Skip to main content
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2020 Oct 19;375(1813):20200081. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0081

Temporal dynamics of competitive fertilization in social groups of red junglefowl (Gallus gallus) shed new light on avian sperm competition

Rômulo Carleial 1,✉,, Grant C McDonald 1,2,✉,, Lewis G Spurgin 3, Eleanor A Fairfield 3, Yunke Wang 1, David S Richardson 3, Tommaso Pizzari 1,
PMCID: PMC7661449  PMID: 33070718

Abstract

Studies of birds have made a fundamental contribution to elucidating sperm competition processes, experimentally demonstrating the role of individual mechanisms in competitive fertilization. However, the relative importance of these mechanisms and the way in which they interact under natural conditions remain largely unexplored. Here, we conduct a detailed behavioural study of freely mating replicate groups of red junglefowl, Gallus gallus, to predict the probability that competing males fertilize individual eggs over the course of 10-day trials. Remating frequently with a female and mating last increased a male's probability of fertilization, but only for eggs ovulated in the last days of a trial. Conversely, older males, and those mating with more polyandrous females, had consistently lower fertilization success. Similarly, resistance to a male's mating attempts, particularly by younger females, reduced fertilization probability. After considering these factors, male social status, partner relatedness and the estimated state of male extragonadal sperm reserves did not predict sperm competition outcomes. These results shed new light on sperm competition dynamics in taxa such as birds, with prolonged female sperm storage and staggered fertilizations.

This article is part of the theme issue ‘Fifty years of sperm competition’.

Keywords: mating order, passive sperm loss, remating rates, reproductive senescence, post-copulatory sexual selection, paternity share

1. Introduction

Parker (1970) [1] revolutionized sexual selection theory by proposing that when females obtain sperm from multiple males (polyandry), competition among males continues after mating as their ejaculates compete to fertilize a set of ova, a process which became known as sperm competition [29]. While Parker's (1970) intuition was inspired by insects, the realization that polyandry is ubiquitous among sexually reproducing organisms and often intense [10] has catalysed the study of sperm competition across different taxa [1113]. An increasing number of studies have sought to experimentally isolate the causal effect of individual sperm competition mechanisms on male reproductive success by quantifying the overall share in paternity at a particular point in time, e.g. the end of a reproductive cycle. This research has been instrumental in identifying male- [4,6,7,1318] and female-driven [19,20] mechanisms underpinning paternity share under controlled conditions.

In natural populations, however, multiple variables act simultaneously and interact with each other to determine the outcome of sperm competition. Understanding these interactions is particularly challenging for many internally fertilizing taxa where prolonged female sperm storage increases the temporal window for sperm competition [21,22] and generates opportunity for complex patterns of sperm storage and precedence [23]. For example, in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, multiple mechanisms including passive sperm release from the female storage organs, active displacement of resident sperm by the second-male sperm, and female-controlled sperm ejection, lead to approximately 80% of eggs being fertilized by the second male to copulate with doubly mated females [24,25]. An understanding of the temporal dynamics of competitive fertilization in natural settings remains elusive, however, largely owing to the difficulty in disentangling multiple mechanisms occurring within the female reproductive tract. Addressing this outstanding challenge is fundamental to resolving the operation of sexual selection and related processes, such as alternative mating strategies, sexual conflict and the maintenance of genetic variation in natural populations [5,26,27].

Historically, birds have represented a key vertebrate model system for the empirical study of sperm competition [12,2830], and a crucial counterpoint to studies of insects [1,13,24,25,3134]. For example, the juxtaposition of avian and insect sperm competition dynamics has helped clarify the differential roles of sperm size and numbers in these clades [35]. The importance of birds is in part owing to field studies of extra-pair copulation and extra-pair paternity in socially monogamous species [8,12,29,3639] and the wealth of information on the reproductive biology of the domestic fowl, Gallus domesticus, and other domestic galliforms, which have informed the development of mechanistic models of sperm competition [30,40,41]. As in most other species of birds, female fowl typically ovulate one egg on each successive day of the laying cycle, with fertilization occurring shortly after ovulation [42]. Females store viable sperm in epithelial invaginations at the utero-vaginal junction of the oviduct—the sperm storage tubules (SSTs)—for up to approximately two weeks [4043]. During this time, sperm are thought to be passively lost from SSTs at a near constant rate, and sperm from competing ejaculates mix randomly within individual SSTs. Passive sperm loss combined with random sperm mixing within SSTs predicts that—all else being equal—the last male to copulate with a female should retain a fertilization advantage for subsequent eggs, which is proportional to the time elapsed between competing inseminations [30,41]. The extent of this advantage is modulated by a range of factors. For example, shortly before and after oviposition, the fertilization success of an ejaculate is reduced owing to oviduct contractions associated with egg laying (reviewed in [30]). Moreover, studies of domestic populations, which experimentally controlled for insemination order through the simultaneous artificial insemination of two competing ejaculates, have demonstrated that relative differences in either sperm numbers [44] or fertilizing efficiency [45,46] explain variation in paternity share between two competitors. Subsequent artificial insemination work in which both sperm number and fertilizing efficiency (mobility) were experimentally manipulated has provided evidence of temporal dynamics in a population of domestic fowl: differences in sperm numbers predicted the paternity of eggs produced shortly following insemination, while differences in mobility predicted paternity later in the laying sequence [47]. Collectively, these experimental studies form the basis of our operational understanding of the mechanisms of avian sperm competition, and suggest that the probability that a given male fertilizes a particular egg is determined by the number of other competitors inseminating the same female, the time elapsed between these inseminations and ovulation, and the size and fertilizing efficiency of the inseminations of this male in relation to those of his competitors, broadly confirming patterns identified by previous work [48]. However, the extent to which this view adequately captures sperm competition dynamics in natural bird populations remains unknown. Some studies of natural populations have shown that the number of sperm reaching an egg and paternity share can change systematically over the course of a laying cycle in some species [4956]. Other studies, however, have failed to confirm these results [54,56], or found less conclusive [57] or more complex patterns [58,59]. A fundamental source of inconsistency is that most studies of free-ranging groups typically lack information about individual mating events, which makes it impossible to assess the level of sperm competition associated with the fertilization of individual ova. In the current study, we aim to address this knowledge gap by combining fine-grained information on socio-sexual interactions and parentage of individual zygotes in replicate flocks of red junglefowl, G. gallus, the main species that has given rise to the domestic fowl.

In nature, red junglefowl live in polygynandrous groups, socially structured in sex-specific hierarchies [60]. Recent work indicates that variance in paternity share is the largest source of variance in male reproductive success, indicating the importance of sperm competition and post-copulatory sexual selection in these populations [61,62]. Sperm competition favours males that remate with the same female at a high frequency [61,62]. A similar advantage derived by frequent remating with the same female has been reported for some wild bird species [63] and in other taxa, e.g. some insects [6466]. Mating in fowl is strongly modulated by the interaction of female and male behaviours. Studies of domestic fowl and red junglefowl indicate that while in small, strongly female-biased groups mating is largely driven by females, in larger groups and groups with a higher proportion of males, the majority of mating attempts is initiated by males [60]. However, differential female behaviour plays an important role in determining the outcome of these male mating attempts. For example, females can influence the success of an insemination by differentially resisting copulation attempts, manipulating male–male competition and copulation interference, and by ejecting semen after insemination [60]. These mechanisms can be modulated by characteristics of individual males and females, and their interactions. For example, male social status mediates competitive access to mates and mating opportunities, and is favoured by female responses before and after mating [60]. On the other hand, male status may be negatively related to sperm fertilizing efficiency [46,67], as observed in species with more distinct alternative mating tactics [68,69], suggesting a possible trade-off between male investment in pre- versus post-copulatory intrasexual competition. Similarly, male age has been linked to lower fertilization success, as older males transfer fewer sperm than younger males [70,71]. Moreover, sperm of older males tend to swim more slowly, possibly owing to lower levels of seminal antioxidants [72]. Female age may also affect the outcome of sperm competition. On the one hand, older females are less polyandrous and avoid males more often than younger females [73], and have a faster rate of sperm loss from the SSTs than younger females [74]. On the other, resistance to males and male coercion are costly [75] and older females might be less able to resist male attempts. Finally, genetic relatedness between a male and a female may influence fertilization success. Experimental evidence suggests that red junglefowl females favour sperm from unrelated males after mating, possibly in order to reduce the risks of inbreeding depression [76,77], a pattern observed in some other taxa [20]. Despite this experimental work, however, we currently lack an understanding of how these mechanisms collectively influence sperm competition dynamics in social groups of freely interacting birds.

Here, we study mechanisms underpinning fowl sperm competition by using detailed mating behaviour and parentage data of freely mating replicate groups of a captive population of red junglefowl. This dataset has been previously analysed to investigate the relationship between male phenotypes and overall male reproductive success [62], the role of female sociality on the structure of socio-sexual networks [73], and changes in female and male phenotype associated with mating trials [75]. This earlier work identified a role for both male and female behaviours in determining variation in individual reproductive success [62,73,75]. The current study consolidates and builds on this earlier work, with a drastically different analytical approach which investigates day-to-day variation in the probability of fertilization of individual eggs laid over successive days of the mating trial. By doing so, the present study moves beyond previous analyses of variation in overall reproductive success and develops a novel methodological platform to examine the relative importance of different factors dynamically through time. We predicted that males which remate more often and those that copulate last with a female will have a higher probability of fertilizing an egg. Based on the passive sperm loss model [30], we predicted that the last male advantage should increase with the time elapsed between competing inseminations. We further predicted that probability of fertilization declines with male age, the level of female resistance (which we predicted to be weaker in older females), the relatedness of the male, the number of males mating with the female and the depletion of a male's extragonadal sperm stores through previous copulations. Male status might have a positive effect, e.g. through preferential female sperm retention, or a negative effect, through a possible trade-off with fertilizing efficiency.

2. Material and methods

We studied a population of red junglefowl at the John Krebs Field Station of the University of Oxford. We analysed the data collected in 20 mating trials conducted in 2011–2013. Detailed accounts of the protocol of the mating trials have been published elsewhere [62,73,75]. Briefly, in each mating trial one of us (G.C.M.) observed a group (males = 10, females = 12) for 3 h in the morning and 3 h in the evening on each of 10 consecutive days (day 1–10). We recorded all male and female mating related behaviours (males: successful and unsuccessful mating attempts, courtship waltzes; females: mating solicitations, level of resistance to mating attempts [78]), and male agonistic interactions (pecks, fights, aggressive waltzes and avoidances). Eggs (n = 982) were collected daily from the second day of the trial (day 2) to the day following the end of the trial (day 11), artificially incubated for 5–9 days, and parentage of the embryos was assigned molecularly with 95% confidence for 849 of these eggs [62].

We analysed the effect of multiple predictor variables on the probability of fertilization of individual eggs by competing males, which were defined as all males that mated successfully with the female up to, and including, the day before the laying of an egg. Thus, copulations on the day an egg was laid were not considered as competing for the fertilization of that egg, because it is unlikely that sperm from these inseminations could have reached the egg in time for fertilization [30,40,42]. We considered the following predictor variables (i.e. fixed effects, see the electronic supplementary material for fuller discussion): (i) number of male competitors, (ii) remating rate (the frequency at which a male mates with the same female up until the day prior to egg-laying), (iii) mating order, (iv) male social status (calculated using Elo scores [79]), (v) male age, (vi) average female resistance to mating attempts of the focal male (scored following [78]), (vii) female age, (viii) male–female relatedness (calculated using Wang's estimator [80,81]), (ix) an index of the male extragonadal sperm reserves (MSR, electronic supplementary material, figure S1), and (x) time (i.e. the day of the mating trial when the egg was laid).

(a). Model comparison

All analyses were conducted in R v. 3.6.2 [82], using the ‘lme4’ v. 1.1–21 package [83]. We built a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with binomial distribution and logit function with the fertilization outcome of each egg (unsuccessful males = 0, successful male = 1) as the response variable, predictors (i–ix) and their interaction with time (x) as fixed effects (mod13, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1). We used relative values of a predictor variable (i.e. the value of each male divided by the mean value of all males competing for that particular egg), to standardize a male's value in relation to his direct competitors and to avoid convergence problems owing to large scaling differences among predictors. Mating order (iii) and relatedness (viii) were not standardized because they were already expressed in relative terms. Collinearity across predictors was calculated using variance inflation factors (VIFs) implemented in the package ‘car’ v. 3.0–3 [84]. All predictors showed VIFs < 3 and were, therefore, retained [85]. We compared this model (mod13) against simpler models with fewer predictors using the Akaike's information criterion (AIC [86]), such that a difference (ΔAIC) < 2 was interpreted as no difference of fit between models, 2 ≤ ΔAIC < 4 as a difference of fit, 4 ≤ ΔAIC < 10 as a considerable difference of fit and ΔAIC ≥ 10 as a strong preference for one model over the other [87]. Overall, we defined 44 models a priori (electronic supplementary material, table S1) using different combinations of predictors, and compared them simultaneously using the package ‘AICcmodavg'v. 2.2–2 [88]. All 44 models also included male identity, female identity, female group identity (i.e. the identity of the unique unit of 12 females used in each trial [42]), and mating group identity (i.e. the identity of the unique group of 10 males 12 females used in each trial [42]) as random effects to account for sources of non-independence [42]. We quantified the variance explained by each model as marginal R2 (R2GLMM(m)), i.e. the variance explained by fixed effects alone, and conditional R2 (R2GLMM(c)), i.e. the variance explained by the combination of the fixed and random effects [89,90]. Finally, while a male's remating rate (predictor (ii)) reflects his sperm investment in a female, it does not take into consideration that sperm are passively lost from the female SSTs over time. Therefore, we also constructed GLMMs replacing (ii) with an estimated value of a male's sperm numbers in the female SSTs at the time of fertilization (female sperm reserves), which was calculated using the rate of sperm loss described for domestic fowl [91,92] (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). We compared AIC scores between models using (ii) against models replacing (ii) with female sperm reserves (electronic supplementary material, table S2).

Table 1.

Comparison of selected binomial generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs), predicting the probability that male red junglefowl, Gallus gallus, fertilize individual eggs, ranked according to AIC. (For all models see the electronic supplementary material, table S1. A, male age; C, number of competitors; FA, female age; FR, female resistance; MO, mating order; MSR, male sperm reserves; R, relatedness; RR, remating rate, SS, social status; T, time; K, number of parameters; AIC, Akaike information criterion; ΔAIC, with respect to mod1; Rm2,marginalR2; Rc2,conditionalR2.)

model ID fixed effects K AIC ΔAIC Rm2 Rc2
mod1 RR*T + MO*T + C + A + FR*FA 15 3621.33 0 0.1498 0.1959
mod13 RR*T + MO*T + C*T + A*T + FR*FA + SS*T + MSR*T + R*T 23 3633.24 11.92 0.1460 0.1917
mod24 C 6 3851.02 229.69 0.0550 0.1318
mod30 FR 6 3968.72 347.4 0.0205 0.1355
mod33 RR 6 3997.33 376.03 0.0112 0.1168
mod38 A 6 4016.99 395.66 0.0189 0.0907
mod39 SS 6 4040.27 418.95 0.0014 0.1097
mod41 MO 6 4041.31 419.98 0.0004 0.1089
mod43 R 6 4042.1 420.78 0.0002 0.1107
mod44 MSR 6 4042.24 420.91 0.0002 0.1076

(b). The effect of individual predictors

We tested the statistical significance of individual predictors in the most complex model (mod13, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1), by performing likelihood ratio tests removing each variable of interest while holding constant the effect of all other predictors. Interaction terms that were not significant were dropped before conclusions were drawn on the significance of the main effects. To further confirm temporal patterns, we explored the effect of individual predictors (i–ix) on fertilization probability, by running separate GLMMs for each individual day of the trial. Results were qualitatively similar to the original approach (see the electronic supplementary material, table S3 and figure S4).

3. Results

Models including female sperm reserves had lower AIC scores than equivalent models with remating in 11 cases (ΔAIC ≥ 6.46, electronic supplementary material, table S2). In the remaining cases (n = 15), mostly when time was included in the model as an interaction term, both models had similar AIC scores (ΔAIC ≤ 2.18, electronic supplementary material, table S2). This suggests that female sperm reserves captures a temporal dimension of sperm competition, and becomes redundant when time is included in the model explicitly. Therefore, we only report models with remating rate below, as this approach does not rely on additional assumptions of patterns of sperm loss from the SSTs.

(a). Model comparison

Using an AIC approach, the best model predicting the fertilization probability of individual eggs (mod1, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) included a male's remating rate and its interaction with time (i.e. day of the trial), male mating order and its interaction with time, number of competing males, male age and the interaction between overall female resistance and female age. Male–female relatedness, his sperm reserves (MSR) and male social status were not included in the best model. The best model was only slightly better than the model without the interaction between female resistance and female age (mod2, ΔAIC = 2.86, electronic supplementary material, table S1), but performed considerably better than all other models (ΔAIC > 4, electronic supplementary material, table S1).

The best model explained 14.98% of the variance by fixed effects alone (i.e. R2GLMM(m)), and 19.59% when random effects were included (i.e. R2GLMM(c)). When considering R2GLMM(m) for models including only a single predictor, the model with number of competing males explained the most variance (5.5%, mod24, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) in the probability of fertilization. The model with female resistance (mod30, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) explained 2.05%, male age (mod38, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) explained 1.89%, remating rate (mod33, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) explained 1.12%, male social status (mod39, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) explained 0.14% and mating order (mod41, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) explained 0.04% of the variance in fertilization success, respectively. The remaining two predictors, relatedness and MSR, explained each 0.02% (mod43, mod44, table 1, electronic supplementary material, table S1) of the variance.

(b). The effect of individual predictors

Results of the model including all main effects and the statistically significant interactions are summarized in table 2.

Table 2.

Summary statistics and likelihood ratio tests results of the generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM) predicting egg fertilization probability in multiple replicate groups of red junglefowl, Gallus gallus. (Model contains all main effects and statistically significant interactions (mod5, electronic supplementary material, table S1). Notes: LRT, likelihood ratio test; σ2, residual variance; τ00, random intercept variance; p-values in bold identify statistically significant effects (<0.05).)

predictors egg fertilization success
estimate (odds ratio) confidence interval LRT p
intercept 30.09 12.19–74.25
remating rate (RR) 0.93 0.61–1.42
time (T) 0.82 0.75–0.89
mating order (MO) 0.92 0.80–1.05
female resistance (FR) 0.12 0.07–0.21
female age (FA) 0.85 0.70–1.02
number of competitors (C) 0.65 0.61–0.69 193.18 <0.001
male age (A) 0.44 0.35–0.55 51.43 <0.001
relatedness (R) 0.88 0.64–1.20 0.64 0.423
social status (SS) 0.99 0.73–1.33 0.01 0.936
sperm reserves (MSR) 1.08 0.86–1.36 0.42 0.515
MO:T 1.08 1.01–1.16 13.17 <0.001
RR:T 1.04 1.02–1.06 5.72 0.018
FR:FA 1.23 1.02–1.49 4.88 0.027
random effects
σ2 3.29
τ00 maleID 0.35
τ00 femaleID 0
τ00 femalegroupID 0
τ00 groupID 0
observations 4134

We found a positive and significant interaction between a male's remating rate and time, such that males which copulated more often with the female had a higher probability of fertilization later in the trial (remating rate *time: χ12=5.72, p = 0.016; figure 1a). Similarly, males that tended to mate last with a female had a higher probability of fertilizing eggs laid later in the trial (mating order * time: χ12=13.17, p < 0.001; figure 1b), as predicted by the passive sperm loss model. Such last male advantage later in a trial may be caused by increasing variation in the time elapsed between the last insemination by competing males and fertilization. We explored this hypothesis by testing post hoc whether the time interval (gap) between the last insemination by the first and last competitor male in the mating order tended to increase over successive days in the trial. Consistent with expectations, the time gap spanning the mating order increased over the course of the trial (log(time gap) ∼ mating order * time: χ12=98.85, p < 0.001, electronic supplementary material, figure S5), suggesting that some males which mate with a female early in a trial are disadvantaged in sperm competition later in the trial as they fail to remate with the female.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Relationships between different predictors and the probability that competing males fertilize individual eggs in semi-natural groups of red junglefowl, Gallus gallus. Panels represent the marginal effects of a predictor based on the generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) in table 2. (a) Relative remating rate and its interaction with time in days, (b) mating order and its interaction with time in days, (c) relative female resistance and its interaction with female age in years, (d) number of competing males, (e) relative male age, (f) relative male sperm reserves (MSR), (g) relatedness between a male–female pair, and (h) relative male Elo score (i.e. social status). Shaded areas represent the 95% confidence intervals. Colour coding in panels (ac) represents three arbitrary values of the interaction term ((a) and (b) time: day 2, day 5, day 9; (c) female age: 1, 5, 7 years old). Asterisks represent statistical significance (table 2), such that * = p < 0.05, and *** = p < 0.001.

Males that experienced more intense female resistance across copulation attempts were significantly less likely to fertilize eggs overall (resistance: χ12=101.82, p < 0.001), and this effect was constant over time (resistance * time: χ12=1.99, p = 0.159), but was dependent on female age. Males were less likely to fertilize when resisted by younger females (resistance * female age: χ12=4.86, p = 0.027; figure 1c). Competing against more males decreased a male's probability of fertilization (competitors: χ12=193.18, p < 0.001; figure 1d), but there was no evidence that this effect varied over the course of a trial (competitors * time: χ12=0.24, p = 0.628). Similarly, older males had a lower probability of fertilization overall (male age: χ12=51.43, p < 0.001; figure 1e), which was consistent throughout the course of a trial (male age * time: χ12=0.46, p = 0.496).

The state of a focal male's MSR did not increase his overall probability of fertilization (MSR: χ12=0.42, p = 0.515; figure 1f), and this relationship did not change over time (MSR * time: χ12=0.26, p = 0.614). Similarly, a focal male's relatedness with the female did not predict fertilization probability (relatedness: χ12=0.64, p = 0.423; figure 1g), and this pattern did not change over time (relatedness * time: χ12=0.16, p = 0.694). Finally, social status was not associated with a focal male's probability of fertilization (Elo score: χ12=0.01, p = 0.936; figure 1h), and this pattern did not change over time (Elo score * time: χ12=0.31, p = 0.580). Male identity explained almost all the variance of random effects (electronic supplementary material, figure S3).

4. Discussion

Parker's intuition of sperm competition [1] kickstarted a new field of research, which has exploded over recent decades [810]. This effort has elucidated the independent role of numerous mechanisms including physiological, genetic, behavioural and morphological traits in determining competitive fertilization. Despite this, we still have a poor understanding of how these mechanisms interact with each other to drive dynamics of sperm competition in natural populations. This is particularly the case for the many taxa, such as birds, with internal fertilization, prolonged female sperm storage and iterated insemination and fertilization events, mostly owing to the difficulty in linking mating to fertilization and following temporal patterns of ejaculate utilization. In this study, we combined detailed information on mating behaviour with molecular parentage data from groups of red junglefowl to explain variation in the probability of individual males fertilizing individual eggs over successive days. We show that around 20% of the variance is explained by information gleaned from mating behaviour data. The degree of female polyandry has an intuitive negative impact on a male's probability of fertilization; however, the outcome of sperm competition among males is modulated by four other factors.

