Abstract
Motor imagery is conceptualized as an internal simulation that uses motor-related parts of the brain as its substrate. Many studies have investigated this sharing of common neural resources between the two modalities of motor imagery and motor execution. They have shown overlapping but not identical activation patterns that thereby result in a modality-specific neural signature. However, it is not clear how far this neural signature depends on whether the imagined action has previously been practiced physically or only imagined. The present study aims to disentangle whether the neural imprint of an imagined manual pointing sequence within cortical and subcortical motor areas is determined by the nature of this prior practice modality. Each participant practiced two sequences physically, practiced two other sequences mentally, and did a behavioural pre-test without any further practice on a third pair of sequences. After a two-week practice intervention, participants underwent fMRI scans while imagining all six sequences. Behavioural data demonstrated practice-related effects as well as very good compliance with instructions. Functional MRI data confirmed the previously known motor imagery network. Crucially, we found that mental and physical practice left a modality-specific footprint during mental motor imagery. In particular, activation within the right posterior cerebellum was stronger when the imagined sequence had previously been practiced physically. We conclude that cerebellar activity is shaped specifically by the nature of the prior practice modality.
Subject terms: Human behaviour, Cerebellum, Motor cortex, Premotor cortex
Introduction
During the last decade, phenomena of embodied cognition have attracted much attention in the field of cognitive neuroscience. The key underlying idea is that cognition is not processed in isolation from the body, but is shaped by bodily states and body-related experiences1–3. Within this framework, motor imagery (MI) has been considered to be a body-based simulation process that uses the motor system as a substrate2,4. In MI, subjects imagine the execution of a bodily movement from a first-person perspective, emphasizing a strong kinaesthetic component but without overt movement5. With regard to the neural substrate, MI is believed to be organized around several core and broader motor regions: the supplementary motor area (SMA), the different sections of the premotor cortex (dPMC, vPMC), the primary motor cortex (M1), posterior parietal regions such as the inferior (IPL) and the superior parietal lobe (SPL), the basal ganglia (BG), and the cerebellum6–10. However, the premotor area, the posterior parietal lobe, and the cerebellum seem to be the critical structures for performing MI11.
Computational models in motor control suggest that these areas store and process internal forward models that predict sensory consequences given the current state and the motor command12–15. Blakemore and Sirigu16 suggested that internal models are also used in MI, and that it is particularly the retrieval of the stored forward model, which estimates the anticipated sensory outcome of the movement, that also seems to be used in motor imagery16–20. Kilteni et al.21 demonstrated impressive support for this notion, showing that motor imagery produces somatosensory attenuation just like executed movement does. This indicates that sensorimotor prediction might be the mechanism that makes MI and motor execution (ME) equivalent.
In applied contexts, not only physical practice but also the additional implementation of mental rehearsal has become an important technique for improving motor performance in athletes and patients22,23. Behavioural studies using MI have shown improvements in speed, strength, and accuracy of motor execution24–27. Trial-by-trial monitoring in phases of skill learning has further revealed a similar asymptotic learning curve during MI and physical practice28. These findings support the notion that MI is not just epiphenomenal, but plays a functional role in the cortical plasticity related to performance. Regarding actual plasticity effects on cortical and subcortical structures, Lacourse et al.29, for instance, showed that physical and mental practice of a finger-sequence task leads to similar shifts in activation in several sensorimotor structures. In an early study, Pascual-Leone et al.30 used TMS to map M1 during a five-day intervention in which training groups had to either execute or imagine a five-finger piano sequence. Along with progressive performance improvements, they demonstrated that both training groups showed comparable modulations in cortical representations within M1, underpinning the notion of congruent training effects of mental and physical training. Looking at these findings, it can be argued that structural and functional changes might arise from two sources: not only from our actual bodily experience and body state estimation but also from our capacity to imaginatively recreate bodily experiences31. However, it is still under debate whether neural plastic changes related to the different training modalities can possibly be the same due to the lack of movement-related sensory feedback in MI. Furthermore, it is not yet clear which impact different training modalities have on the mere simulation of a movement. Two central features of prior studies in the field are that they particularly investigated either the impact on ME and MI of different training modalities on motor execution29,32 or the effect of interventions in just one training modality33–36.
Against this background, the present experiment examined possible modulations in neural activation patterns induced by mental and physical practice in one group of subjects using a motor imagery paradigm. The aim was to try to disentangle whether mental or physical practice modalities form specific neuronal imprints during a mental simulation process. More precisely, we aimed to elucidate whether MI uses the same neural substrate organized around cortical (PMC, M1, PPC) as well as subcortical (basal ganglia, cerebellum) motor areas irrespective of mental, physical, or no previous practice of movement sequences. We focused predominantly on premotor, posterior parietal, and cerebellar areas that prior work has demonstrated repeatedly to be of mandatory importance for mental simulation processes such as MI7. Subjects participated in a two-week practice intervention during which they mentally and physically rehearsed different hand movement sequences. During fMRI scanning, they imagined physically trained, mentally trained, or untrained sequences. We then analysed the collected fMRI data to test whether mental and physical practice of the sequential motor task left an equivalent versus modality-specific footprint in our regions of interest (ROIs). In a first step, we defined neural activation sites of MI irrespective of prior experience. In a second step, we compared MI of the differentially trained sequences by comparing MI of the physically trained with MI of the mentally trained sequences in order to describe possible quantitative activation differences related to different forms of prior practice modality. Based on the notion that forward modelling is processed in MI and ME, but that a crucial need to integrate the output of the forward model with actual sensory feedback is found only in ME, we hypothesized that physically trained sequences would lead to a more pronounced neural imprint within motor and motor-related areas such as premotor, posterior parietal, and cerebellar cortices.
Results
Behavioural data
Vividness ratings for imagery of hand movement sequences trained during practice sessions increased progressively throughout the intervention as assessed with a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (very low) to 7 (very high) (MTS2 = 4.95 ± 0.88; MTS4 = 5.44 ± 0.82; MTS6 = 5.95 ± 0.71; F(1.3, 21.7) = 10.10, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.39, Greenhouse–Geisser-corrected) (Fig. 1A).
During practice, subjects’ execution speed and spatiotemporal accuracy improved in all conditions (Fig. 1B). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of time, showing significantly decreased proportional errors in temporal accuracy across the training intervention, F(1, 23) = 7.67, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.25. There was a positive correlation between execution and imagery durations, r = 0.47, p < 0.001, which was strong for physically trained sequences, rPhysical = 0.70, p < 0.001, and moderate for mentally trained sequences, rMental = 0.52, p = 0.010, as well as for sequences that were not trained at all, rControl = 0.44, p = 0.032. During the scanner session, there was no systematic difference in imagery durations between the three conditions (MMental = 4.29 ± 0.79 s, MPhysical = 4.32 ± 0.81 s, MControl = 4.28 ± 0.77 s; F[2, 69] = 0.01, p = 0.99, ηp2 = 0.00).
Analysis of EOG data showed an average of 6.13 ± 1.22 turning points in task-related eye movements of 24 subjects (Fig. 1C). Considering that each sequence consisted of six targets, compliance with task instructions was represented by six sequence-related eye fixations at turning points as well as an additional shift when eyes moved back to the starting position. Additional behavioural results are reported in the supplementary materials.
Neuroimaging data
In a first step, we identified brain regions associated with motor imagery of sequential finger movements irrespective of prior experience by contrasting all imagery conditions against rest (Table S3) and performing a conjunction analysis over all imagery conditions. In a second step, we identified brain regions involved in the differential processing of MI depending on prior experience.
Imagery of movement sequences
The conjunction analysis of all experimental conditions (physical, mental, control) compared to rest revealed significant activation increases in regions previously shown to be involved in motor imagery: motor, parietal, and visual areas as well as in the cerebellum (Fig. 2). More specifically, activation was identified in the SPL (Area 7A), the superior occipital gyrus, the precentral gyrus, the superior and posterior-medial frontal gyrus of the left hemisphere, as well as the right cerebellar lobule VI and VIII (FWE-corrected < 0.05, Table 1). These results were highly consistent with a broad body of literature demonstrating the role of motor and motor-related areas during MI7.
Table 1.
Left/Right | Coordinates of max. t value | t value | Suit | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Conjunction Physical ∩ Mental ∩ Control | ||||||
Superior parietal gyrus (Area 7A) | L | − 18 | − 64 | 62 | 7.70 | |
Superior occipital gyrus (hOc1, V1) | L | − 18 | − 91 | 2 | 7.02 | |
Precentral gyrus | L | − 54 | − 4 | 47 | 6.82 | |
Precentral gyrus | R | 57 | − 1 | 44 | 6.13 | |
Lingual gyrus (hOc1, V1) | R | 21 | − 85 | − 4 | 6.21 | |
Posterior-medial frontal gyrus | L | − 3 | − 1 | 62 | 6.10 | |
Superior parietal gyrus (Area 7A) | R | 21 | − 64 | 56 | 5.98 | |
Superior frontal gyrus | L | − 27 | − 4 | 65 | 5.32 | |
Cerebellum VI | R | 34 | − 51 | − 24 | 5.16 | x |
Cerebellum VIII | R | 32 | − 50 | − 50 | 5.08 | x |
MNI coordinates. Cluster size > 20. FWE-corrected p < .05.
Practice-modality-dependent activation during MI
A ROI analysis involving those areas generally activated during MI revealed an experience-dependent activation increase for ME compared to MI practiced sequences within the cerebellum of the right hemisphere (Crus VIIb, x = 22, y = − 76, z = − 49; FWE-corrected < 0.05) (Fig. 3, Table 2). We further performed an exploratory analysis using a less rigorous threshold. The respective results are reported in the supplementary materials.
Table 2.
Left/Right | Cluster size | Coordinates of max. t value | t value | Suit | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Physical vs. Mental | ||||||||
Cerebellum Crus VIIb | L | 21 | − 32 | − 44 | − 42 | 3.80 | x | |
Cerebellum Crus VIIb | R | 80 | 22 | − 76 | − 48 | 4.93* | x |
MNI coordinates. *p < .05, FWE-corrected ROI analysis. p < .0001, uncorrected. Mask size = 13,236 voxels (1 × 1x1 mm) including both hemispheres.
Discussion
We tested whether the neural imprint within the cortical as well as subcortical motor areas of an imagined manual pointing movement sequence is determined by the nature of prior practice modality. We found that mental and physical practice of the sequential motor task leaves a modality-specific footprint during mental motor imagery, especially within the posterior cerebellum. It has been suggested that the latter is involved in sensorimotor processing, abstract functions of motor actions, including the implementation of forward models37–42, and motor inhibition while performing MI22. Generally, the present results indicate that the generation of predictions as well as the availability of sensory feedback, which could be integrated during practice, drives a modality-specific impact on cerebellar activation during MI. That is to say, our data reveal that neural activity within posterior cerebellar regions distinguishes between imagery of mentally trained versus physically trained sequences, whereas both training modalities lead to similar performance increases regarding the spatiotemporal accuracy of imagined sequences.
