Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2020 Sep 16;41(18):5255–5281. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25191

Shaky scaffolding: Age differences in cerebellar activation revealed through activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis

Jessica A Bernard 1,2,, An D Nguyen 1,3, Hanna K Hausman 1,4, Ted Maldonado 1, Hannah K Ballard 2, T Bryan Jackson 1, Sydney M Eakin 1, Yana Lokshina 2, James R M Goen 1
PMCID: PMC7670650  PMID: 32936989

Abstract

Cognitive neuroscience research has provided foundational insights into aging, but has focused primarily on the cerebral cortex. However, the cerebellum is subject to the effects of aging. Given the importance of this structure in the performance of motor and cognitive tasks, cerebellar differences stand to provide critical insights into age differences in behavior. However, our understanding of cerebellar functional activation in aging is limited. Thus, we completed a meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies across task domains. Unlike in the cortex where an increase in bilateral activation is seen during cognitive task performance with advanced age, there is less overlap in cerebellar activation across tasks in older adults (OAs) relative to young. Conversely, we see an increase in activation overlap in OAs during motor tasks. We propose that this is due to inputs for comparator processing in the context of control theory (cortical and spinal) that may be differentially impacted in aging. These findings advance our understanding of the aging mind and brain.

Keywords: aging, cerebellum, compensation, meta‐analysis, neuroimaging


Using activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis, this work investigated age differences in cerebellar activation patterns in young and older adults. Unlike in the cortex where more bilateral activation is seen across task domains, in the cerebellum we found less convergence across cognitive tasks in older adults compared to young, but for motor tasks convergence in older adults was more extensive. We propose that this is due to inputs for comparator processing in the context of control theory (cortical and spinal) that may be differentially impacted in aging

graphic file with name HBM-41-5255-g004.jpg

1. INTRODUCTION

Advanced age is accompanied by differences in both cognitive (e.g., Park, Polk, Mikels, Taylor, & Marshuetz, 2001) and motor behavior (reviewed in Seidler et al., 2010). The impact of these differences on quality of life, well‐being, independence, and rehabilitation is large. For example, cognitive complaints and deficits in memory, even in cognitively normal older adults (OAs), have broad impacts on quality of life across social and cognitive domains (Parikh, Troyer, Maione, & Murphy, 2016). Motor system differences include those in learning abilities (e.g., Anguera, Reuter‐Lorenz, Willingham, & Seidler, 2011) and fall risk. Falls are a major cause of disability in OA, and postural control is associated with cognitive performance (Huxhold, Li, Schmiedek, & Lindenberger, 2006). Characterizing the neural underpinnings of age‐related cognitive and motor behavioral differences is critical for both our basic understanding of the aging process and for the elucidation of new remediation targets to improve quality of life for OA.

To this end, the field of the cognitive neuroscience of aging has greatly advanced our understanding of how brain changes and differences in OA impact behavior. Broadly, we know that in advanced age, the brain is smaller (e.g., Walhovd et al., 2011), there are differences in functional networks at rest (Andrews‐Hanna et al., 2007; Langan et al., 2010), and differences in brain activation patterns during task performance (Naccarato et al., 2006; Reuter‐Lorenz, Stanczak, & Miller, 1999; Seidler et al., 2010). Patterns of bilateral activation in OA are commonly seen in cases where young adults (YAs) would typically only activate one hemisphere (Cabeza, 2002; Reuter‐Lorenz et al., 1999). This bilateral activation, particularly in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), has been suggested to be compensatory (Cabeza, 2002; Reuter‐Lorenz & Cappell, 2008) to help maintain performance in advanced age.

While investigating cortical differences in OA has critically informed our understanding of age‐related performance differences, cerebellar contributions to performance have been relatively understudied. There is a growing literature which demonstrates age differences in cerebellar volume (Bernard & Seidler, 2013b; Koppelmans, Young, & Sarah, 2017; Miller et al., 2013; Raz et al., 2005; Han et al., 2020) and connectivity with the cortex (Bernard et al., 2013). As our understanding of the functional contributions of the cerebellum has grown, we know now that it contributes to both motor and cognitive task performance (e.g., Balsters, Whelan, Robertson, & Ramnani, 2013; Chen & Desmond, 2005; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009). Organized with a distinct functional topography (e.g., Stoodley & Schmamann, 2009; Stoodley et al., 2012; King et al., 2019) thought to be derived from closed loop circuits connecting the cerebellum to the cortex, via the thalamus (Middleton & Strick, 2001; Strick, Dum, & Fiez, 2009), more dorsal anterior regions contribute to motor task performance (Lobules I–VI), while the lateral and posterior aspects of the cerebellum (Crus I, Crus II, Lobule VIIb) are engaged during cognitive task performance (Stoodley et al., 2012; King et al., 2019). In addition, there is an inferior secondary motor representation in Lobules VIIIa and VIIIb (Stoodley et al., 2012). Because of the consistent cytoarchitecture of the cerebellum across lobules (unlike in the cerebral cortex), it has been suggested that internal model processing which has been well understood may also occur for cognitive tasks (Ramnani, 2006, 2014; Ito, 2008). Though more recent work has questioned the notion of a universal transform, with respect to cerebellar processing and instead suggesting that the same circuit may conduct different computations (Diedrichsen et al., 2019), to date there have been no clear alternative suggestions as to what computation may be.

As such, with these wide‐ranging behavioral contributions, understanding how the cerebellum may contribute to performance in OA is of great interest and importance. To date, work in this area has demonstrated that differences in both volume and connectivity of the cerebellum are functionally relevant for both motor and cognitive performance in OA (Bernard & Seidler, 2013a; Bernard et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2013). That is, smaller volume in OA is associated with poorer performance, as is lower functional connectivity (Bernard & Seidler, 2013b; Bernard et al., 2013). In these cases, there is also functional specificity. For example, Crus I and Crus II, which are more associated with cognitive tasks, show volumetric and connectivity correlations with cognitive tasks (Bernard & Seidler, 2013a; Bernard et al., 2013).

Recently, we suggested that differences in connectivity with the cortex and smaller lobular volume in the cerebellum in OA contribute to performance differences in aging due to degraded internal models of behavior (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). As noted above, theories of cerebellar function have suggested that the structure acts on copies of commands for behavior and compares the outcomes of a given command with what is expected based on that initial command (Ramnani, 2006, 2014). Ultimately, internal models of a particular movement or thought process are formed (e.g., Balsters et al., 2013; Imamizu et al., 2000) that allow for greater automaticity. However, due to degraded cerebello‐cortical connectivity and the smaller cerebellar volume in OA, the inputs to this structure may be negatively impacted, resulting in degraded internal model processing and, in turn, performance deficits (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). Furthermore, cerebellar function may provide important scaffolding for behavior in OA (Filip, Gallea, Lehéricy, Lungu, & Bareš, 2019). The scaffolding theory of aging and cognition (STAC; Park & Reuter‐Lorez, 2009; Reuter‐Lorenz & Park, 2014) considers individual differences in brain structure, function, life experience, and the broader environmental context that may allow OA to implement scaffolding of function. This scaffolding can be in a variety of forms (e.g., white matter structure, exercise interventions) that can be relied upon to maintain function in advanced age (Park & Reuter‐Lorenz, 2009; Reuter‐Lorenz & Park, 2014). We propose that cerebellar function may be one contributor to this general scaffolding. Indeed, it may be the case that the bilateral processing seen in the cortex in OA (c.f., Cabeza 2002) is to compensate for a lessened ability to rely upon cerebellar resources in advanced age. That is, the brain is less able to offload processing and take advantage of more automatic processing via existing internal models, resulting in a greater need for cortical resources. However, there has been limited work investigating the functional activation patterns of the cerebellum in advanced age, which would provide important insight into this hypothesis. Investigating cerebellar functional activation in OA stands to advance our understanding of the neural underpinnings of behavioral differences and changes in advanced age, providing a more complete perspective on the aging mind and brain.

To better understand the functional engagement of the cerebellum in OA, we conducted a meta‐analysis of the functional brain imaging literature in OA and YA. We tested two competing hypotheses. Based on our prior work suggesting degraded inputs to the cerebellum and internal model processing in OA (Bernard & Seidler, 2014), decreased convergence of cerebellar activation in OA relative to YA would be expected. That is, we would expect to see less consistent overlap in foci of activation across studies. However, the cortical literature consistently demonstrates an increase in bilateral activation in OA during task performance (e.g., Cabeza, 2002; Reuter‐Lorenz et al., 1999). Thus, alternatively, the same pattern may be present in the cerebellum if similar compensatory processes are recruited during task processing. As such, we would expect to see a convergence of foci across studies in both cerebellar hemispheres in OA, while in YA convergence would be limited to one hemisphere (consistent with lateralized findings from prior investigations of the cerebellar functional topography; e.g., E et al., 2014; Stoodley et al., 2012; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009).

2. METHOD

2.1. Literature search and inclusion criteria

All materials associated with the analysis in the form of text files of foci (for more details see below) are freely available for download at https://osf.io/gx5jw/. To identify papers, we completed two separate and sequential literature searches completed using PubMed (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed). The first search used the search term: “cerebell* AND imaging” with the limits “Humans” and “English.” Additionally, we included the limit “Adult 65+” to target the OA literature. Notably, even with this age limit, some manuscripts were included with participants below the age of 65, due to the categorization of papers in our search. However, we carefully investigated the mean ages (when available) as well as the age ranges to ensure that an OA population was studied. As seen in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, the OA in the sample had an average age of approximately 60, though the range was more variable. This resulted in 3,913 articles.

TABLE 1.

Included studies in the “Other Cognitive/Executive Function” category

Study Imaging modality N, YA N, OA Task # YA foci # OA foci Age: Mean (range)
Other cognitive tasks/executive function
Gianaros, Jennings, Sheu, Derbyshire, Matthews (2007). Hypertension, 49, 134–140. 3T fMRI 46 Stroop task 1

YA: N/A

OA: 68.04

Hubert, Beaunieux, Chételat, Platel, Landeau, Viader, Desgranges, Eustache (2009). Hum Brain Map, 30, 1,374–1,386. PET 12 Tower of Toronto task 6

YA: N/A

OA: 65 (60–73)

Luis, Arrondo, Vidorreta, Martínez, Loayza, Fernández‐Seara, Pastor (2015). PLoS ONE, 10, e0131536. 3T fMRI 20 N‐back task combined across spatial and visual domains 2

YA: N/A

OA: 62.2 (58–66)

Moffat, Elkins, Resnick (2006). Neurobiol Aging, 27, 965–972. 1.5T fMRI 30 21 Spatial navigation 1 1

YA: 27.07 (21–39)

OA: 68.43 (60–78)

Belville, Mellah, de Boysson, Demonet, Bier (2014). PLoS ONE, 9, e102710. 3T fMRI 42 Dual‐tasking using alphabetic equations 1

YA: N/A

OA: Not specified

Beauchamp, Dagher, Aston, Doyon (2003). NeuroImage, 20, 1,649–1,660. PET 12 Tower of London task 3

YA: N/A

OA: 56.8 (51–69)

Harrington, Castillo, Greenberg, Song, Lessig, Lee, Rao (2011). PLoS ONE, 6, e17461. 3T fMRI 19 Time perception 14

YA: N/A

OA: 64.6

Drezga, Grimmer, Peller, Wermke, Siebner, Rauschecker, Schwaiger, Kurz (2005). PLoS Med, 2, e288. PET 10 Spatial navigation of 3D environments 4

YA: N/A

OA: 68.8

Grönholm, Rinne, Vorobyev, Laine (2005). Cog Brain Res, 25, 359–371. PET 10 Object naming 10

YA: N/A

OA: 65.5 (56–77)

Ramanoël, Kaufmann, Cousin, Dojat, Peyrin (2015). PLoS ONE, 10, e0134554. 3T fMRI 12 12 Spatial scene processing 0 1

YA: 22.3 (18–26)

OA: 64 (61–71)

Madden, Langley, Denny, Turkington, Provenzale, Hawk, Coleman (2002). Brain Cogn, 49, 297–321. PET 12 12 Visual search 2 7

YA: 23.58 (20–29)

OA: 65.00 (62–70)

Geerligs, Maurits, Renken, Lorist (2014). Hum Brain Mapp, 35, 319–330. 3T fMRI 12 30 Visual oddball task 2 0

YA: 24.1

OA: 63.9

Gilbert, Bird, Brindley, Frith, Burgess (2008). Neuropsychologia, 46, 2,281–2,291.** 3T fMRI 18 Random generation task 3

YA: 32

OA: N/A

Rao, Bobholz, Hammeke, Rosen, Woodley, Cunningham, Cox, Stein, Binder (1997). NeuroReport, 8, 1987–1993.* 1.5T fMRI 11 Conceptual reasoning 3

YA: 29 (19–45)

OA: N/A

Dagher, Owen, Boecker, Brooks (1999). Brain, 122, 1973–1987.** PET 6 Tower of London task 6

YA: N/A

OA: 58.6 (49–70)

Jahanshahi, Dirnberger, Fuller, Frith (2000). NeuroImage, 12, 713–725.* PET 6 Random number generation relative to counting 2

YA: 29.6

OA: N/A

Liddle, Kiehl, Smith (2001). Hum Brain Mapp, 12, 100–109.* 1.5T fMRI 16 Go no‐go task 15

YA: 30.2

OA: N/A

Ernst, Bolla, Mouratidis, Contoreggi, Matochik, Kurian, Cadet, Kimes, London (2002). Neuropsychopharmacol, 26, 682–691.* PET 20 Risk decision making 10

YA: 30.4 (21–45)

OA: N/A

Kondo, Morishita, Osaka, Osaka, Fukuyama, Shibasaki (2004). NeuroImage, 21, 2–14.** 1.5T fMRI 10 Arithmetic and memory 6

YA: 23.6 (22–27)

OA: N/A

Harrington, Boyd, Mayer, Sheltraw, Lee, Huang, Rao (2004). Cog Brain Res, 21, 193–205.* 1.5T fMRI 24 Interval timing decision making 4

YA: 30.6 (21–53)

OA: N/A

Dreher & Grafman (2002). Eur J Neurosci, 16, 1,609–1,619.** 1.5T fMRI 18 Task switching during letter discrimination 4

YA: 25 (20–31)

OA: N/A

Schall, Johnston, Lagopoulos, Jüptner, Jentzen, Thienel, Dittman‐Balçar, Bender, Ward (2003). NeuroImage, 20, 1,154–1,161.** PET/1.5T fMRI 6 Tower of London task 2

YA: 31.0 (21–41)

OA: N/A

Daniels, Witt, Wolff, Jansen, Deuschl (2003). Neurosci Lett, 345, 25–28.* 1.5T fMRI 8 Random number generation at 1 Hz 2

YA: 25.4

OA: N/A

Blackwood, ffytche, Simmons, Bentall, Murray, Howard (2004). Cog Brain Res, 20, 46–53.* 1.5T fMRI 8 Decision making under certain and uncertain conditions 2

YA: 38 (18–53)

OA: N/A

Lenzi, Serra, Perri, Pantano, Lenzi, Paulesu, Caltagirone, Bozzali, Macaluso (2011). Neurobiol Aging, 32, 1,542–1,577. 3T fMRI 14 Visuospatial attention; line bisection judgments relative to color judgment 1

YA: N/A

OA: 64.3 (50–81)

Leshikar, Gutchess, Hebrank, Sutton, Park (2010). Cortex, 26, 507–521. 3T fMRI 19 18 Relational encoding 0 3

YA: 20.9 (18–26)

OA: 65.7 (60–80)

Hartley, Jonides, Sylvester (2011). Brain & Cognition, 75, 281–291. 3T fMRI 12 12 Dual‐task processing with letters 1 0

YA: 21.00 (19–25)

OA: 70.67 (65–77)

Note: Notably, while many of the included studies had both YA and OA, in some instances, OA data came from clinical studies wherein the OA served as a control group. Furthermore, additional YA data came from studies included in prior meta‐analyses. Cases where there were no cerebellar coordinates are indicated by a 0 in the appropriate foci column. Mean age is provided in years, and the range is also provided when available. N/A: not applicable. “—” denotes studies where a particular age group was not included and as such no coordinates are possible.

*

Studies included as part of Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009) and E et al. (2014).

**

Studies only included in E et al. (2014).

TABLE 2.

Included studies of language tasks

Study Imaging modality N, YA N, OA Task # YA foci # OA foci Age: Mean (range)
Language
Rizio, Moyer, Diaz (2017). Brain Behav, 7, e00660. 3T fMRI 20 20 Language interference naming 0 2

YA: 23.7 (18–31)

OA: 67.25 (60–79)

Provost, Brambati, Chapleau, Wilson (2016). Cortex, 84, 90–100. 3T fMRI 16 16 Word reading 0 1

YA: 27.5 (22–33)

OA: 67.0 (60–75)

Martins, Simard, Monchi (2014). PLoS ONE, 9, e99710. 3T fMRI 14 14 Lexical version of the Wisconsin card sorting task 6 3

YA: 26 (21–31)

OA: 63 (55–71)

Whatmough, Verret, Fung, Chertkow (2004). J Cogn Neurosci, 16, 1,211–1,226. PET 15 Semantic judgments of word pairs 4

YA: N/A

OA: 74.3 (69–90)

Olichney, Taylor, Hillert, Chan, Salmon, Gatherwright, Iragui, Kutas (2010). Neurobiol Aging, 31, 1975–1990. 1.5T fMRI 17 Word memory and repetition 3

YA: N/A

OA: 69.7

Daselaar, Veltman, Rombouts, Raaijmakers, Jonker (2003). Neurobiol Aging, 24, 1,005–1,011. 1.5T fMRI 26 39 Semantic characterization after shallow or deep encoding 1 1

YA: 32.4 (30–35)

OA: 66.3 (63–71)

Madden, Langley, Denny, Turkington, Provenzale, Hawk, Coleman (2002). Brain Cogn, 49, 297–321. PET 12 12 Lexical decision task 3 2

YA: 23.58 (20–29)

OA: 65.00 (62–70)

Seki, Okada, Koeda, Sadato (2004). Cog Brain Res, 20, 261–272.* 3T fMRI 19 Vowel exchange compared to reading words and non‐words 2

YA: 23.3

OA: N/A

Rauschecker, Pringle, Watkins (2008). Hum Brain Mapp, 29, 1,231–1,242.** 3T fMRI 14 Listening and cover repetition of non‐words 4

YA: 23.3 (20–34)

OA: N/A

Daselaar, Veltman, Rombouts, Raaijmakers, Jonker (2005). Neuriobiol Learn Mem, 83, 251–262. 1.5T fMRI 25 38 Word‐stem completion 1 0

YA: 32.3 (30–35)

OA: 66.4 (63–71)

Ojemann, Buckner, Akbudak, Snyder, Ollinger, McKinstry, Rosen, Petersen, Raichle, Conturo (1998). Hum Brain Mapp, 6, 203–215.* PET/1.5T fMRI 7 Word‐stem completion 7

YA: 24 (19–28)

OA: N/A

Schlosser, Hutchinson, Joseffer, Rusinek, Saarimaki, Stevenson, Dewey, Brodie (1998). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 64, 492–498.* 1.5T fMRI 6 Verbal fluency task 6

YA: 23 (22–26)

OA: N/A

Lurito, Kareken, Lowe, Chen, Mathews (2000). Hum Brain Mapp, 10, 99–106.* 1.5T fMRI 5 Word generation compared to viewing non‐letter symbols 3

YA: 27

OA: N/A

Seger, Desmond, Glover, Gabrieli (2000). Neuropsychol, 14, 361–369.* 1.5T fMRI 7 Verb generation task 16

YA: 31

OA: N/A

Gurd, Amunts, Weiss, Zafiris, Zilles, Marshall, Fink (2002). Brain, 125, 1,024–1,038.* 1.5T fMRI 11 Semantic fluency relative to overlearned sequence fluency (days of the week) 1

YA: 32

OA: N/A

Noppeney & Price (2002). NeuroImage, 15, 927–935.* PET 12 Semantic decision task 2

YA: 24 (20–30)

OA: N/A

McDermott, Petersen, Watson, Ojemann (2003). Neuropsychologia, 41, 293–303.* 1.5T fMRI 20 Word lists of semantic compared to phonological 3

YA: 22.1 (18–32)

OA: N/A

Xiang, Lin, Ma, Zhang, Bower, Weng, Gao (2003). Hum Brain Map, 18, 208–214.* 1.5T fMRI 6 Semantic discrimination 1

YA: 21–36

OA: N/A

Tieleman, Seurinck, Deblaere, Vandemaele, Vingerhoets, Achten (2005), NeuroImage, 26, 565–572.* 1.5T fMRI 22 Semantic compared to perceptual categorization 3

YA: 29 (22–47)

OA: N/A

Frings, Dimitrova, Schorn, Elles, Hein‐Kropp, Gizewski, Diener, Timmann (2006). Neurosci Letters, 409, 19–23.* 1.5T fMRI 16 Verb generation task 3

YA: 24.9 (18–35)

OA: N/A

Callan, Tsytsarev, Hanakawa, Callan, Katsuhara, Fukuyama, Turner (2006), NeuroImage, 31, 1,327–1,342.** 3T fMRI 16 Listening to and covert production of singing relative to speech 2

YA: 26 (19–47)

OA: N/A

Sweet, Paskavitz, Haley, Gunstad, Mulligan, Nyalakanti, Cohen (2008). Neuropsychologia, 46, 1,114–1,123.** 1.5T fMRI 34 Phonological similarity during verbal working memory 1

YA: 37.24 (18–80)

OA: N/A

Durisko & Fiez (2010). Cortex, 46, 896–906.** 3T fMRI 19 Delayed serial recall task with letters 2

YA: 23 (19–33)

OA: N/A

Davis, Kragel, Madden, Cabeza (2012). Cereb Cortex, 22, 232–242. 3T fMRI 18 16 Semantic matching task 0 1

YA: 21.70

OA: 68.06

Shafto, Randall, Stamatakis, Wright, Tyler (2012). J Cognitive Neurosci, 24, 1,434–1,446. 3T fMRI 14 16 Lexical decision task, imageability of high and low competition words 2 0

YA: 23.86

OA: 75.75

Note: Notably, while many of the included studies had both YA and OA, in some instances, OA data came from clinical studies wherein the OA served as a control group. Furthermore, additional YA data came from studies included in prior meta‐analyses. “—” denotes studies where a particular age group was not included and as such no coordinates are possible. Cases where there were no cerebellar coordinates are indicated by a 0 in the appropriate foci column. N/A: not applicable.

*

Studies included as part of Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009) and E et al. (2014).

**

Studies only included in E et al., 2014.

TABLE 3.

Included studies of motor tasks

Study Imaging modality N, YA N, OA Task # YA foci # OA foci Age: Mean (range)
Motor
Wurster, Graf, Ackermann, Groth, Kassubek, Riecker (2015). Brain Struct Func, 220, 1,637–1,648. 3T fMRI 10 Finger tapping 2

YA: N/A

OA: 64.9

Blumen, Holtzer, Brown, Gazes, Verghese (2015). Hum Brain Mapp, 35, 4,090, 4,104. 3T fMRI 33 Imagined walking and walking while talking 1

YA: N/A

OA: 73.03

Allali, van der Meulen, Beauchet, Rieger, Vuilleumier, Assal (2014). J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 69, 1,389–1,398. 3T fMRI 14 14 Motor imagery 1 3

YA: 27.0

OA: 66.0

Wittenberg, Lovelace, Foster, Maldjian (2014). Brain Imaging Behav, 8, 335–345. 1.5T fMRI 12 12 Ecologically valid motor self‐care tasks (e.g., buttoning & zipping) and finger tapping 11 6

YA: 29.0

OA: 61.0

Crémers, D'Ostilio, Stamatakis, Delvaux, Garraux (2012). Movement Disorders, 27, 1,498–1,505. 3T fMRI 15 Imagined gait relative to imagined standing 3

YA: N/A

OA: 63.8

Vidoni, Thomas, Honea, Koskutova, Burns (2012). J Neurol Phys Ther, 36, 8–16. 3T fMRI 10 Power grip 3

YA: N/A

OA: 73.6

Zwergal, Linn, Xiong, Brandt, Strupp, Jahn (2012). Neurobiol Aging, 33, 1,073–1,084. 3T fMRI 20 20 Imagined movement, standing and walking 0 5

YA: 24–40

OA: 60–78

Askim, Indredavik, Haberg (2010). Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 91, 1,529–1,536. 1.5T fMRI 15 Finger tapping – Paced and self‐paced 3

YA: N/A

OA: 65.9 (50–75)

Allen & Humphreys (2009). Current Biol, 19, 1,044–1,049. 3T fMRI 7 Somatomotor tactile stimulation 1

YA: N/A

OA: >74

Eckert, Peschel, Heinze, Rotte (2006). J Neurol, 253, 199–207. 1.5T fMRI 9 Opening and closing fist 1

YA: N/A

OA: 60.6

Heuninckx, Wenderoth, Debaere, Peeters, Swinnn (2005). J Neurosci, 25, 6,787–6,796. 3T fMRI 12 12 Coordinated hand and foot movements 8 31

YA: 22.4 (20–25)

OA: 64.8 (62–71)

Rowe, Stephan, Friston, Frackowiak, Lees, Passingham (2002). Brain, 125, 267–289. Not specified 12 Motor sequence learning 3

YA: N/A

OA: 62.0

Kalpouzos, Garzón, Sitnikov, Heiland, Salami, Persson, Bäckman (2017). Cereb Cortex, 27, 3,427–3,436. 3T fMRI 22 15 Motor imagery 1 2

YA: 36.8

OA: 69.7

King, Saucier, Albouy, Fogel, Rumpf, Klann, Buccino, Binkofski, Classen, Karni, Doyon (2017). Cereb Cortex, 27, 1,588–1,601. 3T fMRI 26 Motor sequence learning, initial training only included here 3

YA: N/A

OA: 63.5

Wang, Qiu, Liu, Yan, Yang, Zhang, Zhang, Sang, Zheng (2014). Neuroraiol, 56, 339–348. 3T fMRI 19 20 Motor execution and imagery 6 8

