Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Oct 26;9:e58889. doi: 10.7554/eLife.58889

How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity?

Henry WP Dalgleish 1,†,, Lloyd E Russell 1,, Adam M Packer 1,†,§, Arnd Roth 1, Oliver M Gauld 1, Francesca Greenstreet 1,, Emmett J Thompson 1,, Michael Häusser 1,
Editors: Brice Bathellier2, Ronald L Calabrese3
PMCID: PMC7695456  PMID: 33103656

Abstract

Many theories of brain function propose that activity in sparse subsets of neurons underlies perception and action. To place a lower bound on the amount of neural activity that can be perceived, we used an all-optical approach to drive behaviour with targeted two-photon optogenetic activation of small ensembles of L2/3 pyramidal neurons in mouse barrel cortex while simultaneously recording local network activity with two-photon calcium imaging. By precisely titrating the number of neurons stimulated, we demonstrate that the lower bound for perception of cortical activity is ~14 pyramidal neurons. We find a steep sigmoidal relationship between the number of activated neurons and behaviour, saturating at only ~37 neurons, and show this relationship can shift with learning. Furthermore, activation of ensembles is balanced by inhibition of neighbouring neurons. This surprising perceptual sensitivity in the face of potent network suppression supports the sparse coding hypothesis, and suggests that cortical perception balances a trade-off between minimizing the impact of noise while efficiently detecting relevant signals.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

How does activity in neural circuits give rise to behaviour? While mammalian brains are composed of many millions (Herculano-Houzel et al., 2006) or billions (Herculano-Houzel, 2009; Herculano-Houzel et al., 2007) of neurons it has long been postulated that they operate – encoding information and controlling behaviour – through the activity of small subsets of those neurons (Barlow, 1972). Indeed, the hypothesis that brains use sparse, distributed activity patterns is supported computationally (Kanerva, 1993; Olshausen and Field, 1996), energetically (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001; Lennie, 2003; Schölvinck et al., 2008), and experimentally (Barth and Poulet, 2012; Olshausen and Field, 2004; Wolfe et al., 2010). A major factor thought to govern such sparse coding is neuronal inhibition (Haider and McCormick, 2009; Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011) which serves to balance and control recurrent excitation (Denève and Machens, 2016; Haider et al., 2013; Murphy and Miller, 2009; Packer and Yuste, 2011; Pehlevan and Sompolinsky, 2014; Sadeh and Clopath, 2020; Tsodyks et al., 1997; van Vreeswijk and Sompolinsky, 1996; Wehr and Zador, 2003; Wolf et al., 2014) and shape neuronal output (Borg-Graham et al., 1998; Cardin et al., 2010; Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Lee et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2012). Two key questions are therefore: (1) What is the lower bound of activity that can be behaviourally salient? and (2) How does such activity interact with the local network?

Classical microstimulation experiments have demonstrated that focal activation of cortical regions can influence decision-making, providing a direct causal link between neural activity and behaviour (Cohen and Newsome, 2004; Murasugi et al., 1993; Salzman et al., 1990; Salzman et al., 1992). This landmark work has been complemented by more recent studies showing that optogenetic activation of dozens to hundreds of cortical neurons can be directly detected (Huber et al., 2008; Histed and Maunsell, 2014). A further refinement of this approach was provided by patch-clamp recording, which revealed that strong electrical stimulation of even a single neuron can be detected in a cell-type and spike timing-dependent manner (Houweling and Brecht, 2008; Doron et al., 2014), to the extent that they can modulate behaviour in sensory-guided tasks (Tanke et al., 2018). While these studies have provided important approximations of the numbers of neurons required to trigger and manipulate behaviour, they suffered from several important limitations. Firstly, electrical stimulation techniques are ill-suited to titrating the number of neurons stimulated and offer limited targeting specificity, either indiscriminately activating swathes of cortex or activating single neurons. Secondly, one-photon optogenetic approaches, while limiting direct excitation to genetically defined neurons, only offer post-hoc estimation of the number of neurons stimulated from histology (Huber et al., 2008). Finally, in none of these studies has it been possible to carefully assess the impact of the stimulation on the local network, an essential step if we are to understand the link between activity generated by stimulation and behaviour.

A parallel body of work has investigated the influence of neural activity on local networks in vivo, demonstrating that small numbers of active neurons can have a large impact on local network dynamics and brain state. Strong stimulation of single pyramidal neurons in L2/3 has been shown to recruit ~2% of local excitatory neurons and ~30% of local inhibitory neurons (Kwan and Dan, 2012). A single pyramidal neuron spike in L5 is estimated to recruit ~28 post-synaptic neurons (London et al., 2010) and in L2/3 can trigger strong disynaptic inhibition (Jouhanneau et al., 2018). Single neurons can also trigger global switches in brain state (Li et al., 2009), influence network synchronisation (Bonifazi et al., 2009) and have a direct impact on motor output (Brecht et al., 2004). However, work investigating the impact of sparse activation on the local network in vivo has largely been done under anaesthesia (London et al., 2010; Kwan and Dan, 2012; Jouhanneau et al., 2018), which influences state-dependent cortical processing (Niell and Stryker, 2010; Crochet et al., 2011; Harris and Thiele, 2011) and prevents the study of behaviour.

Combining simultaneous targeted stimulation with readout of effects on the local network during behaviour will allow us to define the local network input-output function. This will yield better understanding of neural network operation, analogously to how measuring single-neuron input-output functions has transformed our understanding of information processing in single neurons (Magee, 2000; Poirazi et al., 2003; London et al., 2010; Major et al., 2013). Moreover, it will allow us to determine how this network input-output function in turn influences the psychometric sensitivity to neural activity, which theoretical work predicts is crucial for understanding the link between neural circuit activity and behaviour (Bernardi et al., 2020; Bernardi and Lindner, 2017; Bernardi and Lindner, 2019; Cai et al., 2020). While some studies combining readout with manipulation have made significant progress in this direction (Ceballo et al., 2019a; Ceballo et al., 2019b; Salzman et al., 1990; Znamenskiy and Zador, 2013), they have lacked spatial resolution and targeting flexibility either on the level of readout or stimulation. Measuring network input-output functions at cellular resolution during perception is likely to yield pivotal insights into how neural populations generate behaviour.

Here, we have activated ensembles of varying numbers of neurons in L2/3 barrel cortex of awake mice trained to detect direct cortical photostimulation. We took advantage of recently developed all-optical approaches combining two-photon calcium imaging and two-photon optogenetics with digital holography to allow us to activate specifically targeted ensembles of neurons while simultaneously recording the response of the local network (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Emiliani et al., 2015; Mardinly et al., 2018; Marshel et al., 2019; Packer et al., 2015; Russell et al., 2019; Shemesh et al., 2017). We combined this all-optical approach with an operant conditioning paradigm in which animals were required to report the targeted two-photon optogenetic activation of arbitrary ensembles of pyramidal neurons in L2/3 barrel cortex to gain rewards. In trained animals we investigated how behaviour and network response vary as a function of the ensemble size stimulated. We show that animals are sensitive to the activation of surprisingly small numbers of neurons (~14) and demonstrate that activating roughly double this number of neurons (~37) is sufficient for detection to saturate. Moreover, we show that this perceptual threshold is plastic, and decreases with learning. We also demonstrate that while detection rates increase with increasing stimulation, the surrounding network responds with matched suppression which maintains the balance of activation and suppression at a level consistent with spontaneous epochs.

Results

Targeted two-photon optogenetic activation of neural ensembles in L2/3 barrel cortex can drive behaviour

To investigate both the behavioural and network response to precisely controlled levels of cortical excitation, we combined an upgraded version of our previously reported all-optical setup (incorporating 3D volumetric imaging using an ETL, a more powerful two-photon (2P) photostimulation laser and an additional light-path for one-photon (1P) photostimulation; see Materials and methods, Packer et al., 2015) with an operant conditioning paradigm whereby mice are trained to report the activation of excitatory neurons in barrel cortex with either 1P or 2P optogenetic stimulation (Figure 1). We expressed the calcium indicator GCaMP6s and the two-photon activatable somatically-restricted opsin C1V1 in neurons of L2/3 barrel cortex (Figure 1a: inset right, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, see Materials and methods). This strategy allows us to flexibly activate specific ensembles of neurons in a given cortical population (Figure 1a: Pixel STA, Figure 1—figure supplement 2) with high spatial resolution (Figure 1—figure supplement 2a: HWHM 5 µm laterally, 20 µm axially) while performing simultaneous two-photon calcium imaging. Inspired by previous work (Histed and Maunsell, 2014; Huber et al., 2008) we devised a training paradigm in which animals were conditioned to detect bulk activation of barrel cortex via 1P photostimulation. After task acquisition, we progressively lowered the 1P stimulation intensity (which reduces the number, reliability and spatial extent of activated neurons (see Materials and methods for photostimulation details)), before transitioning animals to detect 2P photostimulation of specific groups of neurons (Figure 1b, Figure 1—figure supplement 3). We used a simple un-cued go/catch trial design where pseudo-randomly interleaved go trials (1P or 2P photostimulation) or catch trials (no photostimulation) were delivered after animals successfully withheld licking for a variable period (Figure 1c: top, see Materials and methods). On go trials, the presence/absence of licks in the post-stimulus response window was scored as hits/misses, with hits triggering delivery of a sucrose reward (Figure 1c: bottom). On catch trials, the presence/absence of licks were scored as false alarms/correct rejects and neither outcome was punished or rewarded.

Figure 1. Driving behaviour with two-photon optogenetics targeted to ensembles of neurons in L2/3 barrel cortex.

(a) Schematic of all-optical setup. Bottom left: Example of flexible ensemble photostimulation. Three 10 neuron groups in barrel cortex (red, green, blue circles joined by group centroids) were photostimulated sequentially (sequence below Pixel STA). Pixel STA is maximum intensity projection across photostimulus groups of activity in post-photostimulation epoch averaged across trials (N = 3 photostimulus groups, 10 trials each). Inset right: sub-region of a full imaging FOV in L2/3 barrel cortex expressing GCaMP6s/C1V1-mRuby. (b) Schematic summarising the strategy used to train animals to respond to two-photon optogenetic (2P) stimulation. Mice are first trained to respond to one-photon optogenetic (1P) stimulation of barrel cortex (S1) by licking at an electronic lickometer. The power of 1P illumination is reduced until they can be transitioned onto 2P stimulation targeted to specific ensembles of barrel cortex neurons. (c) Structure of the behavioural task (top) and stimulus probabilities, response type contingencies and training phase structures (bottom). Note that stimulus durations, which vary across stimulus types, are not to scale. FA: false alarm; CR: correct reject. (d) Lick raster from an example Phase 2 behavioural training session during which a mouse received 2P stimulation trials (pink: 200 neurons), catch trials (grey: no stimulus) and 1P stimulation trials (amber: 0.05 mW, untargeted). Trials were delivered pseudo-randomly (see Materials and methods) but have been sorted by trial type for display. All licks shown in grey with first lick highlighted in black. Hits/false alarms (black) and misses/correct rejects (grey) are indicated as the vertical bar on the right-hand side. Stimulus durations indicated as coloured bars below lick rasters. Behavioural response window indicated as grey shading, label and arrows. (e–g), Response rate and reaction time mean and standard deviation for different trial types in final Phase 2 session. Animals detect 1P photostimulation and 2P stimulation targeted to 200 neurons to similar extents, at a level far above chance (catch trials), with similar reaction time mean and standard deviation. (N = 12 mice, 1 session each). Note only animals which responded on >2 catch trials are included for reaction time panels (f and g) (N = 11 mice, 1 session each). All error bars are s.e.m.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Indicator and opsin expression overlap analysis.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) Detection of indicator/opsin expression overlap. Suite2P followed by manual curation was used to detect functional GCaMP-expressing neurons from time-series data (top row) and Cellpose followed by manual curation was used to detect C1V1 expression from static images (bottom row). GCaMP (top left) and C1V1 (bottom left) expression in an example plane from a volumetric FOV with corresponding Suite2P ROIs (top middle) and detected C1V1+ neurons (bottom middle). C1V1+ neurons with a Suite2P ROI are circled in red (right; see Materials and methods) and the average expression pattern in both C1V1 expression and GCaMP local correlation images is shown for each of the three possible expression configurations (bottom right insets). N = 6250 C1V1+/Suite2P- neurons, 27084 C1V1-/Suite2P+ neurons and 3810 C1V1+/Suite2P+ neurons across 11 volumetric FOVs from 6 mice, 1–2 volumetric FOVs each. (b) Number of C1V1+ neurons in each volumetric FOV in all mice (black), dual injected WT mice (grey) and opsin injected transgenic GCaMP mice (pink). See below and Materials and methods for details. (c) Number of Suite2P ROIs in each volumetric FOV. (d) Number of dual expressing neurons in each volumetric FOV (C1V1+ neurons with Suite2P ROI). (e) Proportion of C1V1+ neurons out of total number of neurons that have a Suite2P ROI and/or are C1V1+. (f) Proportion of neurons with a Suite2P ROI out of total number of neurons. (g) Proportion of dual expressing neurons (C1V1+ with Suite2P ROI) out of total number of neurons. (h) Proportion of C1V1+ neurons with a Suite2P ROI. (i) Proportion of Suite2P ROIs that are C1V1+. For all group data panels N = 11 volumetric FOVs from the 2P psychometric curve sessions (Figures 2, 3 and 4), 3 FOVs from WT (C57/BL6) mice co-injected with hSyn-GCaMP6s/CaMKII-C1V1-Kv2.1 (grey in all plots) and 8 FOVs from transgenic (Emx1Cre;CaMKIIa-tTA;Ai94) mice injected with CaMKII-C1V1-Kv2.1 (pink in all plots). Aggregate data across expression strategies are black in all plots. All boxplots indicate median (horizontal line) and 25th and 95th percentiles (box) with whiskers extending to the most extreme data not considered outliers (see Materials and methods) and outliers indicated in red beyond.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Spatial resolution of spiral-scanned two-photon optogenetic activation of Kv2.1-C1V1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(a) Left: A cross-shaped grid of photostimulation targets was centred on a single target neuron and each photostimulation target was activated 10 times in random order (10 µm inter-target distance, 30 µm maximum distance from centre, 10 s inter-trial interval, 10 × 20 ms spirals at 20 Hz, 6 mW/neuron). Top right inset: Lateral (xy) resolution of photostimulation of the central target neuron. Only the target position centred over the target neuron result in significant activation. Lateral HWHM = 5 µm (two-degree polynomial fit). N = 1 neuron, 10 trials per stimulation site. Note the lateral exclusion radius (10 µm) used to define target neurons in subsequent analyses is plotted in red. All neurons within this region were considered potential target neurons (see Materials and methods). Bottom right inset: Axial (z) resolution of photostimulation. Photostimulation targets were axially translated from 60 µm above target neurons down onto their cell-body locations in the objective focal plane in 10 µm steps using SLM-based axial displacement while the target neuron responses were consistently imaged at the objective focal plane (separate experiments from lateral resolution). HWHM = 20 µm (two-degree polynomial fit). N = 20 neurons, 10 trials per axial offset. To account for these considerable axial off-target effects we defined all neurons within the lateral exclusion zone of 10 µm (see above) to be potential target neurons irrespective of their axial displacement. (b) Example Kv2.1-C1V1-mScarlet expression image with neuron (pink) and control (grey) targets overlaid as circles. (c) Maximum intensity projection across two pixel-wise STAs triggered off neuron and control site stimulation. Neuron stimulation results in robust activation of targeted neurons, control stimulation results in no activation. N = 10 trials. (d) Left: Extracted traces from target neuron ROIs during both neuron site stimulation (pink) and control site stimulation (grey). Right: post-stimulus ∆F/F0 from target neuron ROIs. Transients evoked in target neuron ROIs during neuron site stimulation are completely absent during stimulation of adjacent control sites, resulting in no recorded ∆F/F0 response. N = 20 neurons, 10 trials each. All error bars and shading are s.e.m. unless otherwise stated.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. 1P training protocol.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) Typical training protocol to transition a naïve mouse to being able to detect two-photon stimulation via a series of one-photon priming phases. Note that exact duration of each component varied by mouse (see Figure 1—figure supplement 5f). (b) Lick rasters from an example first training session (left) and an example low power psychometric curve session (right) for a habituated mouse showing first licks on each trial (circles), auto-reward delivery time (blue vertical line) and analysis window (grey shading). Trials were delivered pseudorandomly and are sorted for display only. Hits (black) and misses (grey) are indicated as the vertical bar to the right of the raster, followed by coloured boxes indicating LED power on the far right. (c–e) Moving average response rate (c), average reaction time (d) and standard deviation of reaction time (e) for the first 300 LED (amber) and catch (grey) trials using a 20 trial window. (f) Mean power (right) and mean d-prime (left) over initial manual power staircase sessions. (g) Minimum power used in each session (right) and d-prime at that power (left) over initial manual power staircase sessions. (h) Response rates on three high-power psychometric curve sessions. N = 22 mice that completed at least one high-power psychometric curve session (other animals were transitioned straight to low-power psychometric curves). (i–k), Response rates (i), mean reaction time (j) and standard deviation of reaction time (k) on three low-power psychometric curve sessions. N = 22 mice that completed at least one low-power psychometric curve session (other animals were transitioned straight to subsequent training phases). All error bars and shading are s.e.m. For all group data panels, N = 26 mice unless otherwise stated, however some data-points may have fewer N as not all animals completed the same number of sessions (see Figure 1—figure supplement 5f for summary).
Figure 1—figure supplement 4. Example pre-training selection and mapping of 200 neurons in L2/3 barrel cortex.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

(a) Kv21-C1V1-mScarlet expression from an example four-plane imaging volume with stimulation targets overlaid in pink. Neurons are chosen arbitrarily, with the only criteria being that neurons are expressing C1V1 and not too far from FOV centre. Depths relative to most superficial plane (~130 µm below pia). (b) Pre-training mapping protocol used to assess photostimulatability. Note 200 neurons are mapped in 4 × 50 neuron groups. (c) Example pixel-wise STA maximum intensity projected across the four stimulation groups and the four planes with targets overlaid in pink. (d) Trial-averaged STA traces from ROIs in (c), 50 neurons per group. Note the temporal offset (0.5 s) between activation of each group. Shading denotes s.e.m. N = 1 FOV, 1 animal, 200 neurons, 10 trials.
Figure 1—figure supplement 5. Rapid transfer learning from 1P to 2P optogenetic stimuli.

Figure 1—figure supplement 5.

(a – b) Response rates for different 1P and 2P stimulus types from the first session using 2P stimuli, quantified as proportion of trials where animal licked (a) and d-prime relative to catch trials (b). Note that this includes a combination of Phase 2 (LED/2P/catch trials) and Phase 3 (2P/catch trials) sessions. (c) Quantification of d-prime relative to our learning criterion (d-prime >1) for the first and last session using 2P stimuli of different magnitudes. Note that some animals only had one session using 2P stimulation of 100 neurons. This session is duplicated in the First and Last tests against d-prime >1, but not included in the test for improvement over time. As such these data points appear as grey dots that are not joined by horizontal grey lines. (d–e) Reaction time mean (d) and standard deviation (e) for different trial types from the first session using 2P stimuli. Trial types with <3 responses were excluded from reaction time analyses. (f) Number of sessions of each step in the training paradigm (coloured bars) and total number of sessions from beginning of 1P training to end of initial 2P training (black bar). For all plots maximum N = 12 mice, although individual data-points may have less as not all animals had the same number of training sessions. All error bars are s.e.m.
Figure 1—figure supplement 6. Comparison of behavioural response to somatic and non-somatic C1V1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 6.

(a) Animals detect stimulation of somatic and non-somatic C1V1 to similar extents. (b) Animals respond slightly quicker to stimulation of non-somatic C1V1. (c) Reaction times are similarly consistent across somatic and non-somatic C1V1. All error bars are s.e.m. N = 12 C1V1-Kv2.1 mice, 19 C1V1 mice.

Animals underwent three training phases (Figure 1c: bottom, Figure 1—figure supplement 3a), beginning with Phase 1 where 1P go trials and catch trials were interleaved in equal proportions. In the first training session (Figure 1—figure supplement 3b), go trials of 10 mW 1P photostimuli were delivered and were automatically rewarded irrespective of the animal’s response (Figure 1—figure supplement 3b, blue line). Animals readily learned to detect photostimulation as shown by an increase in proactive lick responses post-stimulus but before the automatic reward (delivered at 0.5 s post-stimulus onset) and decreases in reaction time mean and s.d. across the first 20–100 trials (Figure 1—figure supplement 3c–e), often within the first session (Figure 1—figure supplement 3b). Once animals showed evidence of learning, automatic rewards were turned off and the LED power was sequentially reduced across several daily training sessions from 10 to 0.25 mW resulting in an inverted U-shaped profile of performance as behaviour improved but stimulation powers dropped (Figure 1—figure supplement 3f,g). Subsequently, animals were tested on several ‘high power’ psychometric curve sessions (Figure 1—figure supplement 5f 1.65 ± 1.06 sessions, N = 26 mice, 4 mice skipped this step) where intermediate LED powers (250–50 µW) were pseudorandomly interleaved trial-by-trial (Figure 1—figure supplement 3h), finally finishing Phase 1 by undergoing several ‘low power’ psychometric curve sessions (100–20 µW) (Figure 1—figure supplement 3i–k, Figure 1—figure supplement 5f 2.12 ± 2.07 sessions, N = 26 mice, 4 mice skipped this step). Animals’ response rates decreased with decreasing 1P photostimulation powers (Figure 1—figure supplement 3i P(Lick) for 100 µW 0.94 ± 0.11 vs 20 µW 0.51 ± 0.29, p=5.96 × 10−5 Wilcoxon signed-rank test, N = 22 mice) and their reaction times became slower (Figure 1—figure supplement 3j reaction time for 100 µW 0.47 ± 0.16 s vs 20 µW 0.58 ± 0.15 s, p=4.61 × 10−5 Wilcoxon signed-rank test, N = 22 mice that did this step) and increasingly variable (Figure 1—figure supplement 3k reaction time s.d. for 100 µW 0.09 ± 0.04 s vs 20 µW 0.16 ± 0.08 s, p=5.46 × 10−4 paired t-test, N = 22 mice that did this step), although even the lowest LED powers evoked lick rates significantly higher than catch trials (Figure 1—figure supplement 3i P(Lick) for catch trials 0.12 ± 0.08 vs go trials of 20 µW 0.51 ± 0.29, p=2.31 × 10−4, 40 µW 0.77 ± 0.20, p=2.01 × 10−4, 60 µW 0.90 ± 0.11, p=2.01 × 10−4, 80 µW 0.94 ± 0.07, p=2.01 × 10−4, 100 µW 0.94 ± 0.11, p=2.01 × 10−4, all Wilcoxon signed-rank tests with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 22 mice, average over 1–3 sessions).

At this point, animals were transitioned to Phase 2 where 1P go trials (50 µW), 2P go trials (targeted to 200 and subsequently 100 neurons) and catch trials were pseudorandomly interleaved in equal proportions (Figure 1c,d). Before each training session, we selected 200 neurons based on C1V1-mRuby expression in clearly expressing FOVs in superficial L2/3 barrel cortex (~130–230 µm below pia) and recorded their sensitivity to photostimulation (Figure 1—figure supplement 4). For each animal, we selected similarly positioned FOVs across days, but did not specifically target the same neurons (which is challenging due to angular inconsistencies in FOV position across days, see Materials and methods). Using these stimulus patterns for 2P go trials, we trained animals on Phase 2, and subsequently some animals on Phase 3 (only 2P stim and catch trials), for several sessions (Figure 1d single session, Figure 1—figure supplement 5a,b,f 3.40 ± 1.51 Phase 2/3 sessions 2P 200/100 neurons, N = 12 mice). During this period, we also began interleaving 2P trials stimulating 100 neurons from the 200 neuron group when animals reliably detected 200 neuron stimulations (typically within a single session: 1.08 ± 0.29 sessions to d-prime >1 on 2P 200 neuron trials, N = 12 mice) (Figure 1—figure supplement 5a,b performance over time). We found that animals reliably detected 2P photostimulation of both 200 and 100 neurons consistently across time, from the first session (Figure 1—figure supplement 5c first 200 neuron d-prime: 2.39 ± 0.86 vs 1, p=1.70 × 10−4 paired t-test; first 100 neuron d-prime: 2.13 ± 0.93 vs 1, p=1.4 × 10−3 paired t-test, N = 12 mice) to the last session (Figure 1—figure supplement 5c last 200 neuron d-prime: 2.83 ± 0.62 vs 1, p=5.95 × 10−7 paired t-test; last 100 neuron d-prime: 2.55 ± 0.7 vs 1, p=1.00 × 10−5 paired t-test, N = 12 mice) and only showed a modest improvement over time (Figure 1—figure supplement 5c 200 neuron first 2.39 ± 0.86 vs last 2.83 ± 0.62, p=0.07 paired t-test, N = 12 mice; 100 neuron first 2.02 ± 1.28 vs last 2.86 ± 0.8, p=0.06 Wilcoxon signed-rank test, N = 6 mice with multiple 100 neuron sessions). On their final Phase 2 session (session 2.75 ± 1.42, N = 12 mice) animals detected 2P photostimulation of 200 neurons with high response rates that were similar to 1P photostimulation (Figure 1d single session, Figure 1e group average, p=1.82 × 10−5 Friedman test, P(Lick) for 1P 0.99 ± 0.02 vs 2P 0.88 ± 0.10, p=0.19, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 12 mice, 1 session each) and with similar reaction time mean (Figure 1f p=6.70 × 10−3 one-way repeated measures ANOVA, 1P 0.49 ± 0.14 s vs 2P 0.56 ± 0.15 s, p=0.77, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 11 mice, 1 session each, only mice with >2 catch trial responses included) and standard deviation (Figure 1g p=4.99 × 10−8 one-way repeated measures ANOVA, 1P 0.09 ± 0.05 s vs 2P 0.13 ± 0.02 s, p=0.25, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 11 mice, 1 session each, only mice with >2 catch trial responses included). Both 2P and 1P photostimulation evoked higher lick rates than catch trials (Figure 1e p=1.82 × 10−5 Friedman test, P(Lick) for catch 0.11 ± 0.07 vs 2P 0.88 ± 0.10, p=1.61 × 10−2, vs 1P 0.99 ± 0.02, p=1.04 × 10−5, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 12 mice, 1 session each) with less variable reaction times (Figure 1g reaction time s.d.; p=4.99 × 10−8 one-way repeated measures ANOVA, catch 0.28 ± 0.09 s vs 2P 0.13 ± 0.02 s, p=5.99 × 10−6, vs 1P 0.9 ± 0.05 s, p=7.24 × 10−8, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 11 mice, 1 session each, only mice with >2 catch trial responses included), though only 1P trials showed quicker reaction times (Figure 1f p=6.70 × 10−3 one-way repeated measures ANOVA, catch 0.69 ± 0.11 s vs 1P 0.49 ± 0.14 s, p=5.91 × 10−3, vs 2P 0.56 ± 0.15 s, p=0.10, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, N = 11 mice, 1 session each, only mice with >2 catch trial responses included). We also note that we found similar response rates in animals expressing non-somatically-restricted C1V1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 6a catch subtracted P(Lick) for C1V1-Kv2.1 0.77 ± 0.11 vs C1V1 0.79 ± 0.14, p=0.61 two-sample t-test, N = 12 C1V1-Kv2.1 and 19 C1V1 injected mice), although reaction times were significantly slower for somatically restricted C1V1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 6b C1V1-Kv2.1 0.55 ± 0.15 s vs C1V1 0.41 ± 0.08 s, p=2.09 × 10−3 two-sample t-test, N = 12 C1V1-Kv2.1 and 19 C1V1 injected mice).

Thus, we have demonstrated that two-photon optogenetic stimulation targeted to small ensembles of cortical neurons can reliably drive behaviour and provides a powerful tool for investigating the perceptual salience of different patterns of neural activity.

Very few cortical neurons are sufficient to drive behaviour

We next tested behavioural sensitivity to the activity of neural ensembles of varying size. To test this we transitioned animals to Phase 3 sessions (2P and catch trials only; Figure 1c: bottom) where we precisely titrated the level of activation by targeting different numbers of neurons on a trial-by-trial basis. We again selected 200 neurons on the basis of C1V1-mRuby expression and sub-divided this group into smaller subsets of 100, 75, 50, 25, 10 and 5 neurons (Figure 2a). Animals then underwent 2P photostimulation psychometric curve sessions during which, in addition to stimulating the group of 200 neurons, we also pseudorandomly interleaved stimulations of the smaller subsets of neurons (Figure 2b,c). We leveraged our ability to simultaneously read out neural activity with calcium imaging to refine our estimate of the number of stimulated neurons according to the number of target neurons that were activated averaged across trials. To quantify activation (and suppression; see later sections), we defined thresholds for each neuron on the basis of their response distribution on correct reject catch trials (Figure 2—figure supplement 1b–g, see Materials and methods), where no stimulus or lick occurred, and to differentiate between target and background neurons we defined 3D target zones around each 2P photostimulation target co-ordinate (Figure 2—figure supplement 1a,h–j, see Materials and methods). This resulted in numbers of activated target neurons that were 0.46 ± 0.20 times that of the number of target zones (averaged across trial types) and decreased with decreasing number of zones as intended (Figure 2—figure supplement 1i; see Materials and methods).

Figure 2. Animals detect the targeted activation of tens of neurons.

(a) Example imaging volumes from an experiment showing 200 (top), 50 (middle) and 5 (bottom) targeted C1V1-expressing neurons. (b) Example lick raster concatenating an animal’s two psychometric curve testing sessions. Trials were delivered pseudo-randomly (see Materials and methods) but have been sorted by trial type for display. Stimulus durations are indicated by coloured bars along the bottom of each raster. Animals respond on more trials and with less variable timing as more neurons are targeted. (c) Example responses across the top 200 most responsive neurons in the 200 target zones (see Materials and methods; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Neurons have been sorted separately in each plot. Pink boxes indicate the stimulus artefact exclusion epoch which is consistent across all trial types (see Materials and methods for definition). (d) The psychometric function relating the number of activated target neurons to the behavioural detection rate for all 2P psychometric curve sessions. Individual data (grey dots) are grouped by trial type within session (number of target zones) and plotted as the average number of target neurons activated across all trials of each type. Data point size indicates the number of trials of each type (29 ± 8 trials, range 11–44, across data points). Individual psychometric curve fits for each session are plotted (grey lines) weighted by the total number of stimulus trials in the session (202 ± 50 trials, range 97–245, across sessions). The number of neurons required to reach the 10%, 50% and 90% points of these individual scaled psychometric curves are shown as black error bars and green circles about the x-axis. The aggregate psychometric curve fit across all trial types, all sessions, is plotted in red. Note that individual curves are often steeper than the aggregate curve. (e) The number of neurons required to reach the 10%, 50%, and 90% points of the scaled psychometric curves in (d). Grey data points/lines are quantified from individual psychometric curve fits (grey lines in d) and summarised by the black error bars. Red data points are quantified from the aggregate psychometric curve fit (red line in d) ± confidence intervals. (f) The slope at the 50% point of the scaled curves corresponding to the additional probability of detection (P(Lick)) added per target neuron activated. Grey data points are quantified from individual psychometric curve fits (grey lines in d) and are summarised by the black error bar. The red circle is quantified from the aggregate psychometric curve fit (red line in d) for which no confidence intervals can be calculated (see Materials and methods). N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each. All data error bars are mean ± s.e.m. and all fit parameter error bars are estimate ± confidence intervals.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Quantification of neuronal responses.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a) Example target zones. Left: section of a plane in a volumetric FOV showing Suite2P ROIs (coloured regions), Suite2P centroids (black dots), target co-ordinates (pink dots) and lateral extent of target zone (pink circles; 10 µm radius). ROIs with centroids within the target exclusion zones are considered potential targets (orange ROIs), ROIs outside these regions are considered background neurons (blue ROIs). Right: schematic illustrating axial extent of target zone. (b) Rationale behind activation and suppression thresholds calculated from correct reject (CR) catch trial responses and illustration of the results of our threshold procedure (described in c–e). Left: trial-wise responses for a single non-target neuron on stimulus trials of all types and CR catch trials. The mean and standard deviation of responses on CR catch trials can be used to infer activation and suppression thresholds that separate stimulus evoked activation (red) and suppression (blue) responses from the majority of CR catch trial responses. Middle: activation and suppression thresholds are computed as the mean + or – the standard deviation respectively, scaled by a scaling factor (separate scaling factors for activation and suppression). Right: average responses on trials where this neuron was activated (red), suppressed (blue) or unresponsive (light grey). Catch trials are also included (dark grey). Such a procedure separates positive and negative going responses from non-responsive trials which should themselves be indistinguishable from catch trials. (c) Procedure for inferring cross-validated positive and negative standard deviation scaling factors that yield a 5% false positive response rate on catch trials for each session’s volumetric FOV. For each session’s data, and each threshold type (i.e. activation threshold), we sweep through a series of scaling factors. For each factor we permute a 80:20 train:test split of correct reject (CR) catch trials 10,000 times. On each permutation, we use the scaling factor and that permutation’s training CR catch trials to calculate each neuron’s threshold. Using these neuron-wise thresholds, we then compute the proportion of neurons activated on each testing CR catch trial and average across trials (false positive rate; FP). We then take the median FP across all permutations at this scaling factor before moving to the next scaling factor. We can then infer the scaling factor that yields a 5% FP rate on testing CR catch trials across permutations. (d) Relationship between standard deviation scaling factor and FP rate for the activation threshold. Data points are empirically quantified values, fits are cubic interpolations. The optimal scaling factor for each experiment (horizontal boxplot) is inferred by finding the scaling factor for which the fitted FP rate is 5% (horizontal line). (e) Relationship between the standard deviation scaling factor and FP rate for the suppression threshold. Data conventions and scaling factor inference the same as (d). (f) Scaling factors that yield 5% false positive rate when all correct reject catch trials are used (not cross-validated, i.e. thresholds inferred and tested on same trials) compared to those yielded by the cross-validation procedure described in (c–e). Cross-validation yields more stringent thresholds. (g) Example traces from one session for five activated target neurons (top row), five activated background neurons (middle row) and five suppressed background neurons (bottom row) illustrating responses yielded by our procedure. Averages across all activated/suppressed trials are shown in red/blue and averages across catch trials/non-responsive stimulus trials are shown in light and dark grey, respectively. Note that neurons in each column are unrelated. (h) The number of neurons in all target zones for each trial type (as defined in (a) and Materials and methods). (i) The number of responsive neurons in all target zones for each trial type on both stimulus trials (black) and catch trials (grey). (j) Proportion of responsive neurons in all target zones for each trial type on both stimulus trials (black) and catch trials (grey) quantified as the number of responsive neurons in target zones (i) divided by the number of neurons in target zones (h). Inset: Proportion of target zone neurons that are responsive on stimulus trials compared to catch trials. N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each for all group data plots. Error bars and shading are s.e.m.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Reaction time standard deviation, but not mean, scales with the number of target neurons activated.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(a–b) Relationship between mean (a) and standard deviation (b) of reaction time and the number of activated target neurons.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Detection of small ensembles of neurons improves across days irrespective of whether the same neurons were targeted.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(a) FOV from training sessions across 3 consecutive days where we stimulated the same 30 neurons each day. (b) Average behavioural response rates for 100 neuron, 30 neuron and catch trial conditions for all sessions where we stimulated the same neurons across days (N = 14 mice). (c) Improvement in response rates from session 1 to session 2. Sessions in which we targeted the same neurons are shown in black and sessions where we targeted different neurons in grey. Data-point size denotes number of targeted neurons, scale inset (same neurons group N = 14 mice, different neurons group N = 5 mice, 2 stim types each). (d) Sessions targeting the same neurons showed a similar amount of improvement as sessions targeting different neurons (same neurons group N = 14 mice, different neurons group N = 5 mice, two stim types each). All error bars are s.e.m.

Animals’ response rates increased sigmoidally with increasing numbers of target neurons activated (Figure 2b single animal, Figure 2d all sessions) and both individual sessions and aggregate data across sessions were well fit by log-normal sigmoid psychometric functions (Figure 2d grey dots/lines: individual data/fits, R2 = 0.91 ± 0.09, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each; red line: aggregate fit, R2 = 0.72, cross-validated R2 = 0.67 ± 0.23, N = 10,000 permutations, see Materials and methods for fit details). Using these fitted psychometric functions, we estimate that animals can detect the activation of a minimum of ~14 neurons at their perceptual threshold (Figure 2d,e 10% point of curve: individual fits 14.2 ± 8.96 neurons, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each; aggregate fit 9.86 [95% CI: 6.09 12.8] neurons), with only roughly double this number of neurons (~37) required to saturate performance (Figure 2d,e 90% point of curve: individual fits 37.0 ± 11.3 neurons, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each; aggregate fit 45.1 [95% CI: 37.3 63.5] neurons). At the psychometric function’s 50% point (Figure 2d,e 50% point of curve: individual fits 21.8 ± 8.71 neurons, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each; aggregate fit 21.1 [95% CI: 17.9 23.9] neurons) this results in a very steep slope, with ~0.05 probability of licking added per neuron stimulated (Figure 2f individual fits 0.05 ± 0.03; aggregate fit 0.03). This is notably steeper for individual fits than the aggregate fit (0.05 ± 0.03 vs 0.03, p=9.77 × 10−3 Wilcoxon signed rank test, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each). Mean reaction times did not vary as fewer neurons were activated (Figure 2—figure supplement 2a β = −0.02, R2 = 0.02, p=0.24), although they did become more variable (Figure 2—figure supplement 2b β = −0.05, R2 = 0.30, p=1.41 × 10−6). This demonstrates that animals are exquisitely sensitive to the activation of small numbers of cortical neurons and can read out surprisingly small changes in cortical activity levels.

We next addressed the question of how flexible this perceptual threshold is and how specific it is to neurons used for training during preceding sessions. After training animals to detect the activation of hundreds of barrel cortex neurons, we asked whether their ability to detect the activation of small subsets of these neurons improved across multiple subsequent days, and whether learning was specific to neurons targeted on each day. In a second cohort of animals, we identified and activated the same neurons reliably across multiple days (Figure 2—figure supplement 3a) and measured the detection rate across sessions (Figure 2—figure supplement 3b), whereas in the first cohort mentioned above we moved FOV for each session and stimulated different neurons. Across all animals there was a consistent improvement in detection rate (Figure 2—figure supplement 3c P(Lick) on Session 1: 0.17 ± 0.18 vs Session 2: 0.28 ± 0.25, p=6.29 × 10−3 paired t-test, N = 14 mice testing the same 30 neurons and 5 mice testing different groups of 25 and 50 neurons) which did not differ depending on whether the same or different neurons were stimulated across sessions (Figure 2—figure supplement 3d P(Lick) improvement for same: 0.12 ± 0.18 vs different: 0.09 ± 0.18, p=0.84 Mann Whitney U-Test, N = 14 mice testing the same 30 neurons and 5 mice testing different groups of 25 and 50 neurons).

These experiments use targeted stimulation of cortical neurons to describe the behavioural input-output function for our task, and suggest that the lower bound for detection is a small number of neurons (~14 neurons) and the psychometric function is very steep (saturating at ~37 neurons). We also demonstrate that animals’ ability to detect small numbers of neurons improves with training and that this improvement is not limited to targeted neurons.

Suppression in the local network balances target activation

We took advantage of our ability to simultaneously record activity in both the targeted and untargeted ‘background’ neurons to investigate how local network activity influences or depends on behavioural performance. Using the same activation thresholds, suppression thresholds and target definitions described earlier (Figure 2—figure supplement 1, see Materials and methods) we calculated the proportion of activated and suppressed neurons on each trial averaged across trials of each type (Figure 3—figure supplement 1a,e). Splitting trials by hits and misses we found that hits were associated with more activation (Figure 3—figure supplement 1a,b P(activated) background on hits 4.94 × 10−2 ± 8.63 x 10−3 vs misses 3.83 × 10−2 ± 5.04 x 10−3 on 50 target trials, p=1.95 × 10−3 Wilcoxon signed-rank test, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each) and less suppression (Figure 3—figure supplement 1e,f P(suppressed) background on hits 3.53 × 10−2 ± 4.84 x 10−3 vs misses 3.99 × 10−2 ± 5.5 x 10−3 on 50 target trials, p=2.23 × 10−2 paired t-test, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each) than misses. However, since hits are associated with stereotyped behaviours (licking, whisking, face movements etc.), and significant movement and reward-related activity has been observed in primary sensory cortical areas (Shuler and Bear, 2006; Niell and Stryker, 2010; Musall et al., 2019; Steinmetz et al., 2019; Stringer et al., 2019; Zatka-Haas et al., 2020), we reasoned that such differences might be accounted for by the behaviours themselves irrespective of our manipulations. Indeed we found that in background neurons the level of activation recruited on hits post-photostimulation was not different from false alarms on catch trials where animals licked but no neurons were photostimulated (Figure 3—figure supplement 1d P(activated) background on 50 target hit 4.92 × 10−2 ± 9.05 x 10−3 vs catch false alarm 4.83 × 10−2 ± 1.04 x 10−2, p=0.52 paired t-test, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each, 1 session without any catch false alarms excluded), irrespective of how many neurons we activated (Figure 3—figure supplement 1a). The amount of suppression also did not differ between hits post-photostimulation and false alarms on catch trials (Figure 3—figure supplement 1h 50 neuron hit vs catch false alarm: 3.51 × 10−2 ± 5.05 x 10−3 vs 3.37 × 10−2 ± 6.99 x 10−3, p=0.27 paired t-test, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each, one session without any catch false alarms excluded), although there was some modulation of this difference with the number of neurons activated (Figure 3—figure supplement 1e). It therefore seemed possible that a significant amount of the stimulus-evoked activity that we read out in background neurons was influenced by lick-related behaviours. In line with this, we found that a large fraction of neurons showed activity which was modulated by spontaneous licking (Figure 3—figure supplement 1i–k 46% ± 11 of neurons lick modulated, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each, see Materials and methods) with neurons showing both positive correlation (Figure 3—figure supplement 1j,l,m 2.83 × 10−2 ± 1.81 x 10−2 lick correlation for all positively lick modulated neurons, N = 9547 neurons) and negative correlation (–2.52 x 10−2 ± 1.51 x 10−2 lick correlation for all negatively modulated neurons, N = 4365 neurons).

Unfortunately, the temporal resolution of calcium imaging does not allow us to tease apart the direction of causality in our data, that is whether this activity causes, or is caused by motor output. However, given the literature demonstrating behavioural output-related activity in sensory cortices (Musall et al., 2019; Steinmetz et al., 2019; Stringer et al., 2019), and the fact that manipulating this activity has no effect on behavioural choices (Zatka-Haas et al., 2020), we were concerned that a significant amount of network activity might result from movement rather than causing it. This would be problematic for our interpretation since the amount of lick contamination will vary by trial type (number of neurons activated) in a manner that correlates with the variable under study (due to the increased P(Lick) with number of neurons activated Figure 2d). To take account of this, we devised a hit:miss matching procedure which removes the variance in hit:miss ratio across trial types by ensuring that all trial types have a 50:50 ratio of hits:misses (Figure 3—figure supplement 1o, see Materials and methods). This is achieved for trials of a given type (i.e. a low P(Lick) trial type: 10 activated neurons) by matching the number of trials of the minority response type (i.e. hits) with random resamples, of the same number, of majority response-type trials (i.e. misses) and averaging network response metrics across resamples. Following this procedure should ensure that all trial types have the same proportion of data contaminated by lick responses and any variation in network response across trial types remaining should be due to the variation in number of target neurons activated.

Using this procedure, we investigated how the network response varies as a function of stimulated ensemble size beyond its stereotyped modulation by the behavioural response. Taking the network as a whole (including target neurons), we found that photostimulation causes both activation (Figure 3a right inset; P(activated) all neurons on photostimulus 5.91 × 10−2 ± 1.20 x 10−2 vs catch 4.82 × 10−2 ± 6.65 x 10−3 trials averaged across all trial types, p=1.17 × 10−3 paired t-test, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each) and suppression (Figure 3b right inset; P(suppressed) all neurons on photostimulus 4.58 × 10−2 ± 4.90 x 10−3 vs catch 3.97 × 10−2 ± 4.80 x 10−3 trials averaged across all trial types, p=1.41 × 10−4 paired t-test, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each) and that both scale with the number of neurons activated (Figure 3a,b P(activated) all neurons: β = 7.54 × 10−3, R2 = 0.24, p=2.36 × 10−5; P(suppressed) all neurons: β = 3.00 × 10−3, R2 = 0.25, p=1.54 × 10−5, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each). When we analysed only background neurons (i.e. excluding targets from the calculation), we found that while photostimulation does cause both activation and suppression in the background network (Figure 3d,e right insets; P(activated) network on photostimulus 4.40 × 10−2 ± 6.89 x 10−3 vs catch 4.02 × 10−2 ± 5.60 x 10−3 trials, p=2.31 × 10−3 paired t-test; P(suppressed) background on photostimulation 3.81 × 10−2 ± 4.60 x 10−3 vs catch 3.27 × 10−2 ± 4.60 x 10−3 trials, p=1.06 × 10−4 paired t-test, all averaged across trial types, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each), only background network suppression scales with the number of activated target neurons (Figure 3e β = 2.95 × 10−3, R2 = 0.31, p=7.79 × 10−7), whereas activation does not (Figure 3d β = 1.70 × 10−3, R2 = 0.05, p=0.08). Moreover, activation and suppression have distinct spatial profiles, with significant suppression occurring over a much broader area (Figure 3—figure supplement 3). This suggests that the network reacts to suppress the spread of activation triggered by our photostimulation in a graded manner which tracks the activation strength, whereas network activation changes to a smaller extent. Indeed, across all neurons in the population we see that there is a consistent balance of activation and suppression across all target activation levels (Figure 3c P(Activated)/P(Suppressed) across all neurons β = 7.23 × 10−2, R2 = 0.05, p=0.06) that remains similar to the rates observed spontaneously during catch trials (Figure 3c right inset; P(Activated)/P(Suppressed) on photostimulation 1.30 ± 0.28 vs catch 1.24 ± 0.25 trials across all neurons, p=0.25 paired t-test, N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each).

Figure 3. Increasing target activation is matched by background network suppression.

(a) The proportion of neurons activated across all neurons (targets and background) increases as more target neurons are activated. Inset right: average activation across all trial types is increased on stimulus trials compared to catch. (b) The proportion of neurons suppressed across all neurons (targets and background) increases as more target neurons are activated. Inset right: average suppression across all trial types is increased on stimulus trials compared to catch. (c) The ratio of activation and suppression is similar to that observed on catch trials (inset right) and is not strongly modulated by the number of activated target neurons. (d) Stimulation of target neurons causes mild activation of background neurons (targets excluded; inset right) but this is not modulated by the number of target neurons activated. (e) Stimulation of target neurons causes suppression of background neurons (targets excluded; inset right) which increases as more target neurons are activated. All data are hit:miss matched to remove potential lick signals (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Materials and methods). For all plots N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each. Some trial types from some sessions are excluded for having too few hits or misses to be able to match the hit:miss ratio. Error bars and shading are s.e.m; data points, error bars and linear fits are stimulus trials, shading is catch trials; grey data points: individual trial types, individual sessions; coloured error bars: data averaged within trial type (number of target zones) across sessions; linear fits are to individual data points; fits are reported ± 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Comparison of network activity on hits and misses for both threshold go trials and catch trials in an effort to quantify and account for lick responses.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(a) The proportion of background neurons activated when different numbers of target neurons are activated on hits/false alarms (green) and misses/correct rejects (grey) on stimulus trials (error bars) and catch trials (shading). Background neurons become more active on stimulus hits and catch false alarms and there does not seem to be much modulation of the amount of background activation by the number of activated target neurons. Note that the background neuron activation response on stimulus trials overlaps with the response on catch trials across the full range of number of target neurons activated. (b) The proportion of activated background neurons is higher on stimulus hit trials than miss trials. Note that we used the 50 target zone stimulation trial type as it has a roughly equal number of hits and misses. (c) The proportion of activated background neurons is greater on catch false alarms (FA; when animals lick) then on catch correct rejects (CR; when the animal do not lick). (d) The proportion of activated background neurons does not differ between 50 target zone stimulus trial hits and catch trial false alarms raising the concern that this response is due to licking, not photostimulation. (e) The proportion of background neurons suppressed when different numbers of target neurons are activated. Same colour conventions as in (a). Stimulus hits are associated with decreased levels of background suppression than misses, and this appears to be somewhat modulated by the number of activated target neurons. Catch trial false alarms are associated with marginally more suppression than catch trial correct rejections. (f) The proportion of suppressed background neurons is lower on hits than misses. (g) The proportion of suppressed background neurons does not differ significantly between false alarms and correct rejects on catch trials. (h) The proportion of suppressed background neurons does not differ between 50 target zone stimulus trial hits and catch trial false alarms. (i) Fluorescence trace from an example neuron with photostimulation trial epochs removed (black trace) with licks denoted below (red ticks). This neuron had a lick correlation of 0.14. The fluorescence trace has been Gaussian smoothed (σ = 1 s) for display only. (j) Distribution correlation values across all neurons recorded. (k) Distribution of p-values for correlation values reported in (j). 46 ± 11% of neurons are lick modulated (α = 0.05). (l) Top: Average traces from neurons binned into 10 equal bins by their lick correlation (trace colours) aligned to the onset of spontaneous licking (red line). Bottom: average across all spontaneous lick bouts. (m) Lick – fluorescence cross-correlation for all lick-responsive neurons positively modulated (yellow) or negatively modulated (blue) by licking. (n) Distribution of the time of maximum |cross-correlation| for positively modulated (yellow) and negatively modulated (blue) neurons in a 4 s window centred on the 0 time-lag of the cross-correlation. p=2.75 × 10−160 Mann Whitney U-Test, N = 9547 positively lick-modulated neurons and 4365 negatively modulated neurons. (o) Schematic illustrating the procedure used to ensure a 50:50 ratio of hits:misses for each trial type. This is done to remove the effect of increased recruitment of licking, and thus lick evoked neural responses, by stimulating target ensembles of increasing size. For a given trial type, say five target zones, we find the number of hits and misses and find the minority response type. For five target zone trials, which aren’t reliably detected by animals, this will likely be hits. We then take all hit trials and combine them with random resamples of miss trials of the same number as hit trials. For example if we only have two hits and six misses then for each random resample we would take all two hits and two random misses. In this way, we ensure that we have an equal number of hit and miss trials. We then calculate neuron response metrics across the subset of hits and misses in each resample and average these metrics across resamples. All error bars and shading are s.e.m. N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Neuropil subtraction has a small effect on response amplitude but it is not the sole cause of negative going responses.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(a) Distribution of trial-wise responses on 50+ neuron stimulation trials calculated from raw (Raw; black) and neuropil subtracted (Sub; red) traces (N = 30,894 neurons, 1312 trials). A large fraction of negative responses are observed even in raw traces without neuropil subtraction. Inset: Difference between each neuron’s trial-wise response when responses were calculated from neuropil subtracted or raw traces (Sub – Raw). Responses are slightly reduced when traces are neuropil subtracted (−0.16 ± 0.58 ΔF/σF, p=0 Wilcoxon signed-rank test). (b) Neuropil-subtracted trial-wise responses are highly correlated with raw trial-wise responses (R2 = 0.87, p=0), although slightly reduced (β0 = -0.10 ± 5.32 x 10−4, p=0) with this reduction mainly limited to large positive going responses (β1 = 0.82 x 10−1±3.31 × 10−4, p=0). Red: linear fit to data, Black: neuropil-subtracted data binned into deciles by raw trial-wise response and averaged within decile. Data are mean ± s.d. (c) Trial-wise responses classified as suppressed, activated and no response using neuropil-subtracted traces also show negative (−1.75 ± 1.05 ΔF/σF, p=0 Wilcoxon signed-rank test), roughly zero (0.16 ± 0.94 ΔF/σF, p=0, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and positive (3.72 ± 3.18 ΔF/σF, p=0 Wilcoxon signed-rank test) trial-wise responses calculated from raw traces in the same trials. These groups also differed significantly from each other (p=0 Kruskal-Wallis test; Suppressed vs No response, p=0, Suppressed vs Activated, p=0, No response vs Activated, p=0, Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons) indicating that our thresholds on neuropil subtracted traces reliably separate different classes of trial-wise responses even in the absence of neuropil-subtraction. (d) Example raw (light red, light blue) and neuropil-subtracted (dark red, dark blue) traces for neurons reliably activated (red) and suppressed (blue) across trials (using neuropil-subtracted trial-wise magnitudes). Neuropil subtraction slightly decreases response magnitudes in both groups, though does not induce large changes in response time-course. Negative responses can be seen with and without neuropil-subtraction. Shading is mean ± s.e.m across neurons. Traces have been linearly interpolated across the photostimulation epoch (grey box) to avoid the photostimulation artefact for display only. All box-plots are median (mid-line) with 25th and 75th percentiles (box) and 5th and 95th percentiles (whiskers). N = 3.68 × 106 trial-wise responses, 30894 neurons, 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Activation and suppression have different spatial profiles.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(a) Lateral and axial spatial profile of activation (left) and suppression (right) relative to nearest target site co-ordinate: for each neuron plot its distance to nearest target site co-ordinate vs the proportion of trials it was activated or suppressed. Bin spatial distances and then average across all neurons in each bin. Average this value across stimulus trial types. (b) Quantification of spread of activation and suppression with lateral distance, collapsed across all axial planes. All data in all panels are hit:miss matched (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Materials and methods) and have had catch trial values subtracted. All tests in (b) are Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, Bonferroni corrected for the number of lateral bins. * denotes p<0.05. N = 10 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each. Note one session was excluded as there were no hits on catch trials so we were unable to subtract a hit:miss matched neural response rate on catch trials from the stimulus trial data. Error bars are s.e.m. and spatial bin widths are 10 µm.

These results suggest that activating target neurons produces suppression in the surrounding network which maintains homeostasis of background activity.

Behaviour tracks target neuron activity despite constant, matched suppression in the local network

As it has been suggested that both excitation and inhibition of cortical neurons can drive behaviour (Doron et al., 2014), and since target activation and network suppression correlate in our dataset (Figure 3e), we finally asked which of the factors that we have analysed best correlates with animals’ behavioural responses. Using the hit:miss matched data described above, we modelled behavioural response rates as a function of target activation, background activation and background suppression (Figure 4a–c), cross-validating across training and test datasets to assess the goodness and generalisability of the fits (see Materials and methods). The strongest and most generalisable predictor of behavioural responses was target neuron activation, which had significantly positive R2 across both training and test sets during cross-validation (Figure 4a,d train R2: 0.71 ± 0.06, p=0 permutation test vs 0; test R2: 0.66 ± 0.23, p=1.68 × 10−2 permutation test vs 0, N = 10,000 train:test splits). Network suppression had mild predictive power on training data, but none on testing data (Figure 4c,d train R2: 0.16 ± 0.06, p=2.50 × 10−3 permutation test vs 0; test R2: 0.01 ± 0.39, p=0.39 permutation test vs 0, N = 10,000 train:test splits) which might be explained by its correlation with target activation (Figure 3e). Network activation was a poor predictor of both training and testing data, suggesting that it had little influence on behavioural performance (Figure 4b,d train R2: −0.05 ± 0.05, p=0.11 permutation test vs 0; test R2: −0.21 ± 0.44, p=0.22 permutation test vs 0, N = 10,000 train:test splits). Thus, within the cortical region that we can observe and manipulate we have tested three of the main pathways by which activity in cortex could influence downstream circuitry to ultimately drive behaviour: (1) output of directly activated target neurons; (2) output of background neurons synaptically activated by target neurons; (3) suppression of the output of background neurons through disynaptic inhibition by interneurons activated by target neurons (Figure 4e). Our results indicate that the most robust effect is the number of activated target neurons (1), with only a minor impact of indirectly modulated neurons in the local network.

Figure 4. Behaviour follows the activity of targeted ensembles despite matched suppression in the local network.

Figure 4.

(a–c) Psychometric curve fits relating behavioural detection to the number of targets activated (a), the proportion of the background network activated (b) and the proportion of the background network suppressed (c). Solid lines: psychometric curve fit; dotted lines: fixed lapse rate (upper) and false alarm rate (bottom) for psychometric fit. Note that all neural data has been hit:miss matched (see Materials and methods) so the effective P(lick) for all datapoints is 0.5; however, for each datapoint we fit the actual recorded P(lick). The similarity of panel (a), which is hit:miss matched, to Figure 2d, which is not, demonstrates that the contribution of lick signals to the relationship between target activation and behaviour is negligible. Fits and R2 values reported are quantified on all data (compared to cross-validated fits in following panels). For these panels, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each. Some trial types from some sessions are excluded for having too few hits or misses to be able to match the hit:miss ratio. (d) Variance explained (R2) by the three predictors in (a–c) during the training (grey) and testing (black) phases of cross-validation (10,000 permutations of 80:20 train:test split). Only target activation strongly and reliably explains behaviour across both training and testing. Background suppression is mildly predictive of behaviour in model training datasets, but this relationship does not generalise to test datasets. Background activation does not explain behaviour. Boxplots are median with 25th and 75th percentile boxes and whiskers extending to the most extreme data points not considered outliers (see Materials and methods). (e) Schematic summarising the three tested routes from cortical activation to behavioural report highlighting that only the activity of target neurons has any reliable influence on behaviour despite matched suppression in the local network.

Discussion

By precisely titrating the number of activated neurons to be detected, we have demonstrated that the psychometric function for detecting cortical activity is sensitive, with only tens of neurons required to drive behaviour, and it is steep, with only an approximate doubling in number sufficient to drive asymptotic behavioural performance. Simultaneous imaging of the surrounding network has allowed us to show that, despite this exquisite behavioural sensitivity, the dominant network response matches the activation of targeted neurons with local suppression, flattening the network input-output function to maintain the level of network activation within the spontaneous range. These results support the sparse coding hypothesis (Barlow, 1972; Barth and Poulet, 2012; Kanerva, 1993; Olshausen and Field, 1996), demonstrate that the local network operates in an inhibition-stabilised regime (Denève and Machens, 2016; Murphy and Miller, 2009; Ozeki et al., 2009; Pehlevan and Sompolinsky, 2014; Sanzeni et al., 2020; Tsodyks et al., 1997; van Vreeswijk and Sompolinsky, 1996; Wolf et al., 2014), and suggest a high storage capacity for recurrent networks (Hopfield, 1982; Lefort et al., 2009; Peron et al., 2020). This combination of features likely maximises perceptual sensitivity while minimising erroneous detection of background activity, thus avoiding hallucinations (Carbon, 2014; Cassidy et al., 2018; Corlett et al., 2019; Friston, 2005), runaway excitation (Rose and Blakemore, 1974; Treiman, 2001; Ziburkus et al., 2006) and reducing cortical energy requirements (Schölvinck et al., 2008). We also show that the lower bound of detectable activity is not fixed, but can improve with training in a way that can generalise to other neurons in the surrounding network. This suggests that perceptual learning can reroute cortical resources to meet even the most stringent task demands and generalise to other potentially relevant neurons, increasing cognitive flexibility. This also demonstrates that the brain’s ability to ‘learn to learn’ (Behrens et al., 2018; Harlow, 1949) can extend even to arbitrary activity patterns.

Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual threshold

We leveraged our all-optical system to precisely target different numbers of neurons for two-photon optogenetic activation in order to define the minimum number that is sufficient to drive behaviour. The perceptual threshold we measured is remarkably low: mice can detect the activation of only ~14 cortical neurons. This number is substantially lower than the threshold estimated for one-photon activation of layer 2/3 neurons in rodent barrel cortex (~60; Huber et al., 2008). This might be explained by the fact that we tailor our optogenetic stimulus for activation of individual neurons, whereas one-photon stimulation diffusely activates an entire population of neurons (many of which will receive subthreshold levels of photocurrent). Moreover, our stimuli drive double the number of action potentials (~10 in each neuron) which Huber et al. show increases detectability, though we note that our minimum threshold of ~140 action potentials (10 action potentials in 14 neurons) is still lower than the ~300 that they report. Furthermore, we produce comparatively more clustered activation of stimulated neurons in our ensembles (confined to a ~500 × 500 x 100 µm volume) compared to the more dispersed neuron locations used in Huber et al. (potentially across the whole of S1), which may result in different recruitment of both local and downstream targets due to changes in intra- and inter-laminar connection probability with distance (Holmgren et al., 2003; Lefort et al., 2009; Perin et al., 2011; Thomson and Lamy, 2007; Yoshimura et al., 2005). Finally, the Huber et al., 2008 study relied on post-hoc histological estimates which, as noted by the authors, may have a significant margin of error.

On the other hand, our estimate of the perceptual threshold is an order of magnitude higher than single-cell patch-clamp experiments demonstrating that strong activation of single neurons can in some cases lead to a behavioural report (Doron et al., 2014; Houweling and Brecht, 2008; Tanke et al., 2018). Several key differences could account for this discrepancy. First, these studies mostly stimulated L5 neurons, which serve as the output neurons of the cortical circuit and therefore may drive behaviour more reliably compared to the L2/3 ensembles we target. Indeed, such a dichotomy is confirmed by a recent study comparing the behavioural influence of functionally defined ensembles in L2/3 with those in L5 (Marshel et al., 2019). Secondly, these studies report significant variability in the ability of single neurons to drive behaviour, with many neurons having no effect, and with the most potent behavioural effects being limited mainly to fast-spiking putative interneurons (Doron et al., 2014). Additional factors which may play a role include differences in level of stimulation of individual neurons, differences in the training protocol, or species differences between mice and rats.

Interestingly, the reaction times to optogenetic stimuli that we report (0.4–0.7 s) are comparable to, although slightly slower than, those reported for detection of whisker stimuli in mice (0.3–0.4 s; Chen et al., 2013a; Hires et al., 2015; O'Connor et al., 2010; Sachidhanandam et al., 2013) suggesting that they may be processed differently. This could be because sensory stimuli more robustly recruit many neurons distributed over several parallel thalamo-cortical pathways which provide input to multiple cortical layers (Feldmeyer et al., 2013; Petersen, 2007), including direct projections from thalamus to cortical output L5 (Constantinople and Bruno, 2013), whereas our optogenetic stimuli will only activate small numbers of neurons in L2/3. Moreover, sensory stimuli will likely drive patterns of activity that respect cortical wiring for transmission of sensory information and so may propagate more potently downstream. Future work comparing reaction times to targeted activation of sensory-evoked or random ensembles of neurons in the same animal will yield insight into any distinction between naturalistic and artificial neural activity in driving behaviour. Our results also reveal reduced reaction time for non-somatically-restricted C1V1 with little difference in response rate. This difference in reaction time may be due to increased off-target activation via processes traversing the photostimulation volume as well as larger photocurrent in target neurons through activation of opsin in neuronal compartments in addition to the soma (dendrites/axon). It is possible that this does not result in an increased response rate because animals’ performance is saturated at its upper bound (allowing for a lapse rate that is independent of the salience of stimuli).

What are the functional implications of the perceptual threshold we have defined? The fact that in our study (see also Daie et al., 2019; Gill et al., 2020; Marshel et al., 2019) the lower perceptual bound is well above a single neuron suggests that perceptual thresholds are tuned to minimise conscious perception of the spontaneous neural activity which often co-exists alongside stimulus activity (Bernander et al., 1991; Destexhe and Paré, 1999; Destexhe et al., 2003; London et al., 2010; Musall et al., 2019; Shadlen and Newsome, 1994; Stringer et al., 2019; Tolhurst et al., 1983; Waters and Helmchen, 2006). If the perceptual apparatus is sensitive to single-neuron activation, this may lead to erroneous detection of background cortical activity. While the stimuli in our study do not explicitly mimic sensory evoked activity, such false positives in our behavioural paradigm may be related to the hypothesised role of false sensory percepts in generating hallucinations, which impair normal cognitive function and are associated with an array of pathological conditions (Chaudhury, 2010; Kumar et al., 2009; Llorca et al., 2016). The threshold of ~14 neurons could therefore be important for avoiding perceptual false positives caused by spontaneous background network activity.

The relatively low perceptual threshold may also have significant computational advantages. A large body of theoretical work has suggested that the brain may use a sparse coding scheme to represent information (Barlow, 1972; Kanerva, 1993; Olshausen and Field, 1996). Our demonstration that the perceptual threshold (~14 neurons) is much lower than the dimensionality of barrel cortex (~400,000 neurons in barrel cortex; Hooks et al., 2011; Meyer et al., 2013; ~2000 neurons in superficial layers of a barrel; Lefort et al., 2009), but also significantly higher than a single neuron, is consistent with computational theories proposing that individual items are represented sparsely compared to the dimensionality of the space, but also that they are not represented by single elements of that space (Baum et al., 1988; Kanerva, 1993; Olshausen and Field, 2004; Palm, 1980). Our work is also consistent with work suggesting that barrel cortex can use ensembles on this scale to robustly encode sensory information (Hires et al., 2015; Mayrhofer et al., 2015; Panzeri et al., 2014; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2008). Such sensitivity is beneficial as it suggests that recurrent networks like cortical L2/3 have a high-storage capacity (Hopfield, 1982; Lefort et al., 2009; Ko et al., 2011; Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013; Cossell et al., 2015; Peron et al., 2020) allowing the brain to represent many patterns independently (Amit et al., 1985a; Amit et al., 1985b; McEliece et al., 1987; Brunel, 2016; Folli et al., 2016).

The network input-output function for perception is steep

By recording the response of the local network while carefully titrating the number of targeted neurons, we have defined the network input-output function for perception in our task. This function is sigmoidal and remarkably steep, saturating at only ~37 neurons. These results echo similarly steep perceptual input-output functions found in other systems (Gill et al., 2020; Marshel et al., 2019) but are much steeper than estimated in barrel cortex for one-photon optogenetic stimulation (Huber et al., 2008). Again, this discrepancy may be due to a range of factors associated with one-photon photostimulation, from the spatially diffuse (and largely subthreshold) nature of the activation to differences in network cooperativity associated with our more clustered stimulation patterns. Indeed, the highly synergistic activation of a local network of densely interconnected excitatory neurons (Cossell et al., 2015; Douglas et al., 1995; Ko et al., 2011), rapidly followed by suppression in the local network (London et al., 2010; Kwan and Dan, 2012; Chettih and Harvey, 2019) likely mediated by disynaptic inhibition (Jouhanneau et al., 2018; Mateo et al., 2011; Silberberg and Markram, 2007), may be the basis for the steep and saturating input-output function we have observed.

This steep input-output function may have significant functional consequences. While allowing rejection of noise due to spontaneous activity (see the previous section), it also enables perceptual detection of relevant activity with high sensitivity and efficiency, and yet avoids further unnecessary engagement of the network with additional stimulation. This may represent a circuit mechanism for optimising the canonical trade-off in a sensory system subject to noise (Bialek, 2012) between minimising false positives (a response when there is no signal: a ‘false alarm’) and minimising false negatives (missing a signal when there is one present). The steepness of the input-output function and the low number of neurons at saturation are also consistent with optimal energy efficiency (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001; Lennie, 2003). Indeed, our results offer cellular-resolution support for the proposal that the energy associated with conscious perception is surprisingly low (Schölvinck et al., 2008), since our data predict that the number of additional neurons required to allow a subconsciously processed sensory stimulus to be consciously perceived will be low.

While our behavioural paradigm relies on stimulation of artificially defined ensembles of neurons, we maintain that our results add general insight as to how neural activity can underlie flexible behaviour in barrel cortex since: (1) bulk optogenetic activation of pyramidal neurons in sensory areas can readily replace trained sensory stimuli with minimal behavioural impact (Ceballo et al., 2019b; O'Connor et al., 2013; Sachidhanandam et al., 2013), suggesting that the activity evoked is not so alien as to confuse behavioural processing; (2) the order of magnitude of the numbers that we report corresponds closely with the estimated number of barrel cortex neurons required to decode tactile stimuli of various types (Hires et al., 2015; Mayrhofer et al., 2015; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2008) suggesting that there is limit to this system’s sensitivity that can be found through both observation and causal manipulation; (3) irrespective of the exact numbers, the steepness of the psychometric function suggests a fine distinction between whether neural activity is perceptible or not which is indicative of a highly sensitive yet specific sensory system, as has also been shown for optogenetic stimuli mimicking sensory (visual) percepts (Marshel et al., 2019); (4) the unique ability of two-photon optogenetics to specifically target the same, or different, ensembles of neurons throughout learning has explicitly demonstrated the flexibility of this perceptual threshold and how this flexibility can generalise; (5) our ability to image background neurons in the surrounding network has added a further layer of understanding to seminal papers in the field (Houweling and Brecht, 2008; Huber et al., 2008) and demonstrates that cortical networks largely balance increasing levels of activation with matched suppression.

Nevertheless, it is important to keep in mind caveats inherent to current all-optical approaches that may influence these results, such as limitations in spike readout with calcium indicators (Chen et al., 2013b; Pachitariu et al., 2018), the photostimulation efficiency of two-photon optogenetic activation (reported here and in Mardinly et al., 2018; Marshel et al., 2019; Shemesh et al., 2017) and the fact that calcium imaging subsamples the full extent of neural activity involved in complex behaviours. It is also likely that our results will be influenced by the exact stimulation parameters, such as stimulus duration, strength, and timing, as has been noted for the detectability of direct cortical activation in the past (Doron et al., 2014; Gill et al., 2020; Histed and Maunsell, 2014; Huber et al., 2008). Additionally, as with all studies using trained non-naturalistic behaviour, it is worth considering how training duration might influence our results. We note that animals learn our basic task very quickly and that they continue to quickly generalise learning to new, harder stimuli over time as is often observed in trained sensory paradigms (Andermann, 2010; Gerdjikov et al., 2010; O'Connor et al., 2010). This quick learning means that their performance is stable and effectively saturated by the time we test their perceptual sensitivity, in line with the way in which sensitivity to sensory stimuli is tested in many systems (Britten et al., 1996; Busse et al., 2011; Carandini and Churchland, 2013; Morita et al., 2011). Therefore, our results are interpretable within this standard framework of testing perceptual sensitivity in animals that have learned a non-naturalistic task close to saturation (although see next section), be it contingent on sensory or artificial stimulation. Moreover, as we explore in the next section, we demonstrate that this threshold can change with training, suggesting that the more pertinent feature to consider in relation to perception more generally may be the steepness of animals’ psychometric curves. Indeed, our results using artificial ensembles complement the psychometric functions reported in recent studies driving behaviour by optogenetically mimicking sensory ensembles (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019). Finally, an additional factor influencing our experiments is that bulk 1P optogenetic activation over long training periods may change activity and connectivity patterns, as has been suggested for repeated exposure to 2P optogenetic stimulation of the same neurons over time in the absence of behaviour (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016) and for repeated exposure to the same sensory stimuli over behavioural training (Chen et al., 2015; Khan et al., 2018; Peron et al., 2015; Poort et al., 2015; Wiest et al., 2010). Future work recording neural responses in the population before, during, and after 1P/2P behavioural training will yield important insight into this process and it will be crucial to compare the changes observed between animals trained on optogenetic stimuli and sensory stimuli.

The perceptual threshold is plastic and can generalise

We have used our ability to specifically target the same (or different) neurons across multiple days to show that the perceptual threshold is not fixed and depends on learning in a neuron-agnostic manner. This suggests that the perceptual apparatus can flexibly adapt in order to adjust the trade-offs between different kinds of errors while maximising sensitivity (e.g. minimising false positives vs false negatives). It also underscores the brain’s ability to reroute its resources (Chen et al., 2015; Hong et al., 2018; Huber et al., 2012; Kawai et al., 2015; Law and Gold, 2008; Ölveczky et al., 2011) to adaptively meet task demands with ever increasing sensitivity and accuracy (Carandini and Churchland, 2013; Fahle, 2005; Gilbert et al., 2001; Sasaki et al., 2010). Previous studies addressing this question in the context of sensory tasks have suggested that such learning is associated with changes in the representation of sensory stimuli in primary sensory areas (Chen et al., 2015; Khan et al., 2018; Peron et al., 2015; Poort et al., 2015; Wiest et al., 2010). However, in our case animals learn despite the stimulus (direct stimulation) being held constant in sensory cortex. We therefore hypothesise that, in our task, such learning-related changes likely occur in downstream regions like S2 (Chen et al., 2015; Kwon et al., 2016), motor cortex (Chen et al., 2015; Huber et al., 2012), or striatum (Sippy et al., 2015; Xiong et al., 2015).

The fact that the learning we observe during the 2P training phase generalises to neurons that are not stimulated during this period suggests that animals can ‘learn to learn’ (Harlow, 1949) within the context of detecting arbitrary cortical activity patterns, similarly to what is observed in tasks relying on more naturalistic neural processing (Fahle, 2005; Rudebeck and Murray, 2011; Tse et al., 2007; Walton et al., 2010). Such generalisation of knowledge acquired from one learning epoch to another is a hallmark of the type of powerful statistical learning systems that could underlie some of the brain’s most complex, flexible behaviours (Behrens et al., 2018; Eichenbaum and Cohen, 2014; Fahle, 2005; Gustafson and Daw, 2011; Stachenfeld et al., 2017; Tolman, 1948; Tolman et al., 1946; Whittington et al., 2018; Whittington et al., 2019). Our results, and the experimental paradigm that we present, could provide a useful framework to further investigate how learning and credit are assigned to ensembles of neurons in an appropriate yet generalisable way.

The relationship between the perceptual threshold that we measure and how much learning can generalise also merits further investigation. It would be interesting to investigate how nearby stimulated neurons have to be to previously trained neurons, either physically or in terms of tuning similarity, for learning to generalise to them. Indeed another all-optical study hints that generalisation is limited to neurons that share stimulus tuning congruent with ensembles of neurons that have been previously trained (Marshel et al., 2019) implying that generalisation is not a universal property. Moreover, it would also be interesting to see whether the strength of generalisation scales with the amount of uncertainty in the preceding trained stimulus set. One could imagine that learning would be more likely to generalise if different neurons were activated on each training day (as in our experiments), or even each trial, than if just one pattern was trained for the same duration. Such volatility in the learning environment is indeed thought to change learning dynamics (Behrens et al., 2007; Massi et al., 2018; McGuire et al., 2014) and by extension may influence the level of generalisation at the neural level, as has recently been suggested for hippocampal representations (Plitt and Giocomo, 2019; Sanders et al., 2020).

How might such generalisability of learning arise? It could in part result from increased connectivity between opsin-expressing neurons through plasticity induced during their synchronous activation during 1P training phases, equivalent to how artificial subnetworks might be generated by 2P all-optical methods (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2018, though see also Alejandre-García et al., 2020 for alternative non-Hebbian mechanisms). In this case, subsequent 2P photostimulation of opsin-expressing neurons on a given day might preferentially recurrently excite other non-targeted opsin-expressing neurons on that day to a greater extent than non-opsin-expressing neurons, as is thought to happen with 2P optogenetic recall of artificially generated subnetworks (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016). This could cause them to become active and ‘bound into’ the learning process on that day, despite not being directly targeted, and allow them to better drive behaviours when targeted on subsequent days. This would be an intriguing mechanism and, while such changes in connectivity in our task would be ‘artificial’, other work suggests that similar changes might underlie generalisation in more natural sensory guided tasks. Neurons sharing functional tuning to sensory stimuli tend to form recurrently connected subnetworks (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Chettih and Harvey, 2019; Cossell et al., 2015; Jennings et al., 2019; Ko et al., 2013; Marshel et al., 2019; Peron et al., 2020; Russell et al., 2019; Znamenskiy et al., 2018), which result in non-targeted members being recruited when a subset are targeted for photostimulation (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Jennings et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019; Russell et al., 2019), and learning can preferentially generalise across neurons within such subnetworks in sensory-guided tasks (Marshel et al., 2019). Thus, while the subnetworks that might be generated through our 1P training may be artificial, the process of learning generalisation that we observe may also occur in more naturalistic sensory-driven tasks. Indeed, the fact that this mechanism can extend beyond naturalistic stimuli to aid in detection of arbitrary stimulus patterns speaks to how pivotal it may be in helping the brain generate flexible behaviour.

The combination of sensitivity and flexibility that we report also raises the question of whether animals could be rapidly trained to detect the activity of small numbers of neurons de novo, without prior conditioning. This may be difficult to demonstrate in a realistic experimental timeframe given that animals have a tendency to adopt easy but sub-optimal strategies, such as timing licks to coincide with the mean of the trial-time distribution, when they are faced both with non-naturalistic task design and stimuli that are hard to detect/discriminate. Once these strategies are adopted, such local optima tend to be very hard to train away and are thus better avoided in the first place. Indeed, studies testing perceptual sensitivity to or discrimination of sensory stimuli overwhelmingly begin with easier stimulus types to habituate the animal to the novel task at hand and learning continues as more difficult stimuli are introduced (Abraham et al., 2004; Andermann, 2010; Busse et al., 2011; Carandini and Churchland, 2013; Gerdjikov et al., 2010; Histed et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2012; Morita et al., 2011; O'Connor et al., 2010). These features of sensory-evoked behavioural performance are analagous to how animals in our task constantly adapt over time to reductions in stimulus strength, even down to the lowest stimulus levels tested. Furthermore, all previous studies using all-optical techniques to influence behaviour with cellular resolution optogenetics have incorporated some kind of conditioning phase using either sensory or optogenetic stimuli of progressively lower strength (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Gill et al., 2020; Jennings et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019; Russell et al., 2019) again implying that this may be necessary when probing the limits of perception.

Perception is sensitive despite matched network suppression

The matched suppression that we observed in the local L2/3 network is in accordance with the general net inhibitory effect of pyramidal neuron stimulation observed in vivo (Chettih and Harvey, 2019; Kwan and Dan, 2012; Mateo et al., 2011; Russell et al., 2019) and in detailed network models of cortex (Cai et al., 2020). This supports the idea that such networks operate in an inhibition-stabilised regime where one role of inhibition is to control strong recurrent excitation (Denève and Machens, 2016; Murphy and Miller, 2009; Ozeki et al., 2009; Pehlevan and Sompolinsky, 2014; Sanzeni et al., 2020; Tsodyks et al., 1997; van Vreeswijk and Sompolinsky, 1996; Wolf et al., 2014), although since our perturbations are not targeted to inhibitory neurons with specific tuning we cannot assess how functionally specific this architecture might be (Sadeh and Clopath, 2020). However, these results also seemingly challenge recent work demonstrating that activation of co-tuned ensembles in V1 predominantly activates other similarly tuned neurons in the surrounding network (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019) and that ablation of some neurons within functional sub-groups reduces activity in the spared neurons (Peron et al., 2020). This discrepancy is likely explained by the fact that recurrent excitation is known to increase with tuning similarity such that neurons sharing functional tuning tend to recurrently excite each other (Cossell et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011), whereas inhibition is generally less tuned and structured (Kerlin et al., 2010; Bock et al., 2011; Fino and Yuste, 2011; Hofer et al., 2011; Packer and Yuste, 2011; Scholl et al., 2015, though see Ye et al., 2015; Znamenskiy et al., 2018). Indeed, Marshel et al., 2019 specifically use a V1 network model relying on recurrent excitation between co-tuned neurons and strong general inhibition, which keeps activity in check, to explain the low threshold and steepness of their psychometric functions. The subset of neurons we targeted, which may not share functional tuning, are unlikely to benefit from such preferential recurrent connectivity but they will likely recruit general inhibition (although, as mentioned above, some form of enhanced connectivity or intrinsic excitability may have been induced during early 1P training). Therefore, the matched suppression and steep psychometric functions that we observe are consistent with this model.

Since it has not been possible up until very recently to assess the impact of such titrated activation of cortical neurons on the local network during behaviour (Doron et al., 2014; Histed and Maunsell, 2014; Houweling and Brecht, 2008; Huber et al., 2008; Tanke et al., 2018, though see recent work Gill et al., 2020; Marshel et al., 2019), recent theoretical work inspired by previous behavioural results has explored how simulated neural networks can detect the activation of single neurons (Bernardi et al., 2020; Bernardi and Lindner, 2017; Bernardi and Lindner, 2019). These studies make three key predictions: (1) the pool of readout neurons must be biased in favour of connecting with the stimulated neuron (Bernardi and Lindner, 2017), (2) the readout network must include local recurrent inhibition to mitigate noise-inducing neuronal cross-correlations (Bernardi and Lindner, 2019), and (3) inhibition must lag excitation in the readout network (Bernardi et al., 2020). Since our paradigm allows us to simultaneously monitor the local network response during behavioural detection of similarly sparse activity, we can assess the validity of such predictions in vivo. The strong suppression recruited by our stimulation suggests that powerful inhibition is at work in the network and therefore supports prediction (2). The activity we induce drives behaviour despite this strong local suppression, suggesting that excitation might be transmitted to downstream circuits responsible for driving behaviour before inhibition has a chance to quell it locally. This supports prediction (3). Our results concerning prediction (1) are more mixed. Bernardi and Lindner, 2017 suggest that the predicted bias could arise due to Hebbian plasticity between stimulated and readout neurons during the initial microstimulation phase of training. They take as evidence for this the fact that naïve animals cannot detect single neurons, something which both we and other similar studies also see (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Gill et al., 2020; Histed and Maunsell, 2014; Huber et al., 2008; Marshel et al., 2019). The fact that animals generally require initial one-photon priming before being able to detect targeted two-photon stimuli therefore lends some support to prediction (1). However, somewhat contradictory to this prediction is our finding that the amount by which animals improve in their detection of threshold stimuli across sessions is similar irrespective of whether the same or different neurons were stimulated. The bias in connectivity that supposedly develops between target neurons and readout neurons should not extend to other neurons that are not targeted on that day. The fact that we observe such a transfer, manifested in a similar learning rate across different neurons targeted across days, suggests that this bias may be more general than hypothesised above and may apply across most neurons contained within the area where learning has taken place, potentially via recurrent lateral connectivity (either existing or induced during 1P training).

Outlook

The combination of techniques that we have deployed provide a powerful experimental framework that can be used to test how more nuanced features of cellular identity and specific patterns of cortical activity influence perception. This will bring us closer to the goal of testing precisely which of the candidate features of the neural code underlie the considerable flexibility and processing power of the brain (Jazayeri and Afraz, 2017; Panzeri et al., 2017).

Materials and methods

All experimental procedures were carried out under Project Licence 70/14018 (PCC4A4ECE) issued by the UK Home Office in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act (1986) and were also subject to local ethical review. All surgical procedures were carried out under isoflurane anaesthesia (5% for induction, 1.5% for maintenance), and every effort was made to minimise suffering.

Animal preparation

4–6 week old wild-type (C57/BL6) and transgenic GCaMP6s mice (Emx1-Cre;CaMKIIa-tTA;Ai94) of both sexes were used. A calibrated injection pipette (15 µm inner diameter) bevelled to a sharp point was mounted on an oil-filled hydraulic injection system (Harvard apparatus) and front-loaded with virus (either a 1:10 mixture of AAV1-Syn-GCaMP6s-WPRE-SV40 and AAVdj-CaMKIIa-C1V1(E162T)-TS-P2A-mCherry-WPRE or a 1:8 mixture of AAV1-Syn-GCaMP6s-WPRE-SV40 and either AAV2/9-CaMKII-C1V1(t/t)-mScarlett-Kv2.1 or AAV2/9-CaMKII-C1V1(t/t)-mRuby2-Kv2.1). AAV2/9-CaMKII-C1V1(t/t)-mScarlett-Kv2.1 and AAV2/9-CaMKII-C1V1(t/t)-mRuby2-Kv2.1 virus was diluted in virus buffer solution (20 mM Tris, pH 8.0, 140 mM NaCl, 0.001% Pluronic F-68) 10-fold relative to stock concentration (~6.9 × 1014 gc/ml). These constructs were as in Chettih and Harvey, 2019. One of the latter two somatically restricted (Kv2.1) opsins was used for all experiments except those targeting the same neurons across days (Figure 2—figure supplement 3) where non-restricted opsin was used. Of the 22 mice initially trained on 1P stimulation, 4 were opsin-injected GCaMP6s transgenics and 18 were opsin/indicator-injected WT mice. Of these, 6 mice were used for 2P psychometric curve experiments, 4 of these were opsin-injected GCaMP6s transgenics, and 2 were opsin/indicator-injected WT mice (see Behavioural training below for details of mice used for each training phase). Mice were given a peri-operative subcutaneous injection of 0.3 mg/mL buprenorphine hydrochloride (Vetergesic). They were then anaesthetised with isoflurane (5% for induction, 1.5% for maintenance) and the scalp above the dorsal surface of the skull was removed. A metal headplate with a 7 mm diameter circular imaging well was fixed to the skull over right S1 (2 mm posterior and 3.5 mm lateral from bregma) using dental cement. A 3 mm craniotomy was drilled (NSK UK Ltd.) in the centre of the headplate well and the dura removed. Virus was then injected at a depth of 300 µm below the pia either as a single 750 nL injection at 200 nL/min or as ~5 injections of 150 nL virus at 50 nL/min spaced ~300 µm apart. The pipette was left in the brain for 2 min after each injection. Following the final retraction of the injection pipette, a two-tiered 4 mm/3 mm circle/circle chronic window (UQG Optics cover-glass bonded with UV optical cement, NOR-61, Norland Optical Adhesive) was press-fit into the craniotomy, sealed with cyanoacrylate (Vetbond) and fixed in place with dental cement. After surgery, animals were monitored and allowed to recover for at least 6 days during which they received water and food ad libitum.

Two-photon imaging

For most experiments (Figure 2, 3 and 4) two-photon imaging was performed using a resonant scanning (30 Hz) microscope (Ultima II, Bruker Corporation) driven by PrairieView and a Chameleon Ultra II laser (Coherent). For these experiments a 16x/0.8-NA water-immersion objective (Nikon) and an ETL (Optotune EL-10–30-TC, Gardosoft driver) were used to collect 100 µm z-depth imaging volumes (four planes, 33.3 µm spacing) with FOV sizes ranging from 600 × 600 µm to 850 × 850 µm (due to ETL magnification changes) at a constant image size of 512 × 512 pixels and plane-rate of ~7 Hz. The number of neurons recorded in each 2P psychometric curve experiment was 2809 ± 704, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each. For these experiments, we used an orbital nosepiece that allows pitch, roll and yaw to vary in order to get the light-path through the objective orthogonal to the plane of the cranial window to ensure optimal imaging conditions (via the following procedure: https://github.com/llerussell/MONPangle; Russell, 2020a). Due to inconsistencies in the window position across days and inaccuracies in the orbital nosepiece positioning and readout, pitch, roll, and yaw can vary significantly across days making it difficult to image the same planes, and thus same neurons, in a volume. We therefore chose to reacquire new volumes each day and target new sets of neurons. For some experiments (some in Figure 1, some figure supplements), two-photon imaging was performed using a resonant scanning (30 Hz) microscope (custom-build, Bruker Corporation) driven by PrairieView and a Chameleon Ultra II laser (Coherent). For these experiments a 25x/0.95-NA water-immersion objective (Leica) and an ETL (Optotune EL-10–30-TC, Optotune driver) were used to collect either 490 × 490 × 100 µm imaging volumes (four planes with ~33 µm spacing) at an image size of 512 × 512 pixels and plane-rate of ~7 Hz or single planes of FOV size 490 × 490 µm and image size 512 × 512 pixels at 30 Hz. GCaMP6s was imaged at 920 nm and mCherry was imaged at 765 nm. For functional volumetric GCaMP6s imaging at 920 nm, the maximum power on sample was 50 mW at the shallowest plane (~130 µm below pia), increasing linearly to 80 mW at the deepest plane (~230 µm) to maintain image quality across the volume (maximum 120 mins total duration per experiment). For single plane GCaMP6s imaging, a maximum power of 50 mW was used for all depths. Power on sample for mCherry/mRuby2/mScarlett (conjugated to C1V1), imaged at 765 nm, was 50–100 mW (maximum 1 min continuous duration).

Two-photon optogenetic stimulation

Two-photon photostimulation was carried out using a femto-second pulsed laser at 1030 nm (Satsuma, Amplitude Systèmes, 2 MHz rep-rate, 20 W). The single laser beam was split via a reflective spatial light modulator (SLM) (7.68 × 7.68 mm active area, 512 × 512 pixels, optimised for 1064 nm, OverDrive Plus SLM, Meadowlark Optics/Boulder Nonlinear Systems) which was installed in-line of the photostimulation path. Phase masks used to generate beamlet patterns at the focal plane were calculated from photostimulation target xy co-ordinates centred on cell-bodies of interest via the weighted Gerchberg-Saxton (GS) algorithm. These targets were weighted according to their location relative to the centre of the SLM’s addressable FOV to compensate for the decrease in diffraction efficiency when directing beamlets to peripheral positions. The transformation between SLM co-ordinates and imaging pixel co-ordinates was mapped by burning arbitrary spots in the FOV and calculating an affine transformation between SLM pixel targets and burn targets imaged in 2P. Calibration routines are available at https://github.com/llerussell/SLMTransformMaker3D (Russell, 2020b). Spiral patterns were generated by moving all beamlets simultaneously with a pair of galvanometer mirrors. Each spiral consisted of three rotations (i.e. from centre to edge of spiral), 10 μm diameter, 20 ms duration. Powers were adjusted to maintain 6 mW per target neuron. Due to constraints on the total power output of the photostimulation laser to stimulate 200 neurons, we randomly divided all targeted neurons into two groups of 100 which were stimulated as an alternating pair such that each group of 100 neurons was stimulated 10 times at 20 Hz (stimulate first group with 20 ms spiral, 5 ms inter-spiral interval, stimulate second group with 20 ms spiral, 5 ms inter-spiral interval, then return to first group and repeat a further nine times; each group therefore receives 10 × 20 ms spirals with an effective inter-spiral interval, for that group, of 25 ms; ~500 ms total stimulus duration). To stimulate 100 neurons or less all neurons were stimulated simultaneously 10 times at 40 Hz (10 × 20 ms spirals, 5 ms inter-spiral interval, ~250 ms stimulus duration). From our previous work, we expect a single spiral to produce ~1 action potential (Packer et al., 2015), thus all 2P stimuli should drive ~10 action potentials. Spiral timing and positioning protocols were generated by a custom MATLAB software suite called Naparm (see Online design and execution of photostimulation protocols section of Materials and methods) and executed by the photostimulation modules of the microscope software (PrairieView, Bruker Corporation), the MeadowLark SLM software and our synchronisation software (PackIO; see below).

One-photon optogenetic stimulation

For Phase 1 of behavioural training an amber LED (590 nm peak wavelength, ThorLabs M590D2) was fixed to a manipulatable arm (DTI clamp, RS components) and press-fit onto the chronic window surface. For subsequent training phases an amber LED (595 nm peak wavelength, ThorLabs M595L3) was mounted in the lightpath above the two-photon microscope objective. LED powers ranged from 0.02 mW to 10 mW. Power and timing of LED photostimulation was controlled by custom MATLAB software through National Instruments data acquisition cards (NI USB-6351, NI USB-6211). All LED stimuli consisted of 5 × 20 ms pulses at 25 Hz of varying powers ranging from 10 to 0.02 mW (measured with PM100A power meter/S130C photodiode sensor, ThorLabs). We did not record activity while animals were learning to detect 1P photostimulation. We thus roughly estimate the spatial extent, number and rate of spiking evoked by pulses of this power, duration, and frequency from the literature as follows. Blue light emitted from an LED shows a ~1 mm HWHM spatial spread in brain tissue and ChR2-based pyramidal neuron activation shows relatively little variation with depth (Huber et al., 2008), although this could be due to activation of superficial processes. C-fos expression also shows that recruitment by optic fibre stimulation of ChR2-expressing pyramidal neurons is limited to a ~0.7 mm3 volume around the fibre tip (Gradinaru et al., 2009). Blue and orange light photoactivation of PV interneurons with moderate powers (10 mW) show similar lateral recruitment (~0.5–0.8 mm HWHM) (Li et al., 2019), as does orange light Arch-mediate inhibition of pyramidal neurons (~0.5–0.8 mm HWHM) (Babl et al., 2019). However, high-power photobleaching indicates that orange light excitation has a two-fold larger lateral extent than blue light and extends across all cortical layers to form a roughly symmetric excitation volume (Li et al., 2019), as would be expected from the reduced scattering of red-shifted light in cortical tissue (Helmchen and Denk, 2005). Given the fact that we are using an LED instead of a fibre and we are using orange light, we suggest that our excitation volume has a roughly two-fold larger lateral extent than that measured for a blue LED (Huber et al., 2008) and for orange light from a fibre/laser (Babl et al., 2019; Li et al., 2019) and is roughly symmetric. With respect to stimulus frequency, C1V1 should be able to faithfully follow a 25 Hz pulse train with ~90% fidelity, although our stimulus pulses are longer than those used with C1V1 (Yizhar et al., 2011) or ChR2 (Huber et al., 2008). With respect to pulse duration, for ChR2 a doubling in pulse duration roughly corresponds to a doubling in spike probability (Histed and Maunsell, 2014) suggesting that our 10-fold increase in pulse duration relative to the literature (Yizhar et al., 2011) might correspond to a ten-fold increase in the number of evoked spikes. Although this might be an overestimate for C1V1 as it has slower channel kinetics than ChR2 (Yizhar et al., 2011), it is certainly likely we are driving multiple spikes with each pulse with an upper limit defined by C1V1’s inability to reliably follow trains of >50 Hz. These data, in combination with our observation that <25% of neurons express C1V1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 1e) and ~50% of C1V1-expressing neurons are photoactivatable, suggest that our initial training powers (10 mW) will drive >5 spikes at between 25 and 50 Hz in ~10% of pyramidal neurons in a 1.5–2 mm3 volume. The lowest powers at the behavioural threshold will likely recruit progressively fewer neurons over smaller cortical volumes with longer spike latencies (Huber et al., 2008), and these neurons will fire fewer spikes per pulse with lower reliability (Histed and Maunsell, 2014).

Synchronisation

For synchronisation of imaging frames, photostimulation spirals, sensory stimulation epochs and behavioural trial data during experiments, analogue triggers and waveforms were recorded with National Instruments DAQ cards controlled by PackIO (Watson et al., 2016). Behavioural trial timing and licking response contingency analyses were done online by an Arduino Mega microcontroller board controlled by PyBehaviour (see Behavioural training section of Materials and methods).

Online design and execution of photostimulation protocols

To quickly design photostimulation ensembles online during experiments, we used our custom control software, Naparm (https://github.com/llerussell/NaparmRussell and Dalgleish, 2020). Briefly, this software allows users to import images of neuronal populations of interest (i.e. C1V1 expression images) and semi-automatically detect the xyz centroids of neuron bodies. These potential target neurons can then be divided into different stimulation groups and the photostimulation protocol defined. Once defined this software saves out all files necessary to synchronise the microscope, lasers, SLM, and master clock software PackIO. For behavioural training experiments, we combined this with our custom two-photon behavioural training software (TPBS) which allows users to import a directory of phasemasks and associate one or more phasemasks with trial types that can be read into and executed by our behavioural control software PyBehaviour (see Behavioural training section of Materials and methods).

Behavioural training

Training began ~21 days post virus injection/window installation. Animals were water-restricted to 85–90% of their pre-training weight throughout the training period and their weight was monitored daily. The majority of each animal’s daily water was consumed during behavioural training, topped-up if necessary with additional water post-training. Early one-photon training sessions took place in closed, soundproofed and unlit behavioural training boxes. During training, animals were head-fixed via their headplates and housed in Perspex tubes. A metal reward delivery spout, connected to an electronic lickometer circuit, was positioned within easy reach of the mouse’s tongue to record licks and deliver sugar-water rewards (5 µL, 10% sucrose v/v). Mice were adapted to this procedure over a day or two during which rewards were randomly delivered manually. All subsequent behavioural training was controlled by Arduino-based behavioural control software written in Python (PyBehaviour: https://github.com/llerussell/PyBehaviour; Russell, 2020c). This software acted as the master clock, dictating the sequence and timing of behavioural trials, and recording and scoring licking behaviour. The power and temporal characteristics of LED photostimulation patterns were controlled by custom-written MATLAB software and National Instruments hardware (see ‘One-photon optogenetic stimulation’ section). PyBehaviour controlled the output of this LED software via TTL pulses. At the beginning of each behavioural session, the LED, mounted on a manipulatable arm (DTI clamp, RS components), was press-fit onto the surface of the chronic window. This region was then sealed with tape to minimise direct visual stimulation. Throughout all training sessions and phases, the basic task structure was the same. Trials were triggered after animals withheld licking for 7 ± 3 s. Each trial consisted of a response window during which licking behaviour was scored, followed by a 5 s post-stimulus period. The response window lasted for 2 s in training Phase 1 and 1 s in training Phase 2 and 3, although for all behavioural analyses a 1 s window beginning at 0.15 s (0.15–1.15 s) post stimulus was used to exclude unrealistically quick reaction times and ensure consistency of metrics across analyses of different phases (this window is indicated on all behavioural plots). Sessions consisted of 100–400 trials of two types: go trials and catch trials. During go trials, some form of optogenetic stimulus was delivered, and animals were required to lick. Licking was scored as a hit and was rewarded by delivery of a sugar water reward (see above), non-licking was scored as a miss and was unpunished. During catch trials, no stimulus was delivered and animals were required not to lick. Licking was scored as a false alarm, and was unpunished, non-licking was scored as a correct reject and was unrewarded. Neither of these two trial types were cued. Stimulus trials therefore test an animal’s detection rate on particular stimuli and catch trials assess an animal’s chance response rate. All trial types were pseudorandomly interleaved with a three trial upper limit on consecutive trials of the same type. During the first few sessions, go trials were auto-rewarded 500 ms following the stimulus to encourage learning. As soon as animals reliably began licking in anticipation of the auto-reward, it was turned off for all subsequent sessions. Animals were initially trained on Phase 1 to detect 10 mW. Performance was then manually assessed and the LED power was dropped by half multiple times within a session until animals could detect very low powers (0.05 mW). This usually took ~4 days. Animals then underwent psychometric curve sessions to assess their performance on the lowest LED powers (0.1, 0.08, 0.06, 0.04, 0.02 mW). Animals that could detect 0.1 mW stimuli with d-prime >1 were eligible for transitioning to subsequent experiments (all animals tested achieved this criterion); however, only the subset with clearest expression were transitioned to two-photon training phases to allow for sufficient training time on the all-optical system. These animals were transitioned to Phases 2 and 3 where two-photon stimuli were introduced. For these phases, animals were head-fixed under the microscope on a cylindrical treadmill and the LED was directed through the light-path. Note we expect that animals could be trained from the offset under the microscope using the LED through the objective; however, we opted against this due to time constraints on the all-optical system. An objective well/baffle was used to minimise light leakage. A white-noise mask was played continuously throughout all subsequent training sessions to mask any auditory cues emitted by the galvos during photostimulation. Following selection of neurons for 2P photostimulation (see below) animals began training on Phase 2 where 1P and 2P optogenetic stimuli were interleaved along with catch trials in equal proportions. Initial 2P stimuli targeted 200 neurons and animals required ~1 day to reach good performance. Once animals could reliably detect 200 neurons, we interleaved 2P stimuli targeting a 100 neuron subset of the original 200 neurons. Once animals could reliably detect 100 neurons they were transitioned to Phase 3 where only 2P optogenetic stimulation of those 100 neurons and catch trials were delivered in equal proportions. On some trials in a few Phase 2/3 sessions, auto-rewards were delivered at 0.5, 1 or 1.5 s following the stimulus (depending on the animal’s reaction time) to encourage transition to detecting two-photon stimuli. These trials were scored as hits if the animal licked before the auto-reward, otherwise they were conservatively scored as misses (even if they fell within the response window) (see Behavioural data analysis below). For the above phases, we used the same FOV across days but did not specifically target the same neurons (see Two-photon imaging section of Materials and methods) except in the ‘Same’ condition in Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Following this, a subset of animals were transitioned to 2P psychometric curve sessions with the remaining subset used for other experiments not reported here (for details, see: https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/10095170/). During two-photon psychometric curve sessions trial types were pseudorandomly interleaved to ensure an even distribution of trial types across the behavioural session. Trial type ratios were as follows: 15% catch trials, 15% ‘easy’ 200 neuron trials, 70% trials stimulating smaller numbers of neurons (~12% each for 100, 75, 50, 25, 10, and 5 neurons). All psychometric curve sessions had an initial 10-trial buffer of ‘easy’ 200 neurons trials to allow the animal to warm up. Multiple FOVs were tested in each animal, but each FOV was tested only once. Mice were eligible for transfer from Phase 2 to Phase 3 and from Phase 3 to psychometric curve sessions once they had achieved d-prime >1 for detecting 2P stimulation of 200 neurons. This did not always happen immediately due to time constraints on the all-optical system. If transition was delayed, in the intervening days mice were often trained to keep them familiar with the task. Summary of N for behavioural training: 26 mice did 1P training and of these 18 mice did both high-power and low-power 1P psychometric curves and 4 did just high power and 4 did just low power (these tended to be the best mice which were rushed to later training phases) (Figure 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 3). From these, 12 mice with the clearest expression were transitioned onto 2P training (Figure 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 5) and from these 6 mice were used for 2P psychometric curve sessions (Figures 24) and the remaining 6 were used for other 2P experiments not reported here.

Image processing

All offline data were analysed using custom software and toolboxes in MATLAB. PackIO data acquired during experiments were used to synchronise imaging frames with photostimulation and behavioural epochs. To avoid the large imaging artefacts caused by 2P photostimulation, we implemented a ‘stimulus artefact exclusion epoch’ whereby all imaging frames acquired during photostimulation periods were excluded from all processing, analyses and plotting. This is described below. Since it takes 500 ms to stimulate 200 neurons (see Two-photon optogenetic stimulation above), and since we wanted the amount of data (volume scans) excluded to be the same for all trial types (to facilitate comparison across them), we need to exclude at least 0–500 ms of imaging data post-stimulus onset on all trials (even though stimuli targeting ≤100 neurons take ~250 ms). In reality, we actually have to remove a slightly longer period of imaging data (five full volumes peri-stimulus; ~750 ms of imaging data) due to how the photostimulus epoch overlaps with the time-course of imaging volume acquisition. We explain this below. Photostimulation onsets are not synchronised to begin at the same time as the first plane in a given volume (i.e. they can occur during any plane within a volume). Our volumes contain four planes and if any plane is corrupted by the stimulus artifact then that volume must be discarded. Our imaging frame (plane) rate is 26.8 Hz and, since we acquire four planes per volume, our volume rate is 6.69 Hz. This means it takes ~150 ms to acquire the four planes constituting one volume. Thus, given that the maximum photostimulus duration is ~500 ms (see above), even if photostimulus onset was synchronised to always begin with the first plane of a given volume, the minimum number of volumes we could discard post-stimulus onset would be 4 (3 * 150 = 450, 4 * 150 ms = 600 ms). However, since the photostimulus onset can occur at any time during a volume acquisition, it can also occur during the last plane of the volume immediately preceding the stimulus onset. This volume must also be discarded in addition to the subsequent four volumes which must also be discarded as they will also all contain at least one frame corrupted by the photostimulus. This means that on some trials five volumes will need to be discarded (~750 ms of imaging data). Since we always want to discard the same amount of imaging data across trials to facilitate comparison between them, we have to exclude this maximum value of 5 volumes (~750 ms) peri-stimulus on all trials. Since most reaction times occur before this (560 ± 150 ms reaction time for 2P 200 neuron trials), we are unable to analyse data in the absence of response-related activity (licking, whisking, facial movements). Thus, we had to control for this in our subsequent analyses (see Neurometric curve analyses section of Materials and methods). Imaging time-series were registered and segmented into ROIs using the Python version of Suite2P (Pachitariu et al., 2016). ROIs were manually curated. Neuropil subtracted neuron traces were calculated as:

Fcell=FROI-c*Fneuropil

where the neuropil subtraction coefficient c was estimated separately for each ROI by robust regression between FROI and Fneuropil (Chen et al., 2013b). For this estimation process, all photostimulation epochs were excluded and FROI and Fneuropil were downsampled by a factor of 10 (to 0.7 Hz). Coefficients were post-hoc bounded between 0.5 and 1 and any coefficients that could not be reliably estimated were set to the median of all reliably estimated coefficients in that dataset (usually ~0.7). Once Fneuropil had been subtracted from FROI, Fcell was then re-baselined to the 33rd percentile of FROI values to ensure accuracy in subsequent F/F0 and F/σF calculations. Neuropil subtraction had a small but significant effect on trial-wise response amplitude; however, we observed a large fraction of negative responses even without neuropil subtraction and response classifications (activated, suppressed and no response) were largely consistent between raw and neuropil subtracted trial-wise responses (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). To detect neurons expressing C1V1 offline post experiment (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1), we used the Cellpose algorithm followed by manual curation (Stringer et al., 2020) using the cytoplasm model with diameter = 15 µm. To find Suite2P ROIs that also expressed C1V1, for each Suite2P ROI we copied its spatial footprint to the centroid location of the nearest C1V1 ROI and calculated the overlap between this copied and offset ROI and the original Suite2P ROI. If >50% of pixels overlapped then the Suite2P ROI was considered C1V1+.

Neuronal response analysis

For trial-wise analyses, Fcell traces were divided into epochs triggered on photostimulation onset for stimulus trials and catch trial response window onset for catch trials (stimulus-triggered averages – STAs). All STAs had a 1 s baseline period. STAs for each trial were converted to F/σF by subtracting the baseline mean from the STA trace (F) and dividing the result by the baseline standard deviation (σF). Responses on both photostimulation and catch trials were quantified as the average response ~0.7 – 1 s following photostimulus onset so as to avoid photostimulation artefacts (see Image processing section above for full description of the stimulus artefact exclusion epoch). For our analyses, we wanted to be able to assess neural responses on single trials as our procedure for mitigating potential lick response artefacts requires trial-wise analyses. Moreover, we cannot assume that the same background neurons will be recruited by photostimulation of a given set of target neurons on each trial due to the high variability and low probability of synaptic transmission (London et al., 2010). This therefore precludes the use of a standard statistical test comparing a neuron’s stimulus trial response distribution to its catch trial response distribution to assess responsivity. We therefore sought to define activation and suppression thresholds that we could apply to responses on individual trials. We used each neuron’s response distribution on correct reject (CR) catch trials, when no stimulus or lick response occurred, to define its separate activation and suppression thresholds (since activation and suppression are readout differently by calcium indicators; see Otis et al., 2017; Figure 2—figure supplement 1b–g). For each neuron, we defined its activation threshold as:

Thresholdactivation=Meancatch+S.D.catch  Scaling Factoractivation

and its suppression threshold as:

Thresholdsuppression=MeancatchS.D.catch  Scaling Factorsuppression

where Meancatch and S.D.catch are the mean and standard deviation of the distribution of CR catch trial responses respectively (Figure 2—figure supplement 1b). We estimated the activation and suppression scaling factors separately for each session (i.e. each session will have a single activation and single suppression scaling factor applied to all neurons), and separately from each other, using a cross-validated empirical procedure where the objective was to ensure that only 5% of neurons were activated and 5% of neurons were suppressed on catch trials, effectively fixing the false positive (FP) rate at 5% for each session (Figure 2—figure supplement 1c). For data recorded on each session and for each response type (i.e. in the following example for activation), we swept through a series of potential activation scaling factors (range 1 – 3 in increments of 0.1). At each scaling factor, we ran 10,000 permutations of an 80:20 train:test split over CR catch trials. On each permutation we used the training CR catch trials to define each neuron’s activation threshold (using the equation above with the current scaling factor) and then used these thresholds to calculate the proportion of neurons crossing their threshold on each testing CR catch trial and averaged this across trials to get the average proportion activated across testing trials. We then took the median proportion activated across all permuted train:test splits. We plot this median value for all scaling factors resulting in a distinct curve for each session (Figure 2—figure supplement 1d,e data points). We fit each session’s curve with cubic interpolation (Figure 2—figure supplement 1d,e curves) and then used this to infer the s.d. scaling factor that yields a 5% FP rate across permuted train:test splits for each session and for each response type (Figure 2—figure supplement 1d,e box plots). These scaling factors can then be used in conjunction with the full CR catch response distribution as described in the equations above to define an activation threshold and a suppression threshold for each neuron in each session. For subsequent analyses, to quantify the number of target neurons activated we simply calculated the number of neurons in target zones that passed their activation threshold on each trial and averaged this across trials of the same type. To quantify P(Activated) and P(Suppressed) in all neurons (Figure 3a–c), we divided the total number of activated or suppressed neurons on each trial, irrespective of whether they were in target zones or not, by the total number of neurons (targets and background) and averaged across trials of the same type. To quantify P(Activated) and P(Suppressed) in background neurons (Figure 3d,e), we divided the total number of activated or suppressed background neurons on each trial (target neurons excluded) and divided this by the total number of neurons (targets and background) and averaged across trials of the same type. Note that all neural response data in Figure 3 and Figure 4 are hit:miss matched (see below Hit:miss matching).

Classifying target neurons and background neurons

Since the spatial resolution of two-photon photostimulation is not perfect, we defined conservative 3D photostimulation zones around each targeted location in the imaging volume. These had a 10 µm radius, which is ~2 x the lateral resolution of our 2P photostimulation (Figure 1—figure supplement 2a), and extended through the entire axial range of the volume (99 µm) (Figure 2—figure supplement 1a). Thus, we considered all ROIs with a centroid ≤10 µm from any target location to be a potential target neuron, irrespective of their axial displacement relative to the target location. ROIs outside these zones were considered background neurons. Since these exclusion zones are conservative, many neurons fell within them (Figure 2—figure supplement 1h), although only a fraction were responsive to photostimulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 1i,j 0.18 ± 0.1 fraction activated across neurons within target zones, averaged across trial types, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each). Nevertheless, this fraction was far above the fraction responsive on catch trials (Figure 2—figure supplement 1j stimulus trials: 0.18 ± 0.1 vs catch trials: 0.04 ± 0.01 averaged across trial types p=4.92 × 10−4 paired t-test, N = 11 sessions, 6 mice, 1–2 sessions each) and resulted in numbers of activated target neurons that were 0.46 ± 0.20 times that of the number of target zones (averaged across trial types) and decreased with decreasing number of zones as intended (Figure 2—figure supplement 1i).

Hit:miss matching

We are interested in how the number of target neurons activated modulates responses in the local network (Figure 3, 4). The number of target neurons activated increases the probability of licking (Figure 2). Licking itself activates responses in the background network irrespective of photostimulation of target neurons (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Therefore, we sought to minimise the chance that any modulation in background network activity we observe in response to changes in the number of target neurons activated is due to increased recruitment of licking, and lick-evoked activity, by more salient target stimulation. Thus we fixed a 50:50 ratio of hits:misses (lick:no-lick trials) on every trial type (number of target zones) (Figure 3—figure supplement 1o). In this way, we can ensure that the proportion of lick corruption in hard to detect stimuli (5 target zones), which mainly result in misses, is similar to that of easy to detect stimuli (100 target zones), which mainly result in hits. Below we describe this procedure using the least salient trial type (5 target zones) of a given session as an example. First, we count the number of trials of this trial type with the minority response type which, in this case, is hits since animals rarely detect 5 target site stimulations. This number is logged as the number of trials to match with trials of the majority response type which, in this case, is misses (as animals often fail to detect 5 target zone stimulations). We then run 100 permutations where we take all the minority trial type trials (hits) and random resamples, of the same number, of the majority trial type trials (misses). For example if there were 2 hits and 15 misses overall then on each permutation we would take the same 2 hit trials and a random 2 miss trials (sampled from the 15 misses) and calculate our neural metrics across these trials. We then average these metrics across all permutations to get final values reported in network response figures. This procedure was not possible for a few trial types as they only had trials of one response type (i.e. only hits or only misses). These trial types were excluded from analysis.

Lick analysis

To correlate spontaneous fluorescence traces with spontaneous licking, we removed photostimulation trial periods (0–4 s post-stimulus) and analysed the remaining periods of lick and fluorescence traces spanning the time between the first and last spontaneous lick. We smoothed the lick trace with a Gaussian kernel (sigma = 0.5 s) and calculated the Pearson’s correlation coefficient (and associated p-values) between the smoothed lick trace and each neuron’s fluorescence trace. To detect spontaneous lick bouts for lick-triggered STA analysis, we excluded all trial periods (0–4 s post-stimulus onset) and found groups of at least three licks that occurred with a minimum 0.5 s inter-lick interval that were separated from preceding licks by at least 1 s.

Behavioural data analysis

All behavioural trials with ≤0.15 s reaction time were excluded from analysis. Response windows for analysis of all phases extended for 1 s following this period (i.e. 0.15–1.15 s post-stimulus onset). This is indicated in all raster plots. Behavioural trials during imaging experiments which occurred so close to the beginning/end of imaging epochs that the requisite analysis windows were truncated were excluded from analyses. For calculations of reaction time standard deviation we only included sessions where all trial types had responses on >2 trials. For sessions including some auto-rewarded trials (early Phase 1 and Phase 2 sessions), we scored auto-rewarded trials with response times < tauto-reward + 0.15 s as hits and scored auto-rewarded trials with responses after this time, or no response at all, as misses. The basic behavioural response metric used throughout was the P(response) for each stimulation type, calculated as:

P(response)= nlicknlick+nno lick

where nlick and nnolick are the number of trials where the animal licked and did not lick, respectively. For some analyses the catch trial P(response) was subtracted from the P(response) on go trials. This has been indicated where relevant as “catch subtracted”. For some behavioural analyses where learning relative to our behavioural response criterion (d-prime >1) was relevant we report performance in terms of d-prime which we computed as:

norminv(Hit rate)norminv(False alarm rate)

where norminv is the inverse of the normal cumulative distribution function. We corrected for hit rates/false alarm rates of 1 and 0 according to Macmillan and Kaplan, 1985.

Psychometric curve fitting

We used the MATLAB psignifit toolbox (Schütt et al., 2016a; Schütt et al., 2016b; https://github.com/wichmann-lab/psignifit) to fit log-normal beta-binomial psychometric curves of the following form:

ψ(x;m,w,λ,γ)=γ+(1λγ) S(x;m,w)
Sx;m,w=ΦClogx-mw
C=Φ1(0.95)Φ1(0.05)

For which we fit the width (w) and threshold (m) and fixed the lapse rate (λ) and guess rate (γ). log is the natural logarithm. Φ and Φ1 are the cumulative standard normal distribution and its inverse. The lapse rate was fixed as 1 – max(P(Lick)Stim) where max(P(Lick)Stim) is the maximum response rate to any stimulus trial type (this was always either 100 or 200 target zone stimulation trials). The guess rate was fixed as the catch trial response rate. Only go (stimulus) trials were used for fitting. For fits aggregating across sessions (Figure 2d red curve), the lapse rate and guess rate were fixed using values averaged across sessions. For fits during train-test permutations (Figure 4d), the lapse rate and guess rate were fixed at values computed from all data and fixed for all permutations, whereas the width and threshold were re-fit for every permutation.

Data exclusion criteria

Some animals were removed from analysis of reaction time variability in Phase 1 behavioural sessions as they did not show enough responses in catch trials to compute the mean and standard deviation of reaction time (Figure 1f, g). Some trial types for some sessions were excluded from Figure 3 and 4 as the procedure required to match the number of hit and miss trials resulted in no trials (see Hit:miss matching section of the Materials and methods). Some trial types were removed from network response analyses (Figure 3) as the number of targets activated was <1 on average. No data were removed as outliers. Trials with reaction times ≤0.15 s were excluded from all figures (except for illustrative purposes in Figure 1—figure supplement 3b) and analyses as this was deemed to be too quick to be a reaction to the stimulus. In the latter half of each training session if the response probability on the easiest trial type for that session fell below 0.7 (calculated in a 10-trial sliding window centred on each trial) the animal was deemed to be ‘sated’ and trials beyond this point were excluded from all analyses.

Statistical procedures

No statistical procedures were used to estimate sample sizes. Appropriate sample sizes were estimated from previous experiments of a similar type. Experiments were not randomised and experimenters were not blinded with respect to experiment and outcome. All data were analysed with custom routines and toolboxes in MATLAB except for psychometric curve fitting which was done using the MATLAB psignifit toolbox (see Psychometric curve fitting section of Materials and methods). All error bars are given as mean ± s.e.m and all values in the text are mean ± s.d. unless otherwise stated. All boxplots show median and 25th/75th percentile boxes with whiskers extending to the most extreme data not considered outliers (outliers defined as data > q3+1.5×q3-q1 or < q3-1.5×q3-q1 where q3 and q1 are the 75th and 25th percentiles, respectively). Regression coefficients and psychometric function parameters are reported ± 95% confidence intervals where appropriate. Datasets of N > 8 were tested for normality with D'Agostino-Pearson's K2 test and analysed according to the result. Datasets of N ≤ 8 were analysed as non-parametric. Multiple comparisons were corrected using the Bonferroni correction. All tests were two-tailed. Relevant tests are reported in the text.

Data availability

Import, processing, analysis and figure code is available on Github (Dalgleish, 2020; https://github.com/alloptical/Dalgleish-eLife-2020) for use with analysed data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13135505) and/or unprocessed behavioural session data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13128950). Raw calcium imaging movies are ~1 TB in size and are thus available upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

We thank Mehmet Fişek for useful discussions about experiments and analysis; Selmaan Chettih and Christopher Harvey for developing and sharing the somatically-restricted C1V1 opsin; Soyon Chun and Agnieszka Jucht for mouse breeding; Sarolta Gabulya and Carmen Fernández Fisac for behavioural training during pilot experiments; and Bruker Corporation for technical support. This work was supported by grants from the Wellcome Trust, Gatsby Charitable Foundation, ERC, MRC and the BBSRC.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Michael Häusser, Email: m.hausser@ucl.ac.uk.

Brice Bathellier, CNRS, France.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • European Research Council 695709 to Michael Häusser.

  • Wellcome Trust 201225 to Michael Häusser.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council to Michael Häusser.

  • Medical Research Council to Michael Häusser.

  • Gatsby Charitable Foundation to Michael Häusser.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, HWPD built the behavioural setup, performed surgeries, trained animals, designed and wrote experimental and analysis software, carried out experiments, and analysed data, HWPD, LR, AMP and MH designed the study, HWPD and MH wrote the manuscript with input from LR, AMP and AR.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing, LR performed surgeries, carried out experiments, designed/wrote experimental software, built the microscope and designed and built the behavioural setup, HWPD, LR, AMP and MH designed the study.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Methodology, Writing - review and editing, AMP designed and built the holographic photostimulation module of the microscope, was crucial in preliminary experiments to optimize the apparatus and behaviour, HWPD, LR, AMP and MH designed the study, MH and AMP supervised the study.

Conceptualization, Writing - review and editing, AR provided pivotal suggestions for analyses.

Resources, Software, Writing - review and editing, OMG performed surgeries.

Investigation, Writing - review and editing, FG and EJT helped design the 1P training paradigm and trained animals on this phase of the experiment.

Investigation, Writing - review and editing, FG and EJT helped design the 1P training paradigm and trained animals on this phase of the experiment.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing, HWPD, LR, AMP and MH designed the study, MH and AMP supervised the study, HWPD and MH wrote the manuscript with input from LR, AMP and AR.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experimental procedures were carried out under Project Licence 70/14018 (PCC4A4ECE) issued by the UK Home Office in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act (1986) and were also subject to local ethical review. All surgical procedures were carried out under isoflurane anaesthesia (5% for induction, 1.5% for maintenance), and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

Import, processing, analysis and figure code is available on Github (Dalgleish, 2020; https://github.com/alloptical/Dalgleish-eLife-2020) (copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:5768e890be9fc6428beea16720e488ebd867da67/) for use with analysed data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13135505) and/or unprocessed behavioural session data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13128950). Raw calcium imaging movies are ~1TB in size and are thus available upon reasonable request.

The following datasets were generated:

Dalgleish HWP, Russell LE, Gauld OM, Packer AM, Hausser M. 2020. How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity? figshare.

Dalgleish HWP, Russell LE, Gauld OM, Packer AM, Hausser M. 2020. How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity? figshare.

References

  1. Abraham NM, Spors H, Carleton A, Margrie TW, Kuner T, Schaefer AT. Maintaining accuracy at the expense of speed: stimulus similarity defines odor discrimination time in mice. Neuron. 2004;44:865–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alejandre-García T, Kim S, Pérez-Ortega J, Yuste R. Intrinsic excitability mechanisms of neuronal ensemble formation. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.07.29.223966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  3. Amit DJ, Gutfreund H, Sompolinsky H. Spin-glass models of neural networks. Physical Review A. 1985a;32:1007–1018. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevA.32.1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amit DJ, Gutfreund H, Sompolinsky H. Storing infinite numbers of patterns in a spin-glass model of neural networks. Physical Review Letters. 1985b;55:1530–1533. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.55.1530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andermann ML. Chronic cellular imaging of mouse visual cortex during operant behavior and passive viewing. Frontiers in Cellullar Neuroscience. 2010;4:3. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2010.00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Attwell D, Laughlin SB. An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2001;21:1133–1145. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200110000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Babl SS, Rummell BP, Sigurdsson T. The spatial extent of optogenetic silencing in transgenic mice expressing channelrhodopsin in inhibitory interneurons. Cell Reports. 2019;29:1381–1395. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.09.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barlow HB. Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? Perception. 1972;1:371–394. doi: 10.1068/p010371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barth AL, Poulet JF. Experimental evidence for sparse firing in the neocortex. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baum EB, Moody J, Wilczek F. Internal representations for associative memory. Biological Cybernetics. 1988;59:217–228. doi: 10.1007/BF00332910. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Walton ME, Rushworth MF. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1214–1221. doi: 10.1038/nn1954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Behrens TEJ, Muller TH, Whittington JCR, Mark S, Baram AB, Stachenfeld KL, Kurth-Nelson Z. What is a cognitive map? organizing knowledge for flexible behavior. Neuron. 2018;100:490–509. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bernander O, Douglas RJ, Martin KA, Koch C. Synaptic background activity influences spatiotemporal integration in single pyramidal cells. PNAS. 1991;88:11569–11573. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.24.11569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bernardi D, Doron G, Brecht M, Lindner B. A network model of the barrel cortex combined with a differentiator detector reproduces features of the behavioral response to single-neuron stimulation. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.03.30.016261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. Bernardi D, Lindner B. Optimal detection of a localized perturbation in random networks of Integrate-and-Fire neurons. Physical Review Letters. 2017;118:268301. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.118.268301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bernardi D, Lindner B. Detecting single-cell stimulation in a large network of integrate-and-fire neurons. Physical Review E. 2019;99:032304. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.99.032304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bialek W. Biophysics: Searching for Principles. Princeton University Press; 2012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bock DD, Lee WC, Kerlin AM, Andermann ML, Hood G, Wetzel AW, Yurgenson S, Soucy ER, Kim HS, Reid RC. Network anatomy and in vivo physiology of visual cortical neurons. Nature. 2011;471:177–182. doi: 10.1038/nature09802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bonifazi P, Goldin M, Picardo MA, Jorquera I, Cattani A, Bianconi G, Represa A, Ben-Ari Y, Cossart R. GABAergic hub neurons orchestrate synchrony in developing hippocampal networks. Science. 2009;326:1419–1424. doi: 10.1126/science.1175509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Borg-Graham LJ, Monier C, Frégnac Y. Visual input evokes transient and strong shunting inhibition in visual cortical neurons. Nature. 1998;393:369–373. doi: 10.1038/30735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Brecht M, Schneider M, Sakmann B, Margrie TW. Whisker movements evoked by stimulation of single pyramidal cells in rat motor cortex. Nature. 2004;427:704–710. doi: 10.1038/nature02266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Britten KH, Newsome WT, Shadlen MN, Celebrini S, Movshon JA. A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT. Visual Neuroscience. 1996;13:87–100. doi: 10.1017/S095252380000715X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Brunel N. Is cortical connectivity optimized for storing information? Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:749–755. doi: 10.1038/nn.4286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Busse L, Ayaz A, Dhruv NT, Katzner S, Saleem AB, Schölvinck ML, Zaharia AD, Carandini M. The detection of visual contrast in the behaving mouse. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:11351–11361. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6689-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cai B, Billeh YN, Chettih SN, Harvey CD, Koch C, Arkhipov A, Mihalas S. Modeling robust and efficient coding in the mouse primary visual cortex using computational perturbations. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.04.21.051268. [DOI]
  26. Carandini M, Churchland AK. Probing perceptual decisions in rodents. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:824–831. doi: 10.1038/nn.3410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Carbon CC. Understanding human perception by human-made illusions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:566. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cardin JA, Kumbhani RD, Contreras D, Palmer LA. Cellular mechanisms of temporal sensitivity in visual cortex neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:3652–3662. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5279-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Carrillo-Reid L, Yang W, Bando Y, Peterka DS, Yuste R. Imprinting and recalling cortical ensembles. Science. 2016;353:691–694. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Carrillo-Reid L, Han S, Yang W, Akrouh A, Yuste R. Controlling visually guided behavior by holographic recalling of cortical ensembles. Cell. 2019;178:447–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cassidy CM, Balsam PD, Weinstein JJ, Rosengard RJ, Slifstein M, Daw ND, Abi-Dargham A, Horga G. A perceptual inference mechanism for hallucinations linked to striatal dopamine. Current Biology. 2018;28:503–514. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ceballo S, Bourg J, Kempf A, Piwkowska Z, Daret A, Pinson P, Deneux T, Rumpel S, Bathellier B. Cortical recruitment determines learning dynamics and strategy. Nature Communications. 2019a;10:1479. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09450-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ceballo S, Piwkowska Z, Bourg J, Daret A, Bathellier B. Targeted cortical manipulation of auditory perception. Neuron. 2019b;104:1168–1179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Chaudhury S. Hallucinations: aspects and management. Industrial Psychiatry Journal. 2010;19:5–12. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.77625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Chen JL, Carta S, Soldado-Magraner J, Schneider BL, Helmchen F. Behaviour-dependent recruitment of long-range projection neurons in somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2013a;499:336–340. doi: 10.1038/nature12236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K, Kim DS. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013b;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Chen JL, Margolis DJ, Stankov A, Sumanovski LT, Schneider BL, Helmchen F. Pathway-specific reorganization of projection neurons in somatosensory cortex during learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1101–1108. doi: 10.1038/nn.4046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Chettih SN, Harvey CD. Single-neuron perturbations reveal feature-specific competition in V1. Nature. 2019;567:334–340. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-0997-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Cohen MR, Newsome WT. What electrical microstimulation has revealed about the neural basis of cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Constantinople CM, Bruno RM. Deep cortical layers are activated directly by thalamus. Science. 2013;340:1591–1594. doi: 10.1126/science.1236425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Corlett PR, Horga G, Fletcher PC, Alderson-Day B, Schmack K, Powers AR. Hallucinations and strong priors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2019;23:114–127. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Cossell L, Iacaruso MF, Muir DR, Houlton R, Sader EN, Ko H, Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Functional organization of excitatory synaptic strength in primary visual cortex. Nature. 2015;518:399–403. doi: 10.1038/nature14182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Crochet S, Poulet JF, Kremer Y, Petersen CC. Synaptic mechanisms underlying sparse coding of active touch. Neuron. 2011;69:1160–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Daie K, Svoboda K, Druckmann S. Targeted photostimulation uncovers circuit motifs supporting short-term memory. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/623785. [DOI] [PubMed]
  45. Dalgleish HWP. Dalgleish-eLife-2020. 27679d2Github. 2020 https://github.com/alloptical/Dalgleish-eLife-2020
  46. Denève S, Machens CK. Efficient codes and balanced networks. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:375–382. doi: 10.1038/nn.4243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Paré D. The high-conductance state of neocortical neurons in vivo. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Destexhe A, Paré D. Impact of network activity on the integrative properties of neocortical pyramidal neurons in vivo. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;81:1531–1547. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Doron G, von Heimendahl M, Schlattmann P, Houweling AR, Brecht M. Spiking irregularity and frequency modulate the behavioral report of single-neuron stimulation. Neuron. 2014;81:653–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Douglas RJ, Koch C, Mahowald M, Martin KA, Suarez HH. Recurrent excitation in neocortical circuits. Science. 1995;269:981–985. doi: 10.1126/science.7638624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Eichenbaum H, Cohen NJ. Can we reconcile the declarative memory and spatial navigation views on hippocampal function? Neuron. 2014;83:764–770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Emiliani V, Cohen AE, Deisseroth K, Häusser M. All-Optical interrogation of neural circuits. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:13917–13926. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2916-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Fahle M. Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2005;15:154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Feldmeyer D, Brecht M, Helmchen F, Petersen CC, Poulet JF, Staiger JF, Luhmann HJ, Schwarz C. Barrel cortex function. Progress in Neurobiology. 2013;103:3–27. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Fino E, Yuste R. Dense inhibitory connectivity in neocortex. Neuron. 2011;69:1188–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Folli V, Leonetti M, Ruocco G. On the maximum storage capacity of the HopfieldModel. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2016;10:144. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2016.00144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Friston KJ. Hallucinations and perceptual inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28:764–766. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05290131. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gerdjikov TV, Bergner CG, Stüttgen MC, Waiblinger C, Schwarz C. Discrimination of vibrotactile stimuli in the rat whisker system: behavior and neurometrics. Neuron. 2010;65:530–540. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gilbert CD, Sigman M, Crist RE. The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron. 2001;31:681–697. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00424-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gill JV, Lerman GM, Zhao H, Stetler BJ, Rinberg D, Shoham S. Precise holographic manipulation of olfactory circuits reveals coding features determining perceptual detection. Neuron. 2020;108:382–393. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.07.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gradinaru V, Mogri M, Thompson KR, Henderson JM, Deisseroth K. Optical deconstruction of parkinsonian neural circuitry. Science. 2009;324:354–359. doi: 10.1126/science.1167093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Gustafson NJ, Daw ND. Grid cells, place cells, and geodesic generalization for spatial reinforcement learning. PLOS Computational Biology. 2011;7:e1002235. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Haider B, Häusser M, Carandini M. Inhibition dominates sensory responses in the awake cortex. Nature. 2013;493:97–100. doi: 10.1038/nature11665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Haider B, McCormick DA. Rapid neocortical dynamics: cellular and network mechanisms. Neuron. 2009;62:171–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Harlow HF. The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review. 1949;56:51–65. doi: 10.1037/h0062474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Harris KD, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Cortical connectivity and sensory coding. Nature. 2013;503:51–58. doi: 10.1038/nature12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Harris KD, Thiele A. Cortical state and attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:509–523. doi: 10.1038/nrn3084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Helmchen F, Denk W. Deep tissue two-photon microscopy. Nature Methods. 2005;2:932–940. doi: 10.1038/nmeth818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Herculano-Houzel S, Mota B, Lent R. Cellular scaling rules for rodent brains. PNAS. 2006;103:12138–12143. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604911103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Herculano-Houzel S, Collins CE, Wong P, Kaas JH. Cellular scaling rules for primate brains. PNAS. 2007;104:3562–3567. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611396104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Herculano-Houzel S. The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2009;3:31. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.031.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hires SA, Gutnisky DA, Yu J, O'Connor DH, Svoboda K. Low-noise encoding of active touch by layer 4 in the somatosensory cortex. eLife. 2015;4:e06619. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Histed MH, Carvalho LA, Maunsell JH. Psychophysical measurement of contrast sensitivity in the behaving mouse. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;107:758–765. doi: 10.1152/jn.00609.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Histed MH, Maunsell JH. Cortical neural populations can guide behavior by integrating inputs linearly, independent of synchrony. PNAS. 2014;111:E178–E187. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318750111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hofer SB, Ko H, Pichler B, Vogelstein J, Ros H, Zeng H, Lein E, Lesica NA, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Differential connectivity and response dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1045–1052. doi: 10.1038/nn.2876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Holmgren C, Harkany T, Svennenfors B, Zilberter Y. Pyramidal cell communication within local networks in layer 2/3 of rat neocortex. The Journal of Physiology. 2003;551:139–153. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Hong YK, Lacefield CO, Rodgers CC, Bruno RM. Sensation, movement and learning in the absence of barrel cortex. Nature. 2018;561:542–546. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0527-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Hooks BM, Hires SA, Zhang YX, Huber D, Petreanu L, Svoboda K, Shepherd GM. Laminar analysis of excitatory local circuits in vibrissal motor and sensory cortical Areas. PLOS Biology. 2011;9:e1000572. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hopfield JJ. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. PNAS. 1982;79:2554–2558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Houweling AR, Brecht M. Behavioural report of single neuron stimulation in somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2008;451:65–68. doi: 10.1038/nature06447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Huber D, Petreanu L, Ghitani N, Ranade S, Hromádka T, Mainen Z, Svoboda K. Sparse optical microstimulation in barrel cortex drives learned behaviour in freely moving mice. Nature. 2008;451:61–64. doi: 10.1038/nature06445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Huber D, Gutnisky DA, Peron S, OConnor DH, Wiegert JS, Tian L, Oertner TG, Looger LL, Svoboda K. Multiple dynamic representations in the motor cortex during sensorimotor learning. Nature. 2012;484:473–478. doi: 10.1038/nature11039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Isaacson JS, Scanziani M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron. 2011;72:231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Jazayeri M, Afraz A. Navigating the neural space in search of the neural code. Neuron. 2017;93:1003–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Jennings JH, Kim CK, Marshel JH, Raffiee M, Ye L, Quirin S, Pak S, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Interacting neural ensembles in orbitofrontal cortex for social and feeding behaviour. Nature. 2019;565:645–649. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0866-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Jouhanneau JS, Kremkow J, Poulet JFA. Single synaptic inputs drive high-precision action potentials in Parvalbumin expressing GABA-ergic cortical neurons in vivo. Nature Communications. 2018;9:1540. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03995-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kanerva P. Sparse Distributed Memory and Related Models. In: Hassoun M. H, editor. Associative Neural Memories: Theory and Implementation. Oxford University Press Inc; 1993. pp. 50–76. [Google Scholar]
  88. Kawai R, Markman T, Poddar R, Ko R, Fantana AL, Dhawale AK, Kampff AR, Ölveczky BP. Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron. 2015;86:800–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Kerlin AM, Andermann ML, Berezovskii VK, Reid RC. Broadly tuned response properties of diverse inhibitory neuron subtypes in mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2010;67:858–871. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Khan AG, Poort J, Chadwick A, Blot A, Sahani M, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB. Distinct learning-induced changes in stimulus selectivity and interactions of GABAergic interneuron classes in visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:851–859. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0143-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Ko H, Hofer SB, Pichler B, Buchanan KA, Sjöström PJ, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Functional specificity of local synaptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature. 2011;473:87–91. doi: 10.1038/nature09880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Ko H, Cossell L, Baragli C, Antolik J, Clopath C, Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD. The emergence of functional microcircuits in visual cortex. Nature. 2013;496:96–100. doi: 10.1038/nature12015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Kumar S, Soren S, Chaudhury S. Hallucinations: and clinical implications. Industrial Psychiatry Journal. 2009;18:119–126. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.62273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Kwan AC, Dan Y. Dissection of cortical microcircuits by single-neuron stimulation in vivo. Current Biology. 2012;22:1459–1467. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Kwon SE, Yang H, Minamisawa G, O'Connor DH. Sensory and decision-related activity propagate in a cortical feedback loop during touch perception. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:1243–1249. doi: 10.1038/nn.4356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Law C-T, Gold JI. Neural correlates of perceptual learning in a sensory-motor, but not a sensory, cortical area. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:505–513. doi: 10.1038/nn2070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Lee SH, Kwan AC, Zhang S, Phoumthipphavong V, Flannery JG, Masmanidis SC, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Zhang F, Boyden ES, Deisseroth K, Dan Y. Activation of specific interneurons improves V1 feature selectivity and visual perception. Nature. 2012;488:379–383. doi: 10.1038/nature11312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Lefort S, Tomm C, Floyd Sarria JC, Petersen CC. The excitatory neuronal network of the C2 barrel column in mouse primary somatosensory cortex. Neuron. 2009;61:301–316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Lennie P. The cost of cortical computation. Current Biology. 2003;13:493–497. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00135-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Li CY, Poo MM, Dan Y. Burst spiking of a single cortical neuron modifies global brain state. Science. 2009;324:643–646. doi: 10.1126/science.1169957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Li N, Chen S, Guo ZV, Chen H, Huo Y, Inagaki HK, Chen G, Davis C, Hansel D, Guo C, Svoboda K. Spatiotemporal constraints on optogenetic inactivation in cortical circuits. eLife. 2019;8:e48622. doi: 10.7554/eLife.48622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Llorca PM, Pereira B, Jardri R, Chereau-Boudet I, Brousse G, Misdrahi D, Fénelon G, Tronche A-M, Schwan R, Lançon C, Marques A, Ulla M, Derost P, Debilly B, Durif F, de Chazeron I. Hallucinations in schizophrenia and Parkinson’s disease: an analysis of sensory modalities involved and the repercussion on patients. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:31852. doi: 10.1038/srep38152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. London M, Roth A, Beeren L, Häusser M, Latham PE. Sensitivity to perturbations in vivo implies high noise and suggests rate coding in cortex. Nature. 2010;466:123–127. doi: 10.1038/nature09086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Macmillan NA, Kaplan HL. Detection theory analysis of group data: sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. Psychological Bulletin. 1985;98:185–199. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.1.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Magee JC. Dendritic integration of excitatory synaptic input. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2000;1:181–190. doi: 10.1038/35044552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Major G, Larkum ME, Schiller J. Active properties of neocortical pyramidal neuron dendrites. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2013;36:1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Mardinly AR, Oldenburg IA, Pégard NC, Sridharan S, Lyall EH, Chesnov K, Brohawn SG, Waller L, Adesnik H. Precise multimodal optical control of neural ensemble activity. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:881–893. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0139-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Marshel JH, Kim YS, Machado TA, Quirin S, Benson B, Kadmon J, Raja C, Chibukhchyan A, Ramakrishnan C, Inoue M, Shane JC, McKnight DJ, Yoshizawa S, Kato HE, Ganguli S, Deisseroth K. Cortical layerspecific critical dynamics triggering perception. Science. 2019;365:eaaw5202. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw5202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Massi B, Donahue CH, Lee D. Volatility facilitates value updating in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2018;99:598–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.06.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Mateo C, Avermann M, Gentet LJ, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Petersen CC. In vivo optogenetic stimulation of neocortical excitatory neurons drives brain-state-dependent inhibition. Current Biology. 2011;21:1593–1602. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Mayrhofer JM, Haiss F, Helmchen F, Weber B. Sparse, reliable, and long-term stable representation of periodic whisker deflections in the mouse barrel cortex. NeuroImage. 2015;115:52–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. McEliece R, Posner E, Rodemich E, Venkatesh S. The capacity of the associative memory. IEEE Transactions on Information Theory. 1987;33:461–482. doi: 10.1109/TIT.1987.1057328. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  113. McGuire JT, Nassar MR, Gold JI, Kable JW. Functionally dissociable influences on learning rate in a dynamic environment. Neuron. 2014;84:870–881. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Meyer HS, Egger R, Guest JM, Foerster R, Reissl S, Oberlaender M. Cellular organization of cortical barrel columns is whisker-specific. PNAS. 2013;110:19113–19118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312691110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Morita T, Kang H, Wolfe J, Jadhav SP, Feldman DE. Psychometric curve and behavioral strategies for whisker-based texture discrimination in rats. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e20437. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Murasugi CM, Salzman CD, Newsome WT. Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects of varying pulse amplitude and frequency. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:1719–1729. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-04-01719.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Murphy BK, Miller KD. Balanced amplification: a new mechanism of selective amplification of neural activity patterns. Neuron. 2009;61:635–648. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Musall S, Kaufman MT, Juavinett AL, Gluf S, Churchland AK. Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:1677–1686. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0502-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Niell CM, Stryker MP. Modulation of visual responses by behavioral state in mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2010;65:472–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. O'Connor DH, Clack NG, Huber D, Komiyama T, Myers EW, Svoboda K. Vibrissa-based object localization in head-fixed mice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:1947–1967. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3762-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. O'Connor DH, Hires SA, Guo ZV, Li N, Yu J, Sun QQ, Huber D, Svoboda K. Neural coding during active somatosensation revealed using illusory touch. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:958–965. doi: 10.1038/nn.3419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Olshausen BA, Field DJ. Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature. 1996;381:607–609. doi: 10.1038/381607a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Olshausen BA, Field DJ. Sparse coding of sensory inputs. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Ölveczky BP, Otchy TM, Goldberg JH, Aronov D, Fee MS. Changes in the neural control of a complex motor sequence during learning. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:386–397. doi: 10.1152/jn.00018.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Otis JM, Namboodiri VM, Matan AM, Voets ES, Mohorn EP, Kosyk O, McHenry JA, Robinson JE, Resendez SL, Rossi MA, Stuber GD. Prefrontal cortex output circuits guide reward seeking through divergent cue encoding. Nature. 2017;543:103–107. doi: 10.1038/nature21376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Ozeki H, Finn IM, Schaffer ES, Miller KD, Ferster D. Inhibitory stabilization of the cortical network underlies visual surround suppression. Neuron. 2009;62:578–592. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Pachitariu M, Stringer C, Schröder S, Dipoppa M, Rossi LF, Carandini M, Harris KD. Suite2p: beyond 10,000 neurons with standard two-photon microscopy. bioRxiv. 2016 doi: 10.1101/061507. [DOI]
  128. Pachitariu M, Stringer C, Harris KD. Robustness of spike deconvolution for neuronal calcium imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:7976–7985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3339-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Packer AM, Russell LE, Dalgleish HW, Häusser M. Simultaneous all-optical manipulation and recording of neural circuit activity with cellular resolution in vivo. Nature Methods. 2015;12:140–146. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Packer AM, Yuste R. Dense, unspecific connectivity of neocortical parvalbumin-positive interneurons: a canonical microcircuit for inhibition? Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:13260–13271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3131-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Palm G. On associative memory. Biological Cybernetics. 1980;36:19–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00337019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Panzeri S, Ince RAA, Diamond ME, Kayser C. Reading spike timing without a clock: intrinsic decoding of spike trains. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20120467. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Panzeri S, Harvey CD, Piasini E, Latham PE, Fellin T. Cracking the neural code for sensory perception by combining statistics, intervention, and behavior. Neuron. 2017;93:491–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Pehlevan C, Sompolinsky H. Selectivity and sparseness in randomly connected balanced networks. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e89992. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Perin R, Berger TK, Markram H. A synaptic organizing principle for cortical neuronal groups. PNAS. 2011;108:5419–5424. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016051108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Peron SP, Freeman J, Iyer V, Guo C, Svoboda K. A cellular resolution map of barrel cortex activity during tactile behavior. Neuron. 2015;86:783–799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Peron S, Pancholi R, Voelcker B, Wittenbach JD, Ólafsdóttir HF, Freeman J, Svoboda K. Recurrent interactions in local cortical circuits. Nature. 2020;579:256–259. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2062-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Petersen CC. The functional organization of the barrel cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:339–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Plitt MH, Giocomo LM. Experience dependent contextual codes in the Hippocampus. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/864090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  140. Poirazi P, Brannon T, Mel BW. Pyramidal neuron as two-layer neural network. Neuron. 2003;37:989–999. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00149-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Poort J, Khan AG, Pachitariu M, Nemri A, Orsolic I, Krupic J, Bauza M, Sahani M, Keller GB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB. Learning enhances sensory and multiple Non-sensory representations in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2015;86:1478–1490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Rose D, Blakemore C. Effects of bicuculline on functions of inhibition in visual cortex. Nature. 1974;249:375–377. doi: 10.1038/249375a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Rudebeck PH, Murray EA. Dissociable effects of subtotal lesions within the macaque orbital prefrontal cortex on reward-guided behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:10569–10578. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0091-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Russell LE, Yang Z, Tan PL, Fişek M, Packer AM, Dalgleish HWP, Häusser M. The influence of visual cortex on perception is modulated by behavioural state. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/706010. [DOI]
  145. Russell LE. MONPangle. 219fb1aGithub. 2020a https://github.com/llerussell/MONPangle
  146. Russell LE. SLMTransformMaker3D. ff47cd6Github. 2020b https://github.com/llerussell/SLMTransformMaker3D
  147. Russell LE. PyBehaviour. 7de9363Github. 2020c https://github.com/llerussell/PyBehaviour
  148. Russell LE, Dalgleish HWP. Naparm. dd4c9bbGithub. 2020 https://github.com/llerussell/Naparm
  149. Sachidhanandam S, Sreenivasan V, Kyriakatos A, Kremer Y, Petersen CC. Membrane potential correlates of sensory perception in mouse barrel cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1671–1677. doi: 10.1038/nn.3532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Sadeh S, Clopath C. Patterned perturbation of inhibition can reveal the dynamical structure of neural processing. eLife. 2020;9:e52757. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Salzman CD, Britten KH, Newsome WT. Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction. Nature. 1990;346:174–177. doi: 10.1038/346174a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Salzman CD, Murasugi CM, Britten KH, Newsome WT. Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12:2331–2355. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-06-02331.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Sanders H, Wilson MA, Gershman SJ. Hippocampal remapping as hidden state inference. eLife. 2020;9:e51140. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Sanzeni A, Akitake B, Goldbach HC, Leedy CE, Brunel N, Histed MH. Inhibition stabilization is a widespread property of cortical networks. eLife. 2020;9:e54875. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Sasaki Y, Nanez JE, Watanabe T. Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:53–60. doi: 10.1038/nrn2737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Scholl B, Pattadkal JJ, Dilly GA, Priebe NJ, Zemelman BV. Local integration accounts for weak selectivity of mouse neocortical parvalbumin interneurons. Neuron. 2015;87:424–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Schölvinck ML, Howarth C, Attwell D. The cortical energy needed for conscious perception. NeuroImage. 2008;40:1460–1468. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Schütt HH, Harmeling S, Macke JH, Wichmann FA. Painfree and accurate Bayesian estimation of psychometric functions for (potentially) overdispersed data. Vision Research. 2016a;122:105–123. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Schütt HH, Harmeling S, Macke J, Wichmann F. psignifit. 079e0e6Github. 2016b https://github.com/wichmann-lab/psignifit
  160. Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Noise, neural codes and cortical organization. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1994;4:569–579. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90059-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Shemesh OA, Tanese D, Zampini V, Linghu C, Piatkevich K, Ronzitti E, Papagiakoumou E, Boyden ES, Emiliani V. Temporally precise single-cell-resolution optogenetics. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:1796–1806. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0018-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Shuler MG, Bear MF. Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science. 2006;311:1606–1609. doi: 10.1126/science.1123513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Silberberg G, Markram H. Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by martinotti cells. Neuron. 2007;53:735–746. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Sippy T, Lapray D, Crochet S, Petersen CCH. Cell-Type-Specific sensorimotor processing in striatal projection neurons during Goal-Directed behavior. Neuron. 2015;88:298–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Stachenfeld KL, Botvinick MM, Gershman SJ. The Hippocampus as a predictive map. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:1643–1653. doi: 10.1038/nn.4650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Steinmetz NA, Zatka-Haas P, Carandini M, Harris KD. Distributed coding of choice, action and engagement across the mouse brain. Nature. 2019;576:266–273. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1787-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Reddy CB, Carandini M, Harris KD. Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science. 2019;364:eaav7893. doi: 10.1126/science.aav7893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Stringer C, Wang T, Michaelos M, Pachitariu M. Cellpose: a generalist algorithm for cellular segmentation. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.02.02.931238. [DOI] [PubMed]
  169. Stüttgen MC, Schwarz C. Psychophysical and neurometric detection performance under stimulus uncertainty. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:1091–1099. doi: 10.1038/nn.2162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Tanke N, Borst JGG, Houweling AR. Single-Cell stimulation in barrel cortex influences psychophysical detection performance. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:2057–2068. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2155-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Thomson AM, Lamy C. Functional maps of neocortical local circuitry. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2007;1:19–42. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.002.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Tolhurst DJ, Movshon JA, Dean AF. The statistical reliability of signals in single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex. Vision Research. 1983;23:775–785. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90200-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Tolman EC, Ritchie BF, Kalish D. Studies in spatial learning and the short-cut. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1946;36:13–24. doi: 10.1037/h0053944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Tolman EC. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review. 1948;55:189–208. doi: 10.1037/h0061626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Treiman DM. GABAergic mechanisms in epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2001;42:8–12. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2001.042suppl.3008.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Tse D, Langston RF, Kakeyama M, Bethus I, Spooner PA, Wood ER, Witter MP, Morris RG. Schemas and memory consolidation. Science. 2007;316:76–82. doi: 10.1126/science.1135935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Tsodyks MV, Skaggs WE, Sejnowski TJ, McNaughton BL. Paradoxical effects of external modulation of inhibitory interneurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:4382–4388. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04382.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. van Vreeswijk C, Sompolinsky H. Chaos in neuronal networks with balanced excitatory and inhibitory activity. Science. 1996;274:1724–1726. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5293.1724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Walton ME, Behrens TE, Buckley MJ, Rudebeck PH, Rushworth MF. Separable learning systems in the macaque brain and the role of orbitofrontal cortex in contingent learning. Neuron. 2010;65:927–939. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Waters J, Helmchen F. Background synaptic activity is sparse in neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:8267–8277. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2152-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Watson BO, Yuste R, Packer AM. PackIO and EphysViewer: software tools for acquisition and analysis of neuroscience data. bioRxiv. 2016 doi: 10.1101/054080. [DOI]
  182. Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;426:442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Whittington JCR, Muller TH, Barry C, Behrens TEJ. Generalisation of structural knowledge in the Hippocampal-Entorhinal system. arXiv. 2018 https://arxiv.org/abs/1805.09042
  184. Whittington JCR, Muller TH, Mark S, Chen G, Barry C, Burgess N, Behrens TEJ. The Tolman-Eichenbaum : space and relational memory through generalisation in the hippocampal formation. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/770495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  185. Wiest MC, Thomson E, Pantoja J, Nicolelis MA. Changes in S1 neural responses during tactile discrimination learning. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:300–312. doi: 10.1152/jn.00194.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Wilson NR, Runyan CA, Wang FL, Sur M. Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitory networks in vivo. Nature. 2012;488:343–348. doi: 10.1038/nature11347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Wolf F, Engelken R, Puelma-Touzel M, Weidinger JD, Neef A. Dynamical models of cortical circuits. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2014;25:228–236. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Wolfe J, Houweling AR, Brecht M. Sparse and powerful cortical spikes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2010;20:306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Xiong Q, Znamenskiy P, Zador AM. Selective corticostriatal plasticity during acquisition of an auditory discrimination task. Nature. 2015;521:348–351. doi: 10.1038/nature14225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Ye Z, Mostajo-Radji MA, Brown JR, Rouaux C, Tomassy GS, Hensch TK, Arlotta P. Instructing perisomatic inhibition by direct lineage reprogramming of neocortical projection neurons. Neuron. 2015;88:475–483. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Prigge M, Schneider F, Davidson TJ, O'Shea DJ, Sohal VS, Goshen I, Finkelstein J, Paz JT, Stehfest K, Fudim R, Ramakrishnan C, Huguenard JR, Hegemann P, Deisseroth K. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. 2011;477:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature10360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Yoshimura Y, Dantzker JL, Callaway EM. Excitatory cortical neurons form fine-scale functional networks. Nature. 2005;433:868–873. doi: 10.1038/nature03252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Zatka-Haas P, Steinmetz NA, Carandini M, Harris KD. A perceptual decision requires sensory but not action coding in mouse cortex. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/501627. [DOI]
  194. Zhang Z, Russell LE, Packer AM, Gauld OM, Häusser M. Closed-loop all-optical interrogation of neural circuits in vivo. Nature Methods. 2018;15:1037–1040. doi: 10.1038/s41592-018-0183-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Ziburkus J, Cressman JR, Barreto E, Schiff SJ. Interneuron and pyramidal cell interplay during in vitro seizure-like events. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;95:3948–3954. doi: 10.1152/jn.01378.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Znamenskiy P, Kim M, Muir DR, Iacaruso F, Hofer SB, Mrsic-flogel TD. Functional selectivity and specific connectivity of inhibitory neurons in primary visual cortex. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/294835. [DOI]
  197. Znamenskiy P, Zador AM. Corticostriatal neurons in auditory cortex drive decisions during auditory discrimination. Nature. 2013;497:482–485. doi: 10.1038/nature12077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Brice Bathellier1
Reviewed by: Brice Bathellier2, Luis Carrillo-Reid3

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

The study elegantly quantifies the minimal number of neurons that need to be stimulated in order to be behaviorally reported by a mouse. Combination of holographic stimulation methods and of two photon calcium imaging also allows assessing the network impact of targeted stimulations, demonstrating matched activations and inhibitions produced by artificial stimulations of excitatory neurons.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "How many neurons are sufficient to generate a percept?" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Brice Bathellier as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer

#1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Ronald Calabrese as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Luis Carrillo-Reid (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission. While all reviewers recognize that this study brings new important insights about the minimal number of neural activations that can be behaviorally detected, there was a consensus that some of the analyses must be carefully improved and better justified to reach eLife's standards, that the paper must be re-organized and that some of the claims must be toned down.

Summary:

In this manuscript, Dalgleish et al. combined optical imaging and holographic two-photon stimulation with an operant conditioning paradigm to measure how many neurons need to be stimulated in the mouse primary somatosensory cortex to generate a behavioral percept. The techniques used are state of the art and so these findings add precise information about the number and location of stimulated neurons compared to previous studies employing electrical stimulation or 1P optogenetic stimulation. The authors propose a lower bound of ~13 neurons to evoke a 1P optogenetic induced learned behavior. The data are clearly presented, and the conclusions are, for the most part justified, and they can be of interest for the broad audience of eLife. However, some conclusions could be more straightforwardly reached than with the present analyses used by the authors, and some important quantifications are not apparent or missing. The important question of confounds between evoked and facial movement related activity is not adequately addressed, and some conclusions (indirect inhibition) appear not to be supported statistically. Detailed prescriptions for revision are given below.

Essential revisions:

1) It would be much clearer to state the results presented in Supplementary Figure 9 right at the beginning. The authors use a complex statistical model in Figure 5 to show that direct activation of only ~40 cells in cortex is sufficient to be detected by the animal at plateau performance. It is confusing to mention that a certain number of neurons are needed to generate a percept to then find out at the end of the Results section that this estimate needs to be decreased by ~ 60% because only ~ 40% of 2P stimulated neurons respond to the stimulation protocol (Supplementary Figure 9). Instead, data from Supplementary Figure 9 should be shown in Figure 2 and the minimum number of cells to be directly activated should be given after correcting for activation probability. The text should be edited accordingly and the question addressed in Figure 5 should be reframed to focus on the question of indirect activations independent of direct activation probability.

2) There seems to be a major problem with Figure 3B-D. The false positive rate of activated or inhibited cells seems to be very high (~12-14%), as seen in catch trials (shadings). This is not surprising given that the authors have a 1 SD statistical threshold which predicts 15% false positive rate. Nevertheless, the authors state that there are inactivated cells although their fraction is below false positive detection rate. The conclusion should be corrected. As far as we can judge from the data there is no statistically detectable indirect inhibition and indirect excitation only becomes significant after 50 targeted cells, and if corrected for false positive rate. This also helps understanding why the statistical model shows no impact of indirect activations.

On the same lines, there are other presentation problems: the scale of Figure 3C makes little sense. It is hard to understand why only 1% of T cell are activated while other cells have a 12% activation rate. The authors should verify and explain why false positive rate is much lower for these cells. Supplementary Figure 6 has no unit and no explanation.

Also the authors better describe spontaneous level. How many neurons are spontaneously active in a given field? How is spontaneous activity related to the artificial activation in terms of the spatial distribution of neurons? The authors should include analyses of the spatial distribution of BG active/suppressed neurons in relation to targeted neurons.

3) Once point 2) is clarified, the only question that remains is whether the indirect activations (those significantly above false positive) are in fact coming from a cortical report of licking. This is indeed possible as they follow exactly licking probability and this is a very interesting point made by this study. However, the authors suggest this can be addressed by subsampling hit trials. Unfortunately, they do not give the rational for this and it did strike the reviewer. To be kept in the paper the procedures used in Figure 4 should be greatly improved and justified. In fact, the statistical model in Figure 5 is more convincing but it should also be better explained.

We also noted that stimulation duration is 250ms and the artefacts are removed over 500ms. The authors should explain why, because measuring DF/F signals just after 250ms (i.e. before reaction time) should solve the issue about whether indirect activations are from licks or from the network.

4) The animals learn in 3 steps, and learning curves for each step are not reported. This should be done so that the reader can appreciate if the animals are at their maximum performance. Moreover, are mice immediately transferring between 1P and 2P in step 2? In Figure 2—figure supplement 3B it is shown that weak stimulation can be learnt to be detected more efficiently. But this is a bit out of context as we do not know how much time it took to learn 100 cells.

The proposal of sparse codification of the responses for this task is contradictory to the fact that the perceptual learning improves with the stimulation of different sets of neurons. It is not clear from the experiments and interpretations how such an exquisite behavioral response with a lower bound of ~13 neurons can generalize to different sets of targeted neurons but cannot be learned by the activation of just few neurons with 2P. In other words, the hypothesis of generalization to arbitrary activity patterns should enable the authors to train the animals with 2P photostimulation?

5) Reaction times are not quantified except in Supplementary Figure 4. They should be included in the main figure and compared to reaction times for very simple tactile detection tasks. E.g. Ollerenshaw et al., 2012, show reaction times in rats that are more rapid (300-250 ms) that those shown in this study. Is cortical stimulation fully comparable to a real perception? Is reaction time dependent on the number of activated cells. Of course, we agree that when there is no significant detection (<25 targeted cells) reaction times should not be computed, as there is no reaction of the animal to the stimulus.

In addition, in Supplementary Figure 4B, reaction times are significantly shorter with the non-soma-tagged C1V1 compared to the soma-tagged variant. How do the authors explain this result? Is it due to non-specific stimulation of fibers of passage, which potentially increases the number of stimulated neurons? If so, shouldn't this also affect the probability of licking?

6) Overall, the experiments demonstrated that the activation of few neurons is sufficient to evoke a learned behavior induced by the bulk activation of many neurons using 1P photostimulation, but even though the authors have the technical capability to describe sensory evoked activity in terms of neuronal ensembles, the authors never relate sensory evoked activity with their targeted neurons. What kind of responses are evoked by 1P 10 mW photostimulation? How many neurons? How many action potentials per neuron? How big is the volume affected by 1P photostimulation? Also, in the first paragraph of the subsection “Very few neurons are sufficient to drive a detectible percept”, during the training protocol with 1P photostimulation the authors decrease the power of stimulation and suggest that the number of neurons activated is decreased. They should show that or consider the possibility that the firing (number of action potentials) is reduced but not the number of neurons active. In particular, in the first paragraph of the subsection “Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual 2 threshold”, the difference between ~13 neurons and ~60 neurons in Huber et al., 2008, could also be due the fact that in the previous paper the number of action potential evoked were controlled whereas in the present study the number of action potentials evoked by 1P or 2P photostimulation are not reported.

7) The title and the impact statement don't reflect the experiments. In particular, the volumetric stimulation with 1P optogenetics doesn't seem to evoke physiological responses. The authors propose that their results prove the lower bound of neurons sufficient to generate a percept, but all the literature cited define a percept as neuronal activity that represents specific features of sensory stimuli. For example, in the references cited hallucinations are defined as "perceptions in the absence of objectively identifiable stimuli. Hallucinations are percepts without corresponding external stimuli". (Corlett et al., 2019). According to such definition the authors are describing hallucinations induced by bulk 1P photostimulation and evoked by 2P activation of few neurons, to which the train the animals to respond. However the relation between neurons responding to 1P stimulation (phase 1 of training) and a sensory evoked percept is missing even though the authors have all the necessary tools to perform such experiments.

So, if the authors want to keep their claims about percepts, they should show the relation of the activity evoked by photostimulation and activity evoked by specific sensory stimuli. In other words, despite that all the references and framework of the manuscript postulate intervention experiments related to percepts, the authors never show the relation between sensory stimuli, targeted optogenetics and behavior. This could be addressed either by toning down the claims, and in particular change the title or demonstrate experimentally that the activity evoked by the targeted activation of neurons represents percepts. For example, a more appropriate title could be: "How many neurons are sufficient to detect direct cortical photostimulation?"

8) Discussion, first paragraph. How the perceptual learning could generalize to the surrounding network? The observed result could be an artifact of a massive network reconfiguration due to 1P photostimulation at early phases of training. The 1P stimulation protocol, which precedes 2P stimulation, may artificially increase connectivity among opsin-expressing neurons and this may explain why the effect of 2P photostimulation on behavior is independent on the identity of stimulated cells (Figure 2—figure supplement 3D). If the network is reconfigured then the animals learned to detect such artificial network or parts of it. Comparing spontaneous activity before and after photostimulation entraining is necessary to clarify this point, and this alternative explanation should be mentioned in the text, especially because the authors seem to favor the opposite scenario, i.e. that "the neurons we targeted probably do no benefit from such recurrent excitation".

9) The main conclusion of the study is that a small number (~ 13) of layer II/III neurons in S1 needs to be stimulated in order to generate a percept. However, this number may strongly depend, among other things, on: i) the number of neurons effectively activated by the 2P stimulation, which is potentially underestimated because of the inability of the calcium indicator to read out small increases in spiking activity under the imaging conditions used in this study; ii) the number of 2P stimulation sessions that animals undergo; iii) the duration and intensity of the 1P stimulation paradigm which may induce different levels of increased connectivity in the stimulated neurons depending on the stimulation conditions; iv) the small number of planes imaged. v) the intensity/duration of the 2P stimulation (e.g., stimulation of 7 neurons with the protocol used in this study may not result in the generation of a behavioral percept, but stronger/longer stimulation of the same 7 neurons might). These issues should be clearly stated in the Discussion, because the authors' estimate on the absolute number of required neurons for generating a percept could strongly depend on their specific experimental conditions and could not necessarily generalize to S1 under any condition.

eLife. 2020 Oct 26;9:e58889. doi: 10.7554/eLife.58889.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) It would be much clearer to state the results presented in Supplementary Figure 9 right at the beginning. The authors use a complex statistical model in Figure 5 to show that direct activation of only ~40 cells in cortex is sufficient to be detected by the animal at plateau performance. It is confusing to mention that a certain number of neurons are needed to generate a percept to then find out at the end of the Results section that this estimate needs to be decreased by ~ 60% because only ~ 40% of 2P stimulated neurons respond to the stimulation protocol (Supplementary Figure 9). Instead, data from Supplementary Figure 9 should be shown in Figure 2 and the minimum number of cells to be directly activated should be given after correcting for activation probability. The text should be edited accordingly and the question addressed in Figure 5 should be reframed to focus on the question of indirect activations independent of direct activation probability.

This is an excellent point and we have made the suggested changes. We have clarified our definitions of target and background neurons (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A, Materials and methods “Classifying target neurons and background neurons” subsection) and report the proportion of responsive target neurons at the suggested point both in the text (“Very few cortical neurons are sufficient to drive behaviour” subsection), in the Materials and methods (“Classifying target neurons and background neurons” subsection) and in Figure 2—figure supplement 1H-J. The number of neurons required to drive behaviour is now reported on the basis of the number of target neurons activated in Figure 2 and throughout the manuscript. Figure 4 (which is the equivalent to original Figure 5) now focuses on the contributions of target and background activity to behavioural detection.

2) There seems to be a major problem with Figure 3B-D. The false positive rate of activated or inhibited cells seems to be very high (~12-14%), as seen in catch trials (shadings). This is not surprising given that the authors have a 1 SD statistical threshold which predicts 15% false positive rate. Nevertheless, the authors state that there are inactivated cells although their fraction is below false positive detection rate. The conclusion should be corrected. As far as we can judge from the data there is no statistically detectable indirect inhibition and indirect excitation only becomes significant after 50 targeted cells, and if corrected for false positive rate. This also helps understanding why the statistical model shows no impact of indirect activations.

This comment raises two concerns which we address below: (1) suppression is indefensible, and (2) our thresholds are too lenient.

1) The reviewers are correct in pointing out that there is no suppression beyond that observed on catch trials in the original Figure 3 in which we don’t correct for lick responses (however, we reiterate briefly that there is suppression when we correct for lick responses (original Figure 4D, E, see below)). We apologise for this error in the text and have addressed this through two changes: (i) in the new manuscript we now only reference lick-corrected data (as we feel this is the most accurate representation of responses evoked by photostimulation when behavioural responses are accounted for) meaning that the erroneous passage is no longer included in the manuscript; (ii) inspired by the reviewers’ comments we have implemented more stringent neuronal response thresholds which, while more cleanly separating stimulus-evoked activity from spontaneous activity on catch trials, make almost no qualitative change to our results on lick-corrected data (see point (2) below). This consistency speaks to the robustness of the phenomena that we report. Moreover, we would like to clarify that in the original manuscript our main conclusions concerning suppression were based on the statistically significant suppression that we clearly observed in the background when we correct for lick responses (original Figure 4D) alongside the observation that this scales with the number of target neurons stimulated (original Figure 4E). We believe that this was sufficient evidence for our original claim that we observed network suppression in the original manuscript, though we again apologise for the confusion caused by the description of the original Figure 3.

2) We agree that our neuronal response thresholds were somewhat lenient, but we felt that this was necessary to capture the potentially small responses evoked by synaptic recruitment of background neurons. Our rationale for this was that we expect synaptically recruited responses in the background neurons (particularly activation) to be small and unreliable, due to the features of synaptic connectivity, and we expect suppression to be difficult to detect due to the design of calcium indicators. Moreover, since we wanted to compare the levels of activation and suppression we decided to take thresholds of equal magnitude, but opposite sign, for both. However, as the reviewers point out, this leads to high false positive rates on catch trials that may mask real effects on stimulus trials. While we tried to account for this high false positive rate in the original manuscript by subtracting catch trial response rates from all relevant stimulus trial data (original Figure 4 and 5), as implied by the reviewer, it may be more effective to define more appropriate thresholds.

Therefore, in the revised manuscript we have made these thresholds much more stringent and statistically grounded by empirically estimating them, using cross-validation, from each cell’s distribution of responses on correct reject catch trials (i.e. trials where there should be no signal from photostimulus or licking). This procedure is described in detail in the “Neuronal response analysis” subsection of the Materials and methods and in Figure 2—figure supplement 1. It returns separate activation and suppression thresholds for each neuron, which are defined by the mean and scaling factors for the s.d. of its catch trial correct reject response distribution that yield a 5% false positive rate on catch trials (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B-E). Note that for each experiment we estimate a single activation and single suppression s.d. scaling factor that is applied to all neurons. This procedure is cross-validated in the sense that for each experiment (population of imaged neurons) we sweep through a series of s.d. scaling factors to calculate neuron-wise thresholds on a training subset of catch trials and then calculate the false positive rate (across neurons), using these thresholds, averaged across the remaining testing subset of catch trials. We repeat this for 10,000 train:test splits and infer the s.d. scaling factor that yields a median 5% false positive rate (across neurons, averaged across test catch trials) across all train:test splits. This yields higher s.d. scaling factors than if we simply find the s.d. scaling factor that yields a 5% false positive rate across all catch trials (no cross-validation; Figure 2—figure supplement 1F). These higher cross-validated s.d. scaling factors therefore yield more stringent neuron response thresholds that result in marginally less than 5% false positive rates when used on the full dataset (catch trial response rates in Figure 3, Figure 2—figure supplement 1J).

Despite these more stringent thresholds, the main conclusions of our previous network response analysis, that photostimulation of target neurons recruits matched suppression in background neurons but has little impact on background neuron activation (original Figure 4E), still hold up. We also still observe that background suppression scales with target neuron activation (new Figure 3E), resulting in balanced activation and suppression across all neurons as more target neurons are activated (new Figure 3C), and we still find that background activation does not change (new Figure 3D). Finally, we also still observe that neither background activation nor suppression have any reliable impact on behaviour (Figure 4). The only major qualitative difference we note using these new thresholds is that we now see that photostimulation in general does recruit some background activation (new Figure 3E inset right), though as noted above this is not modulated by the number of activated target neurons (new Figure 3D fit). This is in contrast to our previous observation that photostimulation does not cause activation in the background network (original Figure 4D). As the reviewer points out, this lack of modulation likely underlies the lack of impact of background activation on behaviour.

We thank the reviewers for their excellent suggestion that we adopt more stringent, statistically validated thresholds and we are confident that the qualitive consistency of the large majority of the results between the original and revised manuscripts demonstrates their robustness.

On the same lines, there are other presentation problems: the scale of Figure 3C makes little sense. It is hard to understand why only 1% of T cell are activated while other cells have a 12% activation rate. The authors should verify and explain why false positive rate is much lower for these cells. Supplementary Figure 6 has no unit and no explanation.

We apologise for the confusion in relation to this plot, which is now alleviated by the change in thresholds described above (see our first response to reviewer point 2). Some additional explanation is provided below. In the original version of the manuscript, T neuron activation (proportion of target neurons activated) and BG neuron activation (proportion of background neurons activated) were both defined as proportions using the total number of neurons in the imaging volume as denominators (2000 – 3000 neurons per experiment). This quantification was used to try to describe “what proportion of the population is recruited by direct photostimulation” and “what proportion of the population is recruited indirectly” respectively, and to allow them to be directly compared (since they have the same denominators). However we agree that it is confusing because, despite the fact that photostimulation does cause more robust recruitment of target neurons than background neurons (see below), T neuron activation looks small compared to BG neuron activation because the “maximum possible numerator” is very different. To illustrate this consider the situation where we stimulated 200 target neurons in an imaged volume of 3000 neurons. In this condition the maximum possible numerator for T neuron activation is 200 (if all 200 target neurons responded), whereas for the BG neuron activation it is 2800 (3000 – 200 if all non-targeted neurons responded). Even though it is less likely for background neurons to respond compared to target neurons, this is counteracted by the larger number of neurons we consider in the numerator of BG neurons activated.

We have clarified this in the manuscript and have replaced these acronyms with more interpretable axis labels as follows. BG cell activation and BG cell suppression have been relabelled P(Activated) Background and P(Suppressed) Background respectively and still correspond to the proportion of the population that was activated (or suppressed) but not targeted (Figure 3D, E). We have removed references to T cell activation entirely (since it is confusing) and replaced them with P(Activated) All neurons and P(Suppressed) All neurons which quantify respectively the proportion of all neurons (whole population; targeted and untargeted) that are activated or suppressed (Figure 3A, B). By comparing the P(Activated) and P(Suppressed) in all neurons with the equivalents in the background population one can see that the activation observed in the whole population comes from target neurons (Figure 3A vs. 3D), whereas the suppression is mainly present in the background neurons (Figure 3B vs. 3E). We also point out that we now quantify the proportion of targets activated out of the total number of neurons targeted on each trial type (Figure 1—figure supplement 1J). Comparing all of the above plots it is now clear that the false positive rate is very similar between them (~3 – 5%) and is in line with what one would expect from our threshold estimation procedure (see also our third response to point 2 below). We have also replaced the original Supplementary Figure 6 with the new Figure 3—figure supplement 1A-H for which the units should be more clear.

Also the authors better describe spontaneous level. How many neurons are spontaneously active in a given field? How is spontaneous activity related to the artificial activation in terms of the spatial distribution of neurons? The authors should include analyses of the spatial distribution of BG active/suppressed neurons in relation to targeted neurons.

Spontaneous levels of activation and suppression (quantified on catch trials) are now described as proportions for the whole population (Figure 3A, B), for the background neurons (Figure 3D, E) and for targets (Figure 2—figure supplement 1I, J) and quantified in the text. As expected from our new threshold estimation procedure, ~3 – 5% of neurons are spontaneously active or suppressed on catch trials. This is below the values observed in targets when stimulated (Figure 2—figure supplement 1I, J) and in background when large numbers of targets are stimulated (Figure 3A, B, D, E). We have also described the spatial distribution of activation and suppression in our psychometric curve experiments relative to nearest 2P optogenetic target spot across all neurons (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Note that this spontaneous activity rate (or “false positive rate”) is slightly lower than 5% because our threshold estimation procedure is cross-validated and the criterion used to infer appropriate thresholds is “median 5% false positive rate across all train:test splits”. This is stringent, resulting in higher response thresholds than when we don’t cross-validate (Figure 2—figure supplement 1F) which will result in marginally lower false positive rates on the full dataset. This is described in more detail above (see our first response to point 2).

3) Once point 2) is clarified, the only question that remains is whether the indirect activations (those significantly above false positive) are in fact coming from a cortical report of licking. This is indeed possible as they follow exactly licking probability and this is a very interesting point made by this study. However, the authors suggest this can be addressed by subsampling hit trials. Unfortunately, they do not give the rational for this and it did strike the reviewer. To be kept in the paper the procedures used in Figure 4 should be greatly improved and justified. In fact, the statistical model in Figure 5 is more convincing but it should also be better explained.

We are glad that the reviewers also appreciate the difficulty of analysing network responses to increasing numbers of activated target neurons when neurons respond to licking directly (Figure 3—figure supplement 1), and licking itself correlates with the number of target neurons activated (Figure 2D). We also agree that lick-related activity in cortex is very interesting and something that subsequent studies need to explore further. We apologize that our rationale for our hit:miss matching procedure wasn’t more clearly explained. We now briefly describe this in the subsection “Suppression in the local network balances target activation”, and detail it in the Materials and methods (“Hit:miss matching” subsection) and in a figure supplement (Figure 3—figure supplement 1O). We re-iterate our rationale here for clarity.

Stimulus trials activating few neurons (e.g. 5) are associated with few hits (licks) and many misses (no licks) whereas stimulus trials activating many neurons (e.g. 200) are associated with many hits (licks) and few misses (no licks). Therefore, when averaging network response metrics across trials, many neuron stimulus trials (200) will have lots of lick-related activity whereas few neuron stimulus trials (5) will have little lick related activity. This means that any modulation of activity in the background network could be a reflection of the increased proportion of trials with lick responses going into the analysis. To address this problem, we fixed the ratio of hit:miss trials (lick:nolick) at 50:50 in our analysis, and kept this consistent across all trial types. For example, let us assume we have 10 trials of each type (10 x 200 neuron stim. trials, 10 x 100 neuron stim. trials etc.). For 5 neuron stimulus trials we have 3 hits and 7 misses whereas for 200 neuron stimulus types we have 7 hits and 3 misses. For 5 neuron stimulus trials, our procedure would take the 3 hit trials and combine them with 100 random 3-trial resamples from the 7 miss trials (to give us 3 hits and 3 misses), calculating the network response metrics on each resample and then averaging across resamples. For the 200 neuron stimulus trials our procedure would take the 3 miss trials and combine them with 100 random 3-trial resamples from the 7 hit trials, calculating the network response metrics on each resample and then averaging across resamples. This procedure therefore ensures that analysis of both 5 and 200 neuron stimulus trials use 3 hits and 3 misses while also attempting to use as much of the data as possible (through the resampling procedure). By doing the same thing for all trial types we remove the variation in the hit:miss ratio with the number of neurons activated. Any residual variation in the background response metrics should therefore be directly due to the number of target neurons activated, and not the more reliable behavioural responses that increasing numbers of target neurons recruit. We hope that, having thus better explained the rationale and method for hit:miss matching that we use, the network results that we report in Figure 3 (the equivalent of the original Figure 4 referenced by the reviewers) are easier to understand.

We also noted that stimulation duration is 250ms and the artefacts are removed over 500ms. The authors should explain why, because measuring DF/F signals just after 250ms (i.e. before reaction time) should solve the issue about whether indirect activations are from licks or from the network.

We apologize for the lack of clarity in our description of 2P photostimulus durations and how they relate to volume acquisition times in the original manuscript. We have now emphasised and clarified the relevant “Two-photon optogenetic stimulation” and “Image processing” subsections of the Materials and methods to explain why we chose our excluded epoch post-stimulus. This is also explained below.

The reviewer is correct in stating that stimuli targeting 100 neurons or less last ~250 ms (we stimulate all neurons simultaneously 10 times at 40 Hz – 10 x 20 ms spirals, 5 ms inter-spiral interval, ~250 ms stimulus duration). However, stimuli targeting 200 neurons last 500 ms because, due to constraints on the total power output of the photostimulation laser (600 mW power on sample; 6 mW per target neuron), we randomly divide the 200 targeted neurons into 2 groups of 100 which we stimulate as an alternating pair (each group of 100 neurons is stimulated 10 times at 20 Hz – stimulate first group with 20 ms spiral, 5 ms inter-spiral interval, stimulate second group with 20 ms spiral, 5 ms inter-spiral interval, then return to first group and repeat a further 9 times; each group therefore receives 10 x 20 ms spirals with an effective inter-spiral interval, for that group, of 25 ms; ~500 ms total stimulus duration). Thus while the majority of stimuli in 2P psychometric curve sessions are 250 ms in duration (≤100 neurons), the strong stimulus trial type is 500 ms (200 neurons). We wanted the analysis period post-photostimulus to be the same across all trial types to facilitate comparison between them and, since the maximum stimulus epoch across all trial types is ~500 ms (for 200 neuron stimulation trials), we have to use this for all trial types, even though ≤100 neuron stimulation trials only have a ~250 ms stimulus epoch. In reality we actually have to remove a slightly longer period of imaging data (5 full volumes peri-stimulus; ~750 ms of imaging data) due to how the photostimulus epoch overlaps with the time-course of imaging volume acquisition (this is explained in detail in the “Image processing” subsection of the Materials and methods).

4) The animals learn in 3 steps, and learning curves for each step are not reported. This should be done so that the reader can appreciate if the animals are at their maximum performance. Moreover, are mice immediately transferring between 1P and 2P in step 2? In Figure 2—figure supplement 3B it is shown that weak stimulation can be learnt to be detected more efficiently. But this is a bit out of context as we do not know how much time it took to learn 100 cells.

We have added supplementary figures detailing the 1P training (Figure 1—figure supplement 3) and the early 2P training (Figure 1—figure supplement 5). We have also added considerable detail describing these phases of the experiment in the text (“Targeted two-photon optogenetic activation of neural ensembles in L2/3 barrel cortex can drive behaviour” subsection) and in the Materials and methods (subsection “Behavioural training”).

From these data it should be clear that mice learn the 1P task very rapidly (often within a single session) and that their performance constantly adapts to reductions in stimulus power (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). This implies that by the end of this phase they are expert and their performance is close to saturation. We have now also referenced this in the Discussion (subsection “The network input-output function for perception is steep”). The amount of time they spend in this phase therefore balances the need to entrain reliable/consistent behaviour with the desire to get to the more relevant 2P phases of the task as quickly as possible without too much behavioural saturation. Mice also very quickly learn to detect 2P photostimulation, again often within the first 1P/2P training session (Figure 1—figure supplement 5A-C), suggesting that our 1P staircase training paradigm was well designed to get mice to the point where they can detect the strongest 2P stimuli that we can deliver, also potentially suggesting that our minimum LED powers are activating a similar number of neurons (~200). Thus since mice are pretty quick to learn 2P photostimulation of hundreds of neurons it is difficult to compare their learning of these stimuli with those of tens of neurons which appear to be much harder for animals to initially detect.

The proposal of sparse codification of the responses for this task is contradictory to the fact that the perceptual learning improves with the stimulation of different sets of neurons. It is not clear from the experiments and interpretations how such an exquisite behavioral response with a lower bound of ~13 neurons can generalize to different sets of targeted neurons but cannot be learned by the activation of just few neurons with 2P. In other words, the hypothesis of generalization to arbitrary activity patterns should enable the authors to train the animals with 2P photostimulation?

As the reviewers point out, the relationship between the lower limit on performance and the extent of learning generalization is indeed fascinating and should be investigated further in subsequent work. We have added sections in the Discussion concerning this issue (subsection “The perceptual threshold is plastic and can generalise”). We also consider whether we would be able to train animals with 2P photostimulation de novo without the 1P priming phase of our experiment, as suggested by the reviewers (see the aforementioned subsection). We believe that this would likely be possible, but only if one initially targeted large numbers of neurons. As in all non-naturalistic paradigms (even those using sensory stimuli) animals must first be primed with stimuli that are orders of magnitude more salient than those that they often later have to detect or discriminate between. This is because animals easily fall into sub-optimal behavioural local minima (i.e. constantly licking, timing licks etc.) if they fail to understand the task. Nevertheless, the priming phase of such studies doesn’t weaken the claim that animals can perceive stimuli with a precision that is defined by the final experiments using the least salient stimuli. We maintain that our experiments can be interpreted in this framework.

5) Reaction times are not quantified except in Supplementary Figure 4. They should be included in the main figure and compared to reaction times for very simple tactile detection tasks. E.g. Ollerenshaw et al., 2012, show reaction times in rats that are more rapid (300-250 ms) that those shown in this study. Is cortical stimulation fully comparable to a real perception? Is reaction time dependent on the number of activated cells. Of course, we agree that when there is no significant detection (<25 targeted cells) reaction times should not be computed, as there is no reaction of the animal to the stimulus.

We have now reported reaction times for all stimuli in all phases of behavioural training (Figure 1F, Figure 1—figure supplements 3D, J, 5D, Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). We do not observe a strong modulation of reaction time by the number of target neurons activated (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A), however we do find that the s.d. of reaction time reduces as more neurons are activated (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B). We have also compared reaction times for optogenetic stimuli (~0.4 – 0.7 s) to those for a variety of whisker stimuli in mice (~0.3 – 0.4 s) in the Discussion (subsection “Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual threshold”), though we have not included the reference suggested by the reviewer as it is in rats and therefore potentially less comparable. We discuss the observation that optogenetic stimuli drive slower behavioural responses and describe some factors that could explain this.

In addition, in Supplementary Figure 4B, reaction times are significantly shorter with the non-soma-tagged C1V1 compared to the soma-tagged variant. How do the authors explain this result? Is it due to non-specific stimulation of fibers of passage, which potentially increases the number of stimulated neurons? If so, shouldn't this also affect the probability of licking?

This is a very astute observation and one that is well worth exploring, as suggested by the reviewer. We have added a section in the Discussion discussing this (subsection “Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual threshold”). We agree that the most likely explanation for the difference in reaction time is stronger, quicker depolarisation generated by the additional opsin in more distal neural processes, as well as recruitment of more distal neurons via processes traversing the photostimulation volume. Moreover it is possible that opsin-induced depolarisation could recruit active dendritic conductances (voltage-gated calcium channels etc.). We agree that the lack of impact on detection rate is somewhat confusing. It’s possible that animals’ performance is saturated at its upper limit (allowing for a lapse rate that is independent of the salience of the stimuli) such that a ceiling effect prevents us from seeing the reduction in detectability that occurs between a very easy to detect stimulus and a slightly less easy to detect stimulus. Moreover if one looks carefully at the distributions in Figure 1—figure supplement 6A one can see that the “weight” of the C1V1-Kv2.1 distribution is lower than that of the regular C1V1 distribution. Thus it is possible that the effect is too small to detect with a dataset of this size.

6) Overall, the experiments demonstrated that the activation of few neurons is sufficient to evoke a learned behavior induced by the bulk activation of many neurons using 1P photostimulation, but even though the authors have the technical capability to describe sensory evoked activity in terms of neuronal ensembles, the authors never relate sensory evoked activity with their targeted neurons.

We agree that this question is fascinating, however we did not collect the data necessary to perform this study. We believe that performing an additional study to address this is beyond the scope of this work. Please also see the response below to major point 7.

What kind of responses are evoked by 1P 10 mW photostimulation? How many neurons? How many action potentials per neuron? How big is the volume affected by 1P photostimulation? Also, in the first paragraph of the subsection “Very few neurons are sufficient to drive a detectible percept”, during the training protocol with 1P photostimulation the authors decrease the power of stimulation and suggest that the number of neurons activated is decreased. They should show that or consider the possibility that the firing (number of action potentials) is reduced but not the number of neurons active. In particular, in the first paragraph of the subsection “Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual 2 threshold”, the difference between ~13 neurons and ~60 neurons in Huber et al., 2008, could also be due the fact that in the previous paper the number of action potential evoked were controlled whereas in the present study the number of action potentials evoked by 1P or 2P photostimulation are not reported.

Since 1P photostimulation was only intended to prime animals to detect 2P photostimulation, and since we do not have a PMT shutter to allow us to do imaging during these sessions, we unfortunately did not record the response to 1P photostimulation. To address this we have added an estimate based on comparisons to previous work in the “One-photon optogenetic stimulation” subsection of the Materials and methods. We expect our 2P photostimuli to elicit ~1 action potential per spiral (Packer et al., 2015), meaning that all 2P trial types should elicit ~10 action potentials. We have noted this in the “Two-photon optogenetic stimulation” subsection of the Materials and methods. We have highlighted in the Discussion that the discrepancy between our numbers and those of Huber et al. could also be partially due to differences in action potential numbers (subsection “Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual threshold”).

However, as we point out in the text, this does not completely account for the difference as Huber et al. estimate ~300 action potentials are required to drive behaviour (independent of stimulus pattern, i.e. more action potentials in fewer neurons or fewer action potentials in more neurons), whereas our equivalent estimate would be ~140 action potentials (~10 action potentials in ~14 neurons). We have also added to the Results and Materials and methods the requested additional point that reducing LED power may reduce the probability of firing across a given set of neurons in addition to the number of neurons activated.

7) The title and the impact statement don't reflect the experiments. In particular, the volumetric stimulation with 1P optogenetics doesn't seem to evoke physiological responses. The authors propose that their results prove the lower bound of neurons sufficient to generate a percept, but all the literature cited define a percept as neuronal activity that represents specific features of sensory stimuli. For example, in the references cited hallucinations are defined as "perceptions in the absence of objectively identifiable stimuli. Hallucinations are percepts without corresponding external stimuli". (Corlett et al., 2019). According to such definition the authors are describing hallucinations induced by bulk 1P photostimulation and evoked by 2P activation of few neurons, to which the train the animals to respond. However the relation between neurons responding to 1P stimulation (phase 1 of training) and a sensory evoked percept is missing even though the authors have all the necessary tools to perform such experiments.

So, if the authors want to keep their claims about percepts, they should show the relation of the activity evoked by photostimulation and activity evoked by specific sensory stimuli. In other words, despite that all the references and framework of the manuscript postulate intervention experiments related to percepts, the authors never show the relation between sensory stimuli, targeted optogenetics and behavior. This could be addressed either by toning down the claims, and in particular change the title or demonstrate experimentally that the activity evoked by the targeted activation of neurons represents percepts. For example, a more appropriate title could be: "How many neurons are sufficient to detect direct cortical photostimulation?"

We understand the reviewers’ concerns about the connotations of the word “percept”. We have removed it from the text and reworded the title, therefore removing the explicit link between our results, sensory processing and percepts. While we retain our references to false sensory percepts and their role in hallucinations, we now clearly separate our results from the sensory component of these studies, instead clarifying our basic claim that there may be equivalences between the concept of erroneously detecting neural activity in the absence of either sensory or artificial stimuli (subsection “Activation of only a small number of neurons is required to reach perceptual threshold”). We do however continue to use the word “perception” in the context of behavioural detection, in accordance with its widespread use in similar papers driving behaviour with optogenetic and electrical stimulation of barrel cortex and olfactory bulb (Houweling and Brecht, 2008; Huber et al., 2008; Doron et al., 2014; Gill et al., 2020) which demonstrate that it can be used in similar contexts.

With respect to comparing sensory and optogenetic stimuli, we don’t expect our optogenetic stimuli to evoke physiological responses equivalent to sensory stimuli, nor did we intend to claim this. We apologise for this confusion. The original manuscript intended to report the psychometric curve relating “generalised cortical activity” to behaviour and to look at how this influenced the local network. It was not intended to directly relate this to sensory processing, nor do any of our experiments attempt or claim to do so. While this is an interesting question to pursue, we feel that it is beyond the scope of the present study. Furthermore, studies addressing similar issues have already been published by other groups (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Jennings et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019; Russell et al., 2019) and we believe that our results offer a set of important complementary insights. We now also reference this comparison in the Discussion (subsection “The network input-output function for perception is steep”).

8) Discussion, first paragraph. How the perceptual learning could generalize to the surrounding network? The observed result could be an artifact of a massive network reconfiguration due to 1P photostimulation at early phases of training. The 1P stimulation protocol, which precedes 2P stimulation, may artificially increase connectivity among opsin-expressing neurons and this may explain why the effect of 2P photostimulation on behavior is independent on the identity of stimulated cells (Figure 2—figure supplement 3D). If the network is reconfigured then the animals learned to detect such artificial network or parts of it. Comparing spontaneous activity before and after photostimulation entraining is necessary to clarify this point, and this alternative explanation should be mentioned in the text, especially because the authors seem to favor the opposite scenario, i.e. that "the neurons we targeted probably do no benefit from such recurrent excitation".

We agree that strengthened recurrent connectivity between opsin expressing neurons induced during 1P training could be a potential explanation for the generalisability of learning. However, since we did not record network activity before and after 1P training it is not possible to explore changes in functional connectivity as suggested. We have now highlighted this in the Discussion (subsection “The network input-output function for perception is steep”), also emphasising in detail the interpretation of our generalisability result that the reviewers mention and exploring how it relates to other similar studies using more naturalistic sensory stimuli (subsection “The perceptual threshold is plastic and can generalise”). We have also added a caveat to this effect to the new version of the sentence referred to by the reviewers (subsection “Perception is sensitive despite matched network suppression”). We don’t necessarily see this interpretation as problematic since studies driving behaviour with sensory stimuli/optogenetic stimuli mimicking sensory stimuli often cite such recurrent connectivity as a mechanism for improving performance in behavioural tasks (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Jennings et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019; Russell et al., 2019) and indeed one has explicitly suggested that such recurrence allows rapid transfer learning between neurons sharing functional tuning with neurons that have previously been trained (Marshel et al., 2019). This idea is therefore not specific to the artificial stimuli that we deliver, though as pointed out by the reviewers in our hands it would be set up by artificial generation of neural ensembles (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2018). Indeed we favour this interpretation in the revised manuscript as it would offer a functionally grounded account of how the similarity between stimuli, and the ensembles that encode them, could facilitate rapid transfer of learning. We have also cited an intriguing recent preprint suggesting that repeated optogenetic stimulation of populations of opsin-expressing neurons does not actually increase the strength of recurrent connections between them (Alejandre-García et al., 2020), instead resulting in an increase in intrinsic excitability that unmasks existing weak synaptic connections. This suggests that 1P optogenetic training may not induce the formation of new connections between opsin expressing neurons, but would have to rely on existing connections. Given that during the 2P psychometric curve experiments (when we test for changes in the detectability of small numbers of neurons) we move FOVs by several hundred microns between training days it is likely that the probability of existing excitatory connectivity between the random sets of neurons targeted across days would be quite low. Nevertheless, such subtle intrinsic excitability-induced unmasking of weak synapses could also mediate a recurrent excitation-based explanation for the transfer learning that we observe.

9) The main conclusion of the study is that a small number (~ 13) of layer II/III neurons in S1 needs to be stimulated in order to generate a percept. However, this number may strongly depend, among other things, on: i) the number of neurons effectively activated by the 2P stimulation, which is potentially underestimated because of the inability of the calcium indicator to read out small increases in spiking activity under the imaging conditions used in this study; ii) the number of 2P stimulation sessions that animals undergo; iii) the duration and intensity of the 1P stimulation paradigm which may induce different levels of increased connectivity in the stimulated neurons depending on the stimulation conditions; iv) the small number of planes imaged. v) the intensity/duration of the 2P stimulation (e.g., stimulation of 7 neurons with the protocol used in this study may not result in the generation of a behavioral percept, but stronger/longer stimulation of the same 7 neurons might). These issues should be clearly stated in the Discussion, because the authors' estimate on the absolute number of required neurons for generating a percept could strongly depend on their specific experimental conditions and could not necessarily generalize to S1 under any condition.

We completely agree, and we have explored this in detail in the Discussion: (subsection “The network input-output function for perception is steep”).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Dalgleish HWP, Russell LE, Gauld OM, Packer AM, Hausser M. 2020. How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity? figshare. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    2. Dalgleish HWP, Russell LE, Gauld OM, Packer AM, Hausser M. 2020. How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity? figshare. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Import, processing, analysis and figure code is available on Github (Dalgleish, 2020; https://github.com/alloptical/Dalgleish-eLife-2020) for use with analysed data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13135505) and/or unprocessed behavioural session data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13128950). Raw calcium imaging movies are ~1 TB in size and are thus available upon reasonable request.

    Import, processing, analysis and figure code is available on Github (Dalgleish, 2020; https://github.com/alloptical/Dalgleish-eLife-2020) (copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:5768e890be9fc6428beea16720e488ebd867da67/) for use with analysed data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13135505) and/or unprocessed behavioural session data (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13128950). Raw calcium imaging movies are ~1TB in size and are thus available upon reasonable request.

    The following datasets were generated:

    Dalgleish HWP, Russell LE, Gauld OM, Packer AM, Hausser M. 2020. How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity? figshare.

    Dalgleish HWP, Russell LE, Gauld OM, Packer AM, Hausser M. 2020. How many neurons are sufficient for perception of cortical activity? figshare.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES