Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Bulletin logoLink to Schizophrenia Bulletin
. 2020 Jul 10;46(6):1346–1352. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa091

Enhancing Psychosis Risk Prediction Through Computational Cognitive Neuroscience

James M Gold 1,, Philip R Corlett 2, Gregory P Strauss 3, Jason Schiffman 4, Lauren M Ellman 5, Elaine F Walker 6, Albert R Powers 2, Scott W Woods 2, James A Waltz 1, Steven M Silverstein 7, Vijay A Mittal 8
PMCID: PMC7707066  PMID: 32648913

Abstract

Research suggests that early identification and intervention with individuals at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis may be able to improve the course of illness. The first generation of studies suggested that the identification of CHR through the use of specialized interviews evaluating attenuated psychosis symptoms is a promising strategy for exploring mechanisms associated with illness progression, etiology, and identifying new treatment targets. The next generation of research on psychosis risk must address two major limitations: (1) interview methods have limited specificity, as recent estimates indicate that only 15%–30% of individuals identified as CHR convert to psychosis and (2) the expertise needed to make CHR diagnosis is only accessible in a handful of academic centers. Here, we introduce a new approach to CHR assessment that has the potential to increase accessibility and positive predictive value. Recent advances in clinical and computational cognitive neuroscience have generated new behavioral measures that assay the cognitive mechanisms and neural systems that underlie the positive, negative, and disorganization symptoms that are characteristic of psychotic disorders. We hypothesize that measures tied to symptom generation will lead to enhanced sensitivity and specificity relative to interview methods and the cognitive intermediate phenotype measures that have been studied to date that are typically indicators of trait vulnerability and, therefore, have a high false positive rate for conversion to psychosis. These new behavioral measures have the potential to be implemented on the internet and at minimal expense, thereby increasing accessibility of assessments.

Keywords: clinical high risk, schizophrenia prodrome, conversion


The large majority of people with schizophrenia demonstrate significant disability and premature mortality despite largely successful management of their positive symptoms.1,2 Efforts to address morbidity and mortality have shifted to the earliest phases of the illness, with the idea that early intervention may improve long-term symptomatic and functional outcomes. Motivated by the results of the Recovery After an Initial Schizophrenia Episode (RAISE3) study, intensive intervention in first-episode psychosis (FEP) is now being brought to scale through the combined efforts of the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) to develop a nation-wide network of sites delivering evidence-based specialty care.

Building on the success of early intervention at the first episode, international efforts have sought to determine if it is possible to reliably identify people who appear to be at clinical high risk (CHR) for the onset of psychosis, with the goal of developing interventions to prevent or delay the emergence of a psychotic disorder, thereby altering the clinical course. This first generation of CHR research focused on the development of reliable clinical interview methods to identify people who appeared to be at highest risk for conversion to psychosis so that they could receive careful monitoring and treatment as appropriate.4–12 This approach has substantially improved our understanding of the prodromal stage of the disorder and highlighted biomarkers associated with CHR status and conversion (such as changes in electrophysiology, brain structure, and cognitive performance).13–25

While substantial progress has been made, several limitations of prevailing approaches suggest the need to develop innovative methods:

  • (1) Low Specificity. Current approaches using structured interviews to identify people at CHR have limited specificity: only 15%–30% of individuals who meet CHR criteria actually convert to a psychotic disorder over extended follow-up.26–33 The low conversion rates associated with current assessment methods greatly confound attempts to power primary prevention trials, as seen in the recent NEURAPRO fish oil study.34,35 We acknowledge that conversion to psychosis is not the only relevant clinical outcome and that many people who do not convert experience substantial symptom burden and functional impairment requiring clinical attention. However, we remain convinced that the prevention of conversion to psychosis should remain a public health priority, given the morbidity and mortality associated with these disorders. There is a clear need to increase the specificity and sensitivity of assessment of imminent risk, in order to enrich samples for future preventative intervention trials.36–38