YA: 36.5 (20–23)

OA: 62.5 (52–82)

Heuninckx, Wenderoth, Swinnen (2010). Neurobiol Aging, 31, 301–314. 3T fMRI 12 12 Externally and internally guided movements 6 8

YA: 23.5 (21–27)

OA: 66.9 (63–73)

Riecker, Gröschel, Ackermann, Steinbrink, Witte, Kastrup (2006). NeuroImage, 32, 1,345–1,354. 1.5T fMRI 10 10 Motor tapping 1 1

YA: 23.0 (18–26)

OA: 66.0 (58–82)

Zapparoli, Invernizzi, Gandola, Verardi, Berlingeri, Sberna, De Santis, Zerbi, Banfi, Bottini, Paulesu (2012). Exp Brain Res, 224, 519–540. 1.5T fMRI 24 24 Finger to thumb opposition 1 0

YA: 27.0

OA: 60.0

Jäncke, Loose, Lutz, Specht, Shah (2000). Cog Brain Res, 10, 51–66. 1.5T fMRI 8 Finger tapping 3

YA: 20–32

OA: N/A

Lutz, Specht, Shah, Jäncke (2000). NeuroReport, 11, 1,301–1,306. 1.5T fMRI 10 Finger tapping 4

YA: 24.1 (21–29)

OA: N/A

Riecker, Wildgruber, Mathiak, Grodd, Ackermann (2003). NeuroImage, 18, 2003, 731–739. 1.5T fMRI 8 Finger tapping 12

YA: 23.75 (19–32)

OA: N/A

Hanakwa, Dimyan, Hallett (2008). Cereb Cortex, 18, 2,775–2,788. 3T fMRI 13 Finger tapping 2

YA: 30.0 (21–48)

OA: N/A

Brunne, Skouen, Ersland, Grüner (2014). Neuroreb Neral Repair, 28, 874–884. 3T fMRI 18 Observation and execution of bimanual movements 4

YA: N/A

OA: 60.6

Taniwaki, Okayama, Yoshiura, Togao, Nakamura, Yamsaki, Ogata, Shigeto, Shyagi, Kira, & Tobimatsu (2007). NeuroImage, 36, 1,263–1,276. 1.5T fMRI 12 12 Externally triggered or self‐initiated finger movements 6 4

YA: 24.9 (23–29)

OA: 62.9 (53–72)

Taniwaki, Yoshiura, Ogata, Togao, Yamashita, Kida, Miura, Kira, Tobimatsu (2013). Brain Res, 1,512, 45–59. 1.5T fMRI 12 Externally triggered or self‐initiated finger movements 4

YA: N/A

OA: 62.0 (54–72)

Daselaar, Rombouts, Veltman, Raaijmakers, Jonker (2003). Neurobiol Aging, 24, 1,013–1,019. 1.5T fMRI 26 40 Motor sequence learning 1 3

YA: 32.4 (30–35)

OA: 66.4 (63–71)

Onozuka, Fujita, Watanabe, Hirano, Niwa, Nishiyama, Saito (2003). J Dent Res, 82, 657–660. 1.5T fMRI 10 10 Chewing 1 1

YA: 19–26

OA: 65–73

Rijntjes, Buechel, Kiebel, Weiller (1999). NeuroReport, 10, 3,653–3,658. 2T fMRI 9 Finger flexion and extensions 3

YA: 32.0

OA: N/A

Hankawa, Immisch, Toma, Dimyan, van Gelderen, Hallett (2003). J Neurophysiol, 89, 989–1,002. 1.5T fMRI 10 Finger tapping 3

YA: 32.0

OA: N/A

Loibl, Beutling, Kaza, Lotze (2011). Behav Brain Res, 223, 280–286. 1.5T fMRI 18 17 Passive wrist movement, fist clenching, precision grip 21 17

YA: 25.39 (23–30)

OA: 66.65 (57–72)

Linortner, Fazekas, Schmidt, Ropele, Pendl, Petrovic, Loitfelder, Neuper, Enzinger (2012). Neurobiol Aging, 197, e1‐191‐e9‐17. 3T fMRI 17 Ankle and finger movements 2

YA: N/A

OA: 63.59 (48–84)

Linortner, Fazekas, Schmidt, Ropele, Pendl, Petrovic, Loitfelder, Neuper, Enzinger (2012). Neurobiol Aging, 197, e1‐191‐e9‐17. 3T fMRI 13 Ankle and finger movements 3

YA: N/A

OA: 73.31 (48–84)

Kim, Lee, Lee, Song, Yoo, Lee, Kim, Chang (2010). Neurological Res, 32, 995–1,001. 3T fMRI 20 26 Weighted and unweighted elbow flexion/extension 0 3

YA: 23.0

OA: 65.5

Rieckman, Fischer, Bäckman (2010). NeuroImage, 50, 1,303–1,312. 1.5T fMRI 14 13 Serial reaction time task 2 1

YA: 24.71

OA: 68.08

Huang, Lee, Hsiao, Kuan, Wai, Ko, Wan, Hsu, Liu (2010). J Neurosci Methods, 189, 257–266. 1.5T fMRI 16 Hand flexion 1

YA: 22.0 (18–25)

OA: N/A

Bo, Peltier, Noll, Seidler (2011). Neurosci Letters, 504, 68–72. 3T fMRI 14 14 Motor sequence learning with symbolic and spatial presentation of stimuli 1 0

YA: 21.4

OA: 72.7

Dennis, Cabeza (2011). Neurobiol Aging, 2,318.17‐2,318.e30. 4T fMRI 12 12 Serial reaction time task 1 0

YA: 22.2 (18–30)

OA: 67.4 (60–79)

Note: Notably, while many of the included studies had both YA and OA, in some instances OA data came from clinical studies wherein the OA served as a control group. Furthermore, additional YA data came from studies included in prior meta‐analyses. “—” denotes studies where a particular age group was not included and as such no coordinates are possible. Cases where there were no cerebellar coordinates are indicated by a 0 in the appropriate foci column.The study by Linortner and colleagues (2012) is listed twice. Because two distinct and unique samples of older adults were included, the foci were entered separately into the analyses. Huang and colleagues (2010) looked at older adults in a separate experiment investigating working memory (see Table 4), and the motor task was only conducted in the young adult sample. However, the data are included here as based on our search terms; this study met our criteria. N/A: not applicable.

*

Studies included as part of Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009).

TABLE 4.

Included studies of working memory

Study Imaging modality N, YA N, OA Task # YA foci # OA foci Age: Mean (range)
Working memory
Luis, Arrondo, Vidorreta, Martínez, Loayza, Fernández‐Seara, Pastor (2015). PLoS ONE, 10, e0131536. 3T fMRI 20 N‐back task 1

YA: N/A

OA: 62.2 (58–66)

Boller, Mellah, Ducharme‐Laliberté, Belleville (2017). Brain Imaging Behav, 11, 304–317. 3T fMRI 40 N‐back task 3

YA: N/A

OA: 68.59 (60–84)

Heinzel, Lorenz, Pelz, Heinz, Walter, Kathmann, Rapp, Stelzel (2016). NeuroImage, 134, 236–249. 3T fMRI 32 N‐back task and Sternberg task, baseline assessment only 11

YA: N/A

OA: 66.07 (60–75)

Charroud, Steffener, Le Bars, Deverdun, Bonafe, Abdennour, Portet, Molino, Stern, Ritchie, Menjot de Champfleur, Akbaraly (2015). Neurobiol Learn Mem, 1,250, 211–223. 3T fMRI 337 Delayed‐item recognition task 10

YA: N/A

OA: 82.1

Luis, Arrondo, Vidorreta, Martínex, Loayza, Fernández‐Seara, Pastor (2015). PLoS ONE, 10, e0131536. 3T fMRI 20 Verbal working memory load manipulation 4

YA: N/A

OA: 62.2 (58–66)

Migo, Mitterschiffthaler, O'Daly, Dawson, Dourish, Craig, Simmons, Wilcock, McCulloch, Jackson, Kopelman, Williams, Morris (2015). Aging, Neuropsychol, Cog, 22, 106–127. 3T fMRI 11 N‐back task 2

YA: N/A

OA: 70.27 (60–80)

Griebe, Amann, Hirsch, Achtnichts, Hennerici, Gass, Szabo (2014). PLoS ONE, 9, e103359. 1.5T fMRI 14 N‐back task 3

YA: N/A

OA: 67.0 (55–79)

Emery, Heaven, Paxton, Braver (2008). NeuroImage, 42, 1,577–1,586. 1.5T fMRI 10 11 Letter‐number sequencing 0 3

YA: 21.9 (18–27)

OA: 71.2 (65–82)

Jennings, van der Veen, Melzer (2006). Brain Res, 1,092, 177–189. PET 89 N‐back task 3

YA: N/A

OA: 61.0 (50–70)

Lensinger, Born, Meindl, Bokde, Britsch, Lopez‐Bayo, Teipel, Möller, Hampel, Reiser (2007). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 24, 235–246. 1.5T fMRI 15 19 Location matching task 5 6

YA: 28.0

OA: 71.0

Lamar, Yousem, Resnick (2004). NeuroImage, 21, 1,368–1,376. 1.5T fMRI 16 16 Delayed match to sample 2 1

YA: 27.9 (20–40)

OA: 69.1 (60–80)

Schneider‐Garces, Gordon, Brumback‐Peltz, Shin, Lee, Sutton, Maclin, Gratton, Fabiani (2010). J Cogn Neurosci, 22, 655–659. 3T fMRI 12 30 Sternberg task 1 0

YA: 23.8 (18–27)

OA: 70.9 (65–80)

Vellage, Becke, Strumpf, Baier, Schönfeld, Hopf, Müller (2016). Brain Behav, 6, e00544. 3T fMRI 40 38 Spatial working memory filtering task 2 0

YA: 25.7 (21–32)

OA: 65.8 (58–74)

Valera, Faraone, Beiderman, Poldrack, Seidman (2005). Biol Psychiatry, 57, 439–447.* 1.5T fMRI 20 N‐back task 1

YA: 33.0 (18–55)

OA: N/A

Hayter, Langdon, Ramnani (2007), NeuroImage, 36, 943–954.** 3T fMRI 15 Paced auditory serial addition task 6

YA: 18–29

OA: N/A

Scheuerecker, Ufer, Zipse, Frodl, Koutsouleris, Zetzsche, Wiesmann, Albrecht, Brückmann, Schmitt, Möller, Meisenzahl (2008). J Psychiat Res, 42, 469–476.** 1.5T fMRI 23 N‐back task 5

YA: 32.6

OA: N/A

Hautzel, Mottaghy, Specht, Müller, Krause (2009). NeuroImage, 47, 2073–2082.** 1.5T fMRI 17 N‐back task 19

YA: 25.7

OA: N/A

Marvel and Desmond (2010). Cortex, 46, 880–895.** 3T fMRI 16 Sternberg task 2

YA: 23.69 (19–28)

OA: N/A

Oren, Ash, Tarrasch, Hendler, Giladi, Shapira‐Lichter (2017). Neurobiol Aging, 53, 93–102. 3T fMRI 24 28 N‐back task 2 0

YA: 29.0 (22.35)

OA: 71.8 (65–79)

Grady, McIntosh, Bookstein, Horwitz, Rapoport, Haxby (1998). NeuroImage, 8, 409–425. PET 13 16 Working memory of facial stimuli 1 0

YA: 25.0

OA: 66.0

Fiez, Raife, Balota, Schwarz, Raichle, Petersen (1996). J Neurosci, 16, 808–822.** PET 12 Working memory during PET?? 5

YA: 24.0

OA: N/A

Schumacher, Lauber, Awh, Jonides, Smith, Koeppe (1996). NeuroImage, 3, 79–88.** PET 8 Visual and auditory working memory 8

YA: Not reported

OA: N/A

Jonides, Schumacher, Smith, Koeppe, Awh, Reuter‐Lorenz, Marshuetz, Willis (1998). J Neurosci, 18, 5,026–5,034.** PET 12 Storage and fixation, modeled after Fiez et al., 1996 3

YA: Not reported

OA: N/A

LaBar, Gitelman, Parrish, Marsel Mesulam (1999). NeuroImage, 10, 695–704.* 1.5T fMRI 11 N‐back task 1

YA: 32.6

OA: N/A

Thomas, King, Franzen, Welsh, Berkowitz, Noll, Birmaher, Casey (1999). NeuroImage, 10, 327–338.** 1.5T fMRI 6 N‐back task 1

YA: 22.0 (19–26)

OA: N/A

Honey, Bullmore, Sharma (2000). NeuroImage, 12, 495–503.* 1.5T fMRI 20 N‐back task 1

YA: 39.3 (19–64)

OA: N/A

Gruber (2001). Cereb Cortex, 11, 1,047–1,055.* 3T fMRI 11 Letter memory relative to letter case judgment with or without articulatory suppression 1

YA: 23.6

OA: N/A

Cairo, Liddle, Woodward, Ngan (2004). Cog Brain Res, 21, 377–387.* 1.5T fMRI 18 Sternberg task 13

YA: 27.5 (18–35)

OA: N/A

Kirschen, Chen, Schraedley‐Desmond, Desmond (2005). NeuroImage, 24, 462–472.* 3T fMRI 17 Verbal working memory with increasing load 6

YA: 25.0

OA: N/A

Chen and Desmond (2005b). Neuropsychologia, 43, 1,227–1,237.* 3T fMRI 15 Sternberg task 9

YA: 22.53 (18–28)

OA: N/A

Chen and Desmond (2005a). NeuroImage, 24, 332–338.** 3T fMRI 15 Verbal working memory with high and low loads 9

YA: 28.6

OA: N/A

Tomasi, Caparelli, Chang, Ernst (2005). NeuroImage, 27, 377–386.* 4T fMRI 30 N‐back task 3

YA: 31.0

OA: N/A

Woodward, Cairo, Ruff, Takane, Hunter, Ngan (2006). Neuroscience, 139, 317–325.** 1.5T fMRI 18 Verbal working memory with varying loads 5

YA: 27.5 (18–35)

OA: N/A

Geier, Garver, Luna (2007). NeuroImage, 35, 904–915.** 3T fMRI 18 Occulomotor delayed response task (spatial working memory) 3

YA: 18–30

OA: N/A

Tomasi, Chang, Caparelli, Ernst (2007). Brain Res, 1,132, 158–165.** 4T fMRI 22 N‐back task 3

YA: 30.0

OA: N/A

Yeh, Kuo, Liu (2007). Brain Res, 1,130, 146–157.** 1.5T fMRI 10 Change detection spatial working memory task 1

YA: 23.5 (21–25)

OA: N/A

O'Hare, Lu, Houston, Bookheimer, Sowell (2008). NeuroImage, 42, 1,678–1,685.** 3T fMRI 8 Verbal working memory with varying loads 4

YA: 24.0 (20–28)

OA: N/A

Koelsch, Schulze, Sammler, Fritz, Müller, Gruber (2009). Hum Brain Mapp, 30, 859–873.** 3T fMRI 12 Tonal and verbal working memory 1

YA: 26.7 (25–30)

OA: N/A

Durisko and Fiez (2010). Cortex, 46, 896–906.** 3T fMRI 19 Delayed serial recall task 7

YA: 23.0 (19–33)

OA: N/A

Schulze, Zysset, Mueller, Friederici, Koelsch (2011). Hum Brain Mapp, 32, 771–783.** 3T fMRI 17 Verbal and tonal working memory 5

YA: 25.47 (21–29)

OA: N/A

Kirschen, Chen, Desmond (2010). Behav Neurol, 23, 51–63.** 3T fMRI 16 Load dependent verbal working memory with visual and aural stimuli 10

YA: 21.7

OA: N/A

Piefke, Onur, Fink (2012). Neurobiol Aging, 33, 1,284–1,297. 1.5T fMRI 15 14 N‐back and delayed match to sample tasks 2 2

YA: 23.6

OA: 65.1

Huang, Lee, Hsiao, Kuan, Wai, Ko, Wan, Hsu, Liu (2010). J Neurosci Methods, 189, 257–266. 1.5T fMRI 12 N‐back task 1

YA: N/A

OA: 65.0 (60–74)

Dreher, Koch, Kohn, Apud, Weinberger, Berman (2012). Biol Psychiatry, 71, 890–897. PET 19 17 Spatial n‐back task 2 0

YA: 27.5 (20–36)

OA: 67.5 (54–79)

Note: Notably, while many of the included studies had both YA and OA, in some instances, OA data came from clinical studies wherein the OA served as a control group. Furthermore, additional YA data came from studies included in prior meta‐analyses. “—” denotes studies where a particular age group was not included and as such no coordinates are possible. Cases where there were no cerebellar coordinates are indicated by a 0 in the appropriate foci column. N/A: not applicable.

*

Studies included as part of Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009) and E et al. (2014).

**

Studies only included in E et al. (2014).

TABLE 5.

Included studies of long‐term memory

Study Imaging modality N, YA N, OA Task # YA foci # OA foci Age: Mean (range)
Long‐term memory
Vidal‐Piñeir, Martin‐Trias, Arenaza‐Urquijo, Sala‐Llonch, Clemente, Mena‐Sánchez, Bargalló, Falcón, Pascual‐Leone, Bartrés‐Faz (2014). Brain Stim, 7, 287–296. 3T fMRI 24 Memory for deep and shallow encoding 3

YA: N/A

OA: 71.75 (61–80)

Beason‐Held, Golski, Kraut, Esposito, Resnick (2005). Neurobiol Aging, 26, 237–250. PET 11 Verbal and figural encoding and recognition 10

YA: N/A

OA: 71.1 (63–82)

Peira, Ziaei, Persson (2016). NeuroImage, 125, 745–755. 3T fMRI 15 15 Prospective memory 0 2

YA: 22.4 (20–26)

OA: 68.1 (64–74)

Cohen, Rissman, Suthana, Castel, Knowlton (2016). NeuroImage, 125, 1,046–1,062. 3T fMRI 23 Memory for words with high and low value 3

YA: N/A

OA: 68.7 (60–80)

Gong, Fu, Wang, Franz, Long (2014). Int'l J Aging Hum Dev, 79, 23–54. 3T fMRI 12 Emotional autobiographical memory retrieval 8

YA: N/A

OA: 66.3 (60–74)

Miotto, Balardin, Savege, Martin, Batistuzzo, Amaro, Nitrini (2014). Arq Neuropsiquiatr, 72, 663–670. 3T fMRI 17 Memory for semantically related and unrelated words 1

YA: N/A

OA: 68.12

Brassen, Büchel, Weber‐Fahr, Lehmbek, Sommer, Braus (2009). Neurobiol Aging, 30, 1,147–1,156. 3T fMRI 14 14 Correct retrieval during verbal episodic memory 0 1

YA: 25.6 (21–33)

OA: 64.9 (60–71)

Bartrés‐Faz, Serra‐Grabulosa, Sun, Solé‐Padullés, Rami, Molineuvo, Bosch, Mercader, Bargalló, Falcón, vendrell, Junqué, D'Esposito (2008). Neurobiol Aging, 29, 1,644–1,653. 1.5T fMRI 20 Encoding of face‐name pairs 1

YA: N/A

OA: 66.0

Maguire and Frith (2003). Brain, 126, 1,511–1,523. 2T fMRI 12 12 Autobiographical memory 4 4

YA: 32.42 (23–39)

OA:

Gronholm, Rinne, Vorobyev, Laine (2005). Cog Brain Res, 25, 359–371. PET 10 Memory for learned objects 6

YA: N/A

OA: 74.75 (67.80)

Milton, Butler, Benattayallah, Zeman (2012). Neuropsychologia, 50, 3,528–3,541. 1.5T fMRI 17 Long‐term autobiographical memory 6

YA: N/A

OA: >50

Lam, Wächter, Globas, Karnath, Luft (2013). Hum Brain Mapp, 34, 176–185. 3T fMRI 10 Weather prediction task 2

YA: N/A

OA: 64.6 (43–85)

Antonova. Parslow, Brammer, Dawson, Jackson, Morris (2009). Memory, 17, 125–143. 1.5T fMRI 10 10 Long‐term memory of spatial information 4 4

YA: 23.6 (20–26)

OA: 72.14 (64–79)

Dannhauser, Shergill, Stevens, Lee, Seal, Walker, Walker (2008). Cortex, 44, 869–880. 1.5T fMRI 10 Verbal episodic memory 1

YA: N/A

OA: 68 (50–84)

Bowman and Dennis (2015). Brain Res, 1,612, 2–15. 3T fMRI 17 22 Remember/know judgments of long‐term memory 0 1

YA: 21.28 (18–25)

OA: 74.18 (67–83)

Kircher, Weis, Leube, Freymann, Erb, Jessen, Grodd, Heun, Krach (2008). Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 258, 363–372. 1.5T fMRI 29 Subsequent memory effect 1

YA: N/A

OA: 67.7 (60–81)

Daselaar, Veltman, Rombouts, Raaijmakers, Jonker (2003). Brain, 126, 43–56. 1.5T fMRI 17 19 Correct rejection or recognition at retrieval 3 2

YA: 32.7 (30–35)

OA: 66.4 (63–71)

Haist, Gore, Mao (2001). Nature Neurosci, 4, 1,139–1,145. 1.5T fMRI 8 Remote memory for famous faces 2

YA: N/A

OA: 64.6 (60–70)

Iidaka, Sadato, Yamada, Murata, Omori, Yonekura (2001). Cog Brain Res, 11, 1–11. 1.5T fMRI 7 7 Pictorial information, abstract object encoding 0 1

YA: 25.7

OA: 66.2

Madden, Turkington, Provenzale, Denny, Hawk, Gottlob, Coleman (1999). Hum Brain Map, 7, 115–135. PET 12 12 Recognition memory task 1 2

YA: 23.17 (20–29)

OA: 71.0 (62–79)

Gao, Cheung, Chan, Chu, Mak, Lee (2014). PLoS ONE, 9, e90307. 3T fMRI 13 13 Prospective memory 1 0

YA: 27.1

OA: 76.2

Grady, McIntosh, Horwitz, Maisog, Ungerleider, Mentis, Pietrini, Schapiro, Haxby (1995). Science, 269, 218–221. PET 10 10 Recognition compared to matching in long‐term memory 3 0

YA: 25.2

OA: 69.4

Zamboni, de Jager, Drazich, Douaud, Jenkinson, Smith, Tracey, Wilcock (2013). Neurobiol Aging, 34, 961–972. 3T fMRI 28 Paired associates task 1

YA: N/A

OA: 74.4 (64–91)

Braskie, Small, Bookheimer (2009). Hum Brain Map, 30, 3,981–3,992. 3T fMRI 32 Long term memory of word lists at retrieval 2

YA: N/A

OA: 60.0 (42–77)

Düzel, Scütze, Yonelinas, Heinze (2011). Hippocampus, 21, 803–814. 1.5T fMRI 24 56 Incidental encoding task, activation at recollection 4 0

YA: 23.0

OA: 65.0

Note: Notably, while many of the included studies had both YA and OA, in some instances OA data came from clinical studies wherein the OA served as a control group. “—” denotes studies where a particular age group was not included and as such no coordinates are possible. Cases where there were no cerebellar coordinates are indicated by a 0 in the appropriate foci column. N/A: not applicable.

Articles that focused on structural or morphometric analyses, region of interest analysis, and functional connectivity, as well as those that did not report coordinates in the cerebellum, did not report coordinates in standard spaces (Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] or Talairach), and did not have independent groups contrast analysis were excluded. With respect to this last point, this unfortunately meant that investigations taking a lifespan approach and looking at age effects across adulthood using regression models were not included in this analysis. This is consistent with the exclusion criteria used in recent meta‐analyses from our group, and others (Bernard & Mittal, 2015; Bernard, Russell, Newberry, Goen, & Mittal, 2017; Bernard & Seidler, 2013a; E et al., 2014; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009). After completion of this search and exclusion of papers based on the aforementioned exclusion criteria, we were left with a very small sample of studies (42 studies) and foci on which to complete our analyses (see Figure 1). However, this was limited, at least in part, to the inclusion of the “cerebell*” term in our initial search, as this term may not be in the keywords or abstracts of papers indexed in PubMed. As such, we completed a second search using the terms “aging AND brain imaging” with the same limits as above, which returned 5,982 results as of August 6, 2018. All inclusion/exclusion criteria were identical those for search 1. This second search yielded an additional 73 studies for inclusion. Sixty additional studies were added based on those included in prior meta‐analyses (see more below), for a total of 175 studies.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Flowchart describing the two search processes. In both cases, an initial overview of papers was conducted to eliminate initial obvious exclusions. Secondary screening was conducted while foci were pulled from papers for analysis, though additional exclusions occurred at this stage as well. A full list of included papers can be found in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, organized by task domain. Hundred and fifteen studies were included based off our two literature searches. However, additional studies from prior meta‐analyses of cerebellar function were also added to our sample, bringing the total number of included studies to 175

As cerebellar engagement in both motor and cognitive tasks was of interest, we included studies in the following task domains: motor function, working memory, language, and “other cognitive tasks.” Notably, this last category primarily included executive function tasks (such as the Stroop task, tower of London task, etc.), though several tasks assessing spatial processing were also included here. Categorical determination was made to be consistent with the task domains used by both E et al. (2014) and Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009), with the exception of long‐term memory as it had not been previously included in past meta‐analyses. Tasks included in this category included a memory component with a delay in recall, typically on the order of several minutes. These task domains were chosen for several key reasons. First, we aimed to parallel prior meta‐analyses looking at cerebellar function to compare the functional topography in OA to what is known about this topography in YA. Second, these are domains where there are known age differences in performance and as such are of great interest in the study of motor and cognitive aging. Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 include a complete listing of the studies included in our meta‐analysis divided by task domain, along with the average age and/or age range of participants when available. This also provides information about the brain imaging modality (PET or fMRI), scanner field strength where applicable, and the number of foci from a given study for each age group.

To complete our analyses of age differences, we used the YA control samples from the OA literature, as opposed to doing an additional search focused on YA alone. To date, there have been several meta‐analyses investigating task activation patterns in both the motor and cognitive domains in healthy YA (Bernard & Seidler, 2013a; E et al., 2014; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009), and such an analysis would be redundant, and is beyond the scope of the present investigation. Furthermore, we were concerned about considerable differences in the sample sizes that would potentially bias our group comparison analyses, as the YA literature is substantially larger. However, because many of the studies in our OA sample included OA that served as controls for an age‐related disease group, we had a limited sample of YA studies. To better equate our groups with respect to the number of studies and foci, we included all the studies and foci from prior meta‐analyses investigating cerebellar functional activation (E et al., 2014; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009). In defining the tasks of interest in this analysis, we paralleled those used in this prior work, with two exceptions. We included a long‐term memory category, and we did not investigate emotion tasks. Additional motor foci were extracted from the studies included by Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009), while those for language, working memory, and cognitive function (categorized as executive function by E et al.) were taken from E et al. (2014). Notably, E et al. (2014) also had substantial overlap with Stoodley and Schmahmann (2009) as they included all of the papers from the prior analysis, as well as new additions to the literature. Notably, we did not gather additional YA papers to specifically match the OA papers for two reasons. First, though we could match based on task type, we were concerned that this could introduce selection bias. For many tasks, there are multiple papers that have similar sample sizes as existing OA studies and additional inclusion criteria could have been biased. Second, though additional YA papers may have cerebellar foci, there would be no guarantee that the number of foci would be matched across studies even when taking this approach.

The literature searches and initial inclusion decisions were completed by T. M., A. D. N., Y. L., J. R. M. G., H. K. B., and H. K. H. Inclusion was confirmed and coordinates for each study were checked, prior to analysis, by J. A. B. After scanning the literature and the inclusion of the studies from both Stoodley & Schamhmann (2009) and E et al. (2014), we had 175 studies, including data from 1,710 YA (403 foci) and 2,160 OA (307 foci) individuals, concatenated across all task domains. Figure 1 provides a flowchart demonstrating our article screening procedure and exclusion regions, broadly defined. The initial search, and secondary broader aging search are presented separately.

2.2. Activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis

All analyses were completed using BrainMap GingerALE 3.0.2 (http://brainmap.org; Eickhoff, Bzdok, Laird, Kurth, & Fox, 2012; Eickhoff et al., 2009; Turkeltaub et al., 2012). Activation likelihood estimation (ALE) allows us to combine across studies, sites, scanning modalities, and study designs to investigate overlap in activation patterns, and the algorithm includes a metrics to account for variability in subjects and testing sites (Eickhoff et al., 2012). Unlike behavioral meta‐analyses, because the algorithm looks at activation foci, and models these to account for uncertainty, the variability in design and analysis approaches can be reasonably accounted for. Foci were first organized for analysis by task domain; however, we also performed additional analyses concatenating across all cognitive task domains. As there are two standard atlas spaces used for normalization and presentation of activation (MNI or Talairach), it is critical to ensure that all foci are in the same atlas space prior to analysis for the purpose of comparison across studies. As such, all foci in Talairach space were converted to MNI space prior to analysis. For studies where data were normalized directly to Talairach space, and those that specified the use of the Lancaster transform (icbm2tal; Lancaster et al., 2007), we used this transform to move them to MNI space. This transform was also used for studies published after the icbm2tal transform became available, and for which no specific transform information was provided. For studies where the Brett transform (mni2tal) was used to bring data from MNI space to Talairach space, and for articles published prior to 2007 without any transform details, we used the inverse Brett transform to bring the data into MNI space. All transforms were completed using tools available in GingerALE.

Once in MNI space, all activation foci were organized into text files for analysis with GingerALE. The ALE algorithm computes ALE values for all of the voxels in the brain, producing an estimation of the likelihood that a particular voxel is active under particular task conditions (Eickhoff et al., 2009). During analysis, GingerALE uses a full‐width half‐maximum (FWHM) Gaussian blur on each set of foci, with the size based off of the sample size used to generate each set of foci (Eickhoff et al., 2009). Output of our analyses indicated that the FWHM blur ranged from 8.46 to 11.37 mm, across all analyses. In completing our analyses, we used the smaller more conservative mask option available in GingerALE, in conjunction with the non‐additive ALE method (Turkeltaub et al., 2012). For within group analyses, all ALE maps were thresholded using a cluster‐level family‐wise error p < .001 with 5,000 threshold permutations and a p‐value of p < .001. Group contrasts and conjunctions were evaluated using an uncorrected p < .05 with 10,000 p‐value permutations, and a minimum cluster size of 50 mm3. This approach is consistent with our prior meta‐analyses (Bernard & Mittal, 2015; Bernard et al., 2017), as well as other recent work (e.g., Stawarczyk & D'Argembeau, 2015), and allows us to look at contrasts, even though GingerALE is not very robust when small numbers of studies (fewer than 15 per group) are used for group contrasts. The resulting areas of convergence from all analyses were localized using the Spatially Unbiased Infratentorial Template (SUIT) atlas (Diedrichsen, Balsters, Flavell, Cussans, & Ramnani, 2009). Foci located in the white matter in the area of the cerebellar nuclei were localized using an atlas of cerebellar nuclei (Dimitrova et al., 2002).

3. RESULTS

3.1. Within group activation convergence across studies

Because several meta‐analyses have already been conducted investigating cerebellar activation across task domains in YA (Bernard & Seidler, 2013a; E et al., 2014; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009), we provide only a brief overview of the YA results. Details of the areas of activation overlap across studies for both age groups and each task domain are provided in Table 6 and presented visually in Figure 2. In YA, the motor and working memory analyses replicated prior meta‐analyses investigating patterns of cerebellar functional activation (Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009; E et al., 2014; Bernard & Seidler, 2013a), though notably, there was substantial overlap in the foci used for the analyses. Activation overlap across language tasks was also consistent with prior work with a large cluster extending across Crus I and Lobule VI, while that for other cognitive tasks, which primarily included executive function tasks, also paralleled prior work (Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009; E et al., 2014). Notably, this area is also consistent with recent work mapping function in the cerebellum by King et al. (2019), where tasks similar to those categorized here showed activation in lateral posterior cerebellum. Finally, we extended prior meta‐analyses with the inclusion of long‐term memory. In YA, our results demonstrate activation overlap across tasks in Lobule VI and Crus I. The overlap in Crus I is consistent with Crus I activation seen with autobiographical recall by King et al. (2019).

TABLE 6.

Activation by group and task

Cluster Cluster size (mm3) Extent & weighted center (x, y, z) ALE peaks (x, y, z) Location ALE value (×10−3) Z‐value
Motor
YA
1 22,784

From (−40, −72, −58) to (36, −38, −4) centered at (6.5, −57, −26.4)

16, −54, −24 Lobule V 53.22 10.30
−20, −60, −20 Lobule VI 30.81 7.34
30, −58, −26 Lobule VI 28.10 6.94
4, −56, −12 Lobules I–IV 20.31 5.69
6, −66, −14 Lobule V 16.94 5.07
6, −66, −34 Vermis VIIIa 16.56 5.01
−34, −56, −30 Lobule VI 15.60 4.84
12, −68, −50 Lobule VIIIa 15.32 4.79
8, −66, −42 Vermis VIIIa 13.55 4.43
−6, −66, −28 Vermis VI* 13.25 4.36
28, −56, −52 Lobule VIIIa 13.21 4.35
−26, −42, −32 Lobule V 10.74 3.82
−30, −72, −20 Lobule VI 8.35 3.35
OA
1 18,504 From (−14, −86, −38) to (44, −36, 0) centered at (18.6, −55.4, −21.7) 26, −54, −24 Lobule VI 62.19 10.54
12, −54, −16 Lobule V 36.45 7.54
38, −50, −32 Crus I 30.67 6.76
−2, −66, −14 Lobule V 24.67 5.89
−4, −76, −20 Vermis VI 15.63 4.42
−8, −82, −18 Lobule VI* 14.69 4.25
26, −76, −24 Crus I 14.16 4.15
2 8,840 From (−50, −68, −36) to (−12, −40, −14) centered at (−27.4, −56, −25.8) −26, −52, −26 Lobule VI 40.63 8.07
−22, −58, −22 Lobule VI 36.27 7.52
−28, −62, −26 Lobule VI 35.51 7.42
−46, −60, −26 Crus I 15.93 4.48
3 1952 From (16, −66, −56) to (30, −50, −44) centered at (23, −57.7, −50.1) 24, −56, −50 Lobule VIIIb 22.8 5.62
4 1,360 From (−8, −76, −46) to (14, −60, −30) centered at (3, −68.4, −39.3) −2, −72, −42 Vermis VIIIa 17.29 4.71
10, −64, −40 Lobule VIIIa 14.05 4.13
6, −70, −32 Vermis VIIb 11.53 3.62
Language
YA
1 7,304 From (20, −78, −46) to (52, −48, −14) centered at (36.6, −63.4, −30.7) 36, −54, −36 Crus I 18.04 5.49
34, −74, −18 Crus I* 17.07 5.32
44, −66, −32 Crus I 14.31 4.77
46, −62, −40 Crus I 12.58 4.41
28, −62, −26 Lobule VI 12.43 4.37
22, −72, −26 Lobule VI 9.69 3.82
OA
N/A
Long‐term memory
YA
1 1984 From (−18, −88, −22) to (−6, −70, −10) centered at (−11.8, −81.1, −16) −12, −82, −16 Lobule VI* 18.66 6.17
−14, −72, −12 Lobule VI* 8.53 4.02
2 1,216 From (8, −80, −26) to (20, −68, −16) centered at (14.4, −73.7, −20.5) 14, −74, −20 Lobule VI 17.06 5.89
3 984 From (16, −90, −42) to (26, −80, −34) centered at (21, −85.5, −38.1) 20, −86, −38 Crus II 13.19 5.01
OA
1 5,640 From (−2, −88, −44) to (48, −48, −16) centered at (21.6, −74.5, −31.2) 24, −84, −38 Crus II 15.66 4.96
4, −72, −32 Vermis crus II 14.01 4.63
10, −84, −38 Crus II 13.81 4.58
38, −58, −20 Lobule VI* 13.35 4.48
30, −86, −28 Crus I 12.89 4.38
32, −50, −20 Lobule VI* 10.73 3.95
36, −76, −24 Crus I 9.49 3.71
46, −72, −20 Crus I* 9.21 3.65
2 2,368 From (−54, −82, −32) to (−28, −56, −20) centered at (−44, −68.7, −25.6) −50, −60, −26 Crus I 17.83 5.34
−40, −76, −24 Crus I 14.56 4.74
−28, −80, −30 Crus I 7.50 3.15
3 1976 From (−6, −58, −54) to (16, −48, −20) centered at (7.1, −52.4, −37.5) 6, −52, −46 Lobule IX 14.71 4.77
12, −54, −24 Lobules I–IV 13.59 4.53
−4, −54, −42 Lobule IX 7.78 3.29
Working memory
YA
1 29,840 From (−44, −88, −52) to (50, −46, −10) centered at (7.9, −66.3, −29.8) 28, −66, −34 Crus I 37.51 7.55
−28, −62, −30 Lobule VI 35.33 7.27
38, −64, −36 Crus I 34.41 7.15
8, −76, −24 Lobule VI 34.34 7.14
−38, −58, −40 Crus I 28.42 6.33
36, −54, −42 Crus II 26.75 6.09
−36, −70, −20 Lobule VI* 23.71 5.65
−10, −76, −22 Lobule VI 21.00 5.22
−2, −82, −14 N/A 18.12 4.75
−18, −64, −14 Lobule VI 17.99 4.73
−16, −52, −20 Lobule V 16.18 4.44
14, −54, −36 Lobule IX 15.26 4.78
−6, −48, −14 Lobules I–IV 11.35 3.51
OA
1 3,744 From (−46, −74, −36) to (−28, −46, −14) centered at (−36.4, −59.2, −24.5) −36, −58, −26 Lobule VI 24.12 5.88
−38, −66, −18 N/A 15.67 4.53
2 3,440 From (−10, −82, −32) to (14, −64, −16) centered at (1.3, −74.9, −23.4) −4, −76, −24 Vermis VI 34.86 7.37
8, −76, −24 Lobule VI 31.01 6.86
3 3,216 From (20, −80, −40) to (36, −52, −16) centered at (28.1, −64.8, −25.4) 26, −66, −24 Lobule VI 22.95 5.72
32, −58, −24 Lobule VI 17.42 4.84
Other cognitive tasks (executive function/attention)
YA
1 2,752 From (−44, −78, −52) to (8, −52, −22) centered at (−28.5, −65.4, −33.) −28, −64, −36 Crus I 13.11 4.48
−36, −60, −32 Dentate nucleus 10.96 4.06
−12, −74, −32 Crus I 10.00 3.88
−42, −76, −24 Crus I 9.14 3.72
−40, −70, −26 Crus I 9.11 3.71
−24, −66, −50 Lobule VIIIa 8.71 3.64
2 1,096 From (10, −58, −30) to (32, −48, −22) centered at (24.1, −52, −26) 28, −52, −26 Lobule VI 13.63 4.60
14, −50, −26 Interposed nuclei 9.89 3.86
OA
1 1832 From (−46, −76, −42) to (−32, −60, −22) centered at (−37.1, −66.8, −30.6) −36, −70, −26 Crus I 15.34 5.00
−38, −64, −36 Crus I 13.70 4.64
2 1824 From (−8, −82, −30) to (6, −70, −18) centered at (−.9, −75.4, −24.6) 0, −76, −24 Vermis VI 24.22 6.56
3 1,304 From (−24, −68, −22) to (−12, −56, −12) centered at (−17.9, −61.6, −16.7) −18, −62, −16 Lobule V 22.66 6.30
*

Peak outside of SUIT Atlas space, the closest region to the reported peak is listed.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Activation overlap in the cerebellum across studies for each task domain in YA (blue) and OA (red). Areas of overlap are overlaid onto the SUIT cerebellum template. Notably, there were no significant areas of overlap across studies in OA for language tasks. The color differentials, which are particularly noticeable for the red OA clusters is to help distinguish clusters, and do not convey information with respect to ALE values

In OA, across studies motor task activation largely paralleled YA in the regions where we observed activation overlap. That is, activation was localized largely to the anterior cerebellum in Lobules V and VI, along with the secondary motor representation in Lobules VIIIa and VIIIb. Working memory activation convergence was limited to Lobule VI and Vermis VI; however, unlike in YA, we did not see any convergence across studies in Crus I and II. With respect to long‐term memory, convergence across studies in OA appears to be more extensive than in YA, extending from Lobule VI to Crus II and also including Lobule IX. When looking at other cognitive tasks which were primarily those that tapped into executive functions, broadly defined, OA demonstrated significant overlap across studies in Crus I, Vermis VI, and Lobule V. Finally, for language tasks, there was no significant convergence across studies in OA.

3.2. Age differences in cerebellar activation overlap

Group differences in activation convergence across studies for all task domains, except for language, were computed (Figure 3, Table 7). Due to the nature of the ALE algorithm, comparisons cannot be made when one group does not show any significant activation across tasks. As such, we were unable to analyze language. With that said, it is worth noting that in YA, there was significant convergence across language tasks but this was not the case at all in OA, suggesting less reliable activation across studies in advanced age, perhaps due to less activation overall.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Overlap between age groups as well as age differences across studies in cerebellar activation. Because for several cognitive task domains there were not enough foci to compare the two age groups, or because there was no significant overlap within an age group, all of the cognitive task domains were combined and investigated together as well. The color differentials are to help distinguish clusters and do note convey information with respect to ALE values. Purple: overlap between age groups across tasks. Blue: YA > OA. Red: OA > YA

TABLE 7.

Group comparisons by task

Cluster Cluster size (mm3) Extent & weighted center (x, y, z) ALE peaks (x, y, z) Location ALE value (×10−3) Z‐value
Motor
Group overlap
1 9,008 From (−8, −72, −34) to (36, −38, −4) centered at (17.8, −54.5, −22.6) 18, −52, −24 Lobule V 44.23
30, −58, −26 Lobule VI 28.10
6, −54, −12 Lobules I–IV 19.32
2, −66, −14 Lobule V 14.87
2 4,192 From (−40, −68, −36) to (−14, −44, −16) centered at (−25.1, −57, −25) −20, −60, −22 Lobule VI 30.70
−34, −56, −30 Lobule VI 15.56
−26, −44, −30 Lobule V 9.04
3 728 From (16, −66, −56) to (30, −52, −48) centered at (24.3, −58, −52.1) 28, −56, −30 Lobule VI 13.21
16, −64, −48 Lobule VIIIa 9.26
4 600 From (2, −72, −44) to (14, −60, −30) centered at (8, −66, −38.1) 10, −64, −42 Lobule VIIIa 12.98
6, −68, −32 Lobule VIIIb 11.11
YA > OA
1 1,696 From (8, −64, −36) to (20, −50, −20) centered at (14.7, −56.7, −28.7) 13.3, −58, −30 Dentate nucleus 3.89
2 360 From (4, −68, −22) to (12, −62, −8) centered at (8.6, −65.2, −14.2) 6, −62, −8 Lobule V 2.08
12, −66, −18 Lobule VI 2.05
8, −66, −10 Lobule V 2.01
12, −64, −22 Lobule VI 1.92
3 88 From (−8, −72, −34) to (36, −38, −4) centered at (17.8, −54.5, −22.6) −4, −64, −30 Vermis VIIIa 1.86
OA > YA
1 3,048 From (22, −56, −38) to (44, −42, −16) centered at (33.5, −48.4, −27.6) 33.5, −44.5, −26.5 Lobule VI 3.54
29, −46, −20 Lobule VI 3.23
2 880 From (−12, −86, −24) to (2, −72, −14) centered at (−5.6, −79.3, −18.1) −8, −86, −16 2.64
−8, −78, −16 Lobule VI 2.34
3 624 From (−32, −52, −30) to (−18, −42, −14) centered at (−25.9, −47, −22.1) −30, −46, −22 Lobule VI 2.36
−24, −44, −18 Lobule V 2.15
4 376 From (10, −54, −16) to (20, −46, −10) centered at (14, −50.3, −13) 14, −48, −12 Lobule V 2.15
5 312 From (−34, −68, −32) to (−26, −62, −24) centered at (−29.6, −65.1, −28.2) −30, −64, −28 Lobule VI 1.99
All cognitive tasks combined
Group overlap
1 14,616 From (−24, −88, −42) to (44, −50, −14) centered at (15.7, −70.3, −26.3) 6, −76, −24 Vermis VI 42.26
−6, −80, −24 Lobule VI 29.26
26, −66, −24 Lobule VI 27.19
24, −64, −36 Lobule VI 26.05
32, −56, −26 Lobule VI 21.67
−20, −64, −22 Lobule VI 17.08
−2, −68, −24 Vermis VI 16.75
24, −84, −38 Crus II 16.10
10, −84, −36 Crus II 15.93
38, −76, −22 Crus I 15.67
−14, −66, −22 Lobule VI 14.49
−20, −66, −14 Lobule VI 13.21
2 5,976 From (−44, −78, −38) to (−20, −44, −14) centered at (−36.6, −63.9, −27.1) −40, −68, −24 Crus I 26.46
−36, −58, −28 Lobule VI 26.24
−22, −64, −36 Dentate 14.83
−36, −46, −30 Lobule VI 13.62
3 136 From (12, −58, −28) to (18, −50, −24) centered at (14.8, −53.8, −26.1) 16, −54, −26 Lobule V 12.81
4 24 From (−26, −54, −24) to (−26, −50, −24) centered at (−26, −52, −24) −26, −52, −24 Lobule VI 11.65
YA > OA
1 13,984 From (10, −82, −62) to (54, −48, −14) centered at (33.1, −64.1, −38.6) 34, −64.4, −46.5 Lobule VIIb 3.35
51, −58.7, −34.2 Crus I 3.72
28, −54, −42 Dentate 3.54
22, −58, −28 Lobule VI 2.88
14, −76, −16 Lobule VI 2.81
16, −64, −26 Lobule VI 2.53
34, −70, −16 2.40
2 4,552 From (−44, −70, −52) to (−16, −48, −24) centered at (−30, −61.5, −36.8) −28.6, −62.2, −50.3 Lobule IX 3.89
−34, −64, −48 Lobule VIIb 3.54
−23, −58, −29 Lobule VI 3.43
−30, −60, −38 Crus I 2.77
−16, −52, −26 Lobule V 1.82
3 3,208 From (−18, −88, −36) to (2, −74, −10) centered at (−9.5, −81.8,—18.5) −7.7, −82.7, −15 3.54
−6, −86, −14.4 3.72
−10, −85, −20.5 Crus I 3.43
−14, −78, −30 Crus I 2.59
−8, −78, −34 Crus II 2.19
4 712 From (−6, −64, −32) to (6, −56, −22) centered at (0.1, −60.5, −27.5) 0, −60, −28 Vermis VI 2.62
5 144 From (16, −92, −40) to (20, −88, −32) centered at (18.1, −89.8, −35) 16, −92, −36 Crus II 1.899
6 104 From (−36, −78, −52) to (−30, −74, −46) centered at (−32.6, −76.6, −48.2) −32, −78, −52 Crus II 2.65
7 56 From (6, −84, −24) to (10, −84, −20) centered at (7.4, −84, −21.9) 6, −84, −20 1.86
OA > YA
1 752 From (−2, −56, −54) to (8, −48, −40) centered at (3.1, −51.2, −46.6) 0, −50, −50 Lobule IX 2.25
2, −48, −44 Lobule IX 2.21
2 392 From (−6, −76, −30) to (2, −70, −18) centered at (−2.1, −72.6, −24.1) −2, −74, −22 Vermis VI 2.33
3 320 From (−16, −64, −18) to (−8, −58, −12) centered at (−12.1, −61.3, −14.5) −12, −60, −12 Lobule V 2.35
4 296 From (−52, −74, −30) to (−46, −64, −20) centered at (−49, −67.7, −25) −50, −68, −24 Crus I 2.63
5 88 From (−34, −56, −24) to (−32, −52, −18) centered at (−33.1, −53.6, −20.7) −32, −54, −20 Lobule VI 1.92
6 56 From (4, −52, −22) to (8, −48, −20) centered at (6.3, −50.3, −20.8) 6, −50, −20 Lobules I–IV 1.98
Working memory
Group overlap
1 2,880 From (20, −76, −40) to (36, −54, −16) centered at (28.4, −64.7, −25.7) 26, −66, −24 Lobule VI 22.10
32, −58, −24 Lobule VI 12.42
2 2,200 From (−10, −82, −32) to (14, −70, −18) centered at (3.8, −76.5, −24.1) 8, −76, −24 Lobule VI 31.01
−8, −76, −22 Lobule VI 19.20
3 1,392 From (−42, −74, −34) to (−28, −54, −14) centered at (−35.8, −63.8, −24.4) −38, −66, −18 Lobule VI* 15.67
−34, −58, −30 Lobule VI 14.78
4 8 At (−36, −54, −36) −36, −54, −36 Crus I 8.51
YA > OA
1 4,240 From (22, −74, −50) to (50, −46, −28) centered at (38.1, −63.2, −37.2) 46, −60, −35 Crus I 3.89
48, −66, −36 Crus I 3.72
38, −70, −40 Crus I 3.19
26, −74, −36 Crus I 2.41
22, −74, −34 Crus I 2.17
28, −66, −50 Lobule VIIb 2.16
32, −66, −50 Lobule VIIb 2.04
38, −48, −44 Crus II 1.77
2 1816 From (−42, −70, −52) to (−18, −56, −26) centered at (−31.7, −62, −38.1) −34, −66, −42 Crus II 2.55
−30, −62, −50 Lobule VIIIa 2.44
−20, −62, −32 Dentate nucleus/Lobule VI 2.36
−22, −58, −32 Dentate nucleus/Lobule VI 2.30
−34, −64, −48 Lobule VIIb 2.26
−24, −64, −36 Lobule VI 1.92
3 872 From (10, −60, −42) to (22, −50, −26) centered at (15.3, −55.6, −34.2) 14, −50, −34 Dentate nucleus 2.99
12, −56, −42 Lobule IX* 2.16
4 160 From (34, −76, −22) to (40, −72, −18) centered at (37.6, −74, −20.3) 40, −76, −20 Crus I* 1.95
5 144 From (2, −88, −38) to (6, −84, −30) centered at (4.2, −85.8, −34) 2, −86, −32 Crus II* 1.93
6, −88, −36 Crus II 1.81
OA > YA
1 1,328 From (−42, −62, −30) to (−30, −48, −18) centered at (−36.1, −54.6, −23.9) −36.7, −53.3, −20 Lobule VI* 3.35
2 752 From (−8, −78, −28) to (4, −68, −18) centered at (−2.6, −71.7, −22.3) −2, −70, −20 Vermis VI 2.89
Long term memory
Group overlap
1 696 From (16, −88, −42) to (26, −80, −34) centered at (21.4, −84.4, −38.7) 22, −86, −38 Crus II 12.80
YA > OA
1 1928 From (−18, −88, −22) to (−4, −70, −10) centered at (−11.6, −80.3, −15.8) 12, −72, −14 Lobule VI 2.99
−10.7, −76.7, −13.1 Lobule VI* 2.93
−12.3, −79.8, −16.7 Lobule VI* 2.93
−12, −84, −10 2.89
2 1,120 From (8, −80, −26) to (20, −68, −16) centered at (14, −73.5, −20.2) 12, −70, −16 Lobule VI* 2.95
16, −80, −22 Crus I 2.48
3 168 From (18, −, −90, −40) to (20, −84, −32) centered at (19.3, −88.2, −35.8) 20, −88, −34 Crus II 1.83
OA > YA
1 456 From (34, −78, −26) to (46, −60, −18) centered at (40.2, −70.8, −22.6) 41.2, −70, −22.4 Crus I 1.89
38.5, −76, −25 Crus I 1.77
38, −62, −24 Lobule VI 1.77
40, −66, −21 Lobule VI/crus I 1.75
45.2, −73.6, −20 Crus I* 1.72

2

248 From (26, −54, −26) to (34, −48, −20) centered at (29.7, −50.3, −22.3) 28, −49.3, −24.7 Lobule VI 1.93
32, −50.7, −22.7 Lobule VI 1.89
Executive function
Group overlap
1 600 From (−40, −76, −38) to (−32, −60, −24) centered at (−36.8, −65.1, −30.5) −36, −62, −32 Crus I 10.93
−40, −70, −26 Crus I 9.11
−40, −74, −24 Crus I 8.63
YA > OA
1 120 From (−26, −68, −52) to (−24, −64, −44) centered at (−25.1, −66, −48.8) −26, −68, −50 Lobule VIIb 2.52
OA > YA
1 448 From (−24, −68, −20) to (−12, −58, −12) centered at (−18, −64.3, −16.4) −22, −66, −16 Lobule VI 2.27
−14, −68, −20 Lobule VI 1.9
*

Peak outside of SUIT Atlas space, the closest region to the reported peak is listed.

With respect to motor tasks, it is first notable that there was significant overlap between the two age groups in regions of the anterior cerebellum, including Lobules I–IV, V, and VI. YA showed greater convergence across studies in the dentate nucleus and Lobule V, an area heavily involved in motor processing (Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009). In addition, some convergence extended into Lobule VI. In OA, convergence across studies relative to YA was seen primarily in Lobule VI. Notably, while greater convergence across studies in YA was limited primarily to the right hemisphere, in OA this was bilateral.

Working memory tasks also resulted in a great deal of activation overlap across studies when looking at the conjunction of the two age groups. Not surprisingly, this was localized to bilateral Lobule VI and left Crus I. Greater convergence across studies in YA was seen in Crus I and II, the dentate nucleus, Lobule VIIb, and Lobule VIIIa. Greater activation convergence in OA was much more limited and seen only left Lobule VI and Vermis VI. The spatial extent of the overlap unique to OA was just more than one quarter (28.7%) of that which was unique to YA. With respect to long‐term memory, there was some shared convergence across studies in both age groups localized to Crus II. Greater convergence in YA as compared to OA was seen in Lobule VI, Crus I, and Crus II. Similar lobules were observed when looking at areas where OA had greater convergence, but localization within these lobules was unique relative to YA, and again, the spatial extent of the convergence areas that were greater in YA was much larger. In this instance, the area in OA was only 21.9% of that seen in YA. In both of these memory domains, this suggests that across studies in YA, there is more consistent activation across larger aspects of the cerebellum as compared to OA, where convergence was more limited in its spatial extent.

When investigating other cognitive tasks, which primarily includes executive function tasks, convergence in activation across studies was seen in Crus I and Vermis VI. When looking at the two groups relative to one another greater convergence in YA was seen in VIIIb, and greater convergence in OA was seen in Lobule VI. However, in both cases these were relatively small areas. Because there was no significant convergence across language tasks in OA, we were unable to conduct a group comparison.

Finally, we combined all cognitive tasks to compare overlap between YA and OA. This allowed us to include language in a broader analysis of group differences in convergence across tasks. Consistent with the broader literature suggesting that the lateral posterior cerebellum is involved in cognitive task processing and has connections (both structural and functional) with the PFC (Bernard et al., 2012; Chen & Desmond, 2005; Krienen & Buckner, 2009; Salmi, Pallesen, & Neuvonen, 2010; Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009), there was substantial convergence overlap between the age groups in hemispheric Crus I and Lobule VI, though Vermis VI was also implicated along with the cerebellar dentate nucleus. In YA, greater convergence was seen in a wide swath of the posterior cerebellum. This included Lobule VI, Crus I, and Crus II, as well as Lobule VIIb and Lobule IX. In OA relative to YA, there was convergence in Lobule IX, Crus I, and Lobule VI, though there was also an area in Lobule V. Convergence in Lobules I–IV and V was unique to the OA sample. Most notably, the overall volume of the areas of increased overlap was substantially smaller in the OA group (1,634 mm3) as compared to the YA (22,760 mm3).

4. DISCUSSION

Here, using ALE meta‐analysis, we directly compared cerebellar activation convergence across task domains in OA and YA for the first time. Our results indicate that YA and OA recruit cerebellar resources differently during task performance, as evidenced by group differences in areas of activation convergence across studies. However, there is also substantial overlap in the convergence patterns when comparing the two age groups. These findings represent several important practical and theoretical advances in our understanding of cerebellar contributions to behavior in advanced age. First, this expands our understanding of the cerebellum in aging beyond the anatomical and connectivity domains to include functional activation patterns. Second, more broadly, this extends our understanding of the neural underpinnings of task performance in OA to include the cerebellum. Though as this investigation demonstrates, cerebellar activation has long been found in functional imaging studies of aging, but it has not been the focus of study. Concatenation across investigations in this manner provides a powerful tool to better understand cerebellar functional activation patterns in advanced age. Most notably, there is evidence to suggest potential under‐recruitment of the cerebellum in individual cognitive domains, and when all cognitive tasks are investigated together. This is consistent with our predictions based on degraded connectivity and volume in the cerebellum in OA (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). However, the activation convergence patterns differ for motor tasks. There is less convergence in OA in the primary motor regions of the cerebellum relative to YA, but across studies, there is greater convergence in secondary cerebellar motor regions. Together, these results suggest that with advanced age, cerebellar resources are not relied upon as effectively and efficiently in OA during task performance.

Unlike in the cerebral cortex where an increase in bilateral activation and compensatory recruitment during cognitive task performance in OA has been reported (Cabeza, 2002; Cappell, Gmeindl, & Reuter‐Lorenz, 2010; Reuter‐Lorenz et al., 1999), here we demonstrate a relative decrease in convergence across studies investigating cognitive task domains. Though this does not directly indicate activation, it does imply that the organization of activation across studies is not consistent with cortical bilateral patterns of activation. We suggest that OA may not be consistently engaging bilateral regions of the cerebellum during cognitive task performance as they do in the cortex. Alternatively, it may also be the case that there is more variability in the cerebellar resources that are recruited in OA. This would also result in less convergence across studies, and with this methodology, we cannot fully dissociate these two possibilities. Though surprising in the context of the cortical literature, this is consistent with the hypothesis put forth in our recent review (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). Because connectivity is lower in OA relative to YA, information exchange between the cortex and cerebellum may be degraded. As such, OA may not be able to effectively recruit cerebellar resources for information processing (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). The results here are consistent with this idea, and we suggest that this difference in the recruitment of the cerebellum may be particularly important for behavior. Specifically, cerebellar resources may be especially important scaffolding for performance in advanced age (e.g., Reuter‐Lorenz & Park, 2014). Reliance upon more automatic processing in the cerebellum via internal models of behavior (Ramnani, 2014) would free up cortical resources and help maintain performance. However, those resources are not recruited consistently in OA as evidenced by the activation convergences patterns across studies seen here. Furthermore, it may in fact be the case that the inability to utilize these cerebellar resources contributes, at least in part, to the bilateral cortical activation patterns seen in OA.

Somewhat surprisingly, we found a distinct pattern of convergence differences for motor tasks. OA showed significantly more convergence in Lobule VI compared to YA, and these clusters extend into Crus I, as seen in Figure 3. This region has connectivity patterns with prefrontal cortical and premotor regions (e.g., Bernard et al., 2012; Krienen & Buckner, 2009) and also shows activation during cognitive task performance (Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009), while Crus I has connectivity patterns associated with the lateral PFC (e.g., Bernard et al., 2012; Krienen & Buckner, 2009) and is engaged across a variety of cognitive task domains (Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009; King et al., 2019). This convergence pattern is more consistent with activation patterns seen in the cortex in OA (e.g., Cabeza, 2002; Cappell et al., 2010; Reuter‐Lorenz et al., 1999). This is also consistent with work in OA demonstrating increased recruitment of frontal cortical regions during motor task performance (e.g., Heuninckx et al., 2008; reviewed in Seidler et al., 2010). It is likely that this pattern in the cerebellum is paralleling what has been reported in the cerebral cortex to an extent. Again, though not directly indexing activation, seeing a consistency in this pattern across studies suggests that perhaps OA are engaging these other cerebellar regions in a compensatory manner. Notably, recent work using a predictive motor timing task demonstrated increased activation in the lateral posterior cerebellum with increasing age (Filip et al., 2019). The authors suggest that this activation pattern may provide scaffolding for performance in OA (Filip et al., 2019), consistent with extant models of aging (e.g., Reuter‐Lorenz and Park, 2014). However, given that we did not see this pattern for cognitive tasks, why such scaffolding is present for motor tasks raises interesting mechanistic and theoretical questions.

As described above, in our past work, we had hypothesized that lower connectivity between the cerebellum and cortex coupled with volumetric differences in OA relative to YA would result in decreased activation during task performance, indicative of an inability to rely upon cerebellar resources for performance (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). Data from studies administering cognitive tasks are consistent with this notion, while data from motor tasks better parallel cortical findings (e.g., Reuter‐Lorenz & Cappell, 2008) and are consistent with recent work suggesting that the cerebellum provides scaffolding for motor performance in advanced age (Filip et al., 2019). Though seemingly contradictory, these findings may in fact be quite consistent with inputs to the cerebellum, particularly in the context of control theory (Ramnani, 2006). In this context, and as we previously proposed (Bernard & Seidler 2014), inputs and outputs between the cerebellum and cortex are degraded in advanced age, resulting in less efficient internal models, evidenced through decreased functional activation. However, a key part of these models is the function of the comparator, which compares the predicted behavior to its consequences (Ramnani, 2006). The inferior olive is suggested to be the comparator for both motor and cognitive processes; however, the input to this region differs. For cognitive processes, input to the inferior olive comes from cortico‐olivo‐cerebellar pathways, while those for motor come from the spino‐olivo‐cerebellar pathways (Ramnani, 2006). We speculate that the spinal pathways are relatively intact, particularly as compared to the cortical pathways, and as such, OA are better able to recruit cerebellar resources during motor task performance. Thus, in the context of control theory, while the cerebellum may be capable of providing compensatory activation, because of the input from the cortex for the updating of internal models, these resources cannot be brought online effectively. Together, our findings suggest that in OA cerebellar resources may be under‐recruited during cognitive tasks as evidenced by the relative decrease in convergence across studies when compared with YAs, and we propose that compensatory scaffolding during motor performance is due to spino‐olivo‐cerebellar inputs.

While the meta‐analytic approach employed here allows for insights into cerebellar activation patterns across studies, it is not without limitations. Most notably, we were unable to account for behavioral performance and brain‐behavior relationships. While an understanding of brain‐behavior relationships is a key question moving forward, the inclusion of a behavioral meta‐analysis here is beyond the scope of our work. Furthermore, there are numerous existing behavioral meta‐analyses across domains demonstrating age differences in performance (e.g., Maldonado, Orr, Goen, & Bernard, 2020; Verhaeghen and Cerella, 2002; Wasylyshyn et al., 2011), negating the need for an additional behavioral meta‐analysis here. Furthermore, in some studies, it is possible that there were no cerebellar foci reported due to incomplete coverage of the structure. This also means that activation in some cerebellar regions, particularly the most inferior lobules of the cerebellum, may be more generally under reported. Due to cortically focused hypotheses and scanning limitations, parts of the cerebellum may not have been covered by the field of view. While the more anterior lobules related to motor function, and lateral posterior regions of Crus I and II typically have good coverage, without the original scan data we cannot know for sure. As such, this may have influenced our analyses. Notably, however, this would have a similar impact on YA and OA and should not impact the age differences reported here. Relatedly, some clusters appear in our figures as being outside of the cerebellum. This is likely due to normalization procedures that smoothed the initial data into inferior cortical regions, as processes were optimized for whole‐brain analysis. This also is likely due in part to the estimation around the included foci based on sample size. Additionally, we did not conduct a complete search of the entire YA imaging literature, and our search only goes through August 2018. This would have resulted in large differences in statistical power between groups and potentially biased results in favor of the YA sample. As such, this is not a comprehensive investigation of cerebellar activation patterns in YA, but several meta‐analyses on this topic have already been conducted (Stoodley & Schmahamann, 2009; E et al., 2014). Furthermore, it is notable that even with less power in the YA data sample, we nonetheless see less overlap across studies in OA during cognitive task performance. If anything, this sample was biased in favor of seeing more consistent activation overlap in OA given the size of the sample; however, for cognitive task processing, the opposite pattern was demonstrated, providing powerful evidence for differences in cerebellar engagement with age. With respect to the search cut‐off, this has resulted in the most recent studies not being included here. However, with the timeline of analysis, review, and eventual publication, with this type of work, there will always be a delay. We encourage future meta‐analyses following up on these results to incorporate newer literature, and additional novel analysis angles to further improve our understanding of the cerebellum in advanced age.

Somewhat relatedly, we did not include investigations that looked at age across adulthood using regression‐modeling approaches. Given the way the analyses were set up, if a study did not include contrasts for a given age group, the study could not be included in the analysis here. Foci from studies taking an adult lifespan approach are often the result of correlations with age, and as such include individuals from across adulthood. There is no way to include these studies in the group comparisons here, despite the wealth of knowledge this type of work provides. This would however be an interesting focus in future meta‐analytic work, to follow‐up on this examination of age differences.

One of the greatest benefits of meta‐analyses is the ability to concatenate across large literatures. However, this also means concatenating across studies with different methodological approaches, and varying degrees of information related to the study samples. As such, we included both PET and fMRI studies, consistent with past meta‐analyses of cerebellar function (Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009; E et al., 2014), as well as different inclusion and exclusion criteria. Notably, however, the ALE algorithm accounts for uncertainty and variability across subjects and sites so as to be relatively robust to these methodological differences (Eikhoff et al., 2012). While this means we cannot carefully control for these individual factors, this work also provides a powerful indicator of activation patterns seen in different age groups, and the diversity in the samples is likely more representative of the broader population as a whole.

Together, this work represents the first comprehensive investigation into cerebellar activation patterns in OA. First, we demonstrated that during the performance of cognitive tasks, OA show less convergence in cerebellar foci across studies than YA, perhaps indicative of decreases in activation, in contrast to what is seen in the PFC (e.g., Reuter‐Lorenz et al., 1999; Cabeza, 2002; Reuter‐Lorenz & Cappell, 2008), consistent with our hypothesis and prior work (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). We suggest that cerebellar processing is critical for optimal and efficient behavior. In advanced age, these resources are not brought online, likely due to degraded communication with the cortex (Bernard & Seidler, 2014). As such, OAs are unable to use this critical region and scaffolding for performance, resulting in behavioral declines. Conversely, we see more extensive convergence across studies in OA during motor tasks. However, we propose that this is due to spinal afferents that bypass the cortex and allow for compensatory activation (Filip et al., 2019). Thus, on the basis of these findings, we suggest that cerebellar functional activation differences with advanced age result in dissociable behavioral impacts due to the source of inputs through the inferior olive. While the cerebellum may be able to engage in compensatory activation for motor tasks, this is not the case in the cognitive domain.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

ETHICS STATEMENT

This investigation used published, anonymous data and as such was not subject to ethics review.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

J. A. B. was supported, in part, by a Brain and Behavior Research Foundation NARSAD Young Investigator Award as the Donald and Janet Boardman Family Investigator. She is also currently supported in part by National Institute on Aging (Grant R01AG064010‐01).

Bernard JA, Nguyen AD, Hausman HK, et al. Shaky scaffolding: Age differences in cerebellar activation revealed through activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis . Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41:5255–5281. 10.1002/hbm.25191

Funding information Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, Grant/Award Number: NARSAD Young Investigator Award, Donald and Janet; National Institute on Aging, Grant/Award Number: R01AG064010‐01

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

All materials associated with the analysis in the form of text files of foci are freely available for download at https://osf.io/gx5jw/.

REFERENCES

  1. Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , Snyder, A. Z. , Vincent, J. L. , Lustig, C. , Head, D. , Raichle, M. E. , & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Disruption of large‐scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron, 56(5), 924–935. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anguera, J. A. , Reuter‐Lorenz, P. A. , Willingham, D. T. , & Seidler, R. D. (2011). Failure to engage spatial working memory contributes to age‐related declines in visuomotor learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(1), 11–25. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balsters, J. H. , Whelan, C. D. , Robertson, I. H. , & Ramnani, N. (2013). Cerebellum and cognition: Evidence for the encoding of higher order rules. Cerebral Cortex, 23(6), 1433–1443. 10.1093/cercor/bhs127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernard, J. A. , & Mittal, V. A. (2015). Dysfunctional activation of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 545–566. 10.1177/2167702614542463 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bernard, J. A. , Russell, C. E. , Newberry, R. E. , Goen, J. R. M. , & Mittal, V. A. (2017). Patients with schizophrenia show aberrant patterns of basal ganglia activation: Evidence from ALE meta‐analysis. NeuroImage: Clinical, 14, 450–463. 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernard, J. A. , & Seidler, R. D. (2013a). Cerebellar contributions to visuomotor adaptation and motor sequence learning: An ALE meta‐analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 27 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bernard, J. A. , & Seidler, R. D. (2013b). Relationships between regional cerebellar volume and sensorimotor and cognitive function in young and older adults. Cerebellum, 12(5), 721–737. 10.1007/s12311-013-0481-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bernard, J. A. , & Seidler, R. D. (2014). Moving forward: Age effects on the cerebellum underlie cognitive and motor declines. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 193–207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernard, J. A. , Seidler, R. D. , Hassevoort, K. M. , Benson, B. L. , Welsh, R. C. , Wiggins, J. L. , … Peltier, S. J. (2012). Resting state cortico‐cerebellar functional connectivity networks: A comparison of anatomical and self‐organizing map approaches. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 6, 1–19. 10.3389/fnana.2012.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bernard, J. A. , Peltier, S. J. , Wiggins, J. L. , Jaeggi, S. M. , Buschkuehl, M. , Fling, B. W. , … Seidler, R. D. (2013). Disrupted cortico‐cerebellar connectivity in older adults. NeuroImage, 83, 103–119. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100. 10.1037//0882-7974.17.1.85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cappell, K. A. , Gmeindl, L. , & Reuter‐Lorenz, P. A. (2010). Age differences in prefontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load. Cortex, 46(4), 462–473. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen, S. H. A. , & Desmond, J. E. (2005). Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. NeuroImage, 24(2), 332–338. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Diedrichsen, J. , Balsters, J. H. , Flavell, J. , Cussans, E. , & Ramnani, N. (2009). A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage, 46(1), 39–46. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diedrichsen, J. , King, M. , Henandez‐Castillo, C. , Sereno, M. , & Ivry, R. B. (2019). Universal transform of multiple functionality? Understanding the contribution of the human cerebellum across task domains. Neron, 102(5), 918–928. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.04.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dimitrova, A. , Weber, J. , Redies, C. , Kindsvater, K. , Maschke, M. , Kolb, F. P. , & Forsting, M. (2002). MRI atlas of the human cerebellar nuclei. NeuroImage, 255, 240–255. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. E, K.‐H. , Chen, S.‐H. A. , Ho, M.‐H. R. , & Desmond, J. E. (2014). A meta‐analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Human Brain Mapping, 35(2), 593–615. 10.1002/hbm.22194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eickhoff, S. B. , Bzdok, D. , Laird, A. R. , Kurth, F. , & Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis revisited. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2349–2361. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017.Activation [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Eickhoff, S. B. , Laird, A. R. , Grefkes, C. , Wang, L. E. , Zilles, K. , & Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis of neuroimaging data: A random‐effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2907–2926. 10.1002/hbm.20718 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Filip, P. , Gallea, C. , Lehéricy, S. , Lungu, O. , & Bareš, M. (2019). Neural scaffolding as the foundation for stable performance of aging cerebellum. Cerebellum, 18(3), 500–510. 10.1007/s12311-019-01015-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Han, S. , An, Y. , Carass, A. , Prince, J. L. , & Resnick, S. M. (2020). Longitudingal analysis of regional cerebellum volumes during normal aging. NeuroImage, 220(15), 117062 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Heuninckx, S. , Wenderoth, N. , Debaere, F. , Peeters, R. , & Swinnen, S. P. (2005). Neural basis of aging: The penetration of cognition into action control. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 25(29), 6787–6796. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1263-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Heuninckx, S. , Wenderoth, N. , & Swinnen, S. P. (2008). Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: Recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 28(1), 91–99. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3300-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Huxhold, O. , Li, S.‐C. , Schmiedek, F. , & Lindenberger, U. (2006). Dual‐tasking postural control: Aging and the effects of cognitive demand in conjunction with focus of attention. Brain Research Bulletin, 69(3), 294–305. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Imamizu, H. , Miyauchi, S. , Tamada, T. , Sasaki, Y. , Takino, R. , Pütz, B. , … Kawato, M. (2000). Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature, 403(6766), 192–195. 10.1038/35003194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(4), 304–313. 10.1038/nrn2332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. King, M. , Hernandez‐Castillo, C. R. , Poldrack, R. A. , Ivry, R. B. , & Diedrichsen, J. (2019). Functional boundaries in the human cerebellum revealed by a multi‐domain task battery. Naturre Neuroscience, 22, 1371–1378. 10.1038/s41593-019-0436-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Koppelmans, V. , Young, Y. , & Sarah, H. (2017). Regional cerebellar volumetric correlates of manual motor and cognitive function. Brain Structure & Function, 222, 1929–1944. 10.1007/s00429-016-1317-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Krienen, F. M. , & Buckner, R. L. (2009). Segregated Fronto‐cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(10), 2485–2497. 10.1093/cercor/bhp135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lancaster, J. L. , Tordesillas‐Gutiérrez, D. , Martinez, M. , Salinas, F. , Evans, A. , Zilles, K. , … Fox, P. T. (2007). Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM‐152 brain template. Human Brain Mapping, 28(11), 1194–1205. 10.1002/hbm.20345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Langan, J. , Peltier, S. J. , Bo, J. , Fling, B. W. , Welsh, R. C. , & Seidler, R. D. (2010). Functional implications of age differences in motor system connectivity. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 4, 17 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maldonado, T. , Orr, J. M. , Goen, J. R. M. , & Bernard, J. A. (2020). Age differences in the subcomponents of executive functioning. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(6), e31–e55. 10.1093/geronb/gbaa005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Middleton, F. A. , & Strick, P. L. (2001). Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(2), 700–712. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-02-00700.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Miller, T. D. , Ferguson, K. J. , Reid, L. M. , Wardlaw, J. M. , Starr, J. M. , Seckl, J. R. , … Maclullich, A. M. J. (2013). Cerebellar vermis size and cognitive ability in community‐dwelling elderly men. Cerebellum, 12(1), 68–73. 10.1007/s12311-012-0397-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Naccarato, M. , Calautti, C. , Jones, P. S. , Day, D. J. , Carpenter, T. a. , & Baron, J.‐C. (2006). Does healthy aging affect the hemispheric activation balance during paced index‐to‐thumb opposition task? An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 32(3), 1250–1256. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Parikh, P. K. , Troyer, A. K. , Maione, A. M. , & Murphy, K. J. (2016). The impact of memory change on daily life in Normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Gerontologist, 56(5), 877–885. 10.1093/geront/gnv030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Park, D. C. , Polk, T. A. , Mikels, J. A. , Taylor, S. F. , & Marshuetz, C. (2001). Cerebral aging: Integration of brain and behavioral models of cognitive function. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 3(3), 151–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Park, D. C. , & Reuter‐Lorenz, P. A. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173–196. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ramnani, N. (2006). The primate cortico‐cerebellar system: Anatomy and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(7), 511–522. 10.1038/nrn1953 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ramnani, N. (2014). Automatic and controlled processing in the corticocerebellar system. Progress in Brain Research, 210, 255–285. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00010-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Raz, N. , Lindenberger, U. , Rodrigue, K. M. , Kennedy, K. M. , Head, D. , Williamson, A. , … Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 1676–1689. 10.1093/cercor/bhi044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Reuter‐Lorenz, P. A. , & Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychology Review, 24(3), 355–370. 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Reuter‐Lorenz, P. A. , Stanczak, L. , & Miller, a. C. (1999). Neural recruitment and cognitive aging: Two hemispheres are better than one, especially as you age. Psychological Science, 10(6), 494–500. 10.1111/1467-9280.00195 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Reuter‐Lorenz, P. A. , & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177–182. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Salmi, J. , Pallesen, K. , & Neuvonen, T. (2010). Cognitive and motor loops of the human cerebro‐cerebellar system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2663–2676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Seidler, R. D. , Bernard, J. A. , Burutolu, T. B. , Fling, B. W. , Gordon, M. T. , Gwin, J. T. , … Lipps, D. B. (2010). Motor control and aging: Links to age‐related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 721–733. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Stawarczyk, D. , & D'Argembeau, A. (2015). Neural correlates of personal goal processing during episodic future thinking and mind‐wandering: An ALE meta‐analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 2947(April), 2928–2947. 10.1002/hbm.22818 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Stoodley, C. J. , & Schmahmann, J. D. (2009). Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 44(2), 489–501. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Stoodley, C. J. , Valera, E. M. , & Schmahmann, J. D. (2012). Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 59, 1560–1570. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Strick, P. L. , Dum, R. P. , & Fiez, J. A. (2009). Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 412–434. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Turkeltaub, P. E. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Laird, A. R. , Fox, M. , Wiener, M. , & Fox, P. (2012). Minimizing within‐experiment and within‐group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta‐analyses. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 1–13. 10.1002/hbm.21186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Verhaeghen, P. , & Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control, and attention: A review of meta‐analyses. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 849–857. 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00071-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Walhovd, K. B. , Westlye, L. T. , Amlien, I. , Espeseth, T. , Reinvang, I. , Raz, N. , … Fjell, A. M. (2011). Consistent neuroanatomical age‐related volume differences across multiple samples. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(5), 916–932. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.05.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wasylyshyn, C. , Verhaeghen, P. , & Sliwinski, M. J. (2011). Aging and task switching: A meta‐analysis. Psychology and Aging, 26, 15–20. 10.1037/a0020912 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All materials associated with the analysis in the form of text files of foci are freely available for download at https://osf.io/gx5jw/.


Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES