Abstract
Background
Various online rumors have led to inappropriate behaviors among the public in response to the COVID-19 epidemic in China. These rumors adversely affect people’s physical and mental health. Therefore, a better understanding of the relationship between public emotions and rumors during the epidemic may help generate useful strategies for guiding public emotions and dispelling rumors.
Objective
This study aimed to explore whether public emotions are related to the dissemination of online rumors in the context of COVID-19.
Methods
We used the web-crawling tool Scrapy to gather data published by People’s Daily on Sina Weibo, a popular social media platform in China, after January 8, 2020. Netizens’ comments under each Weibo post were collected. Nearly 1 million comments thus collected were divided into 5 categories: happiness, sadness, anger, fear, and neutral, based on the underlying emotional information identified and extracted from the comments by using a manual identification process. Data on rumors spread online were collected through Tencent’s Jiaozhen platform. Time-lagged cross-correlation analyses were performed to examine the relationship between public emotions and rumors.
Results
Our results indicated that the angrier the public felt, the more rumors there would likely be (r=0.48, P<.001). Similar results were observed for the relationship between fear and rumors (r=0.51, P<.001) and between sadness and rumors (r=0.47, P<.001). Furthermore, we found a positive correlation between happiness and rumors, with happiness lagging the emergence of rumors by 1 day (r=0.56, P<.001). In addition, our data showed a significant positive correlation between fear and fearful rumors (r=0.34, P=.02).
Conclusions
Our findings confirm that public emotions are related to the rumors spread online in the context of COVID-19 in China. Moreover, these findings provide several suggestions, such as the use of web-based monitoring methods, for relevant authorities and policy makers to guide public emotions and behavior during this public health emergency.
Keywords: public emotions, rumor, infodemic, infodemiology, infoveillance, China, COVID-19
Introduction
In December 2019, the earliest cases of COVID-19 were reported in Wuhan, Hubei Province, China. On January 23, 2020, several cities in Hubei Province were quarantined in an attempt to slow down community transmission of the disease. On January 30, 2020, the World Health Organization officially announced that the COVID-19 outbreak was a public health emergency of international concern [1]. As COVID-19 has become a serious global problem, people all over the world are faced with the risk of infection; this has caused a change in people’s behaviors as well as fluctuations in their emotions [2]. During COVID-19 outbreaks in different parts of the world, there was a proliferation of emotional comments and occasional rumors posted on the internet. Although a number of epidemiological and clinical studies have been performed thus far, few studies have examined public emotional response, and to our knowledge, no study has investigated whether public emotions are related to the occasional online rumors spread in the context of COVID-19. In this study, we seek to contribute to the literature by addressing this gap in the existing research.
Rumors have abounded since ancient times, but they have been the focus of researchers only since the Second World War [3]. In a pioneer study, Prasad [4] proposed that rumors are spread due to the anxiety and fear of unknown things in a disaster. Allport and Postman [5] suggested that the spread of rumors is a process of releasing emotions by telling stories to others. The lack of barriers of the internet enables individuals to participate in online discussions anonymously, which creates a favorable environment for the spread of rumors [6]. Some empirical studies have suggested that among the messages forwarded on the internet by users, those with emotional elements (anger, sadness, or anxiety) were 36.7% more frequent than those without emotional elements [7] and that people are more likely to believe online rumors with the same type of emotion that they currently feel. That is, angry people are more receptive to anger-related rumors [8]. However, until now, online rumors have not been extensively researched. All prior research has focused on analyzing rumors generated due to anxiety and fear. It is unclear whether other emotions are related to the spread of rumors. Additionally, no empirical studies have examined whether there is a significant correlation between emotions and rumors in the context of major public health emergencies.
In the context of COVID-19, various online rumors have led to inappropriate behaviors among people in response to the epidemic, which have adversely affected people’s physical and mental health [9]. We believe that the findings of this study can provide some useful strategies to guide public emotions and dismiss rumors, in an effort to fight this global crisis.
Previous studies on public emotions during major public health emergencies have found that people usually experience negative emotions such as panic, anxiety, anger, grief, and disgust [10,11], likely due to the illnesses and deaths that occur during these emergencies. For example, 17 years ago, a study on SARS found that tension, helplessness, panic, and anger were significantly related to an increase in the number of SARS cases [12]. Some other studies also showed that public anxiety changed with the development of the epidemic (eg, avian influenza A [H7N9] or influenza A [H1N1]) [13,14]. In line with some previous studies that used data from different sources [15,16], we aimed to examine the dynamic relationship between the public’s emotions and rumors in the context of COVID-19, by using data from China’s mainstream internet media platforms (Sina Weibo and Tencent). According to a report released by the China Internet Network Information Center, as of March 2020, there were 904 million internet users and the internet penetration rate in China had reached 64.5% [17]. Hence, the data obtained through the online responses from netizens are considered to be representative of the public’s response to major health emergencies [18]. Moreover, social media surveillance can provide real-time information on the public’s response; solve the problem of underrepresentation of samples; and avoid recall or reporting bias caused by personal observation, collection, and recording [19,20]. According to cognitive dissonance theory [21], rumors may serve as a channel for the public to reduce or eliminate cognitive dissonance caused by emotions. In this study, we aimed to explore whether the public’s emotions are related to the generation and dissemination of rumors in the context of COVID-19.
Methods
Epidemiological Data
On January 20, 2020, China launched a monitoring and quarantine system to record daily information about COVID-19 cases. The epidemiological data, issued by the National Health Commission of People’s Republic of China (NHC), were collected at the grassroots level on a daily basis. For this study, we used the epidemiological data (specifically, the daily number of newly confirmed COVID-19 cases) from all provincial-level regions of China that were released by the NHC from January 20 to March 10, 2020.
Data on Public Emotions
Sina Weibo, the Chinese version of Twitter, is the largest social media platform in China. People’s Daily, with more than 116 million followers on Weibo, is one of the most influential and authoritative news media on Sina Weibo. In this study, we used the web crawler Scrapy to gather relevant data published by People’s Daily on Weibo between January 20 and March 8, 2020, as well as netizens’ comments under each Weibo post. Millions of real-time comment texts contain substantial emotional information; we identified and extracted these public emotions from the comments collected.
Although many sentiment analysis tools based on natural language processing or machine learning [22,23] are able to automatically extract emotions from the text by utilizing less time and labor, the accuracy of machine identification is lower than that of manual identification. Some simple classifications, such as positive or negative, can be detected through machine identification; however, for more subtle discrete emotions, such as anger, fear, and happiness, manual identification would yield better accuracy. Thus, we adopted manual identification methods in this study. Recently, Jack et al [24] reported that people universally experience only 4 basic emotions: happiness, sadness, fear, and anger. In this study, public emotions were divided into 5 categories: the 4 abovementioned basic human emotions (ie, happiness, sadness, fear, and anger) and “neutral” (ie, representing no emotions). A total of 143 trained research assistants participated in the manual identification process. Each online comment was identified and classified by at least 3 trained research assistants, and conflicting classification results were resolved by a majority consensus to arrive at the final classification. The interrater reliability of the coders was “substantial” (κ=0.77) [25].
Data on Emotional Rumors
Tencent’s Jiaozhen [26] is the first network-wide, professional, and timely fact-checking platform in China that checks internet news that has been widely circulated and dispels rumors on a daily basis. Data on online rumors about COVID-19 disseminated between January 20 and March 10, 2020, were collected through this platform, and manual identification was performed to identify all kinds of emotional rumors spread during this period.
Data Calibration and Statistical Analysis
Figure 1A shows that the daily number of newly confirmed cases in Hubei Province suddenly surged on February 12, 2020, when the diagnosis criteria were revised. Thereafter, the COVID-19 diagnosis criteria used in Hubei was consistent with those outside the province. Therefore, the epidemiological data of newly confirmed cases in Hubei that were collected before February 14 were calibrated before the follow-up analysis.
According to the Hubei Health Committee, on February 12 and 13, 2020, the ratios of COVID-19 cases that were clinically diagnosed and those that were detected based on nucleic acid testing were 8.44 (13,332/1580) and 1.79 (3095/1728), respectively. Most of these cases were a result of long-term accumulation of suspected cases. The decline in the number of clinically diagnosed cases on February 13, 2020, suggested that the cumulative suspected cases had been processed within those 2 days. The corresponding ratios of COVID-19 cases that were clinically diagnosed and those detected via nucleic acid testing on February 14 and 15, 2020, were 0.89 (1138/1282) and 0.93 (888/955), which average at 0.91. Therefore, from January 20 to February 13, 2020, the calibrated daily number of newly confirmed cases (as shown in Figure 1B) was the number of cases confirmed by nucleic acid detection multiplied by (1+0.91).
As the data on daily epidemic situations and emotional comments published on Weibo showed an exponential distribution, the data were log-transformed to stabilize the variance of time-series before conducting further analysis.
We used time-lagged cross-correlation to examine the relationship between public emotions and the total number of rumors and that between public emotions and the rumors with different types of emotions. We also used Pearson correlation coefficient to examine these relationships, with the maximum time lag of 10 days and a P value smaller than .05 as a threshold.
Results
Rumors in the Context of the COVID-19 Epidemic
Among the 276 rumors collected, 176 (63.8%) were neutral rumors, 62 (22.5%) were fear-related rumors, 19 (6.9%) were happiness-related rumors, 12 (4.4%) were anger-related rumors, and 7 (2.5%) were sadness-related rumors. Figure 2A shows the relationship between the COVID-19 epidemic situation outside Hubei Province and online rumors. The peak correlation was 0.59 (P<.001) when the lag value was 0 days, indicating that the daily number of newly confirmed cases was significantly positively correlated with rumors, with changes occurring simultaneously. Figure 2B shows the relationship between the COVID-19 epidemic situation in Hubei Province and online rumors. The peak correlation was 0.55 (P<.001) with a lag of -1 day, which suggests that the total number of rumors changed 1 day in advance of the change in the number of cases in Hubei Province. In addition, the epidemic situation within and outside Hubei Province was not significantly related to the 4 types of emotional rumors.
Relationship Between Public Emotions and Rumors
During the study period, anger (2,248,362/17,328,675, 12.97%) dominated public emotions on the internet, followed by fear (627,407/17,328,675, 3.62%), happiness (216,072/17,328,675, 1.25%), and sadness (195,708/17,328,675, 1.13%). Figure 3 presents the trend of public emotions on Weibo, which shows that the number of anger-related and fear-related comments were more than the number of happiness-related and sadness-related comments almost every day.
Next, we found that the peak correlation of anger and rumors was 0.48 (P<.001) with a lag of 0 day (Figure 4A). That is, the angrier the public got, the more rumors there would be. Similarly, the time-lagged analysis between fear and rumors (Figure 4B) and between sadness and rumors (Figure 4D) also indicated significant positive correlations among these 2 emotions and rumors. The results showed the correlations for both these emotions reached their peaks of 0.51 (P<.001) and 0.47 (P<.001), respectively, with the lag of 0 days. Additionally, a positive correlation was observed between happiness and rumors (r=0.56, P<.001, Figure 4C), with a time lag of 1 day. That is, happiness would lag the emergence of rumors by 1 day.
Finally, we analyzed the relationship between the 4 types of emotions and different types of rumors. Figure 5 shows that there was a positive correlation only between fear and fearful rumors, with the peak correlation (r=0.34, P=.02)) observed at the lag of 0 day.
Discussion
Principal Results
To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the relationship between public emotions and rumors during COVID-19 by using a web-based monitoring method. A large number of comments and rumors related to COVID-19 were obtained from the Weibo account of People’s Daily and Tencent Myth Busters by using the web crawler Scrapy. This web-based monitoring study overcomes the limitations of traditional survey methods and facilitates data-collection in a rapid and real-time manner during outbreak of a public health emergency. In particular, during a large-scale quarantine period, people are more likely to use the internet and social media to acquire and propagate the latest information about the epidemic [7], as well as actively participate in related topic discussions [27]. The internet and social media have become important platforms for real-time monitoring and assessment of changes in public emotions and behaviors. In addition, compared with traditional methods such as survey and telephone interviews, web-based monitoring tends to be more objective and authentic, effectively avoiding measurement errors caused by self-reporting or recalling [14,28]. Manual identification was employed to code emotional categories, which also improved the classification accuracy of public emotion identification [22,23].
In this study, statistically significant relationships were first observed among the daily number of newly confirmed cases and the total number of rumors both within and outside of Hubei Province, which is in line with a previous study that found that the uncertainty of sudden infectious diseases can lead to a large number of rumors [18]. In another study, Li et al [29] analyzed the emotional reactions of 4607 participants through an online survey and found that the COVID-19 did not lead to changes in the frequency of public emotions. However, our study found that the public expressed more anger and fear than happiness and sadness, and there were several large fluctuations in public emotions. Some researchers have suggested that rumors are an outlet for the public to express fear or pressure after a disaster, and spreading rumors could become an effective way for the public to express their emotions such as anger, fear, and sadness [30]. Our findings are in line with these previous findings. The results of our study revealed that three emotions (ie, anger, fear, and sadness) were positively correlated to the total number of rumors, and the emotions and rumors changed synchronously. Moreover, we found that the increase in the happiness emotion tended to lag the emergence of rumors by 1 day. Previous studies have found that public emotions usually transform from negative to positive after the rumors are disproved [31]. In line with these findings, we observed that when the rumors were verified and clarified, there was a proportional increase in the public’s happiness.
Regarding the relationship between the 4 basic human emotions and 4 different types of emotional rumors, our results showed that fearful rumors were positively associated with fear. This result supports the argument presented by of Na et al [8], which claims the consistency between an individual’s emotional state and the emotional information in rumors could increase the prevalence of the rumors. However, our findings regarding anger and anger-related rumors were inconsistent with their conclusion that anger could lead the public to accept anger-related rumors [8]. This difference might be attributed to the differences of sample choice: our study focused on a Chinese sample, whereas Na et al [8] analyzed an American sample; hence, cultural differences in the relationship between public emotions and rumors also need to be further studied.
Study Limitations
This study, however, has several limitations. First, this study only focused on Sina Weibo for data collection. Other media platforms, such as WeChat in China and Twitter in other countries, were not included to acquire more extensive data. Second, batch-fetching data can only be obtained as a whole; hence, we cannot investigate the change in public emotions at an individual level. In future research, we may use other methods such as surveys or interviews to examine the trajectories of public emotions at an individual level. Third, this study analyzed the relationship between rumors and 4 basic human emotions, and further research could be extended to examine the correlations between rumors and more complex emotions such as anxiety.
Conclusions
Our findings provide several suggestions for relevant authorities and policy makers in guiding emotions of the public during public health emergencies. First, during a large-scale quarantine period, the authorities can use web-based monitoring methods to identify public emotions and behaviors in real time and provide timely guidance to channel public emotions and behaviors. Second, rumors are a catalyst for public emotions, and disproving them in a timely manner would be helpful to increase positive emotions of the public. Third, our findings showed that fearful rumors were associated with fear. Thus, media platforms should strengthen the monitoring of online rumors, identify and verify emotional rumors in a timely manner, and minimize the spread of fearful rumors to reduce fear among the public.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all the research assistants who coded the online comments. Funding was provided by the National Social Science Foundation of China (No. 16CTQ029).
Abbreviations
- NHC
The National Health Commission of People’s Republic of China
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest: None declared.
References
- 1.Novel Coronavirus (2019-nCoV) Situation Report - 10. 2020. World Health Organzation. 2020. [2020-04-28]. https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/coronaviruse/situation-reports/20200130-sitrep-10-ncov.pdf?sfvrsn=d0b2e480_2.
- 2.Xiao C A novel approach of consultation on 2019 novel coronavirus (COVID-19)-related psychological and mental problems: structured letter therapy. Psychiatry investigation. 2020;17(2):175. doi: 10.30773/pi.2020.0047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rosnow R, Foster E. Rumor and gossip research. Psychological Science Agenda. 2005;19(4):1–2. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Prasad J. The psychology of rumour: a study relating to the great Indian earthquake of 1934. British Journal of Psychology. 1935 Jul;26(1):1–15. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1935.tb00770.x. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1935.tb00770.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Allport GW, Postman L. The Psychology of Rumor. New York: Henry Holt and Company; 1947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Marett K, Joshi K. The decision to share information and rumors: examining the Role of motivation in an online discussion forum. CAIS. 2009 Feb;24(1):4. doi: 10.17705/1cais.02404. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chua A, Tee C, Pang A, Lim EP. The Retransmission of Rumor-related Tweets: Characteristics of Source and Message. SMSociety '16: Proceedings of the 7th 2016 International Conference on Social Media & Society; July 11-13, 2016; London, United Kingdom. 2020. Jul, pp. 1–10. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Na K, Garrett RK, Slater MD. Rumor acceptance during public health crises: testing the emotional congruence hypothesis. Journal of Health Communication. 2018 Oct 16;23(8):791–799. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2018.1527877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tasnim S, Hossain M, Mazumder H. Impact of rumors and misinformation on COVID-19 in social media. J Prev Med Public Health. 2020 May;53(3):171–174. doi: 10.3961/jpmph.20.094. doi: 10.3961/jpmph.20.094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yang JZ, Chu H. Who is afraid of the Ebola outbreak? The influence of discrete emotions on risk perception. Journal of Risk Research. 2016 Oct 28;21(7):834–853. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2016.1247378. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Person B, Sy F, Holton K, Govert B, Liang A, National Center for Inectious Diseases/SARS Community Outreach Team Fear and stigma: the epidemic within the SARS outbreak. Emerg Infect Dis. 2004 Feb;10(2):358–63. doi: 10.3201/eid1002.030750. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/15030713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Qian M, Dongmei YE, Dong W. Behaviour, cognition and emotion of the public in Beijing towards SARS. Chinese Mental Health Journal. 2003;17(8):515–520. doi: 10.1023/A:1022289509702. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Liao Q, Wu P, Wing Tak Lam W, Cowling BJ, Fielding R. Trajectories of public psycho-behavioural responses relating to influenza A(H7N9) over the winter of 2014-15 in Hong Kong. Psychol Health. 2019 Feb;34(2):162–180. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2018.1515436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bults M, Beaujean DJ, de Zwart O, Kok G, van Empelen P, van Steenbergen JE, Richardus JH, Voeten HA. Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health. 2011 Jan 03;11:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-2. https://bmcpublichealth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2458-11-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Li Z, Liu T, Zhu G, Lin H, Zhang Y, He J, Deng A, Peng Z, Xiao J, Rutherford S, Xie R, Zeng W, Li X, Ma W. Dengue Baidu Search Index data can improve the prediction of local dengue epidemic: A case study in Guangzhou, China. PLoS Negl Trop Dis. 2017 Mar;11(3):e0005354. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0005354. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pntd.0005354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Liu K, Wang T, Yang Z, Huang X, Milinovich GJ, Lu Y, Jing Q, Xia Y, Zhao Z, Yang Y, Tong S, Hu W, Lu J. Using Baidu Search Index to Predict Dengue Outbreak in China. Sci Rep. 2016 Dec 01;6:38040. doi: 10.1038/srep38040. doi: 10.1038/srep38040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.The 45th China Statistical Report on Internet Development in China in Chinese. China Internet Network Information Center. 2020. [2020-04-28]. http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/202004/P020200428399188064169.pdf.
- 18.Gu H, Chen B, Zhu H, Jiang T, Wang X, Chen L, Jiang Z, Zheng D, Jiang J. Importance of internet surveillance in public health emergency control and prevention: evidence from a digital epidemiologic study during avian influenza A H7N9 outbreaks. J Med Internet Res. 2014 Jan 17;16(1):e20. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2911. https://www.jmir.org/2014/1/e20/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chew C, Eysenbach G. Pandemics in the age of Twitter: content analysis of tweets during the 2009 H1N1 outbreak. PLoS One. 2010 Nov 29;5(11):e14118. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014118. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Signorini A, Segre AM, Polgreen PM. The use of Twitter to track levels of disease activity and public concern in the U.S. during the influenza A H1N1 pandemic. PLoS One. 2011 May 04;6(5):e19467. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019467. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Festinger L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Vol. 2. Redwood City, California, USA: Stanford University Press; 1957. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Singh NK, Tomar DS, Sangaiah AK. Sentiment analysis: a review and comparative analysis over social media. J Ambient Intell Human Comput. 2018 May 23;11(1):97–117. doi: 10.1007/s12652-018-0862-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mäntylä MV, Graziotin D, Kuutila M. The evolution of sentiment analysis—A review of research topics, venues, and top cited papers. Computer Science Review. 2018 Feb;27:16–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cosrev.2017.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jack RE, Sun W, Delis I, Garrod OGB, Schyns PG. Four not six: revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016 Jun;145(6):708–30. doi: 10.1037/xge0000162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977 Mar;33(1):159. doi: 10.2307/2529310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tencent Jiaozhen platform. 2020. [2020-04-28]. https://vp.fact.qq.com/home.
- 27.Henrich N, Holmes B. What the public was saying about the H1N1 vaccine: perceptions and issues discussed in on-line comments during the 2009 H1N1 pandemic. PLoS One. 2011 Apr 18;6(4):e18479. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018479. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rubin GJ, Amlôt Richard, Page L, Wessely S. Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey. BMJ. 2009 Jul 02;339:b2651. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2651. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/19574308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Li J, Yang A, Dou K, Wang L, Zhang M, Lin X. Chinese public's knowledge, perceived severity, and perceived controllability of COVID-19 and their associations with emotional and behavioural reactions, social participation, and precautionary behaviour: a national survey. BMC Public Health. 2020 Oct 21;20(1):1589. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09695-1. https://bmcpublichealth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12889-020-09695-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rosnow RL. Rumor as communication: a contextualist approach. Journal of Communication. 1988 Mar;38(1):12–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1988.tb02033.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zeng R, Zhu D. A model and simulation of the emotional contagion of netizens in the process of rumor refutation. Sci Rep. 2019 Oct 02;9(1):14164. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50770-4. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50770-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]