Skip to main content
NeuroImage : Clinical logoLink to NeuroImage : Clinical
. 2020 Nov 11;28:102494. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102494

Altered functional connectivity of cortical networks in semantic variant Primary Progressive Aphasia

Haroon Popal a, Megan Quimby b, Daisy Hochberg b, Bradford C Dickerson b,, Jessica A Collins b,1
PMCID: PMC7708956  PMID: 33395985

Highlights

  • Default mode network connectivity is disrupted in svPPA.

  • Dorsal attention to visual association network connectivity is increased in svPPA.

  • Focal neurodegeneration can alter large-scale cognitive networks.

Abbreviations: svPPA, semantic variant Primary Progressive Aphasia; OC, older controls; YC, young controls; DAN, dorsal attention network; VisAN, visual association network, DMN, default mode network

Keywords: Functional connectivity, Primary Progressive Aphasia, Semantic dementia, Frontotemporal dementia, Attention

Abstract

As their illness progresses, patients with the semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia (svPPA) frequently exhibit peculiar behaviors indicative of altered visual attention or an increased interest in artistic endeavors. In the present study, we examined changes within and between large-scale functional brain networks that may explain this altered visual behavior. We first examined the connectivity of the visual association network, the dorsal attention network, and the default mode network in healthy young adults (n = 89) to understand the typical architecture of these networks in the healthy brain. We then compared the large-scale functional connectivity of these networks in a group of svPPA patients (n = 12) to a group of age-matched cognitively normal controls (n = 30). Our results showed that the between-network connectivity of the dorsal attention and visual association networks was elevated in svPPA patients relative to controls. We further showed that this heightened between-network connectivity was associated with a decrease in the within-network connectivity of the default mode network, possibly due to progressive degeneration of the anterior temporal lobes in svPPA. These results suggest that focal neurodegeneration can lead to the reorganization of large-scale cognitive networks beyond the primarily affected network(s), possibly contributing to cognitive or behavioral changes that are commonly present as part of the clinical phenotype of svPPA.

1. Introduction

The core symptoms of the semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia (svPPA) include semantic memory impairment that manifests as anomia and impaired single word comprehension (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011, Hodges et al., 1992, Hodges and Patterson, 2007, Snowden et al., 1989, Warrington, 1975). The prominent asymmetrical atrophy and hypometabolism of the anterior temporal lobes in svPPA is usually associated with Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration (FTLD) TDP43 neurodegenerative pathology (Josephs et al., 2011, Mesulam et al., 2014)). From a relatively early stage as the disease progresses, atrophy can also be seen in brain regions that are functionally connected, based of resting-state correlations, with the temporal poles, including—but not limited to—the frontoinsula, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, inferior parietal lobule, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and the posterior lateral and ventral temporal cortex (Brambati et al., 2009, Collins et al., 2017, Rohrer et al., 2009, Seeley et al., 2009). These regions are also structurally connected via white matter pathways (Agosta et al., 2013, Agosta et al., 2010), and previous evidence has suggested that disease proteins may travel along white matter pathways in a prion-like manner via transsynaptic spread (Brown et al., 2019, Collins et al., 2017, Frost et al., 2009, Palop and Mucke, 2010, Sanders et al., 2014, Seeley et al., 2009).

The nomenclature used to describe the network of brain regions primarily affected by svPPA varies, with some authors highlighting overlap with anterior components of the default mode network (Bejanin et al., 2019, Collins et al., 2017, La Joie et al., 2014, Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012), or the anterior temporal/paralimbic network (Seeley et al., 2009). Although the localization of functions that support semantic memory is a matter of debate, both the default mode network (Binder et al., 2009, Jackson et al., 2016, Shapira-Lichter et al., 2013, Wirth et al., 2011) and the anterior temporal/paralimbic network (Damasio et al., 2004, Humphreys et al., 2015, Patterson et al., 2007, Visser et al., 2010) appear critically important. The temporal poles, the primary site of atrophy in svPPA (Collins et al., 2017), act as convergence zones in the brain (Tranel et al., 1998), with connectivity extending to a number of networks including the language network, visual association network, default mode network, and an anterior temporal/paralimbic network (Pascual et al., 2015, Guo et al., 2013). Thus, the neurodegenerative lesion in svPPA may compromise activity in not only the temporal poles, but potentially key nodes of the default mode, language, and visual association networks.

In addition to their well-studied language impairments, svPPA patients also frequently exhibit frank changes in the way they allocate their visual attention (Green and Patterson, 2009, Miller et al., 2000, Viskontas et al., 2011). In many patients, this alteration in visual attention takes the form of intensified focus on specific features of visual stimuli in their environment (Miller et al., 2000). For instance, the husband of an svPPA patient seen in our clinic reported that his wife devotes hours each day to compulsively watching and crushing gnats on her windowsill, possibly being driven by the visual contrast of the black insects against a white backdrop. In other patients, this change in visual attention can lead to an increased interest in visual expression; multiple anecdotes from our and others’ observations describe patients taking up an artistic hobby or dedicating more time to previous artistic pursuits (Miller et al., 2000) [https://youtu.be/3r621iKhATc]. This has been detailed in a case report of an svPPA patient (patient AA) who exhibited an interest in painting before any language symptoms or atrophy in the left anterior temporal lobe was apparent (Seeley et al., 2008). The patient had not painted for over 30 years before suddenly picking up the hobby again, which soon became an obsession.

Some of these behaviors may arise from alterations in two brain networks that normally work in concert to guide visual attention and perception in the natural environment. The visual association network, which extends along the ventral temporal lobe from early visual cortex to the temporal pole, facilitates higher-order object processing (Ungerleider and Haxby, 1994). The anterior temporal node of the visual association network, located within perirhinal cortex, is atrophied early in svPPA (Galton et al., 2001), possibly contributing to widespread dysfunction throughout the visual association network, (Agosta et al., 2014, Guo et al., 2013). Notably, the perirhinal cortex has also been implicated in semantic cognition, particularly for concrete objects (Mion et al., 2010, Ralph et al., 2016), and has been shown to support the integration of complex visual representations with item or person-specific semantic memory (Collins and Olson, 2014, Wang et al., 2017). This region is functionally connected to the anterior temporal region of the default mode network (Pascual et al., 2015, Ritchey et al., 2015), where visual information is integrated with information of other modalities to support semantic memory (Ralph et al., 2016). In addition to their semantic impairment, in svPPA disconnection within the visual association network may contribute to the disproportionate impairments patients experience with naming visual objects (Guo et al., 2013) and visual attribute reporting (Hoffman et al., 2012, Tyler and Moss, 1998), relative to other modalities. Recent eye tracking work has shown that patients with anterior temporal lobe damage due to svPPA or Alzheimer’s disease show aberrant gaze patterns, possibly reflecting a breakdown of top-down contributions to object recognition (Ungrady et al., 2019). The dorsal attention network, consisting of the superior parietal lobule/intraparietal sulcus, frontal eye fields, and area MT, subserves the allocation of visual attention in exteroceptive space (Corbetta et al., 2008), and is largely spared from atrophy in svPPA. Notably, preserved grey matter density in the superior parietal lobule has been associated with faster visual search performance in svPPA patients relative to controls (Viskontas et al., 2011). In addition, svPPA patient AA had increased volume and blood flow in the right superior parietal lobule compared to a group of healthy controls, which the authors suggest may have contributed to her increased interest in visual endeavors (Seeley et al., 2008).

A defining feature of the dorsal attention network is that it activates during effortful tasks requiring attention to the external environment (Sestieri et al., 2010). In many tasks, this activation coincides with the deactivation of the default mode network (Binder et al., 1999, Corbetta and Shulman, 2002, McKiernan et al., 2003, McKiernan et al., 2006, Wirth et al., 2011). At rest, blood oxygen level dependent signal activity within these two networks appears to have inversely correlated or “anti-correlated” oscillation profiles (Fox et al., 2005, Fransson, 2005, Spreng et al., 2016). An exception to this rule is during the perception and interpretation of conceptually meaningful stimuli, during which the default mode network becomes engaged (Binder et al., 2009), and may facilitate the linking of conceptual knowledge to object representations via short range connectivity between the anterior temporal nodes of the default mode and visual association networks (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012). In contrast, the dorsal attention network is activated during the spatial orienting response to distinctive cues, regardless of their conceptual meaning or behavioral relevance (Corbetta et al., 2008). One possibility is that the visual association network provides a sculpting influence on the reciprocal balance between the dorsal attention and default mode networks, supporting the guidance of visual attention to conceptually meaningful objects in the environment. Supporting this hypothesis, previous work has demonstrated structural connections between nodes of dorsal attention and visual association networks (tract tracing work in non-human primates: Felleman and Van Essen, 1991, Ungerleider et al., 2008, Wernicke, 1881; diffusion weighted imaging in humans: Takemura et al., 2016, Yeatman et al., 2014). As a corollary, we hypothesize that when connectivity between the visual association and default mode networks is compromised by a neurodegenerative lesion to the anterior temporal lobes, the dorsal attention network may be “released” from its reciprocal balance with the default mode network leading to increased connectivity with the visual association network. If this reorganization of visual-attentional networks due to neurodegeneration (RVANN) hypothesis is true, such altered brain dynamics could lead to attentional capture by perceptually distinctive features of the visual environment, regardless of their conceptual meaning.

The goal of this study was to test this RVANN hypothesis using intrinsic connectivity measured with functional MRI (fMRI). We predicted the following: 1) In healthy young adults weak between-network connectivity would be observed a) between the perirhinal node of the visual association network and the anterior temporal node of the default mode network, and b) between the posterior nodes of the visual association network and nodes of the dorsal attention network; 2) In svPPA patients, nodes of the dorsal attention network would have increased connectivity to nodes of the visual association network, compared to older controls, and 3) the increased connectivity between dorsal attention and visual association network nodes in svPPA would be associated with a functional fractionation of the anterior temporal node of the default mode network from the rest of the default mode network. This hypothesis is depicted visually in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Hypothesized RVANN functional network change in svPPA patients. A) Dorsal attention network regions of interest are shown in green, visual association network regions of interest are shown in purple, and default mode network regions of interest are shown in orange. The V1c region of interest is shown in white. Bidirectional arrows represent connectivity between regions of interest within the dorsal attention network (green arrows; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), visual association network (purple arrows; Moeller et al., 2008, Ungerleider and Haxby, 1994), and the default mode network (orange arrows; Binder et al., 2009). In healthy adults, the dorsal attention network (green arrows) and the visual association network (purple arrows) receive input from the early visual cortex (white arrows). These networks and the default mode network (orange arrows) have strong within-network connectivity. There is normally weak connectivity between the visual association and the dorsal attention networks and between the visual association and default mode networks (shown as dashed red arrows). B) In svPPA patients, we hypothesize that the default mode network will be fractionated, due to atrophy in the lateral anterior temporal lobe. We further hypothesize that the fractionation of the default mode network will disrupt its reciprocal balance with the dorsal attention network, resulting in increased connectivity between the dorsal attention and visual association networks in svPPA (shown as solid red arrows). FEF = frontal eye fields; SPL = superior parietal lobule; MT = motion-selective cortex; FFA = fusiform face area; OFA = occipital face area; PRC = perirhinal cortex; PCC = posterior cingulate cortex; MTG = middle temporal gyrus; mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex; AG = angular gyrus; V1c = central V1. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

2. Materials and methods

Three samples of participants were included in this investigation. A group of healthy younger adult controls (YC) served to test our first hypothesis regarding intrinsic connectivity within and between the dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode networks. A group of patients with a diagnosis of svPPA and a group of healthy older controls (OC), age-matched to the svPPA sample, were also included. A whole-brain group general linear model (GLM) comparing cortical thickness in the svPPA patient group to the OC group was conducted first to establish the pattern of atrophy in our patient sample. To test our second and third hypotheses, the within-network and between-network connectivity of the dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode networks were compared between the svPPA and OC groups.

2.1. Participants

The YC group included 89 young adults (mean age = 22.4, SD = 3.34) between 18 and 33 years old, (45 female). The OC group included 30 older adults (mean age = 67.83, SD = 6.04) between 60 and 81 years old (16 females). All controls in the YC and OC groups were right-handed, native English speakers with normal vision, and had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders.

The patient sample included 12 svPPA patients (mean age = 64.13, SD = 7.86) between 53 and 78 years old (5 females). Patients were evaluated using a structured clinical assessment performed by a behavioral neurologist and speech pathologist, as previously described (Sapolsky et al., 2010). All svPPA patients met (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011) consensus diagnostic criteria for imaging-supported svPPA based on visual inspection of a clinical MRI showing predominant left anterior temporal lobar atrophy. Ratings on the Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale (PASS) from an evaluation of each patient in the svPPA sample (Daisy Sapolsky et al., 2014) by a licensed speech language pathologist within 6 months of the patient’s MRI, along with age, sex, and years of education, is presented in Table 1. All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the institutional review board of the Massachusetts General Hospital and Partners Healthcare System Human Research Committee.

Table 1.

Demographic information, CDR, MMSE, and PASS scores for individual svPPA case. Ed = years of education, CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating Scale, MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination, Art = articulation, Flu = fluency, S&G = syntax and grammar, WR = word retrieval and expression, Rep = repetition, AC = auditory comprehension, SWC = single word comprehension, Rd = reading, Wt = writing, Func = functional communication. For each PASS domain: 0 = Normal, 0.5 = questionably impaired, 1 = mild impairment, 2 = moderate impairment, 3 = severe impairment.

Age Sex Ed CDR MMSE Art Flu S&G WR Rep AC SWC Rd Wt Func
79 M 18 0.5 19 0 0 0 1 0 0 0.5 1 1 0.5
62 M 18 0 28 0 0 0 0.5 0 0 0.5 0 0 0
53 F 18 0 23 0 0.5 0 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5
70 F 12 0.5 0 0.5 0 2 0.5 1 2 2 2 1
71 M 12 0.5 19 0 0.5 0.5 1 0.5 1 1 1 1 0.5
53 M 20 0.5 20 0 0 0.5 1 0.5 1 1 2 0.5 1
55 F 16 0.5 24 0 0 0.5 1 0.5 0 0.5 1 0.5 0.5
68 M 18 0 0 0 0.5 0 0 0.5 0 0.5 0
68 M 17 0.5 0 0 0 0.5 0 0.5 0.5 0 0.5 0.5
65 F 16 0.5 7 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 3 1 1
67 M 19 2 26 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 0.5 1 1
59 F 12 1 96 0 0.5 0.5 2 0.5 1 1 1 1 1

2.2. Scan acquisition

All participants were scanned on a Siemens Magnetom Trio 3 T scanner with a 12-channel phased-array head coil. The svPPA and OC samples were scanned using a T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2530 ms, TE = 3.48 ms, flip angle = 7 degrees, number of interleaved sagittal slices = 176, matrix = 256x256, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size = 1 mm isotropic. The YC sample was scanned using a T1-weighted MEMPRAGE sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2200 ms, TE = 1.54 ms, flip angle = 7 degrees, number of interleaved sagittal slices = 144, matrix = 192x192, FOV = 230, voxel size = 1.2 mm isotropic.

Whole brain fMRI data were acquired for the OC and svPPA samples using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo-planar sequence with the following parameters: TR = 5000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90 degrees, number of interleaved coronal slices = 55, matrix = 128x112, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size = 2 mm isotropic. Seventy-six TRs of fMRI data were acquired for a total scan duration of 6 min and 20 s.

Whole brain fMRI data was acquired for the YC sample using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo-planar sequence with the following parameters: TR = 3000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 85 degrees, number of interleaved coronal slices = 47, matrix = 72x72, FOV = 216 mm, voxel size = 3 mm isotropic. 124 TRs of fMRI data were acquired for a total scan duration of 6 min and 12 s.

2.3. fMRI preprocessing

All intrinsic connectivity fMRI scans were preprocessed with the following parameters: removal of the first four functional volumes to allow for T1-equilibrium effects, slice timing correction (SPM2), head motion correction using rigid-body transformation in three translations and three rotations (FMRIB), spatial normalization to standard MNI-152 atlas space, resampling to 2 mm isotropic voxels, spatial smoothing with a 6 mm full width half max (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, and low-pass temporal filtering to remove frequencies > 0.08 Hz. Using linear regression, we removed sources of spurious variance and their temporal derivatives, including the six parameters from head-motion correction, the signal average over the whole brain, the average signal from a region located deep in white mater, and the average signal from ventricular cerebral spinal fluid. This preprocessing pipeline has been utilized in our lab and has shown to yield consistent results across analyses (Collins et al., 2017, Touroutoglou et al., 2015, Zhang et al., 2019, Andreano et al., 2017, Zhang et al., 2020). All subjects had 5 or less TRs of fMRI data with 0.5 mm or more movement in any of the x, y, or z directions. A two-tailed t-test ensured that the svPPA and OC groups did not significantly differ in terms of motion, as defined by average relative displacement in the x, y, and z directions, during the fMRI scans [t(40) = 0.24, p = .81, d = 0.08].

2.4. Estimation of intrinsic connectivity

5 mm-radius spherical seeds and regions of interest (ROIs) were created for the dorsal attention network and the visual association network centered on MNI coordinates that have previously been shown to adequately represent their respective networks (Table 2). Seed regions for the dorsal attention network were created in the left and right superior parietal lobules (lSPL/rSPL). Other dorsal attention network ROIs included the left and right frontal eye fields (lFEF/rFEF), and left and right motion-selective cortex (lMT/rMT). Seeds for the visual association network were created in the left and right fusiform face area (lFFA/rFFA), and other visual association network ROIs included the left and right occipital face area (lOFA/rOFA) and left and right perirhinal cortex (lPRC/rPRC). Seeds for the default mode network were created in the left and right posterior cingulate cortex (lPCC/rPCC). Additional default mode network ROIs were created in the left and right medial prefrontal cortex (lmPFC/rmPFC), left and right angular gyrus (lAG/rAG), and left and right middle temporal gyrus (lMTG/rMTG, on its dorsal bank within the superior temporal sulcus) by creating 5 mm spheres around the voxel showing the highest PCC connectivity in the YC group within Harvard/Oxford masks of the frontal medial cortex, angular gyrus, or the anterior and poster middle temporal gyrus, respectively. Although this criteria to create the default mode network ROIs resulted in ROIs that may be slightly different between hemispheres, such as lMTG/rMTG, visual inspection showed that the ROIs are still within their respective regions as defined by the Harvard/Oxford atlas. In addition, central V1 ROIs (lV1c/rV1c) were created in each hemisphere using coordinates from a previous publication (see Table 2). Correlation maps for each seed region were created by computing the Pearson’s product moment correlation ‘r’ between every voxel in the brain and the average mean signal time course of the voxels within this sphere. Positive values in this map were then converted to z-scores using Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. Maps were projected from MNI-152 space to the pial surface of the FreeSurfer average brain (fsaverage) for each ROIs’ respective hemisphere, and spatially smoothed with a 10 mm FWHM kernel. This second smoothing was done for visualization purposes only and was done after the relevant statistical analyses were completed. For each group of subjects, maps were then thresholded to best match results from previous findings, where these ROIs were shown to be part of their discrete network (Amunts et al., 2000, Chen et al., 2017, Fischl et al., 2008, Fransson, 2005, Vincent et al., 2008; X. Wang et al., 2016). The focus of these analyses was on within-hemisphere connectivity between the ROIs, and as such, results will be reported respective to each seed’s or ROI’s hemisphere.

Table 2.

Regions of interest for the dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode networks.

ROI x y z Reference
Dorsal Attention Network lSPL −27 −52 57 Vincent et al. (2008)
rSPL 24 −56 55 Vincent et al. (2008)
lFEF −25 −8 50 Vincent et al. (2008)
rFEF 27 −8 50 Vincent et al. (2008)
lMT −45 −69 −2 Vincent et al. (2008)
rMT 50 −69 −3 Vincent et al. (2008)
Default Mode Network lPCC −2 −55 34 Chen et al., 2017, Fransson, 2005
rPCC 2 −55 34 Chen et al., 2017, Fransson, 2005
lMTG −62 −16 −18
rMTG 60 −6 −24
lmPFC −2 52 −8
rmPFC 2 52 −8
lAG −44 −68 38
rAG 48 −60 30
Visual Association Network lFFA −40 −52 –22 Wang et al. (2016)
rFFA 42 −50 –22 Wang et al. (2016)
lOFA −42 −80 −14 Wang et al. (2016)
rOFA 42 −82 −14 Wang et al. (2016)
lPRC −36 −2 −40 Wang et al. (2016)
rPRC 36 −2 −40 Wang et al. (2016)
lV1c −13 −100 −8 Amunts et al., 2000, Fischl et al., 2008
rV1c 13 −100 −8 Amunts et al., 2000, Fischl et al., 2008

*Bold text indicates the brain region that served as the network seed. lSPL/rSPL = left/right superior parietal lobule; lFEF/rFEF = left/right frontal eye fields; lMT/rMT = left/right motion-selective cortex; lPCC/rPCC = left/right posterior cingulate cortex; lMTG/rMTG = left/right middle temporal gyrus; lmPFC/rmPFC = left/right medial prefrontal cortex; lAG/rAG = left/right angular gyrus; lFFA/rFFA = left/right fusiform face area; lOFA/rOFA = left/right occipital face area; lPRC/rPRC = left/right perirhinal cortex; lV1c/rV1c = left/right central V1.

In addition to the whole-brain connectivity maps, node-to-node connectivity was analyzed within each group using one-sample t-tests to assess whether between-node connectivity differed significantly from zero. The t-tests and related statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB 2018b. Two-sample t-tests were used to test hypotheses about the differences in node-to-node connectivity between the svPPA and OC groups. The z-score normalized correlations of the BOLD signal were used in all node-to-node analyses. Tests which were done to directly test our hypotheses were one-tailed t-tests, with alpha-level of p = .05. For post-hoc tests, two-tailed t-tests were used and to correct for multiple comparisons, a Bonferroni correction was used to determine the significance of both one-sample and two-sample t-tests, with an alpha-level of p = .05/55 = 0.00091. Tests which were not related to a priori hypotheses will be indicated as using a Bonferroni correction, with the corrected alpha-level.

2.5. Cortical thickness analysis

The FreeSurfer analysis suite (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) was used to reconstruct cortical surfaces from the collected T1-weighted images (Dale et al., 1999, Fischl et al., 1999, Fischl and Dale, 2000, Rosas et al., 2002, Salat et al., 2004). Blind to any group affiliation, the cortical reconstruction for each individual was manually inspected to check for accuracy and to correct any errors in the automatic segmentation of grey/white matter boundaries. For each subject, the pial surface was identified using the grey/white matter boundary by a deformable surface algorithm (Fischl and Dale, 2000). Each subject’s reconstructed brain was then morphed and registered to an average spherical space (fsaverage), that optimally aligned gyral and sulcal features. The cortical thickness was calculated across the whole cortex in this new space, and a Gaussian kernel of 10 mm full-width at half-maximum was used to smooth each subject’s cortical thickness map before any further analyses. A GLM was used via FreeSurfer, to compare the cortical thickness maps between the OC and svPPA groups.

2.6. Data availability

Imaging data are available upon request.

3. Results

3.1. Explicating dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode network connectivity in young controls

We first created whole-brain connectivity maps in the YC group bilaterally on the fsaverage surface using two canonical seeds in the dorsal attention and visual association networks: the SPL and FFA. At a typical threshold (min z = 0.10), the connectivity of SPL was largely restricted to the dorsal attention network (Fig. 2A), and the connectivity of FFA was largely restricted to the visual association network (Fig. 2B). However, at a lower threshold (min z = 0.04), the connectivity of these two seeds extended to regions typically considered part of the other network. At the lower threshold the SPL was functionally connected to regions of the visual association network including the OFA and the FFA (Fig. 2C) and the FFA was functionally connected to regions of the dorsal attention network including the SPL and the rFEF (Fig. 2D). In keeping with previous work, we chose a liberal threshold (z > 0.04) for visualizing networks in the young healthy brain that has been frequently used in rs-fMRI studies because it provides results similar to what is seen with independent component analyses (Van Dijk et al., 2010).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Intrinsic connectivity of dorsal attention and visual association networks in young healthy controls. A) At a typical threshold, connectivity of the SPL was mostly selective to regions within the dorsal attention network, including area MT and the FEF. At this high threshold, some with some weak between-network connectivity was observed between the SPL and OFA. The SPL also exhibited an anticorrelated pattern of connectivity with nodes of the default mode network, including AG and MTG. B) The connectivity of FFA, at a typical threshold, was mostly selective to regions within the visual association network, which included the OFA. At this high threshold, between-network connectivity was also observed between FFA and area MT. C) At a low threshold, the connectivity of the SPL extended to regions of the visual association network, including the OFA and the FFA. D) Similarly, the connectivity of the FFA extended to regions of the dorsal attention network, including bilateral SPL, and the rFEF when visualized at a lower threshold. At this lower threshold, FFA also exhibited connectivity to the anterior portions of the medial temporal lobes, including another region of the VisAN, the PRC. The FFA also exhibited an anticorrelated pattern of activity with nodes of the DMN at this lower threshold, similar to the SPL. E) Circles represent nodes for each seed or ROI, and the lines connecting the nodes represent connectivity, with thicker lines representing stronger connectivity. The dorsal attention and visual association networks showed clear within and between-network connectivity. In contrast, the default mode network was not only isolated from the other two networks, but also anti-correlated to the SPL and FFA (A-D) Weak between-network connectivity was observed between the PRC node of the visual association network and the MTG of the default mode network. Scale shows the Fischer r-z transformed connectivity values. LH = left hemisphere; RH = right hemisphere; lFEF/rFEF = left/right frontal eye fields; lSPL/rSPL = left/right superior parietal lobule; lMT/rMT = left/right motion-selective cortex; lV1c/rV1c = left/right central V1; lFFA/rFFA = left/right fusiform face area; lOFA/rOFA = left/right occipital face area; lPRC/rPRC = left/right perirhinal cortex; lPCC/rPCC = left/right posterior cingulate cortex; lmPFC/rmPFC = left/right medial prefrontal cortex; lMTG/rMTG = left/right middle temporal gyrus; lAG/rAG = left/right angular gyrus.

At a typical threshold of z = 0.10, the rSPL was anticorrelated with nodes of the default mode network, including the rAG and the rMTG (Fig. 2A). This replicates previous findings that demonstrate a reciprocal balance in the activity of the dorsal attention and default mode networks (Fox et al., 2005).

To further explore the within- and between-network connectivity of the dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode networks, an adjacency matrix was created by calculating the Pearson correlation between the BOLD signal time series of every node with every other node in all three networks, separately for each hemisphere. The r-values were then converted to z-scores by Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. Using the z-score adjacency matrix, a graph was created in MATLAB, using the biograph function, to visualize the connectivity between the nodes of the three networks. These graphs were thresholded so that only edges with a z-score >0.04 are shown, in order to match the low threshold whole brain connectivity maps shown in Fig. 2E.

Replicating previous work, one-sample t-tests revealed within-network connectivity for all nodes of the dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode network (all ps < 0.05). Supporting our first hypothesis, between-network connectivity was observed bilaterally between area MT and the SPL within the dorsal attention network, and the FFA and OFA in the visual association network (all ps < 0.001; see Supplementary Table 1). Further supporting our first hypothesis, between-network connectivity was also observed bilaterally between the MTG node of the default mode network and the PRC node of the visual association network (lMTG-lPRC: t(88) = 5.10, p < .001; MTG-rPRC: t(88) = 2.76, p = .007; see Supplementary Table 1). Thus, while the FFA of the visual association network demonstrated weakly anticorrelated activity with nodes of the default mode network (Fig. 2D), the rostral temporal lobe nodes of these networks had positively correlated activity patterns, potentially representing a mechanism for the temporal pole’s function as a convergence zone. The Fischer r-z transformed connectivity values for all ROI pairs in both hemispheres is presented in Supplementary Fig. 1.

3.2. Anterior temporal lobe atrophy in svPPA

A whole-brain group GLM comparing the svPPA patient group to the OC group revealed that the svPPA patients had thinner cortex in bilateral anterior temporal lobes, more prominently in the left hemisphere, as expected (Fig. 3). Atrophy in the svPPA group extended from the left temporal pole laterally to the posterior middle temporal gyrus, ventrally to the fusiform gyrus, and medially through the perirhinal cortex extending caudally to the posterior parahippocampal gyrus, as well as to the frontoinsula and orbitofrontal cortex. Less prominent atrophy was present in the right anterior temporal lobe. In both hemispheres, cortical thinning in the svPPA group included the PRC which, as discussed, is typically connected with the visual association network. Additionally, cortical atrophy in svPPA included the MTG node of the default mode network in the left and right hemispheres. That is, the two critical nodes linking the default mode network and visual association network, the PRC and the MTG, are effectively lesioned in svPPA.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Cortical atrophy in svPPA. A whole brain GLM comparing cortical thickness in the svPPA group to the OC group. Areas in blue show indicate cortical thickness that was reduced (atrophic) in the svPPA group compared to OCs, thresholded at p < .05. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

3.3. Dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode network connectivity in svPPA and OCs

In both the svPPA and OC groups, the SPL seed showed strong connectivity with other nodes of the dorsal attention network, consistent with what was observed in the YC group (Fig. 4A–B). In the svPPA patients only, functional connectivity of the rSPL extended beyond the dorsal attention network to include ventral occipitotemporal regions rOFA and rFFA of the visual association network (Fig. 4B). In the left hemisphere, functional connectivity of the lSPL did not extend to regions of the visual association network. The OC group did not demonstrate between-network connectivity of the rSPL and visual association network nodes (Fig. 4A). With the FFA as a seed, the OC group demonstrated similar visual association network connectivity to the YC group (Fig. 4C). However, in the svPPA group, connectivity of the rFFA extended to a number of regions of the dorsal attention network, including the rSPL and rFEF (Fig. 4D). Again, these results were specific to the right hemisphere of svPPA patients, and not the left. A matrix depicting the node-to-node correlations between all right and left hemisphere ROIs for the OC group and the svPPA group are provided in Supplementary Figs. 2 and 3.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

SPL and FFA intrinsic connectivity in svPPA patients and OCs. A) Whole-brain connectivity of the SPL in the OC group was selective to the dorsal attention network as it is in young adults, B) while connectivity of SPL in the svPPA group extended to portions of the visual association network in the right hemisphere. These maps were thresholded at z = 0.10, because in YCs this threshold revealed selective functional connectivity between the SPL and other regions of the dorsal attention network. C) Whole-brain connectivity of the FFA in the OC group remained selective to the visual association network, D) while connectivity of the FFA in the svPPA group extended to portions of the dorsal attention network in the right hemisphere.

To further explore the within- and between-network dynamics of the dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode networks, adjacency matrices were created for the svPPA patient group and for the OC group displaying the connectivity of each node to every other node in all three networks. This analysis focused on nodes of the right hemisphere, as the whole-brain analysis suggested that group differences between the network architecture of the dorsal attention and visual association networks are more robust in this hemisphere. The adjacency matrices for the two groups were used to create graphs (Fig. 5) using methods identical to those used in creating the YC graphs in Fig. 2E, with the exception of thresholding so that only edges which depict Bonferroni-corrected statistically significant correlations are shown.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Connectivity between nodes of the dorsal attention, default mode, and visual association networks in older controls and svPPA patients. A) Older controls showed clear within-network connectivity for each network, and little between-network connectivity. B) In svPPA the within-network connectivity of the default mode network was disrupted with the rMTG node becoming fractionated from the posterior nodes of the network and demonstrating increased connectivity with visual association and dorsal attention network nodes. Between-network connectivity of the dorsal attention and visual association networks was also more prominent in the svPPA group relative to the OC group. Scale shows the Fischer r-z transformed connectivity values. rFEF = right frontal eye fields; rSPL = right superior parietal lobule; rMT = right motion-selective cortex; rV1c = right central V1; rFFA = right fusiform face area; rOFA = right occipital face area; rPRC = right perirhinal cortex; rPCC = right posterior cingulate cortex; rmPFC = right medial prefrontal cortex; rMTG = right middle temporal gyrus; rAG = right angular gyrus.

The OC group demonstrated a pattern of connectivity that was similar to the YC group, with strong within-network connectivity being exhibited by all network nodes. An exception was the rPRC, which did not demonstrate any connectivity with the rest of the visual association network nodes in the OC group, but did show connectivity to the rMTG in the default mode network, t(29) = 3.96, Bonferroni corrected p < .00091; (Supplementary Table 2). In the OC group, between-network connectivity was also observed between the rMT, in the dorsal attention networks, and two nodes of the visual association network, the rOFA, t(29) = 9.04, Bonferroni corrected p < .00091, and the rFFA, t(29) = 4.89, Bonferroni corrected p < .00091 (Supplementary Table 2; Fig. 5A).

The svPPA patient group exhibited disrupted within-network connectivity in the default mode network. Specifically, the rMTG did not retain significant connectivity with rPCC, t(11) = 0.67, p = .5177, or rAG, t(11) = −1.44, p = .1784, but retained weak connectivity to the rmPFC, t(11) = 1.83, p = .0954 (Supplementary Table 2; Fig. 5B). In the svPPA group connectivity between the rmPFC, rPPC, and rAG was preserved. In the svPPA patient group, between-network connectivity was observed between the rMTG and nodes of the visual association and dorsal attention networks, including to the rPRC, t(11) = 3.45, p = .0054, rFFA, t(11) = 2.94, p = .0135, and rMT, t(11) = 3.27, p = .0074, however the results of these tests did not survive a Bonferroni correction (Supplementary Table 2).

The network graphs reinforce what is shown in Fig. 4: svPPA patients showed connectivity between dorsal attention and visual association network nodes that was not present in the OC group, as was hypothesized. Specifically, in the svPPA group the rSPL showed connectivity to the rFFA, t(11) = 2.76, p = .0186, and the rFEF showed weak connectivity to the rFFA, t(11) = 2.02, p = .0686 (Supplementary Table 2). The Fischer r-z transformed connectivity values for all ROI pairs in both hemispheres is presented in Supplementary Figs. 2 and 3.

3.4. Comparison of network connectivity in svPPA patients vs OCs

To directly compare the network architecture of the svPPA and OC groups, 2-sample t-tests were conducted comparing the within- and between-network connectivity of each network node in each group. Supporting our hypothesis, svPPA patients had greater connectivity between rSPL and rFFA [t(40) = 1.72, p = .0463, d = 0.59] and between rFEF and rFFA [t(40) = 2.56, p = .0142, d = 0.88] relative to the OC group. Connectivity of the rSPL to the other dorsal attention network ROIs was not significantly different in the svPPA versus OC groups. Additional one-tailed t-tests supported our hypotheses that the svPPA patients have weakened connectivity between the rMTG and other default mode network nodes relative to OCs. More specifically, we showed that connectivity between the rMTG and rAG was weaker in svPPA patients compared to OCs [t(40) = 2.99, p = .0048, d = 1.02]. Connectivity between the rMTG and PCC did not differ significantly between the svPPA and OC groups [t(40) = 1.72, p = .0933, d = 0.59].

A whole-brain GLM was used to assess the between-group differences in the whole brain connectivity of the rSPL and the rFFA seeds for the OC and svPPA groups. A gaussian random field multiple comparisons correction was applied to the resulting volume maps with a vertex-wise threshold of p < .01 and a cluster-forming threshold of p < .05 (Fig. 6). For the rSPL seed, svPPA patients displayed significantly stronger connectivity to MT, OFA, FFA, and other regions of the VisAN outside of our specific ROIs, relative to the OC group (cluster-wise p-value < 0.0001) (Fig. 6A). For the rFFA seed, svPPA patients demonstrated significantly stronger connectivity with the rFEF and other regions of the DAN outside of our specific ROIs, relative to the OC group (cluster-wise p-value < 0.0001) (Fig. 6B).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Whole-brain comparison of rFFA and rSPL connectivity in svPPA patients versus OCs. Cluster corrected whole-brain GLM analyses identified functional connectivity differences between the dorsal attention and the visual association networks in the svPPA patient versus OC groups. A) The rSPL showed greater functional connectivity with visual association network regions in the svPPA patient group compared to the OC group. B) The rFFA showed greater functional connectivity with dorsal attention network regions in the svPPA patient group compared to the OC group. Uncorrected significance maps from the original GLM are displayed for the right hemisphere, masked by significant clusters that survived the FWE correction. Arrows denote the seed region used in each analysis.

4. Discussion

The goal of the present study was to investigate our RVANN hypothesis, which proposes that, in svPPA, a neurodegenerative lesion to the anterior temporal lobes leads to a functional reorganization of the brain networks supporting visual attention. We first demonstrated novel relationships between three brain networks in young healthy adults: the dorsal attention network, the visual association network, and the default mode network. Supporting our hypotheses, weak between-network connectivity was observed between the visual association and dorsal attention networks in the healthy brain. The PRC, the rostral temporal lobe node weakly connected with the visual association network, is also weakly connected to the lateral anterior temporal node of the default mode network. Supporting our RVANN hypothesis, we showed that—in patients with svPPA—the normal weak connectivity between the visual association and dorsal attention networks is aberrantly heightened, and that this increased connectivity between the visual association and dorsal attention networks in svPPA occurs in the context of a fractionated default mode network. We discuss each of these findings in greater detail below.

4.1. Complex intrinsic relationships between dorsal attention, visual association, and default mode networks in the healthy brain

The visual association and dorsal attention networks—usually considered as distinct, separable networks—actually exhibit replicable weak connectivity with each other (Fig. 2). Specifically, the SPL, a key node of the dorsal attention network, shows weak connectivity to the FFA, a key node of the visual association network, indicating that there is between-network connectivity in the healthy brain. Previous work has implicated functional interactions between the visual association and dorsal attention networks in the utilization of object-related information to guide motor actions, such as the use of a specific grasp to pick up an object (Milner, 2017, van Polanen and Davare, 2015). This evidence of between-network functional connectivity between the dorsal attention and visual association networks is consistent with evidence for structural connectivity linking the two networks (tract tracing work in non-human primates: Felleman and Van Essen, 1991, Ungerleider et al., 2008, Wernicke, 1881; diffusion weighted imaging in humans: Takemura et al., 2016, Yeatman et al., 2014). Connectivity between the FFA and SPL may be mediated by the vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF), which has projections to the posterior intraparietal sulcus of the dorsal attention network and the fusiform gyrus of the visual association network (Budisavljevic et al., 2018, Takemura et al., 2016, Yeatman et al., 2014). Supporting the functional evidence of dorsal attention and visual association network communication, the VOF has been implicated in reading (Greenblatt, 1973, Greenblatt, 1976), depth perception (Oishi et al., 2018), and object related motor actions (Budisavljevic et al., 2018). Although the VOF connects regions of the visual association and dorsal attention networks, the white matter architecture that would support direct connectivity between the nodes of interest in this study (the SPL and FFA) is not understood.

The results presented here also replicated previous findings that the dorsal attention network demonstrates an anticorrelated activity pattern with the default mode network (Fig. 2). The default mode network is thought to support processes involved in semantic memory, episodic memory, prospective memory, self-referential thinking, and theory of mind (Buckner et al., 2008). When a task requiring the external orienting of attention is performed, the default mode network is deactivated and the dorsal attention network is activated (Buckner et al., 2008, Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). These task-induced activations of the dorsal attention network and deactivations of the default mode network, along with the anticorrelated intrinsic activity patterns of the dorsal attention and default mode networks, point to a reciprocal balance between these two networks in the healthy brain (Fox et al., 2005, Fransson, 2005). This reciprocal balance between the dorsal attention and default mode networks has been proposed to support the toggling of attention from internal cognitive processes to external stimuli and vice-versa (Vincent et al., 2008).

Although we also demonstrated that the posterior nodes of the visual association network are anticorrelated with the default mode network (Fig. 2), we showed that the anterior ventromedial temporal node of the visual association network, the PRC, is functionally connected to the anterior lateral temporal node of the default mode network, the anterior MTG (Fig. 2E). We reported this before (Pascual et al., 2015) but refined our understanding of this relationship in the context of the networks under consideration here. That is, the caudal visual association occipitotemporal cortical nodes are weakly linked to the dorsal attention network and anticorrelated with the default mode network, while the rostral ventral temporal extension of the visual association cortex is weakly connected to the default mode network. The PRC is a key node of the anterior default mode network and plays an important role in memory, especially familiarity-based item recognition memory (Collins and Dickerson, 2019, Dickerson and Eichenbaum, 2010, La Joie et al., 2014, Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012, Suzuki and Naya, 2014). The PRC has also been postulated to be the apex of the visual association network (Collins and Olson, 2014), responsible for linking complex visual representations to their associated semantic features (Barbeau et al., 2008, Martin et al., 2013, Taylor et al., 2006). This process in humans may be mediated by short-range connectivity of PRC with adjacent regions in the lateral anterior temporal lobe, including MTG, integrating high-level object perception with semantic and episodic memory (Miyashita, 2019). Supporting this notion, tract tracing work in macaques has established the existence of direct structural connections from the PRC to TGvg and TGsts (Kondo et al., 2003, Kondo et al., 2005, Saleem et al., 2008). Notably, the anterior MTG and the PRC both succumb to neurodegeneration early in the disease course of svPPA (Collins et al., 2017, Davies et al., 2005, Mummery et al., 2000), providing a potential mechanism for these two regions to become fractionated from each other, and from their respective networks.

4.2. Neurodegeneration of anterior temporal hubs disrupts within- and between-network connectivity in svPPA

Consistent with our hypotheses, in svPPA, the normally weak connectivity between nodes of the dorsal attention and visual association networks is heightened relative to healthy older adults (Fig. 4, Fig. 5, Fig. 6). Specifically, we showed that—in svPPA patients—the rSPL had heightened connectivity with rFFA and rOFA, and the rFEF had heightened connectivity with rFFA. Our results expand upon previous findings suggesting that the SPL might play a critical role in driving altered visual behavior in svPPA. In a previous case study of svPPA, increased blood flow and volume in the rSPL was associated with increased artistic abilities (Seeley et al., 2008). In addition, in a group of patients with svPPA, Viskontas et al. (2011) has shown that the volume of the right and left SPL predicts performance on a conjunction search task, in which participants were measured on how quickly they could find target letter stimuli of a particular color (e.g. green Ns among brown Ns and green Xs). The SPL is a heteromodal association area that has been shown to play a critical role in visual search and the allocation of visual attention in healthy adults (Corbetta et al., 2002, Downar et al., 2000, Kim et al., 1999); we speculate that aberrant neurodegeneration-related increases in the SPL’s connectivity with nodes of the visual association network may lead to attentional capture by simple perceptually rich stimuli, without regard to their meaning. These results show that it is possible for networks which do not include the temporal lobes to be affected by atrophy of this brain region, as seen in svPPA.

We also showed that the within-network connectivity of the default mode network becomes disrupted in svPPA, with the rMTG becoming functionally lesioned from the more posterior nodes of the default mode network including the PCC and AG. These results are consistent with previous work showing a selective vulnerability of anterior temporal default mode network regions in svPPA, in contrast to posterior and medial regions of the default mode network, which are commonly atrophied in Alzheimer’s disease and spared in svPPA (La Joie et al., 2014, Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012). The anterior MTG in the default mode network was of interest in the current study because it is consistently atrophied in svPPA (Bejanin et al., 2019, Collins et al., 2017, La Joie et al., 2014, Pascual et al., 2015, Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012). We hypothesized that when this region of the default mode network is lesioned, connectivity between the visual association and default mode networks would be compromised, and the dorsal attention network would be “released” from its inhibition by the default mode network leading to increased connectivity between the dorsal attention and visual association networks. In support of this hypothesis, previous research in healthy young adults has shown that inhibiting the anterior default mode network through repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the left ATL can lead to the emergence of savant-like visual and numerosity skills. More specifically, in one study participants were able to more accurately guess the number of objects presented on a computer screen (between 50 and 150) in a 1–2 s period following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the left ATL (Snyder et al., 2006). In a separate study participants had an increased ability to create naturalistic drawings after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the left ATL (Snyder, 2009). Additional evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation shows that cathodal stimulation of bilateral ATLs modulates visual fixation patterns during naming, where healthy individuals dwell longer on semantically relevant features, which may be associated with a reduction in response latency (Binney et al., 2018). While the functional network basis of these effects remains unclear, the results of these transcranial stimulation studies are consistent with the results presented here in demonstrating that a lesion to the lateral anterior temporal node of the default mode network can affect visual behavior. Our results support a growing literature that indicates that the default mode network should be considered in terms of its subnetworks as there are large differences in connectivity and task-activations related to its posterior-medial and anterior-temporal nodes (Humphreys et al., 2015, Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012, Ritchey et al., 2015)

4.3. Limitations and future directions

In svPPA patients with primarily left hemisphere anterior temporal lobe atrophy, subtle right anterior temporal atrophy can be seen in a circumscribed manner before it spreads posteriorly to cover more of the temporal lobe (Rogalski et al., 2011). One reason our results were confined to the right hemisphere may be that the left hemisphere is too atrophied to reveal these kinds of changes in functional connectivity, even though most of these patients were mildly clinically impaired. It is common for mildly clinically impaired svPPA patients to have relatively prominent left anterior temporal atrophy. If patients could be identified at an earlier stage of neurodegeneration, we would expect to see similar connectivity patterns in the left hemisphere; possibly as early as the first changes in visual attention are reported, which can sometimes be before language symptoms are present (Seeley et al., 2008). For example, eye tracking work has shown that patients with mild svPPA exhibit altered visual gaze patterns in a word-picture matching task, even for trials with correct responses, suggesting that changes in visual search behavior may precede the loss of a semantic concept (Seckin et al., 2016). The right-lateralized findings reported here may also be explained by the right hemisphere’s role in visual processing, as has been shown in patients with semantic dementia where atrophy in the right anterior temporal lobe is predictive of facial recognition deficits (Ding et al., 2020).

In the present study we did not collect behavioral measures of visual attention, and thus we cannot directly relate the changes in brain networks observed in our svPPA sample to changes in behavior. In future work, we aim to study the changes in visual attentional capture in svPPA using behavioral and eye tracking measures, with the goal of better understanding how large-scale functional network changes due to neurodegeneration can give rise to alterations in complex visual behavior.

Funding

This work was supported by the following National Institutes of Health Grants: R21 NS077059, P01 AG005134, R01 DC014296. This research was carried out in part at the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at the Massachusetts General Hospital, using resources provided by the Center for Functional Neuroimaging Technologies, P41EB015896, a P41 Biotechnology Resource Grant supported by the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering (NIBIB), National Institutes of Health. This work also involved the use of instrumentation supported by the NIH Shared Instrumentation Grant Program and/or High-End Instrumentation Grant Program; specifically, grant number(s) S10RR023401, S10RR023043, S10RR021110. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Health.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Haroon Popal: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing, Visualization. Megan Quimby: Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Daisy Hochberg: Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Bradford C. Dickerson: Conceptualization, Writing - review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources. Jessica A. Collins: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing, Visualization, Supervision.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102494.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

The following are the Supplementary data to this article:

Supplementary data 1
mmc1.docx (528.6KB, docx)

References

  1. Agosta F., Sala S., Valsasina P., Meani A., Canu E., Magnani G., Cappa S.F., Scola E., Quatto P., Horsfield M.A., Falini A., Comi G., Filippi M. Brain network connectivity assessed using graph theory in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 2013;81(2):134–143. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31829a33f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agosta F., Galantucci S., Valsasina P., Canu E., Meani A., Marcone A., Magnani G., Falini A., Comi G., Filippi M. Disrupted brain connectome in semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35(11):2646–2655. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Agosta F., Henry R.G., Migliaccio R., Neuhaus J., Miller B.L., Dronkers N.F., Brambati S.M., Filippi M., Ogar J.M., Wilson S.M., Gorno-Tempini M.L. Language networks in semantic dementia. Brain. 2010;133(1):286–299. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amunts K., Malikovic A., Mohlberg H., Schormann T., Zilles K. Brodmann's areas 17 and 18 brought into stereotaxic space—where and how variable? NeuroImage. 2000;11(1):66–84. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andreano, J.M., Touroutoglou, A., Dickerson, B.C., Feldman Barrett, L., 2017. Resting connectivity between salience nodes predicts recognition memory. Social Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. April, 948–955. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  6. Barbeau E.J., Taylor M.J., Regis J., Marquis P., Chauvel P., Liegeois-Chauvel C. Spatio temporal dynamics of face recognition. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18(5):997–1009. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bejanin A., La Joie R., Landeau B., Belliard S., De La Sayette V., Eustache F., Chételat G. Distinct Interplay between Atrophy and Hypometabolism in Alzheimer’s Versus Semantic Dementia. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29(5):1889–1899. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Binder J.R., Frost J.A., Hammeke T.A., Bellgowan P.S.F., Rao S.M., Cox R.W. Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999;11(1):80–93. doi: 10.1162/089892999563265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Binder, Jeffrey R., Desai, R.H., Graves, W.W., Conant, L.L., 2009. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex, 19(12), 2767–2796. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  10. Binney R.J., Ashaie S.A., Zuckerman B.M., Hung J., Reilly J. Frontotemporal stimulation modulates semantically-guided visual search during confrontation naming: a combined tDCS and eye tracking investigation. Brain Lang. 2018;180–182(March):14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brambati S.M., Rankin K.P., Narvid J., Seeley W.W., Dean D., Rosen H.J., Miller B.L., Ashburner J., Gorno-Tempini M.L. Atrophy progression in semantic dementia with asymmetric temporal involvement: a tensor-based morphometry study. Neurobiol. Aging. 2009;30(1):103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown J.A., Deng J., Neuhaus J., Sible I.J., Sias A.C., Lee S.E., Kornak J., Marx G.A., Karydas A.M., Spina S., Grinberg L.T., Coppola G., Geschwind D.H., Kramer J.H., Gorno-Tempini M.L., Miller B.L., Rosen H.J., Seeley W.W. Patient-tailored, connectivity-based forecasts of spreading brain atrophy. Neuron. 2019;104(5):856–868.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Buckner, R.L., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Schacter, D.L., 2008. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. New York Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed]
  14. Budisavljevic S., Dell'Acqua F., Castiello U. Cross-talk connections underlying dorsal and ventral stream integration during hand actions. Cortex. 2018;103:224–239. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chen J.E., Glover G.H., Greicius M.D., Chang C. Dissociated patterns of anti-correlations with dorsal and ventral default-mode networks at rest: dissociated Patterns of Anti-Correlations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38(5):2454–2465. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Collins J.A., Dickerson B.C. Functional connectivity in category‐selective brain networks after encoding predicts subsequent memory. Hippocampus. 2019;29(5):440–450. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Collins J.A., Montal V., Hochberg D., Quimby M., Mandelli M.L., Makris N., Seeley W.W., Gorno-Tempini M.L., Dickerson B.C. Focal temporal pole atrophy and network degeneration in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2017;140(2):457–471. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Collins J.A., Olson I.R. Beyond the FFA: the role of the ventral anterior temporal lobes in face processing. Neuropsychologia. 2014;61(1):65–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Corbetta M., Kincade J.M., Shulman G.L. Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14(3):508–523. doi: 10.1162/089892902317362029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Corbetta M., Patel G., Shulman G.L. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58(3):306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Corbetta M., Shulman G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dale A.M., Fischl B., Sereno M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Damasio H., Tranel D., Grabowski T., Adolphs R., Damasio A. Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition. 2004;92(1-2):179–229. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Davies, R.R., Hodges, J.R., Kril, J.J., Patterson, K., Halliday, G.M., Xuereb, J.H., 2005. The pathological basis of semantic dementia. Brain, 128, 1984–1995. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh582. [DOI] [PubMed]
  25. Dickerson B.C., Eichenbaum H. The episodic memory system: neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):86–104. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ding J., Chen K., Liu H., Huang L., Chen Y., Lv Y., Yang Q., Guo Q., Han Z., Lambon Ralph M.A. A unified neurocognitive model of semantics language social behaviour and face recognition in semantic dementia. Nat. Commun. 2020;11(1):1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-16089-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Downar J., Crawley A.P., Mikulis D.J., Davis K.D. A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3(3):277–283. doi: 10.1038/72991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Felleman D.J., Van Essen D.C. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 1991;1(1):1–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fischl B., Dale A.M. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2000;97(20):11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fischl B., Rajendran N., Busa E., Augustinack J., Hinds O., Yeo B.T.T., Mohlberg H., Amunts K., Zilles K. Cortical folding patterns and predicting cytoarchitecture. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18(8):1973–1980. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fischl B., Sereno M.I., Dale A.M. Cortical surface-based analysis. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fox M.D., Snyder A.Z., Vincent J.L., Corbetta M., Van Essen D.C., Raichle M.E. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2005;102(27):9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fransson P. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005;26(1):15–29. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Frost B., Ollesch J., Wille H., Diamond M.I. Conformational diversity of wild-type Tau fibrils specified by templated conformation change. J. Biol. Chem. 2009;284(6):3546–3551. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M805627200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Galton C.J., Gomez-Anson B., Antoun N., Scheltens P., Patterson K., Graves M., Sahakian B.J., Hodges J.R. Temporal lobe rating scale: application to Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2001;70(2):165–173. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.70.2.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gorno-Tempini M.L., Hillis A.E., Weintraub S., Kertesz A., Mendez M., Cappa S.F., Ogar J.M., Rohrer J.D., Black S., Boeve B.F., Manes F., Dronkers N.F., Vandenberghe R., Rascovsky K., Patterson K., Miller B.L., Knopman D.S., Hodges J.R., Mesulam M.M., Grossman M. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76(11):1006–1014. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821103e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Green H.A.C., Patterson K. Jigsaws-a preserved ability in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(2):569–576. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Greenblatt Samuel H. Alexia without agraphia or hemianopsia: anatomical analysis of an autopsied case. Brain. 1973;96(2):307–316. doi: 10.1093/brain/96.2.307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Greenblatt S.H. Subangular alexia without agraphia or hemianopsia. Brain Lang. 1976;3(2):229–245. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(76)90019-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Guo, C.C., Gorno-Tempini, M.L., Gesierich, B., Henry, M., Trujillo, A., Shany-Ur, T., Jovicich, J., Robinson, S.D., Kramer, J.H., Rankin, K.P., Miller, B.L., Seeley, W.W., 2013. Anterior temporal lobe degeneration produces widespread network-driven dysfunction. Brain, 136(10), 2979–2991. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  41. Hodges J.R., Patterson K. Semantic dementia: a unique clinicopathological syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6(11):1004–1014. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70266-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hodges John R., Patterson Karalyn, Oxbury Susan, Funnell Elaine. Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain. 1992;115(6):1783–1806. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.6.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hoffman, P., Jones, R.W., Ralph, M.A.L., 2012. The degraded concept representation system in semantic dementia: damage to pan-modal hub, then visual spoke. Brain, 135(12), 3770–3780. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws282. [DOI] [PubMed]
  44. Humphreys G.F., Hoffman P., Visser M., Binney R.J., Lambon Ralph M.A. Establishing task- and modality-dependent dissociations between the semantic and default mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112(25):7857–7862. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422760112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jackson R.L., Hoffman P., Pobric G., Lambon Ralph M.A. The semantic network at work and rest: differential connectivity of anterior Ttmporal lobe subregions. J. Neurosci. 2016;36(5):1490–1501. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2999-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Josephs K.A., Hodges J.R., Snowden J.S., Mackenzie I.R., Neumann M., Mann D.M., Dickson D.W. Neuropathological background of phenotypical variability in frontotemporal dementia. Acta Neuropathol. 2011;122(2):137–153. doi: 10.1007/s00401-011-0839-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kim Y.H., Gitelman D.R., Nobre A.C., Parrish T.B., Labar K.S., Mesulam M.M. The large-scale network for spatial attention displays multifactorial overlap but different asymmetry. Neuroimage. 1999;9(3):269–277. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kondo H., Saleem K.S., Price J.L. Differential connections of the temporal pole with the orbital and medial prefrontal networks in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 2003;465(4):499–523. doi: 10.1002/cne.10842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kondo H., Saleem K.S., Price J.L. Differential connections of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex with the orbital and medial prefrontal networks in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 2005;493(4):479–509. doi: 10.1002/cne.20796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. La Joie R., Landeau B., Perrotin A., Bejanin A., Egret S., Pélerin A., Mézenge F., Belliard S., de La Sayette V., Eustache F., Desgranges B., Chételat G. Intrinsic connectivity identifies the hippocampus as a main crossroad between Alzheimer’s and semantic dementia-targeted networks. Neuron. 2014;81(6):1417–1428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Martin C.B., McLean D.A., O'Neil E.B., Kohler S. Distinct familiarity-based response patterns for faces and buildings in perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013;33(26):10915–10923. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0126-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. McKiernan, K.A., D’Angelo, B.R., Kaufman, J.N., Binder, J.R., 2006. Interrupting the “stream of consciousness”: an fMRI investigation. NeuroImage, 29(4), 1185–1191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  53. McKiernan K.A., Kaufman J.N., Kucera-Thompson J., Binder J.R. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003;15(3):394–408. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mesulam, M.M., Weintraub, S., Rogalski, E.J., Wieneke, C., Geula, C., Bigio, E.H., 2014. Asymmetry and heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal pathology in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 137(4), 1176–1192. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  55. Miller, B.L., Boone, K., Cummings, J.L., Read, S.L., Mishkin, F., 2000. Functional correlates of musical and visual in frontotemporal dementia. Brit. J. Psychiatr. 176, 458–463. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.176.5.458. [DOI] [PubMed]
  56. Milner A.D. How do the two visual streams interact with each other? Exp. Brain Res. 2017;235(5):1297–1308. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4917-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mion M., Patterson K., Acosta-Cabronero J., Pengas G., Izquierdo-Garcia D., Hong Y.T., Fryer T.D., Williams G.B., Hodges J.R., Nestor P.J. What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain. 2010;133(11):3256–3268. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Miyashita Y. Perirhinal circuits for memory processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2019;20(10):577–592. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0213-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Moeller S., Freiwalk W.A., Taso D.Y. Patches with links: a unified system for processing faces in the macaque temporal lobe. Science. 2008;320(5881):1355–1359. doi: 10.1126/science.1157436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mummery C.J., Patterson K., Price C.J., Ashburner J., Frackowiak R.S.J., Hodges J.R. A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory. Ann. Neurol. 2000;47:36–45. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(200001)47:1&#x0003c;36::AID-ANA8&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Oishi H., Takemura H., Aoki S.C., Fujita I., Amano K. Microstructural properties of the vertical occipital fasciculus explain the variability in human stereoacuity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115(48):12289–12294. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1804741115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Palop J.J., Mucke L. Amyloid-β–induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: from synapses toward neural networks. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13(7):812–818. doi: 10.1038/nn.2583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pascual B., Masdeu J.C., Hollenbeck M., Makris N., Insausti R., Ding S.L., Dickerson B.C. Large-scale brain networks of the human left temporal pole: a functional connectivity MRI study. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25(3):680–702. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Patterson K., Nestor P.J., Rogers T.T. Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8(12):976–987. doi: 10.1038/nrn2277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Ralph M.A.L., Jefferies E., Patterson K., Rogers T.T. The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;18(1):42–55. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ranganath C., Ritchey M. Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13(10):713–726. doi: 10.1038/nrn3338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ritchey, M., Libby, L.A., Ranganath, C., 2015. Cortico-hippocampal systems involved in memory and cognition: the PMAT framework. In: Progress in Brain Research, 1st ed., Vol. 219. Elsevier B.V. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed]
  68. Rogalski E., Cobia D., Harrison T.M., Wieneke C., Weintraub S., Mesulam M.-M. Progression of language decline and cortical atrophy in subtypes of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2011;76(21):1804–1810. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821ccd3c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rohrer J.D., Warren J.D., Modat M., Ridgway G.R., Douiri A., Rossor M.N., Ourselin S., Fox N.C. Patterns of cortical thinning in the language variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology. 2009;72(18):1562–1569. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a4124e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rosas H.D., Liu A.K., Hersch S., Glessner M., Ferrante R.J., Salat D.H., van der Kouwe A., Jenkins B.G., Dale A.M., Fischl B. Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington's disease. Neurology. 2002;58(5):695–701. doi: 10.1212/WNL.58.5.695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Salat D.H., Buckner R.L., Snyder A.Z., Greve D.N., Desikan R.S.R., Busa E., Morris J.C., Dale A.M., Fischl B. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb. Cortex. 2004;14(7):721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Saleem K.S., Kondo H., Price J.L. Complementary circuits connecting the orbital and medial prefrontal networks with the temporal, insular, and opercular cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Compar. Neurol. 2008;506:659–693. doi: 10.1002/cne.21577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sanders D., Kaufman S., DeVos S., Sharma A., Mirbaha H., Li A., Barker S., Foley A., Thorpe J., Serpell L., Miller T., Grinberg L., Seeley W., Diamond M. Distinct Tau prion strains propagate in cells and mice and define different Tauopathies. Neuron. 2014;82(6):1271–1288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sapolsky D., Bakkour A., Negreira A., Nalipinski P., Weintraub S., Mesulam M.-M., Caplan D., Dickerson B.C. Cortical neuroanatomic correlates of symptom severity in primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2010;75(4):358–366. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ea15e8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sapolsky D., Domoto-Reilly K., Dickerson B.C. Use of the Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale (PASS) in monitoring speech and language status in PPA. Aphasiology. 2014;28(8-9):993–1003. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2014.931563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Seckin M., Mesulam M.-M., Voss J.L., Huang W., Rogalski E.J., Hurley R.S. Am I looking at a cat or a dog? Gaze in the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia is subject to excessive taxonomic capture. J. Neurolinguist. 2016;37:68–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Seeley W.W., Crawford R.K., Zhou J., Miller B.L., Greicius M.D. Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron. 2009;62(1):42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Seeley, W.W., Matthews, B.R., Crawford, R.K., Gorno-Tempini, M.L., Foti, D., Mackenzie, I.R., Miller, B.L., 2008. Unravelling Boléro: progressive aphasia, transmodal creativity and the right posterior neocortex. Brain, 131(1), 39–49. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm270. [DOI] [PubMed]
  79. Sestieri C., Shulman G.L., Corbetta M. Attention to memory and the environment: functional specialization and dynamic competition in human posterior parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30(25):8445–8456. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4719-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Shapira-Lichter I., Oren N., Jacob Y., Gruberger M., Hendler T. Portraying the unique contribution of the default mode network to internally driven mnemonic processes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2013;110(13):4950–4955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1209888110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Snowden J.S., Goulding P.J., Neary D. Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behav. Neurol. 1989;2(3):167–182. [Google Scholar]
  82. Snyder A. Explaining and inducing savant skills: privileged access to lower level, less-processed information. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2009;364(1522):1399–1405. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Snyder A., Bahramali H., Hawker T., Mitchell D.J. Savant-like numerosity skills revealed in normal people by magnetic pulses. Perception. 2006;35(6):837–845. doi: 10.1068/p5539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Spreng R.N., Stevens W.D., Viviano J.D., Schacter D.L. Attenuated anticorrelation between the default and dorsal attention networks with aging: evidence from task and rest. Neurobiol. Aging. 2016;45:149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Suzuki W.A., Naya Y. The perirhinal cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;37(1):39–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Takemura H., Rokem A., Winawer J., Yeatman J.D., Wandell B.A., Pestilli F. A major human white matter pathway between dorsal and ventral visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26(5):2205–2214. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Taylor K.I., Moss H.E., Stamatakis E.A., Tyler L.K. Binding crossmodal object features in perirhinal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2006;103(21):8239–8244. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509704103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Touroutoglou A., Lindquist K.A., Dickerson B.C., Barrett L.F. Intrinsic connectivity in the human brain does not reveal networks for “basic” emotions. Social Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015;10(9):1257–1265. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tranel D., Damasio H., Damasio A.R. Fundamentals of Neural Network Modeling: Neuropsychology and Cognitive Neuroscience. The MIT Press; 1998. The neural basis of lexical retrieval; pp. 271–296. [Google Scholar]
  90. Tyler L.K., Moss H.E. Going, going, gone? Implicit and explicit tests of conceptual knowledge in a longitudinal study of semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36(12):1313–1323. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00029-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Ungerleider L.G., Galkin T.W., Desimone R., Gattass R. Cortical connections of area V4 in the macaque. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18(3):477–499. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Ungerleider L.G., Haxby J.V. “What” and “where” in the human brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1994;4(2):157–165. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90066-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Ungrady M.B., Flurie M., Zuckerman B.M., Mirman D., Reilly J. Naming and knowing revisited: eyetracking correlates of anomia in progressive Aphasia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019;13(October):1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Van Dijk K.R.A., Hedden T., Venkataraman A., Evans K.C., Lazar S.W., Buckner R.L. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103(1):297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. van Polanen V., Davare M. Interactions between dorsal and ventral streams for controlling skilled grasp. Neuropsychologia. 2015;79:186–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vincent J.L., Kahn I., Snyder A.Z., Raichle M.E., Buckner R.L. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2008;100(6):3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Viskontas I.V., Boxer A.L., Fesenko J., Matlin A., Heuer H.W., Miller B.L. Visual search patterns in semantic dementia show paradoxical facilitation of binding processes. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(3):468–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.039.Visual. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Visser M., Jefferies E., Lambon Ralph M., a. Semantic processing in the anterior temporal lobes: a meta-analysis of the functional neuroimaging literature. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2010;22(6):1083–1094. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wang Xu, Zhen Zonglei, Song Yiying, Huang Lijie, Kong Xiangzhen, Liu Jia. The hierarchical structure of the face network revealed by its functional connectivity pattern. J. Neurosci. 2016;36(3):890–900. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2789-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wang Yin, Collins Jessica A., Koski Jessica, Nugiel Tehila, Metoki Athanasia, Olson Ingrid R. Dynamic neural architecture for social knowledge retrieval. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114(16):E3305–E3314. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1621234114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Warrington Elizabeth K. The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quart. J. Experiment. Psychol. 1975;27(4):635–657. doi: 10.1080/14640747508400525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Wernicke C. Vol. 2. Fischer; 1881. (Lehrbuch der gehirnkrankheiten für aerzte und studirende). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Wirth Miranka, Jann Kay, Dierks Thomas, Federspiel Andrea, Wiest Roland, Horn Helge. Semantic memory involvement in the default mode network: a functional neuroimaging study using independent component analysis. NeuroImage. 2011;54(4):3057–3066. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Yeatman Jason D., Weiner Kevin S., Pestilli Franco, Rokem Ariel, Mezer Aviv, Wandell Brian A. The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111(48):E5214–E5223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418503111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Zhang J., Abiose O., Katsumi Y., Touroutoglou A., Dickerson B.C., Barrett L.F. Intrinsic functional connectivity is organized as three interdependent gradients. Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1):1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51793-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Zhang, J., Andreano, J.M., Dickerson, B.C., Touroutoglou, A., Barrett, L.F., 2020. Stronger functional connectivity in the default mode and salience networks is associated with youthful memory in superaging. Cereb. Cortex, 30(1), 72–84. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary data 1
mmc1.docx (528.6KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

Imaging data are available upon request.


Articles from NeuroImage : Clinical are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES