Summary
Objective
To determine if closed-loop optogenetic seizure intervention, previously shown to reduce seizure duration in a well-established mouse model chronic temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), also improves the associated comorbidity of impaired spatial memory.
Methods
Mice with chronic, spontaneous seizures in the unilateral intrahippocampal kainic acid model of TLE, expressing channelrhodopsin in parvalbumin-expressing interneurons were implanted with optical fibers and electrodes, and tested for response to closed-loop light intervention of seizures. Animals that responded to closed-loop optogenetic curtailment of seizures were tested in the object location memory test and then given closed-loop optogenetic intervention on all detected seizures for two weeks. Following this, they were tested with a second object location memory test, with different objects and contexts than used previously, to assess if seizure suppression can improve deficits in spatial memory.
Results
Animals that received closed-loop optogenetic intervention performed significantly better in the second object location memory test compared to the first test. Epileptic controls with no intervention showed stable frequency and duration of seizures, as well as stable spatial memory deficits, for several months after the precipitating insult.
Significance
Many currently available treatments for epilepsy target seizures but not the associated comorbidities, which therefore are often not improved through these treatments. There is a need to investigate new potential therapies that may be able to improve both seizure burden and associated comorbidities of epilepsy. In this study, we showed that optogenetic intervention may be able to both shorten seizure duration and improve cognitive outcomes of spatial memory.
Keywords: behavior, closed-loop, cognitive impairment, comorbidity, temporal lobe epilepsy
Introduction
Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most common form of epilepsy found in adults and tends to be medically refractory to currently available treatments. The seizures that occur in epilepsy are usually difficult to predict and can be greatly disruptive to the lives of patients. However, in addition to seizures, epilepsy is often accompanied by a host of comorbidities that can include a range of cognitive problems.1 In the case of TLE, it is known that there are problems such as deficits in memory retention, remote memory, and spatial memory.2–4 Moreover, many current anti-epileptic drugs (AEDs) have a broad course of action and can potentially cause significant side effects as well, including cognitive problems.5 New treatment strategies for TLE are needed that can both target seizures with high accuracy and precision, and also alleviate aberrant network activity which may arise as a comorbidity of epilepsy or inadvertently as a side effect of AEDs.
Several tools are already available to study such potential new treatment strategies in animal models of TLE, including activation or inhibition of specific neuronal populations by optogenetics or chemogenetics.6–8 In different rodent models of TLE, animals show both spontaneous seizures9 and comorbidities such as cognitive deficits in spatial memory10,11, and thus provide a suitable model to test the effect of specific network manipulations on lowering the seizure burden and cognitive comorbidities. Optogenetics is a particularly promising experimental seizure therapy in animal models of epilepsy due to its ability to target specific cell types at high temporal specificity via light pulses.12–14 Previous studies showed that optogenetic tools can be effectively deployed to stop chronic seizures in animal models of TLE in an on-demand manner to minimize potential off-target effects.15–17 However, it remains unknown if closed-loop seizure intervention, in addition to temporarily decreasing seizure duration, also ameliorates other comorbidities associated with TLE such as cognitive deficits in learning and memory.
As interference with normal neural functions due to epileptic activity can disrupt learning and memory2, we hypothesized that optogenetic suppression of seizures in an on-demand manner, due to its temporal and spatial specificity, may not only alleviate seizure burden but potentially also improve memory performance in TLE. To this end, we tested if closed-loop optogenetic intervention of seizures, in individuals in which it is verifiably effective in shortening seizure duration, can improve spatial memory in the well-established intrahippocampal kainic acid model of TLE.
Methods
Animals
All procedures were approved and performed in accordance to the Administrative Panel of Laboratory Animal Care of Stanford University (Protocol 30183), the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of California, Irvine (Protocol 1999–1719), and with the animal care guidelines of the National Institute of Health. Male and female mice used in this study were generated from crossing lines expressing cre in parvalbumin (PV)-expressing interneurons (PV-Cre;B6;129P2-Pvalbtm1(Cre)Arbr/J; Jackson Labs stock #008069) to cre-dependent floxed-Stop ChR2 animals (Ai32; Rosa-CAG-LSLChR2H134R-EYFP-deltaNeo, obtained from the Allen Institute; also available as Ai32D, Jackson labs stock #012569)18. From this cross, animals expressing channelrhodopsin-2 in PV-expressing cells (PV-ChR2) (Figure S1) were used for all experiments. In addition, siblings negative for the opsin were used as nonepileptic behavioral controls. Animals were given food and water ad libitum and housed in a 12-hour light/12-hour dark cycle. All animals were group housed, except after going through electrode and optical fiber implant surgery, after which they were singly housed.
Stereotaxic surgeries
Intrahippocampal kainic acid (IHKA) injections were performed as previously described.16 Briefly, mice were placed under isofluorane (4%) anesthesia and given local anesthetic, 0.5% bupivacaine, at the site of incision. Kainic acid (80–100 nl, 20 mM in saline, Tocris Bioscience) was injected into the dorsal hippocampus (from bregma: 2.0mm posterior, 1.25mm left, 1.6 mm ventral). Animals were at least six weeks of age or greater. After injection, animals were allowed to recover and then were returned to the vivarium for at least two weeks to allow for emergence of chronic spontaneous seizures.
For optrode implants, animals that had undergone intrahippocampal kainic acid injections were given implants as previously described.16 A bipolar depth electrode (PlasticsOne) attached to an optical fiber (0.37NA, Low OH, 200 μm diameter optical fiber (Thorlabs) terminating in a zirconia ferrule (Kientec Systems) was implanted into the left dorsal hippocampus ipsilateral to the kainic acid injection (from bregma: 2.5mm posterior, 1.75mm left, 1.25m ventral). Another optical fiber was implanted at the contralateral side (from bregma: 2.5mm posterior, 1.75mm right, and 1.25mm ventral). The electrode and optical fibers were cemented to the skull using micro screws (McMaster-Carr and U.S. Micro Screw) and dental cement (Prime Dent Light Curable and Teets Cold Curing). Animals were given buprenorphine SR (0.5 mg/kg) as an analgesic. They were returned to the vivarium after the surgery to recover for at least three days before beginning videoEEG monitoring and closed-loop optogenetic intervention of seizures. Placement of optical fibers were verified post hoc (Figure S2).
VideoEEG monitoring and closed-loop optogenetic intervention
Animals with optrode implants were put through 24/7 video EEG monitoring within a dedicated animal housing and recording room. Each animal was connected to an electrical commutator (PlasticsOne) routed to an amplifier (BrownLee 410), and in turn connected to a digitizer (National Instruments USB-6221) and a computer running custom MATLAB recorder software.19 Animals were continuously monitored to establish the presence of spontaneous electrographic and behavioral seizures through the seizure detection algorithm programmed in the MATLAB recorder.
For closed-loop optogenetic intervention, epileptic animals were connected from the implanted optrode to an optical rotary joint (Doric Lenses) using an optical fiber patch cord (Doric Lenses) and ceramic split sleeve (Precision Fibre Products Inc). The optical rotary joint was then connected through an optical fiber patch cord to a blue laser (473 nm, Shanghai Laser & Optics Century Co., Ltd). Lasers were controlled by the MATLAB recording software that was individually tuned to each mouse by gradual adjustments to light intensity and seizure detection criteria settings as previously described.16,19 Briefly, the custom MATLAB program is able to detect seizures using detection criteria provided by the experimenter for LFP spikes (filtering, amplitude threshold, width, template matching), LFP spike clusters (inter-spike interval, inter-cluster interval, minimal duration) and artefact rejection (filters, signal features), while combing them using Boolean logic. Processed files are verified and if necessary corrected by the experimenter on their detection accuracy of seizure starts and ends. This program was set to detect seizures with the criteria of all spike clusters that lasted at least five seconds in duration and were separated by at least three seconds from another cluster of spikes. The laser was also set to respond to either 50% or 100% of detected seizures, depending on the experiment. When analyzing the effectiveness of laser stimulation by setting response rate to 50% of detected seizures, experimenters were blinded to light status of each seizure during analysis. Laser stimulation consisted of 50 ms light on, 100 ms light off, for 15 seconds total. After light stimulation, another light stimulation was prevented for the next 30 seconds to avoid overstimulation.