  • (2) Limited Availability and Detection Power. The interview methods that are used for CHR identification require extensive training, in addition to the establishment of referral networks or public health awareness campaigns that involve significant resources and support. As a result, only a minority of people who develop FEP access specialty care for CHR syndromes. Even in the United Kingdom, where specialty CHR care is available via the National Health Service,32,36,39–45 detection power of the CHR approach is limited in that only 5% of people who ultimately present with FEP have ever had any prior contact with CHR services.4,31 In the United States, the availability situation is worse: until recently, CHR services have existed only in a few academic institutions,5,46–48 and this has limited the public health impact of the first-generation studies. While the new NIMH Clinical High Risk for Psychosis initiative (U01 and U24 pair) will ultimately support a large treatment development network, different approaches are needed in order to increase the specificity and availability of CHR screening.

One potential solution is the use of biomarkers as stand-alone measures or to complement interviews. The first generation of CHR studies focused on neuropsychological and electrophysiological measures that were identified as markers of risk in family and “high-risk” studies over the last 30 years.49–52 These were sensible measures to study at the time the original CHR studies were undertaken, as these measures were reliably abnormal in ill patients and some proportion of first-degree relatives, suggesting that they were assessing fundamental aspects of illness risk. That approach also inevitably led to poor specificity. That is, risk or trait vulnerability markers are often abnormal in people who never develop diagnosable psychotic disorders.49 Such measures may be highly useful for the study of genetic risk; however, by definition, they will have unacceptable false positive rates in the prediction of conversion to psychosis. There are a number of neuroimaging biomarkers that may have enhanced specificity (eg, positron emission tomography imaging of dopamine synthesis capacity).20 However, it is unlikely that such costly measures will become widely available considering the expense involved and the expertise required.

In our view, the major challenge facing the field is to increase assessment sensitivity and specificity for conversion to psychosis and to do so at scale. We believe that there is a path forward to meet these challenges. The strongest predictor of conversion in the existing CHR literature is symptom severity at baseline—the worse the positive, negative, and disorganization symptoms at initial presentation, the higher the probability of conversion across time.14,53,54 Quantitative measures that assay the latent processes that underline symptom formation may offer a more sensitive assessment of evolving risk.

Recent advances in clinical cognitive neuroscience and computational psychiatry offer a mechanistic understanding of the genesis and maintenance of the defining symptoms of schizophrenia. As such, we might now predict conversion to psychosis, building upon the pioneering CHR work, but exploiting contemporary discoveries made long after those seminal projects were initiated. Taking a translational approach from the basic neuroscience of perception and cognition provides a conceptual framework and a set of behavioral paradigms that elicit symptom-relevant latent constructs.55 These constructs, in turn, have the potential to offer objective measures that are more precise and free from the drawbacks of subjective clinical interview measures: interviewers differ in their skill in eliciting precise information and patients display varying levels of insight and willingness to disclose potentially stigmatizing information. Furthermore, there may be different pathways to the same observed behavior with different implications for conversion. For example, it is now clear that mood and psychotic disorders are both associated with motivational deficits and reward processing abnormalities. However, the nature of these abnormalities differs. Mood disorders appear to be characterized by a true hedonic deficit, whereas people with schizophrenia exhibit motivational deficits consequent to impairment in explicit reinforcement learning (RL) and effort-cost decision-making, in the presence of intact reward sensitivity.56,57 It is possible that hedonic deficits may not prove to be predictive of conversion to psychosis among those at CHR, whereas abnormalities in RL and effort allocation may. We hypothesize that measures derived from specific mechanistic models of symptoms may serve as better predictors of conversion to full psychotic disorder than subjective, interview-based measures and provide a better signal-to-noise ratio in the face of the substantial clinical heterogeneity that is characteristic of CHR samples.

New Directions in the Understanding of Symptom Mechanisms

Here, we briefly describe the cognitive computational mechanisms that underly the positive, negative, and disorganization symptoms that are the major diagnostic features of psychotic disorders and schizophrenia. A burgeoning literature—seeded by Stephan, Friston, Frith and colleagues58—has begun to define the neural and behavioral mechanisms of perception and belief and their interactions,59–61 yielding a new understanding of hallucinations and delusions.62–66The idea that perception involves the combination of beliefs (or predictions) with incoming sensory data goes back at least to Helmholtz.67More recently, this fundamental notion has been cast in Bayesian terms where perception is considered as a form of probabilistic inference; incoming sensory information (likelihood) is compared against predictions (priors), and prediction errors (resulting when there is a mismatch between priors and incoming evidence) are computed between them, with the most likely cause of sensory data (posterior) becoming the percept.59–61 Priors are represented with a certain precision (the inverse variance of the distribution of possible values the data could take). If priors are more precise than sensory inputs, they will dominate inference, we will perceive what we expect, and prediction errors will be ignored. Alternatively, precise sensory evidence will dominate less precise priors and drive belief updating (changing one’s priors for subsequent inference). This scheme may be implemented via the hierarchical structure of cortex (priors are passed top-down, prediction errors bottom-up, through N-methyl-D-aspartate and a-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid, respectively).59–61Neuromodulators such as dopamine, acetylcholine, and serotonin (all implicated in schizophrenia68–71) are thought to underwrite the precision weighting of belief, perception, and their interactions.72 By denying a strict distinction between perception and belief,64 predictive coding provides a framework in which to understand hallucinations, delusions, and their co-occurrence.65

When prior beliefs are imprecise (or absent), many inputs will generate prediction errors and spur rapid learning and updating of the model of the world—humans and other animals are intolerant of uncertainty, even an erroneous belief is better than no explanation at all.72–74 This spurious learning may result from experiences (stimuli, thoughts, and percepts) that have been rendered aberrantly salient75 by virtue of contextually inappropriate dopaminergic signaling.76–78 There are a variety of behavioral methods that are able to capture this kind of spurious prediction-error-driven-learning, and these methods have a track record of being associated with the severity of delusions.76–78

Once formed, prior beliefs can become so over-weighted that no amount of contradictory evidence can generate a prediction error and drive new learning.77 This is a way of understanding tenaciously held delusional beliefs that are strongly resistant to contrary evidence under typical conditions.77 This same mechanism may be relevant for understanding hallucinations.66 Several recent experiments suggest that hallucinations may result from strong priors that result in the perception of sound or of speech in the absence of clear sensory evidence.79–81 The critical insight here is that perceptual systems are never truly quiescent—there is always a tonic activity that can be shaped and amplified by strong priors as seen, for example, in the commonplace experience of mistakenly believing that one’s name is being called.82 There are now a set of behavioral methods that are sensitive to individual differences in the proclivity to hear sounds in the absence of sounds and to perceive comprehensible speech when presented with highly degraded speech samples.80,81,83 We hypothesize that measures from both of these “pathways” toward idiosyncratic percepts and beliefs are relevant for the prediction of conversion to psychosis.

A different mechanistic framework offers a new understanding of the motivational impairments that are critical for functional outcomes. For many years, it was assumed that decreases in goal-directed behavior were caused by anhedonia: there is little reason to pursue goals if attaining them brings little pleasure. However, experimental evidence clearly demonstrates that people with schizophrenia appear to have largely normal “in the moment” experiences of pleasure and neural responses to the experience of rewarding outcomes. However, these experiences do not have the expected motivational impact in terms of driving subsequent behavior.84,85 A large body of experimental work provides two complementary perspectives on this impairment. First, a series of studies using a variety of RL paradigms suggest that people with schizophrenia have difficulty with explicit RL, whereas implicit RL may be intact.86,87 Some studies suggest that this is a consequence of impaired working memory processes that are required for explicit RL, which are critical for updating mental representations of value.88,89 Other studies suggest that learning from rewarding outcomes may be differentially impaired relative to learning from loss avoidance and punishment.90,91 Both lines of evidence suggest that deficits in RL underlie the failure to use value representations to drive decision-making and motivated behavior. There are a number of behavioral and computational modeling approaches that provide the ability to formally quantify the processes involved in generating this impairment.90,92 Formal modeling that is able to quantify the contribution of several different discrete processes to the overall performance offers much finer resolution on origins of motivational impairment than interview-based negative symptom ratings.

There is a closely related body of work looking at how people with schizophrenia weigh potential rewards (benefits) vs the costs of the actions needed to obtain those benefits. Well-replicated evidence indicates that people with schizophrenia tend to avoid making high effort choices associated with higher levels of reward, with this bias being most evident in patients with more severe negative symptoms.93,94 This is a rapidly developing literature, and there are new experimental and computational approaches that have promise in distinguishing the impact of reward devaluation from enhanced effort aversion.95 As with the RL literature discussed above, these behavioral paradigms and computational analytic approaches offer a much more nuanced and specific understanding of the processes implicated in clinically manifest negative symptoms. The frontal-striatal circuits implicated in RL and effort-based decision-making are systems long-implicated in schizophrenia.96–100

Clinically manifest disorganization symptoms such as formal thought disorder and inappropriate affect are generally mild in CHR populations and have not emerged as potent predictors of conversion to psychosis in the various risk calculators that have been developed.14,15,53,54,101 We suspect that the use of more sophisticated performance-based measures may be useful in detecting a wider range of individual differences, potentially enhancing predictive utility. In broad terms, symptoms of disorganization are thought to reflect alterations in the long-range connectivity and integration of distributed neural processing. Such dysconnectivity results in fragmentation in the coherence, or context-based linking, of mental representations, and in the sequencing of thought and motor behavior.102 Tasks assessing the aspects of visual context processing and perceptual organization have been studied in people with schizophrenia, and relationships with the severity of disorganization symptoms have been observed in multiple studies.103 As with positive and negative symptom mechanisms above, this approach to disorganization symptoms implicates a central neural abnormality observed in schizophrenia and conceptualizes laboratory-based examples of reduced representational organization as subtler forms of what is observed clinically as behavioral disorganization.

The utility of this proposed approach remains to be demonstrated. With recent support from NIMH for a five-site longitudinal study of 500 CHR, 500 psychiatric help-seeking controls, and 500 controls (Computerized Assessment of Psychosis Risk, or CAPR), we plan to administer a battery of computerized tasks that are tied to the symptom generation mechanisms described above. By including a help-seeking control group, we will be able to directly address issues of sensitivity as well as specificity and clinical heterogeneity. We hope to demonstrate that our approach enhances the precision of prediction of conversion as a prelude to making this type of screening available at scale on the Internet.

Summary

The translational approach—from the basic neuroscience of perception and cognition to the prediction of conversion to psychosis—involves two critical departures from the prevailing paradigm: (1) a shift from trait markers to cognitive and perceptual state markers related to symptom mechanisms and (2) a shift away from imaging biomarkers toward behavioral paradigms and computational analytic approaches that offer refined measurement of latent constructs implicated in symptom formation. Increased precision in the measurement of the mechanisms underlying symptom formation has the potential to enhance sensitivity and specificity over clinical interview measures. Behavioral measures also have the potential to be administered on the internet, increasing the availability of CHR screening.

Acknowledgments

Gold has consulted for Acadia and receives royalty payments from the BACS; Strauss is one of the original developers of the Brief Negative Symptom Scale (BNSS) and receives royalties and consultation fees from ProPhase LLC in connection with commercial use of the BNSS and other professional activities; these fees are donated to the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. Strauss has received honoraria and travel support from ProPhase LLC for training pharmaceutical company raters on the BNSS. Strauss has consulted for Minerva Neurosciences, Acadia, and Lundbeck. Other authors have no disclosures.

Funding

This work was supported by the following funding sources: National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grants R01 MH120090 (Gold), R01 MH112613 (Ellman), R01 MH120091 (Ellman), R01 MH120092 (Strauss), R01 MH116039 (Strauss/Mittal), R21 MH119438 (Strauss), R01 MH112545 (Mittal), R01 MH1120088 (Mittal), U01 MH081988 (Walker), R01 MH120090 (Waltz), R01 MH112612 (Schiffman), and R01 MH120089 (Corlett/Woods).

References

  • 1. Brown S. Excess mortality of schizophrenia. A meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 1997;171:502–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Olfson M, Gerhard T, Huang C, Crystal S, Stroup TS. Premature mortality among adults with schizophrenia in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(12):1172–1181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Kane JM, Robinson DG, Schooler NR, et al.  Comprehensive versus usual community care for first-episode psychosis: 2-year outcomes from the NIMH RAISE early treatment program. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173(4):362–372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Broome MR, Woolley JB, Johns LC, et al.  Outreach and support in south London (OASIS): implementation of a clinical service for prodromal psychosis and the at risk mental state. Eur Psychiatry. 2005;20(5-6):372–378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Cannon TD, Cadenhead K, Cornblatt B, et al.  Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(1):28–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, et al.  Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):703–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, et al.  Prospective diagnosis of the initial prodrome for schizophrenia based on the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes: preliminary evidence of interrater reliability and predictive validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(5):863–865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Phillips LJ, Yung AR, McGorry PD. Identification of young people at risk of psychosis: validation of Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation Clinic intake criteria. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2000;34(Suppl):S164–S169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Ruhrmann S, Schultze-Lutter F, Salokangas RK, et al.  Prediction of psychosis in adolescents and young adults at high risk: results from the prospective European prediction of psychosis study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):241–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Woods SW, Addington J, Cadenhead KS, et al.  Validity of the prodromal risk syndrome for first psychosis: findings from the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(5):894–908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Yung AR, McGorry PD. The initial prodrome in psychosis: descriptive and qualitative aspects. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1996;30(5):587–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Zhang T, Li H, Woodberry KA, et al.  Prodromal psychosis detection in a counseling center population in China: an epidemiological and clinical study. Schizophr Res. 2014;152(2-3):391–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Cannon TD, Chung Y, He G, et al. ; North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study Consortium Progressive reduction in cortical thickness as psychosis develops: a multisite longitudinal neuroimaging study of youth at elevated clinical risk. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(2):147–157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Cannon TD, Yu C, Addington J, et al.  An individualized risk calculator for research in prodromal psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173(10):980–988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Carrión RE, Cornblatt BA, Burton CZ, et al.  Personalized prediction of psychosis: external validation of the NAPLS-2 psychosis risk calculator with the EDIPPP project. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173(10):989–996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. De La Fuente-Sandoval C, León-Ortiz P, Favila R, et al.  Higher levels of glutamate in the associative-striatum of subjects with prodromal symptoms of schizophrenia and patients with first-episode psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011;36(9):1781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Dean DJ, Walther S, Bernard JA, Mittal VA. Motor clusters reveal differences in risk for psychosis, cognitive functioning, and thalamocortical connectivity: evidence for vulnerability subtypes. Clin Psychol Sci. 2018;6(5):721–734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Fusar-Poli P, Bonoldi I, Yung AR, et al.  Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(3):220–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Fusar-Poli P, McGuire P, Borgwardt S. Mapping prodromal psychosis: a critical review of neuroimaging studies. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(3):181–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Howes OD, Montgomery AJ, Asselin MC, Murray RM, Grasby PM, McGuire PK. Molecular imaging studies of the striatal dopaminergic system in psychosis and predictions for the prodromal phase of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2007;51:s13–s18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Perez VB, Woods SW, Roach BJ, et al.  Automatic auditory processing deficits in schizophrenia and clinical high-risk patients: forecasting psychosis risk with mismatch negativity. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(6):459–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Hamilton HK, Roach BJ, Bachman PM, et al.  Association between P300 responses to auditory oddball stimuli and clinical outcomes in the psychosis risk syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(11):1187–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Chung Y, Allswede D, Addington J, et al. ; North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS) Consortium Cortical abnormalities in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis: findings from the NAPLS2 cohort. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;23:101862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Seidman LJ, Giuliano AJ, Meyer EC, et al. ; North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS) Group Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):578–588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Walker EF, Trotman HD, Pearce BD, et al.  Cortisol levels and risk for psychosis: initial findings from the North American prodrome longitudinal study. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(6):410–417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Hartmann JA, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, et al.  Declining transition rates to psychotic disorder in “ultra-high risk” clients: investigation of a dilution effect. Schizophr Res. 2016;170(1):130–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Nelson B, Yuen HP, Wood SJ, et al.  Long-term follow-up of a group at ultra high risk (“prodromal”) for psychosis: the PACE 400 study. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(8):793–802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Simon AE, Velthorst E, Nieman DH, Linszen D, Umbricht D, de Haan L. Ultra high-risk state for psychosis and non-transition: a systematic review. Schizophr Res. 2011;132(1):8–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Wiltink S, Velthorst E, Nelson B, McGorry PM, Yung AR. Declining transition rates to psychosis: the contribution of potential changes in referral pathways to an ultra-high-risk service. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2015;9(3):200–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Yung AR, Yuen HP, Berger G, et al.  Declining transition rate in ultra high risk (prodromal) services: dilution or reduction of risk?  Schizophr Bull.  2007;33(3):673–681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Fusar-Poli P. The hype cycle of the clinical high risk state for psychosis: the need of a refined approach. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(2):250–253. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Fusar-Poli P, Schultze-Lutter F, Cappucciati M, et al.  The dark side of the moon: meta-analytical impact of recruitment strategies on risk enrichment in the clinical high risk state for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(3):732–743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. van Os J, Guloksuz S. A critique of the “ultra-high risk” and “transition” paradigm. World Psychiatry. 2017;16(2):200–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. McGorry PD, Nelson B, Markulev C, et al.  Effect of ω-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in young people at ultrahigh risk for psychotic disorders: the NEURAPRO randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(1):19–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Nelson B, Amminger GP, Yuen HP, et al.  NEURAPRO: a multi-centre RCT of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids versus placebo in young people at ultra-high risk of psychotic disorders-medium-term follow-up and clinical course. NPJ Schizophr. 2018;4(1):11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Fusar-Poli P, Yung AR, McGorry P, van Os J. Lessons learned from the psychosis high-risk state: towards a general staging model of prodromal intervention. Psychol Med. 2014;44(1):17–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. McGorry PD, Yung AR, Phillips LJ. The “close-in” or ultra high-risk model: a safe and effective strategy for research and clinical intervention in prepsychotic mental disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):771–790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Thompson E, Millman ZB, Okuzawa N, et al.  Evidence-based early interventions for individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: a review of treatment components. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203(5):342–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Birchwood M, Connor C, Lester H, et al.  Reducing duration of untreated psychosis: care pathways to early intervention in psychosis services. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(1):58–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Boydell KM, Gladstone BM, Volpe T. Understanding help seeking delay in the prodrome to first episode psychosis: a secondary analysis of the perspectives of young people. Psychiatr Rehabil J. 2006;30(1):54–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Klosterkötter J, Ruhrmann S, Schultze-Lutter F, et al.  The European Prediction of Psychosis Study (EPOS): integrating early recognition and intervention in Europe. World Psychiatry. 2005;4(3):161–167. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Rietdijk J, Hogerzeil SJ, van Hemert AM, Cuijpers P, Linszen DH, van der Gaag M. Pathways to psychosis: help-seeking behavior in the prodromal phase. Schizophr Res. 2011;132(2-3):213–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Schultze-Lutter F, Michel C, Schmidt SJ, et al.  EPA guidance on the early detection of clinical high risk states of psychoses. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30(3):405–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Singh SP, Grange T. Measuring pathways to care in first-episode psychosis: a systematic review. Schizophr Res. 2006;81(1):75–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Stowkowy J, Colijn MA, Addington J. Pathways to care for those at clinical high risk of developing psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2013;7(1):80–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Cooper S, Kring AM, Ellman LM. Attenuated positive psychotic symptoms and the experience of anhedonia. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2018;12(6):1188–1192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Millman ZB, Pitts SC, Thompson E, et al.  Perceived social stress and symptom severity among help-seeking adolescents with versus without clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2018;192:364–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Pelletier-Baldelli A, Strauss GP, Visser KH, Mittal VA. Initial development and preliminary psychometric properties of the Prodromal Inventory of Negative Symptoms (PINS). Schizophr Res. 2017;189:43–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Cornblatt BA. The New York high risk project to the Hillside recognition and prevention (RAP) program. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114(8):956–966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Cornblatt BA, Lencz T, Smith CW, Correll CU, Auther AM, Nakayama E. The schizophrenia prodrome revisited: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):633–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Light G, Greenwood TA, Swerdlow NR, et al.  Comparison of the heritability of schizophrenia and endophenotypes in the COGS-1 family study. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(6):1404–1411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Snitz BE, Macdonald AW 3rd, Carter CS. Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):179–194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Zhang T, Xu L, Tang Y, et al. ; SHARP (ShangHai At Risk for Psychosis) Study Group Prediction of psychosis in prodrome: development and validation of a simple, personalized risk calculator. Psychol Med. 2019;49(12):1990–1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Osborne KJ, Mittal VA. External validation and extension of the NAPLS-2 and SIPS-RC personalized risk calculators in an independent clinical high-risk sample. Psychiatry Res. 2019;279:9–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Browning M, Carter C, Chatham CH, et al.  Realizing the clinical potential of  computational psychiatry: report from the Banbury Center meeting, February 2019. Biol Psychiatry. 2020. doi: 10.1016/biopsych.2019.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Barch DM, Pagliaccio D, Luking K. Mechanisms underlying motivational deficits in psychopathology: similarities and differences in depression and schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2016;27:411–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Culbreth AJ, Moran EK, Barch DM. Effort-cost decision-making in psychosis and depression: could a similar behavioral deficit arise from disparate psychological and neural mechanisms? Psychol Med. 2018;48(6):889–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(3):509–527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Friston K. Does predictive coding have a future? Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(8):1019–1021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Friston K. The free-energy principle: a rough guide to the brain? Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(7):293–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Friston K, Frith C. A Duet for one. Conscious Cogn. 2015;36:390–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Adams RA, Stephan KE, Brown HR, Frith CD, Friston KJ. The computational anatomy of psychosis. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Corlett PR, Frith CD, Fletcher PC. From drugs to deprivation: a Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;206(4):515–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Fletcher PC, Frith CD. Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):48–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Sterzer P, Adams RA, Fletcher P, et al.  The predictive coding account of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84(9):634–643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Corlett PR, Horga G, Fletcher PC, Alderson-Day B, Schmack K, Powers AR 3rd. Hallucinations and strong priors. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23(2):114–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Helmholtz von H. The Facts of Perception. In: Kahl R, ed. Selected Writings of Herman von Helmholtz. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press; 1878/1971. [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Howes O, McCutcheon R, Stone J. Glutamate and dopamine in schizophrenia: an update for the 21st century. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29(2):97–115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Girgis RR, Zoghbi AW, Javitt DC, Lieberman JA. The past and future of novel, non-dopamine-2 receptor therapeutics for schizophrenia: a critical and comprehensive review. J Psychiatr Res. 2019;108:57–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Terry AV Jr, Callahan PM. α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors as therapeutic targets in schizophrenia: update on animal and clinical studies and strategies for the future. Neuropharmacology 2020;170:108053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Stahl SM. Beyond the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia to three neural networks of psychosis: dopamine, serotonin, and glutamate. CNS Spectr. 2018;23(3):187–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Marshall L, Mathys C, Ruge D, et al.  Pharmacological fingerprints of contextual uncertainty. PLoS Biol. 2016;14(11):e1002575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Feeney EJ, Groman SM, Taylor JR, Corlett PR. Explaining delusions: reducing uncertainty through basic and computational neuroscience. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(2):263–272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Fineberg SK, Corlett PR. The doxastic shear pin: delusions as errors of learning and memory. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2016;21(1):73–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(1):13–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Corlett PR, Honey GD, Fletcher PC. From prediction error to psychosis: ketamine as a pharmacological model of delusions. J Psychopharmacol. 2007;21(3):238–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Corlett PR, Krystal JH, Taylor JR, Fletcher PC. Why do delusions persist? Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3:12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Corlett PR, Taylor JR, Wang XJ, Fletcher PC, Krystal JH. Toward a neurobiology of delusions. Prog Neurobiol. 2010;92(3):345–369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Cassidy CM, Balsam PD, Weinstein JJ, et al.  A perceptual inference mechanism for hallucinations linked to striatal dopamine. Curr Biol. 2018;28(4):503–514.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Powers AR, Mathys C, Corlett PR. Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science 2017;357(6351):596–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Alderson-Day B, Lima CF, Evans S, et al.  Distinct processing of ambiguous speech in people with non-clinical auditory verbal hallucinations. Brain 2017;140(9):2475–2489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Horga G, Abi-Dargham A. An integrative framework for perceptual disturbances in psychosis. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019;20(12):763–778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Kafadar E, Mittal VA, Strauss G, et al.  Modeling perception and behavior in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: support for the predictive processing framework. Schizophr Res. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Strauss GP, Gold JM. A new perspective on anhedonia in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(4):364–373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Prentice KJ, Morris SE, Heerey EA. Reward processing in schizophrenia: a deficit in the representation of value. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(5):835–847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Heerey EA, Bell-Warren KR, Gold JM. Decision-making impairments in the context of intact reward sensitivity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(1):62–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Barch DM, Carter CS, Gold JM, et al.  Explicit and implicit reinforcement learning across the psychosis spectrum. J Abnorm Psychol. 2017;126(5):694–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Collins AGE, Albrecht MA, Waltz JA, Gold JM, Frank MJ. Interactions among working memory, reinforcement learning, and effort in value-based choice: a new paradigm and selective deficits in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82(6):431–439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Collins AG, Brown JK, Gold JM, Waltz JA, Frank MJ. Working memory contributions to reinforcement learning impairments in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2014;34(41):13747–13756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Matveeva TM, et al.  Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: behavioral and computational modeling evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(2):129–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Reinen J, Smith EE, Insel C, et al.  Patients with schizophrenia are impaired when learning in the context of pursuing rewards. Schizophr Res. 2014;152(1):309–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Hernaus D, Frank MJ, Brown EC, Brown JK, Gold JM, Waltz JA. Impaired expected value computations in schizophrenia are associated with a reduced ability to integrate reward probability and magnitude of recent outcomes. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4(3):280–290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Frank MJ. Effort cost computation in schizophrenia: a commentary on the recent literature. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(11):747–753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Cooper JA, Barch DM, Reddy LF, Horan WP, Green MF, Treadway MT. Effortful goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia: computational subtypes and associations with cognition. J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(7):710–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Le Heron C, Plant O, Manohar S, et al.  Distinct effects of apathy and dopamine on effort-based decision-making in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 2018;141(5):1455–1469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Waltz JA, Gold JM. Motivational deficits in schizophrenia and the representation of expected value. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2016;27:375–410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Dandash O, Pantelis C, Fornito A. Dopamine, fronto-striato-thalamic circuits and risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2017;180:48–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Maia TV, Frank MJ. An integrative perspective on the role of dopamine in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81(1):52–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99. McCutcheon RA, Abi-Dargham A, Howes OD. Schizophrenia, dopamine and the striatum: from biology to symptoms. Trends Neurosci. 2019;42(3):205–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Weinstein JJ, Chohan MO, Slifstein M, Kegeles LS, Moore H, Abi-Dargham A. Pathway-specific dopamine abnormalities in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81(1):31–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Oliver D, Radua J, Reichenberg A, Uher R, Fusar-Poli P. Psychosis Polyrisk Score (PPS) for the detection of individuals at-risk and the prediction of their outcomes. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102. Phillips WA, Silverstein SM. Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behav Brain Sci. 2003;26(1):65–82; discussion 82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103. Uhlhaas PJ, Silverstein SM. Perceptual organization in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: empirical research and theoretical implications. Psychol Bull. 2005;131(4):618–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Schizophrenia Bulletin are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES