) via intermittent 3s forced running bouts, followed by five hours of recovery sleep opportunity (ZT 5-10), while auditory stimulation was performed continuously throughout the entire experiment with short (~2s) inter-stimulus-intervals, irrespective of wheel movements.

Rats underwent 5h of sleep deprivation (SD) by intermittent rotations of the wheel43 (3s bouts interleaved with 12-18s idle intervals, Fig. 1D gray). Then, they were left to sleep undisturbed for additional 5h as they spontaneously transitioned between NREM sleep, REM sleep, and short epochs of wakefulness (Fig. 1D, black). Throughout this time, we monitored behavior via synchronized video and intermittently presented auditory stimuli. We focused on comparing the first and last thirds (~100min each) of the 5h SD period, referred to throughout the manuscript as “Vigilant” and “Tired” conditions, respectively (Fig. 1D). We verified that intervals categorized as Tired were not significantly contaminated by sleep attempts using extensive inspection of video data, and examination of slow wave activity (SWA, 1-4 Hz). Indeed, SWA during the Tired condition was much more similar to that observed in the Vigilant condition than to subsequent NREM sleep (mean±SD % of session mean: 43±10% in Vigilant and 56±13% in Tired, versus 180±25% in the first third of recovery NREM sleep).

Frequency tuning, spontaneous firing rates, and onset responses are preserved across Vigilant and Tired conditions during sleep deprivation

Based on previous studies on state-dependent auditory processing (Introduction), we hypothesized that certain features of auditory cortical processing such as frequency tuning will be invariant to SD, whereas other features will be modulated by SD and more generally by arousal state. To test this, we first compared neuronal frequency tuning by examining the responses to dynamic random chord stimuli 41. To exclude possible effects of movement and locomotion on auditory processing, we only analyzed trials where the wheel was still and the animal quiescent (‘quiescent wakefulness’ as opposed to ‘active wakefulness’). Fig. 2A shows a representative spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF) of a neuronal cluster during Vigilant and Tired conditions. As can be seen, frequency tuning remains very stable throughout SD. Next, we quantified this stability across the entire dataset (n=198 significantly tuned units out of 496 total) by calculating the tuning width (FWHM, red lines in Fig. 2A) and computing its Modulation Index (MI) across conditions (Fig. 2b, Methods). In line with our hypothesis, we could not reveal a significant change in tuning width across conditions (p=0.529, t197=-0.631, Linear Mixed Effects [LME] Model). Indeed, the mean modulation across conditions was -1.85±2.09%, representing only a 1.85% mean decrease in tuning width during the Tired condition. Next, we went beyond tuning width and examined more generally whether the frequency tuning profile of each neuron is stable across states, representing additional features such as preferred frequency and temporal dynamics of tuned responses. To this end, we calculated the signal correlation between STRF maps in Vigilant and Tired conditions (Fig. 2C middle). We then compared it with signal correlation benchmarks for minimal correlation (Fig. 2C left: different units in different conditions) and maximum correlation (Fig. 2C right: same units, between 1st and 2nd halves of data within the same condition, Methods). We found that the STRF signal correlation between Vigilant and Tired conditions (middle bar, 0.638±0.012) was significantly higher than between different units (left bar, 0.219±0.009, p=4.53 × 10−18, t197=9.57, LME), and virtually as high as the signal correlation within each condition (middle vs. right bar, 0.638±0.012 vs. 0.647±0.012; p=0.16, t197=-1.41, LME). Given a finite number of trials and some inevitable degree noise in the data, STRF profiles across Vigilant and Tired conditions are as similar as they possibly can be. Thus, both tuning width and the signal correlation of STRF profiles were invariant to changes in arousal states during sleep deprivation.

Figure 2. Auditory cortex processing during sleep deprivation.

Figure 2

A) Representative spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF) of a unit in auditory cortex showing preserved frequency tuning across Vigilant and Tired conditions (left and right, respectively). B) Modulation of frequency tuning width (Tired vs. Vigilant conditions) for all tuned units (n=198 out of 496 total) and sessions (n=16). C) Signal correlations of frequency tuning across the entire dataset between different units in the same session (left bar, benchmark for min. correlation), between Vigilant and Tired conditions of the same individual units (middle bar) and between 1st and 2nd halves of trials in the same condition for the same individual units (right bar, benchmark for max. correlation). Note that signal correlations are nearly as high across Vigilant and Tired conditions as they are within the same condition. D) Representative raster and peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) for a unit in response to 2 and 40 clicks/s click trains (left and right, respectively). Gray shading marks the onset response [0-30]ms period. Green shading represents the post-onset [30-80]ms period where firing rate was especially attenuated during the Tired condition. Yellow shading represents the [130-530]ms period where sustained locking to the 40 click/s train was attenuated during the Tired condition. E) Modulation of activity/response features between Tired and Vigilant conditions across units (n=327) and sessions (n=17). Features (left to right) denote: spontaneous firing rate (FR), onset response FR, population synchrony, 40-Hz locking and post onset FR. 2 click/s train were presented in 11 out of 19 sessions (‘auditory paradigm A’, n=199 units). For Panels B, C and E small gray markers represent individual units. large dark gray markers represent mean of all units in an individual session. Each marker shape represents sessions from an individual animal. Markers with/without black edges represent ‘auditory paradigm A’ and ‘auditory paradigm B’ sessions, respectively. Red dots point to the representative unit presented in panels A and D. Dashed vertical line separates features minimally/not significantly affected by condition (spontaneous FR and onset response FR; on left) vs. features significantly that are disrupted in the Tired condition (population synchrony, 40Hz locking, and post-onset FR; on right).

We proceeded to analyze neuronal responses to 500ms click trains at different rates (Fig. 2D, 2, 10, 20 & 30 clicks/s in 11 experimental sessions and 40 clicks/s in 19 experimental sessions). We first quantified onset response magnitude to the 40 clicks/s stimulus, as well as spontaneous (baseline) firing rate preceding stimulus onset across all responsive units (65.3%, 324 of 496 units) in Vigilant and Tired conditions. As can be seen in a representative unit (Fig. 2D), the spontaneous firing rate did not change between conditions. Similarly, the onset response (gray shading, [0-30]ms relative to stimulus onset) was similar in magnitude across conditions. Quantitative analysis across the entire dataset (Fig 2E, two leftmost bars) revealed a slight reduction (-5.1±1.1%) in spontaneous firing during the Tired condition (p=0.0085, t323=-2.65, LME), while onset FR did not exhibit significant modulation (-0.52±1.17%, p= 0.92, t323=0.1, LME). Overall, some aspects of cortical auditory processing, including frequency tuning, spontaneous firing, and onset responses are largely preserved across Vigilant and Tired conditions during sleep deprivation.

Population synchrony, entrainment to fast click-trains, and post-onset silence are strongly modulated by sleep deprivation

Next, we tested the degree to which sleep deprivation affects other features of cortical auditory processing. We predicted that population synchrony would increase in Tired condition given the increased propensity of local neuronal populations to exhibit synchronous OFF-states in SD 18. Quantifying “population coupling”44, a measure of how correlated each unit’s firing is with the firing of the local population, we found a significant increase (17±1.6%) in population synchrony during the Tired condition (Fig. 2E, p=5.2 × 10−10, t323=6.41, LME). We also predicted that entrainment to fast click trains (40 clicks/s) might be especially sensitive to sleep deprivation36,45,46. As can be seen in a representative example (Fig. 2D, orange shading), the magnitude of sustained locking to the click train decreased during the Tired condition. A quantitative analysis across the entire dataset revealed a significant decrease of 17.7±1.5% in 40Hz Locking (Fig. 2E orange bar, p=1.4×10−6, t323=-4.92, LME).

When presenting click trains at slower rates (2 & 10 clicks/s, n=11 sessions), we observed that the onset response ([0,30]ms) was followed by a post-onset period ([30,80]ms) exhibiting robust firing attenuation in the Tired condition (Fig. 2D left, green shading). Indeed, post-onset firing was significantly attenuated in the Tired condition compared to the Vigilant condition (34.6±1.9%, Fig. 2E green bar, p=4.6×10−14, t195=-8.14, LME). Post-onset firing reduction emerged as a particularly state-sensitive aspect of the cortical auditory response, showing significantly stronger modulation than population synchrony and 40Hz locking (p≤0.0151, df=195, LME). Analysis of variance among the distinct features of cortical auditory processing confirmed that they are differentially modulated by SD (p=2.8×10−4, n=7 animals, Friedman test). Pair-wise comparisons revealed that while spontaneous firing rates and onset responses were largely preserved, population synchrony, 40-Hz click train locking, and post-onset firing were modulated significantly more strongly than the former two features (p≤0.0025, df=323 or 195, LME). In addition, an hour-by-hour analysis revealed that auditory processing features that were sensitive to SD exhibited gradually accumulating changes, corresponding to gradually accumulating sleep pressure (Supp. Fig. 1).

Next, we ruled out the contribution of factors unrelated to the buildup of sleep pressure that could change between Vigilant and Tired conditions and potentially confound the observed differences in auditory processing. Such factors include changes in animal location (and corresponding stimulus intensity), degree of movement and animal locomotion, and stress. First, sound intensity measurements at different positions inside the wheel apparatus (Methods) showed minimal variance (0.7±0.3dB, mean absolute deviation±STD, max difference of 2.4 dB), indicating that changes in auditory responses are not simply driven by variations in sound levels. Second, we determined the peri-trial movement and animal pose in each trial using DeepLabCut47,48(Supp. Fig. 2). We observed qualitatively similar, statistically significant, differences between Vigilant and Tired conditions even when removing trials associated with movements, and also when counter-balancing trials for animal pose (Supp. Fig. 2C). Third, we addressed the potential contribution of corticosterone (CORT) stress responses induced by SD. To this end, we assessed plasma CORT levels in a separate cohort of animals (n=10), using a within-animal design across Vigilant and Tired conditions, as well as in a negative control condition (changes in circadian factors without SD) and a positive control condition (wet cage stress paradigm)49,50. The results of two-way repeated-measures ANOVA (Supp. Fig. 3) revealed no main effects for sleep deprivation (p=0.37, F(1,9)=0.9), or time of day (p=0.21, F(1,9)=1.8), and no significant interaction (p=0.98, F(1,9)<0.01). By contrast, the wet cage paradigm led to a significant 10-fold increase in plasma CORT levels (p<0.0001, Tukey’s multiple comparisons test, Supp. Fig. 3). Thus, it seems that the effects of SD on auditory processing are not mediated by alterations in CORT stress responses.

The effects of sleep deprivation on cortical auditory processing mimic those of NREM sleep

Previous studies have shown that during Tired conditions upon SD, features of NREM sleep activity (e.g. slow/theta activities and OFF-states) ‘invade’ the ongoing activity of cortical circuits16,18,19. We wondered if the same is true for stimulus-driven activity, and whether it can already be observed in early sensory cortex. To test this, we compared neural activity and auditory responses during the Vigilant condition with those during the 5h recovery sleep period (Fig. 3), when rats spent 48±7.5% of time in NREM sleep, 22±7.6% of time in wakefulness, and 6.5±4.2% of time in REM sleep (mean±SD, additional intervals in transition or undetermined states, not analyzed further). We hypothesized that features showing similarity across Vigilant and Tired conditions will also be invariant to full-fledged NREM sleep, whereas changes observed during SD will be accentuated in recovery sleep data. Indeed, frequency tuning, spontaneous firing, and onset responses were similar across Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions. Fig. 3A shows an example STRF during Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions. As observed during SD, full-fledged NREM sleep did not alter neuronal frequency tuning. Across the entire dataset, mean frequency tuning width did not significantly change (-1.18±1.25%, p=0.642, t197=-0.47, LME), and the STRF profile signal correlation (Fig. 3D) between Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions (middle bar) was nearly as high as the signal correlation within each condition (right bar). Although the difference in signal correlation was highly significant statistically (p=3.5×10−11, t197=-7.02, LME), its magnitude was moderate: signal correlation between Vigilant and NREM sleep was 87.4% of the mean signal correlation within each condition (0.589 vs. 0.674).

Figure 3. Auditory cortex processing during recovery NREM sleep vs. vigilant wakefulness.

Figure 3

Same as Fig. 2 but comparing recovery NREM sleep to the Vigilant condition. A) Representative spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF) of a unit in auditory cortex showing preserved frequency tuning across Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions (left and right, respectively). B) Modulation of frequency tuning width (NREM sleep vs. Vigilant conditions) for all units (n=200) and sessions (n=16). C) Signal correlations of frequency tuning across the entire dataset between different units in the same session (left bar), between Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions of the same individual units (middle bar) and between 1st and 2nd halves of trials in the same condition for the same individual units (right bar). Note that signal correlations are nearly as high across Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions as they are within the same condition. D) Representative raster and peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) for a unit in response to 2 and 40 clicks/s click trains (left and right, respectively). Gray shading marks the onset response [0-30]ms period. Green shading represents the post-onset [30-80]ms period where firing rate was especially attenuated during the Tired condition. Yellow shading represents the [130-530]ms period where sustained locking to the 40 click/s train was attenuated during the NREM sleep condition. E) Modulation of activity/response features between NREM sleep and Vigilant conditions across units (n=327) and sessions (n=17). Features (left to right) denote: spontaneous firing rate (FR), onset response FR, population synchrony, 40-Hz locking and post onset FR. 2 click/s train were presented in 11 out of 19 sessions (‘auditory paradigm A’, green bar, n=199 units, 10 session). For Panels B,C,E, small gray markers represent individual units. large dark gray markers represent mean of all units in an individual session. Each marker shape represents sessions from an individual animal. Markers with/without black edges represent ‘auditory paradigm A’ and ‘auditory paradigm B’ sessions, respectively. Red dots point to the representative unit presented in panels A and D. Dashed vertical line separates features minimally/not significantly affected by condition (spontaneous FR and onset response FR; on left) vs. features that are significantly disrupted in the NREM sleep condition (population synchrony, 40Hz locking, and post-onset FR; on right).

Fig. 3D shows that for this representative unit, spontaneous firing and onset responses were also unchanged during NREM sleep, contrasting with strong modulation of post-onset firing and 40Hz-locking (green and yellow shading, respectively). Analysis of the entire dataset confirmed a modest attenuation of spontaneous firing and onset responses during recovery NREM sleep (Fig 3E left, 5.78±1.68% and 7.45±1.38%, respectively), that was statistically significant only for onset responses (p=1.1×10−6, t323=-4.98 for onset response and p=0.16, t323=-1.39 for spontaneous FR, LME). In sharp contrast, these modest changes were overshadowed by strong modulations of population synchrony (48.3±1.3%), 40 Hz Locking (47.4±1.73%) and post-onset firing (55.7±2.19%) during NREM sleep (Fig. 3E right, p≤1.2 × 10−27). As was the case for SD, the differential modulation of specific features of cortical auditory processing by NREM sleep was highly significant (p=1.7×10−4, n=7 animals, Friedman test) where population synchrony, 40Hz-locking, and post-onset firing were significantly more modulated than spontaneous firing and onset responses (p≤2.8×10−16 for all pair-wise comparisons, df=323 or 195, LME). Overall, the same aspects of cortical auditory processing that showed maximal modulation during SD (population synchrony, 40Hz Locking, post onset FR) were maximally modulated during recovery NREM sleep.

Since recording sessions typically lasted ~10 hours, we sought to verify that changes in recording quality and challenges in continuously tracking units in a stable manner did not bias our results (Supp. Fig. 4). We addressed this using two complementary approaches. First, we analyzed each unit’s spike waveform variance throughout the session and quantified its ‘waveform stability’ (Methods). Restricting analysis of auditory responses to a subset (63%) of the most stable units yielded qualitatively similar, statistically significant, differences between Tired, NREM sleep and Vigilant conditions (Supp. Fig. 4). Second, we compared trials in NREM sleep to interleaved trials of quiescent wakefulness that occurred during the recovery sleep period in the last few hours (Supp. Fig. 5). Again, we observed qualitatively similar, statistically significant, differences. These tests support the notion that it is the internal vigilance state (NREM sleep / Tired / Vigilant) that is driving the observed changes in auditory processing rather than technical issues related to long recording sessions.

Sleep deprivation and NREM sleep entail sensory adaptation at lower frequencies

To better understand how Tired and NREM sleep states disrupt locking to click trains, we presented click rates at various rates (2, 10, 20, 30 and 40 clicks/s, n=11 sessions). As can be seen in a representative response (Fig. 4A), sustained locking to slower click trains (2 and 10 clicks/s, yellow shading) was stable during the Tired and NREM sleep conditions relative to Vigilant. In contrast, locking to faster click trains (≥20 clicks/s) showed strong attenuation. We thus quantified the modulation in response locking across the entire dataset during SD (Tired vs. Vigilant, 194 units, Fig 4B) and during NREM sleep (NREM vs. Vigilant, Fig 4C). Locking to different click rates was differentially modulated by SD (Fig. 4B, p=9.7×10-4, n=6 animals, Friedman test). Pairwise comparisons revealed that locking to faster click-trains (20, 30 & 40 clicks/s) was significantly more attenuated than to slower click trains (2 & 10 Clicks/s), with an average attenuation of 18.4% vs. 4.4%, respectively (for all comparisons p≤2.6×10-4, df=193, LME, mean MI: -3.05±1.46%, -5.71±1.36%, -17.2±1.75%, -19.1±1.67% and -18.9±1.87%, for 2, 10, 20, 30 and 40 clicks/s, respectively). NREM sleep showed qualitatively similar and stronger effects (Fig. 4C, p=0.0036, n=6 animals, Friedman test, mean MI: -11.6±1.93%, -31.2±1.99%, -43.7±1.84%, -50.9±1.98% and - 49±2.05%, for 2, 10, 20, 30 and 40 clicks/s, respectively). Pairwise comparisons revealed a gradual modulation during NREM sleep depending on click-train rate (-11.6% for 2 click/s versus -31.2% for 10 clicks/s, and even stronger attenuations for 20, 30 & 40 clicks/s, p≤0.0057, df=193, for all comparisons, LME). To capture how different arousal states affect the entire sensory adaptation curve, we fitted a sigmoid function to describe how response attenuation changes with increasing click rate (Methods). Fig 4D shows this fit for the same unit example shown in Fig. 4A. We then estimated the “adapted rate”, i.e. the click rate for which the response is attenuated to 25% of its maximum (Methods), for each arousal condition separately (crosses in Fig 4D). For the example unit shown, the estimated adapted rate during the Vigilant condition was 45.6 clicks/s, decreased to 23 clicks/s during the Tired condition, and decreased further to 16.6 clicks/s during NREM sleep. A quantitative analysis across the entire dataset (Fig. 4E) revealed that SD decreased the adapted rate by 15.6±1.84% (Fig. 4E left, p=1.1×10-7, t146=-5.58, LME, Vigilant: 32.9 vs. Tired: 26.8 clicks/s). An even stronger decrease of 36.3±1.87% was observed in NREM sleep (Fig. 4E right, p=3.9×10-36, t146=-16.9, LME, Vigilant: 32.9 vs. NREM sleep: 19.7 clicks/s). Overall, Tired and NREM sleep low-arousal states shift the sensory adaptation gain curve to lower frequencies.

Figure 4. Recovery NREM sleep and sleep deprivation both entail a shift in sensory adaptation.

Figure 4

A) Representative unit raster and PSTH responses to 2, 10, 20, 30 and 40 clicks/s responses. Note that locking to click trains is progressively more disrupted in Tired/NREM sleep conditions with increasing click train rate. B) Modulation of locking to different click rates (Tired vs. Vigilant) for all units (n=197) and sessions (n=10). Locking to fast click trains (≥20 clicks/s) is significantly attenuated during sleep deprivation (‘Tired’). C) Same as B but comparing recovery NREM sleep to the Vigilant condition, showing increasingly stronger attenuation for faster click trains. D) Normalized locked responses in a representative unit (y-axis) as a function of click rate (x-axis) separately for Vigilant (cyan), Tired (blue), and recovery NREM sleep (green) conditions. Circles represent the observed locked response to each click rate in each condition. Thick traces connecting the circles represent the best sigmoid fit. Cross represents the estimated ‘adapted click-rate’, i.e. the click rate for which the normalized response would be 25% of maximum. E) Left: scatter plot of the ‘adapted click-rate’ for all units and sessions, comparing Vigilant (y-axis) with Tired conditions (y-axis); Right: same when comparing Vigilant (y-axis) with recovery NREM sleep (x-axis). Yellow cross represents mean±SEM across all units (n=150). F) observed click rate for which units demonstrate maximal attenuation between Vigilant and NREM sleep conditions (x-axis, Methods) vs. the estimated ‘adapted click-rate’ during the Vigilant condition (y-axis). Note that units with lower ‘adapted click-rate’ during wakefulness also show lower attenuation rates when comparing NREM sleep vs. Vigilant. For Panels B,C,E,F: small gray markers represent individual units. Large dark gray markers represent mean of all units in an individual session. Red dots point to the representative unit presented in panels A and D.

Could it be that some neurons are strongly adapted to begin with, and these are the neurons who are most sensitive to changes in state? To examine this, we tested whether neurons that show a low adapted rate during the Vigilant condition (e.g. weak locking already for 10 click/s) may correspondingly show a strong attenuation at lower frequencies during NREM sleep (compared to the Vigilant condition). We calculated for each neuron its estimated ‘adapted rate’ during the Vigilant condition, and compared it to the click rate showing maximal attenuation during NREM sleep (Fig. 4F, Methods). For example, the representative unit in Fig. 4A,D shows a close-to-maximal attenuation during NREM sleep already at 20 clicks/s (red points in Fig. 4C), while its estimated ‘adapted rate’ during the Vigilant condition was 45.6 clicks/s (light blue cross in Fig. 4D). Analysis across the entire dataset confirmed the significant correlation between these two measures (Fig. 4F, p=6.2×10-5, rho=0.324, for n=147 units, Spearman Correlation). Such correlation was not significant when comparing Vigilant and Tired conditions (p=0.46, rho=0.062), possibly due to the weaker modulation observed in SD. Thus, we found that for a given neuron, the attenuation during NREM sleep is dictated by the sensory adaptation curve during vigilance, such that neurons showing significantly adapted response at lower click rates are also attenuated during NREM at lower click rates. Finally, linear modeling revealed that reduced locking to fast click trains cannot be simply explained by post-onset reduction in firing (Supp. Fig 6).

Stimulus-induced silent intervals are more sensitive to sleep deprivation than spontaneous silences

The most sensitive measure of cortical auditory processing in low-arousal states was a reduction or complete cessation of firing following the onset response in Tired and NREM sleep conditions (Fig. 5A Left, [30,80]ms post click, green shading). Given that such stimulus-induced silence was reminiscent of an ‘OFF-state’ observed in ongoing activity of local neuronal populations during NREM sleep and SD18, we examined if it likewise represents a network-wide event or, alternatively, simply reflects a refractory-like period in spiking of individual neurons exposed by the onset response to the auditory stimulus. To test this, we compared each auditory trial (with its stimulus-induced onset response and post-onset silence, Fig 5A, left) with a matched interval of ongoing activity containing similar spiking bursts (Fig. 5A, right). We found that post-onset FR reduction was only apparent following auditory stimulation and onset responses but not present in spontaneous firing (Fig. 5A,B, green shading): baseline-normalized post onset FR was gradually reduced from 0.98±0.039 during the Vigilant condition to 0.63±0.033 during the Tired condition, and even further to 0.38±0.023 during NREM sleep (Fig. 5C, p<3 × 10−7 for all pair-wise comparisons, df=195, LME). By contrast, FR following spontaneous bursts only revealed marginal changes across conditions: Vigilant: 1.08±0.016, Tired:1.01±0.016, NREM: 0.98±0.02 (p=0.032 for comparing Vigilant and Tired conditions, p>0.05 for all other comparisons, df=195, LME). The attenuation in click-induced post-onset FR during the Tired condition (34.2±1.95% relative to vigilance) was significantly larger than that following spontaneous-bursts (5.2±1.7%, p=1.5 × 10−18, t195=9.75, LME), as was also true for NREM sleep (p=1.5 × 10−16, t195=9.04, LME). Thus, post-onset suppression isn’t simply a property of individual neurons that reduce firing after vigorous activity, but represents a network event induced by the stimulus.

Figure 5. Stimulus-induced silent intervals are especially sensitive to sleep deprivation.

Figure 5

A) Representative unit raster and PSTH response to a click across Vigilant, Tired and NREM sleep conditions (left) and trial-by-trial matched, equally strong, spontaneous bursts (matching the [0,30]ms click onset response) of the same unit (right). Note that there is no post-onset FR reduction following spontaneous bursts. Green shading represents the post-onset [30,80]ms period. B) mean normalized PSTH of all units (n=195) for the stimulus(click)-induced response (left), and matched spontaneous bursts (right) across Vigilant, Tired and NREM sleep conditions. C) Post-onset normalized FR across Vigilant, Tired and NREM sleep conditions for the stimulus-induced response (left) and the matched spontaneous bursts (right) for all units (n=195) and sessions (n=10). D) Representative unit raster and PSTH response to 2 clicks/s train across Vigilant, Tired and NREM sleep conditions. Silent intervals (≥50ms firing silence) just preceding ([-50,0]ms) or immediately following ([30,80]ms) stimulus onset are marked in orange and green, respectively. Note that spontaneous silent intervals (orange) are prevalent in NREM sleep but rare during the Tired condition (as in Vigilant), whereas stimulus-induced silent intervals (green) strongly increase in the Tired condition. E) Increase in silent intervals probability (relative to Poisson process with the same spontaneous firing rate) across Vigilant, Tired and NREM sleep conditions, separately for spontaneous (left) and stimulus-induced (right) silent intervals for all electrodes (n=126) and sessions (n=10). F) Modulation of the probability of spontaneous and stimulus-induced silent intervals across Tired vs. Vigilant conditions for all electrodes (n=126) and sessions (n=10). Stimulus-induced silent intervals show a larger and more reliable change upon sleep deprivation (comparing Tired and Vigilant conditions) relative to spontaneous intervals. Bars represent mean across all units/channels. Small gray markers represent individual units/channels. Large dark gray markers represent mean of all units/channels in an individual session. Red dots point to the representative unit presented in panels A and D.

Next, we complemented the analysis of graded firing rate reductions with a binary approach of detecting OFF periods – intervals of neuronal silence ≥50ms, typically observed in ongoing sleep activity and in SD. Both spontaneous and stimulus-induced silent intervals (presumably OFF-states) were rare during the Vigilant condition but more frequent during NREM sleep (Fig. 5D). During the Tired condition (wakefulness after several hours of SD), stimulus-induced silent intervals were very frequent while spontaneous silent intervals continued to be rare. We analyzed the probability of spontaneous silent intervals relative to a random Poisson process (Fig. 5E, Methods) across the entire dataset, and found a graded modulation by arousal state (Vigilant: 4.61±0.48%, Tired: 5.67±0.38%, NREM sleep: 8.87±0.33%, p=0.0057, n=6 animals, Friedman test). Pair-wise comparisons revealed that the probability in NREM sleep was significantly greater than other conditions (p≤9.7 × 10− 6, df=126, LME, compared to Vigilant and Tired conditions), while the increase from Vigilant to the Tired condition exhibited a non-significant trend (p=0.0501, t125=-1.98, LME). By contrast to spontaneous silent intervals, the probability of stimulus-induced silent intervals was higher and more strongly modulated by condition (Vigilant: 7.14±1.38%p, Tired: 25.4±1.63%, NREM sleep = 42±1.6%, p=0.0025, n=6 animals, Friedman test, Fig. 5B right, p≤4.1×10-11, df=125, for all pairwise comparisons, LME). In the Vigilant condition, the probability of stimulus-induced silent intervals was not significantly different than that of spontaneous silent intervals (p=0.236, t125=1.19, LME, Spontaneous: 4.61±0.48% vs. Induced: 7.14±1.38%) but this difference was highly significant in the Tired and NREM sleep conditions (p≤9×10-8, df=125, LME, for all comparisons, Spontaneous: 5.67±0.38%, 8.87±0.33%, vs. Induced: 25.4±1.63%, 42±1.6% for Tired and NREM sleep, respectively). Indeed, the mean modulation index comparing silent interval probability in Tired vs. Vigilant conditions (Fig. 5F) increased significantly from 10.8±5.55% for spontaneous silent intervals to 51.2±2.5% for stimulus-induced silent intervals (p=5.4 × 10−5, t125=4.18, LME). Overall, these results establish that stimulus-induced silent intervals reveal a hidden facet of neural processing during SD that goes beyond what is observed in spontaneous activity.

Auditory processing during REM sleep resembles the Vigilant condition, unlike NREM sleep

REM sleep is a unique (‘paradoxical’) behavioral state that is characterized both by disengagement from the environment co-occurring with desynchronized cortical activity and often accompanied by vivid dreams51. Therefore, REM offers a unique lens through which to examine the changes in cortical auditory processing, potentially revealing which aspects are similar to NREM sleep (likely reflecting a general feature of sleep and sensory disengagement) and which aspects are similar to the Vigilant condition (likely reflecting a general feature of desynchronized cortical states).

We first observed that frequency tuning was stable during REM sleep (Fig. 6A). Across the entire dataset, tuning width was reduced by an average of 15% (Fig. 6B, p=3.3×10-4, t120=-3.7, LME), while the signal correlation between the Vigilant and REM sleep conditions (Fig. 6C middle bar, 0.547±0.014) was nearly as high (89.7%) as the maximal benchmark within each condition (Fig 6C, 0.609±0.014, p=5.5×10-8, t120=-5.8, LME). Next, examining different aspects of the neuronal activity and auditory response revealed that REM sleep exhibits a very similar profile to the Vigilant condition (Fig. 6D,E). Unlike NREM sleep, REM sleep was associated with high post-onset firing and strong locking to the 40 click/s train, as in the Vigilant condition. A quantitative analysis across the entire dataset revealed modest average difference between REM sleep and the Vigilant condition (all mean MI<21%, Fig. 6E). Moreover, in measures such as spontaneous firing and 40Hz-locking, REM sleep was even significantly higher than the Vigilant condition (Fig. 6E, all p<4.3×10-8, df=322, LME). Conversely, when contrasting REM sleep with NREM sleep, strong and reliable differences emerged (Fig. 6F). As was the case when comparing Vigilant condition with NREM sleep, different aspects of the neuronal activity and auditory response were differentially modulated by state (Fig. 6F, p=4.1 × 10−4, df=7 animals, Friedman test). Again, the onset response was minimally affected by the state (MI: 10.3±1.32%, p=1.9×10-13, t323=7.68, LME). Spontaneous firing increased by 26.8±1.4% during REM sleep compared to NREM sleep (p=2.7×10-21, t323=10.2, LME). Even larger changes were observed when comparing population synchrony (MI: - 37.5±1.52%, p=1.04×10-16, t323=-8.77, LME), 40-Hz locking (MI: 61.7±1.35%, p=2.6×10-91, t323=28.8, LME) and post onset firing (MI: 64.4±1.95%, p=1.5×10-11, t195=7.17, LME). The ‘adapted rate’ (Fig. 6G) during REM sleep was higher than during the Vigilant condition, and increased on average from 31.8 to 38.8 clicks/s (Fig. 6H left, p=3.4×10-7, t135=5.37, LME). Conversely, robust differences in the adapted rate emerged when comparing REM sleep (38.8 clicks/s) to NREM sleep (19.5 clicks/s; Fig. 6H right, p=7.1×10-46, t135=21.7, LME). Altogether, cortical auditory processing during REM sleep is dramatically different from that in NREM sleep, showing a profile similar to that observed during the Vigilant condition (and in some aspects exhibiting even stronger activity).

Figure 6. Auditory processing in REM sleep resembles wakefulness rather than NREM sleep.

Figure 6

A) Representative spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF) of an auditory cortex unit showing preserved tuning during the Vigilant and REM sleep conditions (left and right, respectively). B) Modulation of frequency tuning width (REM sleep vs. Vigilant conditions) for all units (n=122) and sessions (n=11). C) Signal correlations of frequency tuning across the entire dataset between different units in the same session (left bar), between Vigilant and REM-sleep conditions of the same individual units (middle bar) and between 1st and 2nd halves of trials in the same condition for the same individual units (right bar). Note that signal correlations are nearly as high across Vigilant and REM-sleep conditions as they are within the same condition. D) An example unit raster and peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) for 2 and 40 clicks/s click trains (left and right, respectively). Green shading represents the post-onset [30,80]ms period and yellow shading represents the [130,530]ms period with sustained locking to the 40 click/s train. Note that in both these intervals, neuronal activity was similar in Vigilant and REM sleep, unlike the attenuation observed in NREM sleep. E) Modulation of spontaneous FR, onset response FR, population synchrony, 40-Hz locking and post onset FR during REM sleep relative to the Vigilant condition for all units (n=327/198) and sessions (n=17/10). 2 click/s train were only presented in 11 sessions (‘auditory paradigm A’). Most auditory processing features were comparable or enhanced in REM sleep compared with the Vigilant condition. Dashed vertical line separates features minimally/not significantly affected by NREM sleep/Tired as in previous figures, for reference. F) same as E but comparing REM sleep to NREM sleep. G) Normalized locked responses in a representative unit (y-axis) as a function of click rate (x-axis) separately for Vigilant (cyan), NREM sleep (green), and REM sleep (pink). Circles represent the observed locked response to each click rate in each condition. Thick traces represent the best sigmoid fit. Cross represents the estimated ‘adapted click-rate’, i.e. the click rate for which the normalized response would be 25% of maximum. H) Left: scatter plot of the ‘adapted click-rate’ for all units (n=138) and sessions (n=10 in 6 animals), comparing REM sleep (y-axis) with Vigilant conditions (x-axis); Right: same when comparing REM sleep (y-axis) with recovery NREM sleep (x-axis). Yellow cross represents mean±SEM across all units. For Panels B, C, E, F and H: small gray markers represent individual units. Large dark gray markers represent mean of all units in an individual session. Each marker shape represents sessions from an individual animal. Markers with/without black edges represent ‘auditory paradigm A’ and ‘auditory paradigm B’ sessions, respectively. Red dots point to the representative unit presented in panels A and D.

Discussion

The present results reveal how SD affects activity and stimulus-evoked responses in the auditory cortex. We find that some aspects of cortical auditory processing – including frequency tuning, spontaneous firing, and onset responses – are preserved across Vigilant and Tired conditions and are largely invariant to SD. By contrast, population synchrony, entrainment to fast click-trains, and post-onset silence are strongly modulated by SD (Fig. 2). The effects of SD on cortical auditory processing mimic those of NREM sleep, when similar effects manifest with stronger intensity (Fig. 3). Both SD and NREM sleep entail sensory adaptation at lower frequencies, suggesting that low-arousal states disrupt cortical processing of fast inputs (Fig. 4). We also find that stimulus-induced neuronal silent intervals are more sensitive to SD than are spontaneous silent intervals (‘OFF-states’, Fig. 5), a result suggesting that perturbation reveals a hidden state of neuronal bi-stability not easily observed in spontaneous activity52,53. Auditory processing during REM sleep (Fig. 6) resembles that in vigilant wakefulness (unlike NREM sleep) and highlights the key role of cortical desynchronization in auditory processing. Extensive control analyses and experiments ruled out changes in locomotion, stress, head/body position, and unit stability across long recordings as significant confounding factors, indicating that changes in auditory processing are driven by internal changes in vigilance state (Supp. Figs. 2-5). Our results extend previous research showing that SD and drowsiness influences sensory processing10,19,5456 by showing that SD-induced changes already occur at primary cortices, earlier along the ascending cortical hierarchy than reported so far.

Are primary cortices robustly modulated, or largely invariant, to brain states and arousal? On one hand, the effects of states such as sleep and anesthesia are typically more modest in primary cortex than in high-order regions3537,46,5762. Similarly, the effects of neuromodulation, attention, and consciousness are more prevalent in high-order regions compared to early sensory cortex26,63,64. On the other hand, many studies report robust changes in early sensory cortex processing associated with arousal and task parameters23,27,7073,28,29,42,6569. Our results support a model in which specific features of the auditory response undergo increasing state-dependent deterioration along the sensory hierarchy. At the earliest processing stages - in peripheral sensory organs, thalamus, and primary cortices - response degradation gradually accumulates but on the whole is often modest and difficult to detect30,70,74. Degradation builds up further along the cortical hierarchy, possibly due to higher sensitivity of inter-cortical signal transmission to behavioral states. Thus, in high-order regions, responses most correlated with perception often exhibit a sharper contrast between states. By focusing on responses in sensory cortex during SD, we were able to reveal state-dependent changes in specific features of neuronal response already at early auditory cortex.

Directly comparing different features (‘motifs’) of AC processing reveals which neural signatures are most sensitive to low-arousal. We find that SD and sleep only weakly affect neuronal tuning, spontaneous firing, and onset responses, compared with other aspects of auditory processing. The observation that frequency tuning is relatively invariant to SD and sleep is in line with the fact that it was traditionally studied successfully in anesthetized animals75. However, while some studies report invariant tuning across states29,73, others report arousal-induced modulations in tuning28,76. Naturally, differences between separate studies can reflect changes in magnitude/type of arousal manipulation (e.g. sleep vs. anesthesia), species, cortical layer, or recorded cell types. The strength of the current study is that by comparing different motifs of auditory processing in the same neurons and experiments, our results provide important context in showing that frequency tuning is one of the most arousal-invariant feature of AC processing compared with other features we measured. We also find that SD and sleep only modestly affect baseline firing rates and onset response magnitudes in AC, in general agreement with previous reports showing modest changes during sleep34,35,77. While previous rodent studies reported increased spiking activity upon prolonged wakefulness and sleep deprivation78,79, we do not observe such increases, possibly due to our focus on early sensory cortex or due to differences in the sleep-deprivation method 79.

By contrast to invariant features, population synchrony robustly increases upon SD (and even more so in NREM sleep), likely reducing the capacity of cortical circuits to represent information and support perception, consciousness, and behavior65,80. Indeed, increased synchrony in neuronal populations at low frequencies (<20Hz) represents a core feature of low arousal states such as SD & sleep, spanning multiple levels from individual neurons, through circuits, to non-invasive global EEG recordings16,18,8183.

Our results extend previous work showing that reduced entrainment to fast inputs is a hallmark of unconscious low-arousal states. During deep anesthesia, responses to high-frequency stimuli are attenuated in cat visual cortex84 and in rodent somatosensory85 and auditory cortex66. In natural sleep and light propofol anesthesia, auditory cortex of both rodents and humans reveals reduced responses to 40Hz click-trains36,38,62, as has been originally observed with scalp EEG86,87. Here, we extend these results to show that already during wakefulness, SD-induced Tired conditions entail sensory adaptation at significantly lower frequencies, acting like a low-pass filter that diminishes rapid transmission of information across brain regions. The underlying mechanism may involve changes in short term synaptic plasticity, as the synaptic proteome was recently shown to be modulated by SD88.

The most sensitive feature of auditory processing modulated by SD and NREM sleep is stimulus-induced neuronal silence, which may reveal an underlying neuronal bi-stability in low-arousal states89. Such bistability may not allow neurons in low-arousal states to maintain sustained firing in response to a stimulus, and its occurrence in some cortical regions may underlie the behavioral inability to successfully maintain sustained attention18. While we cannot definitively demonstrate that stimulus-induced silent intervals reflect genuine membrane potential bi-stability (Up and Down states), we believe our results agree with that interpretation. For one, the fact that silent intervals don’t appear after vigorous spontaneous spiking (Fig. 5A-C), strengthens the notion that stimulus-induced silent intervals indeed reflect a network level phenomenon, not just individual neurons showing suppressed FR after vigorous spiking. Importantly, stimulus-induced activity reveals a hidden facet of neuronal activity during SD (propensity for silent intervals) that is not readily observed in spontaneous activity90. Our results join previous work with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in humans89, as well as electrical intracortical stimulation in rodents53,90, both showing that perturbation can reveal the latent state of cortical activity and trigger a slow wave at any time during NREM sleep, even when the ongoing EEG shows little spontaneous slow wave activity. Indeed, quantifying the brain’s response to perturbation (e.g. with TMS-EEG) offers a more sensitive approach to detect bi-stability that accompanies disorders of consciousness and brain-injured patients91. Our results suggest that examining the response to sensory stimuli might be a particularly effective way to assess the level of drowsiness and sleep deprivation (for example in the context of human EEG during driving).

Our results extend the notion that NREM-sleep-related activities invade the activity of the waking brain after SD. This has been established for spontaneous EEG activity (‘EEG slowing’)16,19,81 and for ongoing neuronal activity and OFF states18. Here we show that SD mimics NREM sleep also in how it affects sensory processing, and specifically in early sensory cortex.

In contrast to NREM sleep, REM sleep resembles vigilant wakefulness for all features of cortical auditory activity and stimulus-evoked responses. REM sleep serves as a unique test-case to determine which elements of neuronal activity and sensory responses reflect disconnection from environmental sensory stimuli vs. elements that reflect the ability of the brain to generate conscious experience (whether externally- or internally-generated). On one hand, REM sleep is similar to NREM sleep in that both entail disconnection from the external world; on the other hand, REM sleep is similar to vigilant wakefulness in that during both states the brain generates conscious experience. Thus, the result that AC activity in REM sleep resembles vigilant wakefulness suggests that the changes in cortical auditory processing observed in SD and NREM sleep may reflect features of an unconscious brain state, and that sensory disconnection can co-occur with desynchronized wake-like processing in AC. These results point to a key role for cholinergic modulation in AC processing, given that high acetylcholine levels drive cortical desynchronization similarly across REM sleep and wakefulness51. Future studies could directly study whether cholinergic modulation of auditory pathways is necessary and sufficient to support specific features of auditory processing as observed in vigilant wakefulness.

Some limitations of the study should be explicitly acknowledged. First, our procedure for implanting microwire arrays did not enable us to obtain reliable information about the cortical layer and type of recorded neurons. Thus, our sample could be biased and best capture specific subpopulations such as large pyramidal cells with higher baseline firing rates that register more readily in extracellular recordings. Second, the state-dependent changes observed in early auditory cortex could possibly be inherited from earlier regions such as the auditory thalamus, not recorded here. Third, our data from NREM and REM sleep reflects recovery sleep, likely associated with deeper sleep and stronger attenuation than usual. Still, the fact that robust changes in auditory processing were observed during SD while the animal was awake, and no such changes were observed during REM sleep, partly alleviates that concern. Fourth, as no behavioral task was included in the study it remains to be seen if the reported changes in auditory processing are associated with the deterioration in behavioral performance (‘lapses’) typical of SD. Future studies could examine if moment-to-moment variability in behavioral performance is associated with moment-to-moment changes in sensory processing. another important aspect to address is the possibility that SD periods were significantly contaminated by brief sleep episodes, which in turn may have driven the changes in auditory processing seen in the Tired condition. We don’t believe this is the case, since video monitoring did not reveal periods of sleep during SD. In addition, EEG slow wave power during the Tired condition was largely comparable to the Vigilant condition, but very different from that during NREM sleep. Finally, we acknowledge that our stimulus set did not allow to examine some important aspects of auditory processing such as randomness and surprise, as well as behavioral relevance. Future studies should examine the state-sensitivity of such aspects in the context of SD, sleep and different arousal states.

In conclusion, we examined the effects of SD and recovery sleep on different aspects of auditory cortex processing and found that SD already affects neural processing in early sensory cortex. We find that SD robustly modulated some aspects of auditory processing (population synchrony, entrainment to fast inputs, and stimulus-induced silent intervals) while other aspects remained stable (neuronal tuning, spontaneous firing and onset responses). Stimulus-induced activity reveals a hidden aspect of neuronal bi-stability that is not observed in spontaneous activity. This is important both conceptually and for practical/clinical applications, as it offers new ways to monitor sleepiness with greater sensitivity. Finally, changes in auditory processing during SD are qualitatively similar to those observed during NREM sleep but not REM sleep, suggesting that NREM-sleep-like processes specifically are invading activity of the waking brain in SD and disrupt behavior.

STAR Methods

Animals

Experiments were performed in 19 adult male Wistar rats (300-350g, >12 weeks old, n=7 for electrophysiological experiments and n=12 for corticosterone measures) housed in transparent Perspex cages with food and water available ad libitum. Ambient temperature was kept between 20°-24° Celsius and a 12:12 hours light/dark cycle was maintained with light onset at 10:00 AM. All experimental procedures, including animal handling, sleep deprivation and surgery, followed the National Institutes of Health’s Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Tel Aviv University.

Surgery and electrode implantation

Electrophysiological measures were performed in 7 rats. Prior to surgery, microwire arrays were coated with a thin layer of DiI fluorescent dye (DiIC18, Invitrogen) under microscopic control to facilitate subsequent localization. Surgery was performed as previously described35. First, induction of general anesthesia was achieved using isoflurane (4%). Animals were then placed in a stereotactic frame (David Kopf Instruments) and maintained for the rest of the surgery under anesthesia (isoflurane, 1.5-2%) and 37°C body temperature (closed-loop heating pad system, Harvard Apparatus). Animals were administered antibiotics (Cefazolin, 20 mg/kg i.m.), analgesia (Carpofen, 5 mg/kg i.p.) and dexamethasone (0.5 mg/kg, i.p.). Their scalp was shaved and liquid gel (Viscotears) was applied to protect the eyes. lignocaine (7 mg/kg) was infused subcutaneously before incision and then the skull was exposed and cleaned. Two frontal screws (one on each hemisphere, 1mm in diameter) and a single parietal screw (left hemisphere) were placed in the skull for recording EEG. Two screws, serving as reference and ground, were placed above the cerebellum. Two single-stranded stainless-steel wires were inserted to the neck muscles to record EMG. EEG and EMG wires were soldered onto a head-stage connector (Omnetics). Dental cement was used to cover all screws and wires. A small craniotomy was performed over the right hemisphere, and the dura was carefully dissected. A 16-electrode microwire array targeting the auditory cortex was implanted (Tucker-Davis Technologies, TDT, 33 or 50µm wire diameter, 6-6.5 mm long, 15° tip angle; arrays consisting of 2 rows × 8 wires, with 375µm medial-lateral separation between rows and 250µm anterior–posterior separation within each row). Implantation was diagonal (angle of 28°, see Fig. 1B) using insertion point center coordinates of P: -4.30mm, L: 4.5mm relative to Bregma, and inserted to a final depth of 4.6mm. Following implantation, a silicone gel was applied to cover the craniotomy (Kwik-Sil; World Precision Instruments) and Fusio (Pentron) was used to fix the microwire array in place. At the end of the surgery, chloramphenicol 3% ointment was applied topically and additional analgesia was provided by injecting buprenorphine systemically (0.025 mg/kg s.c.) as the rat awoke from anesthesia. Dexamethasone (1.3 mg/kg) was given with food in the days following the surgery to reduce pain and inflammation around implantation.

Histology

Upon completion of the experiments, position of electrodes was verified by histology in 4 out of 7 animals (e.g. Fig. 1B). Animals were transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) under deep (5% isoflurane) anesthesia. Brains were refrigerated in PFA for a week, cut into 50–60µm serial coronal sections using a vibrating microtome (Leica Biosystems), and stained with fluorescent cresyl violet/Nissl (Rhenium). Histological verification confirmed that electrodes were located within areas Au1/AuD as defined by92.

Electrophysiology

As previously described in35, data was acquired using a RZ2 processor (TDT) with microwire extracellular activity digitally sampled at 24.4 kHz (PZ2 amplifier, TDT) and EEG and EMG pre-amplified (RA16LI, TDT) and digitally sampled at 256.9 Hz (PZ2 amplifier, TDT). Spike sorting was performed using “wave_clus”93, employing a detection threshold of 5 SD and automatic superparamagnetic clustering of wavelet coefficients. Clusters were manually selected, refined, and tagged as multi- or single-unit based on stability throughout recording, quality of separation from other clusters, consistency of spike waveforms and inter-spike interval distributions as in34.

Experimental Design

In the week preceding the surgery, subjects were individually housed and habituated to spending time inside the motorized running wheel for a few hours every day (Fig 1A), and then gradually to participating in the sleep deprivation protocol (Fig. 1D, see below).

We ran 19 sleep deprivation experimental sessions, as follows. At light onset (10 AM) rats were moved from their home cage to a motorized running wheel (Fig 1A, Model 80860B, Lafayette Instrument) placed inside a sound-attenuation chamber (-55dB, H.N.A) and underwent 5 hours of sleep deprivation. Throughout the sleep deprivation period, the wheel was intermittingly slightly rotated for 3 seconds, forcing a short running bout, with a randomly chosen 12-18 seconds interval break in between running bouts. Next, rats were left undisturbed in the fixed wheel for a recovery sleep opportunity period of 5 hours. Throughout the entire session rat were confined to the wheel apparatus placed directly under the ultrasonic speaker. Auditory stimulation (below) was delivered intermittently throughout each session, during both sleep deprivation and recovery sleep periods, without regard to the wheel’s movement regime.

Auditory stimulation

Sounds were synthesized in Matlab (MathWorks) and transduced into voltage signals by a high-sampling rate sound card (192 kHz, LynxTWO, Lynx), amplified (SA1, TDT) and played free-field through a magnetic speaker (MF1, TDT), mounted 60 cm above the motorized running wheel. We employed two different auditory paradigms on separate sessions/days:

Auditory paradigm A

(11 Sessions, 7 animals, markers with black edges accompanying histograms in figures e.g. Fig. 2B): Stimuli included click trains and a set of Dynamic Random Chords (DRCs,41). Click trains were 500ms in duration at rates of either {2, 10, 20, 30, 40} clicks/sec. DRCs were 2.5s in duration and included a train of randomly chosen 20ms “chords”, each comprised of an average of 6 randomly chosen tone-pips at different frequencies (1-64 KHz, with 1/6 octave intervals, 5ms cosine ramp, fixed sound level). There were 190 different DRC stimuli. A typical 10h session contained 2000 blocks, each consisting of a single DRC stimulus and a single repetition of each click train (presented at random order), and with an inter-stimulus interval of 2s and ±0.25s jitter.

Auditory paradigm B

(8 Sessions, 6 animals, markers without black edges accompanying histograms in figures e.g. Fig. 2B): Stimuli included a 40 clicks/s click-train, and a different set of DRC stimuli with denser sampling of the frequency and intensity axes (better resolution) to allow for quantitative assessment of neuronal tuning curves. We used 6s trains of randomly chosen 20ms “chords”, each comprised of an average of 12 randomly chosen tone pips at different frequencies and different sound levels (1-64 KHz, with 1/10 octave intervals, 5ms cosine ramp, spanning an 80 dB range in 10 dB intervals). There were 120 different DRC stimuli. A typical 10h session contained 600 blocks, each consisting of a single DRC and 4 repetitions of the 40 Hz click train, presented at random order, with an inter-stimulus interval of 2s and ±0.25s jitter.

Both paradigms included an 8s inter-stimulus interval every 2 minutes.

Acoustic sound-level measurements

We performed acoustic sound-level measurements using a high-quality ultrasonic microphone (model 378C01, PCB Piezotronics) at 12 positions at the bottom of the wheel apparatus along its circumference (anterior-posterior axis) and at five positions along its width (left-right axis) with 2cm spacing, covering the spatial field occupied by animals during experimental sessions. The root-mean-square (RMS) intensity in response to 2s white noise sounds showed minimal variance at different positions along the anterior-posterior axis, with 0.7±0.3dB mean absolute deviation (MAD±STD) and max difference of 2.4dB between any two positions. Similarly, along the right-left axis we recorded mean absolute difference of 0.4±0.3dB (MAD±STD) and max difference of 1.5dB. Examining sound-levels at different frequency bands (1, 2, 4, 8, 16, 32 and 64 KHz, in response to the same white noise stimulus) at different positions yielded similar low variation (all below 4dB).

Sleep scoring and analysis of arousal states

Manual sleep scoring was performed offline for the entire experimental session, employing visual inspection of EEGs, EMGs and video/behavior as in previous studies18,34,35,94. First, we excluded any periods when the wheel was moving (forced running bouts during sleep deprivation) and other periods of active wakefulness with behavioral activity (e.g., locomotion, grooming) as confirmed with video. Next, we categorized periods to either wakefulness (low-voltage high-frequency EEG activity and high tonic EMG with occasional phasic activity), NREM sleep (high-amplitude slow wave activity and low tonic EMG activity), REM sleep (low-amplitude wake-like frontal EEG co-occurring with theta activity in parietal EEG and flat EMG), or unknown periods not analyzed further (e.g. state transitions, to conservatively remove these epochs for subsequent analysis).

Next, each auditory stimulation trial was categorized to one of four conditions: Vigilant, Tired, NREM and REM, as follows. Vigilant and Tired categories comprised of the first or last third of (quiet) wakefulness trials during the sleep deprivation period, respectively, while NREM and REM comprised of trials scored as such during the recovery sleep period. To assert that differences between the Vigilant and NREM sleep categories did not stem from temporal order effects (e.g. Vigilant trials always preceding NREM by a few hours), we also defined a fifth condition – quiet wakefulness during the recovery sleep period, denoted as QW-RSP. Neural activity during QW-RSP was very similar to the Vigilant condition earlier in the experiment, qualitatively replicating the results of differences between Vigilant and NREM conditions (data not shown).

Analysis of auditory responses across states

Any trials with auditory stimuli presented while the wheel was moving, or while the animal was active (e.g., locomotion, grooming) were excluded from analysis.

Latency analysis

(Fig. 1C). For each unit, we pooled the responses to all click train presentations (2, 10, 20, 30, 40 Clicks/s in Auditory Paradigm A, or 40 Clicks/s in Auditory Paradigm B). The raster of all responses underwent minimal temporal smoothing (gaussian kernel of σ=1.5ms) and averaging to get the peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH). The peak latency was defined as the peak of this PSTH within [0,50]ms following stimulus onset. Units that had peak firing rate ≥ 2x the baseline firing rate and where the difference was statistically significant (Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test, alpha=0.001, comparing the peak-latency time bin in the raster to baseline; [-1,0]ms relative to stimulus onset) were considered to have a significant excitatory onset response. Only units that had such a significant response during both wakefulness and NREM sleep were included in the latency analysis in Fig. 1C (69.2% of units, 343/496).

Neuronal Tuning analysis

(Fig. 2A-C, 3A-C, 6A-C). To analyze responses to the two sets of DRC stimuli (Paradigms A and B) we performed the following analysis. Given that tone pips at each frequency were presented independently (statistically), we calculated the effects of each tone-pip on neuronal firing rates as: ΔFRfreq=x,soundLevel=y=FR¯freq=x,soundLevel=yFR¯freqx. Tuning width (Fig. 2B, 3B, 6B) was calculated as the Full-Width Half Maximum (FWHM) around the best frequency in octaves (red lines in Fig. 2A, 3A, 6A). In paradigm B, frequency tuning width was calculated for the loudest sound level. The tuning width Modulation Index (MI) between any two conditions was defined as (and similar to Gain Index in 35): MIcondA,condB=WidthcondAWidthcondBmax(WidthcondA,WidthcondB)*100

Due to a technical problem in the presentation of tones at the highest frequency of 59.7kHz, many units exhibited maximal responses to this particular frequency, so these trials were removed from subsequent analysis to ensure result validity. To calculate the signal correlation of the neuronal tuning between any two conditions we conducted the following analysis (Fig. 2C, 3C, 6C): in paradigm A, where there was only a single sound level, the neuronal tuning map was defined as the spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF, see spectrograms in Fig 2A, 3A, 6A) a F×T matrix (where F is number of frequencies- [1,64] KHz with 1/6 octave steps, and T is the number of time points [0,50]ms) with each value representing the ΔFR (above baseline) for a given frequency and time-point. The STRF map was smoothed in the temporal domain with a Gaussian kernel (σ=5ms). In paradigm B the tuning map was defined as the frequency response area (FRA) a F×L matrix (where F is number of frequencies- [1,64] KHz with 0.1 octave steps, and L is the number of sound levels [0,80] dB in 10dB steps), with each value representing the ΔFR (above baseline) for a given frequency and sound-level (in the [5,30]ms temporal window). The FRA map was smoothed in the frequency domain with a square window (length=0.3 octaves). The signal correlation between any two conditions is defined a point-by-point Pearson correlation between the two conditions tuning maps (STRF for paradigm A, and FRA for paradigm B). Realistically however, this correlation will always be smaller than one, since the neural response inevitably contains some noise, and because estimates of the response are limited by a finite number of trials. The signal correlation is also expected to be on average larger than zero, as even different units in the same region might show similar preference to frequency and temporal profile, yielding positive signal correlation. Therefore, to create meaningful benchmarks to compare signal correlations, we compared the following three values for each unit separately: (i) [minimal correlation expected]: signal correlation of each neuron’s tuning map (STRF/FRA for paradigms A/B, respectively) with the tuning maps of other units in the session across different conditions (left bar in Fig. 2C, 3C, 6C), (ii) [main value of interest]: signal correlation of each neuron’s tuning map in one condition (e.g. Vigilant) with its tuning map in the other condition (e.g. Tired, middle bar in Fig. 2C, 3C, 6C), (iii) [maximal possible correlation]: each neuron’s signal correlation of its tuning map in the 1st vs. 2nd half of trials in the same condition (right bar in Fig. 2C, 3C, 6C). Formally:

{u1, u2, …, un} a set of n Units in a given session.

{s1, s2} a set (S) of two Conditions we want to compare (e.g. Vigilant and Tired).

{h1, h2} first and second half of trials for a given condition.

TMu,s,h is the Tuning-Map (STRF/FRA matrix for paradigms A/B, respectively) of Unit u for h half of trials in Condition s.

ρ(TMua,sb,hd,TMue,sf,hg) is the point-by-point Pearson correlation coefficient between the two tuning map matrices TMua,sb,hd and TMue,sf,hg.

ρ(TMua,sb,TMue,sf), the correlation between the tuning maps of unit ua in condition sb and unit ue in condition sf is defined as mean correlation coefficient between all halves combinations:

ρ(TMua,sb,h1,TMue,sf,h1)+ρ(TMua,sb,h1,TMue,sf,h2)+ρ(TMua,sb,h2,TMue,Sf,h1)+ρ(TMua,sb,h2,TMue,sf,h2)4

The three different measures of signal correlation (left, middle and right bars, respectively) for a given neuron ui are defined as:

SignalCorrAcrossUnitsui=j{1,,i1,i+1,,n)ρ(TMui,s1,TMuj,s2)+ρ(TMui,s1,TMuj,s2)2(n1)SignalCorrAcrossStatesui=ρ(TMui,s1,TMuj,s2)SignalCorrWithinStateui=ρ(TMui,s1,h1,TMui,s1,h2)+ρ(TMui,s2,h1,TMui,s2,h2)2

Analysis of responses to click trains

(Fig. 2D-E, 3D-E, 6D-F). Spontaneous firing rate (FR) was calculated as the mean firing rate in the [-500,0]ms window preceding the click-trains stimuli, and post-onset FR as the mean FR in the [30,80]ms window. Onset response and sustained locking to different click rates (Fig. 2D-E, 3D-E, 4A-C, 6D-H) were obtained from the smoothed peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH, Gaussian kernel, σ=2ms). Onset response was obtained by extracting the maximal firing rate during the [0,50]ms window of the smoothed PSTH. Locking to different click rates (2, 10, 20, 30 & 40 clicks/s) was obtained by calculating the mean firing rate for each phase during the inter-click intervals in the [130,530]ms window. Then, firing rate locking was defined by the minimum firing rate (during the least preferred phase relative to the click) subtracted from the maximum firing rate (during the most preferred phase). Population synchrony was defined as population coupling44, the correlation of each unit firing to that of the entire neuronal population average in 50ms bins during baseline ([-1000,0]ms). Population coupling was calculated for each trial baseline period and then averaged for all trials in a given condition.

Modulation index between two conditions for all measures above was calculated as for the tuning width modulation index: ConditionAConditionBMax(ConditionA,ConditionB)×100

Sensory adaptation curve fitting

(Fig. 4D-F, 6I-J). We first normalized each unit’s sustained locking response to each click rate by dividing its firing rate to the maximum of all locked responses across all rates (2,10,20,30,40 clicks/s) and its onset response during the same condition (points in figure 4D and 6I). we then fitted the data (the five normalized responses: 2,10,20,30,40 clicks/s) with the following sigmoid model, where x0 is the click-rate where the normalized response is 0.5 (50% of max) and k is the slope of decay of the response.

NormalizedResponseclickrate=xclicks/s=11+ek(log(x)log(x0))

Using this fitted model (traces in Fig. 4D, 6I) we estimate for each neuron in each condition the ‘adapted rate’, defined as the estimated click rate for which the normalized response would be 0.25 (25% of the maximum, crosses in Fig. 4D, 6I; calculation with other percentile cutoff of maximum did not affect the results). In examining how the adapted rate changes across different conditions for the entire neuronal population (Fig 4E, 6I) and in an effort to exclude noisy responses, we included in the population analysis only units with satisfactory sigmoid fit (rms<0.07) and for which the adapted click rate was within the range of [2,150] clicks/s. This criterion led to the exclusion of a minority of neurons (47/197 units, 23.9%).

Silent intervals analysis

(Fig. 5). To consider the effects of sleep deprivation and NREM sleep on spontaneous and stimulus-induced silent intervals we performed the following analysis. We created a raster plot of spontaneous spiking bursts for each unit (Fig. 5A left), which was trial-by-trial matched to the its click-induced onset response (Fig. 5A right). This was done by matching each trial of 2-Hz click-train onset response ([0,30]ms) with an identical (or as similar as possible) spike train obtained during spontaneous activity in the same arousal condition. Each unit PSTH was normalized to its baseline FR and a grand-mean PSTH was calculated for stimulus-induced responses and matched spontaneous bursts (Fig. 5B). We quantified the effect per unit by calculating the mean baseline-normalized FR in the post-onset temporal window ([30,80]ms) for each condition (Vigilant, Tired and NREM sleep) and for stimulus-induced and spontaneous spiking bursts.

To detect (possibly local) silent intervals we performed our analysis on a per-microwire basis (aggregating the spikes from all clusters recorded in the microwire). We defined silent intervals as periods of 50ms neuron silence78 and checked their probability in the baseline ([-50,0]ms), as well as post-onset ([30,80]ms) period (spontaneous and stimulus-induced silent intervals in Fig. 5, respectively, orange and green lines). To control for changes in silent interval probability stemming simply from changes in the spontaneous firing rate, we took the absolute silent interval probability and subtracted the expected silent interval probability from a simulated Poisson-process unit activity with the same firing rate (Δ50ms silence probability in Fig. 5E). To formally compare the effects of sleep deprivation on spontaneous vs. stimulus-induced silent interval probabilities (Fig 5C) we calculated the following modulation index:

ModulationIndexTired,Vigilant=ΔProbTiredΔProbVigilantmax(ΔProbVigilant,ΔProbTired,ΔProbNREM)min(ΔProbVigilant,ΔProbTired,ΔProbNREM)

Analysis of animal movement and pose

(Supp. Fig. 2). We identified the position of different body parts and face features in our video recordings using DeepLabCut (Supp. Fig. 2A). For each auditory trial, we extracted the video frames spanning [-3,1]s relative to sound onset. We restricted our analysis to trials where at least one of the following face features - right eye, left eye, snout or head cap - was visible in at least 50% of the frames. For each such trial, movement was further quantified as the mean standard deviation of X- and Y-axes position for each visible face feature, in pixels (Supp. Fig. 2B). Next, a maximal threshold of 3 pixels was used to exclude from analysis any trials with meaningful movement. Furthermore, for each trial, head/body pose was defined as the logical vector of the visibility of different face features. For example, a trial where the right eye, snout and head cap were visible but not the left eye (Supp. Fig. 2A V, facing right) represented one such pose. The prevalence of different body poses was counter-balanced between the Vigilant and Tired condition to assure both conditions were matched in their head and body poses. Then, the click-trains analysis described above (Fig. 2) was repeated for all units and sessions containing at least 20 trials (after addressing movement and pose issues) in both the Tired and Vigilant condition (Supp. Fig. 2C).

Unit waveform stability analysis

(Supp. Fig. 4). For each unit, mean spike (action potential) waveform was extracted in 30-min bins (i.e. 20 bins for a typical 10h recording session). To facilitate comparison between different units with different spike amplitudes, waveforms were normalized by division in the mean waveform amplitude for the entire session. Waveform variance throughout the session was calculated as the mean squared deviation across all the 30-min bins from the session mean waveform. The distribution of spike waveform variances for all units is presented in Supp. Fig 4A (blue, ‘Real’). This distribution was compared to a surrogate, artificially-created, distribution of ‘unstable waveform’ units created using the following procedure. For each unit, a random (uniformly sampled) time point in the session was chosen. A new surrogate ‘unit’ was created by concatenating all the spikes from the original unit preceding that time point, with the spikes of a different unit (randomly chosen from the same session) following that time point. Put simply, every surrogate unit was a recombination of two different units concatenated at a randomly chosen time point. This process was repeated ten times for each unit yielding a surrogate distribution of ‘unstable waveform’ units (Supp. Fig 4A, red, ‘Surrogate’). Based on the surrogate units’ distribution, a conservative threshold for waveform stability was picked at a false-positive rate of 0.01 (Supp. Fig 4B). Then, the click-trains analysis described above (Fig. 2,3) was repeated for all (real) units declared ‘waveform stable’ (Supp. Fig. 4C,D, 63% of all units).

Stress and corticosterone measures (Supp. Fig. 3)

Ten adult male Wistar rats (300–350g, >12 weeks old) were habituated to spending time inside the motorized running wheel for a few hours (Fig 1A) and then gradually to participating in the sleep deprivation protocol (Fig. 1D), as was done for rats in the main electrophysiology experiments. Corticosterone (CORT) plasma levels were measured in each animal under five different conditions: i) after short SD period (50 min in SD protocol, ZT+1:20h, reflecting the Vigilant condition). ii) after long SD period (4:10h in SD protocol, ZT+4:40h, reflecting the Tired condition). iii-iv) as a baseline home-cage control condition at the same times of day, ZT+1:20h and ZT+4:40h, without exposure to SD (to control for circadian rhythm in CORT levels), and v) following the wet cage stress paradigm 49,50. Two additional male Wistar rats were sampled just in conditions iii-v. Subjects were randomly allocated to the different experimental groups and counterbalanced across cages. The order of the two baseline and two SD measures (i-iv) was counterbalanced between animals, whereas the stress paradigm was always conducted as the last assessment, in which half of the animals were sampled at ZT+1:20h and the other half at ZT+4:40h. Time interval between sampling in two different conditions within the same animal was at least 1 week.

Blood withdrawal and plasma separation

Animals were anesthetized with 2.0% isoflurane, and 0.5 ml blood was collected from a small cut (1-2mm) at the end of the tail into an EDTA-containing tube (1.8 mg/1ml blood), completing the bleeding procedure within 3 min of approaching the animal’s cage. Blood was centrifuged for 20 minutes at 1000g 4°C for plasma separation, which was then collected and stored at -20°C until assayed for CORT levels.

The wet cage stress paradigm

Animals were placed as cage-mates in new transparent Plexiglas cages filled with 2 cm deep room-temperature water, with free access to food and water, for a total period of 1-2hrs. The cages were placed in an unfamiliar room for the duration of the paradigm. Previous studies indicate this stress paradigm to significantly increase corticosterone (CORT) levels 49,50.

Assessment of plasma corticosterone levels

Plasma CORT levels were measured using ELISA kit K014-H1 (Arbor Assays, Ann Arbor, MI USA), according to the manufacturer’s instructions.

Statistics

Due to the nested and hierarchical nature of electrophysiological neural data95,96 we used a linear mixed-effects model (LME). The LME was used to account for non-independencies in measures from different units that were obtained in the same electrode, experimental session or animal. Animal identity was used as a random effect, together with experimental session and microwire electrode as nested random effects within each animal. Model parameters were calculated using ‘fitlme’ function (Matlab, MathWorks) using restricted maximum likelihood estimation. In the cases where the data samples were obtained on a per-microwire basis (analysis in Fig. 5D-F, instead of per-unit basis) only animal identity and experimental session (nested within animal) were used as random effects. Using conservative non-parametric statistical tests (Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test or Wilcoxon Sign-Rank Test) on the data summarized at the level of animals (n=7) or sessions (n=19) yielded qualitatively very similar results in terms of statistical significance as the LME model (data not shown). In figures depicting mean effects per session (large markers in figures 2B, 2C, 2E, 3B, 3C, 3E, 4B, 4C, 4E, 5C, 5E, 5F, 6B, 6C, 6E, 6F and 6H) only sessions with at least 5 units were included. The LME analysis however, was always applied on all sessions, even those with few units. If not stated otherwise all effect sizes mentioned in main text are described as mean±SEM over all units. When testing for variance across multiple (>2) conditions a Friedman test was used (akin to a non-parametric repeated measures ANOVA) on data summarized at the level of animals (n=7, averaging all the units for each animal).

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

We thank Israel Nelken for input on auditory setup and analyses. Rafael Malach, Mark Shein-Idelson, Aaron Krom, Yael Oran, and Yaniv Sela for comments on an earlier draft, and members of the Nir Lab for discussions.

Footnotes

Author contributions

A.M. and Y.N. designed the project. A.M. built experimental setup, compiled stimuli, performed experiments and conducted data analysis. Y.N. supervised the project and secured funding. A. E. performed and analyzed corticosterone experiments with guidance from S.B.E. B.H. performed unit stability analysis. A.M. and Y.N. wrote the paper.

Declaration of interests

The authors declare that no financial and non-financial competing interests exist.

References

  • 1.Carskadon MA. Sleep deprivation: Health consequences and societal impact. Med Clin North Am. 2004;88:767–776. doi: 10.1016/j.mcna.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1982;1:195–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Doran SM, Van Dongen HPA, Dinges DF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: Evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol. 2001;139:253–267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Krause AJ, Simon E Ben, Mander BA, Greer SM, Saletin JM, Goldstein-Piekarski AN, Walker MP. The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18:404–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Portas CM, Rees G, Howseman AM, Josephs O, Turner R, Frith CD. A specific role for the thalamus in mediating the interaction of attention and arousal in humans. J Neurosci. 1998;18:8979–8989. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-21-08979.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Drummond SPA, Brown GG, Stricker JL, Buxton RB, Wong EC, Gillin JC. Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport. 1999;10:3745–3748. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199912160-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Thomas ML, Sing HC, Belenky G, Holcomb HH, Mayberg HS, Dannals RF, Wagner HN, Thorne DR, Popp KA, Rowland LM, et al. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness II. Effects of 48 and 72 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J Sleep Res. 2000;9:335–352. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00225.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Drummond SPA, Bischoff-Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L, Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep. 2005;28:1059–1068. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Padilla ML, Wood RA, Hale LA, Knight RT. Lapses in a prefrontal-extrastriate preparatory attention network predict mistakes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1477–1487. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:971–978. doi: 10.1038/nn1727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wu JC, Gillin JC, Buchsbaum MS, Chen P, Keator DB, Khosla Wu N, Darnall LA, Fallon JH, Bunney WE. Frontal lobe metabolic decreases with sleep deprivation not totally reversed by recovery sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2783–2792. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chee MWL, Tan JC, Zheng H, Parimal S, Weissman DH, Zagorodnov V, Dinges DF. Lapsing during Sleep Deprivation Is Associated with Distributed Changes in Brain Activation. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5519–5528. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tomasi D, Wang RL, Telang F, Boronikolas V, Jayne MC, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Volkow ND. Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:233–240. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chee MWL. Limitations on visual information processing in the sleep-deprived brain and their underlying mechanisms. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;1:56–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Basner M, Rao H, Goel N, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral dynamics. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:854–863. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Finelli LA, Baumann H, Borbély AA, Achermann P. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: Correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience. 2000;101:523–529. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00409-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lorenzo I, Ramos J, Arce C, Guevara MA, Corsi-Cabrera M. Effect of total sleep deprivation on reaction time and waking eeg activity in man. Sleep. 1995;18:346–354. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Hanlon EC, Nir Y, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Local sleep in awake rats. Nature. 2011;472:443–447. doi: 10.1038/nature10009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nir Y, Andrillon T, Marmelshtein A, Suthana N, Cirelli C, Tononi G, Fried I. Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses following sleep deprivation. Nat Med. 2017;23:1474–1480. doi: 10.1038/nm.4433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Harris KD, Thiele A. Cortical state and attention. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:509–523. doi: 10.1038/nrn3084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lee SH, Dan Y. Neuromodulation of Brain States. Neuron. 2012;76:109–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jaramillo S, Zador AM. The auditory cortex mediates the perceptual effects of acoustic temporal expectation. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:246–251. doi: 10.1038/nn.2688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Otazu GH, Tai L-H, Yang Y, Zador AM. Engaging in an auditory task suppresses responses in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:646–654. doi: 10.1038/nn.2306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kato HK, Gillet SN, Isaacson JS. Flexible Sensory Representations in Auditory Cortex Driven by Behavioral Relevance. Neuron. 2015;88:1027–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Atiani S, Elhilali M, David SV, Fritz JB, Shamma Sa. Task Difficulty and Performance Induce Diverse Adaptive Patterns in Gain and Shape of Primary Auditory Cortical Receptive Fields. Neuron. 2009;61:467–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Atiani S, David SV, Elgueda D, Locastro M, Radtke-Schuller S, Shamma SA, Fritz JB. Emergent selectivity for task-relevant stimuli in higher-order auditory cortex. Neuron. 2014;82:486–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.McGinley MJ, David SV, McCormick DA. Cortical Membrane Potential Signature of Optimal States for Sensory Signal Detection. Neuron. 2015:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lin PA, Asinof SK, Edwards NJ, Isaacson JS. Arousal regulates frequency tuning in primary auditory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116:25304–25310. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1911383116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Zhou M, Liang F, Xiong XR, Li L, Li H, Xiao Z, Tao HW, Zhang LI. Scaling down of balanced excitation and inhibition by active behavioral states in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:841–50. doi: 10.1038/nn.3701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bereshpolova Y, Stoelzel CR, Zhuang J, Amitai Y, Alonso J-M, Swadlow HA. Getting Drowsy? Alert / Nonalert Transitions and Visual Thalamocortical Network Dynamics. J Neurosci. 2011;31:17480–17487. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2262-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Zhuang J, Bereshpolova Y, Stoelzel CR, Huff JM, Hei X, Alonso JM, Swadlow HA. Brain state effects on layer 4 of the awake visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2014;34:3888–3900. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4969-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Issa EB, Wang X. Altered neural responses to sounds in primate primary auditory cortex during slow-wave sleep. J Neurosci. 2011;31:2965–2973. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4920-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Issa EB, Wang X. Increased neural correlations in primate auditory cortex during slow-wave sleep. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109:2732–8. doi: 10.1152/jn.00695.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nir Y, Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Banks MI, Tononi G. Auditory Responses and Stimulus-Specific Adaptation in Rat Auditory Cortex are Preserved Across NREM and REM Sleep. Cereb Cortex. 2013:bht328. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sela Y, Krom AJ, Bergman L, Regev N, Nir Y. Sleep differentially affects early and late neuronal responses to sounds in auditory and perirhinal cortices. J Neurosci. 2020;40:2895–2905. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1186-19.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Krom AJ, Marmelshtein A, Gelbard-Sagiv H, Tankus A, Hayat H, Hayat D, Matot I, Strauss I, Fahoum F, Soehle M, et al. Anesthesia-induced loss of consciousness disrupts auditory responses beyond primary cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1917251117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nourski KV, Steinschneider M, Rhone AE, Kawasaki H, Howard MA, Banks MI. Auditory Predictive Coding across Awareness States under Anesthesia: An Intracranial Electrophysiology Study. J Neurosci. 2018;183:412–424. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0967-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bergman L, Krom AJ, Sela Y, Marmelshtein A, Hayat H, Regev N, Nir Y. Propofol Anesthesia Concentration Rather Than Abrupt Behavioral Unresponsiveness Linearly Degrades Responses in the Rat Primary Auditory Cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2022 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Raz A, Grady SM, Krause BM, Uhlrich DJ, Manning Ka, Banks MI. Preferential effect of isoflurane on top-down vs. bottom-up pathways in sensory cortex. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:191. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lim J, Dinges DF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull. 2010;136:375–89. doi: 10.1037/a0018883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Linden JF. Spectrotemporal Structure of Receptive Fields in Areas AI and AAF of Mouse Auditory Cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:2660–2675. doi: 10.1152/jn.00751.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pachitariu M, Lyamzin DR, Sahani M, Lesica Na. State-Dependent Population Coding in Primary Auditory Cortex. 2015;35:2058–2073. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3318-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Christie MA, Mckenna JT, Connolly NP, Mccarley RW, Strecker RE. 24 Hours of sleep deprivation in the rat increases sleepiness and decreases vigilance: Introduction of the rat-psychomotor vigilance task. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:376–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00698.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Okun M, Steinmetz Na, Cossell L, Iacaruso MF, Ko H, Barthó P, Moore T, Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Carandini M, et al. Diverse coupling of neurons to populations in sensory cortex. Nature. 2015;521:511–515. doi: 10.1038/nature14273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Plourde G. The effects of propofol on the 40-Hz auditory steady-state response and on the electroencephalogram in humans. Anesth Analg. 1996;82:1015–1022. doi: 10.1097/00000539-199605000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sharon O, Nir Y. Attenuated Fast Steady-State Visual Evoked Potentials During Human Sleep. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28:1297–1311. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mathis A, Mamidanna P, Cury KM, Abe T, Murthy VN, Mathis MW, Bethge M. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nath T, Mathis A, Chen AC, Patel A, Bethge M, Mathis MW. Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors. Nat Protoc. 2019;14:2152–2176. doi: 10.1038/s41596-019-0176-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Shaashua L, Sominsky L, Levi B, Sorski L, Reznick M, Page GG, Ben-Eliyahu S. In vivo suppression of plasma IL-12 levels by acute and chronic stress paradigms: Potential mediating mechanisms and sex differences. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26:996–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Levi B, Benish M, Goldfarb Y, Sorski L, Melamed R, Rosenne E, Ben-Eliyahu S. Continuous stress disrupts immunostimulatory effects of IL-12. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25:727–735. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nir Y, Tononi G. Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:88–100. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Huber R, Esser SK, Singh H, Tononi G. Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science (80-) 2005;309:2228–2232. doi: 10.1126/science.1117256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Vyazovskiy VV, Faraguna U, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Triggering slow waves during NREM sleep in the rat by intracortical electrical stimulation : Effects of sleep / wake history and background activity. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101:1921–1931. doi: 10.1152/jn.91157.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kong D, Asplund CL, Chee MWL. Sleep deprivation reduces the rate of rapid picture processing. Neuroimage. 2014;91:169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Muller-Gass A, Campbell K. Sleep deprivation moderates neural processes associated with passive auditory capture. Brain Cogn. 2019;132:89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Wiggins E, Mottarella M, Good K, Eggleston S, Stevens C. 24-h sleep deprivation impairs early attentional modulation of neural processing: An event-related brain potential study. Neurosci Lett. 2018;677:32–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Makov S, Sharon O, Ding N, Ben-Shachar M, Nir Y, Golumbic EZ. Sleep disrupts high-level speech parsing despite significant basic auditory processing. J Neurosci. 2017;37:7772–7781. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0168-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Liu X, Lauer KK, Ward BD, Rao SM, Li SJ, Hudetz AG. Propofol disrupts functional interactions between sensory and high-order processing of auditory verbal memory. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:2487–2498. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Nourski KV, Todd MM, Steinschneider M, Banks MI, Rhone AE, Mueller RN, Kawasaki H, Howard MA. Electrocorticographic (ECoG) delineation of human auditory cortical fields based on effects of propofol anesthesia. Soc Neurosci. 2016;152:1515678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Davis MH, Coleman MR, Absalom AR, Rodd JM, Johnsrude IS, Matta BF, Owen AM, Menon DK. Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:16032–16037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701309104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sellers KK, Bennett DV, Hutt A, Williams JH, Fröhlich F. Awake vs. anesthetized: layer-specific sensory processing in visual cortex and functional connectivity between cortical areas. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113:3798–3815. doi: 10.1152/jn.00923.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hayat H, Marmelshtein A, Krom AJ, Sela Y, Tankus A, Strauss I, Fahoum F, Fried I, Nir Y. Impaired top-down auditory processing despite extensive single-neuron responses during human sleep. bioRxiv. 2021:2021.04.03.438283 [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Leopold DA, Logothetis NK. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys’ percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature. 1996;379:549–553. doi: 10.1038/379549a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gelbard-Sagiv H, Magidov E, Sharon H, Hendler T, Nir Y. Noradrenaline Modulates Visual Perception and Late Visually Evoked Activity. Curr Biol. 2018;28:2239–2249.:e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Downer JD, Niwa M, Sutter ML. Task Engagement Selectively Modulates Neural Correlations in Primary Auditory Cortex. J Neurosci. 2015;35:7565–7574. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4094-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Marguet SL, Harris KD. State-dependent representation of amplitude-modulated noise stimuli in rat auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:6414–6420. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5773-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Niwa M, Johnson JS, O’Connor KN, Sutter ML. Active engagement improves primary auditory cortical neurons’ ability to discriminate temporal modulation. J Neurosci. 2012;32:9323–9334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5832-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Shimaoka D, Harris KD, Carandini M. Effects of Arousal on Mouse Sensory Cortex Depend on Modality. Cell Rep. 2018;22:3160–3167. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.02.092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bagur S, Averseng M, Elgueda D, David S, Fritz J, Yin P, Shamma S, Boubenec Y, Ostojic S. Go/No-Go task engagement enhances population representation of target stimuli in primary auditory cortex. Nat Commun. 2018;9:2529. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04839-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Sakata S. State-dependent and cell type-specific temporal processing in auditory thalamocortical circuit. Sci Rep. 2016;6:1–13. doi: 10.1038/srep18873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Carcea I, Insanally MN, Froemke RC. Dynamics of auditory cortical activity during behavioural engagement and auditory perception. Nat Commun. 2017;8:1–12. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Banks MI, Moran NS, Krause BM, Grady SM, Uhlrich DJ, Manning KA. Altered stimulus representation in rat auditory cortex is not causal for loss of consciousness under general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth. 2018;121:605–615. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.05.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Schwartz ZP, Buran BN, David SV. Pupil-associated states modulate excitability but not stimulus selectivity in primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2020;123:191–208. doi: 10.1152/jn.00595.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Scholvinck ML, Saleem AB, Benucci A, Harris KD, Carandini M. Cortical State Determines Global Variability and Correlations in Visual Cortex. J Neurosci. 2015;35:170–178. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4994-13.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Merzenich MM, Knight PL, Roth GL. Representation of cochlea within primary auditory cortex in the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1975;38:231–249. doi: 10.1152/jn.1975.38.2.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Gaese BH, Gaese BH, Ostwald J, Ostwald J. Anesthesia changes frequency tuning of neurons in the rat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:1062–6. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Issa EB, Wang X. Sensory responses during sleep in primate primary and secondary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:14467–14480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Lazimy YM, Faraguna U, Esser SK, Williams JC, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Cortical Firing and Sleep Homeostasis. Neuron. 2009;63:865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Fisher SP, Cui N, McKillop LE, Gemignani J, Bannerman DM, Oliver PL, Peirson SN, Vyazovskiy VV. Stereotypic wheel running decreases cortical activity in mice. Nat Commun. 2016;7:13138. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Vyazovskiy VV, Tobler I. Theta activity in the waking EEG is a marker of sleep propensity in the rat. Brain Res. 2005;1050:64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Steriade M, Nunez A, Amzica F. Intracellular analysis of relations between the slow (&amp;lt; 1 Hz) neocortical oscillation and other sleep rhythms of the electroencephalogram. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3266–3283. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-08-03266.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Nir Y, Massimini M, Boly M. In: Neuroimaging of Consciousness. Cavanna AE, Nani A, Blumenfeld H, Laureys S, editors. Springer; Berlin Heidelberg: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Rager G. The response of cat visual cortex to flicker stimuli of variable frequency. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:1856–1877. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00197.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Castro-Alamancos MA. Absence of Rapid Sensory Adaptation in Neocortex during Information Processing States. Neuron. 2004;41:455–464. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00853-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Plourde G. Human Auditory Steady-State Response During General Anesthesia. 1990:460–468. doi: 10.1213/00000539-199011000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lustenberger C, Patel YA, Alagapan S, Page JM, Price B, Boyle MR, Frohlich F. High-density EEG characterization of brain responses to auditory rhythmic stimuli during wakefulness and NREM sleep. Neuroimage. 2017;169:57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Noya SB, Colameo D, Brüning F, Spinnler A, Mircsof D, Opitz L, Mann M, Tyagarajan SK, Robles MS, Brown SA. The forebrain synaptic transcriptome is organized by clocks but its proteome is driven by sleep. Science (80-) 2019;366 doi: 10.1126/science.aav2642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Esser SK, Riedner BA, Huber R, Murphy M, Peterson MJ, Tononi G. Triggering sleep slow waves by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:8496–501. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702495104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Prolonged wakefulness alters neuronal responsiveness to local electrical stimulation of the neocortex in awake rats. J Sleep Res. 2013;22:239–250. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Casali AG, Gosseries O, Rosanova M, Boly M, Sarasso S, Casali KR, Casarotto S, Bruno M-A, Laureys S, Tononi G, et al. A Theoretically Based Index of Consciousness Independent of Sensory Processing and Behavior. Sci Transl Med. 2013;5:198ra105. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates: hard cover edition. Elsevier; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Quiroga RQ, Nadasdy Z, Ben-Shaul Y. Unsupervised spike detection and sorting with wavelets and superparamagnetic clustering. Neural Comput. 2004;16:1661–1687. doi: 10.1162/089976604774201631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Rodriguez XAV, Funk XCM, Vyazovskiy XVV, Nir XY, Tononi G, Cirelli XC. Why Does Sleep Slow-Wave Activity Increase After Extended Wake? Assessing the Effects of Increased Cortical Firing During Wake and Sleep. 2016;36:12436–12447. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1614-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Makin TR, Orban de Xivry J-J. Ten common statistical mistakes to watch out for when writing or reviewing a manuscript. Elife. 2019;8 doi: 10.7554/eLife.48175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Aarts E, Verhage M, Veenvliet JV, Dolan CV, Van Der Sluis S. A solution to dependency: Using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:491–496. doi: 10.1038/nn.3648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

RESOURCES