First, a male is more likely to fertilize a female's eggs—especially those ovulated in the last days of a trial—if he mates more frequently with her. Previous work had shown that post-copulatory sexual selection favours males that remate more frequently with their partners in this population [61,62], but there has been no attempt at quantifying temporal effects. Repeated matings between partners are often observed in birds and the significance of this behaviour has long been debated [93]. Because females can continue to ovulate over successive days following an insemination, and because sperm are lost at a constant rate from the female SSTs, male birds may be selected to top up their sperm representation in a female's SSTs by remating rather than inseminating a single large ejaculate. In addition, we now show that this effect is progressively accentuated over successive days; multiple days of remating are required for males to accumulate sufficiently more sperm than rivals within the female SSTs in order to have an advantage in sperm competition. Positive post-copulatory sexual selection on remating rates has also been demonstrated in some insects [64,66], although in these studies the advantage to remating may be related to mating order, as males that mate more frequently with a female may be more likely to mate last and displace the sperm previously inseminated by rivals. Thus, in polyandrous populations characterized by a degree of sperm competition intensity [61,62], selection may favour males that mate with fewer females, but are able to remate with these females more frequently [94].

Second, controlling for remating rates, we found an interaction between mating order and time; such that mating last increases the fertilization probability of males late in a trial. Establishing the effect of mating order is critical to evaluate different models of sperm competition. A last male advantage is commonly observed in a number of taxa [13]. In birds, different sperm competition models have been explored [29,30,41]. First, sperm from different inseminations may stratify within the female's SSTs, with sperm from later copulations ‘sitting’ on top of sperm from earlier copulations. Second, a new insemination may displace sperm of previous inseminations from the female SSTs. Finally, sperm from earlier inseminations may be lost over time owing to passive sperm loss from the SSTs [92,95]. All three models predict some degree of last male advantage in sperm competition, but only the passive sperm loss model predicts that this advantage increases with the time elapsed between competing inseminations [92,95]. Despite early suggestions of sperm stratification in individual SSTs [96], analysis of artificial insemination experiments in domestic fowl indicated that avian sperm competition may conform to passive sperm loss [29,30,41,92,95]. A subsequent careful differential sperm staining experiment in domestic fowl and domestic turkeys, Meleagris gallopavo, found no evidence of sperm stratification [97], with sperm of rival inseminations largely segregated in different SSTs in both species, although some of the visual patterns presented raised the possibility of sperm stratification when rival inseminations mixed within individual SSTs (e.g. fig. 2c [97]). The sperm stratification model predicts that the first insemination may experience a renewed increase in fertilization probability later in the laying sequence, as the sperm from subsequent inseminations are depleted allowing the sperm from the first insemination to leave the SSTs. Our post hoc analysis showed that the time lag between the last copulations of competing males tended to increase over successive days of the trial, which is consistent with passive sperm loss but less so with sperm stratification. This suggests that most males mate early with a female but only some manage to remate with her later on in the trial. Mating later in the trial provides these males with a fertilization advantage as the sperm of previous competitors will have been depleted from a female's SSTs through passive loss. Mating last in the first days of the trial does not convey the same advantage, instead our results suggest that, early in the trial, mating first might be advantageous. This conclusion is also supported by our attempts to model sperm loss directly, which yielded considerably better fits than simply modelling remating rates in almost half of the models. Importantly, segregation of rival ejaculates across different SSTs may be a common pattern observed in galliform [97] and passerine birds [98,99]. Random occupation of different individual SSTs by rival ejaculates would probably result in an overall pattern of sperm loss similar to that predicted under conditions of random sperm mixing within individual SSTs [6]. Non-random segregation patterns, however, might buffer or accentuate patterns of paternity share predicted by the passive sperm loss model. For example, last male sperm precedence might be favoured if sperm from the last insemination were preferentially stored in the SSTs closer to the influndibulum, which mature later, resulting in a possible fertilizing advantage [98]. The lack of remating by many males may occur for two non-mutually exclusive reasons. (i) It may take some time for the social hierarchies to influence differential access to mating opportunities, and mating later in the trial may become increasingly difficult for low-ranking males, as dominant males progressively monopolize females [60]. Consistent with this, dominant males were more likely to fertilize eggs on the eighth day of the trial (see Analysing each day of the trial separately, in the electronic supplementary material). (ii) Mating can be costly for males in these groups [75], and only some males (e.g. younger or in better condition) may be able to maintain high mating rates throughout the trial. These findings highlight the importance of accounting for the temporal dynamics underpinning sperm competition in species with prolonged sperm storage.

Third, our results suggest that female resistance may impair the fertilization probability of successful mating attempts by compromising sperm transfer. Male fowl are on average larger than females, and frequently coerce females into copulating [60], which can undermine female pre-copulatory choice. Female responses to male harassment in fowl range from acquiescence to costly resistance [60]. Previous work in feral domestic fowl has shown that female resistance reduces the probability that a copulation attempt leads to successful sperm transfer [100]. Another possibility is that females are more likely to eject sperm following resisted copulations. Previous studies have demonstrated that female fowl eject sperm differentially [101,102], and the risk of sperm ejection increases over successive matings [101]. Female resistance is common in birds and can be intense in some species [28,103], however, its influence on the reproductive outcome has seldom been quantified. In mallards, Anas platyrhynchos, female resistance reduces the probability that a mating attempt leads to successful copulation [104], while experiments in Japanese quails, Coturnix japonica, indicate that males which are more aggressive to females have a lower fertilization probability [105]. Early models of sperm competition ignored female behaviour. Our results indicate that female behaviour is a key factor in determining the outcome of sperm competition and paternity share, even in species characterized by widespread male sexual coercion. Importantly, we show that the effect of female resistance decreased with female age, suggesting that older females are less effective at dislodging harassing males, or are less able to bias the outcome of sperm competition against their sperm (e.g. through differential sperm ejection [101,102]). Our results suggest that on the one hand, it may be easier for males to coerce older partners, which may be less efficient at resisting, while on the other, mating with older females may result in lower reproductive returns given their reduced fecundity and faster sperm loss rates [74].

Fourth, older males have reduced fertilization probability for individual eggs, confirming the overall effect of male age on male reproductive success previously reported for these trials [62]. In addition, we now show that this negative effect occurs constantly over the course of a trial, suggesting a consistent detrimental effect of ageing on male ability to compete in sperm competition. These findings are consistent with previous studies of domestic fowl showing that older males have lower sperm quality [72], and fail to transfer sperm more often, or transfer smaller ejaculates, than younger males [70,71]. Interestingly, several studies of extra-pair paternity in natural passerine populations have detected the opposite effect, i.e. older males tend to cuckold younger males and sire more extra-pair offspring [106109]. This incongruence may reflect differences between fowl and passerine birds, between the polygynandrous and socially monogamous mating systems, or between captive and natural populations. While the effect of phylogenetic and mating system differences are harder to assess, it is likely that high extrinsic mortality rates in natural populations mean that males seldom age beyond their prime, and male age in these conditions may positively covary with experience and genetic quality. Low extrinsic mortality rates in captive populations on the other hand, allow individuals to age beyond their prime, revealing a stronger impact of reproductive ageing. A study of captive house sparrows, Passer domesticus, however, shows that old males (greater than 6 years) can deliver more sperm than young males [110], suggesting that captivity effects may not be sufficient to explain differences in the role of male age in sperm competition between socially monogamous passerines and polygynandrous galliforms. While male reproductive ageing has long been recognized in several populations [111113], its role in sperm competition dynamics has only recently begun to emerge [114]. Our findings indicate that male age is a key determinant of paternity share in sperm competition in age-structured bird populations and suggest that females could minimize fitness costs associated with mating with older males by promoting sperm competition [112].

Finally, our study suggests that factors previously identified as important determinants of sperm competition outcomes may play a more negligible role in freely mating groups. A male's relatedness to a female, his social status, and the state of his sperm reserves (measured as MSR) were not significant predictors of his fertilization probability. Individuals are expected to avoid reproducing with close relatives to prevent inbreeding depression [115117]. Theory predicts that asymmetries in reproductive costs mean that, for intermediate levels of inbreeding depression, females should avoid fertilization by close relatives more than unrelated males [117]. In line with this, previous experimental studies in fowl have shown that males are as likely to mate with full-sibling sisters as with unrelated females [76], whereas females may counteract this by favouring the sperm of unrelated males [76,77], a pattern that has been also found in other taxa ([20,118124], but see [125127]). While previous studies on post-copulatory inbreeding avoidance have typically used highly controlled experiments with two rival males that differ markedly in their relatedness to the female (e.g. unrelated versus full sibling), studies of natural populations are scarcer (e.g. [123]). It is possible that in the freely interacting groups of the present study, inbreeding risk may be reduced largely at a pre-copulatory stage by mating with less related partners. Inbreeding avoidance is typically heterogeneous across wild populations [128], and the role of inbreeding in sperm competition in natural bird populations remains unclear [38]. A meta-analysis found that the incidence of extra-pair paternity increases with the genetic similarity between social partners [129], suggesting that females are more likely to seek extra-pair copulations when they are closely related to their social partners and/or that post-copulatory sexual selection promotes fertilization by less related males. Others, however, have failed to detect this effect [106]. Long-term studies of some populations, such as some fairy wrens (Maluridae) suggest that the effect of inbreeding avoidance may be non-trivial for small subsets of a population or in some breeding seasons [130]. Additionally, post-copulatory inbreeding avoidance may be captured by other mechanisms, such as female resistance, included in the models and/or may have a negligible independent effect on fertilization probability compared to other factors. Similarly, the lack of an effect of social status on male fertilization success in the present results also contrasts with previous studies suggesting that dominant male fowl are favoured in post-copulatory sexual selection [61] and that females tend to retain more sperm from dominant males [101,102]. One possibility is that the reproductive advantage associated with dominance in fowl and other species [131,132] could be driven mostly by traits, such as younger male age, lower female resistance and higher remating rates [61,62,66], which were directly included in our models, or pre-copulatory strategies such as mate-guarding [133,134]. Alternatively, the benefits of social dominance may be cancelled out if subordinate males produce ejaculates of higher fertilizing efficiency [45,46]. Future studies should disentangle potential positive and negative consequences of status for paternity share variation in polyandrous populations. While our groups were not assembled systematically with respect to male age, social competitive ability and male–female relatedness, future studies could manipulate group composition experimentally to resolve the causal role these factors have in sperm competition, while maintaining a more realistic setting. Exploring additional factors such as the role of sperm fertilizing efficiency (e.g. measures of sperm motility [45,46]) will also be important in improving our understanding of sperm competition in bird populations.

Birds have provided a model system of vertebrate sperm competition. In studying sperm competition in socially monogamous bird species, Birkhead [48] identified four predictors of the probability of extra-pair paternity: (i) the relative timing and success of insemination by competing males, (ii) the relative remating rates by competing males, (iii) the duration of female sperm storage, and (iv) patterns of sperm precedence. Our study has provided evidence that all these factors are important in sperm competition dynamics in polyandrous social flocks. We have provided corroborating evidence of passive sperm loss under more natural conditions, and identified a role for both male and female processes in determining the probability that competing males fertilize individual eggs. Crucially, our results show that the temporal dependency of these effects should be taken into consideration when studying avian sperm competition. Nevertheless, approximately 80% of the variance in probability of fertilization remained unexplained, demonstrating that much work is still needed in order to understand complex patterns of sperm competition in nature. We hope this paper provides a methodological platform for future studies. While the need for behavioural information to study sperm competition in wild birds has long been recognized [36], this has been notoriously difficult to obtain. Recent advances in tracking technologies, however, are beginning to yield behavioural data of unprecedented high-resolution for wild animals (e.g. [135]), and we foresee exciting opportunities to understand dynamics of sperm competition and their eco-evolutionary significance in natural populations.

Supplementary Material

Methodological details, supplementary tables and figures
rstb20200081supp1.docx (1.1MB, docx)

Acknowledgements

We thank Leigh Simmons and Nina Wedell for inviting this contribution, and two anonymous referees for providing valuable comments.

Ethics

Research was conducted according to United Kingdom home office legislation (Home office licenses 30/2418 and 30/2931) following approval by the Departmental Animal Welfare Ethical Review Body (AWERB).

Data accessibility

Supporting data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj1d [136].

Authors' contributions

R.C., G.C.M. and T.P. conceived the study. G.C.M. conducted the fieldwork, R.C. analysed the data. L.G.S., E.A.F. and D.S.R. performed molecular analyses for parentage assignment. Y.W. provided unpublished data for the electronic supplementary material, figure S1. R.C., T.P. and G.C.M. wrote the manuscript, and D.S.R. provided comments. All other authors gave final approval for publication.

Competing interests

We have no competing interests.

Funding

R.C. was supported by a DPhil scholarship from the Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) (grant no. 234988/2014-2), G.C.M. was supported by a PhD CASE scholarship from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and Aviagen Ltd, an industrial LINK award from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and Aviagen Ltd (grant no. BB/L009587/1) to T.P., and by the National Research, Development and Innovation Office, Hungary (grant no. NN 125642). D.S.R. was supported by a research grant from the Natural Environment Research Council (grant no. NE/H006818/1). T.P. was supported by a research grant from the Natural Environment Research Council (grant no. NE/H008047/1) and an industrial LINK award from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and Aviagen Ltd (grant no. BB/L009587/1). L.G.S. was supported by a BBSRC fellowship (grant no. BB/N011759/1).

References

  • 1.Parker GA. 1970. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in insects. Biol. Rev. 45, 525–567. ( 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1970.tb01176.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Parker GA. 1984. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating strategies. In Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating systems (ed. Smith RL.), pp. 1–60. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Parker GA. 1998. Sperm competition and the evolution of ejaculates: towards a theory base. In Sperm competition and sexual selection (eds Birkhead TR, Møller AP), pp. 3–54. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Birkhead T, Pizzari T. 2002. Postcopulatory sexual selection. Nature 3, 262–273. ( 10.1038/nrg774) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Andersson M, Simmons L. 2006. Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 296–302. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Pizzari T, Parker GA. 2009. Sperm competition and sperm phenotype. In Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective (eds Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, Pitnick S), pp. 207–245. Burlington, MA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pitnick S, Hosken D. 2010. Postcopulatory sexual selection. In Evolutionary behavioral ecology (eds Westneat D, Fox C), pp. 378–399. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Parker GA, Birkhead TR. 2013. Polyandry: the history of a revolution. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120335 ( 10.1098/rstb.2012.0335) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pizzari T, Wedell N. 2013. The polyandry revolution. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120041 ( 10.1098/rstb.2012.0041) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Taylor ML, Price TAR, Wedell N. 2014. Polyandry in nature: a global analysis. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 376–383. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2014.04.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Smith RL. 1984. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating systems. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Birkhead T, Møller A. 1998. Sperm competition and sexual selection. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Simmons LW. 2001. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Snook RR. 2005. Sperm in competition: not playing by the numbers. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 46–53. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2004.10.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Parker GA, Pizzari T. 2010. Sperm competition and ejaculate economics. Biol. Rev. 85, 897–934. ( 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00140.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Simmons LW, Fitzpatrick JL. 2012. Sperm wars and the evolution of male fertility. Reproduction 144, 519–534. ( 10.1530/REP-12-0285) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fitzpatrick JL, Lupold S. 2014. Sexual selection and the evolution of sperm quality. Mol. Hum. Reprod. 20, 1180–1189. ( 10.1093/molehr/gau067) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lüpold S, Pitnick S. 2018. Sperm form and function: what do we know about the role of sexual selection? Reproduction 155, R229–R243. ( 10.1530/REP-17-0536) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Eberhard W. 1996. Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Firman RC, Gasparini C, Manier MK, Pizzari T. 2017. Postmating female control: 20 years of cryptic female choice. Trends Ecol. Evol. 32, 368–382. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2017.02.010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Birkhead TR, Møller AP. 1993. Sexual selection and the temporal separation of reproductive events: sperm storage data from reptiles, birds and mammals. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 50, 295–311. ( 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1993.tb00933.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Orr TJ, Brennan PLR. 2015. Sperm storage: distinguishing selective processes and evaluating criteria. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 261–272. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2015.03.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Birkhead TR, Hunter FM. 1990. Mechanisms of sperm competition. Trends Ecol. Evol. 5, 48–52. ( 10.1016/0169-5347(90)90047-H) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Manier MK, Belote JM, Berben KS, Novikov D, Stuart WT, Pitnick S. 2010. Resolving mechanisms of competitive fertilization success in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 328, 354–357. ( 10.1126/science.1187096) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Manier MK, Lüpold S, Pitnick S, Starmer WT. 2013. An analytical framework for estimating fertilization bias and the fertilization set from multiple sperm-storage organs. Am. Nat. 182, 552–561. ( 10.1086/671782) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Arnqvist G, Rowe L. 2005. Sexual conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Neff BD, Svensson EI. 2013. Polyandry and alternative mating tactics. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120045 ( 10.1098/rstb.2012.0045) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McKinney F, Cheng KM, Bruggers D. 1984. Sperm competition in apparently monogamous birds. In Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating systems (ed. Smith RL.), pp. 523–545. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Birkhead T, Møller A. 1992. Sperm competition in birds. Evolutionary causes and consequences. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Birkhead TR. 1998. Sperm competition in birds. Rev. Reprod. 3, 123–129. ( 10.1530/ror.0.0030123) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Simmons LW, Siva-Jothy MT. 1998. Sperm competition in insects: mechanisms and potential for selection. In Sperm competition and sexual selection (eds Birkhead T, Moller A), pp. 341–414. London, UK: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lüpold S, Pitnick S, Berben KS, Blengini CS, Belote JM, Manier MK.. 2013. Female mediation of competitive fertilization success in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 10 693–10 698. ( 10.1073/pnas.1300954110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lüpold S, Manier MK, Puniamoorthy N, Schoff C, Starmer WT, Luepold SHB, Belote JM, Pitnick S. 2016. How sexual selection can drive the evolution of costly sperm ornamentation. Nature 533, 535–538. ( 10.1038/nature18005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gress BE, Pitnick S. 2017. Size-dependent ejaculation strategies and reproductive success in the yellow dung fly, Scathophaga stercoraria. Anim. Behav. 127, 281–287. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.03.027) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Immler S, Pitnick S, Parker GA, Durrant KL, Lüpold S, Calhim S, Birkhead TR.. 2011. Resolving variation in the reproductive tradeoff between sperm size and number. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 5325–5330. ( 10.1073/pnas.1009059108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Westneat DF, Stewart IRK. 2003. Extra-pair paternity in birds: causes, correlates, and conflict. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34, 365–396. ( 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132439) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Forstmeier W, Nakagawa S, Griffith SC, Kempenaers B. 2014. Female extra-pair mating: adaptation or genetic constraint? Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 456–464. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2014.05.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Brouwer L, Griffith S. 2019. Extra-pair paternity in birds. Mol. Ecol. 28, 4864–4882. ( 10.1111/mec.15259) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lifjeld JT, et al. 2019. Evolution of female promiscuity in Passerides songbirds. BMC Evol. Biol. 19, 169 ( 10.1186/s12862-019-1493-1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bakst M, Wishart G, Brillard J. 1994. Oviductal sperm selection, transport, and storage in poultry. Poult. Sci. Rev. 5, 117–143. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Birkhead TR, Pizzari T. 2009. Sperm competition and fertilization success. In Biology of breeding poultry (ed. Hocking P.), pp. 133–150. Wallingford, UK: CABI. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Etches RJ. 1996. Reproduction in poultry. Wallingford, UK: Oxon. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sasanami T, Matsuzaki M, Mizushima S, Hiyama G. 2013. Sperm storage in the female reproductive tract in birds. J. Reprod. Dev. 59, 334–338. ( 10.1262/jrd.2013-038) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Martin PA, Reimers TJ, Lodge J. R, Dziuk PJ. 1974. The effect of ratios and numbers of spermatozoa mixed from two males on proportions of offspring. J. Reprod. Fertil. 39, 251–258. ( 10.1530/jrf.0.0390251) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Birkhead TR, Martínez JG, Burke T, Froman DP. 1999. Sperm mobility determines the outcome of sperm competition in the domestic fowl. Proc. R. Soc. B 266, 1759–1764. ( 10.1098/rspb.1999.0843) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Froman DP, Pizzari T, Feltmann AJ, Castillo-Juarez H, Birkhead TR. 2002. Sperm mobility: mechanisms of fertilizing efficiency, genetic variation and phenotypic relationship with male status in the domestic fowl, Gallus gallus domesticus. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 607–612. ( 10.1098/rspb.2001.1925) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Pizzari T, Worley K, Burke T, Froman DP. 2008. Sperm competition dynamics: ejaculate fertilising efficiency changes differentially with time. BMC Evol. Biol. 8, 332 ( 10.1186/1471-2148-8-332) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Birkhead T. 1988. Behavioral aspects of sperm competition in birds. Adv. Study Behav. 18, 35–72. ( 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60309-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Krist M, Nádvornik P, Uvírová L, Bures˘ S. 2005. Paternity covaries with laying and hatching order in the collared flycatcher Ficedula albicollis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 6–11. ( 10.1007/s00265-005-0002-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Magrath MJL, Vedder O, van der Velde M, Komdeur J.. 2009. Maternal effects contribute to the superior performance of extra-pair offspring. Curr. Biol. 19, 792–797. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.068) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vedder O, Magrath MJL, Harts AMF, Schut E, van der Velde M, Komdeur J.. 2010. Reduced extrapair paternity in response to experimental stimulation of earlier incubation onset in blue tits. Behav. Ecol. 21, 9–15. ( 10.1093/beheco/arp145) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ferree ED, Dickinson J, Rendell W, Stern C, Porter S. 2010. Hatching order explains an extrapair chick advantage in western bluebirds. Behav. Ecol. 21, 802–807. ( 10.1093/beheco/arq056) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Pryke SR, Rollins LA, Griffith SC. 2010. Females use multiple mating and genetically loaded sperm competition to target compatible genes. Science 329, 964–967. ( 10.1126/science.1192407) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Johnsen A, Carter KL, Delhey K, Lifjeld JT, Robertson RJ, Kempenaers B. 2012. Laying-order effects on sperm numbers and on paternity: comparing three passerine birds with different life histories. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 181–190. ( 10.1007/s00265-011-1265-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Vedder O, Magrath MJL, Niehoff DL, van der Velde M, Komdeur J.. 2012. Declining extra-pair paternity with laying order associated with initial incubation behavior, but independent of final clutch size in the blue tit. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 603–612. ( 10.1007/s00265-011-1308-x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hurley LL, Fanson KV, Griffith SC. 2017. Variation in the number of sperm trapped on the perivitelline layer of the egg in three species of estrildid finch. Auk 134, 832–841. ( 10.1642/AUK-17-41.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Dunn PO, Afton AD, Gloutney ML, Alisauskas RT. 1999. Forced copulation results in few extrapair fertilizations in Ross‘s and lesser snow geese. Anim. Behav. 57, 1071–1081. ( 10.1006/anbe.1998.1066) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Stewart SLM, Westneat DF, Ritchison G. 2010. Extra-pair paternity in eastern bluebirds: effects of manipulated density and natural patterns of breeding synchrony. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 463–473. ( 10.1007/s00265-009-0862-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Canal D, Jovani R, Potti J. 2012. Male decisions or female accessibility? Spatiotemporal patterns of extra pair paternity in a songbird. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1146–1153. ( 10.1093/beheco/ars090) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Pizzari T, Mcdonald GC. 2019. Sexual selection in socially-structured, polyandrous populations: some insights from the fowl. Adv. Stud. Behav. 51, 77–141. ( 10.1016/bs.asb.2019.02.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Collet J, Richardson DS, Worley K, Pizzari T. 2012. Sexual selection and the differential effect of polyandry. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 8641–8645. ( 10.1073/pnas.1200219109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.McDonald GC, Spurgin LG, Fairfield EA, Richardson DS, Pizzari T. 2017. Pre- and postcopulatory sexual selection favor aggressive, young males in polyandrous groups of red junglefowl. Evolution 71, 1653–1669. ( 10.1111/evo.13242) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Crowe SA, Kleven O, Delmore KE, Laskemoen T, Nocera JJ, Lifjeld JT, Robertson RJ. 2009. Paternity assurance through frequent copulations in a wild passerine with intense sperm competition. Anim. Behav. 77, 183–187. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.024) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Smith RL. 1979. Repeated copulation and sperm precedence: paternity assurance for a male brooding water bug. Science 205, 1029–1031. ( 10.1126/science.205.4410.1029) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pischedda A, Rice WR. 2012. Partitioning sexual selection into its mating success and fertilization success components. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 2049–2053. ( 10.1073/pnas.1110841109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Morimoto J, Mcdonald GC, Smith E, Smith DT, Perry JC, Chapman T, Pizzari T, Wigby S. 2019. Sex peptide receptor-regulated polyandry modulates the balance of pre- and post-copulatory sexual selection in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 10, 283 ( 10.1038/s41467-018-08113-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Cornwallis CK, Birkhead TR. 2007. Changes in sperm quality and numbers in response to experimental manipulation of male social status. Am. Nat. 170, 758–770. ( 10.1086/521955) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Locatello L, Poli F, Rasotto MB. 2013. Tactic-specific differences in seminal fluid influence sperm performance. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20122891 ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.2891) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bartlett MJ, Steeves TE, Gemmell NJ, Rosengrave PC. 2017. Sperm competition risk drives rapid ejaculate adjustments mediated by seminal fluid. Elife 6, e28811 ( 10.7554/eLife.28811) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Dean R, Cornwallis CK, Løvlie H, Worley K, Richardson DS, Pizzari T. 2010. Male reproductive senescence causes potential for sexual conflict over mating. Curr. Biol. 20, 1192–1196. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.059) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Cornwallis CK, Dean R, Pizzari T. 2014. Sex-specific patterns of aging in sexual ornaments and gametes. Am. Nat. 184, E66–E78. ( 10.1086/677385) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Noguera JC, Dean R, Isaksson C, Velando A, Pizzari T. 2012. Age-specific oxidative status and the expression of pre- and postcopulatory sexually selected traits in male red junglefowl, Gallus gallus. Ecol. Evol. 2, 2155–2167. ( 10.1002/ece3.300) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Mcdonald GC, Spurgin LG, Fairfield EA, Richardson DS, Pizzari T. 2019. Differential female sociality is linked with the fine-scale structure of sexual interactions in replicate groups of red junglefowl, Gallus gallus. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20191734 ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.1734) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Brillard JP. 2003. Practical aspects of fertility in poultry. World‘s Poult. Sci. J. 59, 441–446. ( 10.1079/WPS20030027) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Carleial R, McDonald GC, Pizzari T. 2020. Dynamic phenotypic correlates of social status and mating effort in male and female red junglefowl, Gallus gallus. J. Evol. Biol. 33, 22–40. ( 10.1111/jeb.13541) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Pizzari T, Løvlie H, Cornwallis CK. 2004. Sex-specific, counteracting responses to inbreeding in a bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 271, 2115–2121. ( 10.1098/rspb.2004.2843) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Løvlie H, Gillingham MAF, Worley K, Pizzari T, Richardson DS. 2013. Cryptic female choice favours sperm from major histocompatibility complex-dissimilar males. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20131296 ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.1296) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Løvlie H, Cornwallis CK, Pizzari T. 2005. Male mounting alone reduces female promiscuity in the fowl. Curr. Biol. 15, 1222–1227. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.060) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Neumann C, Duboscq J, Dubuc C, Ginting A, Irwan AM, Agil M, Widdig A, Engelhardt A. 2011. Assessing dominance hierarchies: validation and advantages of progressive evaluation with Elo-rating. Anim. Behav. 82, 911–921. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Wang J. 2017. Estimating pairwise relatedness in a small sample of individuals. Heredity 119, 302–313. ( 10.1038/hdy.2017.52) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Pew J, Muir PH, Wang J, Frasier TR. 2015. related: an R package for analysing pairwise relatedness from codominant molecular markers. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 15, 557–561. ( 10.1111/1755-0998.12323) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.R Core Team. 2019. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; See http://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 64, 1–48. ( 10.18637/jss.v067.i01) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Fox J, Weisberg S. 2019. An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Elphick CS. 2010. A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1, 3–14. ( 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2009.00001.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Akaike H. 1970. A fundamental relation between predictor identification and power spectrum estimation. Ann. Inst. Stat. Math. 22, 219–223. ( 10.1007/BF02506338) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Burnham K.P., Anderson D.R, 2002. Model selection and multimodel inference. A practical information-theoretic approach. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Mazerolle MJ. 2019. AICcmodavg: model selection and multimodel inference based on (Q)AIC(c) See https://cran.r-project.org/package=AICcmodavg.
  • 89.Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. 2013. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 133–142. ( 10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Nakagawa S, Johnson PCD, Schielzeth H. 2017. The coefficient of determination R2 and intra-class correlation coefficient from generalized linear mixed-effects models revisited and expanded. J. R. Soc. Interface 14, 20170213 ( 10.1098/rsif.2017.0213) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Wishart GJ. 1987. Regulation of the length of the fertile period in the domestic fowl by numbers of oviducal spermatozoa, as reflected by those trapped in laid eggs. J. Reprod. Fertil. 80, 493–498. ( 10.1530/jrf.0.0800493) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Lessells CM, Birkhead TR. 1990. Mechanisms of sperm competition in birds: mathematical models. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 27, 325–337. ( 10.1007/BF00164003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Birkhead ATR, Atkin L, Møller AP. 1987. Copulation behaviour of birds. Behaviour 101, 101–138. ( 10.1163/156853987X00396) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Engqvist L, Reinhold K. 2006. Theoretical influence of female mating status and remating propensity on male sperm allocation patterns. J. Evol. Biol. 19, 1448–1458. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01134.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Birkhead TR, Biggins JD. 1998. Sperm competition mechanisms in birds: models and data. Behav. Ecol. 9, 253–260. ( 10.1093/beheco/9.3.253) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Van Krey HP, Balander RJ, Compton MM.. 1981. Storage and evacuation of spermatozoa from the uterovaginal sperm–host glands in the domestic fowl. Poult. Sci. 60, 871–877. ( 10.3382/ps.0600871) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.King LM, Brillard JP, Garrett WM, Bakst MR, Donoghue AM. 2002. Segregation of spermatozoa within sperm storage tubules of fowl and turkey hens. Reproduction 123, 79–86. ( 10.1530/rep.0.1230079) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Briskie JV. 1996. Spatiotemporal patterns of sperm storage and last-male sperm precedence in birds. Funct. Ecol. 10, 375–383. ( 10.2307/2390286) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Hemmings N, Birkhead TR. 2017. Differential sperm storage by female zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20171032 ( 10.1098/rspb.2017.1032) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Pizzari T. 2001. Indirect partner choice through manipulation of male behaviour by female fowl, Gallus gallus domesticus. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 181–186. ( 10.1098/rspb.2000.1348) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Dean R, Nakagawa S, Pizzari T. 2011. The risk and intensity of sperm ejection in female birds. Am. Nat. 178, 343–354. ( 10.1086/661244) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Pizzari T, Birkhead TR. 2000. Female feral fowl eject sperm of subdominant males. Nature 405, 787–789. ( 10.1038/35015558) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hoi H. 1997. Assessment of the quality of copulation partners in the monogamous bearded tit. Anim. Behav. 53, 277–286. ( 10.1006/anbe.1996.0401) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Cunningham EJA. 2003. Female mate preferences and subsequent resistance to copulation in the mallard. Behav. Ecol. 14, 326–333. ( 10.1093/beheco/14.3.326) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Persaud KN, Galef BG. 2005. Eggs of a female Japanese quail are more likely to be fertilized by a male that she prefers. J. Comp. Psychol. 119, 251–256. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.119.3.251) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Akçay E, Roughgarden J. 2007. Extra-pair paternity in birds: review of the genetic benefits. Evol. Ecol. Res. 9, 855–868. [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Cleasby IR, Nakagawa S. 2012. The influence of male age on within- pair and extra-pair paternity in passerines. Ibis 154, 318–324. ( 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2011.01209.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Hsu YH, Schroeder J, Winney I, Burke T, Nakagawa S. 2015. Are extra-pair males different from cuckolded males? A case study and a meta-analytic examination. Mol. Ecol. 24, 1558–1571. ( 10.1111/mec.13124) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Girndt A, Chng CWT, Burke T, Schroeder J. 2018. Male age is associated with extra-pair paternity, but not with extra-pair mating behaviour. Sci. Rep. 8, 8378 ( 10.1038/s41598-018-26649-1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Girndt A, Cockburn G, Sánchez-Tójar A, Hertel M, Burke T, Schroeder J. 2019. Male age and its association with reproductive traits in captive and wild house sparrows. J. Evol. Biol. 32, 1432–1443. ( 10.1111/jeb.13542) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Pizzari T, Dean R, Pacey A, Moore H, Bonsall MB. 2008. The evolutionary ecology of pre- and post-meiotic sperm senescence. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 131–140. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2007.12.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Radwan J. 2003. Male age, germline mutations and the benefits of polyandry. Ecol. Lett. 6, 581–586. ( 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00484.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Reinhardt K, Dobler R, Abbott J. 2015. An ecology of sperm: sperm diversification by natural selection. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 46, 435–459. ( 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-120213-091611) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Ruhmann H, Koppik M, Wolfner MF, Fricke C. 2018. The impact of ageing on male reproductive success in Drosophila melanogaster. Exp. Gerontol. 103, 1–10. ( 10.1016/j.exger.2017.12.013) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Charlesworth D, Charlesworth B. 1987. Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 18, 237–268. ( 10.1146/annurev.es.18.110187.001321) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Charlesworth D, Willis JH. 2009. The genetics of inbreeding depression. Nat. Rev. Genet. 10, 783–793. ( 10.1038/nrg2664) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Parker GA. 2006. Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 235–259. ( 10.1098/rstb.2005.1785) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Bretman A, Wedell N, Tregenza T. 2004. Molecular evidence of post-copulatory inbreeding avoidance in the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Proc. R. Soc. B 271, 159–164. ( 10.1098/rspb.2003.2563) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Garner TWJ, Schmidt BR.. 2003. Relatedness, body size and paternity in the alpine newt, Triturus alpestris. Proc. R. Soc. B 270, 619–624. ( 10.1098/rspb.2002.2284) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Brekke P, Wang J, Bennett PM, Cassey P, Dawson DA, Horsburgh GJ, Ewen JG. 2011. Postcopulatory mechanisms of inbreeding avoidance in the island endemic hihi (Notiomystis cincta). Behav. Ecol. 23, 278–284. ( 10.1093/beheco/arr183) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Firman RC, Simmons LW. 2015. Gametic interactions promote inbreeding avoidance in house mice. Ecol. Lett. 18, 937–943. ( 10.1111/ele.12471) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Fitzpatrick JL, Evans JP. 2014. Postcopulatory inbreeding avoidance in guppies. J. Evol. Biol. 27 2585–2594. ( 10.1111/jeb.12545) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Olsson M, Madsen T, Shine R, Gullberg A, Tegelström H. 1996. Sperm selection by females. Nature 383, 585 ( 10.1038/383585a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Foerster K, Valcu M, Johnsen A, Kempenaers B. 2006. A spatial genetic structure and effects of relatedness on mate choice in a wild bird population. Mol. Ecol. 15, 4555–4567. ( 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2006.03091.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Jennions MD, Hunt J, Graham R, Brooks R. 2004. No evidence for inbreeding avoidance through postcopulatory mechanisms in the black field cricket, Teleogryllus commodus. Evolution 58, 2472–2477. ( 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb00876.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Denk AG, Alois H, Peters A, Vermeirssen ELM, Kempenaers B. 2005. Paternity in mallards: effects of sperm quality and female sperm selection for inbreeding avoidance. Behav. Ecol. 16, 825–833. ( 10.1093/beheco/ari065) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Vuarin P, Bouchard A, Lesobre L, Levêque G, Chalah T, Saint Jalme M, Lacroix F, Hingrat Y, Sorci G. 2018. No evidence for prezygotic postcopulatory avoidance of kin despite high inbreeding depression. Mol. Ecol. 27, 5252–5262. ( 10.1111/mec.14951) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Szulkin M, Stopher KV, Pemberton JM, Reid JM. 2013. Inbreeding avoidance, tolerance, or preference in animals? Trends Ecol. Evol. 28, 205–211. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Arct A, Drobniak SM, Cichón M. 2015. Invited review: Genetic similarity between mates predicts extrapair paternity—a meta-analysis of bird studies. Behav. Ecol. 26, 959–968. ( 10.1093/beheco/arv004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Brouwer L, et al. 2017. Multiple hypotheses explain variation in extra-pair paternity at different levels in a single bird family. Mol. Ecol. 26, 6717–6729. ( 10.1111/mec.14385) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Ellis L. 1995. Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: a cross-species comparison. Ethol. Sociobiol. 16, 257–333. ( 10.1016/0162-3095(95)00050-U) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Wong BBM, Candolin U. 2005. How is female mate choice affected by male competition? Biol. Rev. 80, 559–571. ( 10.1017/S1464793105006809) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Cheng KM, Burns JT. 1988. Dominance relationship and mating behavior of domestic cocks: a model to study mate-guarding and sperm competition in birds. Condor 90, 697–704. ( 10.2307/1368360) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Setchell JM, Charpentier M, Wickings JE. 2005. Mate guarding and paternity in mandrills: factors influencing alpha male monopoly. Anim. Behav. 70, 1105–1120. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Ripperger SP, Carter GG, Page RA, Duda N, Koelpin A, Weigel R et al. 2020. Thinking small: next-generation sensor networks close the size gap in vertebrate biologging. PLoS Biol. 18, e3000655 ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000655) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Carleial R, McDonald GC, Spurgin LG, Fairfield EA, Wang Y, Richardson DS, Pizzari T. 2020. Data from: Temporal dynamics of competitive fertilization in social groups of red junglefowl (Gallus gallus) shed new light on avian sperm competition. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj1d) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Citations

  1. Carleial R, McDonald GC, Spurgin LG, Fairfield EA, Wang Y, Richardson DS, Pizzari T. 2020. Data from: Temporal dynamics of competitive fertilization in social groups of red junglefowl (Gallus gallus) shed new light on avian sperm competition. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj1d) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Supplementary Materials

Methodological details, supplementary tables and figures
rstb20200081supp1.docx (1.1MB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

Supporting data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj1d [136].


Articles from Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES