Skip to main content
Journal of Clinical Biochemistry and Nutrition logoLink to Journal of Clinical Biochemistry and Nutrition
. 2020 Jun 19;67(3):307–316. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.19-116

Cognitive function improvement with astaxanthin and tocotrienol intake: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study

Takahiro Sekikawa 1,*, Yuki Kizawa 1, Yanmei Li 2, Tsuyoshi Takara 3
PMCID: PMC7705074  PMID: 33293773

Abstract

We examined the effects of the mixed ingestion of astaxanthin derived from Haematococcus pluvialis and tocotrienols on the cognitive function of healthy Japanese adults who feel a memory decline. Forty-four subjects were randomly but equally assigned to the astaxanthin-tocotrienols or placebo group. An astaxanthin-tocotrienols or placebo capsule was taken once daily before or after breakfast for a 12-week intervention period. The primary outcome was composite memory from the Cognitrax cognitive test, and the secondary outcomes were other cognitive functions and subjective symptoms for memory. Each group included 18 subjects in the efficacy analysis (astaxanthin-tocotrienols group, 55.4 ± 7.9 years; placebo group, 54.6 ± 6.9 years). The astaxanthin-tocotrienols group showed a significant improvement in composite memory and verbal memory in Cognitrax at Δ12 weeks compared with the placebo group. Additionally, the astaxanthin-tocotrienols group showed a significant improvement in the subjective symptom of “During the last week, have you had trouble remembering people’s names or the names of things?” compared with the placebo group after 12 weeks. No adverse events were observed in this study. The results demonstrated that taking an astaxanthin-tocotrienols combination improves the composite memory and verbal memory of Japanese adults who feel a memory decline (UMIN 000031758).

Keywords: cognitive, astaxanthin, tocotrienol, Cognitrax, antioxidant properties, lipid peroxidation

Introduction

The damage of nerve cells caused by oxidative stress is one of the factors that decrease cognitive function.(1,2) The accumulation of amyloid β (Aβ) induces oxidative stress and damages tissues around the hippocampal region, leading to the onset of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), one of the cognitive impairments.(3) Aβ is also expressed in healthy people, and the elimination mechanism by the enzyme neprilysin and phagocytes works normally.(4) However, when the elimination function is lowered by aging, Aβ accumulates and the risk of developing AD increases.(5) Moreover, Aβ begins to accumulate about 20 years before the onset of AD.(6) Therefore, the removal of oxidative stress in the brain at an early stage is effective in the protection of nerve cells, which may suppress the decline in cognitive function as well as prevent and inhibit the progression of AD.

Astaxanthin has antioxidant properties and plays a role in the protection of oxidative damage through a variety of mechanisms, including elimination of singlet oxygen and radicals, suppression of lipid peroxidation, and regulation of gene expression associated with oxidative stress.(7,8) A previous study in vitro has shown that astaxanthin exerts a neuroprotective effect against Aβ toxicity.(9) Additionally, clinical research has reported that astaxanthin improves antioxidant capacity(10) and improves antioxidant status in red blood cells.(11) Astaxanthin is specifically absorbed into the blood and can cross the blood–brain barrier in rats,(12) suggesting that it is effective in preventing various disorders caused by reactive oxygen species in brain nerve cells.(1315) Thus, astaxanthin may suppress cognitive decline.

Astaxanthin on the market has two structures: one is a fatty acid ester, and the other is a non-fatty acid ester. A fatty acid ester comes from Haematococcus pluvialis (H. pluvialis) or krill, whereas a non-fatty acid ester comes from Phaffia rhodozyma or Paracoccus carotinifaciens or is chemically synthesized.(1618) A previous report in mice showed that the concentrations of astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis in the plasma and liver were higher than those of astaxanthin derived from P. rhodozyma and astaxanthin that was chemically synthesized.(17) This research indicates that, among the sources of astaxanthin, astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis could have high bioavailability.

Tocotrienols are known to cross the blood–brain barrier and reach the brain because of their structure with unsaturated side chains.(19) Furthermore, tocotrienols have neuroprotective effects through their antioxidant effects against oxidative stress induced by Aβ accumulation.(2022) In studies using aged rats, tocotrienols suppress the decline in memory and learning functions and improve spatial memory with aging.(23,24) Tocotrienols have potent antioxidants with lipoperoxyl radical scavenging.(25) In addition, astaxanthin is highly efficient at inhibiting lipid peroxidation(26) that acts on oxidative damage through a variety of other mechanisms.(7,8) Furthermore, astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis has high bioavailability.(17) The antioxidant capacity of the combination of astaxanthin and tocotrienols is significantly stronger than that of either astaxanthin or tocotrienols alone, because of the hydrogen bonding and intermolecular interactions between these two compounds.(27) Therefore, the simultaneous ingestion of astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis and tocotrienols (AT) may enhance lipid resistance to oxidation more when compared with ingestion of H. pluvialis-derived astaxanthin or tocotrienols alone.

To the best of our knowledge, the only evidence on the effects of foods containing the AT combination on human cognitive function was reported by Hongo et al.(28) However, in Hongo et al.’s study, the placebo set was a food product excluding only astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis, and the aim of the study was to evaluate the function of astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis. Thus, no functional evaluation was performed when AT was ingested in a mixture. In addition, Hongo et al.(28) confirmed that healthy Japanese adults between 60 and 79 years retained visual information appropriately for a short period and showed a tendency to improve their self-assessment of memory. However, the primary outcome was a multi-item study, indicating included exploratory elements. Therefore, there is still insufficient evidence for the effect of the AT combination on cognitive function. We investigated the effects of the AT combination on cognitive function in healthy Japanese adults aged ≥40 years who feel a memory decline.

Materials and Methods

Study design

This study was a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study conducted at Medical Corporation Seishinkai, Takara Clinic (Tokyo, Japan), between May 13 and September 1, 2018. The allocation was based on a 1:1 ratio. The study protocol was approved by the independent ethical committee of Medical Corporation Seishinkai, Takara Clinic, on March 13, 2018 (approval no. 1803-1802-BJ01-03-TC). This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (2013) and the Ethical Guidelines for Medical and Health Research involving human subjects of Japan and thoroughly considered medical ethics. The protocol was registered at the University Hospital Medical Information Network Clinical Trials Registry (UMIN000031758).

Subjects

Inclusion criteria were defined as follows: (a) experiencing mild forgetfulness in healthy Japanese adult subjects; (b) eligibility to participate in the study by the principal physician; (c) attaining a Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) score of ≥24 at screening/before intake; and (d) relatively lower normalized Cognitrax (Health Solution, Inc., Tokyo, Japan) composite memory domain scores at screening/before intake. Exclusion criteria were defined as follows: (a) a medical history of current treatment for malignancy, heart failure, or myocardial infarction; (b) current treatment for cardiac arrhythmia, hepatic, renal, or cerebrovascular disease, rheumatism, diabetes mellitus, hyperlipidemia, hypertension, or other chronic diseases; (c) a diagnosis of dementia; (d) a diagnosis of mental illnesses such as major depression and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; (e) daily consumption of medications (including herbal medicines), “foods for specified health uses,” “foods with function claims,” or other functional foods/beverages; (f) daily consumption of food containing docosahexaenoic acid, eicosapentaenoic acid, Ginkgo biloba extract, tocotrienol, astaxanthin, γ-aminobutyric acid, phosphatidylserine, and/or other improved cognitive function foods/beverages; (g) allergic reaction to medications and/or products that contain the study ingredients; (h) being pregnant, lactating, or planning to become pregnant; (i) enrollment in other clinical trials within the last 3 months before agreeing to participate in this study; and (j) ineligibility to participate in the study based on the evaluation of the principal physician.

Regularly, all subjects were enrolled through the website (https://www.go106.jp/) operated by ORTHOMEDICO Inc. (Tokyo, Japan) between March 19 and May 12, 2018. The study protocol was comprehensively explained to all the subjects. Written informed consent was obtained from all of them before enrollment in the study at the ORTHOMEDICO Inc. office. Notably, no subject was part of the sponsors or funding companies.

Sample size

Cognitive functions were assessed using Cognitrax based on the CNS Vital Signs (CNS Vital Signs LLC, Morrisville, NC).(29) The primary outcome was an increment in the score of the composite memory domain, which is one of the domains evaluated by Cognitrax. No studies have evaluated the composite memory domain with the intake of AT. Thus, we referred to the study of Gualtieri et al.,(29) who used the CNS Vital Signs, and calculated the SD of the primary outcome. SD in the composite memory domain was calculated to be 7.88 in healthy subjects aged ≥20 years, and we hypothetically obtained a similar SD in our study. In addition, we hypothesized that a difference in scores of the composite memory domain between the two groups of ≥7.00 points means a clinically significant difference in the improvement of cognitive function. Therefore, the sample size was evaluated with an assumed α value of 0.05 and a (1 – β) value of 0.80. Consequently, the sample size was finalized to be 20 subjects per group. Furthermore, we considered 10% of the dropout rate and added two extra subjects to each group (22 subjects per group).

Enrollment, randomization, and blinding

Of 121 subjects who signed informed consent, eligible subjects who were considered appropriate for the study, attained an MMSE score of ≥24, and did not experience dementia(30) were selected by the physician. In addition, subjects with relatively lower normalized Cognitrax composite memory scores [normalized score evaluated based on the average of scores corresponding to the subjects’ age set at 100 (SD, 15)](31) before intake were selected as priority subjects for enrollment in this study. An allocation controller equally, but randomly, assigned subjects to either the AT group or the placebo group (P group; n = 22 per group). The allocation was performed using StatLight #11 ver. 2.10 (Yukms Co., Ltd., Kawasaki, Japan), a computerized random-number generator. The allocation method was stratified randomization, and the allocation adjustment factor was defined as the normalized Cognitrax composite memory score, sex, and age of screening. Furthermore, subjects, the physician, the assessor of outcomes, and others who were associated with this study were not aware of group assignments and were not involved in the allocation. Moreover, the allocation controller locked the assignment sheet until the key-opening day (October 23, 2018).

Intervention

The test soft capsules included AT (BGG Japan Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan; total weight content, 160 mg; H. pluvialis-derived astaxanthin, approximately 9 mg; tocotrienol, approximately 50 mg) and safflower oil as placebo (total weight content, 160 mg). All the subjects were asked to consume either one capsule containing AT or one placebo capsule per day before or after breakfast for 12 weeks. Both capsules were declared identical in color, odor, and flavor by the Ethics Committee.

Outcomes

Table 1 outlines the schedule for this study. Subjects visited the clinic and underwent examinations before intake and at 8 and 12 weeks after intake. All subjects abstained from excessive alcohol or exercise from the day before the examination until the end of the examination. Furthermore, they abstained from eating or drinking anything including the test food, except water, for 6 h before providing blood samples.

Table 1.

Schedule of enrollment, intervention, and assessments

Test period
Enrollment

Allocation



Before intake (Baseline) Start intake 8 weeks 12 weeks
Enrollment
 Eligibility screen ×
 Informed consent ×
 Allocation ×

Intervention
 AT group graphic file with name jcbn19-116line1.jpg
 P group graphic file with name jcbn19-116line1.jpg

Assessments
 Cognitrax × × ×
 Questionnaire × × ×
 Blood analysis × × ×
 Physical examination × × ×
 Urinalysis × × ×
 MMSE ×
 Visual acuity test ×
 Daily record graphic file with name jcbn19-116line1.jpg
 Medical questionnaire × × ×

(1) Primary outcome: composite memory domain. Cognitrax evaluates various cognitive function domains, such as processing speed, and executive function based on the CNS Vital Signs,(29) evaluating each domain’s score from 10 separate tests. The scores of the composite memory domain were calculated as the sum of the verbal memory (VBM) and visual memory (VIM) domain scores.(29) At the beginning of the VBM test, 15 words were presented on the screen, one by one, every 2 s. Next, a subject was asked to identify those words nested among 30 words, including new words (immediate memory scores). Furthermore, the subject was again asked to identify the learned 15 words nested among 30 words, including new words after all the tests had been taken (delayed memory scores). In the VIM test, words in the VBM test were replaced with geometric figures, and the procedure remained the same as that of the VBM test. The composite memory score was calculated from the total number of correct answers given in the VBM and VIM tests and was converted into a normalized score. The normalized value was calculated from the measured value based on a normal distribution with a mean ± SD of 100 ± 15; if the measured value of a subject is 1 SD greater than the average of his/her own age, his/her normalized score is 115. The standard score in domains was assessed as follows: >109 points, “Above”; 90–109 points, “Average”; 80–89 points, “Low Average”; 70–79 points, “Low”; and <70 points, “Very Low”.(31)

(2) Secondary outcomes. The cognitive functions of the subjects were evaluated using Cognitrax as follows: neurocognitive index domain, VBM domain, VIM domain, psychomotor speed domain, reaction time domain, complex attention domain, cognitive flexibility domain, processing speed domain, executive function domain, social acuity domain, reasoning domain, working memory domain, sustained attention domain, simple attention domain, and motor speed domain.

In addition, subjective symptoms were assessed by the original questionnaire for the Likert scale using the following questions: “Over the past week, have you forgotten things often?”, “Have you been concerned about memory loss during the last week?”, “Is there any time that you cannot remember a story you heard during the last week?”, “During the last week, have you had trouble remembering people’s names or the names of things?”, “Did you leave behind anything over the last week?”, “Did you feel chronically tired during the past week?”, “Were you experiencing eye fatigue during the past week?”, “Did you experience a stiff neck or shoulders during the past week?”, “Have you felt depressed for the past week?”, “Did you experience discomfort in your back over the last week?”, “Was it difficult to get up from the floor or a chair in the last week?”, “Did your knees hurt during crouching or standing up in the last week?”, and “Did your knees hurt while going up and down the stairs in the last week?”. All these questions were assessed on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree).

Subjects’ blood samples (19 ml) were collected at the Medical Corporation Seishinkai, Takara Clinic, and tested for the following: brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), propanoyl lysine (PRL), and pentosidine. In this study, all collected blood samples were entrusted to LSI Medience Corporation (Tokyo, Japan).

(3) Safety assessment. Safety evaluations were assessed in physical examination, urinalysis, and blood analysis (Table 24). All subjects were asked to fill out a medical questionnaire to understand their health conditions. In addition, subjects were asked to record a daily report on health conditions, use of medications, and lifestyles.

Table 2.

Results of physical examination (AT group, n = 20; P group, n = 20)

Baseline 8 weeks 12 weeks p value
Baseline 8 weeks 12 weeks
Body weight (kg) AT group 60.3 ± 16.6 60.1 ± 17 60.3 ± 16.8 0.696 0.747 0.434
P group 62.1 ± 11.7 61.7 ± 11.8 61.8 ± 11.9

BMI (kg/m2) AT group 22.4 ± 3.7 22.3 ± 3.9 22.4 ± 3.9 0.503 0.708 0.368
P group 23.2 ± 3.9 23.1 ± 4 23.1 ± 4.1

Body fat percentage (%) AT group 22.4 ± 4.1 23.1 ± 3.7 23.2 ± 3.8 0.060 0.582 0.785
P group 25.4 ± 5.6 25.4 ± 5.3 25.6 ± 5.1

Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) AT group 124.5 ± 17.4 125.2 ± 19.3 124.1 ± 18.3 0.390 0.177 0.180
P group 120.1 ± 14.3 125.7 ± 18.5 125.5 ± 15.5

Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) AT group 79 ± 12.6 78.5 ± 11.9 78.3 ± 11.3 0.454 0.201 0.412
P group 76.2 ± 10.7 79.3 ± 12.1 78.2 ± 10.9

Pulse rate (bpm) AT group 74.2 ± 9.3 72.3 ± 8.9 70.6 ± 9.2 0.507 0.569 0.588
P group 71.9 ± 12 72.3 ± 11.2 70.1 ± 8.5

Body temperature (°C) AT group 36.1 ± 0.6 36.3 ± 0.4 36.3 ± 0.4 0.948 0.932 0.550
P group 36.1 ± 0.4 36.3 ± 0.2 36.3 ± 0.3

Data are mean ± SD.

Table 3.

Results of urinalysis

Assessment point AT group (n = 20)
P group (n = 20)
p value
Within the reference range Outside the reference range Within the reference range Outside the reference range
Protein Baseline 19 1 20 0 1.000
8 weeks 18 2 18 2 1.000
12 weeks 18 2 19 1 1.000

Glucose Baseline 20 0 20 0 NA
8 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA
12 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA

Urobilinogen Baseline 20 0 20 0 NA
8 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA
12 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA

Bilirubin Baseline 20 0 20 0 NA
8 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA
12 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA

pH Baseline 20 0 20 0 NA
8 weeks 20 0 19 1 1.000
12 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA

Occult blood Baseline 19 1 17 3 0.605
8 weeks 19 1 16 4 0.342
12 weeks 20 0 18 2 0.487

Ketone bodies Baseline 20 0 20 0 NA
8 weeks 20 0 19 1 1.000
12 weeks 20 0 20 0 NA

Data are the number of subjects. NA, not available.

Table 4.

Results of blood analysis (AT group, n = 20; P group, n = 20)

Reference range Baseline 8 weeks 12 weeks p value
Baseline 8 weeks 12 weeks
Leukocyte count (/µl) 3,300–9,000 AT group 5,535.0 ± 1,619.4 5,335.0 ± 1,322.0 5,300.0 ± 1,396.6 0.067 0.349 0.833
P group 4,655.0 ± 1,316.5 5,000.0 ± 1,259.5 4,720.0 ± 973.7

Erythrocyte count (×104/µl) Male 430–570 AT group 463.9 ± 46.3 458.8 ± 52.9 458.3 ± 52.7 0.329 0.588 0.592
Female 380–500 P group 451.0 ± 35.5 449.4 ± 33.6 449.3 ± 34.9

Hemoglobin (g/dl) Male 13.5–17.5 AT group 14.1 ± 1.3 14.0 ± 1.5 14.0 ± 1.5 0.666 0.318 0.440
Female 11.5–15.0 P group 14.0 ± 1.2 14.0 ± 1.2 14.0 ± 1.3

Hematocrit Value (%) Male 39.7–52.4 AT group 44.5 ± 3.8 44.6 ± 4.7 44.1 ± 4.4 0.685 0.810 0.806
Female 34.8–45.0 P group 44.1 ± 3.5 44.2 ± 3.4 43.5 ± 3.5

Platelet count (×104/µl) 14.0–34.0 AT group 25.7 ± 4.5 25.7 ± 5.4 25.8 ± 5.3 0.609 0.886 0.639
P group 25.0 ± 3.7 25.2 ± 4.4 24.8 ± 3.7

MCV (fl) 85–102 AT group 96.3 ± 3.6 97.4 ± 3.4 96.6 ± 4.0 0.226 0.857 0.203
P group 97.7 ± 3.9 98.5 ± 4.2 96.9 ± 4.8

MCH (pg) 28.0–34.0 AT group 30.5 ± 1.1 30.5 ± 1.1 30.6 ± 1.1 0.228 0.319 0.792
P group 31.0 ± 1.3 31.1 ± 1.3 31.1 ± 1.5

MCHC (%) 30.2–35.1 AT group 31.7 ± 0.7 31.3 ± 0.6 31.7 ± 0.6 0.862 0.157 0.042*
P group 31.7 ± 0.9 31.6 ± 0.7 32.1 ± 0.8

Percentage of neutrophils (%) 40.0–75.0 AT group 56.9 ± 7.3 56.4 ± 8.1 57.4 ± 9.5 0.231 0.973 0.292
P group 53.5 ± 10.3 54.7 ± 10.3 52.5 ± 9.5

Percentage of lymphocytes (%) 18.0–49.0 AT group 33.7 ± 6.4 34.2 ± 7.1 34.2 ± 8.6 0.331 0.899 0.611
P group 36.3 ± 10.1 35.7 ± 9.8 37.3 ± 9.6

Percentage of monocytes (%) 2.0–10.0 AT group 5.2 ± 1.0 5.5 ± 1.3 5.1 ± 1.0 0.368 0.590 0.038
P group 5.5 ± 1.3 5.9 ± 1.6 5.9 ± 1.3

Percentages of eosinophils (%) 0.0–8.0 AT group 3.4 ± 2.9 3.2 ± 2.7 2.6 ± 1.6 0.674 0.638 0.116
P group 3.8 ± 3.1 3.1 ± 2.6 3.6 ± 2.7

Percentages of basophils (%) 0.0–2.0 AT group 0.8 ± 0.4 0.8 ± 0.4 0.8 ± 0.5 0.628 0.129 0.459
P group 0.8 ± 0.5 0.7 ± 0.4 0.8 ± 0.5

AST (U/L) 10–40 AT group 23.6 ± 11.5 25.2 ± 15.8 26.1 ± 26.4 0.511 0.392 0.612
P group 21.4 ± 9.4 21.3 ± 9.4 20.9 ± 7.6

ALT (U/L) 5–45 AT group 21.5 ± 16.9 22.1 ± 22.7 24.6 ± 36.4 0.304 0.759 0.815
P group 17.0 ± 8.9 16.1 ± 9.8 16.1 ± 9.6

γ-GT (U/L) Male ≤80 AT group 44.4 ± 48.9 41.7 ± 45.0 45.5 ± 54.6 0.116 0.810 0.681
Female ≤30 P group 26.0 ± 14.5 25.0 ± 12.7 28.2 ± 19.9

ALP (U/L) 100–325 AT group 213.2 ± 70.0 196.0 ± 53.7 193.2 ± 55.7 0.379 0.932 0.149
P group 197.3 ± 38.6 184.0 ± 34.0 191.7 ± 33.6

LD (U/L) 120–240 AT group 187.5 ± 38.8 190.3 ± 42.6 194.2 ± 59.9 0.525 0.644 0.633
P group 181.1 ± 21.9 185.7 ± 28.4 187.7 ± 33.8

LAP (U/L) Male 45–81 AT group 52.2 ± 12.8 52.6 ± 13.8 52.5 ± 14.6 0.472 0.570 0.352
Female 37–61 P group 49.2 ± 12.8 49.2 ± 10.1 50.7 ± 13.7

Total bilirubin (mg/dl) 0.2–1.2 AT group 1.0 ± 0.4 1.0 ± 0.5 1.0 ± 0.4 0.200 0.522 0.469
P group 0.8 ± 0.2 0.9 ± 0.2 0.9 ± 0.2

Direct bilirubin (mg/dl) 0.0–0.2 AT group 0.1 ± 0.1 0.1 ± 0.1 0.1 ± 0.1 0.012* 0.857 0.396
P group 0.1 ± 0.1 0.1 ± 0.0 0.1 ± 0.0

Indirect bilirubin (mg/dl) 0.2–1.0 AT group 0.9 ± 0.4 0.9 ± 0.5 0.9 ± 0.4 0.313 0.646 0.692
P group 0.8 ± 0.2 0.8 ± 0.2 0.8 ± 0.2

Cholinesterase (ChE) (U/L) Male 234–493 AT group 322.5 ± 82.1 310.3 ± 85.4 312.2 ± 87.0 0.369 0.994 0.536
Female 200–452 P group 342.7 ± 56.3 328.8 ± 47.0 336.1 ± 47.9

Total protein (g/dl) 6.7–8.3 AT group 7.2 ± 0.3 7.1 ± 0.4 7.0 ± 0.5 0.646 0.418 0.791
P group 7.1 ± 0.5 6.9 ± 0.4 7.0 ± 0.4

Urea nitrogen (mg/dl) 8.0–20.0 AT group 13.7 ± 3.1 13.3 ± 3.4 14.0 ± 3.7 0.138 0.442 0.432
P group 12.3 ± 2.4 12.8 ± 3.7 12.5 ± 2.4

Creatinine (mg/dl) Male 0.61–1.04 AT group 0.7 ± 0.2 0.7 ± 0.2 0.7 ± 0.2 0.418 0.341 0.746
Female 0.47–0.79 P group 0.7 ± 0.1 0.7 ± 0.1 0.7 ± 0.1

Uric acid (mg/dl) Male 3.8–7.0 AT group 4.9 ± 1.7 5.1 ± 1.8 5.1 ± 1.8 0.350 0.328 0.624
Female 2.5–7.0 P group 4.5 ± 1.0 4.5 ± 1.1 4.6 ± 1.0

CK (U/L) Male 60–270 AT group 104.3 ± 64.5 116.9 ± 92.0 108.4 ± 109.1 0.968 0.819 0.824
Female 40–150 P group 103.6 ± 44.3 111.6 ± 63.9 110.4 ± 52.3

Sodium (mEq/L) 137–147 AT group 141.9 ± 1.2 141.5 ± 1.6 141.3 ± 1.3 0.692 0.173 0.251
P group 141.7 ± 1.9 140.7 ± 2.3 140.6 ± 2.4

Potassium (mEq/L) 3.5–5.0 AT group 4.0 ± 0.4 3.9 ± 0.4 3.9 ± 0.4 0.715 0.157 0.414
P group 4.0 ± 0.3 3.8 ± 0.2 3.8 ± 0.3

Chloride (mEq/L) 98–108 AT group 101.8 ± 1.7 101.7 ± 2.0 101.6 ± 1.8 0.670 0.818 0.585
P group 101.5 ± 1.9 101.4 ± 2.2 101.1 ± 2.2

Calcium (mEq/L) 8.4–10.4 AT group 9.0 ± 0.4 9.0 ± 0.3 8.9 ± 0.4 0.532 0.705 0.861
P group 9.0 ± 0.3 9.0 ± 0.3 8.9 ± 0.3

Inorganic phosphorus (mEq/L) 2.5–4.5 AT group 3.5 ± 0.8 3.7 ± 0.7 3.7 ± 0.9 0.705 0.941 1.000
P group 3.4 ± 0.6 3.6 ± 0.6 3.6 ± 0.4

Serum iron (µg/dl) Male 50–200 AT group 105.9 ± 24.6 104.4 ± 29.8 108.4 ± 26.9 0.171 0.848 0.557
Female 40–180 P group 118.8 ± 33.5 113.5 ± 46.4 116.3 ± 26.6

Serum amylase (U/L) 40–122 AT group 75.6 ± 27.7 72.4 ± 17.9 72.5 ± 23.2 0.167 0.158 0.337
P group 87.2 ± 24.3 84.6 ± 21.0 84.2 ± 19.7

Total cholesterol (mg/dl) 120–219 AT group 213.9 ± 34.6 215.3 ± 41.6 216.6 ± 46.1 0.154 0.046* 0.429
P group 230.0 ± 35.6 215.4 ± 27.3 223.9 ± 33.6

HDL cholesterol (mg/dl) Male 40–85 AT group 64.9 ± 15.1 64.0 ± 14.6 65.0 ± 13.9 0.342 0.308 0.885
Female 40–95 P group 71.0 ± 24.0 66.3 ± 17.9 70.2 ± 19.7

LDL cholesterol (mg/dl) 65–139 AT group 127.6 ± 33.9 131.9 ± 44.4 132.2 ± 44.2 0.293 0.034* 0.466
P group 137.9 ± 26.8 129.1 ± 23.0 136.3 ± 27.8

Triglyceride (mg/dl) 30–149 AT group 119.4 ± 113.2 116.5 ± 96.2 120.2 ± 91.2 0.896 0.861 0.249
P group 115.5 ± 71.8 111.1 ± 73.6 103.7 ± 59.8

Glucose (mg/dl) 70–109 AT group 82.8 ± 7.3 87.0 ± 9.8 86.6 ± 10.7 0.316 0.109 0.801
P group 85.3 ± 8.3 84.8 ± 5.7 87.2 ± 7.0

HbA1c (NGSP) (%) 4.6–6.2 AT group 5.5 ± 0.4 5.5 ± 0.4 5.5 ± 0.4 0.275 0.899 0.346
P group 5.4 ± 0.3 5.4 ± 0.3 5.4 ± 0.4

Glycoalbumin (%) 12.3–16.5 AT group 13.8 ± 1.4 13.8 ± 1.4 14.2 ± 1.5 0.745 0.529 0.657
P group 13.9 ± 1.6 14.1 ± 1.9 14.4 ± 1.7

Data are mean ± SD. *p<0.05 vs P group.

Statistical analysis

All outcomes were assessed before intake and at 8 and 12 weeks after intake. Setting before intake as baseline, each assessment point was subtracted from baseline and reported as the change in the value (Δ8 and Δ12 weeks). In addition, subjects’ background and demographic data were aggregated based on age, MMSE, and IgE (radioimmunosorbent test), and data of the AT and P groups were compared using Student’s t test.

Cognitive function data at baseline and changes are presented as mean ± SD, which were analyzed using Student’s t test. Moreover, subjective symptoms data were analyzed using the Mann–Whitney U test at baseline and at 8 and 12 weeks after intake. Physical examination and blood analysis data are presented as mean ± SD, which were analyzed at baseline using Student’s t test. Furthermore, we analyzed data at 8 and 12 weeks after intake using the two-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). When ANCOVA was used for data analyses, we used the baseline values as covariates. Of note, between-group comparison was used in the post hoc analysis. Furthermore, urinalysis data were set to a code where 1 was identified as within the normal range and 0 as outside the normal range. The χ2 test was used for between-group analyses.

All statistical analyses in this study were two sided, and we set the significance level to 5% with no adjustment for multiple comparisons. Data analyses were performed using Windows SPSS ver. 23.0 (IBM Japan, Ltd., Tokyo, Japan).

Results

Subjects

Figure 1 presents a flowchart of the follow-up of the study subjects. None had an intake rate of less than 90% throughout the intake period. Four subjects who did not submit a diary or return the test food at the post-trial case review meeting and could not confirm the presence or absence of the intervention and four subjects who were judged as “No” by the validity indicator of Cognitrax were judged as ineligible for analysis and excluded from the analysis. When the breakdown of persons who were excluded from the analysis after the key opening was checked, it was found that two subjects in each group were not able to confirm the presence or absence of the intervention, and two subjects in each group were judged as “No” by the validity indicator of Cognitrax. The number of subjects analyzed as the per protocol set was 18 subjects (8 men and 10 women, aged 55.4 ± 7.9 years) in the AT group and 18 subjects (8 men and 10 women, aged 54.6 ± 6.9 years) in the P group. The number of subjects analyzed as the safety analysis set was 20 subjects (9 men and 11 women, aged 55.3 ± 7.5 years) in the AT group and 20 subjects (8 men and 12 women, aged 55.4 ± 7.4 years) in the P group. The background and age distribution of the study subjects are shown in Table 5-1, 5-2, 5-3, and 5-4. There was no item in which there was any significance between groups in the background factor.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Flowchart of participants in this study.

Table 5-1.

Subjects’ background information (per protocol set)

AT group (n = 18) P group (n = 18) p value
Age (years) 55.4 ± 7.9 54.6 ± 6.9 0.721
MMSE (points) 29.2 ± 1.0 29.4 ± 0.8 0.373
IgE (RIST) (IU/ml) 146.5 ± 181.1 176.1 ± 244.1 0.683

Data are mean ± SD.

Table 5-2.

Subjects’ background information (safety analysis set)

AT group (n = 20) P group (n = 20) p value
Age (years) 55.3 ± 7.5 55.4 ± 7.4 0.966
MMSE (points) 29.0 ± 1.2 29.2 ± 1.2 0.604
IgE (RIST) (IU/ml) 156.1 ± 181.2 165.2 ± 233.9 0.891

Data are mean ± SD.

Table 5-3.

Subjects’ age distribution (per protocol set)

Age (years) AT group (n = 18)
P group (n = 18)
Men (n) Women (n) Men (n) Women (n)
40–49 0 3 2 3
50–59 5 6 5 6
60–69 3 0 1 0
≥70 0 1 0 1

Data are the number of subjects.

Table 5-4.

Subjects’ age distribution (safety analysis set)

Age (years) AT group (n = 20)
P group (n = 20)
Men (n) Women (n) Men (n) Women (n)
40–49 0 3 2 3
50–59 6 7 5 7
60–69 3 0 1 0
≥70 0 1 0 2

Data are the number of subjects.

Cognitrax

The results of the cognitive tests are shown in Table 6 and Fig. 2. In composite memory, which was set as the primary outcome, points of Δ12 weeks in the AT group were significantly higher than those in the P group (AT group, 20.1 ± 13.3 points; P group, 9.6 ± 16.2 points; p = 0.040; Fig. 2A). The variable points of the VBM domain for Δ12 weeks were 23.6 ± 14.2 points in the AT group and 12.1 ± 19.0 points in the P group, which were significantly higher in the AT group than in the P group (p = 0.048; Fig. 2B). In the social acuity domain, the score of the AT group was significantly higher than that of the P group at baseline (AT group, 97.4 ± 13.6 points; P group, 69.2 ± 55.3 points; p = 0.043).

Table 6.

Results of Cognitrax (AT group, n = 18; P group, n = 18)

Baseline 8 weeks 12 weeks p value
Baseline 8 weeks 12 weeks
Neurocognitive index (points) AT group 84.6 ± 15.6 92.3 ± 14.6 100.1 ± 8.6 0.133 0.959 0.453
P group 91.8 ± 12.4 95.1 ± 19.3 99.3 ± 11.0
Composite memory (points) AT group 71.1 ± 10.7 81.6 ± 13.7 91.2 ± 7.7 0.346 0.596 0.072
P group 74.6 ± 11.3 86.7 ± 20.7 84.1 ± 17.2
Verbal memory (points) AT group 70.8 ± 13.3 82.0 ± 12.8 94.4 ± 8.8 0.177 0.654 0.153
P group 77.4 ± 15.4 87.4 ± 20.3 89.5 ± 16.0
Visual memory (points) AT group 83.6 ± 12.0 89.1 ± 13.7 91.9 ± 11.4 0.900 0.531 0.171
P group 83.0 ± 14.2 91.7 ± 16.0 85.3 ± 17.6
Psychomotor speed (points) AT group 94.6 ± 14.0 103.6 ± 15.4 104.8 ± 13.1 0.299 0.167 0.264
P group 99.7 ± 15.1 99.3 ± 23.3 103.7 ± 18.8
Reaction time (points) AT group 88.0 ± 17.0 97.6 ± 13.5 98.0 ± 15.5 0.487 0.123 0.778
P group 91.8 ± 15.2 93.9 ± 15.1 99.2 ± 16.3
Complex attention (points) AT group 87.0 ± 30.5 88.7 ± 47.7 104.7 ± 15.4 0.167 0.854 0.621
P group 99.2 ± 20.4 98.3 ± 31.5 104.4 ± 13.0
Cognitive flexibility (points) AT group 82.8 ± 27.9 90.5 ± 20.7 102.2 ± 8.8 0.208 0.556 0.633
P group 93.2 ± 19.8 96.8 ± 26.7 105.1 ± 11.5
Processing speed (points) AT group 102.1 ± 16.1 113.5 ± 11.7 113.6 ± 10.7 0.152 0.473 0.940
P group 109.1 ± 12.3 112.2 ± 12.7 115.7 ± 16.9
Executive function (points) AT group 84.4 ± 28.3 90.9 ± 19.4 102.9 ± 8.0 0.275 0.513 0.832
P group 93.4 ± 19.5 96.9 ± 25.9 104.9 ± 11.7
Social acuity (points) AT group 97.4 ± 13.6 92.2 ± 22.2 95.0 ± 20.9 0.043* 0.196 0.852
P group 69.2 ± 55.3 92.3 ± 21.7 92.1 ± 18.2
Reasoning (points) AT group 102.2 ± 13.2 101.9 ± 11.1 103.5 ± 13.3 0.969 0.468 0.655
P group 102.4 ± 12.1 99.4 ± 13.1 105.3 ± 14.1
Working memory (points) AT group 97.1 ± 9.9 104.4 ± 17.8 108.9 ± 10.8 0.226 0.950 0.439
P group 102.0 ± 13.9 107.7 ± 15.8 107.9 ± 13.9
Sustained attention (points) AT group 92.9 ± 29.1 106.7 ± 13.0 108.4 ± 11.7 0.193 0.977 0.971
P group 103.0 ± 13.7 107.9 ± 12.9 107.8 ± 18.7
Simple attention (points) AT group 92.7 ± 22.6 64.4 ± 137.2 96.9 ± 14.4 0.878 0.422 0.486
P group 93.9 ± 24.9 91.1 ± 20.4 87.1 ± 56.5
Motor speed (points) AT group 92.1 ± 13.5 96.8 ± 16.6 97.6 ± 13.7 0.613 0.199 0.249
P group 94.6 ± 15.1 91.3 ± 25.3 95.1 ± 16.6

Data are mean ± SD. *p<0.05 vs P group.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Change values and actual measured values on the domains in Cognitrax. Data show the change values at baseline and at 8 and 12 weeks after intake (Δ8 and Δ12 weeks): change scores in (A) the composite memory domain and (B) the verbal memory (VBM) domain. Closed circle (●), AT group; open circle (○), P group. Data are mean ± SE. *p<0.05 vs P group. Effect size and 95% confidence interval.

Subjective symptoms

The results of the subjective symptoms are shown in Fig. 3. The scale numbers of the question “During the last week, have you had trouble remembering people’s names or the names of things?” were median 3.0 (Q1–Q3, 2.0–3.8) in the AT group and median 4.0 (Q1–Q3, 3.0–4.8) in the P group at 12 weeks after ingestion, which were significantly lower in the AT group than in the P group (p = 0.036; Fig. 3). No significant differences were identified for the other questions (data not shown).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Results of subjective symptoms. Data show the scale number of “During the last week, have you had trouble remembering people’s names or the names of things?” throughout the intervention period. Black bar, AT group; open bar, P group. Data are median (interquartile range). *p<0.05 vs P group.

Blood BDNF, PRL, and pentosidine levels

There were no significant differences between the groups (data not shown).

Safety assessment

No medically problematic changes were observed with the continued ingestion of the test food.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the cognitive improvement effect of the simultaneous intake of AT (9 mg/day astaxanthin and 50 mg/day tocotrienol) for 12 weeks by healthy Japanese adults who feel a memory decline.

Cognitrax measures a wide range of cognitive areas, including memory, attention, processing speed, and executive function based on the CNS Vital Signs.(29) The scores for each cognitive area were calculated based on the results of 10 tests. A higher score means a higher cognitive function of the region. According to the report of the CNS Vital Signs,(29) a score of composite memory of healthy subjects peaked in their 20s and then decreased gradually with age. Therefore, we considered that an increasing score of composite memory in Cognitrax was an improvement of cognitive function, setting composite memory as the primary outcome in our study. The variation scores of composite memory and VBM in our study were significantly increased in the AT group compared with those in the P group at 12 weeks after ingestion. The score of Cognitrax is a standardized score calculated using a mean ± SD of 100 ± 15 for the same age group, with 90–110 scores considered as “Average” range.(31) Our actual composite memory and VBM scores were less than 90 points (without the range of “Average”) at baseline in both groups, but the actual scores at 12 weeks after ingestion were within the range of “Average” only in the AT group. The scores of composite memory and VBM domains in the AT group were significantly increased from before intake to 12 weeks after intake, which allowed the domain scores to fall within the range of “Average,” indicating that AT intake improves composite memory and VBM. In Cognitrax, the scores of composite memory are calculated as the sum of the scores of VBM and VIM.(29) Therefore, an improvement in VBM may be involved in the improvement in composite memory.

In the evaluation of subjective symptoms, the scale number of “During the last week, have you had trouble remembering people’s names or the names of things?” in the AT group was significantly lower than that in the P group at 12 weeks after ingestion. Each item of the original questionnaire means that the lower the scale number, the better the subjective symptoms. Comparing the scale number between the two groups, the median scale number for both groups was 4.0, which means “slightly agree” at the time point before ingestion. However, at 12 weeks after ingestion, the median value for the AT group was 3.0, which means “slightly disagree,” whereas the median value for the P group was 4.0, with no change from pre-ingestion. In other words, the intake of AT increased the number of persons who felt “slightly disagree” in response to the question “During the last week, have you had trouble remembering people’s names or the names of things?”, which indicates that the intake of AT improved subjective symptoms related to VBM. The results of the objective and subjective evaluations suggested that our results are consistent.

Although no research studies have verified the improvement of cognitive function by a mixed intake of AT, the effectiveness in each component has been reported, including neuroprotection and improvement of cognitive function. Regarding the suppression of cognitive decline because of the neuroprotective effect of astaxanthin, Yook et al.’s(32) study using mice suggested that ingestion of H. pluvialis-derived astaxanthin in the diet at a dose of 0.5% with mild exercise generated hippocampal neurons and enhanced spatial learning and memory. In addition, Katagiri et al.(33) reported that H. pluvialis-derived astaxanthin administered to healthy Japanese adults at a dose of 6 or 12 mg/day was effective on cognitive function. However, Katagiri et al.(33) did not show significant differences between the AT group and the P group. Furthermore, we investigated the effects of H. pluvialis-derived astaxanthin taken at 9 mg/day on the cognitive function of healthy Japanese adults, confirming that H. pluvialis-derived astaxanthin at 9 mg/day improved both VIM and VBM.(34) In contrast, previous studies of Taridi et al.(23,24) in elderly rats have shown that tocotrienols, another implicated component in our test food, improved memory and learning function and spatial memory with age. Although interaction of the simultaneous intake of AT could not be clarified in this study, it was certain that the simultaneous intake improves composite memory and VBM. Each component has been suggested to have neuroprotective effects to suppress cognitive decline. A previous epidemiologic study in Japanese adults reported that the accumulation of Aβ increased with age and increased rapidly in their 40s.(35) The average age of subjects in our study was in the 50s, where damage to neurons had already begun. As these components have inhibitory effects on cognitive function because of their neuroprotective effects, it is possible that each component inhibited neuronal cell damage in our study as well, and it is inferred that AT improved composite memory and VBM.

This study was the first to evaluate the effects of the simultaneous intake of AT on cognitive function. The study clarified that the combined intake of AT improved composite memory and VBM. However, the blood AT levels were not measured. Thus, it was not possible to confirm whether the levels in blood were actually elevated. Astaxanthin is a type of carotenoid, and the decreasing levels of carotenoids, retinol, and tocopherol in the frontal lobe with age are believed to be associated with the pathogenesis of dementia.(36) In addition, epidemiologic studies reported a lower risk of dementia among individuals with high blood tocotrienol levels.(3739) Therefore, it is important to investigate the relationship between blood AT levels and cognitive function in future studies. Furthermore, the effects of the antioxidant ability of the combined intake of AT on cognitive function could not be confirmed because we did no assess oxidative markers. Thus, the mechanisms by which AT improves cognitive function remain unclear and should be clarified in the future. In other organs, such as the salivary gland, the oxidative stress that accompanies aging has been reported to decrease saliva secretory function in vivo, and the oxidative marker is negatively correlated with the salivary flow rate.(40) Oxidative stress markers may also be related to the decrease in cognitive function. Oxidative stress markers will be measured in further studies to investigate the more detailed mechanisms of the effects of AT on cognitive function. Moreover, we did not confirm the participants’ level of cognitive function after the decline of AT consumption in this study, and it is still unknown whether the effect of AT is sustainable or not. However, investigation of the sustainable effects after intake of AT is stopped is a task for future studies, with the aim of determining the mechanisms by which AT improves cognitive function.

Safety evaluations were assessed in physical examination, urinalysis, and blood analysis. Although there were several significant differences between the groups, the mean values for all the items were within the reference values, and the physician judged that there was no effect of the test food intake. Therefore, continuous ingestion of the simultaneous intake of AT for 12 weeks was safe under the conditions of this study.

In conclusion, ingestion of AT (9 mg/day astaxanthin and 50 mg/day tocotrienol) for 12 weeks by Japanese adults who feel a memory decline improves composite memory and VBM. In addition, there were no safety issues under the conditions of this study.

Authors Contributions

ST, KY, and LY designed the research. TT conducted the research. ST wrote the paper. TT had primary responsibility for the final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the subjects and staff who participated in this study.

Abbreviations

amyloid beta

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

AT

astaxanthin and tocotrienols

BDNF

brain-derived neurotrophic factor

H. pluvialis

Haematococcus pluvialis

MMSE

Mini-Mental Status Examination

P group

placebo group

PRL

propanoyl lysine

VBM

verbal memory

VIM

visual memory

Conflict of Interest

The sponsor of this study, BGG Japan Co., Ltd., entrusted ORTHOMEDICO, Inc. with conducting the study. ST and KY are a part of BGG Japan Co., Ltd., and LY is a member of Beijing Gingko-Group Biological Technology Co., Ltd. TT (MD) is a part of Medical Corporation Seishinkai, Takara Clinic. TT was the principal investigator and managed the health of the study subjects.

References

  • 1.Nunomura A, Tamaoki T, Motohashi N, et al. The earliest stage of cognitive impairment in transition from normal aging to Alzheimer disease is marked by prominent RNA oxidation in vulnerable neurons. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2012; 71: 233–241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nunomura A, Castellani RJ, Zhu X, Moreira PI, Perry G, Smith MA. Involvement of oxidative stress in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2006; 65: 631–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Manczak M, Anekonda TS, Henson E, Park BS, Quinn J, Reddy PH. Mitochondria are a direct site of Aβ accumulation in Alzheimer’s disease neurons: implications for free radical generation and oxidative damage in disease progression. Hum Mol Genet 2006; 15: 1437–1449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Iwata N, Saido T. Amyloid-β peptide metabolism and Alzheimer’s disease. Folia Pharmacol Jpn 2003; 122: 5–14. (in Japanese) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Takata K. Molecular targeting and translational research for new therapeutic strategies on Alzheimer’s disease. Yakugaku Zasshi 2013; 133: 1389–1399. (in Japanese) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol 2010; 9: 119–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Galasso C, Corinaldesi C, Sansone C. Carotenoids from marine organisms: biological functions and industrial applications. Antioxidants 2017; 6. pii: E96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dose J, Matsugo S, Yokokawa H, et al. Free radical scavenging and cellular antioxidant properties of astaxanthin. Int J Mol Sci 2016; 17: 103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Chang CH, Chen CY, Chiou JY, Peng RY, Peng CH. Astaxanthine secured apoptotic death of PC12 cells induced by β-amyloid peptide 25–35: its molecular action targets. J Med Food 2010; 13: 548–556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Iwabayashi M, Fujioka N, Nomoto K, et al. Efficacy and safety of eight-week treatment with astaxanthin in individuals screened for increased oxidative stress burden. Anti-Aging Med 2009; 6: 15–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nakagawa K, Kiko T, Miyazawa T, et al. Antioxidant effect of astaxanthin on phospholipid peroxidation in human erythrocytes. Br J Nutr 2011; 105: 1563–1571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tso M, Lam T.Method of retarding and amelorating central nervous systemand eye damage. U.S. Patent; 5527533, 1996.
  • 13.Wu H, Niu H, Shao A, et al. Astaxanthin as a potential neuroprotective agent for neurological diseases. Mar Drugs 2015; 13: 5750–5766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Grimmig B, Kim SH, Nash K, Bickford PC, Douglas Shytle R. Neuroprotective mechanisms of astaxanthin: a potential therapeutic role in preserving cognitive function in age and neurodegeneration. Geroscience 2017; 39: 19–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Galasso C, Orefice I, Pellone P, et al. On the neuroprotective role of astaxanthin: new perspectives? Mar Drugs 2018; 16. pii: E247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sato K, Sugimoto N, Yamada T, Maitani T. Studies on optical isomerism of astaxanthin in natural food colors and principal pigment in phaffia color. Food Hyg Saf Sci (Shokuhin Eiseigaku Zasshi) 2000; 41: 44–47. (in Japanese) [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Aoi W, Maoka T, Abe R, Fujishita M, Tominaga K. Comparison of the effect of non-esterified and esterified astaxanthins on endurance performance in mice. J Clin Biochem Nutr 2018; 62: 161–166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hayashi M, Ishibashi T, Maoka T. Effect of astaxanthin-rich extract derived from Paracoccus carotinifaciens on cognitive function in middle-aged and older individuals. J Clin Biochem Nutr 2018; 62: 195–205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kaneai N, Sumitani K, Fukui K, Koike T, Takatsu H, Urano S. Tocotrienol improves learning and memory deficit of aged rats. J Clin Biochem Nutr 2016; 58: 114–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fukui K, Takatsu H, Koike T, Urano S. Hydrogen peroxide induces neurite degeneration: prevention by tocotrienols. Free Radic Res 2011; 45: 681–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Khanna S, Parinandi NL, Kotha SR, et al. Nanomolar vitamin E alpha-tocotrienol inhibits glutamate-induced activation of phospholipase A2 and causes neuroprotection. J Neurochem 2010; 112: 1249–1260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Packer L, Weber SU, Rimbach G. Molecular aspects of α-tocotrienol antioxidant action and cell signalling. J Nutr 2001; 131: 369S–373S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Taridi NM, Yahaya MF, Teoh SL, et al. Tocotrienol rich fraction (TRF) supplementation protects against oxidative DNA damage and improves cognitive functions in Wistar rats. Clin Ter 2011; 162: 93–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Taridi NM, Abd Rani N, Abd Latiff A, Ngah WZ, Mazlan M. Tocotrienol rich fraction reverses age-related deficits in spatial learning and memory in aged rats. Lipids 2014; 49: 855–869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ahsan H, Ahad A, Siddiqui WA. A review of characterization of tocotrienols from plant oils and foods. J Chem Biol 2015; 8: 45–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Guerin M, Huntley ME, Olaizola M. Haematococcus astaxanthin: applications for human health and nutrition. Trends Biotechnol 2003; 21: 210–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kamezaki C, Nakashima A, Yamada A, et al. Synergistic antioxidative effect of astaxanthin and tocotrienol by co-encapsulated in liposomes. J Clin Biochem Nutr 2016; 59: 100–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hongo N, Tominaga K, Okuda J, Suzuki M, Miura N. Effects of astaxanthin on cognitive function, physical function and mood condition in elderly people: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-control1ed, parallel-group, comparative study. Jpn Pharmacol Ther 2018; 46: 2043–2062. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS Vital Signs. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2006; 21: 623–643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.CNS Vital Signs® Brief Interpretation Guide. 2017; p. 13 http://www. cnsvs.com/WhitePapers/CNSVS-BriefInterpretationGuide.pdf. Accessed 1 June 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yook JS, Rakwal R, Shibato J, et al. Leptin in hippocampus mediates benefits of mild exercise by an antioxidant on neurogenesis and memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116: 10988–10993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Katagiri M, Satoh A, Tsuji S, Shirasawa T. Effects of astaxanthin-rich Haematococcus pluvialis extract on cognitive function: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Biochem Nutr 2012; 51: 102–107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sekikawa T, Kizawa Y, Li Y, Takara T. Cognitive function improvement with astaxanthin intake: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Pharmacometrics 2019; 97: 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morishima-Kawashima M, Oshima N, Ogata H, et al. Effect of apolipoprotein E allele ɛ4 on the initial phase of amyloid β-protein accumulation in the human brain. Am J Pathol 2000; 157: 2093–2099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Craft NE, Haitema TB, Garnett KM, Fitch KA, Dorey CK. Carotenoid, tocopherol, and retinol concentrations in elderly human brain. J Nutr Health Aging 2004; 8: 156–162. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chin KY, Tay SS. A review on the relationship between tocotrienol and Alzheimer disease. Nutrients 2018; 10. pii: E881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mangialasche F, Xu W, Kivipelto M, et al. Tocopherols and tocotrienols plasma levels are associated with cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: 2282–2290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mangialasche F, Westman E, Kivipelto M, et al. Classification and prediction of clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease based on MRI and plasma measures of α-/γ-tocotrienols and γ-tocopherol. J Intern Med 2013; 273: 602–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Yamauchi Y, Matsuno T, Omata K, Satoh T. Relationship between hyposalivation and oxidative stress in aging mice. J Clin Biochem Nutr 2017; 61: 40–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Clinical Biochemistry and Nutrition are provided here courtesy of The Society for Free Radical Research Japan

RESOURCES