Abstract
Social isolation in adolescence leads to lasting deficits, including emotional and cognitive dysregulation. It remains unclear, however, how social isolation affects certain processes of memory and what molecular mechanisms are involved. In this study, we found that social isolation during the post-weaning period resulted in forgetting of the long-term fear memory, which was attributable to the downregulation of synaptic function in the hippocampal CA1 region mediated by EphB2, a receptor tyrosine kinase which involves in the glutamate receptor multiprotein complex. Viral-mediated EphB2 knockdown in CA1 mimicked the memory defects in group-housed mice, whereas restoration of EphB2 by either viral overexpression or resocialization reversed the memory decline in isolated mice. Taken together, our finding indicates that social isolation gives rise to memory forgetting by disrupting EphB2-mediated synaptic plasticity, which may provide a potential target for preventing memory loss caused by social isolation or loneliness.
Subject terms: Molecular neuroscience, Hippocampus
Introduction
Accumulating evidence over the past century, from social psychology to behavioral neuroscience, indicates the importance of social bonds for the survival of social species1,2. The social connection ensures safety and security, supports the survival of offspring, and reduces the need for energy expenditure3. Conversely, the absence of social interaction such as social isolation (SI), which is an aversive state in humans, is detrimental to physical and mental well-being4. SI can produce numerous behavioral, morphological, and functional abnormalities in the central nervous system5,6. In particular, post-weaning SI has been regarded as a risk factor for mental health problems during adolescence4,6,7.
SI contributes to poorer cognitive performance, faster cognitive decline, poorer executive functioning, increased negativity, and depressive cognition8. Increasing evidence shows that post-weaning isolation can impair spatial memory and social memory9–11. However, it remains unclear whether the memory impairments are due to the disruption of encoding, recalling information, or forgetting of events. Among the cognitive nuclei, the hippocampus serves as a functional core to integrate external environmental stimuli and intrinsic circuit activity for memory formation, which does it by receiving inputs from dentate granule cells and sending outputs through CA1 cells to process information12. CA1 neuron functions relying on subregional synaptic transmission/remodeling and also participates in the wiring of inter-nucleus circuits from the hippocampus to the cortex/amygdala13–15, which is crucial for fear memory. The dendritic spine, a specialized structure in neurons, plays a key role in synaptic remodeling for cellular function. Structural abnormalities of dendritic spines may contribute to synaptic dysfunction and have been implicated in memory impairment16–19. The synaptic function depends on the number and location of postsynaptic receptors, especially the glutamate receptor multiprotein complex, as well as their expression and signal properties20–22.
As a key component of the glutamate receptor multiprotein complex, EphB receptors promote synapse formation and maturation by organizing functional presynaptic specializations23, guiding axons24, inducing spine morphogenesis, and recruiting neurotransmitter receptors25. As critical molecules in the developmental phase26, EphB receptors also participate in a variety of functional events in cognition and memory27–29.
In this study, we found that post-weaning SI caused a rapidly forgetting of long-term memory. Furthermore, we identified the critical role of hippocampal EphB2 in SI-caused memory forgetting.
Methods
Mice
Thy1-GFP-M30 transgenic mice and EphB2−/−31 knock-out mice genotyping methods have been described previously. The mice (both males and females) were backcrossed to the 129 strain. All experiments involving mice were carried out in accordance with the US National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Animals under an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approved protocol in an Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care–an approved facility at the Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine.
Social isolation
For the whole program, mice were randomly divided into two groups: group housing (GH) with three or four mice per cage and social isolation (SI) with only one mouse per cage. In the beginning of the experiment, we isolated the mice from postnatal week 4 (PW4) for different time durations: 2, 4, and 12 weeks (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Fig. 1). After the following behavioral experiments, a 4-week social isolation model was chosen in all behavioral trials.
Fear conditioning and extinction
All tests were conducted according to the previous study32. All mice underwent handling and had 1 h to habituate the behavioral room before any tests began. The conditioning chambers (17 × 17 × 25 cm) equipped with stainless-steel shocking grids were connected to a precision feedback current-regulated shocker (Ugo Basile, Italy). The chamber walls were covered with black-and-white checkered wallpaper (context A), and the chambers were cleaned with 75% ethanol. On the day before the fear-conditioned training, mice were placed in context A to explore freely for 20 min and then returned to their home cages. On the training day, mice were also placed in context A to explore freely for 3 min first, then a pure tone (CS) was played for 28 s, followed by an electric foot shock (US, 0.75 mA, 2-s duration) through the floor grid. One minute later, paired CS–US repeated. Conditioned mice were returned to their home cages 30 s after the end of the last tone, and the floor and walls of the cage were cleaned with 75% ethanol for each mouse. One hour after conditioning, mice were placed in context A to test the freezing time (contextual fear memory). Thirty minutes later, animals received CS-alone presentations in a test chamber, which had gray non-shocking plexiglass floor and dark gray wallpaper and was washed with 4% acetic acid solution between the tests for individual mice (context B). In detail, the mice explored freely for 3 min, and then the tone was played continually for 1 min in context B. We analyzed the freezing behavior during tone presentations. The freezing time in context B refers to auditory fear memory. We tested the contextual and auditory fear memory on days 1 and 7 after conditioning. In extinction experiments, we used 3-consecutive-day extinction in this study. Animals were presented with unreinforced exposures to the conditioning context. Mice behaviors were recorded by digital video cameras mounted above the conditioning chamber. FreezeFrame and FreezeView software (Ugo Basile, Italy) were used for recording and analyzing the freezing behavior, respectively.
Electrophysiology
Brain coronal slices were prepared from GH mice and 4-week SI mice. Brains were dissected quickly and chilled in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 25 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, and 12.5 glucose. Coronal brain slices (300-μm thick) were prepared with a vibratome and recovered in ACSF bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 at 31 °C for 1 h and then maintained at room temperature (22–25 °C). Miniature excitatory postsynaptic potential current (mEPSC) was recorded at −70 mV in the presence of 100 μM picrotoxin and 1 μM tetrodotoxin. For PPR recording, slices were stimulated using a bipolar concentric electrode (FHC) that was placed in the CA1 and connected with a stimulator (AMPI) to evoke EPSCs (recorded in 100 μM picrotoxin ACSF solution) in neighboring pyramidal neurons. PPRs were calculated as a ratio of EPSC2 to EPSC1, separated by interstimulus intervals of 25, 50, 100, 200, and 400 ms. For long-term potentiation (LTP) recording, extracellular field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) in the Schaffer collateral pathway were synaptically evoked and recorded in the CA1 region. LTP was induced using high-frequency stimulation (HFS) consisting of 1-se 100 Hz trains, each delivered at 70–80% of the intensity that evoked spiked fEPSPs. Data were analyzed in pCLAMP 10.6 (Molecular Devices), and recordings were made from an average of three cells per slice and two to three slices per mouse.
Stereotaxic surgery
Mice were subjected to the operative procedure using an aseptic technique. The virus of AAV-EphB2 or lenti-EphB2 shRNA (Lenti-sh-EphB2, sequence ACGGACAAGCTACAACACT)33 were stereotaxically injected into CA1 (1.7 mm posterior and 1.25 mm left or right to the bregma, and 1.55 mm deep into the skull surface). After injection, the hand-held needle was kept in place for an additional 5 min to avoid the backflow of the virus containing solution. Following injection, the mice were collected on a 37 °C warm plate for recovery. Mice were allowed to recover for 25–30 days before starting the experiments. At the end of the experiment, mice used for behavioral tests were perfused and checked for the injection sites; off-target virus injection mice were excluded when we performed the statistical analysis.
Immunohistochemistry
Immunofluorescence was performed as described in our previous study34, coronal brain slices were blocked with permeable buffer (0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS) containing 10% donkey serum for 1 h at room temperature and were incubated with primary antibodies in permeable buffer containing 2% donkey serum overnight at 4 °C. The slices were then washed three times with PBS-T (0.1% Tween-20 in PBS) for 10 min each and incubated with Alexa Fluor secondary antibodies (1:200; Molecular Probes) in the PBS buffer for 2 h at room temperature. Slices were washed in PBS-T three times and photographed using a confocal microscope (Leica TCS SP8). Fluorescence microscopic images obtained were imported into Image J (NIH) for analysis, and all the parameters used were kept consistent during capture. For primary antibodies, we used goat anti-EphB2 (1:200, R&D, P54763). To quantify the shape of spine, a procedure was adapted from our previous study33. The shape of neuronal spines in slices was classified by NeuronStudio software package and an algorithm from Rodriguez35 with the following cutoff values: aspect ratio for thin spines (AR_thin (crit)) = 2.5, head-to-neck ratio (HNR (crit)) = 1.3, and head diameter (HD (crit)) = 0.3 μm. The type of these spines was determined based on the following criteria: (a) spines with HNR greater than HNR (crit) were considered to have a neck and could be either thin or mushroom types; (b) spines with HD greater than HD (crit) were classified as mushroom, otherwise thin; (c) spines lacking significant necks and less than AR_thin (crit) were considered stubby, otherwise thin. Protrusions with length 0.2–3.0 μm and maximum width 3 μm were counted. Spine density was calculated by dividing the total spine number by the dendritic branch length. The fluorescence intensity of EphB2 was quantified by Image J. All the pictures were processed with the same threshold.
Western blotting
Western blotting was performed as described in our previous study36. Briefly, hippocampus from GH mice and 4-week SI mice were dissected, homogenized, and solubilized at 4 °C for 1 h in lysis buffer (1% CHAPS, 137 mM NaCl, 2.7 mM KCl, 4.3 mM Na2HPO4, 1.4 mM KH2PO4, 5 mM EDTA, 5 mM EGTA, 1 mM PMSF, 50 mM NaF, 1 mM Na3VO4, and protease inhibitors, pH 7.2). Bound proteins were separated by SDS PAGE, transferred to nitrocellulose membranes, and immunoblotted with the indicated antibodies: goat anti-EphB1 (1:500, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-68317), goat anti-EphB2 (1:1000, R&D, P54763), mouse anti-synaptophysin (1:1000, Abcam, ab8049), rabbit anti-PSD95 (1:1000; Cell Signaling Technology, 3450), mouse anti-β-actin (1:3000; Thermo Fisher Scientific, MA5-15739), rabbit anti-GluN1 (1:1000, BD, 556308), mouse anti-GluA1 (1:1000, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-13152), GluA2 (1:1000, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-7611), GluA6 (1:1000, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-7618), rabbit anti-GluN2A (1:1000, Millipore, ab1555P), and mouse anti-GluN2B (1:1000, BD, 610417).
Statistical analysis
The results are presented as mean ± SEM. Statistical differences were determined by Student’s t test for two-group comparisons or analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by the Tukey test for multiple comparisons among more than two groups. A P value of < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Social isolation induces forgetting of long-term fear memory
To determine how SI impairs the cognitive process, we analyzed the memory performance of GH and SI mice, which were isolated for 2/4/12 weeks from postnatal week 4 (PW4), using the Pavlovian fear conditioning test. The mice received tests 1 h after conditioning, which was considered as short-term memory, and 1 day and 7 days after conditioning, which was considered as long-term memory. We found that 2-week isolated mice showed normal memory in terms of both short-term memory and long-term memory (Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). In the 4-week isolated mice, memory deficits were seen 7 days after conditioning, whereas the 12-week isolated mice showed memory defects earlier, with auditory fear deficits observed even 1 day after conditioning (Fig. 1 a, b and Supplementary Fig. 1c). These results indicate that long-term memory deficits are associated with the duration of SI. We used a 4-week isolation model in the remaining experiments. Notably, we did not see either long- or short-term memory decline after repeated exposure in the control group or find a significant difference between GH and SI mice after an extinction procedure (Supplementary Fig. 1d), suggesting that memory deficits in SI mice were not likely due to the extinction. No obvious difference was seen in Y-maze spontaneous alternation, a short-term memory test (Supplementary Fig. 2a). Furthermore, we did not observe any difference in fear response between male and female mice (Supplementary Fig. 2b, c). Taken together, these results provide evidence that SI leads to a phenotype of accelerated forgetting of long-term fear memory rather than of initially acquired memory or short-term memory.
Considering the critical roles of hippocampal synaptic morphogenesis in cognition12,16,17, we analyzed the spinogenesis in hippocampal neurons and observed the increased number of thin-shaped spines in CA1 area of isolated mice, with no difference in total density (Fig. 1c, d). To examine the synaptic function, we measured the mEPSC with whole-cell patch-clamp electrophysiology in brain slices and observed a decreased amplitude rather than frequency of mEPSC in these neurons from SI mice (Fig. 1e, f). No significant change was observed in the presynaptic neurotransmitter release, as demonstrated by the paired-pulse ratio (Supplementary Fig. 2d, e). We further detected the LTP in the Schaffer collateral-CA1 pathway, a critical cellular mechanism of learning and memory, and we observed a decreased LTP level in the SI group compared with the GH mice, indicating that long-term isolation impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Fig. 1g, h).
EphB2 mediates the social isolation-induced memory forgetting
The hippocampal glutamate receptor complex in the postsynaptic compartment has been implicated in cognition and synaptic function37–39. The complex includes glutamate receptors (AMPA receptors and NMDA receptors) and EphB receptors, which modulate excitatory synaptic transmission through protein clustering at postsynaptic membranes23,29,40. To figure out the molecular mechanism of isolation-induced memory defects, we tested the expression of these key proteins in hippocampal tissues extracted from GH and SI mice. We observed decreased levels of GluN1, GluA1, and GluA2, three glutamate receptor subtypes (Fig. 2a, b and Supplementary Fig. 3), and in particular, the EphB2 receptor, a key protein involved in the multiprotein complex that plays an important role in initial social recognition memory29. Immunofluorescence staining further showed that EphB2 in CA1 was significantly decreased in the SI group, with no apparent change in the cortex and CA3 region (Fig. 2c, d and Supplementary Fig. 4a, b).
As EphB2 participates in memory regulation39,41,42, we hypothesized that the downregulation of EphB2 could mimic the behavioral abnormalities in SI mice. We thus generated a lentiviral vector expressing both green fluorescent protein (GFP) and EphB2 shRNA (Lenti-sh-EphB2) (referred to as “EphB2-shRNA”)14 to knock down EphB2 (Supplementary Figs. 4c, d and 5a), which allowed us to test the role of EphB2 in forgetting of long-term memory. We bilaterally injected EphB2-shRNA into the CA1 region at PW4 and observed that GH mice infused with EphB2-shRNA showed less freezing time than control mice 7 days after conditioning, which was comparable to the level of SI mice (Fig. 3a, b). Morphologically, a considerable number of thin-shaped spines in GH mice with the infusion of EphB2-shRNA was observed, which was similar to SI mice (Fig. 3c, d). Moreover, we found that knockdown of EphB2 decreased mEPSC amplitude and suppressed LTP in CA1 region of GH mice, which mimicked the synaptic dysfunction observed in SI mice (Fig. 3e–h). These results suggest that EphB2 is necessary for SI-induced memory deficits.
Functional reversion of memory forgetting by EphB2 restoration
To further determine the sufficiency of EphB2 as a mediator of synaptic and cognitive function, we tested whether overexpression of exogenous EphB2 in CA1 could rescue the behavioral defects, morphological deficits, and impaired LTP in SI mice. Intra-CA1 administration of AAV-EphB2 resulted in a profound increase in expression levels of EphB2 (Supplementary Figs. 4e, f and 5b), which also successfully rescued long-term memory in SI mice (Fig. 4a). Furthermore, after overexpressing the exogenous EphB2, mEPSC and LTP were fully recovered (Fig. 4d–g), accompanied by a restoration of the subtype of spines in CA1 (Fig. 4b, c). These results provided additional supportive evidence for the role of EphB2 in isolation-induced forgetting of long-term memory.
Environment enrichment or resocialization was previously demonstrated to be beneficial for memory maintenance43–45 and could rescue the cognitive dysfunction induced by isolation9,46. We subjected the 4-week isolated mice to resocialization for 2 weeks and found that the expression of EphB2 in the hippocampus was restored in the re-socialized mice, and the freezing time in these mice also increased compared with the isolated group. These results suggest that a normal social environment can retain long-term memory through EphB2 (Fig. 5b, c). Taken together, our finding reveals the EphB2 is required and sufficient to sustain long-term memory in a social environment, which provides a molecular and synaptic mechanism for isolation-induced memory forgetting.
Discussion
In this study, we revealed that post-weaning isolation resulted in forgetting of long-term fear memory through disruption of the synaptic function in the hippocampal CA1 region. We further demonstrated that EphB2 played an essential role in memory retention by promoting spinogenesis and synaptic plasticity (Fig. 5d). Our study is distinct from previous studies that have focused on adulthood9, and we provide a molecular mechanism that could control the synaptic function for the SI-induced memory forgetting during adolescence. The scientific study of memory set in with Ebbinghaus’ forgetting curve illustrating the progressive deterioration of long-term memory as time goes by47. Since then, much has been learned about possible causes for this gradual memory loss, but the neurobiological processes underpinning the memory forgetting remain poorly understood. SI is associated with a higher risk of cognitive deterioration and neuronal disorders9,48,49. Our present results suggested that SI caused the forgetting of long-term fear memory rather than deficits in short-term memory or learning ability, which was different from previous studies10,50. The results are in agreement with the synaptic remodeling process, in which long-term memory parallels synaptic strengthening from days to weeks, rather than short-term memory, which lasts from minutes to hours51–53.
We demonstrated that EphB2 played an important role in SI-induced memory forgetting, which strengthens the evidence that EphB2 is involved in enhancing synaptic transmission, and the gene expression is believed to underlie memory formation39,41,54,55. EphB2 interacts with NMDA receptors and regulates excitatory synapse formation. Previous studies have shown that EphB2 interacts directly with GluN1 through its extracellular region, which regulates synaptic function56,57. On the one hand, EphB2 enhances the phosphorylation and localization of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptor at synapses37,58. Considering the decreased protein level of GluN1 in the hippocampus of isolation-reared mice (Fig. 2a), we assume that the EphB2–NMDA protein complex may serve as an essential functional core in SI-induced memory deficits. On the other hand, EphB2 also regulates the trafficking of GluA2 depending on its PDZ-binding motif: The PDZ-binding motif of EphB2 binds to the PDZ domain-containing scaffold protein glutamate receptor-interacting protein 1 (GRIP1), which binds to GluA2 and regulates the localization of GluA2-containing AMPA receptors23,40,59. AMPA receptor-dependent endocytosis has been proposed to be an active process underlying the intrinsic forgetting of LTP and the forgetting of relevant memories60. Furthermore, trans-synaptic ephrin-B–EphB2 interactions and forward signaling facilitate neural activation and structural plasticity in learning-associated neurons involved in the generation of fear memory61. Therefore, EphB2 regulates the forgetting of long-term memory induced by SI may pass through different synaptic molecular pathways.
Together with the accumulating evidences, EphB2 mediates the excitatory synaptogenesis during development and coordinates synaptic plasticity by controlling the localization and function of glutamate receptors27,59. Previous studies also show that EphB2 signal is related to anxiety disorders62, autism63,64, and cognitive dysfunction41,65. Our study expands the understanding of the function of EphB2 in the social environment and memory, which may bring about new insights for the treatment of these cognition-associated brain disorders.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank Si Chen for laboratory technique support. This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31371097 and 31671062 to N.-J.X., 81671047 and 81970997 to S.S.), the Grants of Shanghai Brain-Intelligence Project from STCSM (16JC1420500), the Science and Technology Commission of Shanghai Municipality (18JC1420302), and Innovation Program of Shanghai Municipal Education Commission (2017-01-07-00-01-E00046).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Xin-Rong Wu, Yu Zhang
Contributor Information
Nan-Jie Xu, Email: xunanjie@sjtu.edu.cn.
Suya Sun, Email: sunsuya@shsmu.edu.cn.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-020-01051-6).
References
- 1.Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bull. 1995;117:497–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Harlow HF, Zimmermann RR. Affectional responses in the infant monkey; orphaned baby monkeys develop a strong and persistent attachment to inanimate surrogate mothers. Science. 1959;130:421–432. doi: 10.1126/science.130.3373.421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Eisenberger NI. The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:421–434. doi: 10.1038/nrn3231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science. 1988;241:540–545. doi: 10.1126/science.3399889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cole SW. The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:733–767. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Holt-Lunstad J. Why social relationships are important for physical health: a systems approach to understanding and modifying risk and protection. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018;69:437–458. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC. Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009;13:447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Liu Y, Lv L, Wang L, Zhong Y. Social isolation induces Rac1-dependent forgetting of social memory. Cell Rep. 2018;25:288–295.e283. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Okada R, Matsumoto K, Tsushima R, Fujiwara H, Tsuneyama K. Social isolation stress-induced fear memory deficit is mediated by down-regulated neuro-signaling system and Egr-1 expression in the brain. Neurochem. Res. 2014;39:875–882. doi: 10.1007/s11064-014-1283-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wang B, et al. Long-term social isolation inhibits autophagy activation, induces postsynaptic dysfunctions and impairs spatial memory. Exp. Neurol. 2019;311:213–224. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2018.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Neves G, Cooke SF, Bliss TV. Synaptic plasticity, memory and the hippocampus: a neural network approach to causality. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:65–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn2303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Nakazawa Y, Pevzner A, Tanaka KZ, Wiltgen BJ. Memory retrieval along the proximodistal axis of CA1. Hippocampus. 2016;26:1140–1148. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zhu XN, et al. Amygdala EphB2 signaling regulates glutamatergic neuron maturation and innate fear. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:10151–10162. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0845-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kim WB, Cho JH. Synaptic targeting of double-projecting ventral CA1 hippocampal neurons to the medial prefrontal cortex and basal amygdala. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:4868–4882. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3579-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bourne JN, Harris KM. Balancing structure and function at hippocampal dendritic spines. Annu Rev. Neurosci. 2008;31:47–67. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bailey CH, Kandel ER, Harris KM. Structural components of synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 2015;7:a021758. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kasai H, Fukuda M, Watanabe S, Hayashi-Takagi A, Noguchi J. Structural dynamics of dendritic spines in memory and cognition. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Redondo RL, Morris RG. Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;12:17–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn2963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lee H-K, Barbarosie M, Kameyama K, Bear MF, Huganir RL. Regulation of distinct AMPA receptor phosphorylation sites during bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Nature. 2000;405:955–959. doi: 10.1038/35016089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kessels HW, Malinow R. Synaptic AMPA receptor plasticity and behavior. Neuron. 2009;61:340–350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Isaac JTR, Ashby MC, McBain CJ. The role of the GluR2 subunit in AMPA receptor function and synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2007;54:859–871. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kayser MS, McClelland AC, Hughes EG, Dalva MB. Intracellular and trans-synaptic regulation of glutamatergic synaptogenesis by EphB receptors. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12152–12164. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3072-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Robichaux MA, et al. EphB receptor forward signaling regulates area-specific reciprocal thalamic and cortical axon pathfinding. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:2188–2193. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1324215111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Henkemeyer M, Itkis OS, Ngo M, Hickmott PW, Ethell IM. Multiple EphB receptor tyrosine kinases shape dendritic spines in the hippocampus. J. Cell Biol. 2003;163:1313–1326. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200306033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Flanagan JG, Vanderhaeghen P. The ephrins and Eph receptors in neural development. Annu Rev. Neurosci. 1998;21:309–345. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Klein R, Kania A. Ephrin signalling in the developing nervous system. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014;27:16–24. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kullander K, Klein R. Mechanisms and functions of Eph and ephrin signalling. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 2002;3:475–486. doi: 10.1038/nrm856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Liu, X. D. et al. Hippocampal Lnx1-NMDAR multiprotein complex mediates initial social memory. Mol. Psychiatry, 10.1038/s41380-019-0606-y (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30.Feng G, et al. Imaging neuronal subsets in transgenic mice expressing multiple spectral variants of GFP. Neuron. 2000;28:41–51. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00084-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Henkemeyer M, et al. Nuk controls pathfinding of commissural axons in the mammalian central nervous system. Cell. 1996;86:35–46. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80075-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Zhu XN, et al. Ephrin-B3 coordinates timed axon targeting and amygdala spinogenesis for innate fear behaviour. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:11096. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Xu NJ, Sun S, Gibson JR, Henkemeyer M. A dual shaping mechanism for postsynaptic ephrin-B3 as a receptor that sculpts dendrites and synapses. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1421–1429. doi: 10.1038/nn.2931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ai P-H, et al. Paroxetine ameliorates prodromal emotional dysfunction and late-onset memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease mice. Transl. Neurodegeneration. 2020;9:18. doi: 10.1186/s40035-020-00194-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rodriguez A, Ehlenberger DB, Dickstein DL, Hof PR, Wearne SL. Automated three-dimensional detection and shape classification of dendritic spines from fluorescence microscopy images. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e1997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Liu XD, et al. Retrograde regulation of mossy fiber axon targeting and terminal maturation via postsynaptic Lnx1. J. Cell Biol. 2018;217:4007–4024. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201803105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hu R, et al. Overexpression of EphB2 in hippocampus rescues impaired NMDA receptors trafficking and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer model. Cell Death Dis. 2017;8:e2717. doi: 10.1038/cddis.2017.140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nolt MJ, et al. EphB controls NMDA receptor function and synaptic targeting in a subunit-specific manner. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:5353–5364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0282-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cisse M, et al. Reversing EphB2 depletion rescues cognitive functions in Alzheimer model. Nature. 2011;469:47–52. doi: 10.1038/nature09635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Miyamoto T, Kim D, Knox JA, Johnson E, Mucke L. Increasing the receptor tyrosine kinase EphB2 prevents amyloid-beta-induced depletion of cell surface glutamate receptors by a mechanism that requires the PDZ-binding motif of EphB2 and neuronal activity. J. Biol. Chem. 2016;291:1719–1734. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M115.666529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Alapin JM, et al. Activation of EphB2 forward signaling enhances memory consolidation. Cell Rep. 2018;23:2014–2025. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Shi XD, et al. Blocking the interaction between EphB2 and ADDLs by a small peptide rescues impaired synaptic plasticity and memory deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:11959–11973. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1327-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn1970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cortese GP, Olin A, O’Riordan K, Hullinger R, Burger C. Environmental enrichment improves hippocampal function in aged rats by enhancing learning and memory, LTP, and mGluR5-Homer1c activity. Neurobiol. Aging. 2018;63:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Prado Lima MG, et al. Environmental enrichment and exercise are better than social enrichment to reduce memory deficits in amyloid beta neurotoxicity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:E2403–e2409. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1718435115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Famitafreshi H, Karimian M. Assessment of improvement in oxidative stress indices with resocialization in memory retrieval in Y-Maze in male rats. J. Exp. Neurosci. 2018;12:1179069518820323. doi: 10.1177/1179069518820323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 1988;31:47–59. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lacey RE, Kumari M, Bartley M. Social isolation in childhood and adult inflammation: evidence from the National Child Development Study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;50:85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol. aging. 2006;21:140–151. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ouchi H, Ono K, Murakami Y, Matsumoto K. Social isolation induces deficit of latent learning performance in mice: a putative animal model of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 2013;238:146–153. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014;157:163–186. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Castellucci VF, Carew TJ, Kandel ER. Cellular analysis of long-term habituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex of Aplysia californica. Science. 1978;202:1306–1308. doi: 10.1126/science.214854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Carew T, Castellucci VF, Kandel ER. Sensitization in aplysia: restoration of transmission in synapses inactivated by long-term habituation. Science. 1979;205:417–419. doi: 10.1126/science.451611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lamprecht R, LeDoux J. Structural plasticity and memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;5:45–54. doi: 10.1038/nrn1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dines M, et al. The roles of Eph receptors in contextual fear conditioning memory formation. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 2015;124:62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dalva MB, et al. EphB receptors interact with NMDA receptors and regulate excitatory synapse formation. Cell. 2000;103:945–956. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00197-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Henderson JT, et al. The receptor tyrosine kinase EphB2 regulates NMDA-dependent synaptic function. Neuron. 2001;32:1041–1056. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00553-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hanamura K, et al. Extracellular phosphorylation of a receptor tyrosine kinase controls synaptic localization of NMDA receptors and regulates pathological pain. PLoS Biol. 2017;15:e2002457. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2002457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Sheffler-Collins SI, Dalva MB. EphBs: an integral link between synaptic function and synaptopathies. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:293–304. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Davis RL, Zhong Y. The biology of forgetting—a perspective. Neuron. 2017;95:490–503. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Talebian A, Henkemeyer M. EphB2 receptor cell-autonomous forward signaling mediates auditory memory recall and learning-driven spinogenesis. Commun. Biol. 2019;2:372. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0625-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Attwood BK, et al. Neuropsin cleaves EphB2 in the amygdala to control anxiety. Nature. 2011;473:372–375. doi: 10.1038/nature09938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kong A, et al. Rate of de novo mutations and the importance of father’s age to disease risk. Nature. 2012;488:471–475. doi: 10.1038/nature11396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Sanders SJ, et al. De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature. 2012;485:237–241. doi: 10.1038/nature10945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.van Dijken I, van der Vlag M, Flores Hernandez R, Ross A. Perspectives on treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: a closer look into EphB2 depletion. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:11296–11297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0214-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.