Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Nov 12.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2018 Nov 8;29:56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.10.010

Spatial attention alters visual appearance

Marisa Carrasco 1,*, Antoine Barbot 1
PMCID: PMC7661009  NIHMSID: NIHMS1511953  PMID: 30572280

Abstract

It is well established that attention improves performance on many visual tasks. However, for more than 100 years, psychologists, philosophers, and neurophysiologists have debated its phenomenology-whether attention actually changes one’s subjective experience. Here, we show that it is possible to objectively and quantitatively investigate the effects of attention on subjective experience. First, we review evidence showing that attention alters the appearance of many static and dynamic basic visual dimensions, which mediate changes in appearance of higher-level perceptual aspects. Then, we summarize current views on how attention alters appearance. These findings have implications for our understanding of perception and attention, illustrating that attention affects not only how we perform in visual tasks, but actually alters our experience of the visual world.


We confront an overwhelming amount of sensory information at any given moment, yet we have the impression of effortlessly understanding what we see. Selective attention enables us to select a certain location or feature of a visual scene to prioritize its processing and guide behavior. Selection is necessary given that our capacity to process visual information is limited by the high energy cost of cortical computation and the fixed amount of energy consumption available to the brain [1]. By enhancing the representation of relevant information while diminishing the representation of irrelevant signals, attention optimizes our system’s limited resources [27].

We are usually unaware of how attention alters visual representations, much like we are unaware of how the representation quality differs across the visual field [812]. Although visual quality decreases with eccentricity, our subjective perception in the periphery is ‘inflated’ [13] and does not reflect the drop in image quality [1416]. Visual quality and appearance also differ pronouncedly along isoeccentric locations [8,11,12,17,18]. These differences in processing quality shape our subjective perceptual experience.

COVERT ATTENTION ALTERS APPEARANCE

Visual attention can be overtly or covertly deployed, with or without eye movements, respectively [27,1921]. We constantly use both types of attention everyday; e.g. searching for objects, crossing the street, driving, biking, playing sports, and during interpersonal interactions. There are two types of covert spatial attention: Voluntary attention refers to the endogenous and sustained allocation of attention to a specific visual field location. Involuntary attention is the exogenous, transient capture of attention to a location brought about by a sudden change in the environment [26,20,21]. To characterize their effects on perception observers are cued to attend to specific locations while keeping their gaze at a central fixation point.

The observers’ attentional state affects perceptual performance in detection, localization and discrimination tasks [26,20,21] and the activity of sensory neurons throughout the visual cortex [7,20,22]. Recent research has furthered our understanding of how attention modulates the spatiotemporal sensitivity of early perceptual filters, and the neural computations underlying such processes.

Does attention alter our subjective experience of the world? The phenomenology of attention has been a topic of interest for over a century, with the pioneers of experimental psychology—Wundt, Mach, Fechner, Helmholtz and James [19,23,24]. Introspective subjective methods led to conflicting conclusions; e.g., Helmholtz and James claimed that attention intensifies sensory impressions, but Fechner disagreed.

Last century, a few empirical studies reported that attention reduces response variance, but does not change stimulus appearance, rendering a more veridical percept [2527]. However, given their methodology, the results were inconclusive [3,28,29]. Last decade, we implemented a novel psychophysical paradigm to evaluate attention’s phenomenological correlates [28], which enables the objective and rigorous study of subjective experience [29,30].

Exogenous attention alters appearance of spatial dimensions

Contrast is an ideal dimension for investigating attention and appearance. To assess perceived contrast, observers are presented with two equidistant Gabor patches and asked to report the orientation of the higher contrast stimulus (Figure 2). The instructions emphasize the orientation judgment, but the main interest is the contrast judgment. On each trial, one of the patches has a fixed contrast (standard stimuli), whereas the contrast of the other patch (test stimuli) is randomly chosen from a range of contrasts around the standard’s contrast. The point of subjective equality (PSE) is obtained from psychometric functions describing the probability of choosing the test stimulus over the standard as a function of test contrast. The PSE corresponds to the contrast at which the test stimulus appears similar to the standard stimulus. To assess the effects of exogenous attention on perceived contrast, these functions are measured when attention is either distributed across the display, via a neutral cue, or automatically captured at one stimulus location via a peripheral cue. Observers are told that the peripheral cue is uninformative and has equal probability of appearing at either side, independently of the stimulus contrast and orientation.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Exogenous attention alters appearance. (a) Trial sequence: exogenous attention is manipulated using brief, non-informative precues at either both locations (distributed, neutral) or at one location (test cued or standard cued). Observers have to report the orientation of the stimulus with higher contrast. (b) When plotting the proportion of time the test stimulus is chosen as being higher contrast than the standard stimulus (e.g., 20% reference contrast), cueing the test or standard stimulus location shifts the curve leftward (lower PSE) and rightward (higher PSE), respectively. (c) Control conditions: No change is observed when postcues or longer inter-stimulus intervals (ISI) are used. (d) Exogenous attention changes how we subjectively perceive various visual dimensions, except for hue; upper row: stimuli match physical stimuli under neutral condition; bottom row: perceived stimuli change under attention condition.

Using this paradigm, we found that exogenous attention significantly enhances perceived contrast (Figure 2; Table 1). In the neutral condition the PSE matched physical equality. Cueing the test patch shifted the PSE to lower contrasts, whereas cueing the standard patch shifted PSE to higher test contrasts, indicating that the attended stimulus appeared higher in contrast. Moreover, attention improved performance at the cued location in the concurrent orientation discrimination task. Thus, when observers’ attention is drawn to a stimulus location, the stimulus is objectively better discriminated and subjectively perceived as being higher in contrast than it actually was, indicating that attention alters appearance. Several control experiments have ruled out alternative explanations (Figures 2,3; Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of the effects of covert spatial attention on visual appearance.

Visual
Dimension
Perceived
Effect
References Controls and Variants

Contrast Enhanced
Contrast
Exogenous
Carrasco et al., 2004 [28]
Hsieh et al., 2005 [33]
Ling & Carrasco, 2007 [36]
Carrasco et al., 2008 [31]
Fuller et al., 2008 [12]
Fuller et al., 2009 [35]
Störmer et al., 2009 [38]
Anton-Erxleben et al., 2010 [39]
Anton-Erxleben et al., 2011 [40]
Kim et al., 2014 [42]
Cutrone et al., 2014 [32]
Störmer & Alvarez, 2016 [43]
Mishra & Srinivasan, 2017 [44]
Barbot & Carrasco, 2018 [37]
Zhou et al., 2018 [34]

Endogenous
Liu et al., 2009 [60]

• effects eliminated with:
 - postcues [31;35]
 - lengthened ISI [12,31,36,37,38,43,44]
• effects present with:
 - reverse instructions [28;36;39;43]
 - both comparative and equality judgments [34,39,40]
 - auditory precues [38]
 - different cue polarity [36]
 - a wide contrast range; attenuated for very high contrast [32;34]

• effects increase with cue salience:
 - cue contrast [12]
 - cue emotional valence [37]
• changes in performance and appearance correlate [37]
• enhanced apparent contrast can alter higher-level features such as perceived facial attractiveness [43] and emotion [44]

• effects present with reverse instructions [60]

Spatial Dimensions Increased
Spatial
Frequency
Exogenous
Gobell & Carrasco 2005 [46]

• effects eliminated with postcues [46]
• effects present with:
 - both lower and higher spatial frequencies [46]
 - reverse instructions [46]
• effects not due to changes in perceived contrast [46]
Endogenous
Abrams et al. 2010 [61]

• effects eliminated when timing allows voluntarily attention to be redeployed across both stimulus locations [61]

Increased Gap Size Exogenous
Gobell & Carrasco 2005 [46]

• effects eliminated with postcues [46]

Increased
Positional
Repulsion
Exogenous
Pratt & Turk-Browne 2003 [49] Fortenbaugh et al. 2011 [52]
Klein et al. 2016 [51]
Cutrone, et al. 2018 [48]

• effects eliminated with postcues [52]
• apparent shape of objects altered based on precue position [52]
• larger positional biases with eccentricity predicted by an attention field model [51]

Endogenous
Suzuki & Cavanagh 1997 [50] Cutrone, et al. 2018 [48]

• effects modulated by the attentional field size [48]

Increased Object Size Exogenous
Anton-Erxleben et al. 2007 [47]

• effects eliminated with postcues [47]
• effects present with reverse instructions [47]

Enhanced
Perceptual
Organization
Endogenous
Barbot, et al. 2007 [72]

• effects present with reverse instructions [72]
• effects eliminated when timing allows voluntarily attention to be redeployed across both stimulus locations [72]

Color Enhanced
Saturation
Exogenous
Fuller & Carrasco 2006 [53]
Kim et al. 2014 [42]

• effects eliminated with postcues [53]
• effects present:
 - with reverse instructions [53]
 - in ADHD observers [42]

No Change In Hue Exogenous
Fuller & Carrasco 2006 [53]

• even though attention improved performance

Temporal Dimensions Increased
Flickering
Exogenous
Montagna & Carrasco 2006 [56]

• effects present with reverse instructions [56]

Enhanced Motion Coherence Exogenous
Liu et al. 2006 [55]

• effects eliminated with lengthened ISI [55]

Increased
Speed
Exogenous
Turatto et al. 2007 [58]
Fuller et al., 2009 [35]
Anton-Erxleben et al., 2013 [59]

• effects eliminated with:
 - postcues [58]
 - lengthened ISI [35;58]
• effects present with:
 - reverse instructions [58;59]
 - different adaptation states [59]

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Endogenous attention alters appearance. (a) Trial sequence: Observers are asked to report whether a target letter ‘X’ was present in one of two rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) streams. Endogenous attention was manipulated using symbolic precues at fixation indicating one (cued) or both (distributed/neutral) RSVP streams in which the target letter could be presented. In trials in which observers did not see a RSVP target, they were asked to do an appearance task. By varying the delay between the two tasks, we can ensure that voluntary attention is still sustained at the cued location when the stimuli for the appearance task are presented. (b) Similar to exogenous attention, cueing the test or standard stimulus location shifts the curve leftward (lower PSE) and rightward (higher PSE), respectively. (c) Reverse instructions: in addition to other control conditions in which no effect is observed (e.g., lengthened delay), reversing the instructions in the appearance task usually shows changes consistent with an appearance change, rather than a response bias. (d) Endogenous attention changes how we subjectively perceive various visual dimensions; upper row: stimuli match physical stimuli under neutral condition; bottom row: perceived stimuli change under attention condition. For the perceived brightness illustration, maintain fixation on any of the fixation spots while shifting attention from one disk to another. You should notice that the attended disk appears to darken [69].

Attentional changes in appearance have been replicated while manipulating stimulus locations [12], stimulus contrast [3134], cue contrast [35], cue polarity [36], cue type (fearful vs. neutral faces [37]; auditory cues [38], and equality judgments [39,40] (but see [41]), as well as with people with ADHD [42]. Furthermore, increased perceived contrast affects the appearance of higher-level objects like facial attractiveness [43] and facial emotion [44].

Attention also affects performance in spatial resolution tasks [21,45]. The paradigm described above has shown that exogenous attention also increases perceived spatial frequency and gap size [46] and size of moving patterns [47], and alters perceived position. A repulsion effect illustrates that attention can distort the encoding of nearby positions [4851]. Stronger repulsion effects with attention are observed with eccentricity and could be due to attentional shifts in receptive size [9,21,51]. Similarly, drawing attention to the center of a circular stimulus increases its perceived size [47]. Attention also affects the perceived shape of ovals; depending on whether the cue is inside or outside the contour, the stimulus is perceived as longer or shorter [52].

The effects of attention on orientation discrimination and contrast appearance correlate [25]. However, an attention effect on performance does not necessarily lead to enhanced appearance. Despite improving orientation discrimination for both saturation- and hue- defined stimuli, exogenous attention changes apparent color saturation [42,53], but not hue [50]. This differential effect may be related to the nature of perceptual dimensions. Saturation, like contrast, is a directional prothetic dimension. Hue is a metathetic dimension; our percepts of red and blue are qualitatively different; red is neither “more” nor “less” than blue [54].

Exogenous Attention alters appearance of temporal dimensions

Attention also affects the appearance of fundamental dynamic properties (Figure 2). Observers report the motion direction of the stimulus aperture with higher coherence of moving dots out of two simultaneous apertures. Attention enhances perceived motion coherence making the motion direction more salient and improves performance in a concomitant direction discrimination task [55]. Similarly, when using two simultaneously flickering Gabor patches of equal contrast, exogenous attention increases perceived flicker rate [56], much like the effect of increasing stimulus contrast on perceived flicker [57]. Moreover, when observers report the direction of the faster-moving stimulus out of two stimuli, they perceive the stimuli at the attended location as moving faster than at unattended locations [35,58,59].

Voluntary attention alters spatial dimensions

We adapted the appearance paradigm [28] to investigate the effect of voluntary, endogenous attention on subjective experience [60]. We used a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) detection task to engage observers’ focal attention at a location. Observers are instructed to attend either to the cued RSVP stream (central cue) or to both streams (neutral cue) and to detect the presence of a target letter (X, present on 20% of the trials, equally likely at either location). When they detect the X, they press the space bar and ignore the subsequent Gabor patches. When they do not see the X, they report the orientation of the higher-contrast Gabor patch (Figure 3). Crucially, the attention manipulation conveys no information about orientation discrimination, the task of interest. The short inter-stimulus interval (100 ms) between the RSVP streams offset and the Gabor patches is quick enough to prevent attention redeployment, ensuring that attention is still at the peripheral location when the Gabor patches appear.

When observers allocate endogenous attention to a specific location, performance in both the RSVP detection task and the orientation discrimination task improves and perceived contrast increases [60]. Similarly, endogenous attention increases perceived frequency [61] and the repulsion effect [48].

The similar phenomenological consequences of endogenous and exogenous attention, notwithstanding their distinct time courses, control processes and some differential effects on performance [6265], may result from similar cortical computations [66,67].

More evidence that attention influences the contents of visual experience comes from Peter Tse and colleagues, who have shown that voluntary attention increases the duration of afterimages and modulates perceived brightness, color and location [33,6870].

Endogenous attention also alters mid-level vision. Given that attention and perceptual organization modulate each other and affect performance [71], we investigated whether attention alters the perceived perceptual organization of two multi-element arrays, organized by luminance similarity as either rows or columns. We found that endogenous attention intensified apparent perceived organization at the attended location; thus, attention also alters appearance of mid-level vision [72].

Ruling Out Alternative Explanations

Many control experiments (Figures 2,3; Table 1) have ruled out alternative accounts regarding all discussed static and dynamic dimensions:

Reversing the task direction:

When observers report the orientation of the stimulus of lower, rather than higher, apparent contrast, they choose the cued test stimulus less frequently. Were results due to cue bias, observers would have chosen the cued stimulus more often than the other stimulus regardless of the task direction.

Lengthening the cue-target interval:

Due to the ephemeral nature of exogenous attention (~120 ms [5,73], when the cue-target interval is lengthened to 500 ms, neutral and peripheral conditions do not differ.

Postcue:

When observers judge the stimulus followed by a postcue, rather than preceded by a precue, appearance is unaltered, notwithstanding the same spatio-temporal contiguity between cue and stimulus. Were signals integrated over time, the effect would be the same with precues and postcues.

Performance:

Appearance and performance in orientation discrimination or direction discrimination have been concurrently assessed and altered [12,28,36,37,47,53,5861,70]. A cue-bias or a response-bias cannot explain the performance effect.

Visual dimension:

Precueing does not affect hue appearance, although it affects judgments of stimulus saturation [53], as well as of many other static and dynamic dimensions (Table 1). A cue-bias or a response-bias should affect all dimensions similarly.

Notwithstanding the converging results from all these control experiments, alternative explanations have been proposed for the effects of exogenous attention on perceived contrast, but these alternative explanations have been empirically invalidated [[74] but [36]; [75] but [31];[41] but [39,40]].

Peripheral cues were reported to increase perceived brightness only near detection threshold, and it was hypothesized that reversing the cue’s luminance polarity would lead to differential cueing effects on perceived contrast [74]. However, both black and white cues increase apparent contrast of suprathreshold stimuli to the same degree [36], thus ruling out sensory factors.

Location uncertainty has been invoked to explain increased perceived contrast [75]. Such explanation predicts the largest effect for the lowest stimulus contrast, but this is not the case [12,28,3133,3540,42]. Whereas location uncertainty is a relevant factor for near-threshold [75], it is irrelevant for suprathreshold [12,28,31,32,3638,43] stimuli.

Lastly, a cue effect on appearance has been attributed to a decisional bias rather than to attention, and it was reported that there is no attention effect with an equality judgment [41,76]. However, comparing the sensitivity of equality and comparative judgments regarding physical contrast and attentional modulation revealed several methodological limitations that render the equality judgment less sensitive to shifts in perceived contrast [39,40]. Regardless, attention enhances apparent contrast with both comparative and equality judgments [34,39,40].

How does attention alter appearance?

An electrophysiological study confirmed that enhanced perceived contrast at the cued location is attributable to an attention effect on early visual processing. Cueing attention boosts early processing (100–140 ms) of the attended stimulus in the ventral occipito-temporal visual cortex; and the higher the boosting, the higher the perceived contrast [38]. The temporal dynamics and occipito-temporal location of this modulation are consistent with a boost in early sensory processing but not with decision-making processes [38,77]. To further explore the relation between perceived contrast and underlying neural responses, we varied stimulus contrast and modeled perceived contrast as a function of the underlying neural contrast-response functions. An increased input baseline in neural responses accounted for the attention effect on perceived contrast [32,34].

Changes in perceived spatial frequency may be due to heightened sensitivity of higher spatial frequency channels [9,46], which would change the overall pattern of activity across channels, resulting in the phenomenological experience of higher spatial frequency. Such preferential enhancement of high-spatial frequency neurons has been observed with arousal state in mice [78,79], as well as with covert [65,80] and presaccadic [81,82] attention in humans. A similar mechanism may explain increases in perceived flickering rate and apparent speed, which may be correlated with enhanced activity in neuronal populations tuned to higher speed or flickering rate, re-weighting the population response [56,59].

CONCLUSIONS

The appearance paradigms developed to assess the effects of exogenous and endogenous attention on appearance have revealed that attention alters our subjective impression of many dimensions of spatial [12,28,31,33,3538,43,46,52,60,61,72,83] and temporal [9,35,47,5658] vision, mediated by the ventral and dorsal streams, respectively. These findings have implications for models of visual attention, as well as for our understanding of the psychology and philosophy [84] of perception.

Attention effects on contrast appearance have been systematically investigated. Contrast appearance enhancement likely accompanies increased contrast sensitivity [11,66,8590]. The finding that attention increases apparent contrast supports a linking hypothesis stating that the attentional enhancement of neural firing is interpreted as if the stimulus had a higher contrast. Consistent with the hypothesis that both changes reflect a common attentional mechanism modulating contrast sensitivity responses in early visual cortex [7,2830,32,38], changes in performance and in perceived contrast correlate [37]. Converging evidence from neurophysiological, psychophysical, and neuroimaging studies supports this proposal [2,3,7,29,30]. Modeling the appearance task as a function of underlying neural contrast- response functions revealed that an increased input baseline in the neural responses accounted for the enhancement of perceived contrast with covert attention [32].

The visual system optimizes processing resources, often producing non-veridical percepts. Attention augments perception by altering stimulus representation and by emphasizing relevant information with the expense of a sketchy representation of less relevant information. The biophysical machinery of the brain engenders our phenomenological experience of the world: attention affects not only how we perform in a visual task but also how we see and experience our visual world.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Visual appearance varies across the visual field. (a) An original photograph of the Brunnen der Lebensfreude in Rostock, Germany (left image). Using a pooling model, synthetic images (right image) can be generated to appear nearly identical to the original when viewed with fixation at the center (red dot), despite gross distortions in the periphery. Adapted from [14]. (b,c) Visual information appears different across isoeccentric locations; both perceived contrast and perceived spatial frequency are higher along the horizontal than the vertical meridian, and at lower than upper locations along the vertical meridian. Consequently, when viewed with fixation at the center cross, signals of different contrast (b) or different spatial frequency (c) appear to be identical [see 8,12,18]. Given that these effects depend on eccentricity, these figures illustrate qualitative differences.

Carrasco & Barbot - Highlights.

  • covert attention improves sensory processing in many visual tasks

  • covert attention also alters the appearance of visual information

  • many spatial and temporal dimensions are perceived differently with attention

  • attention effects in performance and appearance seem to share a common origin

  • future studies should focus on understanding the underlying mechanisms of appearance effects

Acknowledgments:

M. Carrasco was supported by NIH grants NEI R01 EY027401 and R01 EY019693. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: none

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • [1].Lennie P: The cost of cortical computation. Curr Biol 2003, 13:493–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Carrasco M: Covert attention increases contrast sensitivity: Psychophysical, neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies. Prog Brain Res 2006, 154:33–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Carrasco M: Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vis Res 2011, 51:1484–1525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Carrasco M: Spatial Attention: Perceptual modulation. In The Oxford Handbook of Attention. Edited by Kastner S, Nobre aC: Oxford University Press; 2014:183–230. [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Nakayama K, Mackeben M: Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vis Res 1989, 29:1631–1647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Posner MI: Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 1980, 32:3–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L: Attentional modulation of visual processing. Ann Rev Neurosci 2004, 27:611–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Abrams J, Nizam A, Carrasco M: Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: the extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry. Vis Res 2012, 52:70–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Anton-Erxleben K, Carrasco M: Attentional enhancement of spatial resolution: linking behavioural and neurophysiological evidence. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013, 14:188–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Carrasco M, Frieder KS: Cortical magnification neutralizes the eccentricity effect in visual search. Vis Res 1997, 37:63–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Carrasco M, Talgar CP, Cameron EL: Characterizing visual performance fields: effects of transient covert attention, spatial frequency, eccentricity, task and set size. Spat Vis 2001, 15:61–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Fuller S, Rodriguez RZ, Carrasco M: Apparent contrast differs across the vertical meridian: visual and attentional factors. J Vis 2008, 8:1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Solovey G, Graney GG, Lau H: A decisional account of subjective inflation of visual perception at the periphery. Atten Percept Psychophys 2015, 77:258–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Freeman J, Simoncelli EP: Metamers of the ventral stream. Nat Neuro 2011, 14:1195–1201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Greenwood JA, Bex PJ, Dakin SC: Crowding changes appearance. Curr Biol 2010, 20:496–501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [16].Anderson EJ, Dakin SC, Schwarzkopf SD, Rees G, Greenwood JA: The Neural Correlates of Crowding-Induced Changes in Appearance. Curr Biol 2012, 22:P1199-P1206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Fuller S, Carrasco M: Perceptual consequences of visual performance fields: the case of the line motion illusion. J Vis 2009, 9:13 11–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Montaser-Kouhsari L, Carrasco M: Perceptual asymmetries are preserved in short-term memory tasks. Atten Percept Psychophys 2009, 71:1782–1792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Helmholtz Hv: Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik, Dritter Abschnitt edn Zweite Auflage. Hamburg: Voss; 1896. [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Maunsell JH: Neuronal Mechanisms of Visual Attention. Ann Rev Vis Sci 2015, 1:373–391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Carrasco M, Barbot A: How Attention Affects Spatial Resolution. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 2015, 79:149–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Moore T, Zirnsak M: Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. Ann Rev Psych 2018, 68:47–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].James W: The Principles of Psychology. New York: Henry Holt; 1890. [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Wundt W: Grundriss der Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Engelman. Translated by Judd C. as Outlines of Psychology. 1897, 2nd ed. [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Prinzmetal W, Amiri H, Allen K, Edwards T: Phenomenology of Attention: 1. Color, Location, Orientation and Spatial Frequency. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1998, 21:261–282. [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Prinzmetal W, Nwachuku I, Bodanski L, Blumenfeld L, Shimizu N: The phenomenology of attention. 2. Brightness and contrast. Conscious Cogn 1997, 6:372–412. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Tsal Y, Shalev L, Zakay D, Lubow RE: Attention reduces perceived brightness contrast. Q J Exp Psychol A 1994, 47:865–893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Carrasco M, Ling S, Read S: Attention alters appearance. Nat Neurosci 2004, 7:308–313.This is the first empirical study revealing demonstrating that exogenous attention alters the appearance of information presented within our visual field, enhancing perceived contrast at the attended location while reducing perceived contrast at unattended locations. To investigate this issue, the authors developed a novel protocol to assess effects of exogenous (involuntary) covert attention on appearance, which has since then been used to assess the effect of attention on many other static and dynamic dimensions. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Treue S: Perceptual enhancement of contrast by attention. Trends Cogn Sci 2004, 8:435–437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Luck SJ: Understanding awareness: one step closer. Nat Neurosci 2004, 7:208–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Carrasco M, Fuller S, Ling S: Transient attention does increase perceived contrast of suprathreshold stimuli: A reply to Prinzmetal, Long and Leonhardt. Percept Psychophys 2008, 70:1151–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Cutrone EK, Heeger DJ, Carrasco M: Attention enhances contrast appearance via increased input baseline of neural responses. J Vis 2014, 14:16.This study investigates the effects of exogenous attention on contrast appearance for a wide range of stimulus contrasts, and shows that the attentional enhancement in contrast appearance is best explained by an increase in baseline activity of the underlying neuronal population. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Hsieh PJ, Caplovitz GP, Tse PU: Illusory rebound motion and the motion continuity heuristic. Vis Res 2005, 45:2972–2985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [34].Zhou L, Buetti S, LU S, Cai Y: Attentional effect on contrast appearance: From enhancement to attenuation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2018, 44:806–817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Fuller S, Park Y, Carrasco M: Cue contrast modulates the effects of exogenous attention on appearance. Vis Res 2009, 49:1825–1837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Ling S, Carrasco M: Transient covert attention does alter appearance: a reply to Schneider (2006). Percept Psychophys 2007, 69:1051–1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Barbot A, Carrasco M: Emotion and anxiety potentiate the way attention alters visual appearance. Scientific Reports 2018, 8.This study shows that emotional attentional cues potentiate the effects of exogenous attention on both orientation discrimination performance and perceived contrast. Consistent with a common perceptual origin of both effects, changes in performance and appearance with attention correlate with each other. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [38].Störmer VS, McDonald JJ, Hillyard SA: Cross-modal cueing of attention alters appearance and early cortical processing of visual stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009, 106:22456–22461.This study is the first to provide converging evidence from human electrophysiology and behavior indicating that attention alters appearance. It shows that enhanced perceived contrast with attention is associated with an early boost in activity within early cortical stages of visual processing. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [39].Anton-Erxleben K, Abrams J, Carrasco M: Equality judgments cannot distinguish between attention effects on appearance and criterion: a reply to Schneider (2011). J Vis 2011, 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [40].Anton-Erxleben K, Abrams J, Carrasco M: Evaluating comparative and equality judgments in contrast perception: attention alters appearance. J Vis 2010, 10:1–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Schneider KA, Komlos M: Attention biases decisions but does not alter appearance. J Vis 2008, 8:1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [42].Kim S, Al-Haj M, Fuller S, Chen S, Jain U, Carrasco M, Tannock R: Color vision in ADHD: part 2--does attention influence color perception? Behavioral and brain functions : BBF 2014, 10:39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [43].Störmer VS, Alvarez GA: Attention alters perceived attractiveness. Psychol Sci 2016, 27:563–571.This study shows that by enhancing apparent contrast, attention alters the appearance of higher- level features, such as facial attractiveness. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [44].Mishra M, Srinivasan N: Exogenous attention intensifies perceived emotion expressions. Neuroscience of Consciousness 2017, 3:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [45].Carrasco M, Yeshurun Y: Covert Attention Effects on Spatial Resolution. In Prog Brain Res. Edited by Srinivasan N: Elsevier; 2009:65–86. vol 176.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [46].Gobell J, Carrasco M: Attention alters the appearance of spatial frequency and gap size. Psychol Sci 2005, 16:644–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [47].Anton-Erxleben K, Henrich C, Treue S: Attention changes perceived size of moving visual patterns. J Vis 2007, 7:1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [48].Cutrone EK, Heeger DJ, Carrasco M: On spatial attention and its field size on the repulsion effect J Vis 2018, 18:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [49].Pratt J, Turk-Browne NB: The attentional repulsion effect in perception and action. Experimental Brain Research 2003, 152:376–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [50].Suzuki S, Cavanagh P: Focused attention distorts visual space: an attentional repulsion effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1997, 23:443–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [51].Klein BP, Paffen CLE, te Pas SF, Dumoulin SO: Predicting bias in perceived position using attention field models. Journal of Vision 2016, 16.This study proposes an attention field model to explain how receptive field attraction towards the attended location alters perceived spatial position depending on the size of the underlying receptive fields. Given that receptive field size increases with eccentricity, this model captures the fact that attention results in stronger positional biases as a function of eccentricity. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [52].Fortenbaugh FC, Prinzmetal W, Robertson LC: Rapid changes in visual-spatial attention distort object shape. Psychon Bull Rev 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [53].Fuller S, Carrasco M: Exogenous attention and color perception: performance and appearance of saturation and hue. Vis Res 2006, 46:4032–4047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [54].Stevens SS, Galanter EH: Ratio scales and category scales for a dozen perceptual continua. J Exp Psychol 1957, 54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [55].Liu T, Fuller S, Carrasco M: Attention alters the appearance of motion coherence. Psychon Bull Rev 2006, 13:1091–1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [56].Montagna B, Carrasco M: Transient covert attention and the perceived rate of flicker. J Vis 2006, 6:955–965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [57].Thompson P, Stone LS: Contrast affects flicker and speed perception differently. Vis Res 1997, 37:1255–1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [58].Turatto M, Vescovi M, Valsecchi M: Attention makes moving objects be perceived to move faster. Vis Res 2007, 47:166–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [59].Anton-Erxleben K, Herrmann K, Carrasco M: Independent effects of adaptation and attention on perceived speed. Psychol Sci 2013, 24:150–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [60].Liu T, Abrams J, Carrasco M: Voluntary attention enhances contrast appearance. Psychol Sci 2009, 20:354–362.Implementing a novel protocol to assess effects of endogenous (voluntary) covert attention on appearance, this study reveals that endogenous attention also enhances perceived contrast at the attended location and reduces it at unattended locations. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [61].Abrams J, Barbot A, Carrasco M: Voluntary attention increases perceived spatial frequency. Atten Percept Psychophys 2010, 72:1510–1521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [62].Yeshurun Y, Carrasco M: Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature 1998, 396:72–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [63].Yeshurun Y, Carrasco M: The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation. Nat Neurosci 2000, 3:622–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [64].Yeshurun Y, Montagna B, Carrasco M: On the flexibility of sustained attention and its effects on a texture segmentation task. Vis Res 2008, 48:80–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [65].Barbot A, Carrasco M: Attention Modifies Spatial Resolution According to Task Demands. Psychol Sci 2017, 28:285–296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [66].Herrmann K, Montaser-Kouhsari L, Carrasco M, Heeger DJ: When size matters: attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nat Neurosci 2010, 13:1554–1559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [67].Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ: The normalization model of attention. Neuron 2009, 61:168–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [68].Tse PU, Whitney D, Anstis S, Cavanagh P: Voluntary attention modulates motion- induced mislocalization. J Vis 2011, 11:1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [69].Tse PU: Voluntary attention modulates the brightness of overlapping transparent surfaces. Vis Res 2005, 45:1095–1098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [70].Tse PU, Reavis EA, Kohler PJ, Caplovitz GP, Wheatley T: How can attention alter appearances. In Wiley Handbook on Perception of Appearances, edn First edition. Edited by Albertazzi L: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • [71].Kimchi R: Perceptual organization and visual attention. Prog Brain Res 2009, 176:15–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [72].Barbot A, Liu S, Kimchi R, Carrasco M: Attention enhances apparent perceptual organization. Psychon Bull Rev 2017:1–9.This study shows that endogenous (voluntary) covert attention alters the perceived perceptual organization of multi-element arrays-which corresponds to mid-level stages of visual processing.27368622 [Google Scholar]
  • [73].Remington RW, Johnston JC, Yantis S: Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Percept Psychophys 1992, 51:279–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [74].Schneider KA: Does attention alter appearance? Percept Psychophys 2006, 68:800–814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [75].Prinzmetal W, Long V, Leonhardt J: Involuntary attention and brightness contrast. Percept Psychophys 2008, 70:1139–1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [76].Valsecchi M, Vescovi M, Turatto M: Are the effects of attention on speed judgments genuinely perceptual?. Atten Percept Psychophys 2010, 72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [77].Carrasco M: Cross-modal attention enhances perceived contrast. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009, 106:22039–22040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [78].Mineault P, Tring E, Trachtenberg J, Ringach D: Enhanced Spatial Resolution During Locomotion and Heightened Attention in Mouse Primary Visual Cortex. J of Neurosci 2016, 36:6382–6392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [79].Barbot A: How attention enhances spatial resolution: Preferential gain enhancement of high spatial frequency neurons. J of Neurosci 2016, 36:12080–12082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [80].Carrasco M, Loula F, Ho YX: How attention enhances spatial resolution: evidence from selective adaptation to spatial frequency. Percept Psychophys 2006, 68:1004–1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [81].Li H-H, Barbot A, Carrasco M: Saccade Preparation Reshapes Sensory Tuning. Curr Biol 2016, 26:1564–1570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [82].Li H-H, Pan J, Carrasco M: Beyond prioritization: when presaccadic attention impairs performance. [abstract]. Vision Science Society; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • [83].Abrams J, Barbot A, Carrasco M: Endogenous attention alters the appearance of spatial frequency. [Abstract]. Vision Society Society; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • [84].B N: Attention and mental paint. Philosophical Issues 2010, 20:23–63. [Google Scholar]
  • [85].Cameron EL, Tai JC, Carrasco M: Covert attention affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity. Vis Res 2002, 42:949–967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [86].Carrasco M, Penpeci-Talgar C, Eckstein M: Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement. Vis Res 2000, 40:1203–1215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [87].Ling S, Carrasco M: Sustained and transient covert attention enhance the signal via different contrast response functions. Vis Res 2006, 46:1210–1220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [88].Lu ZL, Dosher BA: External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vis Res 1998, 38:1183–1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [89].Lu ZL, Dosher BA: Spatial attention: different mechanisms for central and peripheral temporal precues? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2000, 26:1534–1548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [90].Pestilli F, Ling S, Carrasco M: A population-coding model of attention’s influence on contrast response: Estimating neural effects from psychophysical data. Vis Res 2009, 49:1144–1153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES