Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Nov 9;15(11):e0241991. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241991

Diurnal variation of motor activity in adult ADHD patients analyzed with methods from graph theory

Ole Bernt Fasmer 1,2,3,*, Erlend Eindride Fasmer 4, Kristin Mjeldheim 5, Wenche Førland 6, Vigdis Elin Giæver Syrstad 1,2,3, Petter Jakobsen 1,3, Jan Øystein Berle 1,2, Tone E G Henriksen 2,7, Zahra Sepasdar 8, Erik R Hauge 1, Ketil J Oedegaard 1,2,3
Editor: Pan Lin9
PMCID: PMC7652335  PMID: 33166350

Abstract

Attention-deficit /hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental syndrome characterized by age-inappropriate levels of motor activity, impulsivity and attention. The aim of the present study was to study diurnal variation of motor activity in adult ADHD patients, compared to healthy controls and clinical controls with mood and anxiety disorders. Wrist-worn actigraphs were used to record motor activity in a sample of 81 patients and 30 healthy controls. Time series from registrations in the morning and evening were analyzed using measures of variability, complexity and a newly developed method, the similarity algorithm, based on transforming time series into graphs. In healthy controls the evening registrations showed higher variability and lower complexity compared to morning registrations, however this was evident only in the female controls. In the two patient groups the same measures were not significantly different, with one exception, the graph measure bridges. This was the measure that most clearly separated morning and evening registrations and was significantly different both in healthy controls and in patients with a diagnosis of ADHD. These findings suggest that actigraph registrations, combined with mathematical methods based on graph theory, may be used to elucidate the mechanisms responsible for the diurnal regulation of motor activity.

Introduction

Attention-deficit /hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental psychiatric syndrome characterized by age-inappropriate levels of motor activity, impulsivity and attention [1]. Other symptoms, like sleep problems and drug abuse are also prevalent, and co-morbidity with mood and anxiety disorders is the rule rather than the exception in patients with this disorder [13].

A high proportion of adolescents with ADHD maintain the syndrome symptoms in adulthood [1]. In addition, a significant proportion of ADHD patients are first diagnosed as adults, indicating that the developmental trajectory of this syndrome is variable [4]. ADHD in adulthood is associated with adverse occupational outcomes, impaired somatic and psychiatric health, as well as high costs for social and health services [4, 5].

Dysregulated activation is central phenomenon in ADHD [6] and for patients with mood disorders [7]. Objective measures of activity, such as actigraphy may be used to gain more knowledge about the dynamics of activation patterns. The core symptom in ADHD, hyperactivity is diagnostically based on behavioural observations [8], but increased activity levels have been objectively documented by use of actigraphs [9, 10] and infrared motion analyses [11]. Hyperactivity subsides with age [1, 8], but still a study in adults found increased activity to be a more discriminative feature than either inattention or impulsivity [11]. However, objective registrations of activity levels in adult patients have given mixed results [9, 1113], and there is limited information regarding a more detailed analysis of motor activity patterns in ADHD.

Teicher reported that actigraphically recorded hyperactivity was caused by the absence of quiet periods rather than to periods with overtly increased activity [9]. In a previous report from the same adult clinical sample as in the present paper we found that ADHD patients did not show evidence of hyperactivity, but rather had levels of mean activity similar to normal controls. However, using Fourier analysis with 300 min recordings the ADHD patients had higher power in the high frequency range, corresponding to the period from 2–8 min, and also lower lag1 autocorrelation [14].

In the present paper we further examined the motor activity of adult ADHD patients, using wrist-worn actigraphs, but now with a focus on diurnal variation. Actigraphy has previously mostly been used to assess sleep but is now increasingly being used to record daytime activity and the 24-h rest-activity cycle in patients with psychiatric disorders [15, 16]. In normal controls actigraphically recorded motor activity display a clear diurnal variation (morning vs. evening), both for activity level and variability [16]. ADHD is characterized by increased intraindividual variability both in patients and in animal models of ADHD [17, 18], but in several test systems the diurnal variation of ADHD patients seems to be less than in controls [19, 20].

Biological systems can seldom be described using simple linear models. We have therefore previously employed methods obtained from the fields of non-linear systems and complexity theory to analyze actigraph recordings [14, 21, 22]. In a recent paper we also used a method based on graph theory to analyze time series with motor activity. With this method, called the similarity graph algorithm, time series are mapped into graphs, and the properties of the graphs are analyzed, using familiar techniques from graph theory. In this way more features of the activity can be analyzed. Several methods have been developed in recent years to analyze time series using similar techniques, such as the visibility graph [23], and the horizontal visibility graph methods [24], and these have been applied to diverse fields, ranging from seismology [25] to the study of human walking rhythm [26].

The aim of the present study was to study diurnal variation of motor activity in adult ADHD patients, by comparing actigraphically recorded time series from morning and evening registrations using the similarity graph method as well as traditional mathematical models. Two comparison groups are used, patients with mood and anxiety disorders without a diagnosis of ADHD and normal controls. The hypothesis was that patients with ADHD have a more static activity pattern from morning to evening than controls.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

The Norwegian Regional Medical Research Ethics Committee West approved the study protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Subjects

Patients were recruited from the private psychiatric practice of KM and WF, both certified psychiatrists with long clinical experience. The patients were consecutive new referrals, in need of diagnostic evaluation of either ADHD, or mood/anxiety disorders, and age between 17 and 65 years. Exclusion criteria were inability to speak Norwegian and not being able to comply with the study protocol. A total of 104 patients were recruited. For different reasons (logistics problems, patients forgetting to wear the actigraphs, technical failures) we were not able to obtain complete recordings for all the patients. We were able to analyze a total of 81 recordings, and these are reported on in the present paper. The group consisted of 38 women and 43 men, and the average age was 37.7 ± 11.0 years (mean ± SD), range 17–61. Most of the patients (n = 55) used no psychotropic drugs, of the rest 15 used antidepressants, two lithium, and three other mood stabilizers (lamotrigine or valproate). One patient used a small dose of an antipsychotic drug (olanzapine 2.5 mg), and three used hypnotics or benzodiazepines. Patients using drugs at referral continued unchanged with these during the actigraph recordings.

The control group consisted of 20 women and 10 men, average age 38.7 ± 13.1 years, range 21–66, medical students (n = 5), patients without serious medical or psychiatric symptoms from a primary care office (n = 4) and employees from Bergen University and a psychiatric nursing home (n = 21). None of the control subjects had a history of mood or psychotic disorders. The controls were recruited during a separate study, using the same actigraph equipment as the patients in this study, and are reported on in two previous papers [21, 27].

Psychiatric assessment

For all diagnostic assessments of the patients a standard clinical interview was used, supplemented when possible with information from collateral sources. The interviews were performed by either KM or WF. The following assessment instruments were also used:

The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINIPlus, version 5.0.0), a module based semi-structured interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnoses [28, 29].

Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), a standard instrument for the assessment of depression [30].

The Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS). This is the World Health Organization`s rating scale designed to measure current symptoms of ADHD in adults. It consists of 18 questions following the DSM-IV criteria for ADHD, with a 5 point scale of 0–4 (0 = never, 4 = very often), and thus a possible range of scores from 0–72. Items 1–9 cover symptoms of inattention and 10–18 hyperactivity and impulsivity [31, 32].

Wender Utah Rating Scale, the 25 questions version (WURS-25), a self-rating scale designed to assess symptoms and signs of ADHD in childhood, using a Likert scale of 0–4 (0 = never, 4 = very often), yielding a possible range of scores 0–100 [33]. WURS-25 has been used in previous studies in Norway [31].

Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ). This is a screening instrument for bipolar disorder. It is a self-report form that consists of 13 questions scored “Yes” of “No”. Positive answer to at least seven questions and confirmation that the symptoms have occurred together and caused problems is suggestive of a bipolar disorder [31, 34].

Cyclothymic temperament scale is a self-report form consisting of 21 questions covering the cyclothymic temperament according to the definition of Akiskal. This scale is part of the larger TEMPS-A auto-questionnaire [3537].

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). This is a self-assessment form for evaluating current depression and anxiety, and has been extensively used, also in Norway [38].

The final diagnostic evaluation, based on DSM-IV criteria, was made after an assessment of all available information, and discussion of each case.

Recording of motor activity

Motor activity was monitored with an actigraph worn at the right wrist (Actiwatch, Cambridge Neurotechnology Ltd, England). In the actigraph, activity is measured by using a piezoelectric accelerometer that is programmed to record the integration of intensity, amount and duration of movement in all directions. The sampling frequency is 32 Hz and movements over 0.05 g are recorded. A corresponding voltage is produced and is stored as an activity count in the memory unit of the actigraph. The number of counts is proportional to the intensity of the movement. The right wrist was chosen to make the procedure more convenient for the participants, since most of them have their watches around the left wrist and it is cumbersome to have two such devices on the same arm. Previous studies have shown that there are only small differences between the right and left wrist [39, 40].

Total activity counts were recorded for one-minute intervals for a period of six days. The first continuous 24 hour period (0000–2359) for each participant (without missing data due to the actigraph having been removed) was used. From this period we selected morning and evening epochs, each with a 6 hour duration. Morning epochs were defined to occur between 8 AM and 2 PM, and evening epochs between 6 PM and midnight.

An example of actigraph recording is shown in Fig 1.

Fig 1. This is an example of an actigraph registration, motor activity over three 24 hour periods.

Fig 1

Mathematical analyses

For all the time epochs we calculated the mean activity, the standard deviation (SD), expressed as percent of the mean, the root mean square successive differences (RMSSD), also expressed as percent of the mean, and the ratio RMSSD/SD.

In addition we calculated the sample entropy of the time series and a number of measures based on a transformation of the time series into graphs.

Sample entropy

Sample entropy is a nonlinear measure, indicating the degree of regularity (complexity) of time series, and is the negative natural logarithm of an estimate of the conditional probability that subseries of a certain length (m = 2) that match point-wise, within a tolerance (r = 0.2), also match at the next point. Sample entropy has been employed for the analysis of different types of biological data since it can be employed with comparatively short time series (>50) and is robust with regard to outliers [41].

Graph theory

Graph theory is one of the most important and useful branches of mathematics. It is concerned with the study of graphs, which are mathematical structures that model relations between objects.

All definitions in this section can be found in these two references [42, 43]. A graph G is an ordered pair (V(G), E(G)) consisting of a set V(G) of nodes and a set E(G), disjoint from V(G), of edges, together with an incidence function ψG that associates with each edge of G an unordered pair of (not necessarily distinct) nodes of G. If e is an edge and u and v are nodes such that ψG(e) = {u, v}, then e is said to join u and v, and the nodes u and v are called the ends of e. For simplicity, we write V and E instead of V(G) and E(G).

A graph may exhibit many different topological properties, of which we will only mention the ones relevant to this article. Most of the definitions and concepts in graph theory are suggested by this graphical representation. The ends of an edge are said to be incident with the edge, and vice versa. Two nodes which are incident with a common edge are adjacent, as are two edges which are incident with a common node, and two distinct adjacent nodes are neighbours.

The degree of a node v in a graph G, denoted by d(v), is the number of edges of G incident with v. If G is a simple graph, d(v) is the number of neighbours of v in G.

A graph H is called a subgraph of a graph G if V(H) ⊆ V(G), E(H) ⊆ E(G), and ψG is the restriction of ψG E(H).

A path is a graph whose nodes can be arranged in a linear sequence in such a way that two nodes are adjacent if they are consecutive in the sequence and are nonadjacent otherwise. Two nodes u and v in a graph G are connected if G contains a u-v path. The graph G itself is connected if every two nodes of G are connected.

A connected subgraph H of a graph G is a connected component of G if H is not a proper subgraph of a connected subgraph of G.

Let e be an edge of a graph G. If G-e has more connected components than G, then e is a bridge of G.

A complete graph is a simple graph in which any two nodes are adjacent. A complete subgraph of G is called a clique of G. A k-clique is a clique with k nodes.

Big O notation is used in computer science to group algorithms according to the growth rate of their running times or space requirements as functions of the input size. We will only consider the running times of the algorithms described in this paper. Although the notation has a precise mathematical definition, the O notation for a function f is usually derived with the following two simplification rules. 1. If f is a sum of several terms, only the one with the largest growth rate is kept. 2. If f is a product of several factors, any constants that do not depend on the input can be removed. To describe the running time of an algorithm with the big O notation is to give an upper bound on its growth rate as a function of the input size. To give some examples, O(1) is the class of algorithms with constant running time, O(log n) is the class of algorithms with logarithmic running time, and O(n) is the class of algorithms with linear running time. Generally, n represents the number of nodes when it comes to graph algorithms, that is, n = |V|.

Similarity graph

In this study we apply a heuristic algorithm that is nonlinear and not chaos-based that transforms a time series S = (x1, x2, …, xn) into a similarity graph G = (V,E), an undirected graph, where each node uV = {1, 2, …, n} corresponds to the element xu ∈ S and where the node u is assigned a weight equal to the value of xu [44]. The distance between two nodes u and v, is |uv|. Two arbitrary nodes u and v are defined to be direct neighbours if their distance is 1. Two nodes u and v are said to be similar to each other (that is, they have a symmetric relationship) if max (xu, xv) / min (xu, xv) < 1.2. This definition sees the world from the perspective of both u and v. The undirected similarity graph is constructed as follows. We introduce an undirected edge between two nodes if and only if they are similar to each other and their distance is below a certain threshold k. The k leftmost and the k rightmost nodes are disregarded when counting the number of neighbors of each node such that the node of every considered value in S may have as much as 2k neighbors. Different values of k are giving different similarity graphs.

The choice of 20% as a threshold for defining two points as similar is based on our previous studies of motor activity with the sample entropy method [21, 41]. The sample entropy method is also based on finding points in time series that are similar to one another, and it is customary to use 20% of the standard deviation for defining two points as similar. With the similarity graph method we do not employ the standard deviation, but use 20% of the value of the time points. The standard deviation of these time series from actigraph recordings are usually large, in the order of 100% of the mean, meaning that 20% of the amplitude of an average time point roughly corresponds to 20% of the standard deviation, and we have therefore chosen 20% to define similarity.

We think it is more reasonable to use a criterion that is dependent on actual values of the time series instead of using a fixed value for the whole series (as in sample entropy). One problem with sample entropy is that it is sensitive to outliers, which will increase the standard deviation and thus increase the probability that two points are considered similar, giving a false impression of high regularity. We therefore think it makes more sense to use our definition similarity. The consequence will be that number of points considered similar will be lower, thus avoiding ceiling effects, and will probably increase the ability of the test to detect differences between groups. This will be the case for all the graph measures we use.

The rationale for using a threshold k as opposed to always considering connecting a node to every other node of the graph is to compare a value in the time series to its nearest past and nearest future in order to obtain a number (i.e. the number of neighbors) designating how much a given value changes compared to its nearest past and future. The higher degree a certain node has, the more similar its weight is to its k preceding and k subsequent nodes. The lower degree the node has, the more different its value is from its preceding and subsequent values in the corresponding time series. A node with few or no adjacencies indicates a jump in the activity level, either from low to high activity or vice versa. Repeating the algorithm for different values of k, gives different similarity graphs, which may reveal different properties of the underlying time series. Another kind of activity jump is revealed by the graph forming connected components and this implies that two periods of time each have smooth changes in activity internally but that the activity changes in one of them are significantly different from the activity changes in the other one. Yet another kind of activity jump is revealed by the graph forming bridges and these are structures that point in the same direction as connected components but they are not as strong. A graph with one bridge is “almost” two connected components. The removal of the bridge would result in the graph forming two connected components. Thus bridges reveal activity jumps that are significant but still not as significant as those formed by connected components. Finally another kind of activity jump is revealed by a pair of direct neighbors in an undirected graph missing a relationship meaning that the activity level in one time interval is significantly smaller or larger than the other. The number of k-cliques reveal how smooth the activity changes are. The more k-cliques the graph exhibits, the more smooth the activity changes are. We have focused on 3-cliques in this article. Every 3-clique reveals that a tuple of three nodes have similar values. The running time of the algorithm is O(|V|) [45]. We have computed the connected components of the graph with a depth-first search in O(|E|) time [45]. The 3-cliques are computed with a O(a(G)m) time algorithm designed by Chiba and Nishizeki, where a(G) is the arboricity of the graph [46].

In our previous paper on graph analyses of motor activity of patients with schizophrenic and depression [44], using 300 min sequences, values of k from 20 up to 80 seemed to give comparable results. In preliminary analyses of the present dataset we found that 40 + 40 neighbours gave the best separation between morning and evening sequences for the control group using the different graph measures. We have therefore presented the results for analyses based on 40 + 40 neighbours, and we have calculated the following measures: The number of edges, components, bridges, cliques (3-cliques), the maximum number of edges, the number of nodes with zero edges (i.e. connected components consisting of a single node) and the number of missing edges between nearest neighbors. The values for cliques are transformed to ln cliques. For number of bridges we have also calculated the values using different number of edges, from 2 + 2 and up to 80 + 80.

An example of a similarity graph is shown in Fig 2.

Fig 2. This is an example of a k = 2 time series (five poles) converted to a graph (five dots, below, with edges as solid lines).

Fig 2

This also illustrates the concepts of components (two components; the first consist of 1,2,3 and 5, the second of 4), bridges (one bridge, the edge between 1 and 3) and cliques (one 3-clique: 2,3 and 5).

Statistics

Independent samples or paired samples t-test were used to compare two groups with continuous data and Chi square test for categorical data. One-way ANOVA was used to evaluate differences between three groups, with post hoc Bonferroni tests. The effect of age on cyclothymic temperament (CT)-scores was evaluated using Pearson correlations, and Pearson correlations were also calculated for the relation between bridges, SD and sample entropy. The effects of age and gender on the differences between groups and on differences between morning and evening registrations were evaluated using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Effect sizes (Cohens d) are indicated where relevant. Linear mixed models (LMM) were employed to test for interaction between time (morning vs. evening) and diagnosis (ADHD or not ADHD vs. healthy controls). SPSS version 25 was used for the statistical analyses.

Results

Table 1 shows the clinical characteristics of the clinical sample according to the presence or not of ADHD. Fortytwo patients received a diagnosis of ADHD while 39 patients did not have ADHD. Age and gender distribution were not significantly different between the groups, the only differences were in higher WURS- and ASRS-scores in the ADHD patients.

Table 1. Characteristics of the clinical sample according to the presence or not of ADHD.

ADHD (n = 42)* Not ADHD (n = 39)* P
Age (mean ± SD) 37.4 ± 10.6 38.0 ± 11.5 0.801
Gender (m/f) 24/18 19/20 0.448
WURS 51.1 ± 19.7 29.4 ± 15.9 <0.001
ASRS 47.6 ± 13.1 33.5 ± 12.4 <0.001
HADS
Depression 4.5 ± 3.8 5.0 ± 4.1 0.527
Anxiety 9.5 ± 4.6 9.19 ± 4.7 0.724
MADRS 13.5 ± 7.6 13.7 ± 8.3 0.928
MDQ 6.8 ± 3.9 6.4 ± 3.6 0.396
CT 11.6 ± 4.7 11.6 ± 3.9 0.974
CT ≥11 57% (18/42) 61% (22/36) 0.722

Chi-square test for gender and CT ≥11, for the other measures independent samples t-test.

* Number of subjects varies somewhat between the different measures, n = 40 to 42 for ADHD and n = 35 to 39 for not ADHD.

Table 2 shows the results of actigraph recordings in the morning. With one-way ANOVA three measures showed differences between the three groups, SD (p = 0.046), RMSSD (p = 0.015) and missing edges (p = 0.008). After Bonferroni corrections two measures were different between ADHD and healthy controls, RMMSD and number of missing edges, and for both measures the values were higher in the ADHD group. For the evening registrations (Table 3) two measures were different between groups, the RMSSD/SD ratio (p = 0.027) and bridges (p = 0.047). After Bonferroni corrections only RMSSD/SD was different between ADHD and healthy controls (higher value in the ADHD group). Table 4 shows differences between morning and evening registrations, separately for healthy controls and ADHD patients. For healthy controls 9 of 12 measures differed between morning and evening registrations, while for ADHD patients only one measure, bridges, was different. The strongest difference, measured with effect size, Cohens d, between morning and evening registrations was for bridges in the healthy controls (Cohens d 1.34). In ADHD patients this value was 0.83. Using linear mixed model analyses to test for interactions between diagnosis (healthy controls vs. ADHD) and time (morning vs. evening) three measures were significant, mean activity level (p = 0.039), SD (p = 0.039) and sample entropy (p = 0.014).

Table 2. Actigraphic registrations in the morning, 360 min (08–14).

Controls and the clinical group divided according to the presence or not of ADHD. For the graph analyses the number of neighbours is 40 + 40.

Controls ADHD Not ADHD ANOVA
(n = 30) (n = 42) (n = 39)
Mean 385 ± 178 289 ± 152 312 ± 192 F (108,2) = 2.774, P = 0.067
SD (% of mean) 105 ± 31 131 ± 49 126 ± 50 F (108,2) = 3.167, P = 0.046
RMSSD (% of mean) 86 ± 17 110 ± 37* 107 ± 42 F (108,2) = 4.484, P = 0.013
RMSSD/SD 0.85 ± 0.12 0.86 ± 0.16 0.86 ± 0.13 F (108,2) = 0.132, P = 0.876
Edges 7.94 ± 2.80 6.49 ± 2.96 7.07 ± 3.52 F (108,2) = 1.879, P = 0.158
Components 104 ± 35 135 ± 68 132 ± 68 F (108,2) = 2.613, P = 0.078
Bridges 38.6 ± 8.5 36.1 ± 11.5 36.6 ± 8.6 F (108,2) = 0.610, P = 0.545
Missing edges 309 ± 14 321 ± 16* 318 ± 17 F (108,2) = 5.075, P = 0.008
Max number of edges 21.8 ± 5.1 20.8 ± 7.1 22.0 ± 7.7 F (108,2) = 0.359, P = 0.699
Nodes with zero edges 117 ± 34 144 ± 62 143 ± 62 F (108,2) = 2.579, P = 0.081
Ln cliques 7.69 ± 0.68 7.32 ± 0.90 7.52 ± 1.03 F (108,2) = 1.518, P = 0.224
Sample entropy 0.98 ± 0.46 0.76 ± 0.45 0.74 ± 0.44 F (108,2) = 2.945, P = 0.057

* P < 0.05, ADHD vs. controls for Bonferroni test.

Table 3. Actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24).

Controls and the clinical group divided according to the presence or not of ADHD. For the graph analyses the number of neighbours is 40 + 40.

Controls ADHD Not ADHD ANOVA
(n = 30) (n = 41) (n = 39)
Mean 309 ±139 299 ± 172 257 ± 139 F (106,2) = 1.188, P = 0.309
SD (% of mean) 137 ± 41 134 ± 41 139 ± 51 F (106,2) = 0.140, P = 0.870
RMSSD (% of mean) 104 ± 31 111 ± 35 116 ± 51 F (106,2) = 0.729, P = 0.485
RMSSD/SD 0.76 ± 0.08 0.84 ± 0.14* 0.84 ± 0.15 F (106,2) = 3.743, P = 0.027
Edges 6.27 ± 2.54 6.25 ± 2.75 5.87 ± 2.74 F (106,2) = 0.248, P = 0.781
Components 146 ± 58 140 ± 54 151 ± 63 F (106,2) = 0.354, P = 0.703
Bridges 25.1 ± 11.5 26.8 ± 10.9 31.7 ± 12.1 F (106,2) = 3.158, P = 0.047
Missing edges 317 ± 19 321 ± 14 322 ± 15 F (106,2) = 0.799, P = 0.453
Max number of edges 21.8 ± 7.7 20.1 ± 5.5 20.5 ± 5.8 F (106,2) = 0.626, P = 0.536
Nodes with zero edges 142 ± 50 141 ± 46 154 ± 57 F (106,2) = 0.740, P = 0.480
Ln cliques 7.33 ± 0.86 7.25 ± 0.79 7.23 ± 0.84 F (106,2) = 0.158, P = 0.854
Sample entropy 0.52 ± 0.28 0.66 ± 0.41 0.62 ± 0.39 F (106,2) = 1.328, P = 0.269

* P< 0.05, ADHD vs. controls for Bonferroni test.

Table 4. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

Healthy controls (n = 30) and ADHD patients (n = 41).

Healthy controls ADHD
Morning Evening P d# Morning Evening P d LMM*
D T D x T
Mean 385±178 309±139 0.049 0.48 289±152 299±172 0.536 0.06 0.012 0.204 0.039
SD 105±31 137±41 0.003 0.88 131±49 134±41 0.868 0.07 0.015 0.422 0.039
RMSSD 86±17 104±31 0.014 0.72 110±37 111±35 0.991 0.03 0.014 0.488 0.107
RMSSD/SD 0.85±0.12 0.76±0.08 0.003 0.88 0.86±0.16 0.84±0.14 0.548 0.13 0.514 0.821 0.155
Edges 7.94±2.80 6.27±2.54 0.022 0.63 6.46±2.99 6.25±2.75 0.711 0.08 0.075 0.684 0.138
Components 104±35 146±58 0.001 0.88 135±68 140±54 0.765 0.08 0.028 0.517 0.057
Bridges 38.6±8.5 25.1±11.5 <0.001 1.34 36.1±11.5 26.8±10.9 <0.001 0.83 0.217 0.050 0.231
Missing edges 309±14 317±19 0.062 0.48 321±16 321±14 0.932 0.00 0.014 0.433 0.111
Max edges 21.8±5.1 21.8±7.7 0.984 0.00 20.8±7.1 20.1±5.5 0.623 0.11 0.907 0.650 0.761
Zero edges 117±34 142±50 0.030 0.58 144±62 141±46 0.744 0.05 0.045 0.328 0.096
Ln cliques 7.69±0.68 7.33±0.86 0.093 0.46 7.32±0.90 7.25±0.79 0.717 0.08 0.124 0.793 0.287
Sample entropy 0.98±0.46 0.52±0.28 <0.001 1.21 0.76±0.45 0.66±0.41 0.292 0.23 0.019 0.399 0.014

M: linear mixed model, diagnosis = ADHD vs. healthy controls, time = morning vs. evening, D = diagnosis, T = time, D x T = interaction diagnosis and time.

#d: effect size (Cohen).

S1 Table is similar to S4 Table, but in this table healthy controls are compared to patients without a diagnosis of ADHD. For the not ADHD clinical group none of the morning/evening differences were significant, and with the linear mixed model interactions analyses between diagnosis (healthy controls vs. not ADHD) and time (morning vs. evening) two measures were significant, bridges (p = 0.018) and sample entropy (p = 0.014).

In the ADHD group, 21 patients had CT and 18 had not CT. S2 Table shows differences between morning and evening registrations for ADHD patients with and without cyclothymic temperament, showing that only bridges were different (p = 0.012 for both CT and not CT).

S3 Table shows calculations of bridges using 6 different values for neighbours, and comparing morning with evening for healthy controls and ADHD patients separately. For all these values of neighbours the difference between morning and evening was higher for the healthy controls than for the ADHD patients. The effect size was highest for 40 + 40 (1.34, healthy controls) and 80 + 80 (1.64, healthy controls). For bridges (40 + 40 neighbours) there was a modest negative correlation with SD (-0.331, p<0.001) and a positive correlation with sample entropy (0.382, p < 0.001).

There was a small negative correlation between age and score on the CT scale (-0.288 p = 0.013), and an interaction with borderline significance (F = 3.898, p = 0.050) between age and time of day (morning vs. evening) for the sample entropy measure in the healthy controls. Apart from this there were no significant effects of age.

In the morning registrations there was an interaction between gender and diagnosis for the sample entropy measure. For the controls sample entropy is higher in females than in males, while for ADHD and not ADHD there were no significant differences between the genders (S4 Table).

In the evening registrations there were significant effects of gender for 8 of the 12 measures; SD, RMSSD, edges, components, max edges, zero edges, ln cliques, and sample entropy. These results are shown in S5 Table. Values for all the 12 measures separately for males and females are shown in S6 and S7 Tables.

In the comparison between morning and evening registrations there were no significant effects of gender in the healthy controls, as shown in S8 Table. Values for all the 12 measures separately for males and females are shown in S9 Table. For males only one measure, bridges, was significantly different between morning and evening registrations, while for females 8 measures were significantly different.

In the comparison between morning and evening registrations there were significant effects of gender for 4 of the 12 measures in the ADHD group, SD, edges, components and sample entropy, as shown in S10 Table. Values for all the 12 measures separately for males and females are shown in S11 Table. For males only one measure, bridges, was significantly different between morning and evening registrations, while for females none of the measures were significantly different.

In the comparison between morning and evening registrations for the not ADHD group there were no effects of gender.

Discussion

We found a clear morning to evening difference in the healthy controls for most actigraphic measures. Contrasting this, both the ADHD and the non-ADHD patient groups, all but one measure did not show significant morning to evening variation. The exception was the graph theoretical concept bridges. The second main finding was that bridges was the measure that most clearly separates morning and evening registrations. This diurnal difference was significant both for healthy controls as well as for the ADHD patients.

In the morning registrations two measures, RMSSD and missing edges, were significantly different in the ADHD group compared to healthy controls. RMSSD was higher in ADHD patients, and there was also a higher number of missing edges in the ADHD patients compared to healthy controls, meaning that more nodes (points in the time series) are different from the nodes immediately before and after the index node. Missing edges is therefore a graph theoretical measure that gives information similar to that of RMSSD.

The SD was also higher in the ADHD group than in healthy controls, but the difference between the three groups was not significant using ANOVA. These findings are consistent with a number of previous studies showing that higher intraindividual variability is a characteristic feature of ADHD [17, 4749], and is also found in animal models of ADHD [18]. However, variability, measured with the SD, is an important finding in a wide range of other conditions and disorders as well [50, 51].

In the evening registrations only one measure, the ratio RMSSD/SD, was different in ADHD patients compared to healthy controls, while there were only small differences in RMSSD, SD and missing edges. Interestingly, this shares similarities with the activity profiled of manic patients, as shown in a study by Krane-Gartiser et al. [22], reporting higher RMSSD/SD in manic patients compared to controls, in 64 min recordings both in the morning and evening. Increased RMSSD/SD ratio has also been found in actigraphic studies of schizophrenic patients [21].

However, there were only small differences between the ADHD group and the clinical controls, mostly patients with mood and anxiety disorders, indicating that the differences in variability are not specific to a diagnosis of ADHD.

When comparing morning and evening recordings in healthy controls there was a striking difference for most of the measures, both with regard to activity level and variability. In general the variability measures were higher in the evening. Number of edges was also lower, meaning more differences between an index node and the neighbours, consistent with a higher variability. The number of components was higher in the evening, indicating that there was more often a change in activity to a lower or a higher level, again consistent with a general higher variability in the evening. Apparently in contrast to this, a measure of complexity, sample entropy, was significantly lower in the evening. With regard to way sample entropy is calculated there is no obvious reason why we have found such an inverse relation between this measure and ordinary variability measures. However, the association between high variability, measured with RMSSD and SD, and low complexity measured with sample entropy, has also been found in another study of motor activity [21]. Reduced complexity has been associated with disease states and aging [52], and in a study of schizophrenic patients motor activity complexity was found to be lower in evening than in the morning [16]. But the present study shows that changes in levels of complexity also may be a feature of normal diurnal variation.

In contrast, both in ADHD patients and in clinical controls (not ADHD), there is little difference between morning and evening recordings, the one exception being bridges. For ADHD patients the difference in number of bridges is substantial, with lower values in the evening, while for the not ADHD group it is smaller, but higher than for the other measures.

The number of bridges is even lower in the evening in the healthy controls, and this measure is the most highly discriminant of all (d = 1.34). There is a lower number of bridges in evening for all choices of neighbours, from 2 +2 and up to 80 + 80, although the largest difference is for 40 + 40 and 80 + 80 neighbours. However, it is not easy to compare this measure to the others we have used, but even though we do not have a clear explanation for these findings concerning bridges, we think that such results may give clues to the underlying dynamics of the time series, and thus the regulation of motor activity.

In a previous study of the same sample [17] we found differences in scores on a neuropsychological test (CPT) when comparing the ADHD patients with and without a cyclothymic temperament. Mood instability, as reflected in the scores on the CT scale, is an important part of the clinical picture of ADHD, but concerning variation between motor activity patterns in the morning and evening there seem to be no differences between patients with and without CT.

In the present study we have found that in ADHD patient motor activity is more stable across the day compared to controls. This is in accordance with the findings of Boonstra et al. [19] that the rhythm measure interdaily stability is higher, and intradaily variability is lower in ADHD patients than in controls indicating a more repeatable daily activity schedule and more consolidated rest and activity periods. Additionally, there are other studies that have found reduced diurnal variation in other functions as well, such as the study of Hunt et al. [53] on cognitive function in young adults with ADHD. The participants lacked the normal significant diurnal variation in neuropsychological performance. Baird et al. [20] looked at actigraphy data, clock genes, salivary melatonin and cortisol secretion in adult ADHD-patients compared to controls. For actigraphy and cortisol secretion they found a significant rhythmicity both in ADHD and controls. However, clock gene expression (PER2) in the oral mucosa exhibited a significant rhythm in controls, which was lost in ADHD patients. Similarly for salivary melatonin, there was a significant rhythm in controls, but a loss of this rhythmicity in the ADHD group. Reduced diurnal variation is also found in actigraphic studies of manic patients, both with regard to activity level, SD and RMSSD [16]. On the other hand, in a study of heart rate variability [54] in children with ADHD as well as normal controls showed pronounced variation from morning to evening in measures related to vagus activity.

Both motor activity, arousal and other biological functions are regulated by endogenous chronobiological rhythms, the most important being the circadian [55]. Loss of diurnal fluctuations in physiological outputs could be an expression of weak circadian rhythms, or in other words, reduced amplitude of circadian function. Reduced circadian amplitude has been found in schizophrenia and affective disorders, and is associated with reduced cognitive function, affective symptoms and subjectively reported wellbeing [56, 57]. There is a high comorbidity and many shared clinical traits between bipolar disorder and ADHD, and also involvement of similar neurotransmitter systems, including dopamine. The stable activity patterns from morning to evening for the patients with ADHD may as well reflect the presence of two consecutive endogenous days with similar activity structures, and is compatible with the fact that in ADHD substantial evidence point to changes in dopaminergic systems [58]. Another reason for loss of diurnal variation could be the possibility of bifurcation of biological rhythms in susceptible individuals [59]. Bifurcation means switching the circadian rhythm from a 24-h cycle to 12-h SCN-generated cycles, and can be provoked in hamsters and mice in extreme light cycles [60]. Human data from bipolar patients in a manic episode support this hypothesis [61].

The recent advances in theories and research on biological rhythms is promising and could lead to better understanding of the mechanisms involved. There were only small effects of age in the present sample of patients and controls, but for gender there were in part large and potential interesting differences between males and females. These differences were found primarily in the evening registrations, but are most conspicuous when looking at the differences between morning and evening for the healthy controls. There were significant differences between morning and evening for females, but not for males, again with the interesting exception of bridges. In females variability in general was higher in the evening, while total level of motor activity is unchanged and complexity was lower (lower sample entropy values). For the ADHD patients there were no significant morning vs. evening differences for either gender, with the exception of bridges for males. We are not aware of similar studies on gender differences in actigraphic measures, and it is therefore difficult to draw conclusions, but regarding the increased variability in motor activity that is previously found in depressed patients [62] one interesting hypothesis may be that the increased variability in the evening recordings in females may be related to the known increased propensity to depression in females [63].

Limitations

It is always possible that treatment with psychotropic medication may have influenced results in studies such as this, but the majority of our patients did not use psychotropic medication.

We have not looked at sleep parameters, and sleep problems is of course common both in patients with ADHD and mood disorders [64, 65]. This may have influenced results, but it is difficult to separate such effects from other effects on rest and activity rhythms. The patients in this study were all outpatients, but we have not access to information that may allow us to compare their activity schedules to those of the controls. This type of bias is therefore difficult to evaluate. Diagnoses were assessed non-blind, but the actigraphic registrations and mathematical analyses did not require subjective evaluations. Participants were asked to remove the actigraphs while showering or taking a bath, but all the registrations were inspected manually to avoid such periods when extracting morning and evening sequences.

Concerning the effects of gender, it is important to note that the groups are rather small, particularly for the healthy controls, making the results more uncertain than for the groups containing both genders.

Conclusions

In the present study we have used actigraph registrations combined with mathematical methods, including the similarity graph algorithm, to study diurnal variation of motor activity. In healthy controls most of the actigraphic measures showed clear differences between morning and evening registrations, in particular higher variability and lower complexity in evening registrations compared to the morning. However this was evident only in the female controls. In a mixed sample of outpatients the same measures were not significantly different, with one exception, the graph theoretical measure bridges. This was the measure that most clearly separated morning and evening registrations and this diurnal difference was significant both in healthy controls and in patients with a diagnosis of ADHD. We suggest that by combining different mathematical methods, including the present measures based on graph theory, analyses of actigraph registrations may give clues to the underlying dynamics of the time series, and thus shed light on the regulation of motor activity in ADHD as well as in other conditions.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

Healthy controls and clinical controls (Not ADHD).

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

ADHD patients with (n = 24) and without cyclothymic temperament (n = 17).

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

ADHD patients (n = 42/41*) and healthy controls (n = 30). Number of bridges for different choices of neighbours.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the morning, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24) for males.

Controls and the clinical group divided according to the presence or not of ADHD. For the graph analyses the number of neighbours is 40 + 40.

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24) for females.

Controls and the clinical group divided according to the presence or not of ADHD. For the graph analyses the number of neighbours is 40 + 40.

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

Healthy controls, results for males and females separately.

(DOCX)

S10 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

(DOCX)

S11 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

ADHD patients.

(DOCX)

S1 File. Background data.

(XLSX)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors have received financial support for the research related to this article from The Western Norway Regional Health Authority and The Norwegian Resource Center for ADHD, Tourette Syndrome and Narcolepsy. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Haavik J, Halmøy A, Lundervold AJ, Fasmer OB. Clinical assessment and diagnosis of adults with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. 2010;10(10):1569–80. 10.1586/ern.10.149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Di Trani M, Di Roma F, Elda A, Daniela L, Pasquale P, Silvia M, et al. Comorbid depressive disorders in ADHD: The role of ADHD severity, subtypes and familial psychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Investigation. 2014;11(2):137–42. 10.4306/pi.2014.11.2.137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Instanes JT, Klungsøyr K, Halmøy A, Fasmer OB, Haavik J. Adult ADHD and Comorbid Somatic Disease: A Systematic Literature Review. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2018. February 1;22(3):203–28. 10.1177/1087054716669589 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fredriksen M, Egeland J, Haavik J, Fasmer OB. Individual Variability in Reaction Time and Prediction of Clinical Response to Methylphenidate in Adult ADHD: A Prospective Open Label Study Using Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Biederman J, Faraone. Seminar Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Seminar. 2005;237–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sergeant J. The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder In: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews [Internet]. Plenum press; 2000. [cited 2019 Oct 21]. p. 7–12. Available from: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00060-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Scott J, Murray G, Henry C, Morken G, Scott E, Angst J, et al. Activation in bipolar disorders a systematic review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(2):189–96. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Jaypee Brothers Medical Publishers; 2000. 943 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Teicher MH. Actigraphy and motion analysis: New tools for psychiatry [Internet]. Vol. 3, Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 1995. [cited 2019 Oct 21]. p. 18–35. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9384925 10.3109/10673229509017161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Alderson RM, Rapport MD, Kasper LJ, Sarver DE, Kofler MJ. Hyperactivity in boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The association between deficient behavioral inhibition, attentional processes, and objectively measured activity. Child Neuropsychology. 2012;18(5):487–505. 10.1080/09297049.2011.631905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Teicher MH, Polcari A, Fourligas N, Vitaliano G, Navalta CP. Hyperactivity persists in male and female adults with ADHD and remains a highly discriminative feature of the disorder: A case-control study. BMC Psychiatry [Internet]. 2012;12(1):1 Available from: BMC Psychiatry 10.1186/1471-244X-12-190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Licht CA, Tryon WW. Are children diagnosed with the combined form of ADHD pervasively hyperactive? Behavior Modification. 2009;33(5):655–81. 10.1177/0145445509344167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lis S, Baer N, Stein-en-Nosse C, Gallhofer B, Sammer G, Kirsch P. Objective measurement of motor activity during cognitive performance in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica [Internet]. 2010. October [cited 2019 Oct 24];122(4):285–94. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20199487 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01549.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fasmer OB, Mjeldheim K, Førland W, Hansen AL, Dilsaver S, Oedegaard KJ, et al. Motor activity in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Investigation. 2015;12(4):474–82. 10.4306/pi.2015.12.4.474 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Krane-Gartiser K, Vaaler AE, Fasmer OB, Sørensen K, Morken G, Scott J. Variability of activity patterns across mood disorders and time of day. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):1–8. 10.1186/s12888-016-1163-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Krane-Gartiser K, Henriksen TEG, Morken G, Vaaler AE, Fasmer OB. Motor activity patterns in acute schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders can be differentiated from bipolar mania and unipolar depression. Psychiatry Research [Internet]. 2018;270(October):418–25. Available from: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fasmer OB, Mjeldheim K, Førland W, Hansen AL, Syrstad VEG, Oedegaard KJ, et al. Linear and non-linear analyses of Conner’s Continuous Performance Test-II discriminate adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder from patients with mood and anxiety disorders. BMC Psychiatry [Internet]. 2016;16(1):1–10. Available from: 10.1186/s12888-016-0993-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fasmer OB, Johansen EB. Patterns of motor activity in spontaneously hypertensive rats compared to Wistar Kyoto rats. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2016;12(1):1–13. 10.1186/s12993-015-0086-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Boonstra AM, Kooij JJS, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA, Buitelaar JK, Van Someren EJW. Hyperactive night and day? Actigraphy studies in adult ADHD: A baseline comparison and the effect of methylphenidate. Sleep. 2007;30(4):433–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Baird AL, Coogan AN, Siddiqui A, Donev RM, Thome J. Adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder is associated with alterations in circadian rhythms at the behavioural, endocrine and molecular levels. Molecular Psychiatry. 2012. October;17(10):988–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hauge ER, Berle JØ, Oedegaard KJ, Holsten F, Fasmer OB. Nonlinear analysis of motor activity shows differences between schizophrenia and depression: A study using fourier analysis and sample entropy. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(1):1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Krane-Gartiser K, Henriksen TEG, Morken G, Vaaler A, Fasmer OB. Actigraphic assessment of motor activity in acutely admitted inpatients with bipolar disorder. PloS one [Internet]. 2014. [cited 2019 Oct 22];9(2):e89574 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24586883 10.1371/journal.pone.0089574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lacasa L, Luque B, Ballesteros F, Luque J, Nuño JC. From time series to complex networks: The visibility graph. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105(13):4972–5. 10.1073/pnas.0709247105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Luque B, Lacasa L, Ballesteros F, Luque J. Horizontal visibility graphs: Exact results for random time series. Physical Review E—Statistical, Nonlinear, and Soft Matter Physics. 2009;80(4):1–11. 10.1103/PhysRevE.80.046103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Aguilar-San Juan B, Guzmán-Vargas L. Earthquake magnitude time series: Scaling behavior of visibility networks. European Physical Journal B. 2013;86(11). [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lacasa L, Luque B, Luque J, Nũo JC. The visibility graph: A new method for estimating the Hurst exponent of fractional Brownian motion. EPL. 2009;86(3). [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Berle JO, Hauge ER, Oedegaard KJ, Holsten F, Fasmer OB. Actigraphic registration of motor activity reveals a more structured behavioural pattern in schizophrenia than in major depression. BMC Research Notes. 2010;3 10.1186/1756-0500-3-149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Leiknes KA, Malt U, Malt EA LS. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview Plus. Norwegian version. Oslo; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sheehan D V., Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. In: Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998. p. 22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1979;134(4):382–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Halmøy A, Fasmer OB, Gillberg C, Haavik J. Occupational outcome in adult ADHD: Impact of symptom profile, comorbid psychiatric problems, and treatment: A cross-sectional study of 414 clinically diagnosed adult ADHD patients. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2009;13(2):175–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine. 2005. February;35(2):245–56. 10.1017/s0033291704002892 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ward MF, Wender PH, Reimherr FW. The WURS: A rating scale to aid in the retrospective diagnosis of attention deficit disorder in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150:885–90. 10.1176/ajp.150.6.885 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hirschfeld RMA, Williams JBW, Spitzer RL, Calabrese JR, Flynn L, Keck J, et al. Development and validation of a screening instrument for bipolar spectrum disorder: The mood disorder questionnaire. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157(11):1873–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Akiskal HS, Akiskal KK, Haykal RF, Manning JS, Connor PD. TEMPS-A: Progress towards validation of a self-rated clinical version of the Temperament Evaluation of the Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego Autoquestionnaire. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2005;85(1–2):3–16. 10.1016/j.jad.2004.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Akiskal HS, Akiskal K, Allilaire JF, Azorin JM, Bourgeois ML, Sechter D, et al. Validating affective temperaments in their subaffective and socially positive attributes: Psychometric, clinical and familial data from a French national study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2005;85(1–2):29–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Landaas ET, Halmøy A, Oedegaard KJ, Fasmer OB, Haavik J. The impact of cyclothymic temperament in adult ADHD. Journal of affective disorders [Internet]. 2012. December 15 [cited 2019 Oct 22];142(1–3):241–7. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22840630 10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Oedegaard KJ, Neckelmann D, Mykletun A, Dahl AA, Zwart JA, Hagen K, et al. Migraine with and without aura: association with depression and anxiety disorder in a population-based study. The HUNT Study. Cephalalgia: an international journal of headache [Internet]. 2006. January [cited 2019 Oct 24];26(1):1–6. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16396660 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Littner M, Kushida CA, Anderson WMD, Bailey D, Berry RB, Davila DG, et al. Practice parameters for the role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms: An update for 2002. Vol. 26, Sleep. American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2003. p. 337–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Van Hilten JJ, Middelkoop HA, Kuiper SI, Kramer CG, Roos RA. Where to record motor activity: an evaluation of commonly used sites of placement for activity monitors. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology [Internet]. 1993. October [cited 2019 Oct 24];89(5):359–62. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7691576 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Richman JS, Moorman JR. Physiological time-series analysis using approximate and sample entropy. American Journal of Physiology—Heart and Circulatory Physiology. 2000. June;278(6 47–6). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bondy JA, Murty USR. Graph Theory (textbook) [Internet]. Vol. 244 2008. [cited 2020 May 4]. 653 p. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-84628-970-5 [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Chartrand G, Zhang P. Chromatic graph theory [Internet]. Chromatic Graph Theory. CRC Press; 2008. [cited 2020 May 4]. 1–484 p. Available from: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9781584888017 [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fasmer EE, Fasmer OB, Berle JØ, Oedegaard KJ, Hauge ER. Graph theory applied to the analysis of motor activity in patients with schizophrenia and depression. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(4):1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sedgewick R, Wayne K. Algorithms, 4th edition—Robert Sedgewick and Kevin Wayne [Internet]. Addison-Wesley; 2011. 955 p. Available from: https://books.google.com.sg/books?hl=en&lr=&id=MTpsAQAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PR6&dq=sedgewick+algorithms+fourth+edition&ots=QfkHEMGckT&sig=tg9Cic6ClfAtBGYUW5BEY5vC5a4#v=onepage&q=sedgewick algorithms fourth edition&f=false [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Chiba N, Nishizeki T. Arboricity and Subgraph Listing Algorithms. SIAM Journal on Computing. 1985;14(1):210–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Castellanos FX, Kelly C, Milham MP. The restless brain: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, resting-state functional connectivity, and intrasubject variability. Vol. 54, Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Canadian Psychiatric Association; 2009. p. 665–72. 10.1177/070674370905401003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Johnson KA, Kelly SP, Bellgrove MA, Barry E, Cox M, Gill M, et al. Response variability in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(4):630–8. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kofler MJ, Rapport MD, Sarver DE, Raiker JS, Orban SA, Friedman LM, et al. Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013;33(6):795–811. 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.MacDonald SWS, Nyberg L, Bäckman L. Intra-individual variability in behavior: links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity. Trends in Neurosciences. 2006;29(8):474–80. 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kaiser S, Roth A, Rentrop M, Friederich HC, Bender S, Weisbrod M. Intra-individual reaction time variability in schizophrenia, depression and borderline personality disorder. Brain and Cognition. 2008;66(1):73–82. 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Goldberger AL. Fractal variability versus pathologic periodicity: Complexity loss and stereotypy in disease. Vol. 40, Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 1997. p. 543–61. 10.1353/pbm.1997.0063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hunt MG, Bienstock SW, Qiang JK. Effects of diurnal variation on the Test of Variables of Attention performance in young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Assessment. 2012. March;24(1):166–72. 10.1037/a0025233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Buchhorn R, Conzelmann A, Willaschek C, Störk D, Taurines R, Renner TJ. Heart rate variability and methylphenidate in children with ADHD. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders. 2012;4(2):85–91. 10.1007/s12402-012-0072-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Bollinger T, Schibler U. Circadian rhythms—from genes to physiology and disease. Swiss medical weekly [Internet]. 2014. [cited 2020 Jan 13];144:w13984 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25058693 10.4414/smw.2014.13984 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bromundt V, Köster M, Georgiev-Kill A, Opwis K, Wirz-Justice A, Stoppe G, et al. Sleep—Wake cycles and cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2011. April;198(4):269–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lyall LM, Wyse CA, Graham N, Ferguson A, Lyall DM, Cullen B, et al. Association of disrupted circadian rhythmicity with mood disorders, subjective wellbeing, and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study of 91 105 participants from the UK Biobank. The lancet Psychiatry [Internet]. 2018. [cited 2020 Jan 13];5(6):507–14. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29776774 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30139-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Faraone S V. The pharmacology of amphetamine and methylphenidate: Relevance to the neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric comorbidities. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews [Internet]. 2018. [cited 2019 Oct 24];87:255–70. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29428394 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Kripke DF, Elliott JA, Welsh DK, Youngstedt SD. Photoperiodic and circadian bifurcation theories of depression and mania Vol. 4, F1000Research. Faculty of 1000 Ltd; 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Raiewski EE, Elliott JA, Evans JA, Glickman GL, Gorman MR. Twice daily melatonin peaks in Siberian but not Syrian hamsters under 24 h light:dark:light:dark cycles. Chronobiology international [Internet]. 2012. November [cited 2020 Jan 13];29(9):1206–15. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23003567 10.3109/07420528.2012.719965 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Nováková M, Praško J, Látalová K, Sládek M, Sumová A. The circadian system of patients with bipolar disorder differs in episodes of mania and depression. Bipolar disorders [Internet]. 2015. May [cited 2020 Jan 13];17(3):303–14. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25359533 10.1111/bdi.12270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Krane-Gartiser K, Henriksen TEG, Vaaler AE, Fasmer OB, Morken G. Actigraphically assessed activity in unipolar depression: A comparison of inpatients with and without motor retardation. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2015. September 1;76(9):1181–7. 10.4088/JCP.14m09106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bangasser DA, Eck SR, Ordoñes Sanchez E. Sex differences in stress reactivity in arousal and attention systems. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(1):129–39. 10.1038/s41386-018-0137-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wajszilber D, Santiseban JA, Gruber R. Sleep disorders in patients with ADHD: Impact and management challenges. Nature and Science of Sleep. 2018;10:453–80. 10.2147/NSS.S163074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Steardo L, de Filippis R, Carbone EA, Segura-Garcia C, Verkhratsky A, De Fazio P. Sleep Disturbance in Bipolar Disorder: Neuroglia and Circadian Rhythms. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2019;10(July):1–12. 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Pan Lin

17 Aug 2020

PONE-D-20-19305

Diurnal variation of motor activity in adult ADHD patients analyzed with methods from graph theory

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Ole Bernt Fasmer,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 01 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Pan Lin

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. Please see http://www.bmj.com/content/340/bmj.c181.long for guidelines on how to de-identify and prepare clinical data for publication. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

3. Please amend your list of authors on the manuscript to ensure that each author is linked to an affiliation. Authors’ affiliations should reflect the institution where the work was done (if authors moved subsequently, you can also list the new affiliation stating “current affiliation:….” as necessary).

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper focuses on the diurnal variation of motor activity in normal, ADHD and clinic not ADHD people, as well as their comparisons. Authors used a portable actigraphy to record the motor series and tried to character motor feature of ADHD by adopting 12 measures. They reported that in normal people, the evening activity show higher variability and less complexity than morning activity, but it is not observed in ADHD. They also considered the effects of age, gender, cyclothymic temperament on the results. In my opinion, using a portable actigraphy to study motor function has a promising future in clinical diagnosis, exploring neural basis and so on, and authors also obtained some interesting results. But before the publication, there are many problems need to be addressed.

1. Can authors declare the difference between this paper and Ref.17?

2. It is really hard for me to understand how a graph is built from a single motor series

3. Discussion needs to be majorly revised. Many descriptions are actually results (e.g., lines 452-456, 464-468). And many sentences are meaningless (e.g., lines 448, 461-463). Most importantly, many measures can characterize the diurnal difference, but their meanings for patients are not clear explained. For example, in lines 425-426, the description on RMSSD is actually a definition, not its meaning for ADHD. I suggest authors pay more effort to dig deeper meaning of this paper.

4. I notice that many results are not provided in a table (e.g., lines 393-398) and just written in main text. I suggest that a summarize on results in a table is more clear for readers.

5. In graph theory, many measures are given, but the degree, path, acyclic and spanning subgraph are not used in later analysis. If true, please delete them.

6. CT is cyclothymic temperament? Please identify the abbreviation while using it first time.

7. Line 323, 39 should be 39 patients or something.

8. The grammatical tense is not consistent, e.g., lines338 uses ‘was’, line 342 uses ‘is’, pleases unite them through the paper.

9. Line 344, what does the (1.34) mean.

10. After tables, *p=0.05 should be p<0.05.

11. Line 385, p=0.05 is not significant, pleases revise the description

12. Lines 397-398, I didn’t found the results corresponding to the description.

13. Line 404, I know that 8/12 indicates only 8 measures are significant, but it is really a strange description, can we just use 8?

14. Line 483, authors should present the current results and then compare them to previous.

15. Line 500, is it a replicated description ‘differently put; reduced amplitude….. Reduced….’

16. In conclusion, I suggest authors don’t use newly, because it is hard to identify what is really new.

17. Finally, can author explain to me why we have a higher variability and lower complexity in the evening, does it relate to our brain in some aspects?

Reviewer #2: Based on previous studies, the authors tested the assumption that diurnal variation of motor activity of ADHD patients is less than normal controls. With measures from variability, complexity and graph theory aspects, they conducted analysis of wrist-worn actigraphs time-series from ADHD patients, normal controls and clinical controls with mood and anxiety disorders. Their results suggested most of the measures being different between morning and evening in normal controls are not significantly different in both ADHD patients and non-ADHD clinical controls, but “bridge” is significantly different in ADHD and not different in non-ADHD. These results largely support their assumptions about reduced diurnal variation in ADHD. In addition, the exception of changes in bridge may suggest specificity about the diurnal regulation of motor activity in ADHD patients, when comparing with other mental disorders showing diurnal regulation disorders.

However, I have several concerns regarding to the results that should be clarified by authors.

Major 1: During the construction of similarity graph, the similar was detected by max(ux,uv)/min(ux,uv)<1.2. The authors explained this definition with analogy to the tolerance in sample entropy. However, unlike sample entropy where the tolerance range is a fix value (0.2*std), this definition will lead to a flexible range depending on the amplitude of the denominator. In other word, smaller signal value in the bottom will have narrower tolerance range and harder to “be similar” to others. The authors should explain why they use this criteria and what will be the consequence in the following computation of graph measures.

Minor 1: For readability, I suggest the authors to give a schematic illustration for the construction of similarity graph on time-series. In addition, authors can show example of actigraphs time-series. In addition, different kinds of activity jumps detected by graph measures, degree, bridge, and k-clique detect should be given to guide understanding.

Minor 2: The discussion about diurnal fluctuations and weak circadian rhythms, etc are too far away from the topic and the presented evidences, given the fact that the authors cannot explain what is told by the bridge changes.

Minor 3: Not clear why in healthy controls the evening registrations showed higher variability and lower complexity compared to morning registrations. In principle sample entropy is associated with measures of variability. The authors can recheck.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Nov 9;15(11):e0241991. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241991.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


25 Sep 2020

Response to Reviewers

Reviewer # 1 :

1. Can authors declare the difference between this paper and Ref.17

Response :

In Reference 17 (Fasmer OB, Mjeldheim K, Førland W, Hansen AL, Syrstad VEG, Oedegaard KJ, et al. Linear and non-linear analyses of Conner’s Continuous Performance Test-II discriminate adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder from patients with mood and anxiety disorders), we did use the same patient sample as in the present study, and furthermore divided tha sample into patients with and without ADHD. However, in Reference 17 we have looked at time series from the the CPT-test, and not from motor activity. In addition, cyclothymic temperament was a cental focus in that paper. In the present paper, besides using time series of motor activity, we study diurnal variation and used methods from graph theory to analyze the time series, in addtion to mathematical methods also utilized in reference 17.

2.It is really hard for me to understand how a graph is built from a

single motor series

Response :

Development of our similarity graph algorithm is based on work done by Lacasa et al (reference 23, Lacasa L, Luque B, Ballesteros F, Luque J, Nuño JC. From time series to complex networks: The visibility graph), where the theoretical background for transforming time series into graphs is described.

Motor activity time series is only one example of time series that can be trasformed into graphs, and in such a graph every node corresponds to a point in the time series, and two nodes are connected if the values of the motor activity are similar.

To explain in more detail : The similarity graph algorithm transforms time series into undirected similarity graphs, were two time points within a certain distance k connects with an edge if they correspond to each other within a given tolerance. The k preceding and the k succeeding time points of a random point in the time series defines to be nearest neighbors of such a time point. The threshold for similarity between two time points (u and v) defines as max (xu, xv) / min (xu, xv) < 1.2. Any time point with few or no connections (edges) to its nearest neighbors indicates an alteration in activity

3.Discussion needs to be majorly revised. Many descriptions are actually

results (e.g., lines 452-456, 464-468). And many sentences are meaningless

(e.g., lines 448, 461-463). Most importantly, many measures can

characterize the diurnal difference, but their meanings for patients are

not clear explained. For example, in lines 425-426, the description on

RMSSD is actually a definition, not its meaning for ADHD. I suggest authors

pay more effort to dig deeper meaning of this paper.

Response :

We have altered the parts of the discussion mentioned.

Concerning lines 425-426 : This must be seen in relation to the rest of this paragraph and the next. We agree with the reviewer that these parts need to be revised, and have made alterations from line 425 to line 440.

Concerning line 448: We have deleted the sentence that starts in line 448.

Concerning lines 452-456: We have made several alterations to this part.

Concerning lines 461-463: We have altered this sentence, and moved it some lines up in the same paragraph, hopefully making the meaning clearer.

Concerning lines 464-468:We have deleted the listing of results in the last sentence of this paragraph.

Concerning the deeper meaning of this paper: The changes we have made may hopefully give more meaning to the results we have presented. However, as we have stated in the conclusion section of the discussion, the present findings do not in any way fully explain the underlying dynamics of the diurnal regulation of motor activity, and activation in general. However, we think the present study may indicate the way foreward, namely using actigraph registrations in natural situations combined with mathematical methods to further unravel these mechanisms.

4.I notice that many results are not provided in a table (e.g., lines

393-398) and just written in main text. I suggest that a summarize on

results in a table is more clear for readers.

Response :

We agree that this would be clearer if presented in tables, and this has been done, by adding four more supplemental tables, and removing results from these paragraphs.

When writing these tables we have corrected some of the results (there are no significant effects of gender in the morning/evening comparisons for the healthy controls, and only four for the ADHD patients).

Concerning the effects of gender we have added a cautionary note with regard to the statistical results, noting that particularly for the healthy controls the groups are rather small, making the results more uncertain than for the groups containing both genders.

5.In graph theory, many measures are given, but the degree, path, acyclic

and spanning subgraph are not used in later analysis. If true, please

delete them.

Response :

We have altered the method section and removed the measures not used in the analyses. The definition of path is needed to define connected components and bridge. The definition of degree is needed to complete the definition of graph. We have however deleted the definition of tree and spanning graph.

6.CT is cyclothymic temperament? Please identify the abbreviation while

using it first time.

Response :

Yes, CT is cyclothymic temperament. We have identified this abbreviation the first time it was used (line 312).

7.Line 323, 39 should be 39 patients or something.

Response :

This has been amended to 39 patients.

8.The grammatical tense is not consistent, e.g., lines338 uses 'was', line

342 uses 'is', pleases unite them through the paper.

Response :

This has been altered, so the grammatical tense should now be consistent (past tense).

9.Line 344, what does the (1.34) mean.

Response :

This value is Cohens d, the sentence has been altered to make this clearer.

10.After tables, *p=0.05 should be p<0.05.

Response :

This has been altered.

11.Line 385, p=0.05 is not significant, pleases revise the description

Response :

This has been altered.

12.Lines 397-398, I didn't found the results corresponding to the

description.

Response :

The results are now presented in the supplemental tables 6 and 7.

13.Line 404, I know that 8/12 indicates only 8 measures are significant,

but it is really a strange description, can we just use 8?

Response :

We agree, and this has been altered.

14.Line 483, authors should present the current results and then compare

them to previous.

Response :

We have altered this part of the discussion and presented the results in more detail.

15.Line 500, is it a replicated description 'differently put; reduced

amplitude….. Reduced….'

Response :

Yes, it is just a different description of the same phenomenon, but we have altered the wording to : “or in other words”, to make this clearer..

16.In conclusion, I suggest authors don't use newly, because it is hard to

identify what is really new.

Response :

We have omitted this.

17.Finally, can author explain to me why we have a higher variability and

lower complexity in the evening, does it relate to our brain in some

aspects?

Response :

We have no clear explanation of this difference between morning and evening, but the inverse relation between variability and complexity is seen in different studies. See also response to reviewer 2.

Reviewer # 2 :

Major 1: During the construction of similarity graph, the similar was

detected by max(ux,uv)/min(ux,uv)<1.2. The authors explained this

definition with analogy to the tolerance in sample entropy. However, unlike

sample entropy where the tolerance range is a fix value (0.2*std), this

definition will lead to a flexible range depending on the amplitude of the

denominator. In other word, smaller signal value in the bottom will have

narrower tolerance range and harder to "be similar" to others. The authors

should explain why they use this criteria and what will be the consequence

in the following computation of graph measures.

Response :

We have tried to explain this a bit more detailed in the method section of the paper. It is correct that with our definition of similarity the tolerance for similarity will depend on the value for the points in the time series in the neighbourhood of the index point, but we think it is more reasonable to use such a criterion compared to using a fixed value for the whole series (as in sample entropy). One problem with sample entropy is that it is sensitive to outliers, which will increase SD and thus increase the probability that two points are considered similar, giving a false impression of high regularity, and a general problem with motor activity studies with actigraphs is that SD is high (and sometimes very high). We therefore think it makes sense to let the tolerance range for similarity depend on the actual values of the points to be compared, rather than to be dependent on all the values of the series (that is on the SD). The consequence will be that number of points considered similar will be lower, thus avoiding ceiling effects, and probably increase the ability of the test to detect differences between groups. This will be the case for all the graph measures we use.

Minor 1: For readability, I suggest the authors to give a schematic

illustration for the construction of similarity graph on time-series. In

addition, authors can show example of actigraphs time-series. In addition,

different kinds of activity jumps detected by graph measures, degree,

bridge, and k-clique detect should be given to guide understanding.

Response :

We have added an illustration of a similarity graph (Fig 2) and an example of an actigraph time series (Fig 1). There are two kind of “jumps” detected by the graph analyses, components and bridges, these are illustated in Fig 2. In addition the figure also illustrates the concept of clique.

Minor 2: The discussion about diurnal fluctuations and weak circadian

rhythms, etc are too far away from the topic and the presented evidences,

given the fact that the authors cannot explain what is told by the bridge

changes.

Response :

We have substantially shortened and rewritten this part of the discussion.

Minor 3: Not clear why in healthy controls the evening registrations showed

higher variability and lower complexity compared to morning registrations.

authors can recheck.

Response :

We do not have a ready explanation of why there is such a difference between morning and evening registrations. Obviously we need more information on the diurnal regulation of motor activity and activation in general to understand we this is so.

As we have noted in the response to reviewer 1, an inverse relation is seen between measures of variability and complexity is found also in other studies, for instance in a previous actigraph study from our group (Hauge ER, Berle JØ, Oedegaard KJ, Holsten F, Fasmer OB. Nonlinear analysis of motor activity shows differences between schizophrenia and depression: a study using Fourier analysis and sample entropy. PLoS One. 2011, 6 : e16291) and in a study on heart rate variability (Fasmer OB, Liao H, Huang Y, Berle JØ, Wu J, Oedegaard KJ, Wik G, Zhang Z. A naturalistic study of the effect of acupuncture on heart-rate variability. J Acupunct Meridian Stud. 2012, 5 : 15-20). The definition of these concepts (SD and sample entropy) are clearly different, but as this study also shows they are in some way related.

Attachment

Submitted filename: fasmer_response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Pan Lin

26 Oct 2020

Diurnal variation of motor activity in adult ADHD patients analyzed with methods from graph theory

PONE-D-20-19305R1

Dear Dr. Fasmer,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Pan Lin

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I recommend the publication because the authors have addressed all my comments. But I still have two minor requests:

1. There is no any label in figures. 1 and 2. In figure 1, three actigraph recordings are for ADHD, normal and control ADHD? please clearly state it. In figure 2, it is more clear if showing the value of max (xu , xv) / min (xu , xv).

2. In method, please first show the construction of Similarity graph and then show the Graph theory.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Pan Lin

29 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-19305R1

Diurnal variation of motor activity in adult ADHD patients analyzed with methods from graph theory

Dear Dr. Fasmer:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Pan Lin

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

    Healthy controls and clinical controls (Not ADHD).

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

    ADHD patients with (n = 24) and without cyclothymic temperament (n = 17).

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

    ADHD patients (n = 42/41*) and healthy controls (n = 30). Number of bridges for different choices of neighbours.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the morning, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

    (DOCX)

    S5 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

    (DOCX)

    S6 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24) for males.

    Controls and the clinical group divided according to the presence or not of ADHD. For the graph analyses the number of neighbours is 40 + 40.

    (DOCX)

    S7 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the evening, 360 min (18–24) for females.

    Controls and the clinical group divided according to the presence or not of ADHD. For the graph analyses the number of neighbours is 40 + 40.

    (DOCX)

    S8 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

    (DOCX)

    S9 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

    Healthy controls, results for males and females separately.

    (DOCX)

    S10 Table. Effect of gender on actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (18–24) using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

    (DOCX)

    S11 Table. Actigraphic registrations in the morning and evening, 360 min (08–14 and 18–24).

    ADHD patients.

    (DOCX)

    S1 File. Background data.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: fasmer_response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES