Abstract
Sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances are associated with development of neurodegenerative diseases and related pathophysiological processes in the brain. We determined the cross-sectional relation of sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances with plasma-based biomarkers that might signal neurodegenerative disease, in 4712 middle-aged and elderly non-demented persons. Sleep and activity rhythms were measured using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index and actigraphy. Simoa assays were used to measure plasma levels of neurofilament light chain, and additionally β-amyloid 40, β-amyloid 42, and total-tau. We used linear regression, adjusting for relevant confounders, and corrected for multiple testing. We found no associations of self-rated sleep, actigraphy-estimated sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with neurofilament light chain after confounder adjustment and correction for multiple testing, except for a non-linear association of self-rated time in bed with neurofilament light chain (P = 2.5*10−4). Similarly, we observed no significant associations with β-amyloid 40, β-amyloid 42, and total-tau after multiple testing correction. We conclude that sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances were not consistently associated with neuronal damage as indicated by plasma neurofilament light chain in this population-based sample middle-aged and elderly non-demented persons. Further studies are needed to determine the associations of sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances with NfL-related neuronal damage.
Subject terms: Sleep, Risk factors, Dementia, Alzheimer's disease, Neural ageing
Introduction
Sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances have been implicated in the etiology of clinical neurodegenerative diseases and several pathophysiological processes in the brain1–4. The habitual level of disturbed sleep and 24-h activity rhythms, as a repetitive exposure of potential insults to the brain over a prolonged period of time1,3, may be particularly important for pathophysiological processes in the brain. Although most studies on the effect of sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances have focused on β-amyloid and tau pathology, central hallmarks of Alzheimer’s disease1,3,5, plasma markers of neuronal damage have been studied less5.
Neuronal damage can be captured in vivo by cerebrospinal fluid levels of the cytoskeletal protein neurofilament light chain (NfL)6,7. Importantly, NfL can also be determined less invasively in blood8, higher levels of which may mark neurodegenerative diseases and other neurological disorders9–11. Moreover, NfL in blood has been shown to consistently rise at a higher rate years before the diagnosis in persons who eventually develop Alzheimer’s disease versus healthy controls10,12. This suggests NfL can be used as a biomarker of clinically relevant pathophysiological processes in the brain, even though NfL in blood may lag behind neuronal damage, e.g. one month after an isolated neurosurgical trauma13. Additionally, at a cellular level, NfL release may signal neuro-axonal damage that does not necessarily lead to neuronal loss14,15. This suggest that NfL may be of added value to existing studies using non-invasive neuroimaging markers as it may detect more subtle damage to the brain5,16,17.
Several studies used NfL to determine the relation of sleep, but not yet 24-h activity rhythms, with neuronal damage18–25. One study showed that persons with chronic insomnia disorder have higher serum NfL than controls, which may decrease after treatment18. Others found no relation of disordered, subjectively impaired or experimentally deprived sleep with NfL in CSF or plasma19–25. No large-scale population-based study has yet investigated associations of objectively estimated habitual sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances with neuronal damage indicated by NfL, even though this repetitive exposure of disturbed sleep might impact neuronal damage.
We therefore tested the association of habitual sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with neuronal damage in middle-aged and elderly non-demented individuals from the population-based Rotterdam Study cohort. We assessed characteristics of habitual sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with a self-reported questionnaire indicating sleep quality over the past month and one week of actigraphy, which estimates sleep and 24-h activity rhythms objectively based on wrist movement. These modalities have been suggested to tap into different aspects of habitual sleep26. We assessed NfL in plasma to indicate neuronal damage. We hypothesized that self-rated and actigraphy-estimated poor sleep, and disturbed 24-h activity rhythms were associated with higher plasma NfL. For comparison, we also studied associations of sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with other plasma biomarkers of pathophysiological processes in the brain (β-amyloid 40 [Aβ40], Aβ42, and total tau [t-tau]).
Methods
Study setting
This study is embedded in the population-based, prospective Rotterdam Study cohort, which includes individuals from a suburban district in Rotterdam, the Netherlands27. The cohort was initiated in 1990, including 7983 participants aged ≥ 55 years, and was expanded in 2000 with 3011 participants aged ≥ 55 years, and again in 2006 with persons aged ≥ 45 years, totaling 14,926 participants. Examination rounds include a home interview and subsequent visits to our dedicated research center, and are repeated every 4 to 5 years.
The Rotterdam Study has been approved by the medical ethics committee of the Erasmus MC (registration number MEC 02.1015) according to the Population Screening Act, executed by the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports of the Netherlands. The study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Study population
Between 2002 and 2005, 6044 participants from the initiation and first expansion cohort underwent venipuncture at the research center. Of those, 5069 had sufficient plasma stores available for analyzing biomarkers. We excluded 232 persons without valid data on plasma NfL, and 20 persons with all-cause dementia to focus on at-risk individuals only. From the remaining 4817 participants, 4712 provided valid data on ≥ 1 questionnaire-derived sleep parameter (4353 persons provided data on all parameters).
Also, out of aforementioned 4817 participants, 1346 individuals were invited to participate in an actigraphy study28; 970 agreed. Of these, 849 persons (88%) provided valid data for a minimum of 4 consecutive 24-h periods28.
Self-rated sleep
Participants rated their sleep using a Dutch version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI29). The PSQI measures sleep over the past month, and has good test–retest reliability and validity in a non-clinical sample of older adults. Items include bedtimes and total sleep time at night, from which we derived time in bed and sleep efficiency, and time to fall asleep (sleep latency). Additionally, all items were summed to obtain the global PSQI score, indicating subjective sleep quality. The PSQI score ranges from 0–21, and higher scores indicate a poorer subjective sleep quality.
We excluded persons missing ≥ 2 PSQI components (n = 60), and calculated a weighted global PSQI score when only 1 component was missing (n = 173) by multiplying the six-component sum score by 7/6. The PSQI was completed a median of 18 days (interquartile range [IQR] = 17–19) before venipuncture.
Objectively estimated sleep and 24-h activity rhythms
Participants wore an actigraph (Actiwatch model AW4, Cambridge Technology Ltd.) which measures acceleration summed as ‘activity counts’ per 30-s epochs. We instructed participants to wear the actigraph for 7 days and nights around the non-dominant wrist, and to remove it only while bathing. Participants had to press a marker button on the device when attempting to fall asleep (hereafter: ‘lights out’), and when getting out of bed the next morning (hereafter: ‘lights on’). They also kept a daily sleep diary28. Missing marker times (25%) were imputed from the diary, or estimated by inspecting recordings if diary times were missing. We removed 24-h periods containing > 3 continuous hours without activity to prevent bias from removal of data around specific times of the day. Actigraphy recordings averaged 137.9 ± 13.6 h, and were initiated a median of 28 days (IQR 9–287) after venipuncture. Within the marker-defined time in bed, we estimated sleep (i.e. total sleep time) and wakefulness using a validated algorithm with a threshold of 20 counts28. We defined ‘sleep start’ as the midpoint of the first immobile ≥ 10 min period after ‘lights out’ with ≤ 1 epoch containing activity28. Sleep onset latency was calculated as the time from ‘lights out’ to ‘sleep start’, and wake after sleep onset as wakefulness after ‘sleep start’. We calculated sleep efficiency as total sleep time divided by time in bed * 100%.
We also used counts to calculate non-parametric indices of the 24-h activity rhythm: Intradaily variability which indicates the amount of alterations of activity-inactivity, interdaily stability which indicates how daily profiles in the recording resemble each other, and onset time of the least active 5 consecutive hours (L5 onset) which indicates the phase of lowest activity. A disturbed 24-h activity rhythm is reflected by high intradaily variability and interdaily stability.
Measurement of plasma concentrations of NfL, Aβ40, Aβ42, and t-tau
Participants came to the dedicated research center where a venipuncture was performed between 8:00 and 10:30 in the morning after an overnight fast. Blood was sampled in ethylenediamine tetra-acetic acid-treated containers and centrifuged. The plasma was aliquoted and frozen at − 80 °C according to standard procedures. In 2018, samples were assessed through the Janssen Prevention Center (Leiden, NL) which sent plasma to the laboratory facilities of Quanterix (Lexington, MA, USA). Analyses were performed on a single molecule array (Simoa) HD-1 analyzer platform in two batches30. Concentrations of biomarkers were measured using the NF-light advantage kit31 (for NfL), and the Simoa Human Neurology 3-Plex A assay (for Aβ40, Aβ42, and t-tau). Samples were tested in duplicate, two quality control samples were run on each plate per biomarker. Technical data on assay performance was published previously10. Data was excluded if duplicates or single measurements were missing, if the concentration coefficient of variation exceeded 20%, or if control samples were out of range.
Covariates
As potential confounders we selected possible causes of the determinant or the outcome, or proxies of such factors32–34, in line with recent literature35. We considered age, sex, education (categorized as primary, secondary/lower vocational, intermediate vocational and higher vocational/university), batch number of biomarker analysis, time interval between measurements of sleep and biomarker, habitual alcohol consumption, presence of self-reported paid employment, smoking status (never, former, current), body mass index (BMI), presence of hypertension (resting blood pressure > 140/90 mmHg, or use of blood pressure-lowering medication), presence of diabetes mellitus (fasting serum glucose level ≥ 7.0 mmol/l, or use of glucose-lowering medication), total cholesterol level in serum in mmol/l, a positive history of heart disease (myocardial infarction, heart failure, or coronary revascularization procedure), and possible sleep apnea defined using PSQI items on loud snoring and respiratory pauses. Measurements were performed during the home interview or center visits, as detailed previously36.
Additionally, we assessed clinically relevant depressive symptoms defined as a score < 16 on the validated Dutch version of the Centre for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale (CES-D), cognitive impairment defined by a Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≤ 25, and a history of stroke ascertained during examination rounds and by continuous monitoring as detailed previously.
Statistical analysis
All sleep parameters were winsorized at 3 SD from the mean, and subsequently standardized. Biomarker values were log-transformed (base = 2) to approach a normal distribution, winsorized to 3 SD and standardized to facilitate comparison across different biomarkers.
We used linear regression to analyze the association of sleep and 24-h activity rhythm parameters with plasma NfL. We investigated self-rated sleep (PSQI score, total sleep time, sleep onset latency, time in bed, and sleep efficiency), actigraphy-estimated sleep (total sleep time, sleep onset latency, wake after sleep onset, time in bed, sleep efficiency), 24-h activity rhythms (intradaily variability, interdaily stability and L5 onset) and times of ‘lights out’ and ‘lights on’. Analyses were adjusted for age, sex, educational level, batch, and time interval between measurements of sleep and biomarkers (model 1), and additionally for alcohol consumption, paid employment status, smoking status, BMI, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, total cholesterol, history of heart disease, and possible sleep apnea (model 2). Furthermore, as total sleep time and time in bed are known to show U-shaped relations with various poor health outcomes, we assessed non-linear associations of these parameters (self-rated and actigraphy-estimated) with NfL by adding their quadratic terms to the model.
We additionally restricted analyses to persons without clinically relevant depressive symptoms, without cognitive impairment, and without prevalent stroke. Depressive symptoms may strongly influence sleep and sleep’s appraisal37, and depression is associated with cortical abnormalities38. Cognitive impairment is a proxy for the accumulation of detrimental processes in the brain potentially influencing the relation of sleep or 24-h activity rhythms with neurodegeneration, and may influence reporting of sleep. Likewise, prevalent stroke is a proxy for higher loads of cerebrovascular disease potentially affecting NfL and sleep33,34.
Besides NfL, other biomarkers may also be potentially important. Therefore, we also examined associations of sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with other plasma biomarkers of neurodegenerative disease: Aβ40, Aβ42, and t-tau.
We performed statistical testing with two-tailed tests, and considered associations below the threshold of P < 0.0046 as statistically significant, which corrected for testing 15 self-rated and actigraphy-estimated parameters in this study. This threshold was defined by computing the number of effective tests (Meff = 11.14) based on correlations between all parameters, and applying a Sidak correction. We considered associations as nominally significant at P < 0.05.
Missing values on covariates were imputed using five multiple imputations with IBM SPSS Statistics version 24 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY). Analyses were performed with R software39.
Results
We included 4712 participants (mean age 72 ± 8 years, 57% female), see Table 1. The median plasma NfL level was 13.3 pg/mL (IQR 10.0–18.4).
Table 1.
Characteristic (unit) | Total sample | Actigraphy subsample |
---|---|---|
N = 4712 | N = 849 | |
Age at sleep measurement (years) | 71.1 (66.1–77.2) | 66.7 (63.7–73.1) |
Female | 2700 (57%) | 433 (51%) |
Educational level | ||
Primary education | 536 (11%) | 53 (6%) |
Lower/intermediate or lower vocational | 2088 (44%) | 368 (43%) |
Higher or intermediate vocational | 1436 (31%) | 281 (33%) |
Higher vocational or university | 652 (14%) | 147 (18%) |
Alcohol consumption (g/day) | 7 (1–20) | 9 (1–20) |
Paid employment | 303 (6%) | 74 (9%) |
Smoking status | ||
Never | 1480 (31%) | 264 (31%) |
Former | 2598 (55%) | 477 (56%) |
Current | 634 (13%) | 108 (13%) |
Body mass index (kg/m2) | 27.6 ± 4.1 | 27.9 ± 4.0 |
Hypertension | 2569 (54%) | 414 (49%) |
Diabetes mellitus | 472 (10%) | 84 (10%) |
Total cholesterol in serum (mmol/l) | 5.6 ± 1.0 | 5.7 ± 1.0 |
History of heart disease | 704 (15%) | 89 (10%) |
Possible sleep apnea | 580 (12%) | 113 (13%) |
Self-rated sleep | ||
Global PSQI score | 3 (2–6) | 3 (1–6) |
Duration (h) | 6.8 ± 1.3 | 6.9 ± 1.2 |
Latency (min) | 10 (5–30) | 10 (5–30) |
Time in bed (h) | 7.7 ± 1.1 | 7.7 ± 1.0 |
Efficiency (%) | 93 (83–99) | 93 (86–100) |
Actigraphic sleep and 24 h activity rhythms | ||
Total sleep time (h) | – | 6.5 ± 0.8 |
Latency (min) | – | 18 (12–26) |
Wake after sleep onset (h) | – | 1.1 (0.9–1.4) |
Time in bed (h)a | – | 8.3 ± 0.8 |
Efficiency (%) | – | 79 (74–83) |
Intradaily variability (score) | – | 0.41 (0.34–0.52) |
Interdaily stability (score) | – | 0.83 (0.76–0.88) |
L5 onset (hh:mm) | – | 01:53 ± 01:10 |
‘Lights out’ time (hh:mm)a | – | 23:51 ± 00:48 |
‘Lights on’ time (hh:mm)a | – | 08:10 ± 00:45 |
Neurofilament light chain (pg/ml) | 13 (10–18) | 11 (9–15) |
Range | 3–390 | 4–214 |
Characteristics of the study population. Values are expressed as frequency (%) for categorical variables and mean ± standard deviation or median (1st quartile–3rd quartile) for continuous variables. Includes imputed values for covariates. Missing values for self-rated sleep parameters were 60 for PSQI score, 58 for sleep duration, 198 for sleep latency, 159 for time in bed, and 212 for sleep efficiency.
aActigraphic time in bed was not automatically calculated but based on ‘lights out’ and ’lights on’ times specified daily by participants using the actigraph marker buttons and a sleep diary.
L5 least active 5 h of the day, N sample size, PSQI Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.
Sleep parameters
For self-rated sleep parameters, we found no significant linear associations with plasma NfL in model 2 (Table 2). The association of self-rated longer time in bed with higher NfL in model 1 (beta per standard deviation [SD] increase in self-rated time in bed of 0.038 SD increase in log2(NfL), 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.015; 0.060, P = 0.0013) attenuated after additional adjustment (Table 2). The quadratic term of self-rated time in bed was significantly associated with NfL in model 2 (P = 2.5*10−4). Compared to a self-rated normal time in bed (7–9 h), spending a long time in bed (> 9 h) was significantly associated with higher NfL (0.171, 95% CI 0.086; 0.256, P = 7.7*10−5), but spending a short time in bed (< 7 h) was not (− 0.008, 95% CI − 0.057; 0.041, P = 0.75).
Table 2.
Independent variables | Model 1 | Model 2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Beta (95% CI) | P | Beta (95% CI) | P | |
Self-rated | ||||
PSQI score | 0.023 (− 0.001; 0.046) | 0.06 | 0.014 (− 0.009; 0.036) | 0.23 |
Sleep duration | 0.005 (− 0.018; 0.027) | 0.68 | 0.007 (− 0.015; 0.028) | 0.56 |
Sleep latency | 0.017 (− 0.010; 0.044) | 0.23 | 0.006 (− 0.020; 0.032) | 0.65 |
Time in bed | 0.038 (0.015; 0.060) | 0.001 | 0.032 (0.009; 0.054) | 0.01 |
Sleep efficiency | − 0.032 (− 0.056; − 0.008) | 0.01 | − 0.025 (− 0.048; − 0.001) | 0.04 |
Actigraphy-estimated | ||||
Total sleep time | − 0.006 (− 0.058; 0.047) | 0.83 | − 0.030 (− 0.082; 0.022) | 0.26 |
Sleep latency | − 0.008 (− 0.062; 0.045) | 0.76 | − 0.004 (− 0.057; 0.048) | 0.88 |
Wake after sleep onset | 0.023 (− 0.028; 0.073) | 0.37 | 0.021 (− 0.028; 0.071) | 0.40 |
Time in beda | 0.001 (− 0.051; 0.053) | 0.97 | − 0.021 (− 0.073; 0.030) | 0.41 |
Sleep efficiency | − 0.004 (− 0.055; 0.047) | 0.87 | − 0.016 (− 0.066; 0.034) | 0.52 |
Estimates represent that, with a standard deviation increase in the independent variable, the level of neurofilament light chain (NfL) increases by beta*standard deviation (SD) log2 pg/mL. Estimates were obtained with linear regression, adjusted for age and sex, educational level, batch, time interval between measurement of sleep and biomarkers (model 1), and additionally for alcohol consumption, employment status, smoking status, body mass index, presence of hypertension, presence of diabetes mellitus, total serum cholesterol level, history of cardiovascular disease, and possible sleep apnea (model 2). Analyses were performed in n = 4652 persons for PSQI score, in n = 4654 for sleep duration, in n = 4514 for sleep latency, in n = 4552 for time in bed, and in n = 4499 for sleep efficiency. Actigraphy analyses were performed in 849 persons.
aPlease note that actigraphy-derived time in bed was not automatically calculated but based on ‘lights out’ and ‘lights on’ times, specified daily by participants using actigraph marker buttons and a sleep diary.
CI confidence interval, PSQI Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.
Actigraphy-estimated sleep parameters were not related to NfL in plasma (Table 2). We found no non-linear associations for actigraphy-estimated total sleep time and time in bed.
24-h activity rhythm parameters
We observed no significant associations of 24-h activity rhythm parameters with NfL beyond the multiple testing corrected threshold (Table 3).
Table 3.
Independent variables | Model 1 | Model 2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Beta (95% CI) | P | Beta (95% CI) | P | |
Intradaily variability | 0.022 (− 0.033; 0.078) | 0.43 | 0.036 (− 0.019; 0.092) | 0.19 |
Interdaily stability | 0.000 (− 0.051; 0.052) | 0.99 | − 0.017 (− 0.068; 0.033) | 0.50 |
L5 onset | − 0.008 (− 0.059; 0.043) | 0.76 | − 0.005 (− 0.054; 0.045) | 0.85 |
‘Lights out’ timea | − 0.050 (− 0.103; 0.004) | 0.07 | − 0.033 (− 0.086; 0.020) | 0.22 |
‘Lights on’ timea | − 0.044 (− 0.095; 0.008) | 0.10 | − 0.049 (− 0.100; 0.001) | 0.06 |
Estimates represent that, with a standard deviation increase in the independent variable, the level of neurofilament light chain (NfL) increases by beta*standard deviation (SD) log2 pg/mL. Estimates were obtained with linear regression, adjusted for age and sex, educational level, batch, time interval between measurement of sleep and biomarkers (model 1), and additionally for alcohol consumption, employment status, smoking status, body mass index, presence of hypertension, presence of diabetes mellitus, total serum cholesterol level, history of cardiovascular disease, and possible sleep apnea (model 2). Analyses were all performed in 849 persons.
aPlease note that actigraphy-derived bedtimes were specified daily by participants using actigraph marker buttons and a sleep diary.
CI confidence interval, L5 average least active 5 h of the day, SD standard deviation.
Sensitivity analysis
Restricting the main analysis to individuals without clinically relevant depressive symptoms, without cognitive impairment or stroke overall did not substantially change effect sizes (Table 4). For self-rated time in bed, estimates were attenuated after excluding persons with cognitive impairment, and to a lesser extent after excluding persons with stroke, but not after excluding those with clinically relevant depressive symptoms (Table 4).
Table 4.
Independent variables | No depressive symptoms | No cognitive impairment | No stroke |
---|---|---|---|
Beta (95% CI) | Beta (95% CI) | Beta (95% CI) | |
Self-rated | |||
PSQI | − 0.001 (− 0.026; 0.024) | 0.007 (− 0.017; 0.032) | 0.010 (− 0.013; 0.033) |
TST | 0.015 (− 0.009; 0.039) | 0.001 (− 0.023; 0.025) | 0.006 (− 0.016; 0.028) |
SOL | 0.001 (− 0.029; 0.030) | − 0.001 (− 0.029; 0.028) | 0.001 (− 0.026; 0.028) |
TIB | 0.036 (0.012; 0.059)† | 0.022 (− 0.002; 0.046) | 0.028 (0.006; 0.051)* |
SE | − 0.017 (− 0.042; 0.009) | − 0.022 (− 0.047; 0.003) | − 0.022 (− 0.046; 0.001) |
Actigraphy | |||
TST | − 0.024 (− 0.077; 0.030) | − 0.027 (− 0.085; 0.030) | − 0.029 (− 0.082; 0.025) |
SOL | − 0.013 (− 0.069; 0.042) | − 0.003 (− 0.061; 0.054) | − 0.003 (− 0.055; 0.049) |
WASO | 0.011 (− 0.040; 0.062) | 0.016 (− 0.037; 0.069) | 0.021 (− 0.029; 0.070) |
TIB‡ | − 0.032 (− 0.086; 0.021) | − 0.029 (− 0.085; 0.028) | − 0.016 (− 0.069; 0.036) |
SE | − 0.001 (− 0.053; 0.051) | − 0.010 (− 0.064; 0.044) | − 0.016 (− 0.066; 0.034) |
IV | 0.018 (− 0.039; 0.076) | 0.046 (− 0.015; 0.107) | 0.042 (− 0.012; 0.097) |
IS | − 0.006 (− 0.058; 0.047) | − 0.018 (− 0.074; 0.038) | − 0.010 (− 0.060; 0.041) |
L5 onset | − 0.014 (− 0.065; 0.037) | − 0.023 (− 0.076; 0.031) | 0.003 (− 0.047; 0.053) |
Lights out‡ | − 0.035 (− 0.089; 0.020) | − 0.029 (− 0.086; 0.027) | − 0.039 (− 0.091; 0.014) |
Lights ona | − 0.063 (− 0.115; − 0.010)* | − 0.055 (− 0.111; 0.001) | − 0.050 (− 0.101; 0.001) |
Absence of depressive symptoms was defined as CES-D score ≥ 16; absence of cognitive impairment was defined as MMSE score > 25.
Estimates represent that, with a standard deviation increase in the independent variable, the level of neurofilament light chain (NfL) increases by beta*standard deviation (SD) log2 pg/mL. Estimates were obtained with linear regression, adjusted for age and sex, educational level, batch, time interval between measurement of sleep and biomarkers, alcohol consumption, employment status, smoking status, body mass index, presence of hypertension, presence of diabetes mellitus, total serum cholesterol level, history of cardiovascular disease, and possible sleep apnea. For self-rated independent variables, cases per analysis ranged from 4048 to 4181 restricted to persons without depressive symptoms, from 3908 to 4042 in persons without cognitive impairment, and from 4288 to 4431 in persons without prevalent stroke. For actigraphy-derived independent variables, cases in analyses were n = 785 (depressive symptoms), n = 756 (cognitive impairment) and n = 817 (stroke).
*Nominal significance at P < 0.05.
†P = 0.0034.
aActigraphic time in bed was not automatically calculated but determined by ‘lights out’ and ’lights on’ times specified through pressing actigraph marker buttons and the sleep diary.
CES-D Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale, CI confidence interval, IS interdaily stability, IV intradaily variability, L5 average least active 5 h of the day, MMSE mini-mental state examination, PSQI Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, SD standard deviation, SE sleep efficiency, SOL sleep onset latency, TIB time in bed, TST total sleep time, WASO wake after sleep onset.
Associations with plasma Aβ-isoforms and total tau
For comparison, we also investigated associations of sleep and 24-h activity rhythm parameters with other biomarkers of neurodegenerative disease. Median (IQR) plasma levels in pg/mL for 4712 persons were 259.5 (230.3–294.0) for Aβ40, 10.3 (8.8–11.9) for Aβ42, and 2.4 (1.9–3.0) for t-tau. In comparison to associations with NfL, we observed more associations exceeding P < 0.05 including associations of poorer subjective sleep quality, longer self-rated time in bed and lower self-rated sleep efficiency with higher plasma concentrations of β-amyloid isoforms (Table 5). Yet, no association was statistically significant beyond the threshold corrected for multiple testing (Table 5).
Table 5.
Independent variables | β-amyloid 40 | β-amyloid 42 | Total tau |
---|---|---|---|
Beta (95% CI) | Beta (95% CI) | Beta (95% CI) | |
Self-rated | |||
PSQI score | 0.020 (− 0.008; 0.047) | 0.030 (0.002; 0.057)* | − 0.016 (− 0.045; 0.013) |
TST | 0.005 (− 0.021; 0.032) | − 0.007 (− 0.034; 0.020) | 0.018 (− 0.009; 0.046) |
SOL | 0.008 (− 0.025; 0.040) | 0.009 (− 0.023; 0.041) | − 0.002 (− 0.036; 0.031) |
TIB | 0.033 (0.006; 0.060)* | 0.032 (0.005; 0.059)* | 0.019 (− 0.009; 0.047) |
SE | − 0.020 (− 0.048; 0.008) | − 0.038 (− 0.066; − 0.010)* | 0.008 (− 0.021; 0.038) |
Actigraphy | |||
TST | − 0.051 (− 0.116; 0.013) | − 0.025 (− 0.086; 0.036) | 0.034 (− 0.032; 0.100) |
SOL | − 0.001 (− 0.066; 0.064) | 0.019 (− 0.042; 0.080) | 0.007 (− 0.060; 0.074) |
WASO | 0.049 (− 0.012; 0.110) | 0.051 (− 0.006; 0.109) | 0.045 (− 0.018; 0.108) |
TIBa | − 0.036 (− 0.099; 0.028) | 0.005 (− 0.055; 0.065) | 0.061 (− 0.004; 0.127) |
SE | − 0.047 (− 0.109; 0.015) | − 0.050 (− 0.108; 0.008) | − 0.028 (− 0.092; 0.036) |
IV | 0.066 (− 0.002; 0.134) | − 0.002 (− 0.067; 0.062) | − 0.007 (− 0.077; 0.063) |
IS | − 0.022 (− 0.085; 0.041) | 0.018 (− 0.041; 0.077) | − 0.025 (− 0.089; 0.040) |
L5 onset | 0.019 (− 0.042; 0.080) | 0.027 (− 0.031; 0.085) | 0.020 (− 0.043; 0.083) |
Lights outa | 0.007 (− 0.058; 0.072) | − 0.017 (− 0.079; 0.044) | 0.019 (− 0.048; 0.085) |
Lights ona | − 0.027 (− 0.089; 0.036) | − 0.017 (− 0.076; 0.042) | 0.074 (0.010; 0.139)* |
Estimates represent that, with a standard deviation increase in the independent variable, the level of neurofilament light chain (NfL) increases by beta*standard deviation (SD) log2 pg/mL. Estimates were obtained with linear regression, adjusted for age and sex, educational level, batch, time interval between measurement of sleep and biomarkers, alcohol consumption, employment status, smoking status, body mass index, presence of hypertension, presence of diabetes mellitus, total serum cholesterol level, history of cardiovascular disease, and possible sleep apnea. Numbers of cases per analysis differed as both independent variables and outcomes had different numbers of missing values. For self-rated independent variables, numbers varied from 4486 (association total sleep time with total tau) to 4145 (sleep efficiency with β-amyloid 42). For actigraphy-derived independent variables (all n = 849), numbers varied from 824 (total tau) to 806 (β-amyloid 42).
*Nominal significance at P < 0.05.
aActigraphic time in bed was not automatically calculated but based on ‘lights out’ and ’lights on’ times specified by participants.
CI confidence interval, IS interdaily stability, IV intradaily variability, L5 average least active 5 h of the day, PSQI Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, SD standard deviation, SE sleep efficiency, SOL sleep onset latency, TIB time in bed, TST total sleep time, WASO wake after sleep onset.
Discussion
In this population-based study in middle-aged and elderly persons, sleep and 24-h activity rhythms were not associated with plasma NfL, except for a non-linear association of self-rated time in bed with NfL.
We only found a non-linear association of self-rated time in bed with NfL. This is in line with findings that more sedentary behavior, although distinct from sleep, is linked to various poor health outcomes which may impact neuronal damage40. We might speculate that the association of self-rated long time in bed with higher plasma NfL could be due to a shared common cause such as overall poor health or underlying subclinical disease41,42. Indeed, the linear association of self-rated time in bed with NfL is attenuated after additional adjustment in model 2 and when persons with cognitive impairment or stroke, but not depressive symptoms, were excluded. This suggests that poor physical health or clinical diseases could underlie the association of time in bed with NfL. Other potential factors underlying the link of longer time in bed with poor health outcomes may be fatigue, immune function, or sleep apnea42. Further research may consider investigating if self-rated time in bed indeed validly marks poor health, and how it relates to other sleep-related markers41. Of note, this association was only present when time in bed was assessed through general retrospective ratings of bedtimes over the last month, but not when time in bed was based on averages obtained from prospectively collected marker buttons or daily sleep diaries. This might be explained by a difference in operationalization: the PSQI assesses time in bed independent of whether a person tries to sleep when in bed whereas this is taken into account in the assessments with actigraphy and sleep diary. Additionally, as retrospective questionnaire ratings tend to suffer more from recall bias than prospective measurements, it might also be that the association is driven by factors related to recall bias rather than time in bed per se, such as cognitive impairment.
Recently, we demonstrated that actigraphy-estimated poor sleep was associated with the risk of clinical all-cause dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in the Rotterdam Study. Yet, sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances are not clearly associated with NfL in the current study which is embedded in the same cohort, suggesting that poor sleep does not affect neuronal damage as indicated by NfL. Our finding partly contradicts findings from other studies implementing non-invasive structural neuroimaging which do suggest that poor sleep and 24-h activity rhythms are related to global or regional loss of tissue or integrity5,16,17. Together, these and our findings suggest a role for non-neuronal, i.e. glial cells in the relation between sleep and pathology of the brain. A methodological explanation may be reverse causation; brain changes picked up by imaging affect sleep, while higher levels of plasma NfL may not necessarily indicate enough damage to the brain to affect sleep or activity rhythms. Of note, our findings are in line with previous studies that show that neuroimaging markers and NfL are correlated in the presence of neurodegenerative disease9,43, but show little to no correlations in otherwise healthy individuals43–45. Possibly, NfL may reflect brain pathology on imaging only once a certain threshold is exceeded, a suggestion also highlighted by a recent study46.
The lack of an association of sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with NfL could be explained in several ways. First, habitually disturbed sleep and 24-h activity rhythms may affect neuronal health yet do not lead to NfL release. At a cellular level, release of NfL, most abundantly present in the axon, occurs after apoptosis or axon-specific neuronal insults14,15. Sleep or 24-h activity rhythm disturbances may involve neuronal insults that, through invoking various stress responses, impair neuronal function but do not lead to apoptosis. Second, we measured sleep with questionnaires and actigraphy. These measurements may not have captured relevant sleep disorders such as insomnia or sleep-disordered breathing, or physiological aspects of sleep such as slow-wave activity. This could explain why a previous study showed higher serum NfL in persons with chronic insomnia versus controls18, while we found no association of subjective sleep quality, an insomnia-related construct, with NfL in the general population. Third, neuronal insults related to sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances may not be severe enough to elevate NfL in plasma. Our hypothesis on the detrimental effects of poor sleep for neuronal health was based on mechanistic, animal-based studies that mostly used experimental sleep deprivation. However, we studied observational differences in habitual sleep, and these more chronic disturbances might be associated with less harm to neuronal health than experimentally induced reductions in sleep. Indeed, a previous study also did not find an association of observational differences in subjective sleep quality with NfL, using CSF measurements20. Additionally, experimentally depriving individuals of sleep to 4 h for five nights did not affect NfL in CSF19 or blood22. One night of total sleep deprivation also did not change NfL in blood24.
Compared to NfL, associations of sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with Aβ40, Aβ42 and t-tau in plasma were largely similar. Estimates suggested nominally significant relations for self-rated time in bed, and the related variable of sleep efficiency, comparable to findings for NfL. Yet, no associations survived multiple testing correction. These null findings for β-amyloid were in contrast to our expectations as sleep has been shown to regulate brain β-amyloid levels47, and habitual sleep has been associated with CSF β-amyloid, and parenchymal β-amyloid deposition5. Also, lower plasma Aβ42 was associated with a higher risk of Alzheimer’s disease in our cohort10. We measured Aβ42 in plasma which may differ or be less precise than measurements in CSF48, thus potentially obscuring detection of an association.
For tau, we could also not confirm findings of previous studies linking disturbed or disordered sleep to tau brain pathology23,49,50 or total tau in blood51, which may in part be explained by the use of polysomnography-derived characteristics of sleep in these studies.
Several methodological considerations need to be mentioned. First, our largely negative findings could indicate that our biomarker measurements, using plasma instead of CSF, were invalid. Yet, high NfL and reduced Aβ42 in plasma have been associated with risk of clinical all-cause dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in non-demented individuals in our cohort10, suggesting they may reflect neurodegenerative disease in a preclinical phase. Second, correlations of NfL between CSF and plasma are lower in healthy versus diseased persons, lowering our sensitivity to detect relevant plasma NfL increases, especially in the actigraphy subgroup. Third, associations with plasma NfL may not reflect increased damage but differential equilibration across fluid compartments, as poor sleep may disturb blood–brain barrier function. Fourth, cross-sectional associations may not have been detected as plasma NfL levels may lag behind neuronal injury, e.g. on average one month after an isolated neurosurgical trauma13. Yet, our single sleep measures are relatively stable over time, as are plasma NfL levels across years in relation to neurodegenerative diseases10,43, and we adjusted analyses for the time interval between measurements. Additionally, our cross-sectional design prevents us from speculating on the temporality of any associations. Fifth, actigraphy estimates may misclassify sleep and only indirectly reflect circadian functioning. Sixth, we could not investigate the influence of physical activity on our estimates, as the Actiwatch model used in this study was not suited for quantifying physical activity. Study strengths include using a large sample anchored in the general population, measuring sleep with two modalities, simultaneously investigating multiple relevant biomarkers, and correcting for various confounders.
In conclusion, our findings do not indicate a consistent relation of sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances with plasma NfL in our population-based sample of middle-aged and elderly non-demented persons. Additionally, sleep and 24-h activity rhythm disturbances seemed also unrelated to Aβ40, Aβ42 and t-tau in plasma. Further studies across different populations are needed to determine whether sleep and 24-h activity rhythms disturbances are not associated with neuronal damage assessed with plasma NfL.
Acknowledgements
We gratefully acknowledge the contributions of the Rotterdam Study participants, and participating general practitioners and pharmacists of Ommoord, Rotterdam.
Author contributions
T.S.L., M.A.I. and A.I.L. conceived or designed the work. T.S.L. and A.I.L. drafted the work. M.A.I., M.G., and A.I.L. oversaw data acquisition. T.S.L. performed data analysis. T.S.L., M.A.I., M.G., A.I.L. substantially revised the manuscript, approved the submitted version, agreed to be personally accountable for their contributions, and agreed to ensure that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of their work is appropriately investigated.
Funding
This work was supported by funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (CoSTREAM Project; Grant No. 667375). The Rotterdam Study is funded by Erasmus Medical Center and Erasmus University, Rotterdam, Netherlands Organization for the Health Research and Development (ZonMw), the Ministry of Education, Culture and Science, the Ministry for Health, Welfare and Sports, the European Commission (DG XII), and the Municipality of Rotterdam.
Data availability
Data can be obtained on request. Requests should be directed toward the management team of the Rotterdam Study (secretariat.epi@erasmusmc.nl), which has a protocol for approving data requests. Because of restrictions based on privacy regulations and informed consent of the participants, data cannot be made freely available in a public repository.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Mander BA, Winer JR, Jagust WJ, Walker MP. Sleep: a novel mechanistic pathway, biomarker, and treatment target in the pathology of Alzheimer's disease? Trends Neurosci. 2016;39:552–566. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bubu OM, Brannick M, Mortimer J, et al. Sleep, cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep. 2017;40:zsw032. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Leng Y, Musiek ES, Hu K, Cappuccio FP, Yaffe K. Association between circadian rhythms and neurodegenerative diseases. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:307–318. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30461-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Shi, L., Chen, S. J., Ma, M. Y., et al. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev.40, 4–16 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 5.Van Egroo M, Narbutas J, Chylinski D, et al. Sleep–wake regulation and the hallmarks of the pathogenesis of Alzheimer's disease. Sleep. 2019;42:zsz017. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Yuan A, Rao MV, Nixon RA. Neurofilaments at a glance. J. Cell Sci. 2012;125:3257–3263. doi: 10.1242/jcs.104729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bridel C, van Wieringen WN, Zetterberg H, et al. Diagnostic value of cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light protein in neurology: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:1035–1048. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bacioglu M, Maia LF, Preische O, et al. Neurofilament light chain in blood and CSF as marker of disease progression in mouse models and in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuron. 2016;91:494–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mattsson N, Andreasson U, Zetterberg H, Blennow K. Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging I. Association of plasma neurofilament light with neurodegeneration in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74:557–566. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2016.6117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.de Wolf F, Ghanbari M, Licher S, et al. Plasma tau, neurofilament light chain and amyloid-beta levels and risk of dementia; a population-based cohort study. Brain. 2020;143:1220–1232. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Khalil M, Teunissen CE, Otto M, et al. Neurofilaments as biomarkers in neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2018;14:577–589. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0058-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mattsson N, Cullen NC, Andreasson U, Zetterberg H, Blennow K. Association between longitudinal plasma neurofilament light and neurodegeneration in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:791–799. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.0765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bergman J, Dring A, Zetterberg H, et al. Neurofilament light in CSF and serum is a sensitive marker for axonal white matter injury in MS. Neurol. Neuroimmunol. Neuroinflamm. 2016;3:e271. doi: 10.1212/NXI.0000000000000271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Petzold A. Neuronal cell death and axonal degeneration: neurofilaments as biomarkers. In: Squire LR, editor. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. New York: Academic Press; 2009. pp. 2768–2772. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Farley MM, Watkins TA. Intrinsic neuronal stress response pathways in injury and disease. Annu. Rev. Pathol. 2018;13:93–116. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pathol-012414-040354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Smagula SF, Chahine L, Metti A, et al. Regional gray matter volume links rest-activity rhythm fragmentation with past cognitive decline. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2020;28:248–251. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2019.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Baillet M, Dilharreguy B, Peres K, Dartigues JF, Mayo W, Catheline G. Activity/rest cycle and disturbances of structural backbone of cerebral networks in aging. Neuroimage. 2017;146:814–820. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zhang P, Tan CW, Chen GH, et al. Patients with chronic insomnia disorder have increased serum levels of neurofilaments, neuron-specific enolase and S100B: does organic brain damage exist? Sleep Med. 2018;48:163–171. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Olsson M, Arlig J, Hedner J, Blennow K, Zetterberg H. Sleep deprivation and cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers for Alzheimer's disease. Sleep. 2018;41:zsy005. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sprecher KE, Koscik RL, Carlsson CM, et al. Poor sleep is associated with CSF biomarkers of amyloid pathology in cognitively normal adults. Neurology. 2017;89:445–453. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jennum PJ, Ostergaard Pedersen L, Czarna Bahl JM, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of neurodegeneration are decreased or normal in narcolepsy. Sleep. 2017;40:zsw006. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Olsson M, Arlig J, Hedner J, Blennow K, Zetterberg H. Sleep deprivation and plasma biomarkers for Alzheimer's disease. Sleep Med. 2019;57:92–93. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.12.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Holth JK, Fritschi SK, Wang C, et al. The sleep–wake cycle regulates brain interstitial fluid tau in mice and CSF tau in humans. Science. 2019;363:880–884. doi: 10.1126/science.aav2546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Benedict C, Blennow K, Zetterberg H, Cedernaes J. Effects of acute sleep loss on diurnal plasma dynamics of CNS health biomarkers in young men. Neurology. 2020;94:e1181–e1189. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000008866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Baiardi S, Pizza F, Polischi B, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of neurodegeneration in narcolepsy type 1. Sleep. 2020;43:zsz215. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Buysse DJ. Sleep health: can we define it? Does it matter? Sleep. 2014;37:9–17. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ikram MA, Brusselle GGO, Murad SD, et al. The Rotterdam Study: 2018 update on objectives, design and main results. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2017;32:807–850. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0321-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Van Den Berg JF, Van Rooij FJ, Vos H, et al. Disagreement between subjective and actigraphic measures of sleep duration in a population-based study of elderly persons. J. Sleep Res. 2008;17:295–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rissin DM, Fournier DR, Piech T, et al. Simultaneous detection of single molecules and singulated ensembles of molecules enables immunoassays with broad dynamic range. Anal. Chem. 2011;83:2279–2285. doi: 10.1021/ac103161b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rohrer JD, Woollacott IO, Dick KM, et al. Serum neurofilament light chain protein is a measure of disease intensity in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 2016;87:1329–1336. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396:413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tiedt S, Duering M, Barro C, et al. Serum neurofilament light: a biomarker of neuroaxonal injury after ischemic stroke. Neurology. 2018;91:e1338–e1347. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sabanayagam C, Shankar A. Sleep duration and cardiovascular disease: results from the National Health Interview Survey. Sleep. 2010;33:1037–1042. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.8.1037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.VanderWeele TJ. Principles of confounder selection. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2019;34:211–219. doi: 10.1007/s10654-019-00494-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Licher S, Heshmatollah A, van der Willik KD, et al. Lifetime risk and multimorbidity of non-communicable diseases and disease-free life expectancy in the general population: a population-based cohort study. PLoS Med. 2019;16:e1002741. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sun Y, Shi L, Bao Y, Sun Y, Shi J, Lu L. The bidirectional relationship between sleep duration and depression in community-dwelling middle-aged and elderly individuals: evidence from a longitudinal study. Sleep Med. 2018;52:221–229. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schmaal L, Hibar DP, Samann PG, et al. Cortical abnormalities in adults and adolescents with major depression based on brain scans from 20 cohorts worldwide in the ENIGMA Major Depressive Disorder Working Group. Mol. Psychiatry. 2017;22:900–909. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [computer program]. (Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2019).
- 40.Patterson R, McNamara E, Tainio M, et al. Sedentary behaviour and risk of all-cause, cardiovascular and cancer mortality, and incident type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and dose response meta-analysis. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2018;33:811–829. doi: 10.1007/s10654-018-0380-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wallace ML, Buysse DJ, Redline S, et al. Multidimensional sleep and mortality in older adults: a machine-learning comparison with other risk factors. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2019;74:1903–1909. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glz044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Grandner MA, Drummond SP. Who are the long sleepers? Towards an understanding of the mortality relationship. Sleep Med. Rev. 2007;11:341–360. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Preische O, Schultz SA, Apel A, et al. Serum neurofilament dynamics predicts neurodegeneration and clinical progression in presymptomatic Alzheimer's disease. Nat. Med. 2019;25:277–283. doi: 10.1038/s41591-018-0304-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mielke MM, Syrjanen JA, Blennow K, et al. Plasma and CSF neurofilament light: relation to longitudinal neuroimaging and cognitive measures. Neurology. 2019;93:e252–e260. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hu H, Chen KL, Ou YN, et al. Neurofilament light chain plasma concentration predicts neurodegeneration and clinical progression in nondemented elderly adults. Aging (Albany, NY) 2019;11:6904–6914. doi: 10.18632/aging.102220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Khalil M, Pirpamer L, Hofer E, et al. Serum neurofilament light levels in normal aging and their association with morphologic brain changes. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:812. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14612-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Xie L, Kang H, Xu Q, et al. Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science. 2013;342:373–377. doi: 10.1126/science.1241224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Teunissen CE, Chiu MJ, Yang CC, et al. Plasma amyloid-beta (Abeta42) correlates with cerebrospinal fluid Abeta42 in Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2018;62:1857–1863. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Holth JK, Mahan TE, Robinson GO, Rocha A, Holtzman DM. Altered sleep and EEG power in the P301S Tau transgenic mouse model. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 2017;4:180–190. doi: 10.1002/acn3.390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Winer JR, Mander BA, Helfrich RF, et al. Sleep as a potential biomarker of tau and beta-amyloid burden in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:6315–6324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0503-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Motamedi V, Kanefsky R, Matsangas P, et al. Elevated tau and interleukin-6 concentrations in adults with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Med. 2018;43:71–76. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.11.1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data can be obtained on request. Requests should be directed toward the management team of the Rotterdam Study (secretariat.epi@erasmusmc.nl), which has a protocol for approving data requests. Because of restrictions based on privacy regulations and informed consent of the participants, data cannot be made freely available in a public repository.