Skip to main content
Heliyon logoLink to Heliyon
. 2020 Nov 20;6(11):e05477. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05477

Critical thinking disposition of undergraduate students in relation to emotional intelligence: Gender as a moderator

Sahanowas Sk a,, Santoshi Halder b,∗∗
PMCID: PMC7689406  PMID: 33294656

Abstract

Undergraduate student, especially first-year students face challenges in the new found academic environment. Critical thinking (CT) has been found to be effective in managing negative experiences. Earlier literature found Emotional intelligence (EI) is accounted for variance in critical thinking (CT) skill irrespective of the gender. However, there is scarcity of research on the relationship between EI and CT disposition with respect to gender. The present research intended to study the effect of EI on CT disposition of students in the transition phase with gender as a moderator. 500 first-year undergraduate students from West Bengal (eastern part of India) were selected to fill up assessment tools; the Profile of Emotional Competence (Brasseur et al., 2013) and Critical Thinking Disposition Assessment (Ricketts & Rudd, 2005). Structural Equation Model (SEM) was followed using AMOS adopting maximum likelihood approach. Path analysis revealed an accepted model fit establishing EI to have significant effect on CT disposition. However, this effect was not moderated by gender. The present research might be helpful in generating awareness regarding the importance of EI and training for developing disposition of CT among college fresher's well-being and emotional health. The study has highlighted the non-significant moderating role of gender in relation to EI and CT disposition that was missing in the existing literature.

Keywords: Critical thinking disposition, Emotional intelligence, Transition to higher education, Clinical psychology, Education, Psychology, Sociology, Philosophy


Critical thinking disposition, Emotional intelligence, Transition to higher education, Clinical psychology; Education; Psychology; Sociology; Philosophy

1. Introduction

Globally around 216 million students enrolled in higher education and India ranked the second position in the world and first position in Asia with 32.4 million of students enrolled in higher education as of 2016 (Calderon, 2018). 36.6 million students got enrolled in higher education during 2017–18 (Government of India Report [G.O.I.], 2018), out of which about 79.2% of the students, were admitted in undergraduate level programme (G.O.I., 2018). Enrolling for undergraduate course transitioning from school level termed as the transition phase, is one of the critical phases of college years leading to various adjustment difficulties (Aderi et al., 2013) and anxiety problems (Lowe and Cook, 2003). Researchers revealed 42.5% of the undergraduate first-year students faced adjustment problem (Ababu et al., 2018). In India about 65% of the first-year undergraduate students had moderate to low level of adjustability issues (Jain, 2017) while the undergraduate students showed a high level of anxiety (86.5%), depression (59.3%) and stress (52.8%) (Singh et al., 2017) which intervene their thinking process (Cartreine, 2016), problems solving (Yen et al., 2011) and decision making (Leykin et al., 2011) and henceforth effecting the overall academic achievement (Aafreen et al., 2018; Muhammed et al., 2018). The possible reasons may be being away from their parents and other family members, sometimes leading to difficulties in socializing, adjusting with others or making friends or sometimes living in Institutional housing which is quite different from home environment. Therefore undergraduate students' especially first-year (fresher) undergraduate students may be a concern and calls for immediate attention.

Research reports students' CT as a crucial factor for problems solving (Lismayani et al., 2017; Snyder and Snyder, 2008) and also decision making (Heidari and Ebrahimi, 2016; Noohi et al., 2012). A study by Franco et al. (2017) with undergraduate students found that low scores in CT predicted student's profiles of “risk-taking” and “lost in translation” (Franco et al., 2017). Further researchers also reported that students high in CT were found to be better in stress management (Mahal et al., 2015), in academic achievement (Taghva et al., 2014) and lower in procrastination (Goroshit, 2018; Joubert, 2015). In support, San et al. (2016) posited that, as self-regulated learning components, CT was found to be negatively correlated with academic procrastination for undergraduate students (San et al., 2016) and it was also found to be an effective predictor for social adjustment (Hashemiannejad et al., 2016). Moreover, it was more effective than cognitive factors like intelligence in preventing negative life event of college students' and community adults (Butler et al., 2017). Hence it is evident that CT should be given priority in higher education (Facione, 2010) as also supported by philosophers who claimed that CT is the fundamental goal of learning and central to higher education (Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011).

Delphi study (1990) has proposed mainly two forms of CT, i.e., CT skill and CT disposition. CT skill is person's ability to do something, but may or may not demonstrate the ability, while CT disposition is habitual behavioural inclination what people actually demonstrate. It is considered as “consistent internal motivation to engage problems and make decisions by using CT” (Facione, 2000, p. 65). The prerequisite for the development of CT is to create an effective context and background for disposition towards it (Khandaghi et al., 2011). It is reported that the training framework for CT without enough dispositions would fail (Khandaghi and Pakmehr, 2012). CT dispositions, attitudes, and habits constitute the intellectual character of students (Kang, 2015) and it is a reliable indicator of how likely a person thinks critically (Bensley, 2006). Hence the researchers intend to consider CT disposition an important factor as also put forward by Facione et al. (2001) that CT disposition is, “discipline neutral and comprises a generalizable description of the ideal critical thinker across multiple contexts and situations” (p.2, as cited in Kang, 2015).

1.1. Emotional Intelligence and Critical Thinking

Neurologist Damasio (2005) pointed out that emotion is the key element and central to the process of rational thought. On the other hand Professor of psychology, Pinker (1997) claimed that reasoning is driven by emotions. In accordance Goleman (1998) emphasizes that emotions and thoughts should not be considered as different concepts, because emotions form the basis of thoughts. Damasio (2000) further stated that all human behaviour is influenced by emotion so for a behavioural disposition; the CT disposition should be affected by emotion. Brookfield (1987) believe that emotion is central to CT, so it can be assumed that EI would be associated with CT as also supported by empirical research. The earlier researchers showed a positive reciprocal association between EI and CT skill for Iranian EFL (English as Foreign Language) learners (Afshar and Rahimi, 2014; Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011) and Iranian nursing students (Vahedi et al., 2015). CT disposition was also positively associated with EI of Malaysian graduate students (Kang, 2015). Further, EI had a significant effect on CT skill for Iranian EFL learners (Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011) and teachers (Ebrahimi and Moafian 2012) and nurses (Madadkhani and Nikoogoftar, 2015). Both the CT skill (Vahedi et al., 2015) and disposition (Kang, 2015; Stedman and Andenoro, 2007) were found to be significantly associated with EI. Dutoğlu and Tuncel (2008) reported that an individual with advanced CT abilities was also likely to have high EI for nursing students (Kaya et al., 2018). It was also found that emotional literacy program designed to develop emotion-related skills can promote overall academic learning by enhancing abstract reasoning and CT (Brackett and Katulak, 2007). Not only overall EI but also some of its factors were also found to be effective for CT, e.g., Saremi and Bahdori (2015) reported two of the factors, i.e., social skills and flexibility effective for CT skill of the school principals. In contrast with existing study, Nosratinia and Sarabchian (2013) showed that CT skill was negatively correlated with EI of EFL learners, while few studies revealed no significant association between EI and CT skill of Iranian first-year medical students (Haghani et al., 2011) and students with auditory, reading, and kinaesthetic learning styles (Leasa, 2018). Further, Hasanpour et al. (2018) revealed that CT skill had no significant relation with all the factors of Bar-On EQ, except empathy of nursing students. Most of the studies' reported concerns over CT skills. Though all the studies with CT disposition reported a positive association with EI but studies reported causal relation with regression effect. Even a limited number of studies measured the disposition with well-validated Critical Thinking Disposition Scale (Ricketts & Rudd, 2005), in terms of engagement, cognitive maturity and innovativeness. Further, no study has been found where CT disposition was evaluated for higher education students in the transition phase with respect to their EI. Hence the present research intends to explore these issues with a focus on Indian context conducted in the eastern part of India which is one of the most populated states with a cosmopolitan culture.

1.2. Gender, Critical Thinking and Emotional Intelligence

Investigations over gender differences on CT measures revealed conflicting results across various geographical locations with varied nature of samples. Some studies reported no significant gender differences (Kang, 2015; Sulaiman et al., 2017) while some found male (Rodzalan and Saat, 2015) and some showed female (Ahmad and Duskri, 2018; Turan, 2016; Vahedi et al., 2015) was comparatively better in CT. Inconsistent results were also found over gender differences in EI. Though few studies reported that gender does not make any differences in EI (e.g., Nasir and Masrur, 2010; Shehzad and Mahmood, 2013), some studies reported in favour of males (e.g., Ahmad et al., 2009) while most studies reported females to be higher in EI (e.g., Fernández-Berrocal et al., 2012; Joseph and Newman, 2010; Kant, 2019; Meshkat and Nejati, 2017; Naghavi and Redzuan, 2011). People with high EI are found to be good in problem-solving (Deniz, 2013), are more confident (Upadhyaya, 2006) and skilled communicator (2023Lakshmi, 2016; Marzuki et al., 2015) which are described as disposition or inclinations of CT (Irani et al., 2007). Further, Fernández-Berrocal et al. (2012) reported that female possess greater emotional knowledge. It was found that emotional knowledge plays an important role in decision making (Bratianu, 2015) and CT dispositions are also helpful in making confident decisions (Irani et al., 2007). Therefore, it can be assumed that that EI may have stronger effect on CT disposition for female, though, researchers (e.g., Ebrahimi and Moafian, 2012; Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011) reported non-significant moderation role of gender in between EI and CT skill. However, theoretical assumptions regarding gender role in relation to EI and CT disposition were contradicted by the empirical outcomes of relationship between EI and CT skill (Ebrahimi and Moafian, 2012; Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011). Hence this calls for further exploration; Whether or not gender would play a consistent non-significant moderating role in relation to EI and CT disposition as found for EI and CT skill in earlier literature.

2. Objective and Hypothesis

Researchers wanted to study the effect of EI on CT disposition of the students during their transition to higher education. Another objective was to explore whether or not the effect of EI on CT disposition will vary with respect to gender.

On the basis of previous literature, it was hypothesised that there will be a positive significant effect of EI on CT disposition. Following the theoretical justification (e.g., Deniz, 2013; Irani et al., 2007; Joseph and Newman, 2010; Kant, 2019; Meshkat and Nejati, 2017; Upadhyaya, 2006) it was expected that effect of EI on CT disposition will vary with respect to gender. The hypothetical framework of the present study is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Hypothetical Framework of the Proposed Study.

3. Methodology

3.1. Design and Participants

A descriptive cross-sectional study was designed to examine the effect of EI on CT disposition with respect to gender. Five hundred (N = 500) first year (fresher) undergraduate willing students (male = 54.6%; female = 45.4%; age 18–21; M = 18.98, SD = 0.82) participated in the study. Sample size was calculated following Yamane's formula (1967) [n = N/1 + N(e2)]. 400 students were required for proper representation of the population of first-year undergraduate students in India. To account for more precision, the number of participants was increased to 500.

3.2. Measuring Tools

Data were collected implementing adopted Profile of Emotional Competence (PEC; Brasseur et al., 2013) and Critical Thinking Disposition Assessment (EMI; Ricketss and Rudd, 2005). Details of the tools are as follows:

3.2.1. Profile of Emotional Competence (Brasseur et al., 2013)

The questionnaire comprised of 50 items and ten sub-scales (5 for intrapersonal 5 for interpersonal) with five core emotional competencies (i.e., identification, understanding, expression/listening, regulation and use of emotions). In a five-point rating scale (1– strongly disagree to 5–strongly agree), the total score ranged from 50-250, with higher scores reflecting greater EI. The scale indicated good internal consistency of the sub-scales (0.60 to .83) and very good consistency of the two factors (0.84) and of the total score (0.88). The reliability of the intrapersonal EI score was 0.86, interpersonal EI score was 0.89 and of the total EI score was 0.92.

3.2.2. Critical Thinking Disposition Assessment (Ricketss and Rudd, 2005)

The scale consists of 26 items based on three constructs, namely, engagement, cognitive maturity, and innovativeness, derived primarily from the work of Facione (1990). It is also termed as UF-EMI (University of Florida- Engagement, Cognitive Maturity and Innovativeness assessment). EMI scale is measured on a five-point likert type scale with 1 representing ‘strongly disagree’, and 5 representing ‘strongly agree’. Reliability estimates for original EMI was with the accepted range; for Engagement 0.91, for Maturity 0.79, for Innovativeness 0.80 and Total 0.94.

3.3. Translation and Standardization of the Tools

The two measures were translated into Bengali for regional convenience. Blind back-translation from English to Bengali and Bengali to English was done. The final version has been finalised on the basis of the consensuses from three independent raters regarding the conceptual similarities of the items in both languages. Percentage of agreement was 81% and 87% for EI and CT disposition scale respectively. A pilot study was conducted for standardization of tools with 100 students. For CT disposition, item – total correlation range was found to be 0.25–0.71 and Cronbach alpha for the full scale was 0.84. For EI scale item-total correlations ranged from 0.20 to 0.55 and Cronbach alpha for the full scale was 0.84. In the EI scale, one item was deleted due to its low validity with dimension total and total dimensions.

3.4. Data Collection Procedure

The sample was collected from randomly selected eleven undergraduate colleges and two undergraduate sections of a state university from three districts of West Bengal, i.e. Kolkata, South and North 24 Parganas (i.e. Eastern part of India). The students were selected from general education background excluding the professional courses (i.e. vocational, medical or engineering courses). Permission was sought from the institutional appropriate authority and informed consent was retrieved from each study participants. Necessary information regarding the study was provided and confidentiality was assured. Only willing participants were finally included in the study. The research was approved by the University of Calcutta Research Advisory Committee (RAC).

3.5. Statistical Techniques

Descriptive statistics, like mean, standard deviation (SD) and percentage as well as inferential statistics, i.e., bivariate correlation (Pearson's product moment) between exogenous (EI) and endogenous variable (CT disposition), were conducted using SPSS version 15. In order to estimate the effect of EI on CT disposition latent variable Structural Equation Model (SEM) was followed using AMOS version 22. Further, moderation effect of gender was evaluated by multi-group moderation analysis using Chi-square (χ2) difference test with the help of ‘Stats tool package’ (Gaskin, 2012).

4. Result and Interpretation

4.1. Normality of the Data

Normality of measures was ensured by skewness (Sk) and kurtosis (Ku) (Table 1). For EI skewness and kurtosis were 0.180 and 0.612 respectively and for CT disposition skewness and kurtosis were -0.420 and 0.385 respectively and these values were in acceptable range according to Byrne (2010) (Sk = ± 2; Ku = ± 7).

Table 1.

Skewness and Kurtosis of the Variables under Study.

EI CT disposition
Skewness 0.180 -0.420
Z = SK/error of Sk 0.180/0.109 = 1.65 -0.420/0.109 = -3.85
Kurtosis 0.612 0.385
Z = Ku/error of Ku 0.612/.218 = 2.94 0.385/0.218 = 1.76

The z score (absolute Sk or Ku/SE) of EI was 0.180/0.109 = 1.65 (z<±1.96) for Sk and 0.612/0.218 = 2.94 (z<±3.29) for Ku. Further, z score of CT disposition was, -0.420/.109 = -3.85 (z > ± 3.29) for Sk and 0.385/0.218 = 1.76 (z < ± 1.96) for Ku. The values can be considered as relatively normal distribution for a large sample (N > 300) (Kim, 2013).

4.2. Descriptive and Correlational Analysis

Findings in Tables 2 and 3 revealed that mean and SD were, M = 159.62, SD = 17.97 for EI and M = 93.36, SD = 12.63 for CT disposition. For male participants' mean and SD were, M = 161.58, SD = 17.90 for EI and M = 94.18, SD = 12.66 for CT disposition. For female participants mean and SD were, M = 157.25, SD = 17.97 for EI and M = 92.38, SD = 12.55 for CT disposition. Correlational analysis revealed a (Table 3) significant positive relation between EI and CT disposition (r = 0.47, p < 0.01).

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics of the Variables under Study with Respect to Gender.

Stat. EI
CT disposition
Male (n = 273) Female (n = 227) Total (N = 500) Male (n = 273) Female (n = 227) Total (N = 500)
Mean 161.58 157.25 159.62 94.18 92.38 93.36
Median 161 157 159 94 94 94
SD 17.903 17.810 17.973 12.661 12.555 12.632
S.E 1.084 1.182 0.804 0.766 0.833 0.565
Sk 0.031 0.3368 0.180 -0.287 -0.599 -0.420
Ku 0.902 0.518 0.612 -0.018 0.837 0.385
Rang 85–212 104–210 85–212 61–125 50–121 50–125

Table 3.

Mean, Standard Deviation and Correlational Matrix of the Study Variables.

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
EI-1 17.53 2.974 1
EI-2 15.57 3.683 .46∗∗ 1
EI-3 15.18 3.425 .28∗∗ .22∗∗ 1
EI-4 16.24 3.853 .33∗∗ .33∗∗ .30∗∗ 1
EI-5 15.31 2.662 .20∗∗ .07 .11∗ .20∗∗ 1
EI-6 15.91 3.066 .33∗∗ .28∗∗ .21∗∗ .23∗∗ .05 1
EI-7 15.46 3.049 .28∗∗ .21∗∗ .21∗∗ .22∗∗ .04 .42∗∗ 1
EI-8 16.95 3.072 .15∗∗ .07 .17∗∗ .16∗∗ .11∗ .23∗∗ .20∗∗ 1
EI-9 15.91 3.348 .26∗∗ .20∗∗ .22∗∗ .38∗∗ .12∗∗ .34∗∗ .34∗∗ .32∗∗ 1
EI-10 15.53 3.124 .21∗∗ .07 .23∗∗ ..32∗∗ .17∗∗ .20∗∗ .27∗∗ .14∗∗ .44∗∗ 1
EI-T 159.62 17.973 .63∗∗ .55∗∗ .18∗∗ .65∗∗ .35∗∗ .59∗∗ .57∗∗ .45∗∗ .66∗∗ .54∗∗ 1
CT-1 39.15 6.287 .28∗∗ .17∗∗ .54∗∗ .31∗∗ .32∗∗ .20∗∗ .28∗∗ .32∗∗ .41∗∗ .38∗∗ .51∗∗ 1
CT-2 28.26 4.193 .13∗∗ .09∗ .01 .23∗∗ .31∗∗ .10∗ .16∗∗ .15∗∗ .24∗∗ .19∗∗ .29∗∗ .52∗∗ 1
CT-3 25.95 4.313 .19∗∗ .10∗ .08 .23∗∗ .35∗∗ .11∗ .12∗∗ .24∗∗ .26∗∗ .27∗∗ .34∗∗ .70∗∗ .51∗∗ 1
CTD-T 93.36 12.632 .25∗∗ .15∗∗ .13∗∗ .31∗∗ .38∗∗ .17∗∗ .23∗∗ .30∗∗ .37∗∗ .35∗∗ .47∗∗ .91∗∗ .77∗ .86∗∗ 1

Note. EI-1 = Identification of own emotions; EI-2 = Understanding of own emotions; EI-3 = Expression of own emotions; EI-4 = Regulation of own emotions; EI-5 = Utilization of own emotions; EI-6 = Identification of others' emotions; EI-7 = Understanding of others' emotions; EI-8 = Listening to others' emotions; EI-9 = Regulation of others' emotions; EI-10 = Utilization of others' emotions; EI-T = Emotional intelligence total; CT-1 = Engagement, CT-2 = Cognitive maturity, CT-3 = Innovativeness, CTD-T = Critical thinking disposition total.

∗∗p < 0.01. ∗p < 0.

4.3. Model Fit

Model fit was confirmed with common goodness-of-fit (GOF) indices like ratio between Chi-square and the degree of freedom (CMIN/DF = χ2/df), Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI) and Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index (AGFI) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) to validate the compatibility with the obtained empirical data.

In the model fit, Chi-square (χ2) ratio to degree of freedom (χ2/df) should not exceed 3 (Byrne, 2006; Xiong et al., 2015). The recommended range is as high as 5.0 to as low as 2.0 (Hooper et al., 2008; Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007). RMSEA should be 0.05 or less (Arbuckle, 2005) but upper limit of 0.07 seems to be accepted (Hooper et al., 2008; Steiger, 2007). GFI and AGFI should be 0.9 or more (Awang, 2012; Hooper et al., 2008), though value of 0.80 is acceptable (Chin and Todd, 1995 as cited in Rivers et al., 2013). CFI should be more than 0.9 (Hooper et al., 2008), though 0.80 or more represent a good fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1993; as cited in Chinda et al., 2012).

In the present study number of distinct sample moment was 91 and the number of distinct parameters to be estimated was 27, hence the degree of freedom was (91-27) = 64. Initial model fit (Model 1) indices were not within the acceptable level, Chi-square (χ2) = 306.052, p < .001; CMIN/DF = χ2/df = 306.052/64 = 4.782; RMSEA = .08; GFI = .90; AGFI = 0.86 and CFI = 0.84. Modification Indices (MI) suggested that there were correlated error terms between EI-1 and EI-2 (e1<-->e2; MI = 50.694) and between EI-6 and EI-7 (e6<-->e7; MI = 28.299). Error terms were allowed to correlate and then results showed an improved model fit (Model 2) where Chi-square value (χ2) = 218.629, p < .001, was significant, indicating the model is not adequate. Since the chi-square statistics (χ2) is sensitive to sample size. Chi-Square statistics nearly always reject the model when large samples are used (Hooper et al., 2008). Therefore it is necessary to look at others indices especially if the sample size exceeds 300 or when other indices of the model are acceptable (Roshani and Bagrecha, 2017). Other indices in the model fit were within the acceptable range, CMIN/DF= χ2/df = 218.629/62 = 3.526, RMSEA = 0.07, GFI = 0.935; AGFI = .905 and CFI = 0.90 (Table 4). Henceforth, it can be assumed that the hypothesised model reasonably fit the data.

Table 4.

Acceptable and Observed Values of SEM Indices.

Model-fit indices Model-1 Model-2 Model- 3 Acceptable fit indices
Chi-sq/df 4.782 3.526 2.306 <3/5
RMSEA .08 .07 .05 ≤0.05/0.08
P-close .000 .000 .000 <0.05
GFI .90 .935 .916 ≥0.9
AGFI .86 .90 .877 >0.09/0.08
CFI .84 .90 .896 ≥0.9

Moreover, all the factor loadings for the indicators on the latent factors were significant (p < .001), indicating that the latent factors (Table 5) were represented by their respective indicators. For EI, indicators of regulation of others' emotions showed the highest correlation coefficient (r = 0.67) followed by regulation of own emotion (r = 0.57), use of others' emotion (r = 0.55), identification of own emotion (r = 0.49), understanding of others emotion (r = 0.47), identification of others' emotion (r = 0.45), expression of own emotion (r = 0.41), listening of others' emotion (r = 0.40), understanding of own emotion (r = 0.35) and use of own emotion (r = 0.31). For CT disposition, indicator of engagement had highest level of coefficient (r = 0.92) followed by innovativeness (r = 0.0.75) and cognitive maturity (r = 0.59) (Table 5).

Table 5.

Standardized Regression Weights of Indicators onto Underlying Latent Factors.

Observed variable (Indicators) Latent variable Regression weights P values
EI- 1 <----- EI 0.489 P < 0.001
EI-2 <----- EI 0.351 P < 0.001
EI-3 <----- EI 0.409 P < 0.001
EI-4 <----- EI 0.572 P < 0.001
EI- 5 <----- EI 0.309 P < 0.001
EI-6 <----- EI 0.451 P < 0.001
EI-7 <----- EI 0.467 P < 0.001
EI-8 <----- EI 0.402 P < 0.001
EI-9 <----- EI 0.671 P < 0.001
EI-10 <----- EI 0.555 P < 0.001
CT-1 <----- CT disposition 0.918 P < 0.001
CT-2 <----- CT disposition 0.589 P < 0.001
CT-3 <----- CT disposition 0.759 P < 0.001

Note. EI-1 = Identification of own emotion, EI-2 = Understanding of own emotion, EI-3 = Expression of own emotion, EI-4 = Regulation of own emotion, EI-5 = Use of own emotions, EI-6 = Identification of others emotion, EI-7 = Understanding of others emotion, EI-8 = Listening of others emotion, EI-9 = Regulation of others emotion, EI-10 = Use of others emotions; CT-1 = Engagement, CT-2 = Cognitive Maturity and CT-3 = Innovativeness.

In the gender (male & female) specific model (Model 3) the number of distinct sample moment was 182 and number of distinct parameters to be estimated was 58, hence the degree of freedom was (182-58) = 124. The model fit indices were in the accepted level, CMIN/DF = 285.942/124 = 2.306, RMSEA = 0.05, GFI = 0.916, AGFI = 0.877 and CFI = 0.896 (Table 4). Therefore, it can be concluded that the hypothesised model indicated a good fit of gender-wise sample model.

4.4. Hypothesis Testing

It was shown in the model that (Table 6, Figure 2) EI had a significant direct effect on CT disposition (β = 0.64, p < 0.001). EI explained 40% (R2 = 0.40) of variance in CT disposition. Thus, the hypothesis that “there will be a positive significant effect of EI on CT disposition” is accepted.

Table 6.

Regression Prediction of CT Disposition by EI.

β R2 S.E C.R P values
All Sample CT disposition < ---- EI 0.64 0.40 0.298 8.485 P < 0.001
Male CT disposition < ---- EI 0.57 0.32 0.373 5.995 P < 0.001
Female CT disposition < ---- EI 0.72 0.52 .453 6.145 P < 0.001

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Latent Variable Regression Path Model for Overall Sample (N = 500).

In gender-specific model it was found that (Table 6) EI also had significant direct effect on CT disposition for both male (β = 0.57, p < 0.001) and female (β = 0.72, p < 0.001) and explained 32% (R2 = 0.32) and 52% (R2 = 0.52) of the variance in CT disposition for male (Figure 3) and female (Figure 4) respectively.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Latent Variable Regression Path Model for Male Sample (n = 273).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Latent Variable Regression Path Model for Female Sample (n = 227).

Chi-square (χ2) difference test was used to check the multi-group moderation across the groups, separately for all groups. First, the unconstrained model fit was evaluated, with path estimates calculated individually for each group. Second, a constrained model was evaluated where the path estimate of one of the path is constrained to be equal between groups. First, the unconstrained model was tested that resulted in χ2 = 285.942, df = 124. Second, constrained model was tested that resulted in χ2 = 293.261, df = 134 (Table 7).

Table 7.

Testing of Gender as Moderator in the Model.

Model
Characteristics
Unconstrained
Model
Constrained
Model
Model Differences (Δχ2)
Chi-Square (χ2) 285.942 293.261 7.319∗∗
df 124 134 10
CFI 0.896 0.898 --
RMSEA 0.051 0.49 --

Note. ∗∗Not significant.

The χ2 difference test (χ2 = 7.319, df = 10) for constrained model and unconstrained model was not found to be significant (p = 0.695) at 5% level. This indicated that the model was invariant at the group level, implying no moderation of gender. This implies that the effect of EI on CT disposition was equal for both male and female, thus, the hypothesis that the effect of EI on CT disposition will vary with respect to gender is rejected.

5. Discussion

As a general stereotypical misconception, that emotion intervenes and affects our thinking process; emerging theoretical conception supported the mutual positive relation of the two. Elder (1996) argued that the human mind is composed of at least three main functions; namely, cognition, feelings, and volition. While processing is in progress, these three functions, though theoretically distinctive, have a dynamic and intimate relation to each other. It was argued that thoughts and emotions are inextricably bound (Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011; McLaughlin, 2003). Theory of EI holds the view that emotion can be utilised to facilitate thinking process (Mayer, 2009). Following this conceptual and theoretical background present research endeavour intended to examine the effect of self-reported EI on behavioural disposition of CT of first-year undergraduate students. It was revealed that EI was effective for CT disposition which was supported in terms of CT skill. Ghanizadeh and Moafian (2011) having EFL (English as a Foreign Language) students and Ebrahimi and Moafian (2012) having EFL high school teachers showed that Bar-On EQ had a significant effect on CT skill. The study by Madadkhani and Nikoogoftar (2015) reported that emotional clarity that is one of the factors of EI was effective for CT disposition.

In the present study first-year undergraduate students' self-perceived EI in terms of identification, understanding, expression or listening, regulation and use of emotions (own and others) were found to be effective for their behavioural disposition of CT in terms of engagement, cognitive maturity and innovativeness.

Substantially, emotions are inter-connected with instincts (Levine, 2017; Gu et al., 2019). Our emotions cater the information of psycho-physiological response that prepares the person to react adaptively (Cosmides and Tooby, 2000; Scherer et al., 2013). For example, happiness emotion promotes social bonding; anger makes relational problems. So, knowing emotion can help in anticipating the responses. Perceive or identify emotion is one of the factors of EI which convey others' mental states, intentions and future behaviour (Jang and Elfenbein, 2015). Further, emotional understanding, implies understanding the complexity of emotional blending and how one emotional situation transforms into another, may forecast the subsequent changes the emotional response of any situation. Therefore, emotional perception and its understanding help to anticipate others behaviour (Bennett et al., 2005; Pons et al., 2004) which is related to dispositional characteristics of CT (i.e., engagement) (Irani et al., 2007; Ricketts and Rudd, 2004).

EI enables people to address the problems from multitude perspectives (Salovey et al., 2000), further, it caters required information, emotional cues and resources regarding problematic situation in which person can determine to use appropriate problem-solving strategies in a specific situation (Saklofske et al., 2007; Salovey et al., 2002). Therefore persons can confidently make decision to use the strategies to solve problems. Deniz (2013) concluded that EI positively improves problem-solving skill. Emotion regulation, processes of controlling of when, which emotion arises and how we experience and express them (Gross, 2008), helps in decision-making process reducing the negative emotional experience under risky situations (Heilman et al., 2010).

Furthermore, utilisation of emotion, one of the factors of EI, assist in our reasoning, problem-solving, decision-making and taking into account other people's perspectives (Mayer et al., 2002, as cited in Fiori and Vesely-Maillefer, 2019). A person with disposition of ‘engagement’ looks for opportunities to use their reasoning skill as they are confident about problem-solving and decision-making. Hence being emotionally intelligent, students might be possessed with the disposition of engagement, one of the factors of CT disposition.

One of the qualities that critical thinker possess is impulsiveness (Arockiasamy, 2014), means they don't want to get unsound solution quickly rather want to think and reason before choosing the conclusion and in that process control of emotion should be pre-conditioned, emotional hijacking should not be entertained to reach a reasonable conclusion, therefore effective management or control of emotion (EI) can inculcate impulsivity, which is the virtue of critical thinker. So it is apparent that emotionally intelligent students can be good critical thinker.

Individuals good at recognizing and understanding emotion are open to different experiences and new ideas (Cherry, 2018). Anemotionally intelligent person possesses the personality of openness to experience and agreeableness Mayer et al., 2004. They are creative, open-minded and broader thinker (Caruso et al., 2002; George, 2000). Therefore, when undergraduate students possess EI they tend to consider others opinion and diverse possibilities of situations or issues. They would not think always right rather tend to accept others thought to change their own if necessary which in turn might lead to disposition of cognitive maturity, that is one of the factors of CT disposition. Persons with ‘cognitive maturity’ are open-minded and listen to others point of views (Irani et al., 2007; Ricketts and Rudd, 2004).

Persons with the disposition of innovativeness (factor of CT disposition) generally shows curiosity while looking for new knowledge (Irani et al., 2007; Ricketts and Rudd, 2004) which are characteristics of intrinsically motivated students who are curious to explore their interests (Valerio, 2012), accepts challenges and are excited to learn (Kivetz, 2003). This intrinsic motivation is mediated and promoted by EI (Atiq et al., 2015). Further, emotionally intelligent persons' locus of control is internal (Omoniyi and Adelowo, 2014) and person with an internal locus of control are generally inquisitive in nature (Beaven-Marks, 2017), curious and tend to seek more information about any task perform (Millet, 2005), which are the dispositional characteristics of CT (i.e., innovativeness). Therefore, students who possess EI might show dispositional characteristics of CT, i.e., innovativeness.

Present research revealed no gender difference in of EI on CT disposition which was supported earlier researchers (Ebrahimi and Moafian, 2012; Ghanizadeh and Moafian, 2011) They reported that gender did not play moderator role in Bar-On EI and Watson-Glaser CT appraisal (CT skill). Inconsistent with correlation study present study revealed no gender moderation in the regression model and also suggested that EI had an independent effect on CT disposition regardless of the gender of students in transition phase. There were some common issues among undergraduate students especially first-year students irrespective of gender, like homesickness, emotional attachment to opposite gender, test anxiety and adjustment difficulties in the new academic environment etc. Researchers (e.g., Busari, 2012; Gefen and Fish, 2012) reported some common stressors like financial stressors, stressors related to interpersonal relationship and academic stressors as experienced by both male and female. Further, no significant gender difference was reported among college students in various spheres like adjustment at home, educational adjustments, social adjustment and emotional adjustment (Chowhan et al., 2019). Both males and females participants may be similar in terms of perceiving, understanding and controlling the surroundings they belong to since the very beginning of the course. Thus the common issues experienced by the first-year students may uphold the non-significant moderating role of gender in relation to EI and CT disposition. Further, the literature showed contradicting outcomes in relation to the role of gender in EI and CT disposition. Therefore assessing the gender role regarding the interplay between EI and CT disposition is difficult which may justify and explain the present research outcomes.

6. Implication and Conclusion

Results revealed a significant effect of EI on CT disposition irrespective of gender. Students admitted to undergraduate level should be oriented about emotional information and literacy to build behavioural disposition of CT which is one of the prime necessity at higher education. Both qualities might help to reduce the pressure at this crucial phase and assist in adjustment and adaptability of students in the newly found environment. Students enter into higher education environment with hope, dreams for a better future ahead and hence go through feelings of tense due to new peers, environment, free from parental control. From the developmental perspective, they belong to late adolescence period when they need to be judgemental and take an effective decision regarding future personal and professional life and solve their own problems on daily basis. In the newfound environment, they should take precautionary measures about problematic situations that they arrive with. Sometimes negative thought, stress and frustration overpower them and limit their abilities. Emotional control and understanding of own and others emotion, proper utilisation of emotion to take action might enable them to overcome the situation. Hence the development of EI and behavioural disposition of CT is important to them. A collective effect of such interaction may lead them to solve their problems effectively.

7. Delimitation of the Study

Nevertheless, the limitations of the study should be taken into account. The participants were selected from only first-year undergraduate students hence the result cannot be generalised for the entire undergraduate level. The same study further may be replicated with time-series design with longitudinal approaches and experimental settings. Data were collected using self-reported questionnaire which is subject to response bias.

Despite these limitations, the study may add the culture and population-specific outcomes regarding the association between EI and CT disposition of first-year undergraduate students which is lacking in the existing literature. Although there is mention of the non-significant moderating role of gender in relation to EI and CT skill, the present study highlights and endorses empirically the non-significant moderating role of gender in relation to EI and CT disposition that was missing in the literature.

Declarations

Author contribution statement

Sahanowas Sk, Santoshi Halder: Conceived and designed the experiments; Performed the experiments; Analyzed and interpreted the data; Wrote the paper.

Funding statement

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Data availability statement

The data that has been used is confidential.

Declaration of interests statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

No additional information is available for this paper.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the head of the institutions and participants for their willingness and voluntary participation.

Contributor Information

Sahanowas Sk, Email: sksahanowas@gmail.com.

Santoshi Halder, Email: santoshi_halder@yahoo.com.

References

  1. Aafreen M.M., Priya V.V., Gayathri R. Effect of stress on academic performance of students in different streams. Drug Invent. Today. 2018;10(9):1776–1780. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ababu G.B., Yigzaw A.B., Besene Y.D., Alemu W.G. Prevalence of adjustment problem and its predictors among first-year undergraduate students in Ethiopian university: a cross-sectional institution based study. Hindawi Psychiatr. J. 2018;1–7 doi: 10.1155/2018/5919743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aderi M., Jdaitawi M., Ishak N.A., Jdaitawi F. The influenceof demographic variables on university students’ adjustment in north Jordan. Int. Educ. Stud. 2013;6(2):172–178. [Google Scholar]
  4. Afshar H.S., Rahimi M. The relationship among critical thinking, emotional intelligence, and speaking sbilities of Iranian EFL learners. Procedia- Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014;136:75–79. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ahmad S., Bangash H., Khan S.A. Emotional intelligence and gender differences. Sarhad J. Agric. 2009;25(1):127–130. [Google Scholar]
  6. Ahmad M.A., Duskri M. Gender differences of mathematical critical thinking skills of secondary school students. J. Phys. Conf. 2018:1–6. [Google Scholar]
  7. Arbuckle J.L. Amos Development Corporation; Spring House, PA: 2005. AMOS 6.0 User’s Guide. [Google Scholar]
  8. Arockiasamy S. Doctoral dissertation, Manonmaniam Sundaranar University; Tamil Nadu, India: 2014. Emotional Intelligence, Critical Thinking and Stress Management of High School Teachers.http://hdl.handle.net/10603/90708 [Google Scholar]
  9. Atiq M.M., Farooq A., Ahmad H.M., Humayoun A.A. The impact of emotional intelligence on motivation, empowerment and organization structures: a case of universities in Islamabad. J. Basic Appl. Sci. Res. 2015;5(6):22–31. [Google Scholar]
  10. Awang Z. Penerbit Universiti Teknologi MARA; 2012. Structural Equation Modeling Using AMOS Graphic. [Google Scholar]
  11. Beaven-Marks K. Hypnotherapy Training Company; 2017. The ‘Locus of Control’ Scale.https://hypnotc.com/locus-of-control-scale-hypnotherapy/ [Google Scholar]
  12. Bennett D.S., Bendersky M., Lewis M. Antecedents of emotion knowledge: predictors of individual differences in young children. Cognit. Emot. 2005;19(3):375–396. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bensley D.A. Why great thinkers sometimes fail to think critically. Skeptical Inq. 2006;30:47–52. [Google Scholar]
  14. Brasseur S., Gre´goire J., Bourdu R., Mikolajczak M. The profile of emotional competence (PEC): development and validation of a self-reported measure that fits dimensions of emotional competence theory. PloS One. 2013;8(5):1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brookfield S. Jossey-Bass Publishers; San Francisco: 1987. Developing Critical Thinkers: Challenging Adults to Explore Alternative Ways of Thinking and Acting. [Google Scholar]
  16. Brackett M.A., Katulak N. The emotionally intelligent classroom: skill-based training for teachers and students. In: Ciarrochi J., Mayer J.D., editors. Improving Emotional Intelligence: A Practitioner's Guide. Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis; New York: 2007. pp. 2–27. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bratianu C. IGI Global; Hershey: 2015. Organizational knowledge dynamics: managing knowledge creation, acquisition, sharing, and transformation. [Google Scholar]
  18. Browne M.W., Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen K.A., Long J.S., editors. Testing Structural Equation Models. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
  19. Busari A.O. Identifying difference in Perceptions of academic stress and reaction to stressors based on gender among first-year University students. Int. J. Humanit. Soc. Sci. 2012;2(14):138–146. http://www.ijhssnet.com/journals/Vol_2_No_14_Special_Issue_July_2012/19.pdf [Google Scholar]
  20. Butler A., Pentoney C., Bong M.P. Predicting real-world outcomes: critical thinking ability is a better predictor of life decisions than intelligence heather. Think. Skills Creativ. 2017;25:38–46. [Google Scholar]
  21. Byrne B.M. second ed. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2006. Structural Equation Modelling with EQS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. [Google Scholar]
  22. Byrne B.M. second ed. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2010. Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. [Google Scholar]
  23. Calderon A. RMIT University; Melbourne, Australia: 2018. Massification of Higher Education Revisited.http://cdn02.pucp.education/academico/2018/08/23165810/na_mass_revis_230818.pdf [Google Scholar]
  24. Cartreine J. More than sad: depression affects your ability to think [Web log post] 2016. https://www.health.harvard.edu/blog/sad-depression-affects-ability-think_201605069551
  25. Caruso D.R., Mayer J.D., Salovey P. Emotional intelligence and emotional leadership. In: Riggio M., editor. Multiple Intelligences and Leadership: LEA’s Organization and Management Series. Lawrence Earlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2002. pp. 55–74. [Google Scholar]
  26. Cherry K. 2018. How Psychologists Evaluate Intelligence.https://www.verywellmind.com/theories-of-intelligence-2795035 [Google Scholar]
  27. Chin W.W., Todd P.A. On the use, usefulness, and ease of use of structural equation modeling in MIS research: a note of caution. MIS Q. 1995;18:237–246. [Google Scholar]
  28. Chinda T., Techapreechawong S., Teeraprasert S. 2012. An Investigation of Relationships between Employees’ Safety and Productivity.https://tinyurl.com/y9l3uofq [Google Scholar]
  29. Chowhan A., Sakshi, Ravees S. Gender differences in adjustment amongst the college students of Jammu city. Int. J. Appl. Res. 2019;5(8):17–20. http://www.allresearchjournal.com/archives/2019/vol5issue8/PartA/5-7-138-320.pdf [Google Scholar]
  30. Cosmides L., Tooby J. Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In: Lewis M., Haviland-Jones J.M., editors. Handbook of Emotions. second ed. Guilford Press; New York: 2000. pp. 91–115. [Google Scholar]
  31. Damasio A.R. A second chance for emotion. In: Lane R.D., Nadel L., editors. Series in Affective Science. Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion. Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 12–23. [Google Scholar]
  32. Damasio A.R. Penguin; New York: 2005. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain.https://tinyurl.com/y55o4xgf [Google Scholar]
  33. Deniz S. The relationship between emotional intelligence and problem solving skills in prospective teachers. Educ. Res. Rev. 2013;8(24):2339–2345. [Google Scholar]
  34. Dutoğlu G., Tuncel M. The relationshıp between candidate teachers’ critical thinking tendecies and their emotional intelligence levels. Abant İzzet Baysal Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi. 2008;8(1):11–32. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ebrahimi M.R., Moafian F. Does emotional intelligence or self–efficacy have something to do with high school English teachers' critical thinking, considering demographic information? Int. J. Ling. 2012;4(4):224–242. [Google Scholar]
  36. Elder L. Critical thinking and emotional intelligence. Inq. Crit. Think. Across Discip. 1996;16(2) https://tinyurl.com/bqahbmt [Google Scholar]
  37. Facione P.A. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED318936. 1990. Critical thinking: a statement of expert consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction: research findings and recommendations.https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED315423.pdf [Google Scholar]
  38. Facione P.A. The disposition toward critical thinking: its character, measurement, and relation to critical thinking skill. Informal Log. 2000;20(1):61–84. [Google Scholar]
  39. Facione P. 2010. Critical Thinking: what it Is and Why it Counts? Insight Assessment.https://www.nyack.edu/files/CT_What_Why_2013.pdf [Google Scholar]
  40. Facione P.A., Facione N., Giancarlo C. California Academic Press; Millbrae, CA: 2001. California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory: CCTDI Inventory Manual. [Google Scholar]
  41. Fernández-Berrocal P., Cabello R., Castillo R., Extremera N. Gender differences in emotional intelligence: the mediating effect of age. Behavioral Psychology/Psicología Conductual. 2012;20:77–89. [Google Scholar]
  42. Fiori M., Vesely-Maillefer A.K. Correction to: emotional intelligence as an ability: theory, challenges, and new directions. In: Keefer K., Parker J., Saklofske D., editors. Emotional Intelligence in Education. The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality. Springer; Cham: 2019. [Google Scholar]
  43. Franco A.R., Costa P.S., Almeida L.S. Do critical thinkers drink too much alcohol, forget to do class assignments, or cheat on exams? Using a critical thinking measure to predict college students’ real-world outcomes. Psychol. Stud. 2017;62(2):178–187. [Google Scholar]
  44. Gaskin J. 2012. Stats Wiki and Stats Tools Package.http://statwiki.kolobkreations.com/ [Google Scholar]
  45. Gefen D.R., Fish M.C. Gender differences in stress and coping in first-year college students. J. Coll. Orientat. Trans. 2012;19(2):18–31. [Google Scholar]
  46. George J.M. Emotions and leadership: the role of emotional intelligence. Hum. Relat. 2000;53:1027–1055. [Google Scholar]
  47. Ghanizadeh A., Moafian F. Critical thinking and emotional intelligence: investigating the relationship among EFL learners and the contribution of age and gender. Iran. J. Appl. Lingu. 2011;14(1):23–48. [Google Scholar]
  48. Goleman D. Working with emotional intelligence. Bantam Books; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  49. Goroshit M. Academic procrastination and academic performance: an initial basis for intervention. J. Prev. Interv. Community. 2018;46(2):131–142. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Government of India . Ministry of Human Resource Development, Department of Higher Education; New Delhi: 2018. All India Survey on Higher Education 2017-18.https://epsiindia.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/AISHE-2017-18.pdf [Google Scholar]
  51. Gross J.J. Emotion regulation. In: Lewis M., Haviland-Jones J.M., Barrett L.F., editors. Handbook of Emotions. third ed. Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 497–512. [Google Scholar]
  52. Gu S., Wang F., Patel N.P., Bourgeois J.A., Huang J.H. A model for basic emotions using observations of behavior in Drosophila. Front. Psychol. 2019;10 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00781. Article 781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Haghani F., Aminian B., Kamali F., Jamshidian S. Critical thinking skills and their relationship with emotional intelligence in medical students of introductory clinical medicine (ICM) course in Isfahan University of Medical Sciences. Iran. J. Med. Educ. 2011;10(5):1–12. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hasanpour M., Bagheri M., Heidari F.G. The relationship between emotional intelligence and critical thinking skills in Iranian nursing students. Med. J. Islam. Repub. Iran. 2018;32(40):1–4. doi: 10.14196/mjiri.32.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hashemiannejad F., Oloomi S., Oloomi S. Examine the relationship between critical thinking and happiness and social adjustment. Int. Acad. J. Soc. Sci. 2016;3(6):42–47. [Google Scholar]
  56. Heidari M., Ebrahimi P. Examining the relationship between critical-thinking skills and decision-making ability of emergency medicine students. Indian J. Crit. Care Med. 2016;20(10):581–586. doi: 10.4103/0972-5229.192045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Heilman R.M., Crisan L.G., Houser D., Miclea M., Miu A.C. Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion. 2010;10(2):257–265. doi: 10.1037/a0018489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hooper D., Coughlan J., Mullen M.R. Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electron. J. Bus. Res. Methods. 2008;6(1):53–60. [Google Scholar]
  59. Irani T., Rudd R., Gallo M., Ricke_ s J., Friedel C., Rhoades E. 2007. Critical Thinking Instrumentation Manual.http://step.ufl.edu/resources/critical_thinking/ctmanual.pdf [Google Scholar]
  60. Jain P. Adjustment among college students. Int. J. Adv. Sci, Res. Eng. 2017;3(5):10–12. [Google Scholar]
  61. Jang D., Elfenbein H.A. Emotion, perception and expression of. In: Wright James D., editor. second ed. Vol. 7. Elsevier; Oxford: 2015. pp. 483–489. (International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences). [Google Scholar]
  62. Joseph D.L., Newman D.A. Emotional intelligence: an integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. J. Appl. Psychol. 2010;95:54–78. doi: 10.1037/a0017286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Joubert C.P. University of South Africa; 2015. The Relationship between Procrastination and Academic Achievement of High School Learners in North West Province, South Africa.https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/d568/a1ba488c1059107b63a88346c747533d33f6.pdf Masters dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  64. Kang F. Contribution of emotional intelligence towards graduate students’ critical thinking disposition. Int. J. Educ. Literacy Stud. 2015;3(4):6–17. [Google Scholar]
  65. Kant R. Emotional intelligence: a study on university students. J. Educ. Learn. (EduLearn) 2019;13(4):441–446. [Google Scholar]
  66. Kaya H., Şenyuva E., Bodur G. The relationship between critical thinking and emotional intelligence in nursing students: a longitudinal study. Nurse Educ. Today. 2018;68:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2018.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Khandaghi M.A., Pakmehr H. Critical thinking disposition: a neglected loop of humanities curriculum in higher education. Cyp. J. Educ. Sci. 2012;7(1):1–13. [Google Scholar]
  68. Khandaghi M.A., Pakmehr H., Amiri E. The status of college students' critical thinking disposition in humanities. Proced. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011;15:1866–1869. [Google Scholar]
  69. Kim H. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Open Lect. Statist. 2013:52–54. doi: 10.5395/rde.2013.38.1.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kivetz R. The effects of effort and intrinsic motivation on risky choice. Market. Sci. 2003;22(4):477–502. [Google Scholar]
  71. Lakshmi N. Influence of emotional intelligence on communication skills: a survey and selective analysis. J. English Lang. Lit. 2016;3(2):97–102. [Google Scholar]
  72. Leasa M. The correlation between emotional intelligence and critical thinking skills with different learning styles in science learning. AIP Conf. Proceed. 2018;2014 020135 (2018) [Google Scholar]
  73. Levine D.S. Modeling the instinctive-emotional-thoughtful mind. Cognit. Syst. Res. 2017;45:82–94. [Google Scholar]
  74. Leykin Y., Roberts C.S., DeRubeis R.J. Decision-making and depressive symptomatology. Cognit. Ther. Res. 2011;35:333–341. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9308-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lowe H., Cook A. Mind the gap: are students prepared for higher education? J. Furth. High. Educ. 2003;27(1):53–76. [Google Scholar]
  76. Lismayani I., Parno, Mahanal S. The correlation of critical thinking skill and science problem-solving ability of junior high school students. J Pendidikan Sains. 2017;5(3):96–101. [Google Scholar]
  77. Madadkhani Z., Nikoogoftar M. Critical thinking in nurses: predictive role of emotional intelligence. J. Hayat. 2015;20(4):77–88. [Google Scholar]
  78. Mahal R., Chawla A., Kanwar V. Critical thinking as a correlate of stress management among rural adolescent girls. Adv. Res. J. Soc. Sci. 2015;6(1):32–35. [Google Scholar]
  79. Marzuki N.A., Mustaffa C.S., Saad Z.M. Emotional Intelligence: its relationship with communication and information technology skills. Asian Soc. Sci. 2015;11(15):267–274. [Google Scholar]
  80. Mayer J.D., Salovey P., Caruso D. Emotional intelligence: theory, findings, and implications. Psychol. Inq. 2004;15(3):197–215. [Google Scholar]
  81. Mayer J.D. What emotional intelligence is and is not. The Personality Analyst. 2009. https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/the-personality-analyst/200909/what-emotional-intelligence-is-and-is-not
  82. Mayer J.D., Salovey P., Caruso D. Multi-Health Systems; Toronto, Canada: 2002. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Version 2.0. [Google Scholar]
  83. McLaughlin C. The feeling of finding out: the role of emotions in research. Educ. Action Res. 2003;11(1):65–78. [Google Scholar]
  84. Meshkat M., Nejati R. Does emotional intelligence depend on gender? A study on undergraduate English majors of three Iranian universities. Saga open. 2017;7(3):1–8. [Google Scholar]
  85. Millet P. Luleå University of Technology Sweden; 2005. Locus of Control and its Relation to Working Life: Studies from the fields of Vocational Rehabilitation and Small Firms in Sweden.http://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:991064/FULLTEXT01.pdf Doctoral dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  86. Muhammed K., Terna I.G., Saanyol D.B. The relationship between depression and academic performance among undergraduate students of Benue State University Makurdi, Nigeria. Int. J. Educ. Eval. 2018;4(6):77–85. [Google Scholar]
  87. Naghavi F., Redzuan M. The relationship between gender and emotional intelligence. World Appl. Sci. J. 2011;15:555–561. [Google Scholar]
  88. Nasir M., Masrur R. An exploration of emotional intelligence of the students of IIUI in relation to gender, age and academic achievement. Bull. Educ. Res. 2010;23:37–51. [Google Scholar]
  89. Noohi E., Karimi-Noghondar M., Haghdoost A. Survey of Critical Thinking and clinical decision making in nursing student of Kerman University. Iran. J. Nurs. Midwifery Res. 2012;17(6):440–444. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Nosratinia M., Sarabchian E. Predicting EFL learners’ emotional Intelligence and critical thinking ability through big-five personality traits: a study on psychological characteristics of EFL learners. Int. J. Sci. Eng. Res. 2013;4(9):500–515. [Google Scholar]
  91. Omoniyi M.B.I., Adelowo A.I. Relationship between locus of control, emotional intelligence and subjective happiness among widows: implications for psychological mental health. Br. J. Arts Soc. Sci. 2014:119–128. http://www.bjournal.co.uk/volume/paper/BJASS_18_2/BJASS_18_02_05.pdf [Google Scholar]
  92. Pinker S. 1997. How the Mind Works: in Emotion and Critical Thinking.http://www.nwlink.com/∼donclark/knowledge/emotions.html [Google Scholar]
  93. Pons F., Harris P.L., de Rosnay M. Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: developmental periods and hierarchical organization. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2004;1:127–152. [Google Scholar]
  94. Ricketts J.C., Rudd R.D. The relationship between critical thinking dispositions and critical thinking skills of selected youth leaders in the National FFA Organization. J. South. Agri. Educ. Res. 2004;54(1):21–33. [Google Scholar]
  95. Ricketts J.C., Rudd R.D. Critical thinking of selected you thleaders: the efficacy of critical thinking dispositions, leadership and academic performance. J. Agric. Educ. 2005;46(1):32–43. [Google Scholar]
  96. Rivers S.E., Brackett M.A., Omori M., Sickler S. Emotion skills as a protective factor for risky behaviors among college students. J. Coll. Student Dev. 2013;54(2):172–183. [Google Scholar]
  97. Rodzalan S.A., Saat M.M. The perception of critical thinking and problem solving skill among Malaysian Undergraduate students. Proced. So. Behav. Sci. 2015;172:725–732. [Google Scholar]
  98. Roshani P., Bagrecha C. A structural equation model for work-life balance of women nursing professionals of government and private hospitals in Bangalore. Amity J Healthcare Manag. 2017;2(2):1–12. [Google Scholar]
  99. Saklofske D.H., Austin E.J., Galloway J., Davidson K. Individual difference correlates of health-related behaviors: preliminary evidence for links between emotional intelligence and coping. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2007;42:491–502. [Google Scholar]
  100. Salovey P., Bedell B.T., Detweiler J.B., Mayer J.D. Current directions in emotional intelligence research. In: Lewis M., Haviland-Jones J.M., editors. Handbook of Emotions. Guilford Press; New York: 2000. pp. 504–520. [Google Scholar]
  101. Salovey P., Stroud L.R., Woolery A., Epel E.S. Perceived emotional intelligence, stress reactivity, and symptom reports: further explorations using the trait meta-mood scale. Psychol. Health. 2002;17:611–627. [Google Scholar]
  102. Saremi H., Bahdori S. The relationship between critical thinking with emotional intelligence and creativity among elementary school principals in Bojnord city, Iran. Int. J. Life Sci. 2015;9(6):33–40. [Google Scholar]
  103. San Y.L., Roslan S.B., Sabouripour F. Relationship between self-regulated learning and academic procrastination. Am. J. Appl. Sci. 2016;13(4):459–466. [Google Scholar]
  104. Scherer K.R., Mortillaro M., Mehu M. Understanding the mechanisms underlying the production of facial expression of emotion: a componential perspective. Emot. Rev. 2013;5:47–53. [Google Scholar]
  105. Shehzad S., Mahmood N. Gender differences in emotional intelligence of university teachers. Pakistan J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2013;11(1):16–21. [Google Scholar]
  106. Singh M., Goel N.K., Sharma M.K., Bakshi R.K. Prevalence of depression, anxiety and stress among students of Punjab University, Chandigarh. Natl. J. Commum. Med. 2017;8(11):666–671. [Google Scholar]
  107. Stedman N.L.P., Andenoro A.C. Identification of relationships between emotional intelligence skill & critical thinking disposition in undergraduate leadership students. J. Leader. Educ. 2007;6(1):190–208. [Google Scholar]
  108. Steiger J.H. Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modelling. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2007;42(5):893–898. [Google Scholar]
  109. Sulaiman T., Kuppusamy S.K., Ayub A.F.F., Rahim S.S.A. Relationship between critical thinking disposition and teaching efficacy among special education integration program teachers in Malaysia. AIP Conf. Proceed. 2017;1795 2017. [Google Scholar]
  110. Snyder L.G., Snyder M.J. Teaching critical thinking and problem solving skills. Delta Pi Epsilon J. 2008;50:90–99. [Google Scholar]
  111. Tabachnick B.G., Fidell L.S. fifth ed. Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education; Boston, MA: 2007. Using Multivariate Statistics. [Google Scholar]
  112. Taghva F., Rezaei N., Ghaderi J., Taghva R. Studying the relationship between critical thinking skills and students’ educational achievement (Eghlid Universities as Case Study) Int. Lett. Soc. Humanist. Sci. 2014;25:18–25. [Google Scholar]
  113. Turan H. Comparison of critical thinking dispositions of prospective teachers. Educ. Res. Rev. 2016;11(8):867–876. [Google Scholar]
  114. Upadhyaya P. Personality of emotionally intelligent student-teachers. J. Educ. Stud. 2006;4(1):37–39. [Google Scholar]
  115. Vahedi S., Heydarlou M.M., Imanzadeh A. The relationship between emotional intelligence and critical thinking in nursing students of Tabriz University of Medical Sciences. Develop. Strate. Med. Educ. 2015;2(1):47–57. [Google Scholar]
  116. Valerio K.M. Intrinsic motivation in the classroom. J. Stud. Eng.: Educ. Matt. 2012;2(1):30–35. [Google Scholar]
  117. Xiong B., Skitmore M., Xia B. A critical review of structural equation modeling applications in construction research. Autom. ConStruct. 2015;49(Pt. A):59–70. [Google Scholar]
  118. Yamane T. 2nd Ed. Harper and Row; 1967. Statistics, An Introductory Analysis. [Google Scholar]
  119. Yen Y., Rebok G.W., Gallo J.J., Jones R.N., Tennstedt S.L. Depressive symptoms impair everyday problem-solving ability through cognitive abilities in late life. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 2011;19(2):142–150. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181e89894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data that has been used is confidential.


Articles from Heliyon are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES