Skip to main content
The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America logoLink to The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
. 2020 Nov 25;148(5):3117–3130. doi: 10.1121/10.0002485

Early aging and postural control while listening and responding

Karen S Helfer 1,a),, Richard van Emmerik 2, Jacob J Banks 2, Richard L Freyman 1
PMCID: PMC7690971  PMID: 33261409

Abstract

It is not unusual for communication to take place while people are involved in another activity. This paper describes a study that measures the impact of listening while also completing an active postural control task. The focus was on whether the combination of listening and balancing was more detrimental to middle-aged adults than it was to younger adults as age-related changes in both hearing and postural control can occur within this age range. Speech understanding in the presence of noise and speech maskers was measured when participants (n = 15/group) were simply standing still, as well as when they were asked to complete a balancing-with-feedback postural control task, requiring different levels of effort. Performance on the postural control task also was measured in isolation. Results indicated that dual-task costs for postural control were larger when the masker was speech (vs noise) for the middle-aged group but not for the younger group. Dual-task costs in postural control increased with degree of high-frequency hearing loss even when age was controlled. Overall, results suggest that postural control in middle-aged adults can be compromised when individuals are communicating in challenging environments, perhaps reflecting an increased need for cognitive resources to successfully understand messages.

I. INTRODUCTION

Difficulty following conversations in situations with multiple talkers is a common experience as people begin to age. Although a portion of this phenomenon is undoubtedly related to the pure-tone hearing loss that accompanies aging, research has demonstrated that middle-aged adults' ratings of self-perceived hearing problems in adverse acoustic conditions are greater than what would be expected by the amount of hearing sensitivity loss that they experience (e.g., Bainbridge and Wallhagen, 2014; Demeester et al., 2012; Helfer et al., 2017). Results of laboratory-based studies of speech understanding have found that middle-aged adults do not perform as well as younger adults, especially in certain situations with competing speech messages (e.g., Başkent et al., 2014; Cameron et al., 2011; Füllgrabe et al., 2015; Goossens et al., 2017; Glyde et al., 2013; Hannula et al., 2011; Helfer and Freyman, 2014, 2016; Helfer et al., 2018; Shinn-Cunningham et al., 2013; Tremblay et al., 2015; Wiley et al., 1998). Collectively, this information suggests that the ability to communicate in noisy situations may be compromised relatively early in the aging process.

Real-life communication frequently involves talking to someone while simultaneously performing another task: for example, taking a walk with a friend or cooking dinner while conversing with one's partner. This raises questions regarding whether age-related changes in hearing and/or motor control negatively impact the ability to negotiate a combination of tasks successfully and safely. Changes in postural control or balance become increasingly apparent as people age and can be demonstrated as early as mid-life (e.g., Ku et al., 2016; Park et al., 2016). Like listening, age-related changes in postural control become more substantial when individuals are engaged in multiple tasks (see Boisgontier, 2013; Frazier and Mitra, 2008; and Ruffieux et al., 2015, for reviews of the extensive literature on dual-task costs on postural control in older adults). The purpose of this study was to examine how early age-related changes in speech understanding and balance are impacted when individuals are listening while performing a postural control task. Speech perception research is typically conducted when individuals are sitting. The identification of early aging changes in communicating while needing to maintain balance has implications for the potential sequence of adverse events that is linked to falls in older adults.

This type of research also may be able to uncover differences in speech processing between younger and middle-aged adults that occur even when the two groups perform similarly in terms of percent-correct speech recognition. There is a relatively extensive body of prior research using various types of dual-cost paradigms to quantify listening effort (see Gagné et al., 2017, for a review of this information). Dual-task costs can provide a window into the underlying cognitive load that is necessary for individuals to complete tasks (including listening tasks) successfully. Resource-based theories (e.g., Kahneman, 1973) posit that humans have a limited capacity for information processing; if one task requires more resources, then performance on the other will decline. Our previous work in this area (Helfer et al., 2020) directly addressed this idea. In that study, postural control in both younger and middle-aged adults was mediated by the difficulty of a concurrent speech perception task. Presenting the stimuli at a more adverse signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) led to a decline in postural control. Critically, even though speech understanding accuracy was essentially equivalent between the two groups, postural control in our middle-aged participants was poorer when the masker consisted of two competing talkers vs when the masker was steady-state noise (SSN). These results are consistent with the concept that listening in competing speech takes more cognitive resources than listening in less complex maskers. Age-related changes in cognitive resources revealed themselves in our middle-aged participants as increased dual-task costs when the masker was understandable speech. Moreover, our prior study (Helfer et al., 2020) found that greater high-frequency hearing loss (rather than older age) was associated with poorer postural control in both single- and dual-task conditions. Similarly, a study by Bruce et al. (2017) found larger dual-task costs for older adults with hearing loss as compared to normally hearing older adults when combining an auditory task with a postural control task.

The present study extends our previous work by using a task that allowed us to manipulate postural control difficulty and effort via visual feedback. Prior work has demonstrated that tasks requiring controlled (vs automatic) postural control processing are more sensitive to subtle age-related changes (e.g., Boisgontier et al., 2013) as they consume more cognitive resources. We speculated that using a postural control task which necessitated additional attentional resources would allow us to better quantify dual-task costs associated with listening while balancing as resources devoted to one of these tasks would leave fewer remaining for the other.

Another difference between our prior study and the present study is that our previous work compared single-task postural control measured in quiet to dual-task postural control measured in the presence of maskers (because the second task in that study was speech understanding in noise). Others have demonstrated that the presence of noise can lead to enhanced postural control (e.g., Dozza et al., 2011; Gandemer et al., 2017; Ross et al., 2016), perhaps because it provides an auditory anchor or reference. This suggests that dual-task costs in studies that use postural control and speech-in-noise tasks should be derived from single-task postural control measured in noise rather than in quiet. Previous work in this area has suggested that the presence of some types of noise improves postural control in older adults (Deviterne et al., 2005; Ross et al., 2016; Stevens et al., 2016) but perhaps to a lesser extent than for younger adults (Vitkovic et al., 2016). In the present study, we measured single-task postural control in noise for two reasons: to provide a more appropriate comparison with our dual-task conditions, which were always conducted in the presence of maskers; and to determine whether the benefit to postural control provided by noise occurs to a similar extent for younger and middle-aged adults.

The study described in this paper also was designed to address the overall idea that communicating in the presence of competing speech is particularly effortful for middle-aged participants even if they can perform a speech perception task at accuracy levels at or approaching those achieved by younger adults. We tested this idea using a dual-task paradigm in which we assessed both speech understanding in the presence of noise and speech maskers and postural control using a balancing-with-feedback postural control task with two levels of difficulty. The postural control task consisted of real-time visual feedback through which participants had to limit the excursions of their postural sway. Our hypothesis was that postural control differences between middle-aged and younger adults would be exacerbated in dual-task conditions, especially when that task was completed in the presence of an understandable speech masker and when the more difficult postural control condition was used. Based on the results of our previous study (Helfer et al., 2020), we also predicted that high-frequency thresholds would be more closely associated with postural control than age per se.

II. METHODS

A. Participants

Groups of younger adults and middle-aged adults (n = 15/group) participated in this study. The younger adults (mean age 21 years old, range 19–24 years old) all had audiometrically normal hearing [pure-tone thresholds ≤ 25 dB hearing level (HL) from 250 to 8000 Hz]. Middle-aged participants (mean age 54 years old, age range 46–63 years old) were required to have average high-frequency pure-tone thresholds (between 2 and 6 kHz) ≤ 60 dB HL in either ear; the mean better-ear high-frequency average was 19.75 dB and ranged from 3 to 51 dB (audiograms for each participant group are shown in Fig. 1). None of the participants had ever worn hearing aids. Potential participants were screened to rule out self-reported otologic, vestibular, motor, or neurological problems that would affect hearing or balance. All participants had normal tympanograms on the test day. Middle-aged participants had scores within normal limits (≥26) on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005). Each participant gave informed consent for the protocol, which was approved by the University of Massachusetts Amherst Institutional Review Board.

FIG. 1.

FIG. 1.

(Color online) Mean audiograms for the younger (left) and middle-aged (right) participants. Dashed lines indicate the minimum and maximum thresholds. Thresholds for the right ear are in red; thresholds for the left ear are in blue.

B. Procedures

Participants completed a speech recognition task and a postural control task, singularly (single-task conditions) and simultaneously (dual-task conditions). The acoustic conditions for the speech perception task were selected with the goal of producing percent-correct performance that would be approximately equal between groups. This task consisted of target TVM-Colors sentences (Helfer et al., 2016) presented with either competing TVM-Colors sentences or SSN shaped with the envelope of the long-term spectrum of utterances from two female talkers. The target sentence was presented from a loudspeaker located in front of the participant while the maskers were presented from speakers 60 deg to the left and right. Loudspeakers were adjusted to be ear-height for each participant. When the masker was competing sentences, all three talkers were the same sex with utterances from one masking talker presented from the left loudspeaker and the other presented from the right loudspeaker. Each masking sentence was presented in a looped fashion where it started at a random point within the sentence with the beginning portion of the sentence appended to the end of the sentence (see Helfer et al., 2016). Independent samples of SSN were presented from the right and left loudspeakers during trials using that masker. Target sentences were presented at 67 dBA at −3 dB SNR. The SNR was quantified relative to the total masker energy (in other words, the combined levels of the maskers were 70 dBA). It should be noted that there was a second speech perception task [listening to the Connected Speech Test (Cox et al., 1987) passages and answering questions] completed by all participants; those results are not discussed in the present paper.

Testing was completed in a double-walled sound-treated audiometric chamber (IAC No. 1604A, Naperville, IL). Participants were instructed to face forward and look at a computer monitor located directly in front for the duration of a block of trials and to keep their hands on their hips. The participants removed their shoes prior to testing, which was conducted with participants standing on a 40 × 60 cm piezoelectric force platform (Kistler Instruments Corporation, Amherst, NY) with a standard side-by-side foot placement. A custom LabView program (National Instruments, Austin, TX) time-synced with the audio tasks continuously recorded [at 100 Hz analog-to-digital (A/D) conversion rate] and displayed the participant's ground reaction force information (i.e., center of pressure, CoP) from the force platform.

Participants completed an active postural control task in which real-time visual feedback corresponding to the participant's CoP was shown as a moving point within a rectangular box displayed on the computer monitor located in front of the listener. They were instructed to keep the moving point within the boundaries of the box. Two levels of the postural control task were incorporated to manipulate the attentional resources needed to complete this task, with task difficulty manipulated by altering the size of the box. In the easier condition, the dimensions of the box (approximately 6 × 10 cm) were larger and less restrictive than in the harder postural control condition (approximately 4 × 6 cm). We recognized that CoP might not differ between the two levels of postural control task difficulty if participants were able to limit their movements to keep the visual feedback within the box. However, we anticipated that reducing the box size would lead to an increased demand on the postural control system with a corresponding increase in effort required to complete the task. Baseline postural control (measured in quiet with no visual feedback) also was assessed both before and after the active postural control blocks; participants were instructed to stand as still as possible on the force plate while looking straight ahead at a plus sign on a computer monitor during these two trials.

Conditions were blocked by postural control task (easier vs harder) and masker (SSN or competing speech) with blocks presented in randomized order. Each block involving speech understanding consisted of eight sentences (with four scoring words per sentence) with two blocks presented for each combination of masker and postural control task. Single-task postural control (that is, with the visual feedback described above) was measured in quiet as well as in the presence of the SSN masker, presented from the right and left loudspeakers at 70 dBA. Single-task speech perception was considered as the blocks in which participants were instructed to simply stand as still as possible.1 During these single-task trials, the monitor displayed a plus sign, which participants were asked to visualize for the duration of the block of trials. For dual-task blocks, participants were instructed to place equal effort on both the speech perception task and the balancing-with-feedback task. At the end of each block of trials, they were asked to rate their perceived effort separately for the listening and postural tasks using a scale of one (very little effort) to ten (very high effort).

C. Data and statistical analyses

Postural control was defined as the CoP excursion area measured from the force platform (Duarte and Freitas, 2010; Swanenburg et al., 2008). A 95% confidence ellipse based on the anterior-posterior and medial-lateral displacements of the CoP was calculated in a custom matlab script (The MathWorks, Natick, MA). Prior to all calculations, the CoP data were low-pass filtered with a 10 Hz zero-lag Butterworth filter. Ground reaction forces were collected continuously across an entire block of trials (approximately 80 s).

Our prior work in this area examined postural control only during the period of time when participants were listening as the focus of that paper (Helfer et al., 2020) was on listening effort. However, most dual-task studies that combine a balance task with a listening or cognitive task using a verbal response consider postural control across the entire trial (that is, when the participant is listening and processing the information, as well as when they are responding). We conducted an initial analysis of the data in which we compared postural control for time intervals corresponding to when participants were listening vs when they were responding (see the Appendix). This analysis found that the listen-only and respond-only data did not differ significantly from each other in either postural control condition, and there were no significant interactions between the analysis interval and participant group. We therefore decided to conduct our full analysis on postural control data from the entire trial in order to be better able to compare our work to previous research. An additional benefit of using the entire trial is that a longer analysis window leads to a more stable measurement of postural CoP data (Carpenter et al., 2001).

Repeated-measures multivariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to identify main and interaction effects in single-task and dual-task conditions for both postural control and speech recognition. ANOVAs were also used to analyze the self-reported effort data obtained in all conditions. We calculated two derived metrics that also were analyzed via ANOVA: the difference in postural control found during the easier vs the more difficult balance task; and absolute dual-task costs. All ANOVA results can be found in Table I. Post hoc t-tests or one-way ANOVAs were conducted to explore significant interactions. The question of how age and hearing loss contributed to postural control was addressed with Pearson r and partial correlation analyses (see Table II).

TABLE I.

Summary of ANOVA results. Statistically significant results (p < 0.05) are in bold typeface.

Baseline postural control: F p η2
Group 4.11 0.052 0.128
Single-task postural control: F p ηp2
Task 0.35 0.561 0.012
Noise 0.44 0.512 0.016
Group 5.09 0.033 0.153
Task × group 0.45 0.506 0.016
Noise × group 0.01 0.908 0.000
Task × noise 0.10 0.758 0.003
Task × noise × group 0.02 0.892 0.001
Single-task balance effort: F p ηp2
Task 229.20 <0.001 0.891
Noise 1.11 0.301 0.038
Group 4.31 0.047 0.133
Task × group 0.21 0.651 0.007
Noise × group 2.01 0.167 0.067
Task × noise 0.57 0.457 0.020
Task × noise × group 0.57 0.457 0.020
Speech recognition accuracy: F p ηp2
Task 1.04 0.369 0.071
Masker 10.16 0.004 0.266
Group 0.19 0.670 0.007
Task × group 0.46 0.638 0.033
Masker x group 2.31 0.139 0.076
Task × masker 4.56 0.020 0.253
Task × masker × group 2.20 0.130 0.140
Speech recognition effort: F p ηp2
Task 5.13 0.013 0.275
Masker 0.01 0.921 0.000
Group 5.91 0.022 0.174
Task × group 1.58 0.225 0.105
Masker × group 0.95 0.339 0.033
Task × masker 2.41 0.109 0.151
Task × masker × group 2.20 0.130 0.013
Dual-task postural control: F p ηp2
Task 4.05 0.054 0.126
Masker 2.49 0.126 0.082
Group 4.48 0.043 0.138
Task × group 5.37 0.028 0.161
Masker × group 5.56 0.026 0.166
Task × masker 0.74 0.402 0.025
Task × masker × group 0.39 0.536 0.014
Dual-task balance effort: F p ηp2
Task 129.90 <0.001 0.823
Masker 2.16 0.153 0.072
Group 5.91 0.022 0.174
Task × group 0.36 0.553 0.013
Masker × group 0.40 0.534 0.014
Task × masker 0.01 0.913 0.000
Task × masker × group 0.01 0.913 0.000
TABLE I. (Continued)
Single-task: Harder–easier postural control: F p ηp2
Noise 0.10 0.758 0.003
Group 0.45 0.506 0.016
Noise × group 0.02 0.892 0.001
Dual-task: Harde–easier postural control: F p ηp2
Masker 0.72 0.402 0.025
Group 5.37 0.028 0.161
Masker × group 0.39 0.536 0.014
Dual-task costs: F p ηp2
Task 1.46 0.238 0.049
Masker 2.49 0.126 0.082
Group 0.10 0.759 0.003
Masker × group 5.56 0.026 0.166
Task × group 2.29 0.142 0.075
Task × masker 0.72 0.402 0.025
Task × masker × group 0.39 0.536 0.014

TABLE II.

Correlation coefficients for analyses examining associations among age, better-ear high-frequency average (beHFPTA; average of thresholds from 2 kHz to 6 kHz), and postural control in the single-task (SingE for the easier postural control task; SingH for the harder postural control task) and dual-task (DualE for the easier postural control task; DualH for the harder postural control task) conditions. *, significant at the 0.05 level; **, significant at the 0.01 level.

Pearson r beHFPTA SingE SingH DualE DualH
Age 0.701** 0.452* 0.419* 0.349 0.415*
beHFPTA 0.465* 0.428* 0.541** 0.694**
Partial correlation controlling for age SingE SingH DualE DualH
beHFPTA 0.233 0.208 0.443* 0.621**
Partial correlation controlling for beHFPTA SingE SingH DualE DualH
Age 0.199 0.185 −0.050 −0.139

III. RESULTS

A. Single-task postural control

Performance on the baseline postural control trials (in which participants were instructed to stand as still as possible without visual feedback), as well as on the single-task active postural control blocks with visual feedback of their CoP, are shown in Fig. 2. The first analysis compared performance between the groups in the baseline condition. Larger CoP in this baseline condition is indicative of poorer postural control (that is, participants were less successful at standing still). Although middle-aged participants had poorer baseline postural control than younger participants, one-way ANOVAs showed that the difference between group baselines just missed statistical significance (p = 0.052).

FIG. 2.

FIG. 2.

(Color online) Baseline and single-task postural control for younger (red) and middle-aged (white) participants. Baseline postural control (left bars) was measured with no visual feedback provided. Single-task active postural control was assessed in easier and harder conditions by having participants use visual feedback. Each box is bounded by the 25th and 75th percentiles. The horizontal line within each box displays the median, “×” indicates the mean, and the whiskers show the minimum and maximum values.

A second analysis examined how single-task postural control was influenced by the balancing-with-feedback postural control task and the acoustic condition. A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted with the postural control task (easier vs harder) and listening condition (quiet vs noise) as within-subject factors and the participant group as a between-subjects factor. Results of this analysis revealed a significant main effect of group (p = 0.033). Main and interaction effects involving task type or listening condition were not statistically significant. This demonstrates that the presence of noise did not have a significant effect on the ability to complete the postural control task, and middle-aged adults exhibited larger excursions during the active postural control task than did the younger participants.

Different results were found when analyzing how much effort participants perceived to use during the active postural control task. These data are shown in Fig. 3. A repeated-measures ANOVA with postural control task (easier vs harder) and listening condition (quiet or noise) as within-subject factors and participant group as a between-subjects factor revealed significant main effects of both group (p = 0.047) and postural task difficulty (p < 0.001). This verifies that although CoP values did not differ significantly between the easier and harder postural control task (see Fig. 2), participants judged the harder postural control task to be much more effortful than the easier postural control task. Overall, middle-aged adults found the postural control task to be more effortful than did the younger participants. However, the presence of background noise did not significantly influence the effort ratings.

FIG. 3.

FIG. 3.

(Color online) Self-rated effort for single-task postural control for younger (red) and middle-aged (white) participants (1 = very little effort, 10 = very high effort).

B. Speech recognition

Percent-correct speech recognition data for single-task conditions (when participants were standing but not completing the postural control task) and dual-task conditions (when participants completed both the speech perception task and postural control task) can be seen in Fig. 4. Minimal differences were found between the two groups in any of these conditions. A repeated-measures ANOVA, with masker (SSN vs speech) and postural control task (none/single-task speech perception vs easier postural control task vs harder postural control task) as within-subject factors and group as a between-subjects factor, found significant main effects of masker (p = 0.004) and a significant masker × task interaction (p = 0.020). Post hoc analysis of this interaction (paired-samples t-tests with adjustment for multiple comparisons) showed that percent-correct speech recognition was better for the easier postural control condition (vs the harder postural control condition) when the masker was speech but not when the masker was noise. However, the numerical difference in percent-correct between conditions was small (between two and five percentage points).

FIG. 4.

FIG. 4.

(Color online) Speech perception (in percent correct) by group (red, younger; white, middle-aged), masker (SSN or competing speech), and postural control task.

Self-rated listening effort for the speech recognition task is shown in Fig. 5. Although, as seen in Fig. 4, there were only minimal differences between groups in terms of accuracy in speech perception scores, middle-aged participants consistently reported that the speech recognition task was more effortful, as compared to the younger participants. Participants also rated the speech perception task as more effortful when conducted in the harder postural control condition than in the easier postural control condition. A repeated-measures ANOVA using the same variables as for the percent-correct analysis showed significant main effects for task (p = 0.013) and group (p = 0.022). Post hoc t-tests indicated that listening effort ratings were higher for the harder postural control task vs either the single-task ratings or the easier postural control task ratings.

FIG. 5.

FIG. 5.

(Color online) Self-rated effort for the speech perception task for younger (red) and middle-aged (white) participants (1 = very little effort, 10 = very high effort).

C. Dual-task postural control

Participants' performance on the postural control task when completed in conjunction with the speech perception task is shown in Fig. 6. Repeated-measures ANOVA with task (easier vs harder) and masker (noise vs speech) as within-subject variables and group as a between-subjects variable was completed for these data. While there was considerable overlap in CoP values between groups, this analysis indicated a significant main effect of group (p = 0.043), as well as a significant task × group interaction (p = 0.028) and a significant masker × group interaction (p = 0.026). Post hoc t-tests on the masker × group interaction indicated that the difference in postural control between noise and speech maskers was significant for middle-aged adults (p = 0.034) as the CoP area was greater (that is, participants made more or larger postural movements) in the presence of the speech masker vs in the presence of the the noise masker. This was not the case for the younger participants, for whom postural control in dual-task conditions was essentially stable across conditions.

FIG. 6.

FIG. 6.

(Color online) Postural control for dual-task conditions for younger (red) and middle-aged (white) participants.

We further examined the significant task × group interaction by comparing the difference in postural control for the easier postural control task vs the harder postural control task (CoP for the harder task minus CoP for the easier task). This derived metric, which indexes the toll on postural control that occurs when that task increases in difficulty, is shown in Fig. 7. Difficulty of the postural control task had only a small effect on CoP for single-task blocks. However, when participants were asked to complete the postural control task at the same time as the speech perception task, a different pattern emerged. Making the postural control task more difficult in dual-task blocks had an adverse effect on postural control for the middle-aged participants but not for the younger participants. This pattern was confirmed with repeated-measures ANOVA. For the single-task blocks, neither the effect of acoustic condition (noise or quiet) nor group nor their interaction was significant. Conversely, a significant main effect of group (p = 0.028) was found for the dual-task data: during dual-task conditions, middle-aged participants had more difficulty negotiating the harder postural control task than the easier postural control task. It appears that when faced with performing both a speech perception task and a postural control task, middle-aged adults made more or larger corrections in order to keep their CoP within the boundaries of the smaller box used in the harder postural control condition.

FIG. 7.

FIG. 7.

(Color online) The difference in postural control between the easier task and the harder task (CoP for harder task minus CoP for easier task); red = younger participants; white = middle-aged participants.

Dual-task self-rated postural control effort can be seen in Fig. 8. ANOVA revealed only a significant main effect of task (p < 0.001) with no significant main or interaction effects involving either masker or group (Table I).

FIG. 8.

FIG. 8.

(Color online) Self-rated effort for dual-task postural control for younger (red) and middle-aged (white) participants (1 = very little effort, 10 = very high effort).

Finally, we calculated absolute dual-task costs by subtracting the CoP values obtained in the single-task condition in noise from the dual-task CoP for both the easier and harder postural control tasks (see Fig. 9). ANOVA with task (easier vs harder) and masker (noise vs speech) as within-subject factors and group as a between-subjects factor revealed a significant main effect of group (p = 0.003) and a significant masker × group interaction (p = 0.026). Post hoc t-tests indicated that dual-task costs were significantly larger for the speech vs the noise masker for middle-aged participants (t = −2.35, p = 0.034) but not for younger participants (t = 0.71, p = 0.489).

FIG. 9.

FIG. 9.

(Color online) Dual-task costs for postural control for younger (red) and middle-aged (white) participants (CoP for dual-task - CoP for single-task).

D. Hearing loss and individual variability

A series of correlation analyses was conducted to identify associations between age, hearing, and postural control. The specific variables used in the analyses were age, better-ear high-frequency pure-tone average (beHFPTA; average of thresholds from 2 to 6 kHz) and single- and dual-task CoP values for the easier postural control task and the harder postural control task. These CoP metrics were calculated by averaging across all other factors (for single-task, this averaging was across values obtained in quiet vs in the presence of noise; for dual-task, the averaging was across trials with speech maskers and noise maskers). Results of these analyses can be found in Table II.

As anticipated, age and beHFPTA were strongly intercorrelated. Both age and beHFPTA were significantly associated with all four CoP metrics; the older the participant and the more hearing loss they had, the larger the excursions they exhibited during the postural control task. Two partial correlation analyses were then conducted: one controlling for age and the other controlling for beHFPTA. When age was controlled, beHFPTA continued to be associated with dual-task CoP measures for both the easier task and the harder task. When beHFPTA was controlled, age was no longer significantly associated with either of these variables. These results replicate those of our previous study (Helfer et al., 2020), demonstrating that amount of high-frequency hearing loss in our middle-aged participants was associated with poorer postural control, especially in dual-task conditions.

IV. DISCUSSION

The study described in this paper was designed to determine the extent to which having to complete an active postural control task and a challenging speech understanding task together (i.e., a dual-task) compromises performance on each of those tasks. Results indicate that it was primarily the balance task that was negatively impacted during dual-task conditions. Although there was a significant task × masker interaction in the speech perception data (where performance was poorer during the harder postural control task vs the easier postural control task when the masker was speech), the numerical difference in means between these conditions was quite modest (two percentage points for the younger participants and five percentage points for the middle-aged participants). As was our intention, percent-correct speech understanding varied little between groups for either the single- or dual-task condition. Adding this speech perception task to the postural control task had minimal impact for our younger adults but, at least for some of our middle-aged participants, led to a decrement in postural control, especially when the masker was competing speech. Even though the middle-aged participants were able to achieve approximately the same speech perception accuracy as the younger adults during dual-task conditions, it came at a cost to postural control. Additionally, on average, our middle-aged participants judged the speech understanding task to be more effortful than did the younger adults.

Our interpretation of this pattern of results is that communicating in the presence of competing messages required greater cognitive resources for our middle-aged than for our younger participants, leaving fewer resources for performing the postural control task. This is consistent with prior work showing that speech understanding in noise is more effortful for middle-aged adults than it is for younger adults (e.g., Degeest et al., 2015; Helfer et al., 2020), and that middle-aged adults are particularly susceptible to masking by competing speech (e.g., Başkent et al., 2014; Cameron et al., 2011; Füllgrabe et al., 2015; Glyde et al., 2013; Goossens et al., 2017; Hannula et al., 2011; Helfer and Freyman, 2014, 2016; Helfer et al., 2018; Shinn-Cunningham et al., 2013; Tremblay et al., 2015; Wiley et al., 1998). Moreover, it extends findings of prior work showing greater dual-task costs in older than younger adults when postural control tasks are combined with a cognitive or speech perception task requiring a verbal response (e.g., Bohle et al., 2019; Bruce et al., 2017; Granacher et al., 2011). Finally, the present results also confirm a smaller body of work indicating that dual-task costs for motor control tasks can be demonstrated in middle-aged adults (Brustio et al., 2017; Helfer et al., 2020; Hollman et al., 2007; Lindenberger et al., 2000).

As reported in our previous paper on balancing while listening (Helfer et al., 2020), greater hearing loss in the present study's participants was associated with poorer postural control. When age was controlled, beHFPTA was correlated with postural control in dual-task conditions. Other research has shown that hearing loss in older adults can impact posture and gait (e.g., Kowalewski et al., 2018; Li et al., 2013; Thomas et al., 2018; Tomioka et al., 2015; Viljanen et al., 2009a) and is associated with an increased risk of falls (e.g., Jiam et al., 2016; Lin and Ferrucci, 2012; Viljanen et al., 2009b). Also relevant to the current work is the finding that older adults with hearing loss have greater dual-task costs than younger adults in a paradigm that combined an auditory working memory task with a postural control task involving recovery from perturbations (Bruce et al., 2017).

Our research extends the previously established connection between hearing and postural control to middle-aged adults. Perhaps the most parsimonious explanation for this association is that the early age-related changes in the inner ear that bring about hearing loss in middle-aged adults coexist with subtle changes in vestibular function. However, there are several other potential explanations, including that hearing loss may lead to a need to allocate more cognitive resources to listening, or that age-related changes reduce the ability to use auditory cues to monitor one's body position in space (Campos et al., 2018). Although additional studies need to be conducted to clarify the factors underlying the connection between hearing loss and postural control, it appears that hearing loss that occurs relatively early in the aging process is accompanied by changes in balance performance. This may help explain why individuals with mild hearing loss are three times more likely to fall than their age-matched normally hearing peers (Lin and Ferrucci, 2012).

Prior work has shown that noise can reduce postural sway in both younger (e.g., Gandemer et al., 2017; Zhong and Yost, 2013) and older (Vitkovic et al., 2016) adults, although there are reports of hearing loss reducing this effect (Vitkovic et al., 2016). We sought to determine whether the presence of noise would improve postural control to the same degree in our younger and middle-aged participants. We were unable to adequately address this question as in the present study noise was not significantly beneficial for either group, whether quantified in terms of objectively measured CoP or subjectively measured self-rated postural control effort. Perhaps the nature of our active postural control task, which required greater cognitive mediation and postural guidance than the tasks used in previous studies, led to this negative finding. We did not measure baseline postural control (with participants simply standing without an active postural task) in the presence of noise, and it is feasible that having to perform a second task at the same time as the postural control task masked any potential benefit of noise. If additional research confirms this finding, it would suggest that the presence of noise is not beneficial to postural control performance when individuals are simultaneously engaged in another task.

As is typical in most dual-task studies that combine a motor control task with a second task that requires a verbal response, postural control in the current work was measured across the entire trial (that is, when the participant was listening as well as when they were responding). Research has established that talking influences performance on balance and kinematic tasks (Dault et al., 2003; Frazier and Mitra, 2008; Raffegeneau et al., 2018; Yardley et al., 1999), perhaps to a greater extent in older adults than in younger adults (Ayers et al., 2014; Kemper et al., 2003). This raises a question regarding the extent to which group differences in postural control identified in our work, as well as in similar studies, are due to age-related changes in speech processing (i.e., speech perception) vs age-related changes in talking (i.e., speech production). Our preliminary analysis on the postural control data excised into listen-only and respond-only intervals (shown in the Appendix) found that CoP values did not differ systematically between listen-only and respond-only data for either participant group. However, there was a significant interaction between analysis interval and postural task difficulty: the difference between postural control for the easier vs the harder postural task was significant in the listen-only intervals but not in the respond-only intervals. Additionally, group differences in postural control during dual-task conditions analyzed across the entire trial (listen + respond) were larger when the masker was competing speech than when it consisted of SSN (see Fig. 6). Since there is no reason to think that the influence of talking on postural control would differ between trials with competing speech vs with the SSN masker (especially since the maskers were not being presented when individuals were making their verbal responses), we believe that group differences in CoP in the present study were primarily driven by degradations in speech understanding. This idea is supported by previous studies showing that middle-aged adults, under certain conditions, have more difficulty understanding speech in the presence of competing speech vs when speech is presented in less complex noise (e.g., Goossens et al., 2017; Helfer and Freyman, 2014, 2016; Helfer et al., 2018).

The active postural control task that we incorporated in the present study is an example of an augmented information feedback task, commonly used in motor learning, aging, and rehabilitation studies. One potential limitation is that this task is not one that individuals would encounter in their day-to-day lives. Nevertheless, we believe that it can inform the postural challenges that people may face during everyday tasks as they age. People inherently use the feedback provided by the visual, proprioceptive, and the vestibular systems to stay balanced. Both older and younger adults also can use extrinsic visual feedback to enhance balance. For example, Pinsault and Vuillerme (2008) reported reductions in postural displacement in older individuals when given visual feedback of their CoP as compared to conditions with no visual feedback. Vaillant et al. (2004) found that adding external postural information from a mirror reduced postural sway when both younger and older individuals were asked to stand still. In our study design, the visual CoP feedback was intentionally manipulated to allow us to alter task difficulty and increase cognitive demands. Further, our methodology required participants to constrain their CoP excursions, similar to what occurs naturally when we move closer to stability boundaries formed by the base of support at the feet, such as when we lean to reach forward, move backward or sideways (Van Emmerik and Van Wegen, 2002; Van Wegen et al., 2002).

V. CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Overall, our results support the idea that middle-aged adults can be challenged in complex listening situations, especially when they are also performing another task concurrently. It is worth noting that there has been relatively little research devoted to identifying functional hearing changes in middle age, even though people in this age group are likely to still be working and so may frequently need to multitask in adverse acoustical environments. Additional research is needed to define the nature of tasks that interfere with multitasking in middle-aged adults and how we might help remediate these problems. For example, there is some evidence that postural control is better in people with hearing loss when they are using their hearing aids vs when they are unaided (Negahban et al., 2017; Rumalla et al., 2015; Weaver et al., 2017), although other studies have failed to show this beneficial effect (Kowalewski et al., 2018; McDaniel et al., 2018). Conceptually, if hearing aids can reduce listening effort, they should lead to smaller dual-task costs, including less of an impact on balance. To date, research has not examined whether hearing aids can improve postural control in dual-task studies involving listening while balancing. Clearly, more research needs to be conducted to clarify whether providing amplification leads to enhanced postural control when individuals are called upon to perform more than one task at a time. Future research also should be conducted to identify the real-life implications of early aging changes in the ability to multitask.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Michael Rogers, Lincoln Dunn, and Mike Clauss for their assistance with this project. This work was supported by research Grant No. R01 DC 012057 from the National Institutes of Health (NIH)/National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD).

APPENDIX

We conducted an initial analysis that compared CoP excised just for the intervals of time during each trial when participants were listening to CoP measured during the time intervals when participants were responding (i.e., repeating back what they heard). The trends in these two sets of data were quite similar (see Fig. 10). ANOVAs were conducted separately for single-task and dual-task data with analysis interval (listen-only vs respond-only), postural control task difficulty (easier vs harder), and masker (noise vs speech) as within-subject variables and group as a between-subjects variable. The single-task analysis showed nonsignificant main effects of analysis interval and task difficulty with no significant interactions. The main effect of group just missed statistical significance [F(1,28) = 4.07, p = 0.054, ηp2 = 0.127]. For the dual-task data, there was a significant main effect of task difficulty [F(1,28) = 10.21, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.267], a significant task difficulty × group interaction [F(1,28) = 7.80, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.218], and a significant interaction of analysis interval × task difficulty [F(1,28) = 6.43, p = 0.017, ηp2 = 0.187]. Post hoc pairwise t-tests adjusted for multiple comparisons indicated that the analysis interval × task difficulty interaction was due to the fact that the easier and the harder postural control conditions differed for the listen-only data but not for the respond-only data.

FIG. 10.

FIG. 10.

(Color online) Postural control (CoP) for listen-only intervals and respond-only intervals for single-task conditions (A) and dual-task conditions (B). Red, younger participants; white, middle-aged participants.

Footnotes

1

It could be argued that our single-task speech recognition condition was also dual-task because it was measured with participants standing rather than seated. Indeed, research has shown that reaction time measured during an attentional task is longer when individuals are standing vs when they are seated (Lajoie et al., 1993), suggesting that standing may be more attentionally demanding than sitting.

References

  • 1. Ayers, E. I. , Tow, A. C. , Holtzer, R. , and Verghese, J. (2014). “ Walking while talking and falls in aging,” Gerontology 60, 108–113. 10.1159/000355119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Bainbridge, K. E. , and Wallhagen, M. I. (2014). “ Hearing loss in an aging American population: Extent, impact, and management,” Ann. Rev. Pub. Health 35, 139–1520. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Başkent, D. , van Engelshoven, S. , and Galvin, J. J. III. (2014). “ Susceptibility to interference by music and speech maskers in middle-aged adults,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 135, EL147– EL153. 10.1121/1.4865261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Bohle, H. , Rimpel, J. , Schauenburg, G. , Gebel, A. , Stelzel, C. , Heinzel, S. , Rapp, M. , and Granacher, U. (2019). “ Behavioral and neural correlates of cognitive-motor interference during multitasking in young and old adults,” Neural Plast. 2019, 9478656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Boisgontier, M. P. , Beets, I. A. M. , Duysens, J. , Nieuwboer, A. , Krampe, R. T. , and Swinnen, S. P. (2013). “ Age-related differences in attentional cost associated with postural dual tasks: Increased recruitment of generic cognitive resources in older adults,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1824–1837. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Bruce, H. , Aponte, D. , St.-Onge, N. , Phillips, N. , Gagne, J.-P. , and Li, K. Z. H. (2017). “ The effects of age and hearing loss on dual-task balance and listening,” J. Gerontol. B: Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 74, 275–283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Brustio, P. R. , Magistro, D. , Zecca, M. , Rabaglietti, E. , and Liubicich, M. E. (2017). “ Age-related decrements in dual-task performance: Comparison of different mobility and cognitive tasks. A cross sectional study,” PLoS One 12(7), e0181698. 10.1371/journal.pone.0181698 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Cameron, S. , Glyde, H. , and Dillon, H. (2011). “ Listening in Spatialized Noise—Sentences Test (LiSN-S): Normative and retest reliability data for adolescents and adults up to 60 years of age,” J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 22, 697–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Campos, J. , Ramkhalawansingh, R. , and Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2018). “ Hearing, self-motion perception, mobility, and aging,” Hear. Res. 369, 42–55. 10.1016/j.heares.2018.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Carpenter, M. G. , Frank, J. S. , Winter, D. A. , and Peysar, G. W. (2001). “ Sampling duration effects on centre of pressure summary measures,” Gait Post. 13, 35–40. 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00093-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Cox, R. M. , Alexander, G. C. , and Gilmore, C. (1987). “ Development of the Connected Speech Test (CST),” Ear Hear. 8, 19s–126s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Dault, M. C. , Yardley, L. , and Frank, J. S. (2003). “ Does articulation contribute to modifications of postural control during dual-task paradigms?,” Brain Res. Cog. Brain Res. 16, 434–440. 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00058-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Degeest, S. , Keppler, H. , and Corthals, P. (2015). “ The effect of age on listening effort,” J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58, 1592–1600. 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-H-14-0288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Demeester, K. , Topsakal, V. , Hendrickx, J.-J. , Fransen, E. , Van Laer, L. , Van Camp, G. , Van de Heyning, P. , and van Wieringen, A. (2012). “ Hearing disability measured by the speech, spatial, and qualities of hearing scale in clinically normal-hearing and hearing-impaired middle-aged persons, and disability screening by means for a reduced SSQ (the SSQ5),” Ear Hear. 33, 615–626. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31824e0ba7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Deviterne, D. , Gauchard, G. C. , Jamet, M. , Vancon, G. , and Perrin, P. P. (2005). “ Added cognitive load through rotary auditory stimulation can improve the quality of postural control in the elderly,” Brain Res. Bull. 64, 487–492. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Dozza, M. , Chiari, L. , Peterka, R. J. , Wall, C. , and Horak, F. B. (2011). “ What is the most effective type of audio-biofeedback for postural motor learning?,” Gait Post. 34, 313–319. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.05.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Duarte, M. , and Freitas, S. M. (2010). “ Revision of posturography based on force plate for balance evaluation,” Rev. Bras. Fisioter. 14, 183–192. 10.1590/S1413-35552010000300003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Frazier, E. V. , and Mitra, S. (2008). “ Methodological and interpretive issues in posture-cognition dual-tasking in upright stance,” Gait Post. 27, 271–279. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Füllgrabe, C. , Moore, B. C. , and Stone, M. A. (2015). “ Age-group differences in speech identification despite matched audiometrically normal hearing: Contributions from auditory temporal processing and cognition,” Front. Aging Neurosci. 6, 347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Gagné, J.-P. , Besser, J. , and Lemke, U. (2017). “ Behavioral assessment of listening effort using a dual-task paradigm: A review,” Int. J. Audiol 21, 1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Gandemer, L. , Parseihian, G. , Kronland-Martinet, R. , and Bourdin, C. (2017). “ Spatial cues provided by sound improve postural stabilization: Evidence of a spatial auditory map?,” Front. Neurosci. 11, 357. 10.3389/fnins.2017.00357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Glyde, H. , Cameron, S. , Dillon, H. , Hickson, L. , and Seeto, M. (2013). “ The effects of hearing impairment and aging on spatial processing,” Ear Hear. 34, 15–28. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182617f94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Goossens, T. , Vercammen, V. , Wouters, J. , and van Wieringen, A. (2017). “ Masked speech perception across the adult lifespan: Impact of age and hearing impairment,” Hear. Res. 344, 109–124. 10.1016/j.heares.2016.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Granacher, U. , Bridenbaugh, S. A. , Muehlbauer, T. , Wehrle, A. , and Kressig, R. W. (2011). “ Age- related effects on postural control under multi-task conditions,” Geronotology 57, 247–255. 10.1159/000322196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Hannula, S. , Bloigu, R. , Majamaa, K. , Sorri, M. , and Maki-Torkko, E. (2011). “ Self-reported hearing problems among older adults: Prevalence and comparison to measured hearing impairment,” J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 22, 550–559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Helfer, K. S. , and Freyman, R. L. (2014). “ Stimulus and listener factors affecting age-related changes in competing speech perception,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 136, 748–759. 10.1121/1.4887463 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Helfer, K. S. , and Freyman, R. L. (2016). “ Age equivalence in the benefit of repetition for speech understanding,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 140, EL371–EL377. 10.1121/1.4966586 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Helfer, K. S. , Freyman, R. L. , and Merchant, G. R. (2018). “ How repetition influences speech understanding in younger, middle-aged, and older adults,” Int. J. Audiol. 57, 695–702. 10.1080/14992027.2018.1475756 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Helfer, K. S. , Freyman, R. L. , van Emmerik, R. , and Banks, J. (2020). “ Postural control while listening in younger and middle-aged adults,” Ear Hear. 41, 1383–1396. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000861 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Helfer, K. S. , Merchant, G. R. , and Freyman, R. L. (2016). “ Aging and the effect of target-masker alignment,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 140, 3844–3853. 10.1121/1.4967297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Helfer, K. S. , Merchant, G. R. , and Wasiuk, P. A. (2017). “ Objective and subjective speech perception in complex listening environments across the adult age span,” J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 3009–3018. 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-H-17-0030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Hollman, J. H. , Kovash, F. M. , Kubik, J. J. , and Linbo, R. A. (2007). “ Age-related differences in spatiotemporal markers of gait stability during dual task walking,” Gait Post. 26, 113–119. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Jiam, N. T.-L. , Li, C. , and Agrawal, Y. (2016). “ Hearing loss and falls: A systematic review and meta-analysis,” Laryngoscope 126, 2587–2596. 10.1002/lary.25927 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and Effort ( Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: ). [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Kemper, S. , Herman, R. E. , and Lian, C. H. (2003). “ The costs of doing two things at once for young and older adults: Talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech or noise,” Psychol. Aging 18, 181–192. 10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Kowalewski, V. , Patterson, R. , Hartos, J. , and Bugnariu, N. (2018). “ Hearing loss contributes to balance difficulties in both younger and older adults,” J. Prevent. Med. 3, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Ku, P. X. , Abu Osman, N. A. , and Wan Abas, W. A. B. (2016). “ The limits of stability and muscle activity in middle-aged adults during static and dynamic stance,” J. Biomech. 49, 3943–3948. 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2016.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Lajoie, Y. , Teasdale, N. , Bard, C. , and Fleury, M. (1993). “ Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium,” Brain Res. 97, 139–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Li, L. , Simonsick, E. M. , Ferrucci, L. , and Lin, F. R. (2013). “ Hearing loss and gait speed among older adults in the United States,” Gait Post. 38, 25–29. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Lin, F. R. , and Ferrucci, L. (2012). “ Hearing loss and falls among older adults in the United States,” Arch. Intern. Med. 172, 369–371. 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Lindenberger, U. , Marsiske, M. , and Baltes, P. B. (2000). “ Memorizing while walking: Increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age,” Psychol. Aging 15, 417–436. 10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. McDaniel, D. M. , Motts, S. D. , and Neeley, R. A. (2018). “ Effects of bilateral hearing aid use on balance in experienced adult hearing aid users,” Am. J. Audiol. 27, 121–125. 10.1044/2017_AJA-16-0071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Nasreddine, Z. S. , Phillips, N. A. , Bedririan, V. , Charbonneau, S. , Whitehead, V. , Collin, I. , Cummings, J. L. , and Chertkow, H. (2005). “ The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment,” J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Negahban, H. , Bavarsad Cheshmeh Ali, M. , and Nassadj, G. (2017). “ Effect of hearing aids on static balance function in elderly with hearing loss,” Gait Post. 58, 126–129. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.07.112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Park, J.-H. , Mancini, M. , Carlson-Khuta, P. , Nutt, J. G. , and Horak, F. B. (2016). “ Quantifying effects of age on balance and gait with inertial sensors in community-dwelling healthy adults,” Exp. Gerontol. 85, 48–58. 10.1016/j.exger.2016.09.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Pinsault, N. , and Vuillerme, N. (2008). “ The effects of scale display of visual feedback on postural control during quiet standing in healthy elderly subjects,” Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab. 89, 1772–1774. 10.1016/j.apmr.2008.02.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Raffegeneau, T. E. , Haddad, J. M. , Huber, J. E. , and Rietdyk, S. (2018). “ Walking while talking: Young adults flexibly allocate resources between speech and gait,” Gait Post. 64, 59–62. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.05.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Ross, J. M. , Will, O. J. , McGann, Z. , and Balasubramaniam, R. (2016). “ Auditory white noise reduces age-related fluctuations in balance,” Neurosci. Lett. 630, 216–221. 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.07.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Ruffieux, J. , Keller, M. , Lauber, B. , and Taube, W. (2015). “ Changes in standing and walking performance under dual-task conditions across the lifespan,” Sports Med. 45, 1739–1758. 10.1007/s40279-015-0369-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Rumalla, K. , Karim, A. M. , and Hullar, T. E. (2015). “ The effect of hearing aids on postural stability,” Laryngoscope 125, 720–723. 10.1002/lary.24974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Shinn-Cunningham, B. , Ruggles, D. R. , and Bharadwaj, H. (2013). “ How early aging and environment interact in everyday listening: From brainstem to behavior through modeling,” in Basic Aspects of Hearing, Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, edited by Moore B. C. J., Patterson R., and Winter I. ( Springer, New York: ), Vol. 787, pp. 501–510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Stevens, M. N. , Barbour, D. L. , Gronski, M. P. , and Hullar, T. E. (2016). “ Auditory contributions to maintaining balance,” J. Vestib. Res. 26, 433–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Swanenburg, J. , de Bruin, E. D. , Favero, K. , Uebelhart, D. , and Mulder, T. (2008). “ The reliability of postural balance measures in single and dual tasking in elderly fallers and non-fallers,” BMC Musculoskel. Disord. 9, 162. 10.1186/1471-2474-9-162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Thomas, E. , Martines, F. , Bianco, A. , Messina, G. , Giustino, G. , Zangla, D. , Iovane, A. , and Palma, A. (2018). “ Decreased postural control in people with moderate hearing loss,” Medicine (Baltimore) 97(14), e0244. 10.1097/MD.0000000000010244 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Tomioka, K. , Harano, A. , Hazaki, K. , Morikawa, M. , Iwamoto, J. , Saeki, K. , Okamoto, N. , and Kurumatani, N. (2015). “ Walking speed is associated with self-perceived hearing handicap in high-functioning older adults: The Fujiwara-kyo study,” Geriat. Gerontol. Intern. 15, 745–754. 10.1111/ggi.12344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Tremblay, K. L. , Pinto, A. , Fischer, M. E. , Klein, B. E. K. , Klein, R. , Levy, S. , Tweed, T. S. , and Cruickshanks, K. J. (2015). “ Self-reported hearing difficulties among adults with normal audiograms: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study,” Ear Hear. 36, e290–e299. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Vaillant, J. , Vuillerme, N. , Janvy, A. , Louis, F. , Juvin, R. , and Nougier, V. (2004). “ Mirror versus stationary cross feedback in controlling the center of foot pressure displacement in quiet standing in elderly subjects,” Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab. 85, 1962–1965. 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Van Emmerik, R. E. , and Van Wegen, E. E. (2002). “ On the functional aspects of variability in postural control,” Exer. Sport Sci. Rev. 30, 177–183. 10.1097/00003677-200210000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Van Wegen, E. E. H. , Van Emmerik, R. E. A. , and Riccio, G. E. (2002). “ Postural orientation: Age-related changes in variability and time-to-boundary,” Human Mov. Sci. 21, 61–84. 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00077-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Viljanen, A. , Kaprio, J. , Pyykko, I. , Sorri, M. , Koskenvuo, M. , and Rantanen, T. (2009a). “ Hearing acuity as a predictor of walking difficulties in older women,” J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 57, 2282–2286. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02553.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Viljanen, A. , Kaprio, J. , Pyykko, I. , Sorri, M. , Pajala, S. , Kauppinen, M. , Koskenvuo, M. , and Rantanen, T. (2009b). “ Hearing as a predictor of falls and postural balance in older female twins,” J. Gerontol. A Bio. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 312–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Vitkovic, J. , Le, C. , Lee, S.-L. , and Clark, R. A. (2016). “ The contribution of hearing and hearing loss to balance control,” Audiol. Neurotol. 21, 195–202. 10.1159/000445100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Weaver, T. S. , Shayman, C. S. , and Hullar, T. E. (2017). “ The effect of hearing aids and cochlear implants on balance during gait,” Otol. Neurotol. 38, 1327–1332. 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Wiley, T. L. , Cruickshanks, K. J. , Nondahl, D. M. , Tweed, T. S. , Klein, R. , and Klein, B. E. (1998). “ Aging and word recognition in competing message,” J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 9, 191–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Yardley, L. , Gardner, M. , Leadbetter, A. , and Lavie, N. (1999). “ Effect of articulatory and mental tasks on postural control,” Neuroreport 10, 215–219. 10.1097/00001756-199902050-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Zhong, X. , and Yost, W. A. (2013). “ Relationship between postural stability and spatial hearing,” J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 24, 782–788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America are provided here courtesy of Acoustical Society of America

RESOURCES