Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Nov 30;15(11):e0243024. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243024

Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about COVID-19 in Ethiopia:  A need to address information gaps and enable combating efforts

Yohannes Kebede 1,*, Zewdie Birhanu 1, Diriba Fufa 2, Yimenu Yitayih 3, Jemal Abafita 4, Ashenafi Belay 5, Abera Jote 6, Argaw Ambelu 7
Editor: Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi8
PMCID: PMC7703946  PMID: 33253268

Abstract

Background

The endeavor to tackle the spread of COVID-19 effectively remains futile without the right grasp of perceptions and beliefs presiding in the community. Therefore, this study aimed to assess myths, beliefs, perceptions, and information gaps about COVID-19 in Ethiopia.

Methods

An internet-based survey was conducted in Ethiopia from April 22 to May 04, 2020. The survey link was promoted through emails, social media, and the Jimma University website. Perceptions about COVID-19 have considered the World Health Organization (WHO) resources and local beliefs. The data were analyzed using Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS) software version 20.0. Classifications and lists of factors for each thematic perception of facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs were generated. Explanatory factor analysis (EFA) was executed to assist categorizations. Standardized mean scores of the categories were compared using analysis of variance (ANOVA) and t-tests. A significant difference was claimed at p-value <0.05.

Results

A total of 929 responses were gathered during the study period. The EFA generated two main categories of perceived facilitators of COVID-19 spread: behavioral non-adherence (55.9%) and lack of enablers (86.5%). Behavioral non-adherence was illustrated by fear of stigma (62.9%), not seeking care (59.3%), and hugging and shaking (44.8%). Perceived lack of enablers of precautionary measures includes staying home impossible due to economic challenges (92.4%), overcrowding (87.6%), and inaccessible face masks (81.6%) and hand sanitizers (79.1%). Perceived inhibitors were categorized into three factors: two misperceived, myths (31.6%) and false assurances (32.9%), and one correctly identified; engagement in standard precautions (17.1%). Myths about protection from the virus involve perceived religiosity and effectiveness of selected food items, hot weather, traditional medicine, and alcohol drinking, ranging from 15.1% to 54.7%. False assurances include people’s perception that they were living far away from areas where COVID-19 was rampant (36.9%), and no locally reported cases were present (29.5%). There were tremendous information needs reported about COVID-19 concerning protection methods (62.6%), illness behavior and treatment (59.5%), and quality information, including responses to key unanswered questions such as the origin of the virus (2.4%). Health workers were perceived as the most at-risk group (83.3%). The children, adolescents, youths were marked at low to moderate (45.1%-62.2%) risk of COVID-19. Regional, township, and access to communication showed significant variations in myths, false assurances, and information needs (p <0.05).

Conclusions

Considering young population as being at low risk of COVID-19 would be challenging to the control efforts, and needs special attention. Risk communication and community engagement efforts should consider regional and township variations of myths and false assurances. It should also need to satisfy information needs, design local initiatives that enhance community ownership of the control of the virus, and thereby support engagement in standard precautionary measures. All forms of media should be properly used and regulated to disseminate credible information while filtering out myths and falsehoods.

Introduction

The novel-coronavirus disease 2019 abbreviated as COVID-19 is currently a pandemic as declared by the World Health Organization (WHO) on January 30, 2020 [1]. The outbreak was first reported in late December 2019, when clusters of pneumonia cases of unknown etiology were found to be associated with epidemiologically linked exposure to the seafood market and untraced exposures in the city of Wuhan of China [2, 3].

The disease is highly infectious, and its main clinical symptoms include fever, dry cough, fatigue, myalgia, and dyspnea. Globally, 1 in 6 of the patients with COVID-19 develops to the severe stage, which is characterized by acute respiratory distress syndrome, septic shock, difficult-to-tackle metabolic acidosis, and bleeding and coagulation dysfunction [4, 5]. Epidemiologically, the distribution of the disease is exponentially growing across the globe. For example, on this date, June 9, 2020, the pandemics registered 7, 216,252 cases, and 409, 092 deaths in the world. Of the 3, 961,425 closed cases (10%) ended up in deaths. Ethiopia has become one of the COVID-19 affected countries as of March 12, the date on which one imported case was first detected. Since then, the infection by the virus has kept mounting. For example, on June 5, 2020, there were 2,152 total notified cases and 27 deaths in Ethiopia [1, 6, 7].

According to the WHO reports, the COVID-19 has no effective cure, yet early recognition of symptoms and timely seeking of supportive care and preventive practices enhance recovery from the illness and combat the spread of the virus. Older men with medical comorbidities are more likely to get infected, with worse outcomes [810]. Available evidence has shown that the virus spreads from human-to-human mainly through respiratory droplets and body contacts. Contact with contaminated surfaces, hands, and touching of faces-eye-nose-mouth are predominant ways to get exposed to the infected droplets [1114].

The battle against COVID-19 continues in Ethiopia. To guarantee the final success of stopping the virus, understanding myths and perceptions are so vital. Some questions require answers. For example, who do people think that they are most at risk? What are community perspectives about factors that facilitate the spread of the virus? What about perceived inhibitors? How scientifically accepted are these perceptions? Are the perceived facilitators or inhibitors correct or misperceived? Do people own the responsibility to fight the virus or externalize it? The answers to the above questions are of paramount importance to curb the pandemic by enhancing the probability of people’s practicing the necessary precautions. Standard precautionary measures include avoidance of contact with surfaces, keeping physical distance, hand hygiene, respiratory hygiene, using sanitizers, and protective pieces of equipment [1214].

The WHO recommends the risk communication and community engagement efforts to investigate and control “infodemics”, myths, beliefs, and stigma so that the spread of the coronavirus would be effectively combated [10, 15, 16]. For example, the WHO reported risk perception, drinking alcohol, hot weather, and antibiotics related myths on COVID-19. Moreover, up-to-date information regarding causes, means of protection, modes of transmissions, diagnostic symptoms, and treatment/isolation procedures are relevant to withstand myths, beliefs, perceptions and support preventive efforts [12, 15, 17, 18].

The public health importance of COVID-19 has been recognized by the government of Ethiopia. There are movements to decentralize screening opportunities, quarantine, and treatment centers, and promoting precautionary measures. At the moment of the study, the government declared a state emergency in support of the precautionary measures, and has taken public measures such as the closure of schools, including universities; worked with public service outlets to install locally available preventive technologies, including handwashing machines; limiting the number of passengers in public transport, among others. Moreover, the ministry of health engaged in public awareness creation, risk communication, and community engagement tasks, and rallying voluntary activities. Now, addressing community beliefs, perceptions, and information gaps would reinforce the efforts to stop the virus. Therefore, this study aimed to assess community myths, beliefs, perceptions, and information needs via an online nationwide survey in Ethiopia.

Methods and materials

Study settings and designs

An internet-based online cross-sectional study was conducted in all regions of Ethiopia. At the time of the study, administratively, Ethiopia is divided into nine regional states and two federal cities. The regions have zonal divisions and district sub-divisions, with respective regional capitals and zonal/district towns. Internet services are rarely accessible at the district level. The Ethiopia 2020 population is estimated at 114,963,588 people in mid-year according to UN data. 21.3% of the population is urban (24,463,423 people in 2020). The median age in Ethiopia is 19.5 years [19]. The Ethiopian 2020 average literacy rate is 49.1% (lower among adults: male, 57.2; female, 41.1%, and higher among youths: male, 71.1%; female,67.8%) [2021]. The survey tool was created through Google Form and the survey link was promoted through e-mail communications, social media (Facebook and LinkedIn), and the Jimma University website. The survey link was shared on April 22, 2020, and the responses were collected until May 04, 2020.

Measurement and operationalization

The beliefs and perceptions tool about the spread and control of the virus were partly adapted from WHO resources [10, 15, 16, 22, 23]. Additionally, open-ended options were addressed to participants to report local beliefs and perceptions regarding COVID-19. Overall, four main themes of perceptions were asked: perceived facilitators for the spread of the virus (9 items), perceived inhibitors (9 items), information needs (7 items), risk labeling (8 items), access to communication resources (7 items), and socio-demographic variables, including residential regions and townships. Access to communication channels/platforms was measured by a score between 1 and 7 made on counting ownership or follow-up of television, official websites, social media, health workers, radio, friends/neighbors, and internet services/Wi-Fi. Townships referred to big towns/cities (regional/national capitals), zonal-level towns, and district-level (semi-urban/rural) towns. Perceived facilitators refer to people’s perception of factors exacerbating the spread of COVID-19, while perceived inhibitors refer to people’s misperceived or correctly perceived of factors that slow down the virus. These themes of perceptions were further categorized into a group of factors using explanatory factor analysis (EFA). A factor loading score of 0.4 was used as a cutoff value to retain items in each category [24]. Kaiser Mayer Olkin’s (KMO>50%) indicated that the sample was adequate for executing EFA [24].

Data analysis

Participants’ online responses were first encoded on an Excel database and later exported to SPSS version 20.0 for analysis. Respondents’ background variables and specific belief items are presented in the frequency tables. Standardized mean scores (0–100) and standard deviations were used to describe lists of categories of factors according to themes of perceptions they belonged to. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and t-test were computed to compare the mean differences by region, township, and access to communication. A multi-response analysis was performed for every perception. A 95% confidence interval and a p-value of less than 0.05 were used to claim statistically significant association.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Jimma University, institutional review board approved the study. A reference number is IRB 00097/20.

Results

Socio-demographic characteristics of participants

A total of 929 participants from all regions of Ethiopia responded to this online survey questionnaire. Table 1 presents background information of the survey respondents. A majority of the respondents were in the age range of 30–39 years (50.8%), from Zonal towns (56.0%), and the Oromia region (56.6%).

Table 1. Selected demographic characteristics of respondents, May 2020, Ethiopia.

Variables Response category Frequency Percentage
Age in years 18–29 285 30.7
30–39 472 50.8
> = 40 172 18.5
Gender Male 828 89.1
Female 101 10.9
Religion Orthodox 417 44.9
Protestant 336 36.2
Muslim 114 12.3
Others 62 6.7
Township Big (regional capitals/national) city 319 34.3
Zonal level town 520 56.0
District level/Semi-urban/town 90 9.7
Region Oromia 526 56.6
Addis Ababa 139 15.0
SNNP* 103 11.1
Amhara 52 5.6
Tigrai 49 5.3
Other regions 60 6.5

*SNNP: Southern Nations and Nationalities People

Perceived facilitating factors: How do people think about the spread of COVID-19?

Classifications of facilitators

Table 2 presents categories and lists of perceived facilitators of COVID-19 spread with their respective prevalence. Explanatory factor analysis (EFA) produced two principal categories of perceived factors exacerbating COVID-19 in Ethiopia. The first category of factors was labeled as behavioral adherence, indicating that non-adherence to expected precautions is facilitating the virus; needing behavioral and social change. The items that contributed to behavioral non-adherence include that people still shake each other’s hands, do not seek care for symptoms suggestive of COVID-19, use crowded transport means, are not being screened for flu-like symptoms, and fear of stigma with respective decreasing order of factor loading scores (0.714–0.503). The second category of perceived facilitating factors was the lack of enabling environmental conditions that are supposed to support adaptations of precautionary measures. The lack of enablers was made up of economic reasons that challenge stay at home principle, overcrowded living/working conditions, absence of PPE like face masks, and sanitizers with decreasing order of factor loading scores (0.786–0.718). The behavioral non-adherence and lack of enablers related factors explained an overall variance of perceived facilitators of the virus by 48.8%.

Table 2. Perceived categories and lists of exacerbating factors of COVID-19, May 2020, Ethiopia.
Perceived COVID-19 exacerbating factors Principal components and factor loading score Descriptive statistics
Behavioral non- adherence Lack of enabling environment Freq. % (95% CI)
People fear stigma and bias related to COVID-19 .503 584 62.9 (59.7,65.9)
People still use crowded transportation means .654 562 60.5 (57.4,63.3)
People with flu-like symptoms are not well screened for COVID-19 .638 551 59.3 (56.1, 62.5)
People do not often seek care for symptoms that looks like COVID-19 .681 481 51.8 (48.7, 55.1)
People still hug and shake each other’s hands while greeting .714 416 44.8 (41.5. 47.8)
People do not stay at home for economic and social reasons .786 858 92.4 (90.6,94.2)
People still live and work in a very crowded condition .705 814 87.6 (85.4, 89.6)
People do not have PPE like face masks .727 758 81.6 (78.9, 84.0)
People do not have hand rub alcohol or sanitizers .718 735 79.1 (76.3,81.6)

Notes: KMO = 81.9%); Variance Explained (VE = 48.8%); and PPE: Personal Protective Equipment.

Prevalence of facilitators

Descriptive statistics columns in Table 2 indicate the prevalence of specific factors in categories of facilitators they belonged to. Accordingly, the prevalence of specific factors that contributed to behavioral non-adherence ranged between 584 (62.9%) and 416 (44.8%). The fear of stigma and people’s continued use of suffocated transport means accounted for a higher extent of non-adherences. The magnitudes of lack of enablers that would support behavioral adherence range between 858 (92.4%) and 735 (79.1%). Staying at home is impossible for economic reasons (92.4%) and living/working in overcrowded conditions accounted for a major share of the lack of enablers. Deterring environmental conditions were perceived at a higher prevalence than behavioral non-adherence, indicating a high tendency of externalizing factors that could aggravate a spread of COVID-19 in the community. There were 53 (5.7%, 95%CI:4.3%-7.4%) factors reported by respondents as unknown.

Perceived inhibiting factors: How do people think about the slow down of COVID-19?

Classifications of inhibitors

Table 3 presents categories and lists of perceived inhibitors of COVID-19 spread with their respective prevalence. EFA produced three principal categories of perceived factors inhibiting COVID-19. Two of the three categories were misperceived (myths and false assurances), while one was correctly perceived inhibitor. The myths category was composed of factors that are believed to inhibit the virus without having been scientifically proven. In this case, the myths include: eating selected foods (garlic, onion, ginger, etc) for prevention and cure; perceived religiosity (perceiving oneself as an effective religious man/woman in controlling challenges); drinking alcohol; people’s perceived confidence that they owned effective traditional medicines that were, however, not clinically confirmed; and living in hot weather. The factor loading scores in respective order ranged between 0.764–0.488. The second category of perceived inhibitors was still local sayings that were often related to false assurances that people were protected from COVID-19 (unlike myths, the second category of beliefs may not need scientific approval or disapproval). The category consisted of two main beliefs: “we live far away from COVID-19 rampant areas” and “there are no locally reported COVID-19 cases so far”, with factor loading scores (0.770–0.661). The beliefs looked false assurances in that people perceive themselves as living out of a risk zone that is an impression of invulnerability. The third, correct, and promotable category of perceived inhibitors was a single item about people having been engaged in standard precautions (factor score loading = 0.775). Factors related to the above three categories explained an overall variance of perceived inhibitors by 54.6%, indicating the presence of several other unreported myths and unhealthy beliefs that need further assessment.

Table 3. Perceived categories and lists of inhibiting factors of COVID-19, May 2020, Ethiopia.
Perceived COVID-19 inhibiting factors Principal components and factor loading scores Descriptive statistics
Myths Invulnerability (false assurances) Engaged in precautions Freq. % (95% CI)
We are religious enough to control COVID-19 .496 508 54.7 (51.5, 58.0)
We are eating garlic, onion, honey among others to prevent COVID-19 .764 455 49.0 (45.7, 54.3)
The weather we live-in is too hot for coronavirus to survive .488 242 26.0 (23.6, 29.1)
We are eating garlic, onion, honey among others to cure COVI-19 .728 227 24.4 (21.6, 27.2)
We believe we have traditional medicine against COVID-19 .511 165 17.8 (15.5, 20.3)
We are drinking alcohol to protect against COVID-19 .676 140 15.1 (12.9, 17.3)
There are no locally reported COVID-19 cases so far .770 343 36.9 (33.8, 39.7)
We live far away from COVID-19’s rampant areas .661 274 29.5 (26.8, 32.4)
Engaged in standard precautions measures of COVID-19 .775 159 17.1 (14.9, 19.7)

Notes: KMO = 77.3%, Variance explained (VE = 54.6%)

Prevalence of inhibitors

Descriptive statistics columns in Table 3 indicate the prevalence of perceived inhibitors. Myths and false assurances were the most prevalent perceived inhibitors of the spread of COVID-19 compared to the perception that engagement in precautionary measures protect from exposure to and spread of the virus. Specifically, perceived religiosity, effectiveness of selected foods, and perceived protectiveness of hot weather were the commonest myths, accounting for 508 (54.7%), 455 (49.0%), and 242 (26.0%), respectively. Beliefs that there were no locally reported cases of COVID-19, and the specific localities where respondents are currently living are far away from coronavirus rampant areas contributed to 343 (36.9%) and 274 (29.5%) respective prevalence of false assurances. On the other hand, the prevalence of a perception that the spread of COVID-19 would be controlled as a result of people’s active engagement in standard precautionary measures was as low as 159 (17.1%). Overall, false beliefs and myths were more rampant than accurate perceptions about factors that potentially inhibit the spread of COVID-19. About153 (16.5%, 95%CI:14.2–18.8%) respondents reported that they were unsure of other factors which potentially inhibit the distribution of COVID-19 given the virus is newly introduced

Perceived information needs: What do people want to learn more about COVID-19?

Classifications of information needs

Table 4 presents the information needs of the community concerning COVID-19. The EFA generated four categories of information needs. The first category of information needs was related to prevention that is composed of how to surely protect from the virus, exhaustive transmission modes, and distinguishable symptoms. The factor loading score ranged from 0.816–0.842. The second category was related to illness and treatment. Specifically, in this category, people want to know about the nature of the treatment, details about isolation and quarantine, what to do when at risk or as a high-risk group, and procedures to follow when symptomatic (factor loading range, 0.534–0.786). The third category was related to quality, including true and up-to-date, and change provoking information. The fourth was diverse information needs, ranging from the need to know about the readiness of the health facility to confirmation of the origin of the virus.

Table 4. Perceived categories and lists of information needs about COVID-19, May 2020, Ethiopia.
Perceived information need factors about COVID-19 Principal components and factor loadings scores Descriptive statistics
Preventive Illness and treatment Quality information Diverse questions Freq. % (95% CI)
How to protect from COVID-19 .816 605 65.2 (62.2, 68.2)
Exhaustive transmission modes .839 554 59.6 (56.3, 62.9)
Distinguishable symptoms .842 529 56.9 (53.9, 60.3)
Details on isolation and quarantine .683 611 65.8 (62.8, 68.9)
What to do when they or someone become symptomatic (illness behavior) .534 581 62.5 (59.3, 65.7)
Nature and process of treatment .786 552 59.4 (56.4, 62.4)
What to do with risk factors or as a risk group .587 412 44.3 (41.1, 47.6)
Change provoking information** .643 27 2.9 (1.8, 4.1)
True and update information .867 12 1.3 (0.5, 2.0)
Diverse information needs* .907 14 1.5 (0.6, 2.2)

Notes: Kaiser Mayer Olkin’s measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 80.5%), Variance explained (VE = 65.4%).

* Diverse information need: learn about capacity and readiness of the health facilities to manage in transmission peaks, costs related to treatment services, community screening service, want to differentiate the origin of the disease itself as to whether it is a Wrath of the Creator or biological weapon, need praying, among others.

** Change provoking information: bridging knowledge to behavior change, Alleviation of reluctance to precautions, messages involving a specific audience, increasing vulnerability perception, repeatedly accessing with messages, enforcement of laws that save guard lives, implementations of command posts in favor of combating COVID-19, how the jobless can be economically supported, where to get sanitizers, among others.

Magnitude of information needs. Descriptive statistics columns in Table 4 indicate the prevalence of the information needs according to their respective categories. For example, the magnitude of people who need to prevent the virus by knowing mechanisms of protection, exhaustive transmission modes, and diagnostic symptoms were 605 (65.2%), 554 (59.6%), and 529 (56.9%), respectively. The highest information needs about COVID-19 was related to illness behavior and treatment, for example, isolation and quarantine accounted for 611 (65.8%). In terms of quality information, about 27 (2.9%) of people needed to know about how to alleviate community reluctance. There were mixed communication needs, 14 (1.5%).

Perceived risk labeling: Who is perceived to be more vulnerable?

Table 5 presents COVID-19 risk labels and groups. The study showed that 656 (70.8%, 95% CI: 68.0%, 73.1%) of the community felt COVID-19 as a dangerous disease. The perception of vulnerability to an infection of COVID-19 looked somewhat lower, 536 (57.8%, 95% CI: 54.6%, 61.1%). The community perceived that health workers (83.2%), people with underlying illnesses (78.8%), and elderly people (76.3%) are at high-risk of COVID-19. Age ranges between 0–30 years old were classified into low-moderate risk (45.1–62.2%).

Table 5. Perceived COVID-19 risk groups and labels, May 2020, Ethiopia.

Perceived high-risk groups Descriptive statistics
Freq. % (95% CI)
Health workers 773 83.2 (80.7, 85.7)
People with underlying illness conditions 732 78.8 (76.1, 81.4)
Elderly people 709 76.3 (73.6, 78.9)
Adults (30–50 years old) 597 64.3 (60.9, 67.3)
Youth (16–29 years old) 578 62.2(59.1, 65.2)
Pregnant women 552 59.4 (56.5. 62.5)
Adolescents (10–15 years old) 448 48.2 (45.0,51.3)
Children (0–9 years old) 419 45.1 (41.9,48.3)

Description of overall perceptions of facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs

The above mentioned specific beliefs about inhibitors, facilitators, and information needs were merged based on categories the items belonged to (as referred to in Tables 24). Standardized means scores ranging from 0–100 were calculated for all categories of perceptions and information needs. Table 6 provides the details of overall standardized mean scores and regional ranges. Without noting significant variations in regions, there was high (59.5%) perceived nationwide information needs about illness behavior and treatment procedures (p = 0.317). Likewise, the lack of enablers and behavioral non-adherence that were perceived as facilitators of the spread of the virus were high, 86.5% (p = 0.262) and 55.9% (p = 0.323), respectively.

Table 6. Descriptive statistics and regional ranges for perceptions and needs, May 2020, Ethiopia.

Beliefs and information need categories Median %mean(±SD) Regional ranges p-value
Perceived facilitators (overall) 66.7 69.5 (±15.6) 62.8–73.5 0.239
Behavioral non-adherence 60.0 55.9 (±11.2) 49.0–61.0 0.323
Lack of enabling conditions 85.5 86.5 (±6.5) 80.1–89.2 0.262
Perceived inhibitors (overall)** - - - -
Misperceived inhibitor: Myths 33.3 31.6 (±11.2) 24.8–36.9 0.002*
Misperceived inhibitor: False assurance 36.3 32.9 (±4.6) 25.5–49.5 <0.001*
Engagement in standard precautions 17.0 17.1 (±2.5) 6.7–22.5 0.146
Information need (overall)*** 58.3 59.3 (±3.4) 52.4–65.3 0.031*
Prevention related 66.7 62.6 (±8.1) 50.6–66.7 0.021*
Illness and treatment-related 53.2 59.5 (±8.9) 53.1–63.6 0.317
Quality information 3.6 2.4 (±1.4) 0.0–2.4 0.590
Mixed information need 1.5 1.7 (±0.8) 1.1–4.1 0.443

* Statistically significant at p <0.05 (two-tailed)

**Overall perceived inhibitor has two misperceived (myths and false assurances) and one correctly perceived (engaged in standard precautions) aspect, needing no further merging for an overall score.

***The overall mean of information needs to exclude the two dimensions-quality and mixed needs because of extreme values.

Spatial distributions of the perceptions: variations by regions and townships

Regional distribution and variation

One-way ANOVA showed significant regional differences, particularly on factors perceived to inhibit the spread of the virus and information needs. Specifically, the variations were on myths (F = 3.75, p = 0.002), false assurances (F = 6.57, p <0.001), and overall (F = 2.48, p = 0.031) and preventive information needs (F = 2.68, p = 0.021). Moreover, Fig 1 shows a specific regional concentration of the perceptions about the spread and control of the virus. Accordingly, a slight but significant higher prevalence of myths was observed in Addis Ababa compared to Tigrai and Oromia regions, with MD(95%CI) of 13.4 (1.0,24.9%) and 9.1 (1.3,16.9%). There were higher scores false assurances (an impression of invulnerability) in the Southern region compared to Oromia and Addis Ababa. The variation ranged 18.7% (7.4–30.0%, p <0.001) and 24.0% (10.3%-37.6%, p<0.001) with Oromia and Addis Ababa, respectively. Information needs were highest in Southern and Oromia regions compared to Addis Ababa, with respective MD = 16.4, p = 0.038, and 12.4, p = 0.028.

Fig 1. Diagram of regional distribution of perceptions about COVID-19, May 2020, Ethiopia.

Fig 1

Township distribution and variation

Respondents’ township showed significant differences in myths (F = 10.62, p <0.001), overall information need (F = 6.91, p = 0.001), and particularly preventive information (F = 5.23, p = 0.006), Fig 2 shows diagrammatic township distribution of the perceptions concerning the virus. Hence, myths that are perceived to inhibit the spread of the virus were more prevalent in big cities/towns including Addis Ababa compared to the zonal (MD (95CI%) = 8.8% (4.0–13.6%), p <0.001), and district/semi-urban (MD = 9.4% (1.4–17.4%, p = 0.015) towns. Community residing in the zonal and district towns felt that there was higher information need in their community, particularly about protection ways compared to big towns/cities, with respective MD of 8.3% (1.2–15.4%, p = 0.015) and 16.4% (4.5–28.3%, p = 0.003).

Fig 2. Diagram of township distribution of perceptions about COVID-19, May 2020, Ethiopia.

Fig 2

Communication resources and perceptions

Fig 3. presents variations of COVID-19 related perceptions by the number of communication sources accessed. One-way ANOVA revealed significant differences in mean scores of perceived facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs about a spread and control of COVID-19 by the number of a mix of communication channels accessed. Specifically, Overall perceived facilitators (F = 3.40, p = 0.03), behavioral non-adherence (F = 3.47, p = 0.002), myths (F = 5.01, p <0.001), false assurances (F = 2.2, p = 0.042), engagement in precautions (F = 2.40, P = 0.029), and overall information need (F = 2.31, p = 0.032). Access to television, official websites, social media, health workers, radio, friends/neighbors, and internet/Wi-Fi platforms/channels scored. Accordingly, for most of the variables with significant differences by communication sources, access to only 1 or2 sources led to lower means of perceptions compared to access to 6 sources. This indicated that the number of communication channels accessed may not be as important as the quality of messages they carried in affecting information needs and beliefs.

Fig 3. Diagram of distribution of perceptions by access to communication platforms, May 2020, Ethiopia.

Fig 3

Perception of threat and perceived facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs

Community perception of threat (the result effect of perception of susceptibility to a dangerous virus) from COVID-19 showed a statistically significant mean difference (MD) in scores of perceived lack of enabling environment that facilitate a spread of the virus (MD (95% CI) = 3.43 (0.11,6.77), p = 0.043), presence of myths (MD (95%CI) = 4.75(1.13,8.36), p = 0.010), perceived engagement in standard precautions (MD (95% CI) = 12.15(7.35,16.94), p <0.001), overall information needs (MD (95%CI = 4.47(1.67,7.27), p = 0.002), preventive information need (MD (95%CI) = 6.26(2.75,11.65), p = 0.023), and treatment procedures related information needs (MD (95%CI) = 7.10(2.77,211.41), p = 0.001).

Discussion

This online survey has generated pertinent findings of nationwide community perceptions concerning factors that facilitate and inhibit a spread of COVID-19, risk labeling, and information needs in Ethiopia. The perceived factors were aligned into the following main categories: behavioral adherence, lack of enabling environmental conditions, myths, false assurances, engagement in standard precautions, and information needs about prevention, illness behavior and treatment, including answers to diverse questions related to the origin, a spread and control of the coronavirus. Each perceived factor was discussed step by step as follows:

This study found a moderate perception of severity by the community, 70.8%, while, somewhat low perceived vulnerability, 57.8%. This indicates the community’s perception of risk should be increased further. The perceptions were measured by a single item for each. There were two forms of risk labeling and groups in the community: As perceived by the community, young people below 30 were perceived as a low-moderate risk with an increasing order: 0–9 years old (45.1%), 10–15 years old (48.2%), 16–29 years old (62.2%), and 30–50 years old (64.3%). Health workers, people with underlying illnesses, and the elderly were perceived as high-risk groups with the respective prevalence of 83.2%, 78.8%, and 76.3%. Perhaps, the high-risk groups perceived by the community, in this study, were consistent with that of the WHO. According to WHO, frontline health workers, people with underlying illness, and elderly people are high-risk groups [22, 23]. The correct perception of the high-risk group is important for giving protection priorities against infection by COVID-19. However, this study reported that children, adolescents and youths were relatively perceived as lower risk groups (45.1%, 48.2%, and 62.2%, respectively). This would be concerning to the control efforts to some extent. We argue that those who were perceived as being at low-risk would act as reservoirs for a spread of COVID-19 for a couple of reasons: one, about 63% of the Ethiopian population aged < 25, with a median age of 19.5, and these segments pass time searching for jobs like daily labors [20, 21]. Two, in one of the previous studies conducted in Ethiopia, 179 (72.5%) of respondents knew that the elderly and people with underlying illnesses are high-risk groups, while only 15 (6.1%) knew that young adult people must engage in precautions just like any other segment [25] Therefore, some enforcement needs to control a potential contribution of youths in the transmission loop as the current perception of risk groups stands.

Factors that were perceived to exacerbate the spread of the virus were teamed up into two thematic categories: behavioral non-adherence (55.9%), and lack of enabling environmental conditions (86.5%). Behavioral non-adherence, in this case, referred to individuals and social ignorance, disregard, and lack of commitment to convert standard precautionary measures that seem to be under the control without needing much material support. The ignorance and lack of commitment were illustrated by the following community’s experiences: people still hug each other and shake hands while greeting, do not often seek care while showing symptoms that look like COVID-19, still feel comfortable to use crowded unventilated transport means, and fear stigma-related to the virus. Interestingly, the use of crowded/unventilated transport means was not only due to lack, but rather it also was involved in behavioral non-adherence. Theoretically, people often rationalize their engagement in preventive actions, and rationalities should be carefully studied and justified [26]. On the other hand, lack of enabling environments is about condition and resource factors whose presence or absence enable people to take precautionary actions. Some of them can be illustrated as such people cannot stay at home for economic and social reasons, do not have personal protective equipment (PPE) like face masks, do not have hand-rub alcohol or sanitizers, and still live and work in crowded condition. In this study, the magnitudes of both behavioral non-adherence and perceived lack of environmental conditions were high, irrespective of regions and townships. Behavioral and communication theories indicate that people’s perceived lack of resources negatively affects actual practices [26]. Nonetheless, the high prevalence of perceived facilitators signals two main urgencies. One, it suggests strong work to alleviate behavioral non-adherence, and lack of enablers that facilitate the spread of the virus. Two, even a higher perceived lack of enabling conditions looks concerning given that it may lead people to externalize the capacity to control the virus, while ignoring to their personal efforts. Thus, to convert this perception into opportunity, local initiatives that support engagement in standard precautions should address the locally perceived barriers, and enhance a shared responsibility and community ownership to involve in efforts of combating COVID-19 [10].

Factors that were perceived as inhibitors of the spread of the virus were classified into three: false assurances (32.9%), myths (31.6%), and engagement in standard precautions (17.1%). Interestingly, the first two of the three factors were wrongly perceived inhibitors, that was why we labeled them myths and false assurances. False assurances were impressions of invulnerablities, and characterized by people’s perception that they were living out of the COVID-19 risk zone. In the current study, the two main false assurances were the perceived absence of locally reported COVID-19 cases and residence far away from COVID-19 rampant areas. One study from the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia presented walking through sanitized gates could give a false sense of protection and potentially deceit the passersby from taking the recommended preventive actions [27]. In the current study, myths include: perceived effectiveness of religiosity (54.6%), food items (49.0%), living in hot weather (26.0%), traditional medicines (17.8%), and drinking alcohol (15.1%) to protect from COVID-19. WHO myth busters list out most of the misperceptions presented in this study, indicating that these were globally shared altogether with the pandemic [15]. Pieces of evidence indicate that myths or misperceptions like denial of the presence, and misperceived causes, transmissions modes, and protection ways can set back preventive and control efforts in times of the pandemics of HIV, Zika virus, Yellow fever, and Ebola, unless traced and addressed [2831]. The magnitude of the correctly perceived factor (engagement in standard precautionary measures) for inhibiting the spread of COVID-19 was too low (17.1%), demanding hard work to promote this perception until a larger segment of the community embraces an accurate reason for protection from the virus.

The finding from the current study revealed that the majority of the information needs were related to protection methods that are symptoms, mode of transmission and prevention (56.9%-65.2%), and procedures to be followed when someone feels ill from COVID-19 or at risk of contracting it, including isolation, quarantine, and treatment (44.3–65.8%). Particularly, people want to access information about isolation and quarantine–how it works (65.8%), and what to do when someone becomes symptomatic (65.2%). One study in 2018 on health information needs during the outbreak of Ebola showed that there was a need to an uninterrupted access to an up-to-date information including about causes, transmission modes, cures, the readiness of health facilities, and even the role of government [31]. Some studies related to illness behavior and drug repurposing from Pakistan and Saudi Arabia revealed that misinterpretation or misinformation (less quality or inadequate) about treatment/medicines that were delivered by press, electronic and social media has been leading to self-medication by chloroquine, hydroxychloroquine, and Ivermectin as COVID-19 cure [32, 33]. Interestingly, though minor proportion, there were people who sought quality and change provoking information that is true, up-to-date, how it is possible to alleviate ignorances that exist in the community regarding the adaptation of precautions of COVID-19, at the presence of basic knowledge. Cognitive dissonance theory recommends audience-specific messages that satisfy the information needs to close the gaps between knowledge and practices [34]. This study found out that some questions were left unanswered about COVID-19, one of these was the need for information about the origin of the virus. No matter the reported magnitude of such a question, providing convincing responses would enhance the uptake and support for preventive and treatment efforts. For example, one study from Pakistan reported that some recognized political figures claimed conspiracy (the virus was aimed to affect Muslim countries) as to the origin of the virus and raised public hesitancy to the COVID-19 vaccine which is under development [35].

In this study, significant regional differences were observed on myths, false assurances, and preventive information needs. Specifically, a slightly higher magnitude of myths and lower information need was observed in Addis Ababa. From the date of onset until 9 June 2020, Addis Ababa constituted about 3/4th (1,625 of 2,156 cases) of an accumulation of people with COVID-19, as referred to in most of the daily notification note on COVID-19 situational updates [36, 37]. Addis Ababa is located at the center of Ethiopia, geographically, politically, and economically. Thereof, it has an enormous connection with most Ethiopian regions and towns, which would later lead to a massive spread of the virus to the rest of the regions, due to myths. Additionally, this study found variations in the distribution of myths based on the township, a significantly higher accumulation was observed in big towns than zonal or district towns. Therefore, serious attention needs to be paid to further understand and clear the myths, particularly in Addis Ababa and other big cities/towns in Ethiopia. False assurances that are perceived to inhibit the spread of the virus were common in the Southern region compared to others. Crudely speaking, the false assurances related to the perception of living out of risk zones may seem to go with the prevalence of COVID-19 cases reported in the Southern region. covid-19 case distributions notified by the ministry of health currently indicated, only 15 of 2,156 (0.70%) of cases and zero death were found in the southern region until June 9, 2020 [37]. However, there is no warranty that the virus has not yet been spread across the region, given the testing centers or testing capacity have not yet reached out well in Ethiopia at the moment of the study. The perceptions that there were no locally reported COVID-19 cases and people were living far away from case rampant areas may remain deceitful. Concerning information gaps, southern regions, and zonal and district towns showed higher needs, particularly for preventive information. Currently, a vaccine is one of the most common topics people want to get informed about, but largely affected by conspiracy theories as one of the studies from Pakistan revealed [35].

The above records about perceptions justify that the community’s readiness and responses against a spread of the virus would not withstand the fast-growing rate of infection, suggesting a lot of risk communication and community engagement works. There were a couple of reasons to support this idea. First, the magnitude of the correctly perceived inhibitor (engagement in precautions) of the spread of the virus was as low as 17.1%. Second, there were high perceived magnitudes of behavioral non-adherence and lack of required resources regarding efforts to combate COVID-19. Third, myths and false assurances were rampant.

Limitations of the study

This online questionnaire survey gathered nationwide data capturing community perceptions and experiences that are helpful to have input for risk communication and community engagement. In times of crisis like this pandemic, an online survey looks partly cost-effective and partly ethical. Nonetheless, the study was not without limitations. For example, as with any other online survey, the respondents were relatively educated ones who had access to internet services. On top of this, the perceptions were analyzed from participants’ responses about what people in their locality think, feel, and need about a spread and control of COVID-19. This is an entirely proxy indicator for community perceptions and information needs. Although the study was nationwide, participation from some regions was limited compared to others. Perhaps, extended data collection period would have increased their involvement and representations. Moreover, the current study did not report correlations of the perceptions and community practices. The findings were not well compared with literature due to the absence of similar studies. Nonetheless, we assert that the findings are pertinent to address information gaps and support preventive and treatment efforts. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first kind of community perceptions and myths on COVID-19 in Ethiopia.

Conclusions

This assessment of the community’s perceived factors facilitating and inhibiting a spread of COVID-19, risk group labeling, and information needs provides important signals to control the spread of the virus. There were substantial magnitudes of perceived behavioral non-adherence, lack enabling resources, myths, false surety, information needs, and low perceived adaptations of standard precautions. These sum up to a high likelihood of ignorance of protective measures and externalization of the capacity to control the virus, thereby facilitating the spread of the virus. A lot of myths and false assurances were perceived that were wrongly labeled as inhibitors of the spread of the virus such as perceived religiosity, perceived effectiveness of selected food/spice items, living in hot weather environment, traditional medicines, drinking alcohol, and residence out of risk zone. Regional and township variations in magnitudes of myths, false assurances, and information gaps suggest a need for disproportionate and local framing of communication and interventions that enhance community ownership of the fight against the pandemic. Myths and false assurances should urgently be addressed in higher and lower COVID-19 incidence settings, respectively. Zonal and district towns had higher information needs. Access to multiple mixes of communication channels that deliver quality messages is required to fill information needs rather than mere number of sources. People’s commonest information needs include: how surely people can protect from the virus, isolation and quarantine, and procedures that a symptomatic person needs to follow to keep onself healthier. Though health workers, elderly people, and people with underlying illnesses were perceived high-risk groups as labeled by WHO, perceiving adolescents and youths as low-moderate risk groups would be challenging in a country with a high percentage of young population, like Ethiopia. The young population deemed special attention so that they would actively participate in the prevention efforts. The risk communication and community engagement efforts should: 1) consider regional and township variations in myths and false assurances, 2) investigate more beliefs that could facilitate/inhibit the spread of the virus, 3) satisfy the information needs, 4) design local initiatives that enhance community ownership of tasks of controlling the virus, and thereby support and advocate engagement in standard precautionary measures, and 5) properly utilize media in filtering and disseminating credible information amid increasing volume of disparate falsehoods against COVID-19, supported with the appropriate regulatory system.

Supporting information

S1 Questionnaire

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We express our heartfelt thanks to all individuals who participated in the study: respondents, individuals who have supported data collection across the regions, and professionals who assisted the operations of this online survey.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

Decision Letter 0

Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi

29 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-17606

Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about COVID-19 in Ethiopia:  The need to address information gaps

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Kebede,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Thank you for submission to Plos One. This manuscript has been assessed by two experts and found it interesting. However, authors raised serious concerns in the readability, methodology and results. I invite you to consider these suggestions in point-by-point manner. Since COVID-19 is associated with massive infodemic resulting in various false beliefs, this manuscript is timely and well needed during this time, particularly in African regions. Myths and misleading beliefs during the pandemic has also raised serious concerns such as vaccine hesitancy, self-medication, inappropriate use of devices, drug shortages and price hikes. Various drafts have raised this issue but scientific evidence is currently lacking. I will suggest authors to consider following recently published articles in introduction and discussion section as most of the author`s claim are supported by them. 1. Threat of COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy in Pakistan: The Need for Measures to Neutralize Misleading Narratives (ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0654), 2. Misinformation in wake of the COVID-19 outbreak: Fueling shortage and misuse of lifesaving drugs in Pakistan (https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/disaster-medicine-and-public-health-preparedness/article/misinformation-in-wake-of-the-covid19-outbreak-fueling-shortage-and-misuse-of-lifesaving-drugs-in-pakistan/6048D98D3E44BAA3A3732343FE8C8A27), 3. Walkthrough Sanitization Gates for COVID-19: A Preventive Measure or Public Health Concern? (http://www.ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0533), 4. Drug repurposing for COVID-19: a potential threat of self-medication and controlling measures (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7448118/). Moreover, this manuscript requires extensive editing for English and syntax.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 13 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi, Ph.D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

Thank you for submission to Plos One. This manuscript has been assessed by two experts and found it interesting. However, authors raised serious concerns in the readability, methodology and results. I invite you to consider these suggestions in point-by-point manner. Since COVID-19 is associated with massive infodemic resulting in various false beliefs, this manuscript is timely and well needed during this time, particularly in African regions. Myths and misleading beliefs during the pandemic has also raised serious concerns such as vaccine hesitancy, self-medication, inappropriate use of devices, drug shortages and price hikes. Various drafts have raised this issue but scientific evidence is currently lacking. I will suggest authors to consider following recently published articles in introduction and discussion section as most of the author`s claim are supported by them. 1. Threat of COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy in Pakistan: The Need for Measures to Neutralize Misleading Narratives (ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0654), 2. Misinformation in wake of the COVID-19 outbreak: Fueling shortage and misuse of lifesaving drugs in Pakistan (https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/disaster-medicine-and-public-health-preparedness/article/misinformation-in-wake-of-the-covid19-outbreak-fueling-shortage-and-misuse-of-lifesaving-drugs-in-pakistan/6048D98D3E44BAA3A3732343FE8C8A27), 3. Walkthrough Sanitization Gates for COVID-19: A Preventive Measure or Public Health Concern? (http://www.ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0533), 4. Drug repurposing for COVID-19: a potential threat of self-medication and controlling measures (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7448118/). Moreover, this manuscript requires extensive editing for English and syntax.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include additional information regarding the survey or questionnaire used in the study and ensure that you have provided sufficient details that others could replicate the analyses. For instance, if you developed a questionnaire as part of this study and it is not under a copyright more restrictive than CC-BY, please include a copy, in both the original language and English, as Supporting Information.

3. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Review of study entitled, Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about COVID-19 in Ethiopia: The need to address information gaps Context of study

The study is timely and relevant for the country of study as well as the rest of Africa. However it needs strengthening in various areas.

1. I recommend professional editing of the entire manuscript. The entire manuscript needs improvement in language, as articles, wrong commas, wording, punctuations among others have been used

2. I am not sure the style of reporting figures and proportions in some parts of the manuscript is standard

3. Some of the wording in the tables need improvement

4. The three figures are blur and do not reflect nor represent the narrative. The authors also appear to assume that the figures will complement the narratives in some sections, however tables are equally required in those sections

5. How has COVID-19 been contained in Ethiopia such as government and community efforts with references from the relevant ministries, other stakeholders? The authors provide scanty report on government’s efforts in the prevention of COVID-19. Authors need to provide some reasonable detail on the Ethiopian government and relevant bodies intervention strategies and efforts in the fight against COVID-19 ,

6. Authors do not provide contextual information of the study regions such as level of urbanization/rural, educational levels, population estimates among others. A table on such details will be useful. To ensure that an online study is at least credible and useful such information is critical.

7. While it is no doubt an online study it is still important that authors disaggregate data to explain the situation between men and women, educated and illiterate to enable a better appreciation of the results.

8. Percentages are sometimes placed at the wrong places of sentences

Specific comments

• The use of etc. in some sections of the manuscript, please state all factors or use “among others”

• The repeated use of i.e. should be in full

• Line 133 correct “that slow down it”

Background characteristics of participants

• Disaggregate according to sex, age and educational level

• Why were women that few, which region had the most women and men and why?

Section on measurement and operationalization

Line 133 correct sentence “that slow down it”

Section on data base

Line 143, correct “Multi response analysis was performed for every perceptions.”

Table 2

• What does “People still use suffocated transportation” mean?

• Incomplete: “People do not often seek care for symptoms that looks like it”

• Note on “Kaiser Mayer Olkin’s measure of sampling adequacy”, if the name is an authority kindly reference appropriately.

Section on category of inhibitors

• Lines 191, authors should explain what these terms mean “religiosity”, “general reliance in unconfirmed traditional medicine”

• Lines 193-194, authors state “The second category of perceived inhibitor i.e false assurances was constructed from two beliefs: “we live far away from hot spot areas” and “ there are no cases reported in our locality”,

o What kinds of interviews were conducted, how were text analyzed?

o

o use proper citing with italics of quotation

o Authors should include the study questionnaires/guides as appendix

Section on prevalence of inhibitors

o Lines 201-203: Do not say anything

o What does the following mean? “Accordingly, the prevalence of specific beliefs that built myths ranged between (54.7%) and 140 (15.1%).”

o The same issues have been reported in the sections on Categories of inhibitors and Prevalence of inhibitors.

Section on Spatial distributions of the perceptions: variations by regions and townships

o This section does not mean anything without contextual information on the regions to help the reader to understand the context.

Township distribution and variation

o Authors should provide a table

o Line 297-299. Authors indicate seven but six were reported in this study, “seven commonest communication channels and 298 platforms were used for scoring access: Television, mobile data, social media, health workers, radio, and Wi-Fi.”

o “dot” in front of the following section title should be removed, “. Perception of threat and perceived facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs”

Discussion section

o Line 331-334, authors mention two studies but report on one as follows: “Two, in one of the previous studies conducted in Ethiopia, 179 (72.5%) %) of respondents knew that older ages and people with 333 underlying illnesses are high-risk groups, while only 15 (6.1%) knew that young adult people must engage 334 precautions just like any other segment of people (22).”

o Paragraph 336-360 seems more or less as a report than a discussion paragraph. Besides authors appear to use previously stated quotations that do not follow the standard reporting guidelines for quotations.

o Lines 403 to 411 does not appear to be a discussion paragraph as the study is not compared or contrasted with literature

o Lines 412-418 does not appear to be a discussion paragraph

Under limitation of study

o Authors state “To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first of its kind in reporting community perceptions and myths in Ethiopia.” Authors should correct it to include “first kind in community perceptions and myths on COVID-19”

Conclusion

o What do the following phrases mean “ownership of traditional medicines”; “”people with old ages“; a country like Ethiopia whose major portion is populated with this age segments"?

Reviewer #2: The manuscript needs typographical , grammatical errors and some sentence constructions corrections for more clarity , some of them are indicated below:-

- Line 75 &76 : Of 3, 961,425 closed cases,(10%) ended 76 up in deaths. (what are closed cases?)

- Line 98 & 99: WHO warns the investigation and control of “infodemics”, myths, and stigma, while fighting the pandemic 99 through appropriate risk communication and community engagement principles (the sentence lacks clarity)

- Line 101-104: Moreover, an up-to-date information needs regarding causes, means of protection, modes of transmissions, diagnostic symptoms, and treatment/isolation procedures are basic knowledge to withstand myths, an impression of invulnerability, and support preventive efforts (sentence lacks clarity)

- Line 174 : the phrase “suffocated transport means” , which also appears in several parts of the manuscript is an ambiguous phrase. Assuming that it is meant to describe “crowded unventilated transport means”, is it possible to change the phrase ?

- Line 222: …….and procedures to follow when felt symptomatic (correct "felt symptomatic")

- Line 364: Interestingly, the first two of the three factors were misperceived inhibitors i.e. why we labeled them as myths and false assurances (the use of the abbreviation “ i.e.” does not fit in this sentence, it is better to use the full phrase “that is “

- Line 407 : ……..of cases and zero death are found in the region till a moment of June 9, 2020 (instead of " till a moment of June 2020" use the phrase "as of June 9, 2020)

Additional comments and questions:

- There are 9 National regional states and two administrative states in Ethiopia. Table 1 (line 153) shows that majority of the respondents were from 4 regions and one of the administrative state (i.e Addis Ababa). This means that five out of the 9 regions and one administrative state is within the "others" which is only 6.5% ? Do you think this could be representative of all regions in the country, a country with diverse cultures and beliefs. Do you believe that the data allows you to interpret regional and township variations and thus affect your recommendation for communication and community engagement ? Are there any regions that were not included? If , yes, that data should be reflected

- Line 371 & 372: Please include a reference for this sentence

" Pieces of evidence indicate that myths or misperceptions can set back preventive and control efforts in times of crisis, and pandemics of HIV, Zikavirus, Yellow fever, Ebola, etc, unless traced and addressed ".

- Line 350 : One of the factors for enabling environmental conditions is : people do not have hand rub alcohol or sanitizers. Why was the question only focused on sanitizers and alcohol and why was the availability of water and soap not considered?

- As the authors have rightly indicated one major limitation of the study is the selection bias of educated participants who have access to internet , in addition to being a proxy indicator, But the authors ascertain that the findings are pertinent in that the respondents lived in the community that they represented. This argument is not convincing , since still the community that they represent might be limited to their own circle of educated people

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Nov 30;15(11):e0243024. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243024.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


6 Nov 2020

Responses to reviewers: A rebuttal letter

Dear editor and reviewers (Profs./Drs),

Many thanks for your review and comments to our manuscript on myths about COVID-19. We are so thankful for the insights, feedbacks, and articles you shared to strengthen our work. We hope that we have now addressed your concerns and questions in text the revised version. We provided point-by-point response to your comments, suggestion, and questions. We used track changes to mark where the changes are in the revised document. We also assure you that the manuscript style meets the PLOS ONE’s requirements. We provided additional information about the like questionnaire we adapted for use in this work. We also have presented ethics statement only in methods section. Please, follow our responses to the comments. The responses indicated lines where revisions were made as referred in to “manuscript with track changes”.

Comments

PONE-D-20-17606

Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about COVID-19 in Ethiopia: The need to address information gaps

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Kebede,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Thank you for submission to Plos One. This manuscript has been assessed by two experts and found it interesting. However, authors raised serious concerns in the readability, methodology and results. I invite you to consider these suggestions in point-by-point manner. Since COVID-19 is associated with massive infodemic resulting in various false beliefs, this manuscript is timely and well needed during this time, particularly in African regions. Myths and misleading beliefs during the pandemic has also raised serious concerns such as vaccine hesitancy, self-medication, inappropriate use of devices, drug shortages and price hikes. Various drafts have raised this issue but scientific evidence is currently lacking. I will suggest authors to consider following recently published articles in introduction and discussion section as most of the author`s claim are supported by them. 1. Threat of COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy in Pakistan: The Need for Measures to Neutralize Misleading Narratives (ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0654), 2. Misinformation in wake of the COVID-19 outbreak: Fueling shortage and misuse of lifesaving drugs in Pakistan (https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/disaster-medicine-and-public-health-preparedness/article/misinformation-in-wake-of-the-covid19-outbreak-fueling-shortage-and-misuse-of-lifesaving-drugs-in-pakistan/6048D98D3E44BAA3A3732343FE8C8A27), 3. Walkthrough Sanitization Gates for COVID-19: A Preventive Measure or Public Health Concern? (http://www.ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0533), 4. Drug repurposing for COVID-19: a potential threat of self-medication and controlling measures (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7448118/). Moreover, this manuscript requires extensive editing for English and syntax.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 13 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

• A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

• A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

• An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi, Ph.D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Responses

Comment 1.

Additional Editor Comments:

Thank you for submission to Plos One. This manuscript has been assessed by two experts and found it interesting. However, authors raised serious concerns in the readability, methodology and results. I invite you to consider these suggestions in point-by-point manner. Since COVID-19 is associated with massive infodemic resulting in various false beliefs, this manuscript is timely and well needed during this time, particularly in African regions. Myths and misleading beliefs during the pandemic has also raised serious concerns such as vaccine hesitancy, self-medication, inappropriate use of devices, drug shortages and price hikes. Various drafts have raised this issue but scientific evidence is currently lacking. I will suggest authors to consider following recently published articles in introduction and discussion section as most of the author`s claim are supported by them. 1. Threat of COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy in Pakistan: The Need for Measures to Neutralize Misleading Narratives (ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0654), 2. Misinformation in wake of the COVID-19 outbreak: Fueling shortage and misuse of lifesaving drugs in Pakistan (https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/disaster-medicine-and-public-health-preparedness/article/misinformation-in-wake-of-the-covid19-outbreak-fueling-shortage-and-misuse-of-lifesaving-drugs-in-pakistan/6048D98D3E44BAA3A3732343FE8C8A27), 3. Walkthrough Sanitization Gates for COVID-19: A Preventive Measure or Public Health Concern? (http://www.ajtmh.org/content/journals/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0533), 4. Drug repurposing for COVID-19: a potential threat of self-medication and controlling measures (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7448118/). Moreover, this manuscript requires extensive editing for English and syntax.

Response 1

Dear Editor, thank you for evaluating this work as timely and is important. We are indebted to you for sharing these pertinent studies. We have used them all in the discussion sections of the revised version (please check reference # 27, 32, 33, and 35). We also have improved the writing in English using grammarly and R Pubsure software online.

Comment 2

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include additional information regarding the survey or questionnaire used in the study and ensure that you have provided sufficient details that others could replicate the analyses. For instance, if you developed a questionnaire as part of this study and it is not under a copyright more restrictive than CC-BY, please include a copy, in both the original language and English, as Supporting Information.

3. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

Response 2

We are thankful for sharing the Journal’s requirements and styles. We have checked, the following:

1. We checked the journal’s requirements in the revised version, including formatting issues.

2. We have provided additional information about the study. The questionnaire we used is annexed as supplementary file in revised submission.

3. We have provided ethics statement only in methods section of the revised version of the manuscript.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments 3:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

________________________________________

Response 3

Thank you for your evaluation. We believe that the conclusion based entirely based on the study. In fact, we have improved specific comments provided regarding conclusion. We also believe we have copyedited the manuscript for English using online accessed software like grammarly and R Pubsure.

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer 1 general comments

Comment 4

Reviewer #1: Review of study entitled, Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about COVID-19 in Ethiopia: The need to address information gaps Context of study

The study is timely and relevant for the country of study as well as the rest of Africa. However it needs strengthening in various areas.

1. I recommend professional editing of the entire manuscript. The entire manuscript needs improvement in language, as articles, wrong commas, wording, punctuations among others have been used

Response 4: We are so grateful to Prof/Dr. reviewer 1 for his/her priceless comments that strengthened the manuscript. We also thank for your view of this study as timely and worthy of investigation even for the rest of Africa. Regarding improvement of its English writing, we believe we have now improved the document using online software.

Comment 5

2. I am not sure the style of reporting figures and proportions in some parts of the manuscript is standard

Response 5: Thank you for your concern regarding reporting figures and proportions. The figures were drawn for latent variables constructed from summations of specific variables in that particular factor. Then, for easy comparison all variables were converted to 100%. So, line graphs reported scores of that particular factor out of 100. Therefore, we can confirm you that the figures and proportions used are accurate and carefully analyzed.

Comment 6

3. Some of the wording in the tables needs improvement

Response 6: Thank you for your comment. Although this comment is general and non-specific, we have tried to have improved some wording where we felt was appropriate in the table.

Comment 7:

4. The three figures are blur and do not reflect nor represent the narrative. The authors also appear to assume that the figures will complement the narratives in some sections, however tables are equally required in those sections

Response 7: Regarding blurring effect of the figure, I think the quality goes to the SPSS software we are using. Nonetheless, we can confirm you that the figures were uploaded into the PACE (the journal’s requirement for figure) and has successfully passed it. So we can warrant you that the journal itself don’t accept the figures that don’t fulfill the journals’ minimum requirement. Regarding your comment that tables are required in addition to the figures, we are thankful. However, we argue that using tables in addition to figures will increase redundancy while the added value is minimal. We believed that the figures carried significant information about distribution of myths and other perceptions across regions and towns. In fact, table 6 carried descriptive statistics of the main concepts that latter appeared in figures about regional distributions. Therefore, we politely ask you accept our response.

Comment 8

5. How has COVID-19 been contained in Ethiopia such as government and community efforts with references from the relevant ministries, other stakeholders? The authors provide scanty report on government’s efforts in the prevention of COVID-19. Authors need to provide some reasonable detail on the Ethiopian government and relevant bodies intervention strategies and efforts in the fight against COVID-19,

Response 8: We are so thankful for your comment. We have inserted some lines in the introduction section referring to what government in Ethiopia has been doing to contain the virus. For example, we have inserted (by current lines 117-122 on manuscript with track changes)the following statements in the introduction section, “At the moment of the study, the government has declared state emergency in support of the precautionary measures, and has taken public measures such as: closure of schools including Universities; installation of locally available technologies at public service outlets, including hand washing basins; limiting number of passengers in public transports, among others. Moreover, the ministry of health engaged on public awareness creation, risk communication, and community engagement tasks supported by advisory councils established from recognized Universities.”

Comment 9:

6. Authors do not provide contextual information of the study regions such as level of urbanization/rural, educational levels, population estimates among others. A table on such details will be useful. To ensure that an online study is at least credible and useful such information is critical.

Response 9: Thank you very much this comment. We have now included in “study setting sub-section of the methods section” contextual information about the regions, overall literacy, and population estimates. But we used only text to include the information, not actually table. Refer, to the information in specified sub-section, please. The following statement added by lines 130-137, “At the time of the study, administratively, Ethiopia is divided into nine regional states and two federal cities. The regions have zonal divisions and district sub-divisions, with respective regional capitals and zonal/district twons. Internet services are rarely accessible at district level but mobile data. Ethiopia 2020 population is estimated at 114,963,588 people at mid-year according to UN data. 21.3% of the population is urban (24,463,423 people in 2020). The median age in Ethiopia is 19.5 years. Ethiopian 2020 average literacy rate is 49.1% (lower among adults: male, 57.2; female, 41.1%, and higher among youths: male, 71.1%; female,67.8%).

Comment 10

7. While it is no doubt an online study it is still important that authors disaggregate data to explain the situation between men and women, educated and illiterate to enable a better appreciation of the results.

Response 10: We are thankful for your comment. Detail report about background of participants was reported in separate article, which we believe it will shortly be published soon. As indicated in table 1, the women are disproportionately low in this study, only 10%. Plus, given an online study no illiterate people involved in this study. We emphasized in picking existing myths and perceptions. Please, our apologies we have presented more details in separate article. We would love to avoid redundancy.

Comment 11

8. Percentages are sometimes placed at the wrong places of sentences

Response 11: We have checked percentages that were out at the wrong places of sentences, and corrected which ever we noticed.

Specific comments

Comment 12:

• The use of etc. in some sections of the manuscript, please state all factors or use “among others”

• The repeated use of i.e. should be in full

• Line 133 correct “that slow down it”

Response 12: Thank you for your comments. We corrected them all.

• We have removed the use of “etc”. Instead, we used the list of all factors or “among others” in the revised version.

• The used of “i.e” changed to full text “that is/are/was”

• Line 133 (currently by line 157), we corrected “slow down it” into “….slow down the spread of COVID-19”.

Comment 13

Background characteristics of participants

• Disaggregate according to sex, age and educational level

• Why were women that few, which region had the most women and men and why?

Response 13:

o We have recently provided the response to this comment; refer to response #10 above. With limitation of online as is, we would like to report the myths and beliefs belong to community. All participants are educated, and we didn’t intend to look variations by education or age.

o Perhaps, females participates were low because 1) literacy generally is lower among females, and 2) most respondents for this study were from higher academic institutions were females’ involvement generally is very low compared to males.

Comment 14

Section on measurement and operationalization

Line 133 correct sentence “that slow down it”

Response 14: corrected, as responded already by response # 13. (check current line 157)

Comment 15

Section on data base

Line 143, correct “Multi response analysis was performed for every perceptions.”

Table 2

• What does “People still use suffocated transportation” mean?

• Incomplete: “People do not often seek care for symptoms that looks like it”

• Note on “Kaiser Mayer Olkin’s measure of sampling adequacy”, if the name is an authority kindly reference appropriately.

Response 15: Thank you for the comments given to table 2, we revised the manuscript accordingly,

• Line 143 (current line 168) we corrected the statement by inseting article “a” and removed “s” from perceptions.

In Table 2

• We have changed “people still use suffocated transport” into “people still use crowded transport means”.

• We completed the statement “People do not often seek care for symptoms that looks like it” by adding “ COVID-19 instead of ‘it’.

• Regarding “Kaiser Mayer Olkin’s measure of sampling adequacy” we added a sentence about it in methods section, ‘measurement and operationalization’ sub-section. We also included citation # 24. We did this because it is uncommon to include citation in results. We included a statement that KMO>50% indicate the sample is adequate for running EFA. Under table 2, we now used abbreviation, KMO.

Comment 16:

Section on category of inhibitors

• Lines 191, authors should explain what these terms mean “religiosity”, “general reliance in unconfirmed traditional medicine”

• Lines 193-194, authors state “The second category of perceived inhibitor i.e false assurances was constructed from two beliefs: “we live far away from hot spot areas” and “ there are no cases reported in our locality”,

o What kinds of interviews were conducted, how were text analyzed?

o use proper citing with italics of quotation

o Authors should include the study questionnaires/guides as appendix

Response 16: Thank you for your careful review on category of inhibitors, the specific comments were explained and corrected, accordingly.

• Line 191 (the current line 218) the term “religiosity” is referred to the extent to which people feel themselves as religious enough to be able to effectively manage challenges in their faith. We latter used “perceived religiosity” so we stick to “perceived religiosity” throughout the revision. The variable included in table 3 can provide self-explanation to the term we used as “religiosity”, that is, “we are religious enough to control COVID-19”. We have explained in bracket by line 218 that ‘perceived religiosity” meat that “perceiving oneself as effective religious man/woman in controlling challenges). We also have changed “general reliance in unconfirmed traditional medicine” to “people’s perceived confidence that they owned effective traditional medicines that are, however, not clinically confirmed”

• Line 193-194 (current line 222-226). Similar to the above response, we have improved the statements as such, “The second category of perceived inhibitors was still local sayings that were often related to people’s false assurances that they were protected from COVID-19 (unlike myths, the second category of beliefs may not need scientific approval or disapproval). The category consisted of two main beliefs: “we live far away from COVID-19 rampant areas” and “ there are no locally reported COVID-19 cases so far ”

• Regarding interviews conducted, we have plainly put that this was an online cross-sectional survey conducted using questionnaire. Sometimes, we left open spaces, to type their responses. Any responses about perceptions of facilitators or inhibitor was considered as variable and counted.

• Quotations reported in italics in the revised version.

• We have included questionnaire as supplementary files

Comment 17

Section on prevalence of inhibitors

o Lines 201-203: Do not say anything

o What does the following mean? “Accordingly, the prevalence of specific beliefs that built myths ranged between (54.7%) and 140 (15.1%).”

o The same issues have been reported in the sections on Categories of inhibitors and Prevalence of inhibitors.

Response 17: Thank you for your comments on this section

o Lines 201-203 (current lines234-238) were improved to convey clear idea as such: “Myths and false assurances were the most prevalent perceived inhibitors of the spread of COVID-19 compared the perception that engagement in precautionary measures protect from exposure to and spread of the virus. Specifically, perceived religiosity, perceived effectiveness of selected foods, and perceived protectiveness of hot weather were the commonest myths, accounting 508 (54.7%), 455 (49.0%), and 242 (26.0%), respectively. Beliefs that there were no locally reported cases of COVID-19 and the specific localities where respondents are currently living are far away from corona virus rampant areas contributed to 343 (36.9%) and 274 (29.5%) prevalence of false assurances”

Comment 18:

Section on Spatial distributions of the perceptions: variations by regions and townships

o This section does not mean anything without contextual information on the regions to help the reader to understand the context.

Response 18: We have now provided contextual information about regions to help understand the context in study setting sub-section in methods. We also noted about this in our response # 9.

Comment 19:

Township distribution and variation

o Authors should provide a table

o Line 297-299. Authors indicate seven but six were reported in this study, “seven commonest communication channels and 298 platforms were used for scoring access: Television, mobile data, social media, health workers, radio, and Wi-Fi.”

o “dot” in front of the following section title should be removed, “. Perception of threat and perceived facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs”

Response 19: We are thankful for your critical review and comments.

o Regarding table we also provided response to this comment in our response #7. The added value of inserting table is minimal. We have presented the required data through the line graphs. Adding table mostly causes redundancy of data presentation.

o Line 297-299 (current lines 247-248) we added, “broad band internet service”. Now, the list is seven.

o By current line 356, We removed the “dot” in front of the section title ““. Perception of threat and perceived facilitators, inhibitors, and information needs”

Comment 20:

Discussion section

o Line 331-334, authors mention two studies but report on one as follows: “Two, in one of the previous studies conducted in Ethiopia, 179 (72.5%) %) of respondents knew that older ages and people with 333 underlying illnesses are high-risk groups, while only 15 (6.1%) knew that young adult people must engage 334 precautions just like any other segment of people (22).”

o Paragraph 336-360 seems more or less as a report than a discussion paragraph. Besides authors appear to use previously stated quotations that do not follow the standard reporting guidelines for quotations.

o Lines 403 to 411 does not appear to be a discussion paragraph as the study is not compared or contrasted with literature

o Lines 412-418 does not appear to be a discussion paragraph

Response 20: Thank you for your comment on the discussion section. The comments were addressed in the revised version.

o Line 331-334 (current line 387-389). In a paragraph stating the said concepts, we did not say two studies. We did say a couple reasons opposed to two studies. Please check. Reference 21 and 22 were cited for reason number 1 and 2.

o Line 336-360 (current line 392-421): Regarding discussions in those lines, we provided conceptual discussion. For example, a factor with “lack of enabling environment” would conceptual mean “the need for much resources” for COVID-19, and “behavioral non-adherence” would mean “social ignorance and lacks of commitment” . We have supplemented some theoretical explanation as what does these mean in terms of behavioral and communication theories and WHO’s preparedness and response with regard to community engagement and risk communication. Statements that have sense of quotations were removed from this paragraph. In fact, we did not use any quotations. We used variables that illustrated the themes in which they belonged.

o Line 403-411 (current line 474-484). Obviously literatures are limited on this study. We have now compared the findings against available resources. We have used reference # 28, 29 to explain what these findings mean. We also have discussed these in terms of the geographical presence and interactions between the regions.

o Line 412-418 (current line 485-497): Again, we emphasized on conceptual meaning of it. In fact, we also have used reference #29 in order to support our argument. At the same time, we added one more study with false assurance cited as # 35.

Comment 21

Under limitation of study

o Authors state “To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first of its kind in reporting community perceptions and myths in Ethiopia.” Authors should correct it to include “first kind in community perceptions and myths on COVID-19”

Response 21: Thank you very much, we accepted the comment and changed the statement accordingly.

o The revised statement reads, “To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first kind of community perceptions and myths on COVID-19 in Ethiopia” refer by line 519-20.

Comment 22:

Conclusion

o What do the following phrases mean “ownership of traditional medicines”; “”people with old ages“; a country like Ethiopia whose major portion is populated with this age segments"?

Response 22: Thank you for your careful review of this manuscript. We addressed the comment in the revised version.

o We have revised our conclusion regarding statements/phrases about “ownership of medicine, people with old ages, and Ethiopian population”. We deleted “ownership”, used “elderly people”, and “ a country with high percentage of young population, like Ethiopia” instead of the previous phrases.

Reviewer 2 comments

Comment 23

Reviewer #2: The manuscript needs typographical , grammatical errors and some sentence constructions corrections for more clarity , some of them are indicated below:-

- Line 75 &76 : Of 3, 961,425 closed cases,(10%) ended 76 up in deaths. (what are closed cases?)

- Line 98 & 99: WHO warns the investigation and control of “infodemics”, myths, and stigma, while fighting the pandemic 99 through appropriate risk communication and community engagement principles (the sentence lacks clarity)

- Line 101-104: Moreover, an up-to-date information needs regarding causes, means of protection, modes of transmissions, diagnostic symptoms, and treatment/isolation procedures are basic knowledge to withstand myths, an impression of invulnerability, and support preventive efforts (sentence lacks clarity)

- Line 174 : the phrase “suffocated transport means” , which also appears in several parts of the manuscript is an ambiguous phrase. Assuming that it is meant to describe “crowded unventilated transport means”, is it possible to change the phrase ?

- Line 222: …….and procedures to follow when felt symptomatic (correct "felt symptomatic")

- Line 364: Interestingly, the first two of the three factors were misperceived inhibitors i.e. why we labeled them as myths and false assurances (the use of the abbreviation “ i.e.” does not fit in this sentence, it is better to use the full phrase “that is “

- Line 407 : ……..of cases and zero death are found in the region till a moment of June 9, 2020 (instead of " till a moment of June 2020" use the phrase "as of June 9, 2020)

Response 23: Thank you very much, indeed for your careful reviewing of our manuscript. We have addressed all your comments in the revised version point-by-point as follows:

-Line 75 and 76 (current line 80-81): In the statement, “Of 3, 961,425 closed cases,(10%) ended 76 up in deaths”. Closed cases mean cases of COVID-19 that resulted in discharge either because of cure or deaths. We used “closed cases” as referred by worldometer. We believe using “closed cases” as it is will be good give that worldometer dashboard for COVID-19 uses the same term.

-- Line 98 & 99 (current line 105-108): The statement. “WHO warns the investigation and control of “infodemics”, myths, and stigma, while fighting the pandemic 99 through appropriate risk communication and community engagement principles” was changed to, “At this moment of the pandemic, WHO recommends the risk communication and community engagement efforts to investigation and control “infodemics”, myths, beliefs, and stigma so that the spread of the spread of corona virus would be appropriatel combated” in the revised version.

- Line 101-104 (current line 110-113): The statement, “ Moreover, an up-to-date information needs regarding causes, means of protection, modes of transmissions, diagnostic symptoms, and treatment/isolation procedures are basic knowledge to withstand myths, an impression of invulnerability, and support preventive efforts” was changed to “Moreover, an up-to-date information regarding causes, means of protection, modes of transmissions, diagnostic symptoms, and treatment/isolation procedures are basic to withstand myths, beliefs, perceptions, and support preventive efforts”.

-Line 174 (current line 189): the phrase, “suffocated transport means” was changed to “crowded unventilated transport means”, as you suggested, and across the manuscript.

- Line 222 (current line 263): as suggested we corrected "felt symptomatic" to “symptomatic” we removed “felt”

- Line 364 (current line xxx): we used full phrase for “i.e.” in a statement “Interestingly, the first two of the three factors were misperceived inhibitors i.e. why we labeled them as myths and false assurances”

- Line 407 (current line 477) : the comment to change “till a moment of June 2020” into “as of June 9, 2020” in a statement, “……..of cases and zero death are found in the region till a moment of June 9, 2020 (instead of " till a moment of June 2020" needs some clarification. We didn’t want to imply “as of June 9, 2020”. Instead, we wanted “until June 9, 2020”. Thus, we modified the phrase in that statement into, “……..until June 9, 2020”

Comment 24: Additional comments and questions:

- There are 9 National regional states and two administrative states in Ethiopia. Table 1 (line 153) shows that majority of the respondents were from 4 regions and one of the administrative state (i.e Addis Ababa). This means that five out of the 9 regions and one administrative state is within the "others" which is only 6.5% ? Do you think this could be representative of all regions in the country, a country with diverse cultures and beliefs. Do you believe that the data allows you to interpret regional and township variations and thus affect your recommendation for communication and community engagement ? Are there any regions that were not included? If , yes, that data should be reflected

- Line 371 & 372: Please include a reference for this sentence

" Pieces of evidence indicate that myths or misperceptions can set back preventive and control efforts in times of crisis, and pandemics of HIV, Zikavirus, Yellow fever, Ebola, etc, unless traced and addressed ".

- Line 350 : One of the factors for enabling environmental conditions is : people do not have hand rub alcohol or sanitizers. Why was the question only focused on sanitizers and alcohol and why was the availability of water and soap not considered?

- As the authors have rightly indicated one major limitation of the study is the selection bias of educated participants who have access to internet , in addition to being a proxy indicator, But the authors ascertain that the findings are pertinent in that the respondents lived in the community that they represented. This argument is not convincing , since still the community that they represent might be limited to their own circle of educated people

Response 24: Thank you for your key comments and questions. We have clarified them. We also have modified the manuscript based on the comments.

-Regarding regions and cities included in the study, let us explain it. The study has not excluded any region or city. We have now provided information about regions and towns in the study setting sub-sections. During analysis, we merged regional or federal cities as big towns. Many regions like Somali, Benishangul, Gambela, Afar were merged as “other regions”. Honestly speaking, we didn’t intend to merge any town or region at the moment we started the online data collection. Because of the urgent need of the study to inform risk communication and community engagement, we closed the data collection within 2 weeks, after getting some sample of 929 respondents across the country. As we think now, the problem was that we closed the data collection early. Unfortunately, during analysis we observed that the involvement of the above merged regions and cities were low. So, we were forced to merge some of them. We appreciate the critical view provided to us regarding adequacy of representations. We include a statement, “Although the study was nationwide, participation from some regions were limited compared to others. Perhaps, extended data collection period would have increased their involvement and representations “ in the limitation section of the study. However, we still believe the analysis reveals pertinent finding with exception of the merging of some. Regarding towns, we considered sizes and communication opportunities. So, we don’t think that would be a big problem. Regarding, recommendation we have shown two points: 1) to check regional variations in addressing myths and false assurances, 2) further investigation of beliefs and myths through additional assessments. The limitation would be embraced here.

-- Line 371 & 372 (current line 440-444): Four references for this sentence, " Pieces of evidence indicate that myths or misperceptions can set back preventive and control efforts in times of crisis, and pandemics of HIV, Zikavirus, Yellow fever, Ebola, etc, unless traced and addressed ". The new reference # 28-31 were about this.

-- Line 350 (current line 408): Regarding inclusion of water and soap in this study, we would say two points. 1) there was open space, where respondents can add more. Unfortunately, soap and water were not touched in their open response. 2) we did not put this as major list of factor, (we did mistake in missing that). Perhaps, we have previously published on knowledge and practice about COVID-19 as used in reference number 25. In that study, the dominantly practiced behavior was hand washing with soap and water. That could have affected us unknowingly to make water/soap on list of options. Moreover, that the moment of this study, water/soap was found in every corner of towns and it was less likely to bear in mind. Still, we don’t imply that was a good idea to include soap/water in this study to know how that was prevalent lack as COVID-19 preventive resource. Thank you once again.

-Thank you once more for critical ideas you always raised toward strengthening this manuscript. Regarding the comment you provided on limiting community in the circle of their educated people, we have completely saluted. We have now removed that idea from the limitation of the study. It is true mentioning “communities are represented by their educated people” is conflicting with previously mentioned idea, “only educated people are more likely to access internet”.

________________________________________

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Comment 25:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step

Response 25: The figures we uploaded even at the initial submission was passed the PACE criteria. They meet the PLOS’s requirement.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi

16 Nov 2020

Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about the spread of COVID-19 in Ethiopia:  A need to address information and community engagement gaps

PONE-D-20-17606R1

Dear Dr. Kebede,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi, Ph.D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have responded to all the issues raised adequately and for that matter I have no further comments,

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi

18 Nov 2020

PONE-D-20-17606R1

Myths, beliefs, and perceptions about COVID-19 in Ethiopia:  A need to address information gaps and enable combating efforts

Dear Dr. Kebede:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Tauqeer Hussain Mallhi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES