Abstract
Public compliance with social distancing is key to containing COVID-19, yet there is a lack of knowledge on which communication ‘messages’ drive compliance. Respondents (224 Canadians and Americans) rated combinations of messages about compliance, systematically varied by an experimental design. Independent variables were perceived risk; the agent communicating the policy; specific social distancing practices; and methods to enforce compliance. Response patterns to each message suggest three mindset segments in each country reflecting how a person thinks. Two mindsets, the same in Canada and the US, were ‘tell me exactly what to do,’ and ‘pandemic onlookers.’ The third was ‘bow to authority’ in Canada, and ‘tell me how’ in the US. Each mindset showed different messages strongly driving compliance. To effectively use messaging about compliance, policy makers may assign any person or group in the population to the appropriate mindset segment by using a Personal Viewpoint Identifier that we developed.
Keywords: communication, messages, compliance, COVID-19, health policy, social distancing
1. Introduction
Scientists and policy makers have been concerned about the emergence of an influenza pandemic for which there is neither a strain-specific vaccine nor sufficient antiviral medications [1]. Social distancing is effective in reducing infection rates within communities [2,3,4,5]. Measures of social distancing undertaken in Italy, Spain, India, and most of the United States are drastic [6]. Local governments across the world impose four practices of social distancing including isolation of people with probable influenza; voluntary home quarantine for people with confirmed or probable influenza; preventing the congregation of people in community or employment settings; and social distancing of adults [4,7,8].
The implementation of social distancing requires significant public cooperation [4]. Previously, compliance was high in the early stages of the pandemic but decreased significantly over the course of several months [9]. Further, the compliance of people belonging to racial and ethnic minorities and to low socio-economic groups has been low [10,11]. Practical, logistical and ethical limitations of social distancing have also affected compliance [5]. Compliance with the policy of social distancing may maintain the resilience of communities. Designing communication interventions may reduce the burden of the psychological and social consequences of social distancing and reduce trauma that may transcend generations, constraining coping capacities [12]. In previous pandemics, people with low tolerability to policies fled from infected cities, participated in riots, attacked government officials and harmed those suspected of plaguing [1]. Compliance with social distancing may also be influenced by one’s level of perceived risk, one’s perception of individual and family disruption, and the perceived effectiveness of agencies dealing with the crisis [13,14,15]. The resilience of communities may depend on the extent of compliance with social distancing maintained in those communities. The maintenance of compliance will depend on appropriate communication [16]. Thus, communication-/messaging regarding social distancing, must consider reactions to the policy, and avoid the creation of resistance, which can endanger compliance [17]. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the specific communication messages that have the potential to act as drivers of compliance to social distancing.
Most studies on public behaviors in a pandemic tested the extent of communication that the public needs for compliance [16]. Research on the impact of specific communication messages on compliance is scant [17]. Previous studies called to close the gap and examine how to persuade the public to comply with social distancing [15,18,19]. Studies also suggested using controlled research designs rather than observational studies [20]. This study responds to previous calls seeking to discover communication messages that are likely to resonate positively with the individual, thus enhancing compliance. Higher compliance with social distancing will allow hospitals to manage their overflow, until a vaccination is available [1]. Research also acknowledged that a strategy using various communication messages to optimize compliance with social distancing may be more effective than using one message for all [21]. Furthermore, during pandemics, the communication resources of governments become scarce. Effective messaging enables policy makers to allocate resources based on real, immediate, and relevant data.
Effective health communication encompasses narrative messaging [22,23]. Narrative messages may be effective in the context of pandemic behavior as well, because they are the basic mode of interaction that people use to influence others [24,25,26,27]. The impact of messages may be explored by asking questions about whom, under what circumstances, how, and when does each message achieve an optimum effect [28]. The power of specific messages may vary across individuals, possibly as a consequence of their perceived risk. Furthermore, the effect of narrative messaging on compliance to social distancing may depend, in part, on the extent to which people “identify” with the message [28]. People may, therefore, be defined by the similarity in their pattern of responses to narrative persuasion messages (i.e., mindset segments) regarding social distancing. Therefore, we hypothesized that various messages will be strong drivers of compliance, while others will be weak drivers. We further hypothesize that the power of the messages as drivers of social distancing will differ based on the similarity in patterns of response to the messages.
2. Materials and Methods
Ethics: This study protocol was approved by the Ryerson University Research Ethics Board (#2020-149).
2.1. Study Sample
Respondents were comprised of 106 Canadians and 118 Americans—100 males and 124 females, who were 18–77 years of age (Table 1). This sample size is consistent with the suggested sample size in conjoint analysis studies, particularly when aiming at stability of coefficients rather than stability of means [29]. The recruiting service (Luc.id, Inc., New Orleans, LA, USA) invited the respondents, incentivized them, and rerouted the respondent to a Mind Genomics® online study on compliance to social distancing during a pandemic.
Table 1.
Variable | Canada | United States of America |
---|---|---|
Gender (Male/Female) | 52/54 | 48/70 |
Age (Mean ± SD) | 45.6 ± 15.1 | 48.3 ± 15.6 |
2.2. Procedure and Instrument
The dependent variable is social distancing rated on a 1–5 anchored scale (1 = disagree, 5 = agree). The independent variables were four categories of messages representing known drivers of compliance [13,14,15]. Narrative messages tested in this study are drivers of the intention to comply, which were found to strongly relate to the behavior of compliance [30]. The independent variables result in sixteen messages that were randomly mixed into 24 combinations. The approach varies the combinations by a permutation scheme [31]. Each respondent rated a unique set of 24 combinations of messages comprised of 2–4 variables. Respondents were instructed to rate each combination of messages as a whole [32,33,34,35]. Messages may be incoherent, pulling in different directions [35,36,37]. Underlying the combinations is a well-crafted experimental design [35,36]. The uniqueness of combinations is ensured by the permutation scheme, which keeps the structure of the 24 combinations the same from one respondent to another but varies the specific combinations [38]. The Mind Genomics® approach parallels complexity in the real world, where people are presented with compound information from different sources and must make rapid decisions. The response to these combinations, uncovered by ordinary least-squares regression (OLS), reveals the importance that each respondent attributes to each message, with the judgment bias reduced [38,39]. Table 2 presents the study instrument.
Table 2.
Code | Elements |
---|---|
Independent Variable A: The Perceived Risk of COVID-19 | |
A1 | A dangerous virus spreading wildly |
A2 | Media have overblown new strain of influenza… people panicking |
A3 | All news seems to be about the COVID-19 virus |
A4 | Health experts suggest… but government is reactive rather than proactive to pandemic |
Independent Variable B: Practices of Social Distancing | |
B1 | Socialize… work only from home through internet, e.g., Zoom/Skype |
B2 | Everyone stays 6 feet (2 m) apart |
B3 | Confinement to within 300 feet (100 m) from home |
B4 | Wear your mask everywhere |
Independent Variable C: Ways to Ensure Compliance to Social Distancing | |
C1 | Military lockdown |
C2 | Food shopping (3 people at a time) … pharmacy (1 person at a time) … gas (attendant dispenses) |
C3 | Designated young volunteer for priority shopping… for elderly and disabled |
C4 | Only age 60+ allowed to buy groceries during first 2 h of store day |
Independent Variable D: The Agent Communicating the Policy | |
D1 | State or provincial government should communicate the policy |
D2 | Federal government should communicate the policy |
D3 | Religious clergy should communicate the policy |
D4 | The media should communicate the policy |
2.3. Statistical Analysis
We used conjoint analysis, a statistical procedure, to determine the messages that drive social distancing. The experimental design enabled the deconstruction of responses to the messages by OLS [34,35]. We created models for willingness to comply using OLS, one for each respondent, each with an additive constant and 16 coefficients (i.e., one coefficient for each message). The additive constant is an estimated parameter representing the intercept in a linear equation that may be interpreted as the predisposition of the respondent group to agree to a set of messages in the absence of any specific message.
To highlight the best-performing messages and eliminate a high variability due to lack of calibration among respondents, we transformed the ratings to a binary scale. Ratings 4 and 5 (upper 40% of the scale) were transformed to 100, classified as positive outcomes (‘Top2′); and ratings below 4 (lower 60% of the scale) were transformed to 0, classified as negative outcomes. After the transformation, a small random number between 0.1 and 0.01 was added to make the variable continuous. OLS analysis was performed to create an individual-level regression model for each respondent. This type of individual regression approach has been widely used in conjoint analysis studies [40]. The OLS model was written as follows:
(1) |
where is the predicted or expected value of the dependent variable (here, the transformed, binarized ratings), through are distinct independent or predictor variables, is the value of when all of the independent variables ( through ) are equal to zero, and through are the estimated regression coefficients.
The OLS coefficient is the conditional probability that the specific message adds to the perceived importance of the additive constant for social distancing. OLS was run for the entire panel, incorporating all relevant data into one regression model for the sample. The regression model, estimated at the level of each respondent, is appropriate because of the permuted design and allowed clustering based upon the 16 coefficients. k-means clustering was applied on the 16 coefficients to create clusters. These clusters represent mindsets because they suggest what is important to the respondent for the specific topic. Mindsets emerge from the pattern responses to the specific, relevant messages, not from stated attitudes. Following mathematical clustering, the equation for each key subgroup was estimated using all data from the appropriate group. Analysis of variance and post hoc tests indicate whether the distinct mindset models were significant, highlighting the different messages that drive social distancing for each mindset. The pattern of positive high coefficients across different mindsets guided the assignment of respondents to mindset.
3. Results
3.1. Interpretation
To simplify the analysis, we present only messages with positive coefficients, driving agreement with compliance. Negative coefficients mean either that the element is neutral (irrelevant for compliance) or counterproductive, driving non-compliance. Table 3 shows messages that drive compliance among Canadians. Table 4 shows messages that drive compliance among Americans. Strong drivers of compliance with social distancing for the total sample were “To practice social distancing, everyone should stay six feet apart from others” and “The media or central government should communicate the social distancing policy”. Respondents perceived the government as reactive rather than proactive, failing to understand the significance of COVID-19.
Table 3.
MS1: Tell Me Exactly What to Do | MS2: Pandemic Onlookers | MS3: Bow to Authority | |
---|---|---|---|
Sample Size | 28 | 39 | 39 |
Additive constant (agree, without any messages) | 60 | 41 | 41 |
Independent A—The perceived risk of COVID-19 | |||
A1: Dangerous virus spreading wildly | 17 * | ||
A2: Media have overblown new strain of influenza… people panicking | 14 * | ||
A3: All news seems to be about the COVID-19 virus | 12 * | ||
A4: Health experts suggest…government is reactive rather than proactive to pandemic | 11 * | ||
Independent B—Practices of social distancing | |||
B1: Socialize… work only from home on internet, e.g., Zoom/Skype | 3 | ||
B2: Everyone stays 6 feet (2 m) apart | 2 | 1 | |
B3: Confined to within 300 feet (100 m) from home | 1 | ||
B4: Wear your mask everywhere | 6 | ||
Independent C—Ways to ensure compliance to social distancing | |||
C1: Military lockdown | 15 * | 0 | |
C2: Food shopping (3 people at a time) … pharmacy (1 person at a time) … gas (attendant dispenses) | 17 * | ||
C3: Designated young volunteer for priority shopping… for elderly and disabled | 21 * | 2 | |
C4: Only age 60+ allowed to buy groceries during first 2 h of store day | 6 | ||
Independent D—The agent communicating the policy | |||
D1: Provincial government should communicate the policy | 14 | ||
D2: Federal government should communicate the policy | 13 | ||
D3: Religious clergy should communicate the policy | 19 * | ||
D4: The media should communicate the policy | 13 |
Regression coefficients for models relating the presence/absence of the elements to the rating of disagree/agree, after binary transformation. * denotes significant, positive model parameters (p < 0.05).
Table 4.
MS1: Tell Me Exactly What to Do | MS2: Pandemic Onlookers | MS3: Tell Me How | |
---|---|---|---|
Sample Size | 29 | 43 | 46 |
Additive constant (agree, without any messages) | 43 | 52 | 40 |
Independent A—The perceived risk of COVID-19 | |||
A1: Dangerous virus spreading wildly | 4 | 7 | |
A2: Media have overblown new strain of influenza… people panicking | 13 * | 2 | |
A3: All news seems to be about the COVID-19 virus | 13 * | ||
A4: Health experts suggest…government is reactive rather than proactive to pandemic | 0 | 6 | |
Independent B—Practices of social distancing | |||
B1: Socialize… work only from home on internet, e.g., Zoom/Skype | 10 * | ||
B2: Everyone stays 6 feet (2 m) apart | 9 * | ||
B3: Confined to within 300 feet (100 m) from home | 14 * | ||
B4: Wear your mask everywhere | 14 * | ||
Independent C—Ways to ensure compliance to social distancing | |||
C1: Military lockdown | 15 * | ||
C2: Food shopping (3 people at a time) … pharmacy (1 person at a time) … gas (attendant dispenses) | 6 | 1 | |
C3: Designated young volunteer for priority shopping… for elderly and disabled | 14 * | 2 | |
C4: Only age 60+ allowed to buy groceries during first 2 h of store day | 9 * | 2 | |
Independent D—The agent communicating the policy | |||
D1: State government should communicate the policy | 2 | ||
D2: Federal government should communicate the policy | 3 | ||
D3: Religious clergy should communicate the policy | 3 | 1 | |
D4: The media should communicate the policy | 1 | 5 |
Regression coefficients for models relating the presence/absence of the elements to the rating of disagree/agree, after binary transformation. * denotes significant, positive model parameters (p < 0.05).
The additive constant shows the estimated, baseline likelihood that a respondent ‘agrees’ with the message. The data show that the readiness to comply is higher among Canadians than Americans, as shown in Table 3 and Table 4.
3.2. Mindset Segments
Three mindsets emerged from mathematical K-clustering based upon similarity in the patterns of responses to the individual messages [41]. The mindsets transcend the typical patterns used to divide respondents, namely WHO they are, summarizing how people THINK, or the nature of what people feel about social distancing.
Mindsets 1 (MS1) and 2 (MS2) are similar in both countries. People belonging to MS1, “Tell me exactly what to do”, may be driven to higher compliance by messages regarding ways to ensure compliance. Strong messages were “To ensure compliance, people should be under military lockdown”, “Compliance may be ensured by restricting shopping for food (3 at a time) and pharmacy (1 at a time)”, and “Designated young volunteers should shop for the elderly and disabled”. People belonging to MS2, “Pandemic onlookers”, may be driven to higher compliance by messages relating to the perceived risk of the virus. Strong messages were “A dangerous virus is spreading wildly”, “All the news seems to be about the COVID-19 virus”, and “Health experts suggest…government is reactive rather than proactive to the pandemic”. People belonging to this mindset may agree with any social distancing practice.
Mindset 3 (MS3) differs dramatically between countries. Canadian respondents appear to respond to authority, whereas American respondents appear not to respond to authority. Canadians belonging to MS3, “Bow to authority”, are driven to higher compliance by the communicator of the policy and believe that either provincial government, federal government, the media or religious clergy should communicate the social distancing policies. Americans belonging to MS3, “Tell me how”, may be driven to higher compliance by messages that focus on the practices of social distancing. Strong messages were “Everyone stays 6 feet apart”, “Socialize and work only from home on the internet”, “Confined to within 300 feet from home”, and “Wear your mask everywhere”.
3.3. Personal Viewpoint Identifier
For effective knowledge translation, the emergence of these findings requires an innovative way to assign a new person or group in the population to these previously discovered mindsets. To translate the knowledge derived in this study for implementation, we developed a prediction tool called a Personal Viewpoint Identifier (PVI). The PVI tool is a method by which health authorities may assign a person or group in the population to a mindset. This is based on the summary data, converting six of the strong distinguishing messages to questions answered by a “agree or disagree”. The six messages were chosen using a Monte-Carlo simulation where random variability is added to the data, and the most discriminating messages emerge in the face of random variability [42]. Based on the answers to the questions in the PVI, the individual or group is assigned to one of the three mindsets, and thus, the proper messages are established for that individual (Table 5).
Table 5.
DRIVERS OF SOCIAL DISTANCING PVI | |
---|---|
Question 1: How do you feel about COVID-19? |
|
| |
| |
| |
Question 2: What do you fear most right now about COVID-19? |
|
| |
| |
| |
Question 3: Who do you trust most? |
|
| |
| |
| |
Question 4: How long do you think COVID-19 will last? |
|
| |
| |
| |
Question 5: Situation: Media have overblown new strain of influenza… people panicking |
|
| |
Question 6: Situation: Dangerous virus spreading wildly |
|
| |
Question 7: Compliance Policy: Food shopping (3 people at a time) … pharmacy (1 person at a time) … gas (attendant dispenses) |
|
| |
Question 8: Who Communicates: Religious clergy |
|
| |
Question 9: Who Communicates: Provincial government |
|
| |
Question 10: Compliance Policy: Designated young volunteer for priority shopping… for elderly and disabled |
|
|
4. Discussion
This study examined the influence of numerous communication messages on compliance with social distancing in North America. Communication messages represent relevant, topic-specific ideas. Ordinarily, one would tailor the messaging to who a person IS demographically. This study suggests tailoring messaging by the way the person THINKS about compliance to a health policy in a pandemic, particularly because messaging shaping public behavior during a pandemic is an undertested phenomenon. This study revealed messages that drive positive responses to social distancing, varying by individuals or groups that think alike. The first hypothesis was corroborated as some messages were strong drivers of compliance and some were weak drivers of compliance. Results echo previous research claiming that the extent to which messages drive social distancing may depend, in part, on the extent to which people “identify” with the different narrative messages [28].
The second hypothesis was also corroborated as three mindsets emerged from the similarities in patterns of responses to narrative communication messaging regarding social distancing. Even during the midst of a pandemic, when social distancing is critical and novel, approaching each of the three different mindset segments with appropriate communication messages should enhance compliance with social distancing. Health policy makers and government leaders should take into consideration the proclivities and sensitivities of these three mindsets when suggesting how to properly implement the policy of social distancing over time.
Findings of this study should be interpreted in light of its limitations. Our study variables reflect our choice of constructs, which was based on prior literature, but does not encompass additional independent variables that are not yet acknowledged as potential drivers of compliance with social distancing. Further, given the continuing pandemic, respondents may have been previously exposed to messages on social distancing in real life before participating in this study. The exposure to messages, the frequency of the exposure and the potential prime effect of messages may have influenced the extent of agreement with messages regarding social distancing. These external cofounders, however, cannot be controlled in a cross-sectional design.
Directions for Future Studies
Future studies may track the relationship between using communication messages by mindset segment membership and compliance with social distancing as measured by cellular data. Longitudinal future studies may also test the prime effect of communication messages and the effect of exposure to messages and their frequency on the level of compliance.
5. Conclusions
The use of the Personal Viewpoint Identifier for enhancing public compliance with social distancing may be a non-intrusive way to understand the individual. This allows us to communicate a message with greater specificity and have a better chance of driving compliance with physical/social distancing, highlighting the potential collaboration between academia and health officials to contain COVID-19 [43].
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, N.B., G.G., A.G., M.K., and H.M.; data curation, N.B. and H.M.; formal analysis, N.B., G.G., A.G., and H.M.; investigation, N.B., G.G., A.G., M.K., and H.M.; methodology, N.B., G.G., A.G., M.K., and H.M.; project administration, N.B.; resources, N.B.; software, N.B.; supervision, N.B.; validation, N.B.; visualization, N.B. and H.M.; writing—original draft, N.B., G.G., A.G., and H.M.; writing—review and editing, N.B., G.G., A.G., and M.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This study was funded by a Ryerson University, Faculty of Community Services COVID-19 Rapid Response Research Grant. A.G. thanks the support of the Premium Postdoctoral Researcher Program of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences.
Conflicts of Interest
N.B. and H.M. own shares in MindCart AI Inc. The remaining authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Aledort J.E., Lurie N., Wasserman J., Bozzette S.A. Non-pharmaceutical public health interventions for pandemic influenza: An evaluation of the evidence base. BMC Public Health. 2007;7:208. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Abouk R., Heydari B. The Immediate Effect of COVID-19 Policies on Social Distancing Behavior in the United States. SSRN Electron. J. 2020 doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3571421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chu D.K., Akl E.A., Duda S., Solo K., Yaacoub S., Schünemann H.J., Chu D.K., Akl E.A., El-harakeh A., Bognanni A., et al. Physical distancing, face masks, and eye protection to prevent person-to-person transmission of SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet. 2020;395:1973–1987. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)31142-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Courtemanche C., Garuccio J., Le A., Pinkston J., Yelowitz A. Strong social distancing measures in the United States reduced the COVID-19 growth rate. Health Aff. 2020;39:1237–1246. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2020.00608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lewnard J.A., Lo N.C. Scientific and ethical basis for social-distancing interventions against COVID-19. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2020;20:631–633. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30190-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Torbay R. Are we ready for the next pandemic? Health Aff. 2020;39:1104. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2020.00467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bodas M., Peleg K. Self-isolation compliance in the COVID-19 era influenced by compensation: Findings from a recent survey in Israel. Health Aff. 2020;39:936–941. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2020.00382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cronin C., Evans W.N. Private Precaution and Public Restrictions: What Drives Social Distancing and Industry Foot Traffic in the Covid-19 Era? (July 2020). NBER Working Paper No. w27531. [(accessed on 7 October 2020)]; Available online: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3649895.
- 9.Luby S.P., Agboatwalla M., Feikin D.R., Painter J., Billhimer W., Altaf A., Hoekstra R.M. Effect of handwashing on child health: A randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2005;366:225–233. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66912-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Patel J.A., Nielsen F.B.H., Badiani A.A., Assi S., Unadkat V.A., Patel B., Ravindrane R., Wardle H. Poverty, inequality and COVID-19: The forgotten vulnerable. Public Health. 2020;183:110–111. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2020.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stern S. The Dark Side of Communities: Illiberal Religious Communities’ Compliance with COVID-19 Public Health Regultaions (June 28, 2020). Bar Ilan University Faculty of Law Research Paper No. 20-11. [(accessed on 7 October 2020)]; Available online: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3637335.
- 12.Elcheroth G., Drury J. Collective resilience in times of crisis: Lessons from the literature for socially effective responses to the pandemic. British J. Soc. Psychol. 2020;59:703–713. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Asmundson G.J.G., Taylor S. How health anxiety influences responses to viral outbreaks like COVID-19: What all decision-makers, health authorities, and health care professionals need to know. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020;71:102211. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Leiras G., Arriaga M., Gaspar R., Raposo B., Domingos S. Implementing health literacy Intelligence during COVI-19 outbreak. Eur. J. Public Health. 2020;30:ckaa165.566. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckaa165.566. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Oosterhoff B., Palmer C.A., Wilson J., Shook N. Adolescents’ motivations to engage in social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: Associations with mental and social health. J. Adolesc. Health. 2020;67:179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bonell C., Michie S., Reicher S., West R., Bear L., Yardley L., Curtis V., Amlôt R., Rubin G.J. Harnessing behavioural science in public health campaigns to maintain ‘social distancing’ in response to the COVID-19 pandemic: Key principles. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2020;74:617–619. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bhanot S.P., Hopkins D.J. Partisan polarization and resistance to elite messages: Results from survey experiments on social distancing. J. Behav. Public Adm. 2020;3 doi: 10.30636/jbpa.32.178. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Allcott H., Boxell L., Conway J., Gentzkow M., Thaler M., Yang D. Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. J. Public Econ. 2020;191:104254. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gualano M.R., Lo Moro G., Voglino G., Bert F., Siliquini R. Effects of Covid-19 lockdown on mental health and sleep disturbances in Italy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:4779. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17134779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Morrison L.G., Yardley L. What infection control measures will people carry out to reduce transmission of pandemic influenza? A focus group study. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:258. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Painter M., Qiu T. Political Beliefs affect Compliance with COVID-19 Social Distancing Orders (3 July 2020) [(accessed on 7 October 2020)];2020 Available online: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3569098.
- 22.Hinyard L.J., Kreuter M.W. Using narrative communication as a tool for health behavior change: A conceptual, theoretical, and empirical overview. Health Educ. Behav. 2007;34:777–792. doi: 10.1177/1090198106291963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schank R.C., Berman T.R. Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; Mahwah, NJ, USA: 2002. The pervasive role of stories in knowledge and action; pp. 287–313. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bálint K., Bilandzic H. Health communication through media narratives: Factors, processes and effects—introduction. Int. J. Commun. 2017;11:1–7. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Frank L.B., Murphy S.T., Chatterjee J.S., Moran M.B., Baezconde-Garbanati L. Telling stories, saving lives: Creating narrative health messages. Health Commun. 2014;30:154–163. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2014.974126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gollust S.E., Lantz P.M., Ubel P.A. The polarizing effect of news media messages about the social determinants of health. Am. J. Public Health. 2009;99:2160–2167. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.161414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shen F., Sheer V.C., Li R. Impact of narratives on persuasion in health communication: A meta-analysis. J. Advert. 2015;44:105–113. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2015.1018467. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Clarke C.E., Niederdeppe J., Lundell H.C. Narratives and images used by public communication campaigns addressing social determinants of health and health disparities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2012;9:4254–4277. doi: 10.3390/ijerph9124254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cattin P., Wittink D.R. Commercial use of conjoint analysis: A survey. J. Mark. 1982;46:44–53. doi: 10.1177/002224298204600308. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hagger M.S., Smith S.R., Keech J.J., Moyers S.A., Hamilton K. Predicting social distancing intention and behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic: An integrated social cognition model. Ann. Behav. Med. 2020;54:713–727. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gofman A., Moskowitz H. Improving customers targeting with short intervention testing. Int. J. Innov. Manag. 2010;14:435–448. doi: 10.1142/S1363919610002726. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Boring E. History of Experimental Psychology. Genesis Publishing Pvt Ltd.; Guildford, UK: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Green P.E., Srinivasan V. Conjoint analysis in marketing: New developments with implications for research and practice. J. Mark. 2018;54:3–19. doi: 10.1177/002224299005400402. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Stewart W.E. Statistics for experimenters, George, E.P.; Box, William, G. Hunter and J. Stuart Hunter, John Wiley & Sons, Inc. AIChE J. 1979;25:383. doi: 10.1002/aic.690250233. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dubes R., Jain A.K. Clustering methodologies in exploratory data analysis. In: Yovits M.C., editor. Advances in Computers. Volume 19. Elsevier; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 1980. pp. 113–228. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gabay G., Gere A., Moskowitz H., Zemel G. Proceedings of the Euromed 2020 Conference, Virtual Conference, 9–10 September 2020. Somaiya University; Mumbai, India: 2020. A Mind-Genomics Cartography for Raising Public Awareness to Risks of Diabetes. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kahneman D. Thinking, Fast and Slow. 1st ed. Farrar, Straus and Giroux; New York, NY, USA: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Moskowitz H.R. ‘Mind genomics’: The experimental, inductive science of the ordinary, and its application to aspects of food and feeding. Physiol. Behav. 2012;107:606–613. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Porretta S., Gere A., Radványi D., Moskowitz H. Mind Genomics (Conjoint Analysis): The new concept research in the analysis of consumer behaviour and choice. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2019;84:29–33. doi: 10.1016/j.tifs.2018.01.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Green P.E., Carmone F.J., Schaffer C.M. An individual importance weights model for conjoint analysis. In: Gaul W., Schader M., editors. Data Expert Knowledge and Decisions. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 1988. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hastie T., Tibshirani R., Friedman J. Elements of Statistical Learning. Data Mining, Inference, Prediction. Springer; New York, NY, USA: 2001. Prototypes and Nearest-Neighbors; pp. 460–462. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Milligan G.W. A review of Monte Carlo tests of cluster analysis. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1981;16:379–407. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr1603_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cruz-Correa M., Díaz-Toro E.C., Falcón J.L., García-Rivera E.J., Guiot H.M., Maldonado-Dávila W.T., Martínez K.G., Méndez-Latalladi W., Pérez C.M., Quiñones-Feliciano M.L., et al. Public health academic alliance for COVID-19 response: The role of a national medical task force in Puerto Rico. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:4839. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17134839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]