Behavior testing
The object location memory (OLM) test was performed as previously described.20 To test for hippocampus-based place learning and memory, animals were first handled for four days for two minutes per day. Following this, animals were habituated to the empty testing arena, which was an acrylic box (30×23×23 cm) lined with bedding, for five minutes per day for six days, with handling days overlapping with habituation days. After this phase, two identical objects were placed into the arena, and the mice were allowed to freely explore the objects for 10 minutes and then returned to their homecage. After 24 hours, one object was moved to a new location and mice were returned to the arena and allowed to explore for five minutes. All tests were performed approximately between 2pm and 6pm. Videos were taken of each session using a Panasonic Video Camera or Logitech Webcam and manually scored for the time duration in which animals explored the objects (oriented towards the objects and within 1 cm from the object), using previously described criteria for exploration.20 Experimenters were blinded to the intervention type of the animals. Animals that showed side preference during training (DI>20) 20 or explored less than 2.8 seconds in testing, or had a behavioral seizure during testing, were excluded. In cases where a follow-up OLM test was performed, all aspects of the test were the same except the arena was changed to a novel colored box, new bedding and different objects. A discrimination index was calculated using the time duration spent exploring each object: (time exploring moved object – time exploring unmoved object)/total exploration time*100. Other analyses such as quantification of running speed and distance traveled was calculated using Any-Maze (Stoelting Co).
Statistics
In all experiments, normality was tested using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to determine the use of parametric and nonparametric tests. Comparison of discrimination index between control and IHKA animals was made using the two-tailed Mann-Whitney Test. Correlation between discrimination index and time from IHKA treatment was made with linear regression analysis and calculation of Pearson’s R. Running speed comparison between IHKA and control animals utilized the student’s t-test. Comparison of seizure duration and seizure frequency between different months from IHKA was evaluated using the Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA followed by Dunn’s Test for individual comparisons. Seizure duration in no light and light conditions was compared using the two-tailed Mann-Whitney test. Comparisons of discrimination index between two sequential behavior tests utilized the Wilcoxon signed rank test. For OLM tests, it was estimated that 8 animals per group would allow a detection of discrimination index change of 20 with a standard deviation of 14, based on a sample size calculation for a 0.05 probability of type-1 error and a 20% false negative rate. All values are presented as mean ± s.e.m., and a p-value < 0.05 was considered significant. Statistical analyses were performed using OriginPro 2019 and Microsoft Excel 365.
Results
Spatial memory deficits occur early and remain consistent in a rodent model of TLE
Along with chronic seizures, rodent models of TLE showed deficits in learning and memory, especially in tasks requiring spatial memory10,21,22, similar to that observed in patients with TLE.3 Previous experiments with IHKA rats and mice established that after an initial status epilepticus, the seizure probability successively increases in a sigmoid fashion until eventually reaching a plateau during the so-called chronic period.23–27 To determine if learning and memory deficits are dependent on the time from the initial insult, we tested animals using the OLM test as described previously.20 This task has been shown to be hippocampus dependent28 and relies on the animals’ ability to recall the location of items that they encountered previously and preferentially explore the object that has been moved (Figure 1A). We tested three separate groups of animals, each at a different time point: 1) 10 days post IHKA, during the so-called latent period23, 2) 3–4 months post IHKA, during the chronic phase, and 3) 6–7 months post IHKA, which is well into the chronic period.
The three IHKA groups displayed deficits in the OLM task compared to non-epileptic controls at 10 days post IHKA (control: DI=29.5±2.2, n=6; IHKA: DI=5.9±4.6, n=10, p<0.001), 3–4 months post IHKA (control: DI=35.6±4.0, n=5; IHKA: DI=11.0±2.8, n=9, p=0.002), and 6–7 months post IHKA (control: DI=30.6±3.7, n=14; IHKA: DI=7.9±2.0, n=9, p<0.001)(Figure 1B, Table S1). As we showed in a previous study(29), IHKA mice displayed deficits compared with non-epileptic controls and did not discriminate effectively between the moved and unmoved object (DI<20). Quantification of the total exploration time of the objects remained the same between IHKA-treated and control animals at 10 days (control: 4.0±0.4s; IHKA: 4.5±0.3s), 3–4 months after IHKA (control: 3.9±0.4s; IHKA: 4.1±0.2s), and 6–7 months after IHKA (control: 5.0±0.2s; IHKA: 4.5±0.4s), indicating that the deficit in OLM performance in IHKA animals was not due to lack of exploration (Figure 1C).
More specifically, when comparing each animal’s OLM performance to the time of IHKA injection, there was no significant correlation between time the animals’ performance and the time since they received IHKA (Fig 1D, Pearson’s R=0.06). In addition, we found no significant differences between the discrimination indexes of male and female animals in either IHKA and controls (control female: DI=30.3±3.5, n=16; control male: DI=27.1±4.0, n=12; IHKA female: DI=9.5±3.4, n=23; IHKA male: DI=6.0±1.8, n=27)(Figure 1E), therefore all behavior data were combined and presented as a combination of both sexes. While the velocity of animals in the OLM tests was significantly increased in IHKA compared to control animals during the five minutes of exploring the empty box over six days of habituation (control: 0.028±0.002 m/s, n=34; IHKA: 0.048±0.002 m/s, n=50, p<0.001)(Figure 1F), overall exploration time in OLM tests between IHKA and controls remained similar despite this difference in overall speed of movement (Figure 1C). Note that all animals were detached from the closed-loop stimulation equipment during all phases of OLM testing.
Seizure frequency and duration in the intrahippocampal kainic acid model of TLE are stable in the chronic period
As our goals are to possibly alter the seizure burden and deficit in spatial learning after epileptogenesis during the chronic phase in IHKA animals, we tested if the seizure burden continues changing over time or if the animals eventually reached a relatively constant seizure phenotype with a steady state in seizure frequency as previously suggested in IHKA rats and mice.23–27 While there have been several studies that describe the early development of epilepsy in the IHKA mouse model23,24,26,27,30,31, few if any have investigated the stability of seizure burden across several months into the chronic period.
To test this, we recorded IHKA treated animals that were implanted after reaching the chronic phase of temporal lobe epilepsy utilizing continuous videoEEG. The recorded videoEEG data were analyzed post hoc to identify and quantify seizures in a semi-automated manner. While animals were observed to have both seizures without behavioral manifestation, i.e. electrographic seizures (Figure 2A), and seizures with behavioral manifestation (Figure 2B), electrographic seizures were much more frequent in the murine IHKA model.32 We first recorded animals over the course of 24 hours to observe seizure duration and frequency over time (n=4)(Figure 2C, D). Though seizure frequency fluctuated over the course of the day, there was no significant difference in the number of seizures over different hours (Figure 2D), but there was a significant difference in seizure duration over the course of the day (p<0.001) (Figure 2C). Due to this, we chose to analyze only a specific time period of the day, i.e. the first five hours after the light-on phase, for long term videoEEG analysis.
VideoEEG data was taken from this time span from epileptic animals on one day per month over the course of six months and was analyzed for seizure frequency and duration (n=4)(Figure 2E, F). Overall, there was no difference in seizure frequency or duration (p=0.76 and p=0.24 respectively), suggesting relative stability of the seizure phenotype this far into the chronic phase of TLE.
Optogenetic intervention of seizures utilizing PV interneuron activation improves spatial memory
The results of the OLM tests at different time points after IHKA administration suggest that the deficit in OLM performance is consistent from the time of epileptogenesis and into the chronic phase of epilepsy. The results also indicate that the seizure parameters are relatively stable well into the chronic period of the IHKA model and do not continue to increase in seizure frequency as has been observed during earlier stages.23–27 Previous work showed that optogenetic activation of PV interneurons in animals expressing channelrhodopsin-2 specifically in PV cells was able to significantly decrease the duration of electrographic seizures.16 To test if using this method of seizure suppression has effects on spatial learning ability, we designed an experimental paradigm (Figure 3A) in which IHKA animals were first tested for effective closed-loop optogenetic seizure suppression and then those animals where the intervention was efficacious in curtailing seizures (see Discussion) were given an initial OLM test along with control IHKA and non-epileptic controls, to measure baseline performance. Following the initial OLM test, the experimental IHKA group received continuous closed-loop optogenetic seizure intervention for one week, after which time the second OLM test was performed. This group received continuous optogenetic intervention for 15 days until the final OLM testing day with the exception of a 3-hour span daily for the last eight days of the intervention in which the second OLM test took place. This experimental paradigm required multiple successful sequential steps lasting over a minimum of nine weeks, and therefore acquiring epileptic animals that reached the endpoint was extremely challenging (Table S2).
We first confirmed that PV-ChR2 animals that received a modified closed-loop optogenetic intervention paradigm16 showed a significant decrease in seizure duration when blue light was provided compared to when no light was provided during a seizure (No light: 16.5±1.5s; Light: 8.0±0.8s, n=8, p<0.001)(Figure 3B, C). The individual seizure detection settings determined during this time were utilized for all later closed-loop optogenetic seizure suppression experiments.
We found that the epileptic group receiving optogenetic intervention performed significantly better in the second OLM test compared to the first test (OLM 1: DI=8.5±4.1; OLM 2: DI=21.3±4.1, n=8, p=0.04)(Figure 3D, left panel). In comparison, the epileptic control group with no intervention did not significantly change their performance (OLM 1: DI=9.5±2.7; OLM 2: DI=8.5±3.9, n=18, p=0.58)(Figure 3D, center panel), while non-epileptic controls showed a significant decrease in performance between the two tests (OLM 1: DI=27.5±2.6; OLM2: DI=21.7±2.4, n=34, p=0.01)(Figure 3D, right panel). Opsin negative siblings were used as nonepileptic controls as their performance in OLM remained the same as opsin positive animals (Figure S3). The nonepileptic controls scored an average DI of greater than 20 in both OLM tests (in spite of the decreased performance in OLM, most likely attributable to retesting effects), signifying that they were still able to clearly discriminate between the moved and unmoved object in both tests.20 No differences were found in the exploration times between the first and second OLM tests within each group (Epileptic with laser, OLM 1: 4.4±0.3s, OLM 2: 4.6±0.5s; Epileptic w/o laser, OLM 1: 4.6±0.2s, OLM 2: 4.5±0.2s, Nonepileptic, OLM 1: 4.8± 0.2s, OLM 2: 4.4±0.2s)(Figure 3E, Table S1).
Discussion
Epilepsy treatments that reduce the burden of seizures and improve comorbidities associated with epilepsy represent an unmet need in the development of new therapies. In this study, we confirmed that optogenetic activation of PV interneurons in a closed-loop manner is able to significantly decrease the duration of electrographic seizures in the IHKA rodent model of TLE.16 The key finding of the current study is that in addition to reducing the seizure burden, this treatment also significantly improved performance in a spatial memory task compared to performance prior to the optogenetic seizure suppression. This suggests that an on-demand seizure treatment is able to improve memory even without treatment during the performance of the actual task, as animals are not given light stimulation during the OLM task. In addition, the stability in seizure duration and frequency (Figure 2) and consistent deficits in OLM performance over time after IHKA (Figure 1) are significant because they suggest the therapeutic window for closed-loop optogenetic seizure treatment may potentially remain open throughout the chronic phase.
PV interneuron stimulation and improved cognition
Our findings are further supported by studies that found that targeting and activation of parvalbumin interneurons at specific stimulation frequencies was able to ameliorate spatial learning deficits. For example, rhythmic PV interneuron activation in the hippocampal CA1 in sleep-deprived animals during non-REM sleep enhanced performance in memory tasks.33 As seizures in IHKA mice can occur during all brain states, with a majority (~90%) arising from a non-theta state34, animals in our present study likely received some light activation of PV interneurons during non-REM sleep at a theta frequency (~7 Hz) similar to that used in the described study33 Rhythmic activation of PV interneurons has been shown to have therapeutic effects in the gamma frequency as well. This stimulation paradigm has been shown to reduce the pathology in a model of Alzheimer’s disease, by activating glial responses.35 Glia are also important in in TLE30 thus providing another possible means by which our intervention may have affected learning outcomes in this study. Other results also have shown that PV activation using chemogenetic methods is able to decrease seizure burden but does not improve learning performance.36 However, one caveat in the latter study is that the chemogenetic intervention occurred during the learning phase of the OLM task, unlike this current study where optogenetic intervention was instead provided outside of the behavioral task. This suggests that intervention may need to be provided at specific timepoints outside of the behavioral task, or in an on-demand manner which could reduce interference with normal PV function.
Limitations of the current results and their implications for future studies
This study purposely tested the effects of seizure suppression on memory performance only in those chronically epileptic animals where the closed-loop optogenetic intervention was successful in significantly curtailing the seizures. This experimental design was implemented in order to minimize variability arising from the fact that not all mice in which closed-loop optogenetic intervention of seizures is attempted actually show successful seizure suppression (e.g., due to a malfunctioning or incorrectly placed optrode and other technical confounds16,19). Given the latter caveat, a number of interesting questions arise from the findings from this study. First, whether varying the timing or targeting of the optogenetic seizure intervention can affect the improvement in memory. For example, it is possible that intervention is only required during the week the animal spends in behavior testing, or even only the period of memory consolidation between training and testing. Second, the mechanism by which the optogenetic intervention of seizures is able to ameliorate spatial memory deficits remains unknown. It is possible that the shortening of the seizure durations and thus lowering the overall seizure burden in epileptic animals in itself is able to improve cognition by reducing overall disruption of network activity. Third, it would have to be established in future studies if PV interneuron activation is actually necessary for the improvement in location discrimination or if the same effect can be achieved using other neuronal targets. For example, seizure suppression was achieved not only by PV interneuron activation, but also by inhibition of principle neurons in the dorsal hippocampus.16 Fourth, it is unknown if targeting seizures is required for the beneficial effects in learning and memory or if random or periodic PV activation could also work, especially considering experimental results indicating that PV interneuron activation itself can improve different pathologies.33,36 Fifth, the long-term effects of closed-loop intervention and optogenetic stimulation will need to be determined, i.e., whether the beneficial effects of the closed-loop seizure suppression end immediately after the end of the treatment, or if there are any lasting effects on seizures or cognitive ability. Sixth, it should be noted that it is not yet entirely clear whether unilateral optogenetic intervention in this paradigm achieves curtailment of seizures primarily by exciting the somata, dendrites or axon terminals of those ipsilateral PV cells that survived the kainate administration (Figure S1), despite being greatly reduced in number37,38, and/or by activating the ipsilateral axon terminals of contralaterally located PV cells that have been recently shown to undergo a prominent sprouting response after IHKA treatment.39 Due to the difficulty of acquiring enough animals that reach the endpoint of our prolonged multi-step experimental paradigm (Figure 3A), these interesting and important questions will need to be pursued in carefully designed future studies.
Conclusions
We show here for the first time that closed-loop optogenetic intervention of seizures in a model of temporal lobe epilepsy is able to improve spatial memory. By taking advantage of the benefits of the spatial, temporal, and cell specificity of optogenetics, this type of intervention avoids nonspecific targeting of other neurons and other cells. Our findings demonstrate that the closed-loop optogenetic intervention improves the learning ability of epileptic animals and decreases seizure duration, providing strong support for further work in this area as a therapeutic for epilepsy and its comorbidities.
Supplementary Material
Key Points.
After the initial insult and the eventual development of spontaneous seizures in a rodent model of temporal lobe epilepsy, seizure frequency and duration in the absence of intervention remain relatively constant for several months
Similarly, spatial memory deficits are also stable over several months in epileptic mice without intervention
Closed-loop optogenetic seizure intervention in a model of temporal lobe epilepsy is able to improve spatial memory
Together, these findings indicate that even well into the chronic phase of epilepsy, closed-loop seizure intervention in individuals in which it is able to shorten seizure duration may also improve spatial memory
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Mikko Oijala, Rose Zhu, Cecilia Lozoya, and Judit Vargane for technical assistance. We also thank all members of the Soltesz lab for useful discussions. Funding was provided to H.K.K by NIH T32 NS45440, to T.G. by the Swiss League Against Epilepsy, and also by NIH R01 NS094668 to I.S.
Footnotes
Ethical Publication Statement
We confirm that we have read the Journal’s position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines.
Conflicts of Interest
We declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Elger CE, Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M. Chronic epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurol 2004;3:663–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Holmes GL. Cognitive impairment in epilepsy: the role of network abnormalities. Epileptic Disord 2015;17:101–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cánovas R, León I, Serrano P, Roldán MD, Cimadevilla JM. Spatial navigation impairment in patients with refractory temporal lobe epilepsy: Evidence from a new virtual reality-based task. Epilepsy Behav 2011;22:364–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Amlerova J, Laczo J, Vlcek K, Javurkova A, Andel R, Marusic P. Risk factors for spatial memory impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2013;26:57–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Perucca P, Gilliam FG. Adverse effects of antiepileptic drugs. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:792–802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bui A, Kim HK, Maroso M, Soltesz I. Microcircuits in Epilepsy: Heterogeneity and Hub Cells in Network Synchronization. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med; 2015;5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kim HK, Alexander AL, Soltesz I. Optogenetics: Lighting a path from the laboratory to the clinic In: Neuromethods. Humana Press, New York, NY; 2018. p. 277–300. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Krook-Magnuson E, Soltesz I. Beyond the hammer and the scalpel: selective circuit control for the epilepsies. Nat Neurosci 2015;18:331–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cavalheiro EA, Riche DA, Le Gal La Salle G. Long-term effects of intrahippocampal kainic acid injection in rats: a method for inducing spontaneous recurrent seizures. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1982;53:581–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gröticke I, Hoffmann K, Löscher W. Behavioral alterations in a mouse model of temporal lobe epilepsy induced by intrahippocampal injection of kainate. Exp Neurol 2008;213:71–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pearson JN, Schulz KM, Patel M. Specific alterations in the performance of learning and memory tasks in models of chemoconvulsant-induced status epilepticus. Epilepsy Res 2014;108:1032–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gradinaru V, Thompson KR, Zhang F, Mogri M, Kay K, Schneider MB, et al. Targeting and Readout Strategies for Fast Optical Neural Control In Vitro and In Vivo. J Neurosci 2007;27:14231–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gradinaru V, Zhang F, Ramakrishnan C, Mattis J, Prakash R, Diester I, et al. Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending optogenetics. Cell 2010;141:154–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Boyden ES, Zhang F, Bamberg E, Nagel G, Deisseroth K. Millisecond-timescale, genetically targeted optical control of neural activity. Nat Neurosci 2005;8:1263–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Paz JT, Davidson TJ, Frechette ES, Delord B, Parada I, Peng K, et al. Closed-loop optogenetic control of thalamus as a tool for interrupting seizures after cortical injury. Nat Neurosci 2012;16:64–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Krook-Magnuson E, Armstrong C, Oijala M, Soltesz I. On-demand optogenetic control of spontaneous seizures in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat Commun 2013;4:1376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wykes RC, Heeroma JH, Mantoan L, Zheng K, Macdonald DC, Deisseroth K, et al. Optogenetic and Potassium Channel Gene Therapy in a Rodent Model of Focal Neocortical Epilepsy. Sci Transl Med 2012;4:161ra152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Madisen L, Mao T, Koch H, Zhuo J, Berenyi A, Fujisawa S, et al. A toolbox of Cre-dependent optogenetic transgenic mice for light-induced activation and silencing. Nat Neurosci 2012;15:793–802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Armstrong C, Krook-Magnuson E, Oijala M, Soltesz I. Closed-loop optogenetic intervention in mice. Nat Protoc 2013;8:1475–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vogel-Ciernia A, Wood MA. Examining object location and object recognition memory in mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci 2014;69:1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Van Den Herrewegen Y, Denewet L, Buckinx A, Albertini G, Van Eeckhaut A, Smolders I, et al. The Barnes Maze Task Reveals Specific Impairment of Spatial Learning Strategy in the Intrahippocampal Kainic Acid Model for Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Neurochem Res 2019;44:600–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rattka M, Brandt C, Löscher W. The intrahippocampal kainate model of temporal lobe epilepsy revisited: epileptogenesis, behavioral and cognitive alterations, pharmacological response, and hippoccampal damage in epileptic rats. Epilepsy Res 2013;103:135–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Riban V, Bouilleret V, Pham-Lê BT, Fritschy J-M, Marescaux C, Depaulis A. Evolution of hippocampal epileptic activity during the development of hippocampal sclerosis in a mouse model of temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroscience 2002;112:101–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Arabadzisz D, Antal K, Parpan F, Emri Z, Fritschy J-M. Epileptogenesis and chronic seizures in a mouse model of temporal lobe epilepsy are associated with distinct EEG patterns and selective neurochemical alterations in the contralateral hippocampus. Exp Neurol 2005;194:76–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Williams PA, White AM, Clark S, Ferraro DJ, Swiercz W, Staley KJ, et al. Development of spontaneous recurrent seizures after kainate-induced status epilepticus. J Neurosci 2009;29:2103–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Haussler U, Bielefeld L, Froriep UP, Wolfart J, Haas CA. Septotemporal Position in the Hippocampal Formation Determines Epileptic and Neurogenic Activity in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Cereb Cortex 2012;22:26–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Janz P, Schwaderlapp N, Heining K, Häussler U, Korvink JG, von Elverfeldt D, et al. Early tissue damage and microstructural reorganization predict disease severity in experimental epilepsy. Elife 2017;6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Vogel-Ciernia A, Matheos DP, Barrett RM, Kramár EA, Azzawi S, Chen Y, et al. The neuron-specific chromatin regulatory subunit BAF53b is necessary for synaptic plasticity and memory. Nat Neurosci 2013;16:552–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bui AD, Nguyen TM, Limouse C, Kim HK, Szabo GG, Felong S, et al. Dentate gyrus mossy cells control spontaneous convulsive seizures and spatial memory. Science 2018;359:787–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zattoni M, Mura ML, Deprez F, Schwendener RA, Engelhardt B, Frei K, et al. Brain infiltration of leukocytes contributes to the pathophysiology of temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosci 2011;31:4037–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gschwind T, Lafourcade C, Gfeller T, Zaichuk M, Rambousek L, Knuesel I, et al. Contribution of early Alzheimer’s disease-related pathophysiology to the development of acquired epilepsy. Eur J Neurosci 2018;47:1534–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bouilleret V, Ridoux V, Depaulis A, Marescaux C, Nehlig A, Le Gal La Salle G. Recurrent seizures and hippocampal sclerosis following intrahippocampal kainate injection in adult mice: electroencephalography, histopathology and synaptic reorganization similar to mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroscience 1999;89:717–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ognjanovski N, Broussard C, Zochowski M, Aton SJ. Hippocampal Network Oscillations Rescue Memory Consolidation Deficits Caused by Sleep Loss. Cereb Cortex 2018;28:3711–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ewell LA, Liang L, Armstrong C, Soltész I, Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK. Brain State Is a Major Factor in Preseizure Hippocampal Network Activity and Influences Success of Seizure Intervention. J Neurosci 2015;35:15635–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Iaccarino HF, Singer AC, Martorell AJ, Rudenko A, Gao F, Gillingham TZ, et al. Gamma frequency entrainment attenuates amyloid load and modifies microglia. Nature 2016;540:230–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wang Y, Liang J, Chen L, Shen Y, Zhao J, Xu C, et al. Pharmaco-genetic therapeutics targeting parvalbumin neurons attenuate temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurobiol Dis 2018;117:149–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bouilleret V, Loup F, Kiener T, Marescaux C, Fritschy J-M. Early loss of interneurons and delayed subunit-specific changes in GABAA-receptor expression in a mouse model of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Hippocampus 2000;10:305–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marx M, Haas CA, Häussler U. Differential vulnerability of interneurons in the epileptic hippocampus. Front Cell Neurosci 2013;7:167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Christenson Wick Z, Leintz CH, Xamonthiene C, Huang BH, Krook-Magnuson E. Axonal sprouting in commissurally projecting parvalbumin-expressing interneurons. J Neurosci Res 2017;95:2336–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.