Based on the present results, we conclude that cerebellar activity is shaped specifically by the nature of the practice modality. Prior motor experience leads to more pronounced activation particularly within the cerebellum. This suggests that practice leads to the development of modality-specific neural imprints even when participants are asked to only simulate trained movements. In this regard, we would argue that functional changes arising from actual bodily experience versus our capacity to imagine bodily experiences have specific neural substrates. This substrate is then used to process an offline motor state that is a prediction based on representations of prior experiences.
On the behavioural level, our data showed an increased performance reflected by spatiotemporal accuracy for physically and mentally trained as well as untrained sequences. These data revealed that mental as well as physical training relate to a similar performance increase for all simulated motor sequences. Furthermore, they suggest a transfer effect to untrained control sequences. Increasing vividness of motor imagery ratings during the intervention period as well as mental chronometry data from the scanner sessions revealed compliance with the task at all times. Other studies corroborate that imagery ability is also related to the neural MI footprint43–45 as well as to motor expertise6. To the best of our knowledge, a clear experimental disentanglement of these factors has not been achieved before. The inspection of eye movements via EOG data during MI confirmed that participants engaged in similar imagery behaviour during practice and, assumingly, scanner sessions. This did not just support comparable neural activation but has also been shown to enhance the efficiency of MI training46. Because the sample in this study showed high general imagery ability, high vividness of imagery ratings during the intervention, as well as behavioural improvements, it can be concluded that MI training sessions of motor hand sequences were as efficient as physical practice—at least in the present setting.
MI comprises the conscious ability to simulate a movement in one’s imagination. Of necessity, this requires an internal representation of that imagined movement47. Based on Jeannerod’s mental simulation theory48, multiple studies have investigated a possible functional equivalence between motor imagery and actual execution. Results have demonstrated overlapping activation within several cortical and subcortical motor areas7. The congruence of functional neuroanatomy between executed and imagined actions both before and after training has been found repeatedly in PMC and SMA, with less consistent findings in M1, S1, visual cortex, cerebellum, and orbitofrontal cortex29,36,49–51. For instance, Nyberg et al.32 reported more prominent activation in the cerebellum and SMA following physical practice of a similar finger tapping task, whereas MI practice was associated more with SMA and the visual association cortex. However, one has to consider the different lengths of training interventions in these studies (ranging from an intermediate practice within one scanner session35 to extensive 6-week interventions51), differences in the choice of effectors, or inconsistent imagery instructions. Furthermore, most implemented a between-subject design leading to the possibility of inhomogeneity across the investigated groups.
In the present experiment, a conjunction analysis of MI of either physically or mentally trained, as well as untrained sequences revealed neural activation clusters in bilateral SPL (Area 7A), precentral gyrus, superior occipital gyrus, the left posterior-medial and superior frontal gyrus, as well as the right cerebellar lobules VI and VIIIa. These areas have previously been associated with MI, sensorimotor control, and specifically with the execution of aiming movements11,52–55. Especially, the SPL, as a part of the posterior parietal cortex, is associated with state estimation, sensorimotor integration, movement intention, and decision making56–60. Cerebellar activation in lobule VIII has been associated with the sensorimotor processing of, in particular, arm and hand movements61–63. In this regard, sensorimotor tasks activated the anterior lobe (lobule V) and the adjacent lobule VI with additional foci in lobule VIII. Motor activation was in VIIIa/b; somatosensory activation was confined to VIIIb64. Our analyses also revealed strong activation sites in the visual cortex, which, according to Guillot et al.65, is associated particularly with visual rather than kinaesthetic imagery processes. This effect could indeed be linked to the nature of the task, assuming that participants generated an image of the target grid as well as their moving hand during motor imagery and/or eye movements during imagery. In this regard, Krüger et al.66 recently investigated task-dependent imagery modality in a large group of participants. Their data showed that for a large set of imagined actions, the sensory impression of a motor image could be explained by the environmental demands of the action. For example, in the case of aiming movements that require a certain degree of precision by hitting a target, visual information might be a key feature of the respective action representation. The consequence is that the prediction and the imagination of this action type might be more related to visual aspects of the action.
It is well documented that the cerebellum is one of the critical structures for performing MI67–70. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that increasing activation in anterior cerebellar areas is associated with spatial accuracy demands of imagined pointing movements71. The cerebellum is known to store internal models and action representations built up by individual motor learning processes (that were modulated here by experiencing different sequences either purely mentally or physically)20,72. More specifically, it has been discussed as a principal brain area for the storage of internal forward models that predict movement outcomes supporting predictive motor control14,73–77. Thereby, the cerebellum in particular seems to play an important role by providing precise timing information for predictions78. Alongside its role in sensorimotor control, several findings provide support for a broader concept of cerebellar function by highlighting the involvement of the cerebellum in diverse cognitive processes such as attention and working memory79. Based on the present data, it can be reasoned that cerebellar activity during MI is tuned specifically by physical training (see also Nyberg et al.32 for an ME task). Physical practice seems to build up a modality-specific footprint in the cerebellum that is then used to perform an offline mental simulation of the respective movement. Interestingly, several studies have observed that motor expertise is associated with an increase of cerebellar activation during action simulation (observation and anticipation tasks), suggesting that it is particularly motor-related cognitive functions of the cerebellum that can be modified by physical experience80–82.
When characterizing the different experiences in the present experiment, it can be stated that a simulation of a movement in the imagination comprises a very conscious and effortful act that requires the retrieval of a stored forward model of that particular movement in every single trial during training. This mental simulation runs offline and is therefore not associated with physical execution. This means that sensory feedback is totally lacking, and, therefore, might not be represented in a significant manner during practice. During physical practice, however, participants are receiving both visual and sensory feedback on the performed movement. Thus, it is evident that both practice modalities are not totally equivalent when it comes to incoming information and feedback on the planned movement. In this context, Zabicki et al.11 have argued that execution and imagery representations are neither purely distinct nor purely equivalent. They are best captured by models assuming that ME and MI are distinguishable from each other while preserving a low-to-moderate degree of similarity; and it appears that the similarity between MI and ME is highest on this level of action plans. Heuer83 already discussed the idea that different movement characteristics will be learned when real sensory feedback is lacking. Therefore, in the present case, mental practice might build up a rather spatial representation of the given task, whereas physical training also generates a kinaesthetic, more embodied representation reflected by the neural imprint in the cerebellum. This specific representation, however, is used for the upcoming offline simulation.
Another line of research has postulated that the posterior cerebellum might be strongly involved in the inhibition of the motor command during motor imagery9,22,84. This inhibitory mechanism is thought to prevent efferent impulses triggered through MI from reaching the medullar and muscle levels85. Therefore, we might also speculate that it is especially MI of physically trained sequences that requires such an increased inhibition of the motor command9,22,84,85.
On a phenomenological level, the specific representation might indeed result in different ways to imagine a movement (conceptualized as an experience-based prediction). Thus, imagers might create motor images based on their individual (motor) experience that affects the way they create the imagined action. Therefore, we suggest that the (motor) imaging human brain seems to access acquired (motor) experiences. The integrated multisensory experience of visual, spatial, and kinaesthetic action aspects is conditional on the upcoming motor images, because individuals ‘re-experience’ the action using their own experience-dependent representations.
One potential limitation of the present results is due to the nature of employing a within-subjects design in which all participants physically rehearsed several sequences, imagined another set of sequences, and had no practice with a third set of sequences. One possible criticism of using such a design to study differential learning effects is that MI and ME training does not occur in a purely MI or ME context. In other words, the different training modalities are not strictly compartmentalized, and training of sequences in one modality could influence the representation of similar sequences trained in the other. However, if this was the case, it would diminish differential effects of the practice modality, thereby making the substantial effect sizes we observed a conservative estimate. At the same time, the statistical power of a within design is superior, because it controls for confounding inter-individual differences. Furthermore, our design did not allow us to probe the effects of practice modality on motor representations during execution. Future experiments could include such a condition in the scanner to test this issue. A more detailed investigation of subjective imagery quality would also facilitate the testing of possible differential effects of practice modality on different sensory qualities of the motor images. Another interesting extension of the present design might be a pre–post approach, because this would offer the opportunity to qualify representational changes over time. Our main hypothesis, however, was that different training modalities (i.e. either a mental or a physical practice intervention) would lead to a distinguishable neural imprint during mental simulation of trained movements. In this regard, the present design is a very conservative approach to test the building up of specific representations by investigating a simulation that is considered to run on these representations.
Our findings imply that imagery-evoked neural activity in the posterior cerebellum depends on the practice modality. Practice-induced changes in the posterior cerebellum form a specific neural substrate that is used to process an offline motor state. Thus, even when you are only thinking of a movement, the involvement of your cerebellum will depend on whether you have practiced this movement mentally or physically. This has implications for practical applications of motor imagery. It will evoke a different representation, which is probably closer to a true simulation, if it is preceded by physical practice as opposed to imagery practice in isolation.
Material and methods
Subjects
Twenty-four volunteers (16 female, 8 male, Mage = 22.2 ± 2.6 years) participated in the present study. All were right-handed (EHI = 92.4 ± 9.9) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and no history or current use of any psychoactive medication. Subjects showed good initial imagery abilities for visual (externalpre = 2.22 ± 0.73; internalpre = 1.83 ± 0.53) as well as kinaesthetic (Mpre = 2.20 ± 0.68) perspectives as revealed by the Vividness of Motor Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ-25), in which lower scores reflect more vivid imagery experience. For further information on the investigated subjects, see supplementary materials. The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee of the Psychology and Sports Science Department of the University of Münster, Germany. All subjects gave written informed consent prior to the experiment in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Design and task
The experiment consisted of a two-week training intervention followed by an fMRI scan (Fig. 4). Subjects participated in seven training sessions, each lasting 30–40 min and scheduled for approximately every second day. In each of the seven training sessions, participants trained two sequences mentally and two sequences physically (for a detailed description of the training session, see supplementary materials).
The study included three mutually exclusive conditions: an MI condition in which sequences were practiced solely mentally without overt movements; an ME condition in which sequences were practiced physically on a target grid; and a control condition in which sequences were not practiced at all. Overall, six movement sequences were designed as aiming tasks in which the right hand moved across the target grid to touch six targets in a given order. Following a within design, all subjects were assigned two sequences for every condition. These sequences were permuted (in pairs) between participants and conditions. For the physical execution, a quadratic 55.5 × 55.5 cm target grid consisting of nine evenly arranged targets (Ø 9.7 cm; see Fig. 5B) was fixed to the wall at participants’ eye level.
During the scanner session, subjects imagined all six sequences regardless of which modality each sequence had been trained in beforehand. For the course of the intervention as well as the scanner session, they were instructed to keep their eyes closed while imagining the hand movements from a first-person perspective. This called for visual and kinaesthetic impressions similar to those present during actual execution as well as emphasizing naturally occurring eye movements.
Stimuli
The stimulus material consisted of six 6-s video clips showing hand movement sequences and six corresponding static arrow images—that is, one video and one static image were created for each sequence (Fig. 5A + B). All videos were filmed from a first-person perspective. Six targets were touched in each sequence with four adjacent targets and two targets that were further apart. The sequences were matched in terms of displacement path length, number of directional changes, and angular sum. The shown hand always started and returned to the same local position below the target grid and touched one of the outer dots to start a sequence. A PC running Presentation software (v19.0, Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, NY) was used during both training and the scanner session to present instructions and stimuli. Task progression was self-paced during the training sessions but pre-set for fMRI scanning. During the scanner session, only static arrow images were presented. Instructions and stimuli were projected onto a screen arranged outside the scanner that could be seen through an adjustable mirror attached to the head coil.
Scanner experiment
Participants performed eleven blocks of twelve trials each (corresponding to two trials in each of the six sequences), amounting to 132 trials and a 50-min scanner session overall. A 6-s rest condition was added that appeared twice every block. Its end was indicated by a sound signal. Sequences from all conditions were presented in a pseudo-randomized order counterbalanced across participants. Each block was introduced by a written instruction presented for 2.5 s (e.g., ‘Please imagine the following hand movement’) followed by a jitter (0–90% of TR) implemented by varying durations of presentation of a fixation cross (Fig. 5C). Static images of the sequences (6 s) or an instruction to rest (2.8 s) were presented followed by the respective imagery or rest phase (6 s). Participants closed their eyes in both conditions and reopened them only to receive instructions. Imagery duration was logged because subjects indicated the beginning and end of their imagery process by button presses when mentally touching the first and last target, again providing a manipulation check as well as information on possible systematic differences in duration of task engagement between training conditions.
Manipulation checks
In order to monitor subjects’ participation and progress throughout the training sessions, the following manipulation checks were implemented: Prior to the experiment and post-experiment, general motor imagery abilities were assessed using the VMIQ-25. Here, subjects visually or kinaesthetically imagined a set of movements and indicated the perceived vividness on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (perfectly clear and as vivid as normal vision) to 5 (no image at all).
After the first and last (7th) training session, subjects physically performed all six sequences so that the sound of hand–wall contacts could be recorded. This allowed accurate temporal identification of absolute duration as well as relative duration of the intervals within the performed sequence. Comparing these to the model provided in the videos revealed progress of temporal accuracy—that is, whether participants showed velocity or temporal proportion changes throughout the training sessions. In addition, possible transfer effects to non-trained sequences were evaluated. In order to assess the degree of temporal congruence, durations of imagery processes during the scanner session and duration of sequence execution as recorded in the post-behavioural session were matched for every individual, providing information on the quality of the temporal organization of the imagined hand movement as well as compliance with the instructions.
Because motor imagery has been shown to be accompanied by eye movements corresponding to the spatiotemporal evolution of imagined movements, electrooculography (EOG) data were acquired once during the first training session86. This made it possible to assess and record spatiotemporal patterns of eye movements objectively, thereby controlling active participation and compliance with task instructions and sequence specifics during initial imagery trials. This procedure further allowed us to give subjects feedback on whether they moved their eyes effectively; and it identified participants who did not do so automatically. In the latter case, they received additional instructions to make sure all participants would engage in comparable behaviour during the scanner session. Participants were also asked to rate perceived vividness of their imagery on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (very low) to 7 (very high) after every second training session as well as after the scanner session.
Image acquisition
fMRI data were collected on a Siemens Prisma 3-T whole-body scanner using a 20-channel head coil. A structural image was acquired from each participant consisting of 176 T1-weighted sagittal images (1-mm slice thickness; MPRAGE) and a field map (40 slices; TR = 1000 ms; short TE = 10 ms, long TE = 12.46 ms) using a double-echo gradient echo field map sequence. For functional imaging, 11 runs with 90 volumes per run (i.e. a total of 990 volumes) were registered using a T2*-weighted gradient echo-planar imaging sequence covering the whole brain with 40 slices (slice thickness = 3 mm; 0.75 mm gap; descending interleaved; TR = 2500 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 87 degrees; field of view = 210 mm × 210 mm). The orientation of the axial slices was parallel to the AC–PC line. Trial onsets were jittered within 0–90% of the TR Image.
Statistical analysis of behavioural data
Behavioural data on the temporal accuracy of execution were analysed using a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with time (pre-training and post-training) and condition (mentally trained, physically trained, and control) as within-subject factors. In addition, mental chronometry was assessed by running three separate linear regression analyses between imagery durations in the scanner as indicated by the push of a button and execution times of the post-behavioural session for the three experimental conditions. All behavioural data were analysed using SPSS Statistics (v. 24, IBM inc., Chicago, USA) and MATLAB (v. 2018a, MathWorks inc., Natick, MA). A Shapiro–Wilk test of normality preceded statistical analyses and Mauchly’s test of sphericity was implemented for ANOVAs. Additionally, the number of eye movements during imagery (i.e. EOG data) was monitored in BrainVision Analyzer (v. 2, Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). Given that participants were instructed to move their eyes corresponding to the imagined movement sequence, episodes of the imagined hand touching a target were indicated by short eye fixations at turning points. The number of these was compared to the model. Ideally, six fixations at turning points could be distinguished as part of the sequence. An additional directional change was identified when eyes moved back to the starting position.
fMRI analysis
Pre-processing
Before conducting statistical tests on fMRI data, spatial and temporal corrections were made in the course of pre-processing using SPM12. To begin with, all collected volumes were realigned to compensate for head movement in the scanner. Field maps were used to generate voxel displacement maps that were then used to unwarp distortions created by differences in susceptibility to the magnetic field. Furthermore, slice time correction was applied to control for slight differences in acquisition times. Functional data were then normalized to MNI space (Montreal Neurological Institute) and co-registered with individual structural images of the participants. As a last step, functional data were smoothed using a three-dimensional Gaussian kernel (9 mm full-width half-maximum [FWHM]).
For the cerebellar data, a specific normalization method was applied to allow a more accurate localization of activation within the small structures of the cerebellum. Because of the low contrast in the 152 ICBM template (MNI space) of the cerebellum, a whole brain normalization that is used as a standard in SPM12 would have led to a large spatial variance between participants87–90. Therefore, we used the template of the SUIT toolbox for SPM12 (Version 3.4) that is based on the average cerebellar anatomy of 20 participants. This procedure preserved the fine details of the cerebellum and improved the inter-subject alignment compared to the standard normalization87. In a first step, the automatic isolation algorithm provided by the toolbox was used to segregate the cerebellum and the brainstem. If necessary, the isolation maps were corrected manually based on anatomical information, and then these were normalized to the SUIT template via a nonlinear transformation. The resultant deformation maps were used to normalize the functional images of each participant. In contrast to the whole brain data, in which normalization and the ensuing smoothing were performed before the first‐level analysis, in the SUIT normalization, these steps were conducted after the functional data had been analysed on the single-subject level. On the second level, the whole brain and the cerebellar data were analysed in exactly the same way.
General linear model and contrasts
First, the six different imagined sequences were matched to the experimental conditions (i.e. mentally trained, physically trained, or control) based on the permutation protocol. Then, first-level analysis was computed using separate general linear models (GLMs) for each subject. Each GLM consists of all eleven runs. For each run, seven boxcar regressors were created corresponding to the six sequences (duration was adjusted to the individual button presses in the scanner) and rest conditions. In addition, the stimulus presentation phase and button presses (with an approximated duration of 100 ms) were included as regressors of no interest. Further regressors were obtained from an in-house Fmri Artefact Correction Tool that identifies outlier volumes due to motion during the scan of a volume. Hence, the detection of outlier volumes was based on a comparison of each volume with its two neighbours in a motion‐corrected time series. This procedure was performed by calculating the mean-squared differences to the previous and the next volume. The smaller difference was used as the outlier score for each volume. Scores were thresholded using Hubert and van der Veeken's91 method of calculating a skewness‐corrected interquartile range. To threshold outlier scores, the range was multiplied by 1.5 and added to the 75th percentile.
Each regressor was convoluted with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Moreover, six movement parameters from the rigid-body transformation of the motion-correction procedure were entered as covariates. The voxel-based time series were filtered with a high-pass filter (time constant = 128 s).
Then, several contrasts were computed. Practice conditions were contrasted to rest (mental > rest, physical > rest, and control > rest, imagery [including all training conditions] > rest) as well as compared to each other (mental < > physical, mental < > control, and physical < > control). These contrast images were fed into a second-level analysis in order to conduct random effects, group-average analysis. Furthermore, a conjunction analysis identified voxels that were active in all three conditions (mental > rest ∩ physical > rest ∩ control > rest). Significance was assessed via whole-brain analysis (p < 0.05, FWE-corrected) and anatomical locations were identified based on MNI coordinates using the Anatomy Toolbox (Version 2.2b92) for SPM12. This toolbox was used to label all activations on the basis of cytoarchitectonic probability maps. Significant results within the cerebellum were assigned to the cerebellar lobuli by means of the probabilistic atlas included in the Anatomy toolbox88.
For all contrasts comparing the different training conditions against each other, a small-volume correction was conducted with a priori search volumes. These ROIs were selected on the basis of previous findings reported in the literature7 as well activation sites observed for the conjunction of all imagery contrasts against rest (mental > rest ∩ physical > rest ∩ control > rest): the superior parietal lobe, the posterior cerebellum, and the precentral gyrus. The cerebral ROIs were defined and masks for small-volume correction were created with probabilistic maps based on cytoarchitectonic data92. The cerebellar masks were based on the probabilistic atlas of the cerebellum provided by Diedrichsen et al.88. Significance was tested on the voxel level (p < 0.05, family-wise error [FWE]-corrected). We further calculated effect size maps using Cohen’s d for our main contrast of interest (physical > mental).
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank Jonathan Harrow for native speaker advice and all our subjects for participating in this study. DFG Project No. 222641018 SFB/TRR 135, TP A8 and ERC Starting Grant INDIVISUAL (852882) supported BdH.
Author contributions
Mainly K.Z. and M.H. designed the study with advice from B.K., J.M., B.dH.; M.H. conducted the tests, fMRI scanning, and the training interventions together with K.Z.; M.H., A.Z., and B.K. analysed the data; BK drafted the first version of the manuscript; K.Z., M.H., A.Z., B.dH., and J.M. reviewed the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-76214-y.
References
- 1.Gallese V. The roots of empathy. The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology. 2003;36:171–180. doi: 10.1159/000072786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gallese V. Embodied simulation: from neurons to phenomenal experience. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2005;4:23–48. doi: 10.1007/s11097-005-4737-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wilson M. Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2002;9:625–636. doi: 10.3758/BF03196322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Svensson H, Lindblom J, Ziemke T. In Body, Language and Mind (eds. Ziemke, T. et al.) (Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, New York, 2008).
- 5.Roberts R, Callow N, Hardy L, Markland D, Bringer J. Movement imagery ability: development and assessment of a revised version of the vividness of movement imagery questionnaire. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2008;30:200–221. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.2.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Guillot A, et al. Functional neuroanatomical networks associated with expertise in motor imagery. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1471–1483. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hardwick RM, Caspers S, Eickhoff SB, Swinnen SP. Neural correlates of action: Comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;94:31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hetu S, et al. The neural network of motor imagery: an ALE meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013;37:930–949. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lotze M, et al. Activation of cortical and cerebellar motor areas during executed and imagined hand movements: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999;11:491–501. doi: 10.1162/089892999563553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Munzert J, Lorey B, Zentgraf K. Cognitive motor processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Res. Rev. 2009;60:306–326. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.12.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zabicki A, et al. Imagined and executed actions in the human motor system: testing neural similarity between execution and imagery of actions with a multivariate approach. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:4523–4536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rizzolatti G, Matelli M. Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions. Exp. Brain Res. 2003;153:146–157. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1588-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Winstein C, Grafton ST, Pohl PS. Motor task difficulty and brain activity: Investigation of goal-directed reciprocal aiming using positron emission tomography. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;77:1581–1594. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wolpert DM, Goodbody SJ, Husain M. Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 1998;1:529–533. doi: 10.1038/2245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Miall RC. Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. NeuroReport. 2003;14:2135–2137. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000098751.87269.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Blakemore S-J, Sirigu A. Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobe. Exp. Brain Res. 2003;153:239–245. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1597-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Desmurget M, Grafton S. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000;4:423–431. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01537-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Johansson RS, Flanagan JR. Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:345–359. doi: 10.1038/nrn2621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wolpert DM, Miall RC. Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 1996;9:1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR. Motor prediction. Curr. Biol. 2001;11:R729–R732. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kilteni K, Andersson BJ, Houborg C, Ehrsson HH. Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:1617. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03989-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lotze M, Halsband U. Motor imagery. J. Physiol. 2006;99:386–395. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Murphy SM. Imagery interventions in sport. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1994;26:486–494. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199404000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Feltz DL, Landers DM. The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: a meta-analysis. J. Sport Psychol. 1983;5:25–57. doi: 10.1123/jsp.5.1.25. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schuster C, et al. Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Med. 2011;9:75. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yágüez L, et al. A mental route to motor learning: improving trajectorial kinematics through imagery training. Behav. Brain Res. 1998;90:95–106. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00087-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yue G, Cole KJ. Strength increases from the motor program: comparison of training with maximal voluntary and imagined muscle contractions. J. Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1114–1123. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.5.1114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gentili R, Han CE, Schweighofer N, Papaxanthis C. Motor learning without doing: trial-by-trial improvement in motor performance during mental training. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:774–783. doi: 10.1152/jn.00257.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lacourse MG, Turner JA, Randolph-Orr E, Schandler SL, Cohen MJ. Cerebral and cerebellar sensorimotor plasticity following motor imagery-based mental practice of a sequential movement. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 2004;41:505–524. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2004.04.0505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Pascual-Leone A, et al. Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. J. Neurophysiol. 1995;74:1037–1045. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.1037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lakoff G, Johnson M. Philosophy in the Flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nyberg L, Eriksson J, Larsson A, Marklund P. Learning by doing versus learning by thinking: An fMRI study of motor and mental training. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:711–717. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards CL, Doyon J. Functional cerebral reorganization following motor sequence learning through mental practice with motor imagery. NeuroImage. 2003;20:1171–1180. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00369-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lacourse MG, Orr ELR, Cramer SC, Cohen MJ. Brain activation during execution and motor imagery of novel and skilled sequential hand movements. NeuroImage. 2005;27:505–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lafleur MF, et al. Motor learning produces parallel dynamic functional changes during the execution and imagination of sequential foot movements. NeuroImage. 2002;16:142–157. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zhang H, et al. Behavioral improvements and brain functional alterations by motor imagery training. Brain Res. 2011;1407:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bernard JA, Seidler RD. Cerebellar contributions to visuomotor adaptation and motor sequence learning: an ALE meta-analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:27. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Diedrichsen J, Verstynen T, Lehman SL, Ivry RB. Cerebellar involvement in anticipating the consequences of self-produced actions during bimanual movements. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;93:801–812. doi: 10.1152/jn.00662.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Imamizu H, Kawato M. Cerebellar internal models: implications for the dexterous use of tools. Cerebellum (London, England) 2012;11:325–335. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0241-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.O’Reilly JX, Mesulam MM, Nobre AC. The cerebellum predicts the timing of perceptual events. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2008;28:2252–2260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2742-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Scheidt RA, et al. Remembering forward: neural correlates of memory and prediction in human motor adaptation. NeuroImage. 2012;59:582–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Taig E, Küper M, Theysohn N, Timmann D, Donchin O. Deficient use of visual information in estimating hand position in cerebellar patients. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2012;32:16274–16284. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1153-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lebon F, Byblow WD, Collet C, Guillot A, Stinear CM. The modulation of motor cortex excitability during motor imagery depends on imagery quality. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012;35:323–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07938.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lorey B, et al. Activation of the parieto-premotor network is associated with vivid motor imagery—a parametric fMRI study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e20368. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zabicki A, et al. Subjective vividness of motor imagery has a neural signature in human premotor and parietal cortex. NeuroImage. 2019;197:273–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Heremans E, et al. Keeping an eye on imagery: the role of eye movements during motor imagery training. Neuroscience. 2011;195:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.07.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Munzert, J. & Zentgraf, K. In Mind and Motion: The Bidirectional Link between Thought and Action, Vol. 1 (eds. Heekeren, H. et al.) 219–229 (Elsevier textbooks, s.l., 2009).
- 48.Jeannerod M. Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage. 2001;14:S103–109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hanakawa T, et al. Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;89:989–1002. doi: 10.1152/jn.00132.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hugdahl K. Overlapping areas of neuronal activation after motor and mental imagery training. Front. Neurosci. 2009;3:5–6. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.008.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Olsson C-J, Jonsson B, Nyberg L. Internal imagery training in active high jumpers. Scand. J. Psychol. 2008;49:133–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00625.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Filimon F, Nelson JD, Hagler DJ, Sereno MI. Human cortical representations for reaching: mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery. NeuroImage. 2007;37:1315–1328. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Filimon F, Rieth CA, Sereno MI, Cottrell GW. Observed, executed, and imagined action representations can be decoded from ventral and dorsal areas. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25:3144–3158. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lorey B, et al. How equivalent are the action execution, imagery, and observation of intransitive movements? Revisiting the concept of somatotopy during action simulation. Brain Cogn. 2013;81:139–150. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Stippich C, Ochmann H, Sartor K. Somatotopic mapping of the human primary sensorimotor cortex during motor imagery and motor execution by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci. Lett. 2002;331:50–54. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00826-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Andersen, R. A. In Comprehensive Physiology, (eds. Terjung, R.) (Wiley, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011).
- 57.Andersen RA, Buneo CA. Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;25:189–220. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Desmurget M, et al. Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science. 2009;324:811–813. doi: 10.1126/science.1169896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Graziano MSA, Gross CG. Spatial maps for the control of movement. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1998;8:195–201. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80140-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kalaska JF, Scott SH, Cisek P, Sergio LE. Cortical control of reaching movements. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1997;7:849–859. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80146-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Dimitrova A, et al. Probabilistic 3D MRI atlas of the human cerebellar dentate/interposed nuclei. NeuroImage. 2006;30:12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Grodd W, Hülsmann E, Lotze M, Wildgruber D, Erb M. Sensorimotor mapping of the human cerebellum: fMRI evidence of somatotopic organization. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001;13:55–73. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.O’Reilly JX, Beckmann CF, Tomassini V, Ramnani N, Johansen-Berg H. Distinct and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting state functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex (New York, N. Y.: 1991) 2010;20:953–965. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage. 2009;44:489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Guillot A, et al. Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2157–2172. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Krüger B, Zabicki A, Grosse L, Naumann T, Munzert J. Sensory features of mental images in the framework of human actions. Conscious. Cogn. 2020;83:102970. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.102970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Gerardin E, et al. Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:1093–1104. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Nair DG, Purcott KL, Fuchs A, Steinberg F, Kelso JAS. Cortical and cerebellar activity of the human brain during imagined and executed unimanual and bimanual action sequences: a functional MRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 2003;15:250–260. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00197-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Stephan KM, et al. Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 1995;73:373–386. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.1.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Suchan B, et al. Neural correlates of visuospatial imagery. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;131:163–168. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00373-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Lorey B, et al. Your mind’s hand: motor imagery of pointing movements with different accuracy. NeuroImage. 2010;49:3239–3247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kawato M. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1999;9:718–727. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)00028-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bastian AJ. Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006;16:645–649. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Imamizu H, et al. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature. 2000;403:192–195. doi: 10.1038/35003194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Miall RC, King D. State estimation in the cerebellum. Cerebellum (London, England) 2008;7:572–576. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0072-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Synofzik M, Vosgerau G, Newen A. Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Conscious. Cogn. 2008;17:219–239. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Wolfensteller, U. In Mind and Motion: The Bidirectional Link between Thought and Action, Vol. 1 (eds. Heekeren, H. et al.) 289–301 (Elsevier textbooks, s.l., 2009).
- 78.Ghajar J, Ivry RB. The predictive brain state: timing deficiency in traumatic brain injury? Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2008;22:217–227. doi: 10.1177/1545968308315600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Strick PL, Dum RP, Fiez JA. Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;32:413–434. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Balser N, et al. The influence of expertise on brain activation of the action observation network during anticipation of tennis and volleyball serves. Front Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:568. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Iacoboni M. Playing tennis with the cerebellum. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:555–556. doi: 10.1038/88365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Calvo-Merino B, Grezes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P. Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr. Biol. 2006;16:1905–1910. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Heuer H. Wie wirkt mentale Übung? Psychologische Rundschau. 1985;36:191–200. [Google Scholar]
- 84.Cengiz B, Boran HE. The role of the cerebellum in motor imagery. Neurosci. Lett. 2016;617:156–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.01.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Jeannerod M. Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:1419–1432. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00073-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Heremans E, Helsen WF, Feys P. The eyes as a mirror of our thoughts: quantification of motor imagery of goal-directed movements through eye movement registration. Behav. Brain Res. 2008;187:351–360. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Diedrichsen J. A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2006;33:127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, Ramnani N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2009;46:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Diedrichsen J, et al. Imaging the deep cerebellar nuclei: a probabilistic atlas and normalization procedure. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1786–1794. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Diedrichsen J, Zotow E. Surface-based display of volume-averaged cerebellar imaging data. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0133402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Hubert M, van der Veeken S. Outlier detection for skewed data. J. Chemom. 2008;22:235–246. doi: 10.1002/cem.1123. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Eickhoff SB, et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.