Abstract
Psychophysical studies characterize hyperacusis as increased loudness growth over a wide-frequency range, decreased tolerance to loud sounds and reduced behavioral reaction time latencies to high-intensity sounds. While commonly associated with hearing loss, hyperacusis can also occur without hearing loss, implicating the central nervous system in the generation of hyperacusis. Previous studies suggest that ventral cochlear nucleus bushy cells may be putative neural contributors to hyperacusis. Compared to other ventral cochlear nucleus output neurons, bushy cells show high firing rates as well as lower and less variable first-spike latencies at suprathreshold intensities. Following cochlear damage, bushy cells show increased spontaneous firing rates across a wide-frequency range, suggesting that they might also show increased sound-evoked responses and reduced latencies to higher-intensity sounds. However, no studies have examined bushy cells in relationship to hyperacusis. Herein, we test the hypothesis that bushy cells may contribute to the neural basis of hyperacusis by employing noise-overexposure and single-unit electrophysiology. We find that bushy cells exhibit hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns, which are comprised of enhanced sound-driven firing rates, reduced first-spike latencies and wideband increases in excitability.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Diseases of the nervous system
Introduction
Psychophysical studies characterize hyperacusis as increased loudness growth1 over a wide frequency band2–4, reduced behavioral response latencies5 and decreased tolerance to loud sounds3.
Neurons that contribute to hyperacusis should show hyperexcitable firing patterns that reflect the psychophysical characteristics of hyperacusis6,7. First, neurons contributing to hyperacusis should show increased firing rates at high intensities. Second, firing rate enhancements should be not restricted to a frequency region, consistent with the wideband nature of hyperacusis. Third, first-spike latencies should be shorter at high intensities, as spike latency reflects the faster reaction times seen in hyperacusis. Fourth, neural responses to sound are expected to be more synchronous, reflecting increased perceptual binding of stimuli.
Previous studies suggest that bushy cells of the ventral cochlear nucleus (VCN) exhibit hyperacusis-like neural activity following noise-overexposure8,9. In rodents and other mammalian species, compared to other VCN output neurons, bushy cells show increased phase-locking, a form of neural synchrony, at suprathreshold intensities10–12. Moreover, bushy cells show lower and less variable first-spike latencies than other VCN output neurons10,13,14 across multiple species11,12,15. Following cochlear damage, bushy cells show increased spontaneous firing rates (SFR) across a wide frequency range compared to other CN cell types16,17. Following noise-overexposure and putatively in hyperacusis, we predict that bushy cells will show increased sound evoked responses and reduced latencies to higher-intensity sounds. However, no studies have examined bushy cell firing patterns as a function of suprathreshold sound intensity after noise damage with respect to hyperacusis.
While few studies have examined the neural basis of hyperacusis, many studies have examined the neural basis of tinnitus, or phantom sound perception18–20. Tinnitus is frequently co-morbid with hyperacusis4,21. Unlike hyperacusis, tinnitus occurs in silence and is spectrally similar to hearing loss profiles22. Previous studies show that principle output neurons of the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), the fusiform cells, exhibit narrowband increases in SFR and cross-unit spontaneous synchrony to form a neural signature of tinnitus23,24. As tinnitus and hyperacusis are frequently co-morbid, it is important to discover whether fusiform cells have a role in hyperacusis, or if bushy cells have a role in tinnitus.
Herein, we hypothesize that VCN bushy cells exhibit hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns that are independent of the DCN-fusiform-cell neural signature of tinnitus. To test this hypothesis, we employ noise-overexposure and single-unit electrophysiology to relate bushy-cell firing patterns to hyperacusis. In noise-overexposed animals, bushy cells exhibit hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns, consisting of 1) increased firing rates, 2) reduced and less variable first-spike latencies, and 3) increases in sound-evoked cross-unit synchrony as a function of intensity across a wide range of frequencies. Furthermore, we compare the hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns seen in bushy cells to the previously established neural signature of tinnitus from fusiform cells. We show that unlike fusiform cells, bushy cells do not show an association between SFR, synchrony and behavioral measures of tinnitus. Rather, bushy cells exhibit enhanced responses as a function of intensity across a wide best-frequency band, consistent with the psychophysics of hyperacusis.
Results
Noise-overexposure produces temporary threshold shifts
While hearing loss is the most common factor associated with hyperacusis, it is not essential for its production4. Thus, to induce hyperacusis while maintaining normal cochlear function, guinea pigs were noise-overexposed twice in a temporary-threshold shift induction paradigm (see "Methods", Fig. S1 for further detail) (Fig. 1A)24. Noise-exposed guinea pigs demonstrated an average threshold shift of 15.9 ± 1.13 dB. Consistent with our previous studies utilizing the same noise-overexposure paradigm, ABR thresholds were not significantly different pre-recording compared to baseline (two-way ANOVA; p(group × time) = 0.14). ABR wave 1 (W1) amplitude-intensity functions (AIFs), which are used to estimate cochlear function, were calculated. Noise-overexposed animals showed no significant reduction in ABR W1 amplitude compared to controls across all frequencies and time points (Fig. 1B; two-way ANOVA; F = 2.67; p = 0.087), although there was a trend for W1 amplitudes to be smaller in noise-overexposed animals. Startle amplitudes showed no significant differences in noise-exposed animals compared to controls (Fig. 1C; two-way ANOVA; F = 1.04, p = 0.38), although a trend for larger startle amplitudes was seen in noise-exposed animals.
Ventral cochlear nucleus bushy cells exhibit hyperacusis-like firing patterns after noise-exposure
Hyperacusis is characterized by enhanced loudness growth at suprathreshold intensities for multiple frequencies1,25. Thus, we hypothesized that neurons contributing to hyperacusis would show greater firing rates at higher sound intensities and that these would occur over a wide range of BFs. To test this hypothesis, we recorded from putative VCN bushy cells (n = 1111) across a wide range of BFs (see "Methods" for further detail). Bushy-cells typically show either primary-like or primary-like-with-notch responses to tones at BF26. No significant differences were seen between primary-like and primary-like-with-notch units in BF (Student’s t-test; p = 0.0531), threshold (two-way ANOVA; p(mean × freq) = 0.16) or SFR (two-way ANOVA; p(mean × freq) = 0.0531). Thus, data from both unit types were pooled for remaining analyses. We then measured bushy-cell RIFs to BF tone and broadband noise. Noise-exposed animals showed significantly greater RIF slopes at higher sound levels (inset dashed orange boxes) compared to controls for BF tones (Fig. 2A; filled symbols; p = 3.56e-4; two-sample t-test) and broadband noise (Fig. 2B; filled symbols; p = 6.2e-3; two-sample t-test), consistent with an expected enhanced suprathreshold loudness growth seen in hyperacusis25,27. Human psychoacoustic studies also demonstrate reduced reaction times in subjects with hyperacusis5,28, which are thought to arise from a hyperexcitable auditory pathway. To assess neural excitability, we analyzed bushy-cell first-spike latencies (FSL) as a function of intensity. Bushy cells in noise-exposed animals showed steeper FSL slopes as a function of intensity for BF tones at suprathreshold intensities compared to control animals (Fig. 2A; open symbols; two-sample t-test; p < 0.0081). The decrease in FSL was even more pronounced in response to broadband noise (Fig. 2B; open symbols; two-sample t-test; p < 0.0019). Moreover, bushy-cell RIFs can show non-monotonicity at high intensities, partially arising from wideband inhibition provided by VCN D-stellate neurons29–31. Noise-overexposure reduces inhibition in other cochlear nucleus neurons8. Thus, a loss of inhibition of bushy-cells due to noise-overexposure might result in more monotonic RIFs. RIFs were tested for monotonicity32, which was quantified by calculating the non-Monotonicity Fraction (nMF). Compared to control animals, noise-exposed animals had a significantly greater proportion of monotonic RIFs (BF-tones see Fig. S2A; p = 0.0154, chi-stat = 5.86; BBN RIFs see Fig. S2B; p = 2.31e-12, chi-stat = 49.2). The greater fraction of monotonic RIFs in noise-exposed animals is reflected in steeper monotonic RIF slopes compared to monotonic RIFs from control animals for BF-tone (Fig. 2C) and BBN noise (Fig. 2D). Neural excitability can also be assessed by spiking reliability, or spike jitter, in response to a stimulus. More excitable neurons should more reliably produce spikes at stimulus onset compared to less excitable neurons. We quantified the bushy cell spike jitter by measuring FSL standard deviation. Bushy cells in noise-exposed animals exhibited reduced FSL jitter to BF tones (Fig. 2E; two-sample t-test, p < 5.34e-5) and to broadband noise (Fig. 2F; two-sample t-test, p < 5.46e-23) compared to control animals.
Next, we examined the relationship between bushy-cell evoked firing rate patterns and hyperacusis characteristics by constructing a neural “Hyperacusis Index” (HI) for each neuron. The HI is equal to the geometric mean of (1) the RIF slope in response to sounds from 40 dB SPL to 90 dB SPL (dashed black line in Fig. 3A) and (2) the peak firing rate of the RIF (orange stars in Fig. 3A). Neither primary-like nor primary-like-with-notch units exhibited significant differences in HI (two-way ANOVA; p(mean × freq) = 0.75), and so data from both unit types were pooled for the following analyses. To determine whether an animal had hyperacusis, an unsupervised cluster analysis was performed by fitting HI with a two-member Gaussian-mixture model. A unit was defined as a Hyperacusis Unit if the probability of assignment to the elevated HI cluster was greater than 0.5, which corresponded to HI = 35.5 (Hyperacusis threshold) (Fig. S2C). 25.5% of units were classified as Hyperacusis Units, while the remaining 74.8% of units were Non-Hyperacusis Units. Noise-exposed animals had more units with HIs above the Hyperacusis threshold (n = 189) compared to non-exposed control animals (n = 94; dashed black line in Fig. 3B). Hyperacusis Units were found across a wide range of frequencies in individual animals (BF range: 0.32–24.3 kHz), consistent with the reported wideband characteristics of hyperacusis1,2 (Fig. 3B). To ensure that Hyperacusis Units were composed only of single-units, threshold-triggered mean spike waveforms were assessed. Spike waveforms did not show any evident differences across a range of HIs or animal’s noise-exposure status (sample spike waveforms shown in Fig. 3C). The Hyperacusis Units also showed significantly elevated SFR across all frequencies (left panel in Fig. 3D; two-way ANOVA; p(SFR) = 5.38e-87, p(freq) = 0.26; mean indicated by dark red line) compared to the non-Hyperacusis Units (right panel in Fig. 3D; mean indicated by dark red line). A Gaussian mixed model fit to HI and SFR data revealed that the Hyperacusis Units could be separated into two clusters, one of which demonstrated elevated HI (cluster 1 in Fig. 3E), while the second showed elevated HI and elevated SFR (cluster 2 in Fig. 3E).
We found that RIFs in noise-exposed animals were disproportionately monotonic compared to controls animals, which suggested that disinhibition may play a role in hyperacusis-like firing in the bushy cells. To relate disinhibition to HI, each unit’s Monotonicity Fraction (MF) was calculated (100-nMF) and plotted with HI (Fig. 3F). While HI and MF were strongly correlated for both exposed (Pearson’s linear correlation; r = 0.34, p = 3.32e-18) and control (r = 0.19, p = 8.14e-4) animals, the exposed animal MF-HI correlation was nearly twice as large.
Hyperacusis and tinnitus have distinct neural substrates
Tinnitus and hyperacusis are frequently, but not always, co-morbid2,4. Since our data suggested that some noise-exposed animals show hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns, we considered that some of the noise-exposed animals might also show neural and behavioral evidence of tinnitus. Prior to single-unit recordings, animals were tested for tinnitus using gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle (GPIAS)24,33,34, in which animals are diagnosed with tinnitus if they exhibit impairments in gap-prepulse detection (see "Methods" for further detail). Ten out of twenty-two noise-exposed animals demonstrated significantly impaired gap-prepulse detection in at least one tested frequency-band, while no control animals demonstrated gap-prepulse impairments at any frequency-band. To control for potential frequency-specific temporal-processing deficits, noise-PPI was also assessed18. No animals showed significant deficits in noise-PPI, consistent with previous studies utilizing the same paradigm23,24,35,36.
To assess whether bushy-cell spontaneous activity contributed to tinnitus, frequency-specific SFR and cross-unit spontaneous synchrony (X-corr coef), two neural hallmarks of tinnitus24,37,38, were examined. We found that mean SFR was increased in bushy cells in tinnitus animals compared to exposed, no-tinnitus animals and controls (exposed, tinnitus = 30.18 Hz (ET, red symbols); exposed, no-tinnitus = 22.42 Hz (ENT, blue symbols); control = 18.89 Hz (CONT, black symbols)). Further, bushy-cell SFR was increased across all BFs (two-way ANOVA; p(mean) = 3.04e-2; p(freq) = 0.18). Wideband increases in bushy cell spontaneous activity may be more reflective of hyperacusis than tinnitus, which shows BF-restricted increases. To further quantify tinnitus behavior within a frequency band, we computed a tinnitus index (TI), a startle response-based metric to quantify “tinnitus strength” for each animal (see "Methods" for further detail). When binned by BF and TI carrier-band-frequency, bushy-cell SFR did not significantly correlate with the TI (Fig. 4A; Pearson’s correlation; r = 0.06, p = 0.14) compared to that of DCN fusiform cells (Fig. 4B; Pearson’s correlation, r = 0.21, p = 1.7e-8)24. Furthermore, BF-specific increases in cross-unit spontaneous synchrony did not correlate with an animal’s TI (Fig. 4C; Pearson’s correlation; r = 6.7e-4, p = 0.87). In contrast, fusiform cell cross-unit spontaneous synchrony highly correlates with TI (Fig. 4D; Pearson’s correlation; r = 0.21, p = 0.026). These findings suggest bushy-cell spontaneous activity does not contribute to tinnitus, in contrast to spontaneous activity from fusiform cells.
Previous studies have demonstrated that synchronous activation of neurons contributes to perceptual binding39,40. In tinnitus, enhanced cross-unit spontaneous synchrony would thus signal the presence of sound in the absence of sound18,24,41,42. However, synchrony measurements should control for baseline spontaneous activity, as more spiking can create more opportunities for correlations. Thus, when correlation coefficients are normalized by the number of spikes in each spike train24,43, enhanced correlations between synchrony and SFR indicate that spiketrains are more similar than predicted by chance. While bushy-cell SFR significantly correlated with synchrony in tinnitus animals (Fig. 4E; Pearson’s correlation; r = 0.049, p = 3.38e-5), the correlation was four times smaller than the correlation between SFR and synchrony previously shown in fusiform cells (Fig. 4F; Pearson’s correlation; r = 0.21, p = 0.026)24. These findings suggest that, while bushy cells are more spontaneously active in animals with tinnitus, bushy cells with elevated spontaneous activity do not show cross-unit synchronous firing like that in fusiform cells in animals with tinnitus.
To relate tinnitus behavior to hyperacusis in bushy cells, we binned HI by carrier frequencies used in the GPIAS tests. HI in a tinnitus frequency-band did not correlate with the corresponding TI (Fig. 5A; Pearson’s correlation, p = 0.43, r = 0.12). Furthermore, evoked cross-unit synchrony, a measure of stimulus binding potentially reflective of hyperacusis, did not correlate with the corresponding frequency-matched TI (Fig. 5B; Pearson’s correlation, r = -0.23, p = 0.11). However, HI significantly correlated with the percent change in startle amplitude from baseline to pre-recording (Fig. 5C; Pearson’s correlation, p = 0.05, r = 0.35). Further, evoked synchrony correlation coefficients significantly correlated with binned HI (Fig. 5D; Pearson’s correlation, r = 0.43, p = 0.021). These findings suggest that bushy-cell evoked activity unlikely contributes to tinnitus, but instead is more consistent with hyperacusis.
We then asked how bushy-cell evoked activity compared to fusiform-cell evoked-activity. The most striking difference between bushy cells and fusiform cells is the order-of-magnitude greater evoked firing rates in bushy cells compared to fusiform cells, with concurring increases in firing-rate slope. HIs and RIFs from bushy cells and fusiform cells were grouped by tinnitus-status. Bushy-cell RIFs showed greater suprathreshold increases in firing in tinnitus animals compared to non-tinnitus animals and controls (upper purple arrow in Fig. 6A). In contrast, fusiform cell RIFs were not enhanced at suprathreshold intensities (upper purple arrow in Fig. 6B) in exposed, tinnitus animals compared to either exposed, no-tinnitus animals or controls. Moreover, bushy cells in both tinnitus and no-tinnitus animals had HIs above the previously established Hyperacusis threshold (orange line in Fig. 6C), while fusiform cells did not show any distinct elevations of HI over BF in noise-exposed animals compared to controls (two-way ANOVA; p(freq) = 0.81, p(mean) = 0.31). Furthermore, less than 1% of fusiform-cell HIs were above the bushy-cell Hyperacusis threshold (orange line in Fig. 6D). To verify the lack of HI clustering in fusiform cell data, Gaussian-mixture model fits with different starting centroids did not consistently produce clusters that converged to final values, suggesting that distinct HI clusters are not an intrinsic feature of this dataset. As shown in Fig. 3E, bushy cells show distinct clusters when plotting HI versus SFR. While most of the data points in each cluster were from animals with tinnitus, data from animals without tinnitus (either exposed or control) were also present (Fig. 6E), suggesting that the clusters are not part of a tinnitus signature. In contrast to bushy cells, there were no distinct clusters of HI versus SFR in fusiform cells, which instead showed large increases in SFR compared to control animals (Fig. 6F). These findings reiterate that fusiform-cell firing patterns following noise-overexposure and tinnitus induction are more reflective of tinnitus, as previously shown, and are inconsistent with the hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns found in bushy cells.
Discussion
In the present study, we showed that VCN bushy cells in noise-overexposed guinea pigs show hyperacusis-like neural firing patterns consisting of steepened rate-intensity functions, reduced and less variable first-spike latencies, and increases in sound-evoked cross-unit synchrony across a wide BF range. Consistent with human studies, in which there is significant co-morbidity between hyperacusis and tinnitus, some putative hyperacusis animals also showed behavioral evidence of tinnitus. We also find that analysis of data from fusiform cells indicated that they do not exhibit hyperacusis-like firing patterns, in contrast to bushy cells.
Mechanisms underlying enhanced bushy-cell excitability
There are several possible mechanisms by which bushy cells could become more excitable after noise exposure, resulting in hyperacusis-like firing. Bushy cells in rodent and mammalian species receive somatosensory input on their dendrites which can modulate firing sensitivity36,44. Following noise-exposure, the anterior VCN receives increased glutamatergic input from non-auditory structures36. Increased glutamatergic somatosensory input45 could potentially reduce the threshold for bushy cell spiking, resulting in enhanced firing rates and reduced latencies, consistent with the present findings.
In addition to somatosensory input, cholinergic signaling is altered in VCN following noise-exposure46,47 indicated by increases in choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) and muscarinic ACh receptors in the CN. These studies propose that cochlear damage triggers homeostatic increases in ACh-mediated excitability to compensate for reduced auditory nerve output. Interestingly, cochlear insult with carboplatin, a ototoxic anticancer drug, was shown to enhance VCN, but not DCN, expression of Growth Associated Protein (GAP) 4348. Increases in GAP-43, reflecting axon growth and synaptogenesis, could result in enhanced synchronization of bushy cells through putatively increased collateralization of bushy-cell projections. Increases in VCN neural excitability could also arise through local disinhibition, as measured through reductions in glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 (GAD65) expression8, a GABA-terminal marker. In that study, reductions in GAD65 co-occurred with increases in startle amplitude and ABR wave 2:1 amplitude ratio, both measures which are predicted to correlate with hyperacusis.
However, reductions in cochlear output does not need to be permanent to produce increases in CN excitability. Transient reductions in auditory nerve input can also elicit homeostatic increases in CN excitability. Ear plugging can result in increased AMPA in the post-synaptic density (PSD) of bushy cells, making them cells more excitable49. This increase is sustained after the ear canal is re-opened. Animals with increased AMPA receptor expression and thicker PSDs also showed enhanced later ABR wave amplitudes compared to controls. ABR waves 2 and 3 are predominantly generated by the synchronous firing of bushy cells while waves 4 and 5 are generated by bushy-cell targets in the auditory pathway14, suggesting that the bushy cell pathway becomes more excitable as a result of enhanced AMPA receptor expression. Other studies have shown that following conductive hearing loss, contralateral inputs to the VCN become more excitatory, resulting in increased SFR and enhanced sensitivity to broadband noise50. These findings suggest that transient insults to the auditory pathway can have long-lasting changes resulting in a hyperexcitable auditory pathway.
Hyperacusis-like firing patterns could also arise from bushy-cell network changes beyond cellular changes. Bushy cells are part of an electrotonically-coupled network that may allow for the rapid spread of excitation51,52. Enhanced network connectivity could allow bushy cells to fire more rapidly in response to sound with increased synchrony and reduced latency, consistent with the present findings. A similar mechanism has been proposed to account for perceived brightness in visual cortex: increases in synchrony of stimulus-evoked firing rates correlated with enhancements in perceived brightness53,54.
Bushy cells receive inhibitory interneuron input, including wideband inhibition from VCN D-stellate neurons, and narrowband input from DCN vertical cells55–58. In normal-hearing animals, inhibitory input onto bushy cells is essential for maintaining phase-locking in the presence of background noise59,60. By transiently raising bushy-cell spike thresholds, out-of-phase subthreshold membrane summation is prevented from eliciting spikes. Disinhibition of D-stellate or vertical-cell input would result in more spikes that are less phase-locked to stimuli, than those influenced by auditory-nerve input, consistent with the present results. Moreover, reduced D-stellate cell input onto bushy cells would also result in enhanced bushy-cell sensitivity to off-BF sounds, consistent with the wideband nature of hyperacusis. Reduced d-stellate input onto the bushy cell is consistent with the reduction in non-monotonicity at suprathreshold intensities in noise-exposed animals compared to controls: sideband inhibition increases with increasing sound bandwidth as well as at higher intensities61. Loss of inhibitory input from d-stellate neurons would result in greater bush-cell firing rates at the highest intensities. Moreover, we found that BBN RIFs in exposed animals show far greater loss of non-monotonicity compared to BF-tone RIFs (Fig. S2A, B). BBN RIFs arise from stimuli with greater bandwidth, which implies a greater role for d-stellate neurons in hyperacusis compared to the narrowband vertical cell inhibition. Regardless, future studies should investigate the roles of D-stellate and vertical cells in hyperacusis.
Co-morbidity of hyperacusis and tinnitus
Tinnitus and hyperacusis are frequently co-morbid, with an average co-incidence rate across studies of 60%1–3. Consistent with this observation, we found that a subset of noise-exposed animals shows electrophysiological evidence for both hyperacusis and tinnitus. However, not all hearing loss leads to either tinnitus or hyperacusis; moreover, hyperacusis and tinnitus can occur independently of each other4. In the present study, all unique combinations of hyperacusis and tinnitus were seen. We found that 50.8% of Hyperacusis Units come from animals with tinnitus (tinnitus-with-hyperacusis), while 27.3% of non-Hyperacusis Units were from tinnitus animals (tinnitus-without-hyperacusis). These proportions suggest that tinnitus-with-hyperacusis is the most common condition resulting from our noise-exposure, which is also consistent with human studies, as the average coincidence rate of tinnitus and hyperacusis is greater than 50%. Further, 25.1% of Hyperacusis Units came from exposed, non-tinnitus animals (hyperacusis-without-tinnitus), suggesting that some of the non-tinnitus animals might also have hyperacusis.
We found that most Hyperacusis Units are found in tinnitus animals. However, bushy-cell HI did not correlate with an animal’s TI when binned with TI-carrier frequencies. These findings suggest that there could instead be a fusiform-cell contribution to tinnitus in animals with bushy-cells showing elevated HI. A reduction in wideband-inhibition from D-stellate neurons could likewise reduce inhibition of the fusiform cell31,44, potentially linking hyperacusis arising from bushy cells in the VCN with tinnitus arising from fusiform cells in the DCN. Future studies should perform concurrent bushy-cell and fusiform-cell recordings in the same animals, along with behavioral measures of tinnitus and hyperacusis.
Behavioral models of hyperacusis
While several behavioral tests for hyperacusis have been proposed25,28,62–65, there are no widely-adopted behavioral tests for hyperacusis66,67. Current paradigms utilize either enhanced startle amplitudes or reduced reaction times as hyperacusis-measures. As bushy-cells in noise-exposed animals showed both increases in firing rate and reductions in first-spike latency, we propose that a combined measure of both reaction times and startle amplitudes will measure hyperacusis. We predict that animals with hyperacusis will show enhanced startle response amplitude slopes as well as reduced startle reaction time slopes as a function of stimulus intensity, as suggested by the bushy cell RIF enhancements and LIF reductions seen in noise-exposed animals. Further, because bushy cells in noise-overexposed animals show greater absolute RIF and LIF slopes in response to broadband noise than to BF tones, we predict that animals with hyperacusis will show stronger responses to broadband noise, as wideband sounds are perceived to be louder than SPL-equivalent narrowband sounds68.
In the present study, noise-exposed animals showed a trend for increases in startle amplitude at several frequency bands. This trend is consistent with another hyperacusis study, which showed that suprathreshold startle enhancements, reflective of hyperacusis, were seen only in response to startle pulses greater than 100 dB25, but not in response to the 90 dB SPL stimulus used in the present study. Future studies should measure startle-amplitude-intensity functions, with peak sound outputs greater than utilized in the present study, to assess hyperacusis-behavior.
Methods
Ethical treatment of animals
All animal procedures were performed in accordance with protocols established by the National Institutes of Health (Publication 80–23) and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Michigan.
Experimental design
N = 29 female pigmented guinea pigs were obtained from the Elm-Hill colony at 2–3 weeks of age. Animals were noise-overexposed using a paradigm previously established in the lab (Fig. S1A). Baseline auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) were measured to establish normal hearing followed by four weeks of behavioral testing. All noise-exposed animals received two noise exposures separated by 4 weeks, followed by a second 4-week session of behavioral testing. Single-unit electrophysiology was performed within one week of the final behavioral testing session.
Gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle (GPIAS) for tinnitus assessment
A rapid-onset sound (the startle pulse) (Fig. S1B) results in a startle response in guinea pigs, which can be reduced by presenting a prepulse stimulus (detectable above a background noise) before the startle pulse. Similarly, a background-noise gap of silence, before the startle pulse, will decrease the animal’s startle amplitude. Tinnitus that is spectrally similar to the background noise is indicated as impaired gap-detection ability34.
Guinea-pig startle responses were assessed by measuring their pinna-reflex displacements33 in response to a 20 ms startle pulse (rise-fall time: 2 ms). Pinna movements were tracked by video capture of green ink dots, manually applied to both pinnae. Offline, green pixels were identified using a custom-written k-nearest neighbors algorithm (Mathworks MATLAB)69,70. Frames in which green points constituted less than 0.01% of pixels were excluded, as this indicated the ears were not detectable by the camera. Pinna locations were identified by clustering green pixels and computing the centroids of a two-dimensional Gaussian mixture model fit using the Expectation–Maximization algorithm71. Euclidean distance between (Xear(t), Year(t)) points was computed over the trial duration. Trajectory accuracy was verified by trained observers. Startle amplitudes were computed by fitting the trajectory trace to a Gaussian-modulated sine-wave cycle.
Gaps in background noise (65 dB SPL; 50 ms with 5 ms rise/fall times) were presented 100 ms before a broadband noise startle pulse (90 dB SPL; 20 ms with 2 ms rise/fall times). At a given background frequency band (center frequencies of 9, 13, 17 kHz with 2 kHz bandwidths, 25 kHz with a 10 kHz bandwidth, or high pass Gaussian broadband noise), a randomized series of 10 pre-pulses (either silent gap, or a noise pre-pulse at 75 dB SPL) and 10 no-prepulse control background noises were delivered. All testing was performed in sound-proof booths (Acoustic Systems, Inc), with greater than 100 dB acoustic isolation between testing chambers. Trials were randomly presented every 20 to 30 s, with prepulse and no-prepulse trials combined into a single per-frequency testing session, and randomly interleaved. Each per-frequency testing session lasted between 9 and 10 min. Eight testing sessions were performed each testing day, for an average testing time of approximately 80 min. Guinea pigs were tested twice per week, with at least two non-testing days in between each testing day (Mondays and Thursdays or Tuesdays and Fridays) to prevent habituation. Startle amplitudes from each test session were pooled over four weeks. Startle amplitudes greater than two standard deviations above the mean were excluded from analysis. For each frequency band, a normalized startle ratio (R) was computed as the mean pre-pulse startle distribution divided by the mean of the non-pre-pulse distribution. The normalized amplitudes of the startle reflexes were compared at baseline and after noise-overexposure. An animal was defined as having tinnitus if, at a given frequency, the mean of the post-exposure distribution was significantly greater than the mean of the pre-exposure distribution (Mann–Whitney U-test; alpha = 0.05). The changes in gap R values from pre- to post-exposure were quantified by the standardized tinnitus index [(x – µ)/σ]23,24,37, where x is the post-exposure gap R value, µ and σ are the mean and standard deviation of pre-exposure gap R value. A significantly higher (positive) index value indicates worsened GPIAS performance and is assumed to indicate tinnitus.
Auditory brainstem responses
All electrophysiology testing was performed in a double-walled, soundproof booth (Acoustic Systems, Inc), consistent with previous studies done in this lab24,72,73. Animals were anesthetized (40 mg/kg ketamine (Putney Inc.); 10 mg/kg xylazine (Lloyd Inc.)) and unilateral ABRs (Fig. S1C) were measured (tone pip, up to 1024 repetitions, 5 ms duration, 0.5 ms rise/fall time, cos2 gating; 8, 12, 16, 20, 24 kHz; TDT RX8 DAC, HB7 amplifier, and PA-5 attenuator). Sounds were presented closed field (DT770 Speaker) coupled to the ear canal through custom-built hollow ear bars. Calibration was performed using TDT SigCalRP, a custom MATLAB script and a ¼″ microphone (B&K 4136 and Stanford Research Systems SR760 spectrum analyzer; RX8 and PA5; 0.5 mL volume). The system transfer function was flattened with a maximum sound intensity output of 90 dB SPL using FFTs from 200 Hz-32 kHz. Stainless-steel needle electrodes were inserted into the skin overlying the bullae and at vertex. Evoked potentials were digitized (TDT RA4LI head stage; PZ2-64 pre-amp; and filtered between 300 Hz-3 kHz with a 60 Hz notch). Sounds were presented starting at 90 dB SPL and decreased in 10 dB steps to 0 dB SPL. ABR threshold for a frequency was defined as the lowest sound intensity that did not elicit ABRs with at least three identifiable peaks and troughs73.
Noise overexposure
22 Guinea pigs were noise-overexposed, twice, to narrow-band noise previously shown to induce a temporary threshold shift23,24,36,72,73. A subset of guinea pigs (7) served as sham-exposed (anesthesia-only) controls. Guinea pigs were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (40:10 mg/kg). Sound-overexposures (7 kHz centered, half-octave noise at 97 dB SPL) were delivered via microphone inserts into the left ear for 2 h. ABRs were recorded before and immediately after each noise exposure, as well as prior to single-unit recordings.
Surgical access of the cochlear nucleus
Animals were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (40:10 mg/kg) and placed in a hollow-ear-bar stereotaxic frame (Kopf). A custom-built heating pad with closed-loop controller was used to regulate body temperature (38 °C). Anesthetic depth was checked using a toe-pinch, and supplemental anesthesia (0.15 ketamine/xylazine dose) was administered ~ every 30 min. A craniotomy and duratomy were performed to expose the cerebellum for electrode insertion. The AVCN was accessed using previously established stereotaxic co-ordinates44,74.
Single-unit electrophysiology
In vivo unit responses were recorded using multichannel recording electrodes (Neuronexus; 32 channels with 16 channels per 2 shanks; custom headstage), consistent with previous experiments in this lab24,72,73. Voltages were digitized (PZ2-64 pre-amp) and bandpass filtered (300 Hz-3 kHz, with a 60 Hz and harmonic comb-filter). Spike thresholds were identified when voltage amplitude crossed 2 standard deviations above the mean voltage arising from spontaneous activity (see Fig. 3C for example threshold-triggered mean waveforms). Units were identified by their responses to 65 dB SPL wideband (200 Hz-40 kHz) search stimuli. Neuron thresholds were stable throughout the experiment with thresholds varying between 40- and 50-dB SPL. Neural spike data was imported into MATLAB and analyzed offline. Spike waveforms were projected into principle component (PC) space and manually clustered by the first three coefficients by a trained user. Unit consistency was maintained by clustering all PC coefficients from a given recording location throughout the experiment. Timestamps were grouped by cluster into isolated units, and spiketrains constructed in MATLAB. Putative bushy-cell single units were identified by their receptive fields (10 dB steps from 0 to 90 dB SPL; frequencies logarithmically spaced from 2 to 24 kHz in 0.25 octave steps) (colormap in Fig. S1D), and either primary-like or primary-like-with-notch peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTH) (lower inset of Fig. S1D)75–77. Bushy cells can sometimes be identified by the presence of a pre-potential, arising from the large, tightly coupled auditory nerve endbulb of Held input on the bushy-cell soma10,59,78. Using silicon substrate electrodes, prepotentials can sometimes be identified on putative bushy cells due to low signal-to-noise ratios, as shown previously74. Thus, in a subset of units, pre-potentials were identified (upper inset in Fig. S1D). Once putative bushy cells were identified, spontaneous activity was collected (at least 150 s) followed by unit responses to BF tones and broadband noise over a range of intensities (5 dB steps from 0 to 90 dBSPL) (rate-intensity function: filled symbols in Fig. S1E). 1111 putative bushy cells were identified for analysis, and non-bushy cells were excluded from further analysis. First-spike latency (FSL) was assessed by recording the first spike timestamp post-stimulus onset for each trial (n = 100 trials) during rate-intensity function recordings, and the mean for all trials computed (latency-intensity function: open symbols in Fig. S1E). FSL jitter was assessed as the standard deviation of the FSL distribution.
Monotonicity fraction
RIFs non-monotonicity was quantifed32 by computing the Non-Monotonicity Fraction, which is calculated as the percent of slope decreases over the sound intensity range used to elicit the RIF above threshold. A unit was defined as non-monotonic if the non-monotonicity fraction exceeded 12.5% of the RIF (BF-tone for Fig. S2A, BBN-noise RIFs for Fig. S2B).
Hyperacusis Index
A Hyperacusis Index (HI) was computed for each unit as the geometric mean of the unit’s 1) average rate-intensity-function slope at intensities greater than 40 dB SPL (nearest base-10 multiple of the average population threshold; 42.1 ± 0.48 dB SPL) and less than 90 dB SPL (the maximum calibrated system output for all tested frequencies) and 2) peak firing rate at best frequency from 40 dB SPL to 90 dB SPL.
Synchrony and spontaneous firing rate assessments
Cross-unit synchrony was computed using cross-correlograms for evoked43 and spontaneous activity23,24,38. For spontaneous synchrony, 150 s of spontaneous activity was recorded. SFR was computed as the average spike rate during this trial. For spontaneous synchrony calculations, spikes co-occurring within a 150 µs window were removed. For evoked activity, spikes from frequency-intensity stimulus pairs between receptive fields were pooled. Cross-correlation coefficients (p(τ)) were computed as a function of time lag for each pairwise combination of spike trains.
is the unbiased cross-correlation of spike trains A and B; NA and NB indicate spike counts in the respective spike trains. E is the mean probability of coincident firing for Poisson-distributed data, defined by the multiplication of NA and NB over the number of bins (n). Bin size was constant at 0.3 ms43. A unit-pair was considered synchronous when the peak p value was greater than ± 4 standard deviations from the mean p(τ). In the present study, negative cross-correlations were removed from further analysis.
Statistical analysis
Correlation coefficients were computed using Pearson’s algorithm. Gaussian-mixture models were fit using the Expectation–Maximization algorithm (MATLAM fitgmdist). Distribution differences were assessed for significance with ANOVAs, Kruskal–Wallis or two-sample KS tests where appropriate (alpha = 0.05). Post-hoc corrections for multiple comparisons were done using the Bonferroni method. The experimenter was blinded as to the status of each animal regarding exposure of behavioral outcome.
Dorsal cochlear nucleus fusiform cell data and analyses
Data published in Wu et al.24 were further analyzed in this study with the permission of the authors. Tinnitus behavioral status in all animals was determined following the GPIAS paradigm presented earlier. SFR, spontaneous synchrony and HI analyses were performed identically between DCN and VCN data. Data were normalized by the control-animal SFR maximum where indicated.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01-DC004825 (SES), RF1-MH114244-01 (SES), T32-DC00011 (DTM) and P30-DC05188. We thank Calvin Wu and Michael Roberts for technical and analytical assistance and review of an earlier version of this manuscript, Adam Hockley and Mike Selesko for behavioral data collection and data analysis assistance, and Mr. Deepak Dileepkumar for technical support.
Author contributions
D.T.M. and S.E.S. designed the study; D.T.M. collected the data; D.T.M. and S.E.S. analyzed the results; D.T.M. and S.E.S. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-77754-z.
References
- 1.Baguley DM. Hyperacusis. J R Soc Med. 2003;96:582–585. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.96.12.582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sheldrake J, Diehl PU, Schaette R. Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis patients. Front Neurol. 2015;6:105. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tyler RS, et al. A review of hyperacusis and future directions: part I. Definitions and manifestations. Am. J. Audiol. 2014;23:402–419. doi: 10.1044/2014_AJA-14-0010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schecklmann, M., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B. & Group, T. R. I. D. S Phenotypic characteristics of hyperacusis in tinnitus. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e86944. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lauer AM, Dooling RJ. Evidence of hyperacusis in canaries with permanent hereditary high-frequency hearing loss. Semin. Hear. 2007 doi: 10.1055/s-2007-990718. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brotherton H, Plack CJ, Maslin M, Schaette R, Munro KJ. Pump up the volume: could excessive neural gain explain tinnitus and hyperacusis? Audiol. Neurootol. 2015;20:273–282. doi: 10.1159/000430459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Zeng FG. An active loudness model suggesting tinnitus as increased central noise and hyperacusis as increased nonlinear gain. Hear Res. 2013;295:172–179. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schrode KM, Muniak MA, Kim YH, Lauer AM. Central compensation in auditory brainstem after damaging noise exposure. Neuro. 2018 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0250-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Boettcher FA, Salvi RJ. Functional changes in the ventral cochlear nucleus following acute acoustic overstimulation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1993;94:2123–2134. doi: 10.1121/1.407484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bourk, T. R. Electrical responses of neural units in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. (1976).
- 11.Kuenzel T. Modulatory influences on time-coding neurons in the ventral cochlear nucleus. Hear Res. 2019;384:107824. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2019.107824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cant NB, Benson CG. Parallel auditory pathways: projection patterns of the different neuronal populations in the dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei. Brain Res Bull. 2003;60:457–474. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(03)00050-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Spirou GA, Brownell WE, Zidanic M. Recordings from cat trapezoid body and HRP labeling of globular bushy cell axons. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;63:1169–1190. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.63.5.1169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Melcher JR, Kiang NY. Generators of the brainstem auditory evoked potential in cat. III: identified cell populations. Hear Res. 1996;93:52–71. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(95)00200-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ryugo DK, Parks TN. Primary innervation of the avian and mammalian cochlear nucleus. Brain Res. Bull. 2003;60:435–456. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(03)00049-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bledsoe SC, Jr, et al. Ventral cochlear nucleus responses to contralateral sound are mediated by commissural and olivocochlear pathways. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;102:886–900. doi: 10.1152/jn.91003.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vogler DP, Robertson D, Mulders WH. Hyperactivity in the ventral cochlear nucleus after cochlear trauma. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:6639–6645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6538-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shore SE, Wu C. Mechanisms of noise-induced tinnitus: insights from cellular studies. Neuron. 2019;103:8–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Shore SE, Roberts LE, Langguth B. Maladaptive plasticity in tinnitus–triggers, mechanisms and treatment. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2016;12:150–160. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2016.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Roberts LE, et al. Ringing ears: the neuroscience of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:14972–14979. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4028-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shore SE, Roberts LE, Langguth B. Maladaptive plasticity in tinnitus - triggers, mechanisms and treatment. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2016;12:150–160. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2016.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Roberts LE, Moffat G, Bosnyak DJ. Residual inhibition functions in relation to tinnitus spectra and auditory threshold shift. Acta Otolaryngol. Suppl. 2006 doi: 10.1080/03655230600895358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Marks KL, et al. Auditory-somatosensory bimodal stimulation desynchronizes brain circuitry to reduce tinnitus in guinea pigs and humans. Sci. Transl. Med. 2018 doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aal3175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wu C, Martel DT, Shore SE. Increased synchrony and bursting of dorsal cochlear nucleus fusiform cells correlate with tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:2068–2073. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3960-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chen G, et al. Behavioral evidence for possible simultaneous induction of hyperacusis and tinnitus following intense sound exposure. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2013;14:413–424. doi: 10.1007/s10162-013-0375-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Winter IM, Palmer AR. Responses of single units in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of the guinea pig. Hear Res. 1990;44:161–178. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(90)90078-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Knudson IM, Melcher JR. Elevated acoustic startle responses in humans: relationship to reduced loudness discomfort level, but not self-report of hyperacusis. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2016;17:223–235. doi: 10.1007/s10162-016-0555-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Zhang C, Flowers E, Li JX, Wang Q, Sun W. Loudness perception affected by high doses of salicylate–a behavioral model of hyperacusis. Behav. Brain Res. 2014;271:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Oertel D, Wright S, Cao XJ, Ferragamo M, Bal R. The multiple functions of T stellate/multipolar/chopper cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus. Hear Res. 2011;276:61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ferragamo MJ, Golding NL, Oertel D. Synaptic inputs to stellate cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;79:51–63. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Campagnola L, Manis PB. A map of functional synaptic connectivity in the mouse anteroventral cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:2214–2230. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4669-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cai S, Ma WL, Young ED. Encoding intensity in ventral cochlear nucleus following acoustic trauma: implications for loudness recruitment. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2009;10:5–22. doi: 10.1007/s10162-008-0142-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Berger JI, Coomber B, Shackleton TM, Palmer AR, Wallace MN. A novel behavioural approach to detecting tinnitus in the guinea pig. J Neurosci. Methods. 2013;213:188–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Turner JG, et al. Gap detection deficits in rats with tinnitus: a potential novel screening tool. Behav. Neurosci. 2006;120:188–195. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Martel DT, Pardo-Garcia TR, Shore SE. Dorsal cochlear nucleus fusiform-cell plasticity is altered in salicylate-induced tinnitus. Neuroscience. 2019;407:170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Heeringa AN, et al. Glutamatergic projections to the cochlear nucleus are redistributed in tinnitus. Neuroscience. 2018;391:91–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kalappa BI, Brozoski TJ, Turner JG, Caspary DM. Single unit hyperactivity and bursting in the auditory thalamus of awake rats directly correlates with behavioural evidence of tinnitus. J. Physiol. 2014;592:5065–5078. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.278572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Norena AJ, Eggermont JJ. Changes in spontaneous neural activity immediately after an acoustic trauma: implications for neural correlates of tinnitus. Hear Res. 2003;183:137–153. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00225-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Engel AK, Singer W. Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001;5:16–25. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01568-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Singer W. Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron. 1999;24(49–65):111–125. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80821-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Womelsdorf T, et al. Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science. 2007;316:1609–1612. doi: 10.1126/science.1139597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Voigt HF, Young ED. Cross-correlation analysis of inhibitory interactions in dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;64:1590–1610. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.5.1590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wu C, Shore SE. Multisensory activation of ventral cochlear nucleus D-stellate cells modulates dorsal cochlear nucleus principal cell spatial coding. J. Physiol. 2018;596:4537–4548. doi: 10.1113/JP276280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zhou J, Nannapaneni N, Shore S. Vessicular glutamate transporters 1 and 2 are differentially associated with auditory nerve and spinal trigeminal inputs to the cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2007;500:777–787. doi: 10.1002/cne.21208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jin YM, Godfrey DA, Wang J, Kaltenbach JA. Effects of intense tone exposure on choline acetyltransferase activity in the hamster cochlear nucleus. Hear Res. 2006;216–217:168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Jin YM, Godfrey DA. Effects of cochlear ablation on muscarinic acetylcholine receptor binding in the rat cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. Res. 2006;83:157–166. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kraus KS, Ding D, Zhou Y, Salvi RJ. Central auditory plasticity after carboplatin-induced unilateral inner ear damage in the chinchilla: up-regulation of GAP-43 in the ventral cochlear nucleus. Hear Res. 2009;255:33–43. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Clarkson C, Antunes FM, Rubio ME. Conductive hearing loss has long-lasting structural and molecular effects on presynaptic and postsynaptic structures of auditory nerve synapses in the cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:10214–10227. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0226-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Sumner CJ, Tucci DL, Shore SE. Responses of ventral cochlear nucleus neurons to contralateral sound after conductive hearing loss. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:4234–4243. doi: 10.1152/jn.00401.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rubio ME, et al. Target- and input-dependent organization of AMPA and NMDA receptors in synaptic connections of the cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2014;522:4023–4042. doi: 10.1002/cne.23654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gomez-Nieto R, Rubio ME. A bushy cell network in the rat ventral cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2009;516:241–263. doi: 10.1002/cne.22139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Biederlack J, et al. Brightness induction: rate enhancement and neuronal synchronization as complementary codes. Neuron. 2006;52:1073–1083. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Uhlhaas PJ, et al. Neural synchrony in cortical networks: history, concept and current status. Front Integr. Neurosci. 2009;3:17. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.017.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nelken I, Young ED. Two separate inhibitory mechanisms shape the responses of dorsal cochlear nucleus type IV units to narrowband and wideband stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 1994;71:2446–2462. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.6.2446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rhode WS. Vertical cell responses to sound in cat dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 1999;82:1019–1032. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.2.1019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Arnott RH, Wallace MN, Shackleton TM, Palmer AR. Onset neurones in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus project to the dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2004;5:153–170. doi: 10.1007/s10162-003-4036-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wickesberg RE, Oertel D. Delayed, frequency-specific inhibition in the cochlear nuclei of mice: a mechanism for monaural echo suppression. J. Neurosci. 1990;10:1762–1768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-06-01762.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Keine C, Rubsamen R. Inhibition shapes acoustic responsiveness in spherical bushy cells. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:8579–8592. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0133-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Xie R, Manis PB. Target-specific IPSC kinetics promote temporal processing in auditory parallel pathways. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1598–1614. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2541-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kuenzel T, Borst JG, van der Heijden M. Factors controlling the input-output relationship of spherical bushy cells in the gerbil cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:4260–4273. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5433-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Radziwon K, et al. Salicylate-induced hyperacusis in rats: Dose- and frequency-dependent effects. Hear Res. 2017;350:133–138. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Salloum RH, Yurosko C, Santiago L, Sandridge SA, Kaltenbach JA. Induction of enhanced acoustic startle response by noise exposure: dependence on exposure conditions and testing parameters and possible relevance to hyperacusis. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e111747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hickox AE, Liberman MC. Is noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? J. Neurophysiol. 2014;111:552–564. doi: 10.1152/jn.00184.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Hayes SH, Radziwon KE, Stolzberg DJ, Salvi RJ. Behavioral models of tinnitus and hyperacusis in animals. Front Neurol. 2014;5:179. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Brozoski TJ, Bauer CA. Animal models of tinnitus. Hear Res. 2016;338:88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Eggermont JJ, Roberts LE. Tinnitus: animal models and findings in humans. Cell Tissue Res. 2015;361:311–336. doi: 10.1007/s00441-014-1992-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Wagner E, Florentine M, Buus S, McCormack J. Spectral loudness summation and simple reaction time. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004;116:1681–1686. doi: 10.1121/1.1780573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Altman NS. An introduction to kernel and nearest-neighbor nonparametric regression. Am. Stat. 1992;46:10. doi: 10.1080/00031305.1992.10475879. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Friedman JH, Bentely JL, Finkel RA. An algorithm for finding best matches in logarithmic expected time. ACM Trans. Math. Softw. 1977;3:209–226. doi: 10.1145/355744.355745. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.McLachlan GJ, Chang SU. Mixture modelling for cluster analysis. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 2004;13:347–361. doi: 10.1191/0962280204sm372ra. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Koehler SD, Shore SE. Stimulus timing-dependent plasticity in dorsal cochlear nucleus is altered in tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:19647–19656. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2788-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Dehmel S, Pradhan S, Koehler S, Bledsoe S, Shore S. Noise overexposure alters long-term somatosensory-auditory processing in the dorsal cochlear nucleus–possible basis for tinnitus-related hyperactivity? J. Neurosci. 2012;32:1660–1671. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4608-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Heeringa AN, Wu C, Shore SE. Multisensory integration enhances temporal coding in ventral cochlear nucleus bushy cells. J. Neurosci. 2018 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2244-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Winter IM, Palmer AR. Temporal responses of primarylike anteroventral cochlear nucleus units to the steady-state vowel /i. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1990;88:1437–1441. doi: 10.1121/1.399720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Blackburn CC, Sachs MB. Classification of unit types in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus: PST histograms and regularity analysis. J. Neurophysiol. 1989;62:1303–1329. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.62.6.1303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Ingham NJ, Itatani N, Bleeck S, Winter IM. Enhancement of forward suppression begins in the ventral cochlear nucleus. Brain Res. 2016;1639:13–27. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.02.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Keine C, Rubsamen R, Englitz B. Signal integration at spherical bushy cells enhances representation of temporal structure but limits its range. eLife. 2017 doi: 10.7554/eLife.29639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.