Olfactory dysfunction affects between 3 and 25% of the general population and has severe impacts on quality of life1. Given the prevalence of olfactory disorders and increased risk of hazardous events and mortality among those affected, one could argue that olfactory dysfunction should be considered a major public health issue. Unfortunately, there are a lack of proven interventions that can recover olfactory ability in affected individuals, despite the unique neural plasticity of the olfactory epithelium and olfactory tract that would suggest potential for regeneration and/or recovery. In an approach that exploits this regenerative capacity, olfactory training has recently been introduced as a potential treatment for olfactory dysfunction due to multiple etiologies. This intervention has been rapidly incorporated as a foundational treatment for olfactory disorders both within specialized smell and taste clinics and in general otolaryngology practices. So exactly what is the evidence for olfactory training and should this be considered standard of care in the patient with olfactory dysfunction?
Odor detection is not static, but rather a developing sense that develops from childhood to adulthood. While olfactory sensitivity is generally equivalent among age groups, ‘odor learning’ requires repeated experiences with different smells to develop discriminatory ability, and this is a process that takes years 2. This learning process has been confirmed in multiple psychosocial studies 3, 4 and has served as a foundational tenet for clinical olfactory training as we know it today. In the late 2000s, Hummel et al. performed a prospective, controlled, nonblinded study to investigate the potential efficacy of olfactory training in patients with olfactory loss of multiple etiologies5. Training was performed over a period of 12 weeks and involved patients exposing themselves twice daily to four different odors for 10 seconds each. Patients undergoing olfactory training experienced a mean improvement of 10.3 points on TDI (threshold, discrimination, identification) score. The TDI score is a widely used semi-objective global measure of olfaction administered using standardized odor dispensing devices. There is no published minimal clinically important difference (MCID) in TDI score, however, the correlation between patient’s self-reported noticeable improvement in olfaction and objective changes in the total TDI score after repeat testing has been reported as between 5.5 and 6 points6. Significant (> 6 points) improvement occurred in 28% of patients in the training group versus 6% in the no training group, suggesting potential promise as an effective intervention for a subset of individuals. Multiple additional studies have been performed over the last 10 years, with a comprehensive systematic review showing a statistically significant improvement in TDI score in patients undergoing training versus a control population7. However, it should be noted that the mean difference in post-treatment TDI between groups was less than 4 points, suggesting that intervention may be effective in some patients but that there may be either substantial variability in response among individuals or improvements in many patients that may not be clinically significant.
Despite increasing acceptance as an appropriate intervention, questions with respect to both efficacy and mechanism of action have persisted. It is well established that many patients with olfactory loss will have some level of spontaneous recovery, and deconvoluting this spontaneous recovery from improvement due to olfactory training itself has proved difficult. Appropriate selection of a true placebo has been logistically challenging, and most studies have been of low quality and heterogeneous with respect to patient populations, protocols, and outcome measures7. Doty et al. evaluated olfactory training in a prospective observational study and found that the effectiveness of olfactory training in their population approximated or was worse than that expected from spontaneous recovery alone8–10. In addition, adherence was relatively low when patients did not experience noticeable improvements within the first 3 months8.
Olfactory training has also been rapidly and directly introduced into patient care despite a lack of any clearly established mechanism of action, and it remains unknown whether any potential improvements are due to effects on central or peripheral components of the olfactory system. Negoias et al. found that olfactory training increased olfactory bulb volumes in healthy participants, however, the study (which employed single nostril training) did not include a control group and similar increases in bulb volume were noted between the side of training and the untrained side11. Furthermore, olfactory thresholds actually worsened after treatment. A functional MRI study in humans undergoing olfactory training identified modifications in functional connections between different signaling networks, though no control group was included12. Notwithstanding what could be interpreted as marginal evidence in human studies, the efficacy of olfactory training is supported by a small number of animal experiments. Kim et al. evaluated olfactory recovery in olfactory-lesioned mice via a food-finding test and simulated olfactory training using common odorants over a period of 3 weeks13. Olfactory training improved olfactory recovery time and was accompanied by changes in gene transcriptional pathways associated with neurogenesis. The authors subsequently showed that olfactory ensheathing cells may play an integral role in recovery14. Another study showed that simulated olfactory training in lesioned mice prevented olfactory dysfunction, caused increased neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb, and resulted in an increase in dopaminergic interneurons15.
So what should clinicians and investigators conclude from this assorted evidence? There appears to be substantial experimental data to suggest that olfactory training may modulate the olfactory system and potentially improve olfactory function. However, the degree of improvement may be marginal, and current evidence suggests that the real-world results of olfactory training as a stand-alone intervention could prove disappointing. There may also be subgroup effects, with differences in improvement among populations based on duration of smell loss and/or etiology of olfactory dysfunction16–20. Ultimately, a combination of interventions may prove superior as suggested by a small number of studies that combine olfactory training with systemic or topical corticosteroids21, 22. As evidenced by this review, many questions remain with respect to both the efficacy and mechanism of olfactory training, and high-quality investigations are needed in order to optimize management and identify the populations who will derive the most benefit.
Acknowledgments
Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest: J.H. Turner receives grant support from the NIH/National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (NIAID)
References
- 1.Doty RL. Epidemiology of smell and taste dysfunction. Handb Clin Neurol 2019; 164:3–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cain WS, Stevens JC, Nickou CM, Giles A, Johnston I, Garcia-Medina MR. Life-Span Development of Odor Identification, Learning, and Olfactory Sensitivity. Perception 1995; 24:1457–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Engen T, Bosack TN. Facilitation in olfactory detection. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1969; 68:320–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Livermore A, Laing DG. Influence of training and experience on the perception of multicomponent odor mixtures. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1996; 22:267–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hummel T, Rissom K, Reden J, Hahner A, Weidenbecher M, Huttenbrink KB. Effects of olfactory training in patients with olfactory loss. Laryngoscope 2009; 119:496–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gudziol V, Lotsch J, Hahner A, Zahnert T, Hummel T. Clinical significance of results from olfactory testing. Laryngoscope 2006; 116:1858–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pekala K, Chandra RK, Turner JH. Efficacy of olfactory training in patients with olfactory loss: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Forum Allergy Rhinol 2016; 6:299–307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fornazieri MA, Garcia ECD, Lopes NMD, Miyazawa INI, Silva GDS, Monteiro RDS, et al. Adherence and Efficacy of Olfactory Training as a Treatment for Persistent Olfactory Loss. Am J Rhinol Allergy 2020; 34:238–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.London B, Nabet B, Fisher AR, White B, Sammel MD, Doty RL. Predictors of prognosis in patients with olfactory disturbance. Ann Neurol 2008; 63:159–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cavazzana A, Larsson M, Munch M, Hahner A, Hummel T. Postinfectious olfactory loss: A retrospective study on 791 patients. Laryngoscope 2018; 128:10–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Negoias S, Pietsch K, Hummel T. Changes in olfactory bulb volume following lateralized olfactory training. Brain Imaging Behav 2017; 11:998–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kollndorfer K, Fischmeister FP, Kowalczyk K, Hoche E, Mueller CA, Trattnig S, et al. Olfactory training induces changes in regional functional connectivity in patients with long-term smell loss. Neuroimage Clin 2015; 9:401–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kim BY, Park JY, Kim EJ, Kim BG, Kim SW. The neuroplastic effect of olfactory training to the recovery of olfactory system in mouse model. Int Forum Allergy Rhinol 2019; 9:715–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kim BY, Park J, Kim E, Kim B. Olfactory Ensheathing Cells Mediate Neuroplastic Mechanisms After Olfactory Training in Mouse Model. Am J Rhinol Allergy 2020; 34:217–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marin C, Laxe S, Langdon C, Alobid I, Berenguer J, Fuentes M, et al. Olfactory Training Prevents Olfactory Dysfunction Induced by Bulbar Excitotoxic Lesions: Role of Neurogenesis and Dopaminergic Interneurons. Mol Neurobiol 2019; 56:8063–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jiang RS, Twu CW, Liang KL. The effect of olfactory training on odor identification in patients with traumatic anosmia. Int Forum Allergy Rhinol 2019; 9:1244–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lamira JM, Soler ZM, Schlosser RJ. A Pilot Study of Olfactory Training in Older Hyposmic Adults. Am J Rhinol Allergy 2019; 33:650–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pellegrino R, Han P, Reither N, Hummel T. Effectiveness of olfactory training on different severities of posttraumatic loss of smell. Laryngoscope 2019; 129:1737–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Oleszkiewicz A, Hanf S, Whitcroft KL, Haehner A, Hummel T. Examination of olfactory training effectiveness in relation to its complexity and the cause of olfactory loss. Laryngoscope 2018; 128:1518–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hura N, Xie DX, Choby GW, Schlosser RJ, Orlov CP, Seal SM, et al. Treatment of post-viral olfactory dysfunction: an evidence-based review with recommendations. Int Forum Allergy Rhinol 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Nguyen TP, Patel ZM. Budesonide irrigation with olfactory training improves outcomes compared with olfactory training alone in patients with olfactory loss. Int Forum Allergy Rhinol 2018; 8:977–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bratt M, Moen KG, Nordgard S, Helvik AS, Skandsen T. Treatment of posttraumatic olfactory dysfunction with corticosteroids and olfactory training. Acta Otolaryngol 2020:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]