Abstract
Background
Apathy is one of the most common symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), however, correlations of apathy with demographic variables, cognitive functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms, activity of daily living and olfactory functions in AD patients are still lacking comprehensive investigations.
Methods
This is a cross-sectional study. Total 124 typical AD patients were consecutively recruited from April 2014 to April 2017. In 124 AD patients, 47 cases (37.9%) were male and 77 cases were female; patients’ age were 43–93 years with an average of 68 years. Patients were divided into AD with apathy (AD-A) and AD with no apathy (AD-NA) groups according to the score of Modified Apathy Evaluation Scale, then were evaluated cognitive functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms and activity of daily living, and tested olfactory functions. Above variables were compared between AD-A and AD-NA groups. Further correlation analyses and linear regression analysis were performed between apathy and above variables.
Results
Compared with AD-NA group, global cognitive level, verbal memory, verbal fluency and activity of daily living were significantly compromised in AD-A group (P < 0.002); depression and agitation were severely displayed in AD-A group (P < 0.002). Apathy was negatively correlated with global cognitive function, verbal memory, verbal fluency and activity of daily living (P < 0.05). There was no significant difference of olfactory functions between the two groups (P > 0.002), and correlations between apathy and olfactory threshold, olfactory identification and global olfactory function were significant (P < 0.05) but quite weak (|r| < 0.3). Further linear regression analysis showed that only verbal fluency and instrumental activities of daily living were independently associated with apathy.
Conclusions
Independent correlations among apathy, verbal fluency and instrumental activities of daily living in AD patients might be related to the common brain area involved in their pathogeneses.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, Apathy, Olfactory functions, Cognitive functions, Neuropsychiatric symptoms, Activity of daily living
Background
Apathy is characterized by the lack of motivation, decreased initiative, akinesia, and emotional indifference. Apathy has been proposed to be a signal of imminent cognitive decline and future risk for dementia in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [1]. The prevalence of apathy also increases as dementia severity worsens [2]. It is known that the multiple cognitive domains of AD patients are progressively impaired in the process of disease, however, the specific cognitive domain associated with apathy is rarely reported.
Other neuropsychiatric symptoms, such as depression, anxiety and agitation are also common in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia due to AD. However, the relation between apathy and other neuropsychiatric symptoms in AD, are barely reported and not clear now.
It was reported that patients with MCI and dementia due to AD exhibited significantly worse performance in olfactory identification (ID) compared with healthy individuals [3]. Furthermore, longitudinal studies showed that olfactory identification ID was able to predict cognitive decline due to neuropathology of AD [4]. Olfactory disorder and apathy both could predict the development of AD, however, the relation between them is not clear now, and the relation between apathy and other domains of olfaction, such as olfactory threshold (THR) and discrimination (DIS), are rarely reported.
In a word, this investigation aims to provide comprehensive investigations on correlations between apathy and cognitive functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms, activity of daily living and olfactory functions in AD patients, which may give better understandings of AD with apathy.
Methods
Subjects
This is a cross-sectional study. Total 124 AD patients who were community dwelling were consecutively recruited from April 2014 to April 2017. All patients were diagnosed with typical AD phenotype according to the International Working Group-2 criteria [5]. Patients with history of sinus surgery or nasal fracture, depression or other major mental health disorders were excluded.
Patients’ demographic information, including gender, age, disease duration, educational level and smoking condition was collected. Disease duration was based on the retrospective clinical information of the illness timeline. All AD patients were evaluated by Modified Apathy Evaluation Scale (MAES). MAES [6] is an abridged version of an apathy scale designed by Robert Mann [7]. MAES has 14 items, and the score of each item ranges from 0 to 3 (points), representing “not at all”, “slightly”, “some”, and “a lot”, respectively. Thus, the total score of MEAS ranges from 0 to 42 (points), and the higher score indicates more severe apathy. Using a cutoff point of 14 points, the sensitivity and specificity were 66 and 100%, respectively [6]. According to the results of MAES, patients were divided into AD with apathy (AD-A, MAES> 14 points) group and AD with no apathy (AD-NA, MAES ≤14 points) group, respectively.
Assessments of clinical symptoms
Cognitive functions
Patients were evaluated for global cognitive function by Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Score ranges from 0 to 30 (points); lower score indicates more severe cognitive impairment [8]. Individual cognitive domain was assessed by scales as followed:
Memory: Memory was assessed by the Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) [9]. It includes immediate recall with the score range from 0 to 36 (points), short- delayed recall and long-delayed recall, both with the score range from 0 to 12 (points). Lower score indicates worse memory.
Attention: The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) was used to evaluate attention [10] with score range from 0 to 95 (points). Lower score reflects worse attention. The Trail Making Test A (TMT-A) was another scale used for testing attention [11]. Longer time spent in the test (the maximum is 4 min) means worse attention.
Executive function: The Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT) was used to evaluate executive function [11] with score range from 0 to 50 (points).The lower the score was, the worse the executive function was. The Trail Making Test B (TMT-B) was another scale used for assessing attention [11]. The maximum time used for the test is 4 min. The longer the time consumed, the worse the executive function presented.
Language: The Animal Fluency Test (AFT) was used to assess language [12]. Patients were asked to enumerate animals as more as possible in 1 min. Score was the number of animals enumerated. If the score was lower, the language function was poorer.
Visuospatial ability: The Complex Figure Test (CFT) was used to evaluate visuospatial ability [13] with score range from 0 to 36 (points). Lower score represents worse visuospatial ability.
Neuropsychiatric symptoms
Depression was evaluated by the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD)-24 items [14] with scores range from 0 to 80 (points), and score > 8 points implies depression. Anxiety was assessed by the Hamilton Anxiety Scale (HAMA)-14 items [15] with score range from 0 to 56 (points) and score > 8 points indicated anxiety. Agitation was tested by the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI) [16] with score range from 0 to 203 (points), and the higher the CMAI score was, the severer the agitation was.
Activities of daily living (ADL)
The scales established by Lawton and Brody were used to assess ADL of each patient, including Physical Self-maintenance Scale as basic ADL scale (BADL Scale, score ranges from 6 to 24 points) and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale (IADL Scale, score ranges from 8 to 32 points) [17]. The higher the score was, the worse the ADL was.
Test of olfactory functions
Olfactory functions of AD patients were evaluated by Sniffin’ Sticks test, which was from Burghart Messtenik Company, German (product number: LA-13-00005). There were total 112 sticks, among which, 48, 48 and 16 were used for testing olfactory THR, DIS and ID, respectively.
THR: Participants were exposed to n-butyl alcohol from the lowest concentration to the highest one. The score of olfactory THR was the number of Sniffin sticks with the minimal concentration of n-butyl alcohol that participants identified. The lower the score of olfactory THR was, the worse the function of olfactory recognition was.
DIS: Participants were instructed to distinguish the target odor from the other two. The score of olfactory DIS was the number of Sniffin sticks that participants correctly chose. The lower the score of olfactory DIS was, the worse the function of olfactory discrimination was.
ID: Participants were required to identify the odor he (she) smelled among the 4 given answers. The score of olfactory ID was the number of Sniffin sticks that participants correctly answered. The lower the score of olfactory ID was, the worse the function of olfactory identification was.
Overall olfactory function was assessed by summing up the scores of olfactory THR, DIS and ID, which was abbreviated as TDI. Olfactory dysfunction was identified by the criteria provide by a cross-sectional study in 3282 people [18]. If the age of an individual was between 36 and 55 years old, and male one with TDI score ≤ 24 points, or female one with TDI score ≤ 28 points, was thought to have olfactory dysfunction; if the age of an individual was > 55 years old, a female or male one with TDI score ≤ 19 points was thought to have olfactory dysfunction.
Data analyses
Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS Statistics 20.0 (IBM Corporation, New York, USA). Continuous variables were presented as mean ± standard deviations and compared by 2-tailed t-test if they were normally distributed, and presented as median (quartile) and compared by nonparametric test if they were not normally distributed. Discrete variables were compared by Chi square test. Demographic information, cognitive functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms, olfactory functions and activity of daily life were compared between AD-A and AD-NA groups. Because there were total 25 comparisons between the two groups,Bonferroni correction was performed and the corrected P value was significant when it was less than 0.002(0.05/25 = 0.002). Due to MEAS scores were ordinal data, Spearman correlation analyses were performed between MAES scores and related factors. Further multiple linear regression analysis was performed between apathy and variables that had linear correlations with MAES score. Correlations were considered as significant when P < 0.05 and |r| > 0.3.
Results
In this study, among 124 AD patients, 47 cases (37.9%) were male, and age from 45 to 93 years with an average of 68 years; 77 cases were female and age was 43–88 years with an average of 68 years. Sixty out of 124 (48.4%) AD patients had apathy. The mean apathy score of AD-A group was significantly higher than that of AD-NA group [26.0(18.3 ~ 35.6) vs. 4.5(0.3 ~ 9.0), P < 0.002].
Demographic information, cognitive functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms, ADL and olfactory functions were compared between AD-A and AD-NA groups. The results showed that, gender, age, disease duration, educational level and smoking condition were not significantly different between two groups (P > 0.002) (Table 1).
Table 1.
AD-A group (60 cases) | AD-NA group (64 cases) | P value | |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | 0.580 | ||
Male [case (%)] | 21(35.0%) | 26(40.6%) | |
Female [case (%)] | 39(65.0%) | 38(59.4%) | |
Age [year, mean ± SD] | 67.6 ± 9.9 | 68.4 ± 10.6 | 0.663 |
Disease duration [year, median (quartile)] | 3.0(2.0 ~ 5.0) | 2.0(1.6 ~ 4.0) | 0.225 |
Educational level | 0.238 | ||
Illiteracy [case (%)] | 4(6.7%) | 2(3.1%) | |
Primary school [case (%)] | 12(20.0%) | 8(12.5%) | |
Middle school [case (%)] | 13(21.7%) | 17(26.6%) | |
High school [case (%)] | 20(33.3%) | 16(25.0%) | |
College and above [case (%)] | 11(18.3%) | 21(32.8%) | |
Smoking condition | 0.364 | ||
Smoking [case (%)] | 14(23.3%) | 10(15.6%) | |
Non-smoking [case (%)] | 46(76.7%) | 54(84.4%) |
*P < 0.002 after Bonferroni correction.aAD-A Alzheimer disease with apathy, bAD-NA Alzheimer disease with no apathy
AD-A group had more serious cognitive dysfunctions, including global cognitive function, immediate recall, short-delayed recall, verbal memory and verbal fluency than AD-NA group (P < 0.002). Depression and agitation were more serious in AD-A group than AD-NA group (P < 0.002). AD-A group also had worse ADL, including BADL and IADL (P < 0.002) (Table 2). Further correlation analyses indicated that apathy was negatively correlated with global cognitive function, immediate recall, short-delayed recall, verbal memory and verbal fluency, and positively correlated with ADL, including BADL and IADL (P < 0.05) (Table 3). There was no significant difference of olfactory functions between two groups (P > 0.05) (Table 4). Correlation analyses implied that apathy was negatively correlated with olfactory THR, ID and TDI (P < 0.05). However, the absolute value of coefficients was less than 0.3 (Table 5). Further linear regression analysis was made between apathy and variables that had linear correlations with it, including MMSE, AVLT, AFT, BADL and IADL (Table 6). The results showed that only the scores of AFT and IADL were independently associated with MAES score (P < 0.05).
Table 2.
AD-A group (60 cases) | AD-NA group (64 cases) | P value | |
---|---|---|---|
Global cognitive function | |||
MMSEc [points, median (quartile)] | 18.0(10.0 ~ 25.0) | 26.0(21.3 ~ 29.0) | 0.000* |
Memory | |||
AVLTd−Immediate recall [points, median (quartile)] | 9.0(6.0 ~ 12.0) | 13.0(8.8 ~ 17.0) | 0.001* |
AVLT-Short-delayed recall [points, median (quartile)] | 0(0 ~ 3.0) | 2.5(0 ~ 6.5) | 0.000* |
AVLT-long-delayed recall [points, median (quartile)] | 0(0 ~ 3.0) | 3.0(0 ~ 6.0) | 0.003 |
Attention | |||
SDMTe [points, median (quartile)] | 22.0(12.0 ~ 29.0) | 21.0(15.0 ~ 38.0) | 0.445 |
TMTf-A-time consuming [seconds, median (quartile)] | 108.0(67.0 ~ 187.5) | 75.0(60.0 ~ 122.0) | 0.075 |
Executive function | |||
SCWTg [points, median (quartile)] | 46.5(41.5 ~ 49.25) | 48.0(42.3 ~ 50.0) | 0.265 |
TMT-B- time consuming [seconds, median (quartile)] | 238.0(194.0 ~ 240.0) | 222.0(168.5 ~ 240.0) | 0.550 |
Language | |||
AFTh (points, mean ± SD) | 11.5(8.3 ~ 14.8) | 15.5(12.0 ~ 20.0) | 0.000* |
Visuospatial function | |||
CFTi-Copy [points, median (quartile)] | 22.0(5.0 ~ 32.0) | 26.0(10.5 ~ 34.0) | 0.427 |
Neuropsychiatric symptoms | |||
HAMDj [points, median (quartile)] | 10.0(3.0 ~ 15.0) | 4.0(1.0 ~ 8.0) | 0.001* |
HAMAk [points, median (quartile)] | 6.0(3.0 ~ 12.0) | 3.0(0 ~ 6.0) | 0.004 |
CMAIl [points, median (quartile)] | 29.0(29.0 ~ 38.0) | 29.0(29.0 ~ 29.0) | 0.000* |
Activities of daily living | |||
BADLm [points, median (quartile)] | 6.0(6.0 ~ 10.0) | 6.0(6.0 ~ 6.0) | 0.001* |
IADLo [points, median (quartile)] | 11.5(8.0 ~ 25.5) | 8.0(8.0 ~ 11.0) | 0.000* |
*P < 0.002 after Bonferroni correction, aAD-A Alzheimer disease with apathy, bAD-NA Alzheimer disease with no apathy, c Mini-Mental State Examination, dAVLT Auditory Verbal Learning Test, eSDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, fTMT Trial Making Test, gSCWT Stroop Color Word Test, hAFT Animal Fluency Test, iCFT Complex Figure Test, jHAMD Hamilton Depression Scale, kHAMA Hamilton Anxiety Scale, lCMAI Cohen- Mansfield Agitation Inventory, mBasic Activity of Daily Living, oInstrucmental Activity of Daily Living
Table 3.
Factors | Coefficient | P value |
---|---|---|
Age [year, mean ± SD] | −0.057 | 0.530 |
Disease duration [year, median (quartile)] | 0.113 | 0.220 |
Global cognitive function | ||
MMSEa [points, median (quartile)] | − 0.516 | 0.000* |
Memory | ||
AVLTb− Immediate recall [points, median (quartile)] | −0.346 | 0.000* |
AVLT- Short-delayed recall [points, median (quartile)] | −0.328 | 0.001* |
AVLT- Long-delayed recall [points, median (quartile)] | −0.270 | 0.005* |
Attention | ||
SDMTc [points, median (quartile)] | −0.185 | 0.184 |
TMTd-A-time consuming [seconds, median (quartile)] | −0.042 | 0.752 |
Executive function/attention | ||
SCWTe- correct number [points, median (quartile)] | −0.126 | 0.354 |
TMT-B- time consuming [seconds, median (quartile)] | −0.123 | 0.361 |
Language | ||
AFTf (points, mean ± SD) | −0.453 | 0.000* |
Visuospatial function | ||
CFTg-Copy [points, median (quartile)] | −0.176 | 0.129 |
Neuropsychiatric symptoms | ||
HAMDh [points, median (quartile)] | 0.115 | 0.365 |
HAMAi [points, median (quartile)] | 0.128 | 0.314 |
CMAIj [points, median (quartile)] | −0.096 | 0.450 |
Activities of daily living | ||
BADLk [points, median (quartile)] | 0.415 | 0.000* |
IADLl [points, median (quartile)] | 0.444 | 0.000* |
*P < 0.05, aMini-Mental State Examination, bAVLT Auditory Verbal Learning Test, cSDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, dTMT Trial Making Test, eSCWT Stroop Color Word Test, fAFT Animal Fluency Test, gCFT Complex Figure Test, hHAMD Hamilton Depression Scale, iHAMA Hamilton Anxiety Scale, jCMAI Cohen- Mansfield Agitation Inventory, kBasic Activity of Daily Living, lInstrucmental Activity of Daily Living
Table 4.
AD-A group (60 cases) | AD-NA group (64 cases) | P value | |
---|---|---|---|
THRc [points, median (quartile)] | 4.0(2.0 ~ 6.0) | 5.0(3.0 ~ 6.0) | 0.274 |
DISd [points, median (quartile)] | 8.0(4.5 ~ 10.0) | 8.0(6.0 ~ 11.0) | 0.081 |
IDe [points, median (quartile)] | 8.0(5.0 ~ 10.5) | 9.0(7.0 ~ 12.3) | 0.068 |
TDIf[scores, median (quartile)] | 21.0(14.0 ~ 25.0) | 22.0(17.8 ~ 29.0) | 0.074 |
*P < 0.002 after Bonferroni correction, aAD-A Alzheimer disease with apathy, bAD-NA Alzheimer disease with no apathy, cOlfactory Threshold, dOlfactory Discrimination, eOlfactory Identification, fSum of olfactory threshold, discrimination and identification
Table 5.
Coefficient | P value | |
---|---|---|
THRa [points, median (quartile)] | −0.254 | 0.012* |
DISb [points, median (quartile)] | −0.154 | 0.127 |
IDc [points, median (quartile)] | −0.235 | 0.019* |
TDId [scores, median (quartile)] | −0.242 | 0.016* |
*P < 0.05, aOlfactory Threshold, bOlfactory Discrimination, cOlfactory Identification, dSum of olfactory threshold, discrimination and identification
Table 6.
Factor | Included variables | Excluded variables | P value | ∆R2 | Adjusted R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MAESa |
AFTb IADLd |
MMSEc AVLTe− Immediate recall AVLT- Short-delayed recall AVLT- Long-delayed recall BADLf |
0.000* | 0.230 | 0.215 |
*P < 0.05, aModified Apathy Evaluation Scale, bAFT: Animal Fluency Test, cMini-Mental State Examination, dInstrumental Activity of Daily Living, eAVLT: Auditory Verbal Learning Test, fBasic Activity of Daily Living
Discussion
In this investigation, some of participants were with MCI due to AD, which also had apathy. Patients with MCI due to AD and AD dementia had the same pathological changes of AD, therefore, we did not specifically distinguish MCI due to AD from AD dementia. We hope to figure out the common influencing factors of apathy in AD patients. In AD patients recruited in this study, the frequency of apathy reached up to 48.4%. Thus, clinician should pay great attention to this easily ignored symptom of AD patients.
The relation between apathy and global cognitive function was investigated in this research. Global cognitive function was significantly impaired in AD-A group than AD-NA group (P < 0.002). The relation between apathy and individual cognitive domains was then explored. Memory was the first one we investigated. The results showed that scores of immediate recall and short-delayed recall were significantly lower in AD-A group than AD-NA group (P < 0.002), and both were negatively correlated with the scores of MAES (P < 0.05). It was reported that one pivotal pathology of apathy in AD was the lack of cholinergic transmitters in the frontal and marginal lobes, especially in the frontal lobe [19]. Interim results from another study suggested that combination of donepezil, an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, and choline alphoscerate, a cholinergic precursor, alleviated apathy and reduced caregiver distress [20]. Open label studies with cholinesterase inhibitors, including donepezil, rivastigmine and galantamine, showed an improvement in apathy for AD patients [21]. Therefore, the damage of cholinergic function might connect apathy and memory decline in AD patients.
In this research, AFT score was significantly lower in AD-A group than AD-NA group (P < 0.002), and also negatively correlated with MAES score (P < 0.05). AFT is widely used for assessing semantic function of language, which mainly reflects the function of temporal lobe. The database from the Alzheimer Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, a larger study, showed that cortical thinning in temporal cortex was associated with more severe apathy over time after correcting for multiple covariates, such as sex, age, APOE genotype, premorbid intelligence, memory performance, processing speed, antidepressant use, and AD duration [22]. There are extensive projections from temporal lobe to frontal lobe, so the damage in temporal lobe may lead to apathy and problem of verbal fluency at the same time in AD patients.
In this study, AD-A group had more serious depression than AD-NA group (P < 0.002). This might because that apathy and depression are often comorbid. Apathy and depression have similar clinical manifestations, and share some common pathological changes, such as hypoperfusion. However, they also have their unique pathogenic processes [23], which might explain that no significant correlation was found between depression and apathy in this study (P > 0.05). Apathy was specifically correlated with hypometabolism in left orbitofrontal areas while depression was associated with hypometabolism in left dorsolateral prefrontal regions [24]. Apathy score was positively correlated with PIB signal in bilateral frontal and right anterior cingulate cortices, but no correlation was found between PIB and depression [25]. It was reported that apathy but not depression was strongly associated with the transition from MCI to AD [26].
In this study, agitation was more serious in AD-A group than AD-NA group (P < 0.002). Apathy and agitation in AD patients may share common mechanisms. For example, it was reported that the scores of apathy and agitation were both correlated with the elevated level of Aβ1–42 [27] and the decreased volume of anterior cingulate [28]. Therefore, it can be speculated that both apathy and agitation may simultaneously worsened when the common mechanisms they share become serious. However, they also have their own pathogeneses. For example, it was found that apathy was positively correlated to hypofrontality [24] as well as increased gamma-aminobutyric acid level in serum [29], and agitation was connected with the damage of locus coeruleus [30] and positively related to increased brain derived neurotrophic factor level in plasma [31] . The specific pathogeneses may explain why agitation was not significantly correlated with apathy in this study (|r| < 0.3).
We found that apathy was associated with both basic and instrumental ADL in AD patients (P < 0.002). There might be a few potential explanations. First, the presence of apathy was more common in dementia patients with worse ADL; second, patients with reduced ADL were more likely to have aggravated apathy.
In this study, the scores of olfactory THR, DIS, ID and TDI were not significantly different between AD-A and AD-NA groups (P > 0.002), and further correlation analyses showed that MEAS score was positively correlated with the scores of olfactory THR, ID and TDI (P < 0.05). However, the absolute value of coefficient was < 0.3, which meant correlations between apathy and olfactory dysfunctions was quite weak. These contradictory results might be due to limited sample number of 124 patients in this study, which was more likely to cause the result to be false positive. Therefore, further study with larger sample size is much needed. However, other study has reported there was a correlation between apathy and olfactory dysfunctions. An exploration comparing 172 patients with AD dementia, 112 patients with MCI due to AD and 132 controls by using olfactory ID performance showed that the two types of patients were significantly worse in identifying odors, and smell identification deficit was significantly correlated with the degree of apathy, but not depression or other neuropsychiatric symptoms. Importantly, this correlation was observed even after controlling for the severity of dementia [32]. The anatomic proximity of the olfactory network and apathy-related structures might provide a potential explanation for the close correlation between olfaction and apathy [4].
In this research, a linear regression analysis was made between MAES score and variables that had linear correlations with apathy, including MMSE, AVLT, AFT, BADL and IADL. The results showed that only the scores of AFT and IADL were independently associated with MAES score (P < 0.05). AFT is widely used to test verbal fluency, which reflects semantic function as well as executive function. IADL involves ability of planning and executive function. Executive dysfunction is closely connected with hypofrontality, which also participates in the pathogenesis of apathy. Therefore, we speculate that hypofrontality may be the connection of apathy, verbal fluency and IADL.
The strength of this research is total 11 scales and Sniffin’ Sticks test were used to comprehensively evaluate the clinical symptoms of AD. This investigation also has deficiencies. The sample size was relatively insufficient, and healthy people as control were not included. We did not consider contextual factors that could impact on motivation, such as whether patients were provided with activities that might interest them, and whether the patients’ background was enriched to avoid social isolation. These deficiencies are needed to be avoided in the future study.
Conclusions
In summary, apathy is a very common symptom of AD. It is independently correlated with the impairments of verbal fluency and IADL, which may reflect the association of brain area involved in their pathogeneses.
Acknowledgements
We express sincere thanks to professor Gai-fen Liu of China National Clinical Research Center for Neurological Disease, Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, for providing the valuable advice for statistical analyses.
Abbreviations
- AD
Alzheimer disease
- AD-A
Alzheimer disease with apathy
- AD-NA
Alzheimer disease with no apathy
- MCI
mild cognitive impairment
- MAES
Modified Apathy Evaluation Scale
- MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
- AVLT
Auditory Verbal Learning Test
- SDMT
Symbol Digit Modalities Test
- TMT
Trail Making Test
- SCWT
Stroop Color-Word Test
- AFT
Animal Fluency Test
- CFT
Complex Figure Test
- HAMD
Hamilton Depression Scale
- HAMA
Hamilton Anxiety Scale
- CMAI
Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory
- BADL
Basic Activities of Daily Living
- IADL
Instrumental Activities of Daily Living
- THR
Olfactory Threshold
- DIS
Olfactory Discrimination
- ID
Olfactory Identification
- TDI
Sum of Olfactory Threshold, Discrimination and Identification
Authors’ contributions
Conceptualization, WZ and XW; Methodology, WZ; Investigation, SY, TL, PG, LL, DD, DL LL, HZ, YH, LZ, JG, QY, ZJ, RW and RZ; Formal Analysis, SY; Writing-Original Draft Preparation, SY; Revising the manuscript: TL, PG, LL, DD, DL LL, HZ, YH, LZ, JG, QY, ZJ, RW, RZ and WZ. All authors have read and approved the final version of this manuscript.
Funding
The following programs provided financially support to the conduct of this research, including the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data and writing manuscript: The National Key Research and Development Program of China (2016YFC1306300, 2016YFC1306000); The National Key R&D Program of China-European Commission Horizon 2020 (2017YFE0118800–779238); The National Natural Science Foundation of China (81970992, 81571229, 81071015, 30770745); The Key Technology R&D Program of Beijing Municipal Education Commission (kz201610025030); The Key Project of Natural Science Foundation of Beijing, China (4161004); The Natural Science Foundation of Beijing, China (7082032); Project of Scientific and Technological Development of Traditional Chinese Medicine in Beijing (JJ2018–48); Capital Clinical Characteristic Application Research (Z121107001012161); High Level Technical Personnel Training Project of Beijing Health System, China (2009-3-26); Project of Beijing Institute for Brain Disorders (BIBD-PXM2013_014226_07_000084); Excellent Personnel Training Project of Beijing, China (20071D0300400076); Important National Science & Technology Specific Projects (2011ZX09102–003-01); National Key Technology Research and Development Program of the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (2013BAI09B03); Project of Construction of Innovative Teams and Teacher Career Development for Universities and Colleges Under Beijing Municipality (IDHT20140514); Beijing Healthcare Research Project, China (JING-15-2); Basic-Clinical Research Cooperation Funding of Capital Medical University, China (2015-JL-PT-X04, 10JL49, 14JL15); Natural Science Foundation of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China (PYZ2018077); Youth Research Funding, Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, China (2015-YQN-14, 2015-YQN-15, 2015-YQN-17).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All subjects, or their legal representatives if patients were not capable of providing ethical consent for their participation due to cognitive decline, gave their written informed consent for inclusion before participating in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Beijing Tiantan Hospital review board (KY2013–003-02).
Consent for publication
All authors have seen and agreed with the contents of the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Shu-yang Yu, Email: yushuyang615@163.com.
Teng-hong Lian, Email: lth1990@126.com.
Peng Guo, Email: pengpeng5529@126.com.
Li-xia Li, Email: lixia188@163.com.
Du-yu Ding, Email: 13717783637@126.com.
Dan-ning Li, Email: lidanning1995@126.com.
Li Liu, Email: qingyangxiaoli@163.com.
Hui Zhao, Email: 13521275315@163.com.
Yang Hu, Email: huyang1818@aliyun.com.
Li-jun Zuo, Email: zuolijun1987@126.com.
Jun-hua Gao, Email: 2008gjhde@163.com.
Qiu-jin Yu, Email: qiujin2014@163.com.
Zhao Jin, Email: 352487538@qq.com.
Rui-dan Wang, Email: wrd-1213@163.com.
Rong-yan Zhu, Email: 13722899936@163.com.
Xiao-min Wang, Email: xmwang@ccmu.edu.cn.
Wei Zhang, Email: ttyyzw@163.com.
References
- 1.Onyike CU, Sheppard JM, Tschanz JT, Norton MC, Green RC, Steinberg M, et al. Epidemiology of apathy in older adults: the Cache County study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15(5):365–375. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000235689.42910.0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mega MS, Cummings JL, Fiorello T, Gornbein J. The spectrum of behavioral changes in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1996;46(1):130–135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.46.1.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wesson DW, Wilson DA, Nixon RA. Should olfactory dysfunction be used as a biomarker of Alzheimer's disease? Expert Rev Neurother. 2010;10(5):633–635. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wilson RS, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Olfactory impairment in presymptomatic Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1170:730–735. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04013.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Hampel H, Molinuevo JL, Blennow K, et al. Advancing research diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease: the IWG-2 criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(6):614–629. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70090-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Starkstein SE, Mayberg HS, Preziosi TJ, Andrezejewski P, Leiguarda R, Robinson RG. Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci. 1992;4(2):134–139. doi: 10.1176/jnp.4.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Marin RS. Differential diagnosis and classification of apathy. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147(1):22–30. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.1.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cockrell JR, Folstein MF. Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(4):689–692. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Guo Q, Zhao Q, Chen M, Ding D, Hong Z. A comparison study of mild cognitive impairment with 3 memory tests among Chinese individuals. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23(3):253–259. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181999e92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.A S. Symbol digits modalities test: manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Guo QHSY, Yuan J, Hong Z, Lu CZ. Application of eight executive tests in participants at Shanghai communities. Chin J Behavioral Med Sci. 2007;16:4. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sebaldt R, Dalziel W, Massoud F, Tanguay A, Ward R, Thabane L, et al. Detection of cognitive impairment and dementia using the animal fluency test: the DECIDE study. Can J Neurol Sci. 2009;36(5):599–604. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100008106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Guo QLC, Hong Z. Application of Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test in Chinese normal old people. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2000;8:3. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Whisman MA, Strosahl K, Fruzzetti AE, Schmaling KB, Jacobson NS, Miller DM. A structured interview version of the Hamilton rating scale for depression: reliability and validity. Psychol Assess. 1989;1(3):238–241. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.1.3.238. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Guy W. HAMA Hamilton anxiety scale. ECDEU Assessment Manual. 1976;5.
- 16.Lin LC, Kao CC, Tzeng YL, Lin YJ. Equivalence of Chinese version of the Cohen-Mansfield agitation inventory. J Adv Nurs. 2007;59(2):178–185. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04303.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Katz S, Ford AB, Moskowitz RW, Jackson BA, Jaffe MW. Studies of illness in the aged. The index of Adl: a standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA. 1963;185:914–919. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hummel T, Kobal G, Gudziol H, Mackay-Sim A. Normative data for the “Sniffin’ sticks” including tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds: an upgrade based on a group of more than 3,000 subjects. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2007;264(3):237–243. doi: 10.1007/s00405-006-0173-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Boyle PA, Malloy PF. Treating apathy in Alzheime’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;17(1–2):91–99. doi: 10.1159/000074280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rea R, Carotenuto A, Traini E, Fasanaro AM, Manzo V, Amenta F. Apathy treatment in Alzheimer's disease: interim results of the ASCOMALVA trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;48(2):377–383. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cohen-Mansfield J, Golander H, Cohen R. Rethinking psychosis in dementia: an analysis of antecedents and explanations. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. 2017;32(5):265–271. doi: 10.1177/1533317517703478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Porsteinsson AP, Drye LT, Pollock BG, Devanand DP, Frangakis C, Ismail Z, et al. Effect of citalopram on agitation in Alzheimer disease: the CitAD randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2014;311(7):682–691. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Li XL, Hu N, Tan MS, Yu JT, Tan L. Behavioral and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Biomed Res Int. 2014;2014:927804. doi: 10.1155/2014/927804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Holthoff VA, Beuthien-Baumann B, Kalbe E, Ludecke S, Lenz O, Zundorf G, et al. Regional cerebral metabolism in early Alzheimer’s disease with clinically significant apathy or depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(4):412–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Husebo BS, Ballard C, Sandvik R, Nilsen OB, Aarsland D. Efficacy of treating pain to reduce behavioural disturbances in residents of nursing homes with dementia: cluster randomised clinical trial. BMJ. 2011;343:d4065. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d4065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Guercio BJ, Donovan NJ, Munro CE, Aghjayan SL, Wigman SE, Locascio JJ, et al. The apathy evaluation scale: a comparison of subject, informant, and clinician report in cognitively Normal elderly and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;47(2):421–432. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kurz A, Thone-Otto A, Cramer B, Egert S, Frolich L, Gertz HJ, et al. CORDIAL: cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive-behavioral treatment for early dementia in Alzheimer disease: a multicenter, randomized, controlled trial. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2012;26(3):246–253. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318231e46e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nowrangi MA. Neuropsychiatric aspects of Alzheimer dementia: from mechanism to treatment. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2020;43(2):383–397. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2020.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lanctot KL, Herrmann N, Rothenburg L, Eryavec G. Behavioral correlates of GABAergic disruption in Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2007;19(1):151–158. doi: 10.1017/S1041610206003899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Matthews KL, Chen CP, Esiri MM, Keene J, Minger SL, Francis PT. Noradrenergic changes, aggressive behavior, and cognition in patients with dementia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51(5):407–416. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01235-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nagata T, Kobayashi N, Shinagawa S, Yamada H, Kondo K, Nakayama K. Plasma BDNF levels are correlated with aggressiveness in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer disease. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2014;121(4):433–441. doi: 10.1007/s00702-013-1121-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Seligman SC, Kamath V, Giovannetti T, Arnold SE, Moberg PJ. Olfaction and apathy in Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2013;17(5):564–570. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2013.768208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.