Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2020 Dec 9;40(50):9634–9649. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0276-20.2020

Complementary Inhibitory Weight Profiles Emerge from Plasticity and Allow Flexible Switching of Receptive Fields

Everton J Agnes 1,, Andrea I Luppi 1, Tim P Vogels 1,2
PMCID: PMC7726533  PMID: 33168622

Abstract

Cortical areas comprise multiple types of inhibitory interneurons, with stereotypical connectivity motifs that may follow specific plasticity rules. Yet, their combined effect on postsynaptic dynamics has been largely unexplored. Here, we analyze the response of a single postsynaptic model neuron receiving tuned excitatory connections alongside inhibition from two plastic populations. Synapses from each inhibitory population change according to distinct plasticity rules. We tested different combinations of three rules: Hebbian, anti-Hebbian, and homeostatic scaling. Depending on the inhibitory plasticity rule, synapses become unspecific (flat), anticorrelated to, or correlated with excitatory synapses. Crucially, the neuron's receptive field (i.e., its response to presynaptic stimuli) depends on the modulatory state of inhibition. When both inhibitory populations are active, inhibition balances excitation, resulting in uncorrelated postsynaptic responses regardless of the inhibitory tuning profiles. Modulating the activity of a given inhibitory population produces strong correlations to either preferred or nonpreferred inputs, in line with recent experimental findings that show dramatic context-dependent changes of neurons' receptive fields. We thus confirm that a neuron's receptive field does not follow directly from the weight profiles of its presynaptic afferents. Our results show how plasticity rules in various cell types can interact to shape cortical circuit motifs and their dynamics.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Neurons in sensory areas of the cortex are known to respond to specific features of a given input (e.g., specific sound frequencies), but recent experimental studies show that such responses (i.e., their receptive fields) depend on context. Inspired by the cortical connectivity, we built models of excitatory and inhibitory inputs onto a single neuron, to study how receptive fields may change on short and long time scales. We show how various synaptic plasticity rules allow for the emergence of diverse connectivity profiles and, moreover, how their dynamic interaction creates a mechanism by which postsynaptic responses can quickly change. Our work emphasizes multiple roles of inhibition in cortical processing and provides a first mechanistic model for flexible receptive fields.

Keywords: cortex, disinhibition, EI balance, receptive field, synaptic plasticity

Introduction

Inhibitory neurons exhibit large variability in morphology, connectivity motifs, and electrophysiological properties (Markram et al., 2004; Jiang et al., 2015; Jouhanneau et al., 2018; Gouwens et al., 2019). Inhibition often balances excitatory inputs, thus stabilizing neuronal network activity (Van Vreeswijk and Sompolinsky, 1996; Vogels et al., 2011) and allowing for a range of different functions (Hennequin et al., 2017; Maffei, 2017; Sprekeler, 2017; Chiu et al., 2019; Nicola and Clopath, 2019). When both inhibitory and excitatory inputs share the same statistics and their weight profiles are similar (Froemke et al., 2007), the resulting state of the postsynaptic neuron is one of precise balance of input currents (Hennequin et al., 2017). Modulation of inhibition (e.g., a decrease or increase in local inhibitory activity) and, consequently, a change in the balance between excitation and inhibition, can control the activity of neuronal groups (Letzkus et al., 2015; Kuchibhotla et al., 2017), and it is believed that disinhibition is an important mechanism for the implementation of high-level brain functions, such as attention (Zhang et al., 2014; Kuchibhotla et al., 2017), memory retrieval (Vogels et al., 2011; Barron et al., 2016; Vallentin et al., 2016), signal gating (Vogels and Abbott, 2009; Kremkow et al., 2010), and rapid learning (Nicola and Clopath, 2019).

The state of balance is thought to be achieved and maintained by inhibitory plasticity, for example, a Hebbian-like inhibitory plasticity rule (Vogels et al., 2011) (increase in synaptic weights for correlated presynaptic and postsynaptic activity), as observed in auditory cortex (D'amour and Froemke, 2015). Other types of inhibitory plasticity have also been observed, for example, changes in chloride reversal potential (Woodin et al., 2003) that locally decrease the driving force of inhibitory synapses following correlated presynaptic and postsynaptic activity, suggesting a form of anti-Hebbian inhibitory plasticity that has been proposed as a mechanism for memory formation (Hendin et al., 1997).

Given that cortical circuit motifs feature multiple interneuron types (Jiang et al., 2015; Maffei, 2017; Gouwens et al., 2019), we wondered how these opposing types of plasticity may act in concert on the same postsynaptic target, and how the resulting synaptic weight profiles can help to shape the receptive field (i.e., the neurons' response to presynaptic stimuli). We speculated that two plasticity rules could form complementary synaptic weight profiles for inhibitory connections, such that synapses following a Hebbian-like inhibitory plasticity rule would mirror excitatory inputs; anti-Hebbian plasticity should impose strong inhibitory inputs for weak excitatory ones, and vice versa. Such opposite wiring profiles of distinct inhibitory synapse populations are in line with intracellular recordings showing that strong inhibitory postsynaptic potentials can be elicited by stimuli with preferred orientations of the postsynaptic neuron (Ferster, 1986; Douglas and Martin, 1991), but also by stimuli with nonpreferred orientations (Volgushev et al., 1993; Pei et al., 1994). What's more, dynamically changing receptive fields could be achieved through targeted modulation of a specific type of inhibition.

Altered receptive field properties have been widely observed (e.g., in mouse auditory cortex where neurons change their preferred sound frequency with varying sound intensity) (Bathellier et al., 2012). In macaque primary visual cortex (V1), neurons can modulate their response according to an extra cue of a different (auditory) sensory nature (McClure and Polack, 2019). Intriguingly, they responded either more strongly to their preferred stimulus or, on the contrary, they were more suppressed when a pure tone was played alongside the presentation of the visual stimuli. In macaque V4 (Ruff and Cohen, 2014) and V5 (Ruff and Cohen, 2019), neurons have been shown to change how they represent different stimuli during detection and discrimination tasks; and in macaque V4 (Benjamin et al., 2019), some neurons change their hue preference when subjected to single-hue or naturally colored images. Receptive field profiles have also been shown to induce localized changes (around preferred inputs) in attention tasks (Fritz et al., 2003), and adaptation of the postsynaptic activity to repetitive stimulation (Kohn and Movshon, 2004). Finally, recent work by Billeh et al. (2019) showed that, in mice, visual neurons change their response to the direction of motion of visual stimuli depending on either the temporal or the spatial frequency of the stimulus (drifting grating). These results suggest that receptive fields of sensory neurons are dramatically affected by input (i.e., contextual/attentional states) (Fritz et al., 2003; Kohn and Movshon, 2004; Ruff and Cohen, 2014, 2019; Benjamin et al., 2019; McClure and Polack, 2019), or different aspects of the sensory stimulus (Bathellier et al., 2012; Billeh et al., 2019), but it is unclear by which mechanisms such changes can transpire.

Here, we tested how the response of a single neuron is affected when the activity of presynaptic inhibitory populations is modulated. We combined two hypotheses to address this question. First, we considered that different types of inhibitory interneurons may follow distinct synaptic plasticity learning rules (Fig. 1A), thus creating different connectivity profiles onto postsynaptic neurons (Hennequin et al., 2017) (Fig. 1B), such as those observed for parvalbumin-positive (PV+) and somatostatin-positive (SOM+) interneurons (Wilson et al., 2012). PV+ interneurons may follow a Hebbian-like plasticity rule (Vogels et al., 2011; D'amour and Froemke, 2015), thus targeting pyramidal neurons with similar preferred orientation (Wilson et al., 2012). SOM+ interneurons, on the other hand, could follow a non-Hebbian plasticity rule (e.g., anti-Hebbian or homeostatic), which results in a nonselective connectivity (Wilson et al., 2012). Our second hypothesis posits that changes in the activity of inhibitory neurons are responsible for the highly variable receptive fields observed in recent experiments (Bathellier et al., 2012; Ruff and Cohen, 2014, 2019; Benjamin et al., 2019; Billeh et al., 2019; McClure and Polack, 2019) (Fig. 1C). This hypothesis extrapolates from evidence of cortical disinhibition during functional tasks (Pi et al., 2013; Letzkus et al., 2015), and requires that a different brain region provide attentional or contextual signals, such as observed in PFC and regions in the frontal lobe (Noudoost et al., 2010; Anderson et al., 2011; Baluch and Itti, 2011; Benchenane et al., 2011).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Learning of two distinct inhibitory populations and postsynaptic response because of attentional switch between contexts. A, Schematic of co-active plasticity rules. A postsynaptic neuron (black triangle) receives tuned excitatory input (red population) and inhibition from two distinct populations (blue populations). The two inhibitory populations follow different synaptic plasticity rules. Δw indicates change in synaptic weight, and Δt indicates interval between presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes. B, Initially untuned inhibitory weights (blue lines) acquire different synaptic weight profiles after learning that depend on the excitatory weight profile (red dashed line). C, Contextual changes (e.g., because of attention), which we hypothesize to be responsible for modulating the activity of inhibitory populations, result in different postsynaptic responses to the same stimulus (Bathellier et al., 2012; Ruff and Cohen, 2014, 2019; Benjamin et al., 2019; Billeh et al., 2019; McClure and Polack, 2019), such that preferred (green) and nonpreferred (purple) stimuli elicit postsynaptic responses with different amplitudes.

To determine the origins of such varying responses from the same cell, we investigated the behavior of a single postsynaptic neuron model receiving tuned excitatory inputs, and inhibition from two distinct populations. Input tuning may correspond to preference for a specific sound frequency (Froemke et al., 2007), orientation of visual cues (Smith et al., 2013), or to color hue (Benjamin et al., 2019), taste (Haley et al., 2016), whisker stimulation (Estebanez et al., 2018), or position in space (Harvey et al., 2009). We show that, when distinct biologically plausible plasticity rules operate on the synapses of different inhibitory populations, at least three different tuning profiles may emerge. After learning, the postsynaptic neuron arrives at a balanced state with respect to its excitatory and inhibitory inputs. In this state, preferred signals are transiently revealed, but steady-state responses are indiscriminate of the stimulus preference (Vogels et al., 2011) (i.e., its “orientation,” etc.), regardless of the inhibitory connectivity. However, we could substantially alter the responses of the postsynaptic neuron by modulating the activity of either of the two presynaptic inhibitory populations, allowing for the propagation of facets of the input patterns that were previously quenched by inhibition. Such inhibitory modulation can thus serve as a mechanism to selectively filter stimuli according to, for example, attentional cues, as observed in recent experiments (Bathellier et al., 2012; Ruff and Cohen, 2014, 2019; Benjamin et al., 2019; Billeh et al., 2019; McClure and Polack, 2019). In summary, our work proposes a simple biological implementation for an attentional switch of input selectivity, and provides a solution for how such a neuronal circuit can emerge with autonomous and unsupervised, biologically plausible plasticity rules. To our best knowledge, our model is the first proof of principle that the receptive field of a neuron (i.e., its response to presynaptic stimuli) does not have to follow directly from the (excitatory) presynaptic weight profiles.

Materials and Methods

Neuron model

To investigate changes in neuronal response because of specific inhibitory connectivity motif, we simulated a postsynaptic leaky integrate-and-fire neuron (LIF) receiving excitatory and inhibitory afferents. Postsynaptic neuronal membrane potential dynamics is governed by the following:

τmdu(t)dt=[u(t)urest]
gE(t)[u(t)EE]gI(t)[u(t)EI], (1)

where u(t) is the somatic voltage at time t, τm = RC is the membrane time constant (membrane resistance, R, times membrane capacitance, C), urest is the resting membrane potential, and EE and EI are the reversal potential for excitatory and inhibitory synapses, respectively. Synaptic conductances, gE (t) and gI (t), evolve according to the following:

dgE(t)dt=gE(t)τE + j=1NEwj(t)Sj(t) (2)

and

dgI(t)dt=gI(t)τI+j=NE+1Nwj(t)Sj(t). (3)

Both excitatory and inhibitory conductances decay exponentially to zero with time constants τE and τI,, respectively. Presynaptic action potentials trigger increase in synaptic conductances through the sum of Dirac δ functions as follows:

Sj(t)=kδ(ttkj), (4)

where tkj is the time of the kth spike of presynaptic afferent j. The contribution of a given presynaptic afferent j to changes in conductances is given by the synaptic weight wj(t), which was fixed for excitatory synapses and could change over time because of plasticity mechanisms for inhibitory synapses. The total number of presynaptic afferents is N=NE + NI, with NE being the number of excitatory and NI of inhibitory presynaptic afferents.

An action potential is triggered at the postsynaptic neuron once its membrane potential u(t) crosses the spiking threshold uth from below. The membrane potential is then instantaneously reset to ureset, being clamped at this value for the duration of the refractory period, τref. The postsynaptic spike train is described as a sum of Dirac deltas as follows:

Spost(t)=kδ(ttk), (5)

where tk is the time of the kth spike of the postsynaptic neuron, or the time the membrane potential crosses the spiking threshold from below. Parameters used for the postsynaptic neuron are detailed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Simulation parameters for the postsynaptic neuron

Parameter Symbol Value Figures
Membrane time constant τm 30 ms 411
Resting potential urest –65 mV 411
Excitatory reversal potential EE 0 mV 411
Inhibitory reversal potential EI –80 mV 411
Excitatory time constant τE 5 ms 411
Inhibitory time constant τI 10 ms 411
Spiking threshold uth –50 mV 411
Reset potential ureset –65 mV 411
Refractory period τref 5 ms 411
Simulation time step Δt 0.1 ms 411

Inputs

To mimic experimentally observed synaptic input profiles (Froemke et al., 2007), we divided the synaptic inputs into P signal groups (μ = 1, …, P) that share the same fluctuation in firing rate. We tested two cases: natural input and pulse input. Both are described below.

Natural input

For presynaptic activity mimicking a natural input, activity follows an inhomogeneous Poisson process that changes according to a modified Ornstein-Uhlenbeck (OU) process. We first defined an auxiliary variable for each pattern, yμ(t), that follows a stochastic first-order differential equation given by the following:

dyμ(t)dt=yμ(t)τOU+ξμ(t), (6)

where μ is the signal group index, τOU is the time constant for the decaying process that changes because of a Gaussian noise term ξμ(t) with unitary standard deviation (SD). The mean value of the variable yμ is zero, and thus it assumes positive and negative values for long periods.

The spike train of an afferent in a given signal group μ is given by the variable, νXμ(t), which is a rectified version of the auxiliary variable plus a term to generate background firing rate, νXbg, where X indicates the presynaptic population (X = E for excitatory and X = I for inhibitory). The spike trains of the afferents of signal group μ are generated by the following:

νXμ(t)=νX0[yμ(t)]+ + νXbg, (7)

where νX0 is the amplitude of the modulated firing rate fluctuations, and [·]+ is a rectifying function, as follows:

[y]+={y, if y > 00,otherwise. (8)

Because of the symmetry of yμ(t), an afferent is half the time in the background state and half the time in the active state.

Presynaptic action potentials were generated as an inhomogeneous Poisson process according to the modified OU process described above and a fixed background firing rate. Additionally, we implemented a refractory period, τEref for excitatory and τIref for inhibitory inputs. Given the time step of the simulation Δt, spikes of a presynaptic afferent that is part of the signal group μ are generated with a probability pXμ(t)=νXμ(t)Δt if there was no spike elicited during the refractory period beforehand, and thus the average firing rate of a X = E (excitatory) or X = I (inhibitory) afferent that is part of the signal group μ becomes the following:

FXμ(t)=1ΔtpXμ(t)(1pXμ(t))τXref/Δt. (9)
Pulse input

To test transient responses to brief changes in presynaptic activity, we also quantified postsynaptic responses to pulse inputs. In this case, we simulated the postsynaptic neuron receiving inputs with constant background firing rate. For 100 ms, we increased the probability of presynaptic spikes for a given signal group μ by a factor kν*, with k being an integer larger or equal than zero and ν* = 5 Hz. Thus, presynaptic spikes are generated by the following:

νXμ(t)=αXνkν* + νXbg, (10)

during the 100 ms step and by the following:

νXμ(t)=νXbg (11)

during only background activity. Parameter α is a scalar that sets the ratio of excitatory and inhibitory firing rate.

Responses to the pulse input were divided in two bins: phasic and tonic. Phasic responses were defined as the postsynaptic activity elicited in the first 50 ms of the pulse input. Tonic activity was correspondingly defined as having occurred in the last 50 ms of the stimulus. We simulated 100 trials per input strength kν*, and defined the response (for both phasic and tonic) as the average number of spikes on the period for the strength kν* minus the average number of spikes on the same period without extra input, multiplied by 20 to convert to Hz. We subtracted background spikes to ascertain that we quantified the response to the extra step input alone. The tonic input window was used to assess persistent, steady-state postsynaptic responses to elevated inputs. To confirm that the firing behavior within these 50 ms indeed reflected a steady state, we ran control simulations with a variety of intervals: from 50 to 500 ms. All tests showed that our 50 ms window was sufficient to obtain a representative sample of steady-state, “tonic” activity. Parameters used for inputs are detailed in Table 2.

Table 2.

Simulation parameters for the inputs

Parameter Symbol Value Figures
No. of excitatory afferents NE 3200 411
No. of inhibitory afferents NI 800 411
No. of signal groups P 16 411
Refractory period for excitatory afferents τEref 5 ms 2, 411
Refractory period for inhibitory afferents τIref 2.5 ms 2, 411
OU time constant τOU 50 ms 2, 411
OU update time step ΔT 1 ms 2, 411
Excitatory firing rate amplitude for OU νE0 5 Hz 2, 411
Inhibitory firing rate amplitude for OU νI0 10 Hz 2, 411
Excitatory background firing rate νEbg 2 Hz 2, 411
Inhibitory background firing rate νIbg 4 Hz 2, 411
Pulse amplitude reference ν* 5 Hz 4, 8, 10, 11
Excitatory ratio for pulse input αEν 1 4, 8, 10, 11
Inhibitory ratio for pulse input αIν 2 4, 8, 10, 11
Synaptic weight profile amplitude r0 4 411
Synaptic weight profile slope b 0.25 411
Preferred pattern index μ0 9 411
Synaptic weight profile power c 2 411
Simulation time step Δt 0.1 ms 2, 411

Synaptic tuning

Based on Vogels et al. (2011), we used a synaptic weight profile for the excitatory population given by the following:

r(μ)=(11 + r0) + (r01 + r0)(11 + b(μμ0)c), (12)

where r0, b, and c are parameters defining the shape of the synaptic weight profile and μ0 defines the preferred signal group, which maximizes r(μ); r0) = 1. Note that r01,0 < b1,μ0 > 0, and c is an even positive integer.

For simplicity, we define ζj as the signal group of which that afferent j is part. Thus, excitatory synapses are set as follows:

wj=wE0r(ζj) + ϵj,j=1,...,NE, (13)

where wE0 is a normalization factor for excitatory weights, and ϵj is a noise term drawn from a uniform random distribution between ϵE* and ϵE*.

Initial conditions for all inhibitory populations were flat with noise (see Figs. 5, 7, 9), as follows:

wj(0)=wIF + ϵj,j=NE + 1,...,N. (14)
Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Inhibitory plasticity acting on one inhibitory population compensates global inhibition from a second inhibitory population. A, Schematic of the synaptic weight profile for excitatory synapses (red) and different initial conditions for inhibitory synapses (pink to purple color code). Inhibitory Population 1 has its inhibitory synapses changing according to a plasticity mechanism, whereas Population 2 remains fixed. B, Time course of the postsynaptic firing rate for different initial conditions (colors as in A). Inhibitory plasticity on Population 1 is set to achieve a balanced state with target of 5 Hz (arrowhead). C, Stabilized postsynaptic firing rate as a function of the initial inhibitory synaptic weight. D, Individual synaptic weight profiles for excitatory (red), inhibitory Population 1 (blue, after synaptic stabilization), and inhibitory Population 2 (colors as in A). E, Total synaptic weight per signal (excitatory – inhibitory) for different initial conditions after stabilization of synapses from Population 1. F, Example of synaptic dynamics of inhibitory Population 1 for a given initial condition. Colors represent different signal groups. G, Final weights as a function of initial inhibitory weights. Plotted are excitatory (red), plastic inhibitory (blue), and sum of total inhibitory synapses (gray).

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

Simultaneous learning of two inhibitory profiles via Hebbian and homeostatic scaling plasticity rules. A, Temporal evolution of inhibitory synaptic weights when one inhibitory population follows a Hebbian plasticity rule (top), and the other population follows a synaptic scaling plasticity rule (bottom). B, Initial (top) and final (bottom) weight profiles from A, with excitatory weights for reference. C, Individual synaptic weights before and after learning for synapses following the Hebbian plasticity rule (top) and synapses following the scaling plasticity rule (bottom).

Figure 9.

Figure 9.

Simultaneous learning of two inhibitory profiles via Hebbian and anti-Hebbian plasticity rules. A, Temporal evolution of inhibitory synaptic weights when one inhibitory population follows a Hebbian plasticity rule (top), and the other population follows an anti-Hebbian plasticity rule (bottom). B, Initial (top) and final (bottom) weight profiles from A, with excitatory weights for reference. C, Individual synaptic weights before and after learning for synapses following the Hebbian plasticity rule (top) and synapses following the anti-Hebbian plasticity rule (bottom).

In all cases, except for one (see Fig. 5), all inhibitory synapses changed according to inhibitory plasticity rules (details below). For Figure 5, one population of inhibitory afferents were plastic and another was kept fixed throughout the learning period of the simulations. Parameters used for synaptic weights are detailed in Tables 2 and 3.

Table 3.

Simulation parameters for the weights

Parameter Symbol Value Figures
Excitatory baseline weight wE0 0.5 411
Noise parameter for excitatory weights ϵE* 0.01 411
Inhibitory baseline weight (one inhibitory population) wIF 0.4 Data not shown
Noise parameter for inhibitory weights (one inhibitory population) ϵI* 0.01 Data not shown
Inhibitory baseline weight wIF Varying 5, 6
Noise parameter for inhibitory weights ϵI* 0.01 5, 6
Inhibitory baseline weight (Hebbian & scaling) wIF 0.8 7
Noise parameter for inhibitory weights (Hebbian & scaling) ϵI* 0.3 7
Inhibitory baseline weight (Hebbian & anti-Hebbian) wIF 0.55 9
Noise parameter for inhibitory weights (Hebbian & anti-Hebbian) ϵI* 0.01 9
Correcting factor for plot αw 4.4 5, 7, 9

Because of the small number of inhibitory afferents compared with the excitatory ones, and the difference in driving force, inhibitory weights were much larger than excitatory ones. Thus, we plotted excitatory weights multiplied by the parameter αw.

Plasticity models

In this work, we used three different inhibitory synaptic plasticity rules. We termed them “Hebbian,” “scaling,” and “anti-Hebbian.” Both Hebbian and anti-Hebbian plasticity rules are triggered by presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes, and depend on a low-pass filter of these spike trains. The presynaptic trace (low-pass filter) is given by the following:

dxj(t)dt=xj(t)τSTDP + Sj(t), (15)

where xj(t) is the value of the trace of the spike train of presynaptic afferent j at time t; τSTDP is the time constant of the trace, and Sj(t) is a sum of Dirac δ functions (Eq. 4) representing the spike train of afferent j. The same is considered for the postsynaptic neuron, as follows:

dxpost(t)dt=xpost(t)τSTDP + Spost(t), (16)

where xpost (t) is the postsynaptic trace, and Spost (t) is the spike train of the postsynaptic neuron (Eq. 5). We used the same time constant for both presynaptic and postsynaptic traces.

Hebbian inhibitory plasticity

Precise balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs was learned by a Hebbian inhibitory plasticity rule (Vogels et al., 2011). The weight of the jth inhibitory synapse changes according to the following:

dwj(t)dt=ηH[xj(t)Spost(t) + xpost(t)Sj(t)αHSj(t)], (17)

where ηH is the learning rate, and αH is a parameter that defines the postsynaptic firing rate. The first two terms on the right-hand side of Equation 17 are Hebbian terms that increase the weights when both presynaptic and postsynaptic activities are correlated. The last term on the right-hand side of Equation 17 is a penalty term for inhibitory spikes alone, which creates a homeostatic setpoint for the postsynaptic firing rate given by the following:

ρ0αH2τSTDP. (18)

Later we describe how to arrive at this approximation.

Inhibitory synaptic scaling for flat tuning

One of the synaptic weight profiles we used for inhibitory synapses was flat (i.e., all synapse groups had the same mean strength). To learn the flat profile from random initial weights, we implemented a scaling plasticity rule, partially based on experimental work that observed synaptic scaling on inhibitory synapses (Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006; Zhong et al., 2018). Weights are increased if the postsynaptic firing rates are too high, and decreased otherwise, as follows:

dwj(t)dt=ηswIs[ypost(t)ρ0]Θ(ypost(t)αsρ0)
ηswj(t)[ρ0ypost(t)]Θ(ρ0αsypost(t)), (19)

where ηs is a learning rate, wIs is a reference weight, ρ0 is a firing rate reference value, chosen to be the same as the one for Hebbian plasticity rule, Θ(·) is the Heaviside function, and αs is a term that sets the firing rate range for which synapses do not change. Postsynaptic neuron's firing rate is computed with a slow averaging of the postsynaptic activity through the following:

dypost(t)dt=ypost(t)τscaling + 1τscalingSpost(t), (20)

where τscaling is the time constant for the postsynaptic activity and Spost(t) is the postsynaptic spike train (Eq. 5). The last term on the right-hand side of the equation above is divided by τscaling so that ypost (t) is in units of rate. Synaptic depression is weight-dependent, whereas synaptic potentiation is not, which ensures that all synaptic weights tend to the same value. When the postsynaptic neuron is firing below a threshold ρ0/αs, all inhibitory synapses in the flat group have their weights decreased proportionally to the difference between the target firing rate and the average firing rate, but also proportional to the current weight value. This way, strong synapses undergo stronger decrease than weak ones. Conversely, when the postsynaptic neuron is firing above a threshold αs ρ0, all synapses increase in value by the same amount. Intuitively, these mechanisms ensure that all synapses converge to the same value for a long run (see below).

Anti-Hebbian inhibitory plasticity

The third inhibitory plasticity rule we used is an anti-Hebbian rule inspired by experimental data (Woodin et al., 2003; Ormond and Woodin, 2009, 2011) and theoretical work on recurrent networks (Hendin et al., 1997). Synaptic weights change according to the following:

dwj(t)dt=ηaH(t)[xj(t)Spost(t) + xpost(t)Sj(t)αaHSj(t)], (21)

where ηaH(t) is a variable learning rate and αaH is a parameter to counterbalance the anti-Hebbian term (see also Discussion). The resulting rule dictates that coincident events decrease inhibitory synapses, whereas noncoincident ones increase synaptic weights. Because of the unstable nature of this plasticity rule (see details below), we implemented a time-varying learning rate, which evolves according to the following:

dηaH(t)dt=ηaH(t)τaH + MaH(t), (22)

where τaH is the decay time constant for the learning rate, and MaH (t) is an external signal to transiently activate plasticity. We speculate that such signal could come from modulatory neurons, such as dopaminergic or cholinergic and assumed that the external signal peaks at a time t0 (beginning of the simulation), so that

MaH(t)=ηaH*δ(tt0), (23)

where ηaH* is the maximum learning rate before decaying to zero, and t0 is the time when plasticity at these synapses is initiated. Parameters used for plasticity models are detailed in Table 4.

Table 4.

Simulation parameters for the plasticity rules

Parameter Symbol Value Figures
STDP time constant τSTDP 20 ms 5, 7, 9
Hebbian learning rate ηH 10−3 5, 7, 9
Hebbian decay term αH 0.2 5, 7, 9
Firing rate setpoint ρ0 5 Hz 5, 7, 9
Scaling time constant τscaling 1000 ms 7
Scaling learning rate ηs 10−7 7
Scaling learning rate weight wIs 0.8 7
Scaling threshold parameter αs 2 7
Anti-Hebbian initial learning rate ηaH* 10−3 9
Anti-Hebbian learning rate time constant τaH 250 s 9
Anti-Hebbian increase term αaH 0.165 9
Anti-Hebbian peak time t0 0 ms 9
Simulation time 30 min 5, 7, 9
Mean-field analysis of the plasticity rules

We were interested in plasticity rules with stable dynamics. For a better intuition on fixed-point dynamics and stability, we consider here a simplified dynamics of a mean-field model for both the Hebbian (Vogels et al., 2011) and the anti-Hebbian models. We define the postsynaptic firing rate as νpost (t) and the presynaptic firing rates as νj(t). The traces of both presynaptic afferent and postsynaptic neuron thus have an average of τSTDPνj(t) and τSTDPνpost(t), respectively (Zenke et al., 2015). Neglecting any correlation between presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes, the average weight change for Hebbian synapses is given by the following:

dwj(t)dt=ηH[2τSTDPνj(t)νpost(t)αHνj(t)], (24)

where · represents average over time. Intuitively, the postsynaptic firing rate, νpost (t), changes negatively with changes in inhibitory weights: increased inhibition generates fewer postsynaptic spikes and vice versa for decreased inhibition. This means that average firing rates are inversely linked to average inhibitory weights, as follows:

dνpost(t)dtdwj(t)dt=2ηHνj(t)τSTDP[αH2τSTDPνpost(t)]. (25)

The steady state is computed by considering the vanishing point of the equation above (we assume that the presynaptic activity is nonzero); thus,

νpost(t)=αH2τSTDPρ0. (26)

This means that the postsynaptic activity νpost(t) increases (via reduction in inhibitory efficacy) when below ρ0 and decreases when above ρ0, creating a stable fixed point for the postsynaptic firing rate.

The opposite is true for the anti-Hebbian plasticity rule. Changes in postsynaptic firing rate (with the same assumption as for the Hebbian plasticity rule) follow the following:

dνpost(t)dtνpost(t)αaH2τSTDP=νpost(t)ρ1. (27)

Because postsynaptic activity increases when it is above threshold ρ1 and decreases when it is below, this rule is unstable. The postsynaptic firing rate eventually explodes or vanishes. We chose the simplest way to overcome these problems by setting a time-varying learning rate. Other intricate mechanisms could be implemented, but this is not the scope of our work.

Convergence of weights following the scaling plasticity rule

Our scaling plasticity rule has two different mechanisms: one for LTD and one for LTP. LTD is multiplicative, and LTP is additive (Eq. 19). The combined effect ensures that all incoming weights collapse to the same value (synaptic changes do not depend on presynaptic activity either). Here we present a mathematical intuition to explain how synaptic weights can converge to the same value. First, we rewrite the scaling plasticity rule into two simplified terms. We consider constant postsynaptic firing rate during LTD, ypost(t)=ρ0y¯postLTD, with ypost(t)<ρ0/αs. Consequently, the LTD part is described by the following:

dwj(t)dt=ηswj(t)y¯postLTD (28)

with solution as follows:

wj(t)=wj(0) exp(ηSy¯postLTDt). (29)

Doing the same for LTP (ypost(t)=y¯postLTP+ρ0, with ypost(t)>ρ0αs), we arrive at the following:

dwj(t)dt=ηswIsy¯postLTP (30)

with solution as follows:

wj(t)=wj(0) + ηswIsy¯postLTPt. (31)

Defining tiLTD as the ith interval in which LTD occurred and tiLTP as the ith interval in which the synapse underwent LTP, we can combine Equations 29 and 31 to rewrite the synaptic strength, wj(t), at time t as follows:

wj(t)=wj(0) exp(ηsy¯postLTDi=1TLTDtiLTD)
+ηswIsy¯postLTPi=1TLTPtiLTP exp(ηsy¯postLTDk=i+1TLTDtkLTD), (32)

where TLTD and TLTP are the number of intervals with LTD and LTP, respectively. We assume that LTP is always followed by LTD in Equation 32 for simplicity, and thus TLTD =TLTP ± 1. The first term on the right-hand side of Equation 32 vanishes for long times, and the second term dominates with the late terms (i >> 1, or, from TLTPκ to TLTP for small κ), as follows:

limtwj(t)ηswIsy¯postLTPi=TLTPκTLTPtiLTP exp(ηsy¯postLTDk=i+1TLTDtkLTD), (33)

which is finite given that the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate fluctuates around the target firing rate, ρ0, and does not depend on the initial weight wj(0).

Correlation

We quantified the response of the postsynaptic neuron to natural inputs with the Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and input firing rate fluctuations, per signal group. We computed the firing rate of a signal groups as the low-pass filter of the spike trains of its excitatory afferents, as follows:

τZdZμ(t)dt=Zμ(t) + jμSj(t), (34)

where Zμ(t) is the firing rate of the signal group μ at time t, filtered with a time constant τZ. The postsynaptic activity is also computed through a low-pass filter of its spike train, as follows:

τYdY(t)dt=Y(t) + Spost(t), (35)

where Y(t) is the activity of the postsynaptic neuron at time t filtered with a time constant τY. The correlation is then computed as follows:

Cμ=cov(Zμ,Y)σZμσY=(ZμZμ)(YY)(ZμZμ)2(YY)2, (36)

where cov(z, y) is the covariance between variables z and y, σz is the SD of variable z, and · represents time average.

Subsequently, we computed a performance index ΔC as the difference between the correlation measure for preferred (μ = 9) and nonpreferred (μ = 1) input signals, as follows:

ΔC=12(C9C1). (37)

Maximum positive performance index (ΔC = 1) means that the preferred signal group has maximum correlation (C9 = 1) while the nonpreferred signal group has maximum anticorrelation (C1 = −1), indicating that the postsynaptic neuron is responding solely to the preferred signal group. Consequently, ΔC = −1 indicates that the postsynaptic neuron is responding solely to the nonpreferred signal group. A flat response is indicated by ΔC = 0. Maximum ΔC (either positive or negative) is only achievable if the preferred (signal group 9) and nonpreferred (signal group 1) are strictly anticorrelated. This is unlikely given that all input signals are independent, and they will feature periods of coactivation. We define as best performance when ΔC = 0 for all inhibitory inputs active (control), ΔC = 1 (or ΔC > 0) for one inhibitory population inactive, and ΔC = –1 (or ΔC < 0) when the other inhibitory population is inactive. Parameters used for computing correlations are detailed in Table 5.

Table 5.

Simulation parameters for the correlation measure

Parameter Symbol Value Figures
Presynaptic time constant τZ 10 ms 4, 6, 8, 10, 11
Postsynaptic time constant τY 250 ms 4, 6, 8, 10, 11
Simulation time 30 min 4, 6, 8, 10, 11

The SD of the output firing rate (see Figs. 4C, 8C, 10C) was computed with a 1 s bin. The coefficient of variation of the interspike interval (CVISI; see Figs. 4C, 8C, 10C) was calculated as the SD of the interspike interval (ISI) of the output spike train divided by the mean ISI.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Postsynaptic response for a model with a single inhibitory population. A, Schematic of the circuit with a single inhibitory population (top). Presynaptic spikes were generated as point processes (pp), for both excitatory (red; 16 signals) and inhibitory (blue; 16 signals) inputs, and fed into a single-compartment LIF neuron. Schematic of the synaptic weight profiles (bottom). Average weight (y axis) for different input signals (x axis); preferred signal is pathway no. 9 (gray dashed line). B, Firing rate of the preferred, and two nonpreferred inputs and mean of all inputs (top row), excitatory and inhibitory input currents (middle row), and membrane potentials (bottom row), for control (left), decreased (middle), and increased (right) inhibition. Decreased (increased) inhibition lowered (raised) inhibitory firing rates by 10%, respectively. C, CVISI, and mean and SD of the postsynaptic firing rate in response to natural input for the 3 explored cases (top), and as a function of the inhibitory firing rate (bottom). Arrowheads indicate the analyzed cases. D, Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and excitatory input firing rates for different input signals for the three conditions in B (top). Correlation between output activity and preferred (continuous line) or nonpreferred (dashed line) inputs as a function of the inhibitory firing rate (bottom). E, Response to a pulse input in the phasic (left; first 50 ms) and tonic (right; last 50 ms) periods. Firing rate computed as the average number of spikes (for 100 trials) normalized by the bin size (50 ms). Each line corresponds to a different input strength: from light (low-amplitude pulse) to dark (high-amplitude pulse) colors. Insets, Tonic response for control and decreased inhibitory firing rates.

Figure 8.

Figure 8.

Postsynaptic response for the model with co-tuned and flat inhibitory populations. A, Schematic of the circuit with two inhibitory populations (top): I1, co-tuned population; I2, flat population. Presynaptic spikes were generated as point processes (pp) and fed into an LIF. Schematic of the synaptic weight profile (bottom). Average weight (y axis) for different input signals (x axis); preferred signal is pathway no. 9 (gray dashed line). B, Firing rate of the preferred and two nonpreferred inputs and mean of all inputs (top row), total excitatory current and inhibitory currents of both populations (middle row), and membrane potential (bottom row), for control (left), co-tuned (middle), and flat (right) population inactive. C, CVISI and postsynaptic firing rate (mean and SD) in response to natural input for the 3 cases (top). Output firing rate as a function of the firing rate of the (compensatory) active inhibitory population (bottom). Arrowheads indicate the analyzed cases where output rate is equal to 5 Hz (i.e., where the green and purple lines cross the black line). D, Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and excitatory input firing rates for different input signals for the three conditions in B. Correlation between output activity and preferred (continuous line) or nonpreferred (dashed line) as a function of the inhibitory firing rate of each inhibitory population (bottom). E, Response to a pulse input in the phasic (left; first 50 ms) and tonic (right; last 50 ms) periods. Firing rate computed as the average number of spikes (for 100 trials) normalized by bin size (50 ms). Each line corresponds to a different input strength: from light (low-amplitude pulse) to dark (high-amplitude pulse) colors. Insets, Tonic response for control firing rates.

Figure 10.

Figure 10.

Postsynaptic response for the model with co- and counter-tuned inhibitory populations. A, Schematic of the circuit with two inhibitory populations (top): I1, co-tuned; I2, counter-tuned population. Presynaptic spikes were generated as point processes (pp) and fed into an LIF. Synaptic weight profile (bottom). Average weight (y axis) for different input signals (x axis); preferred signal is pathway no. 9 (gray dashed line). B, Firing rate of the preferred and two nonpreferred inputs and mean of all inputs (top row), total excitatory current and inhibitory currents of both populations (middle row), and membrane potentials (bottom row), for control (left), co-tuned (middle), and counter-tuned (right) population inactive. C, CVISI and postsynaptic firing rate (mean and SD) in response to natural input for the 3 cases (top). Output firing rate as a function of the firing rate of the (compensatory) active inhibitory population (bottom). Arrowheads indicate the analyzed cases where output rate is equal to 5 Hz (i.e., where the green and purple lines cross the black line). D, Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and excitatory input firing rates for different input signals for the three conditions in B. Correlation between preferred (continuous line) or nonpreferred (dashed line) with the output activity as a function of the inhibitory firing rate of each inhibitory population (bottom). E, Response to a pulse input in the phasic (left; first 50 ms) and tonic (right; last 50 ms) periods. Firing rate computed as the average number of spikes (for 100 trials) normalized by the bin size (50 ms). Each line corresponds to a different input strength; from light (low-amplitude pulse) to dark (high-amplitude pulse) colors. Insets, Tonic response for control firing rates.

Software and code availability

Simulations were run in Fortran, compiled with Intel Fortran Compiler 19.0 on an Intel-based Linux computer (Debian 9; i9-9900× processor; 32 GB memory). Codes are available online (https://github.com/ejagnes/flexible_switch_2ISP/). Individual plots were generated with Gnuplot (http://www.gnuplot.info/). Figures were generated with Inkscape (https://inkscape.org/).

Results

To study the effect of interacting populations of feedforward inhibition, we investigated the response of a single postsynaptic LIF neuron receiving tuned excitatory inputs and inhibition from two distinct populations. Excitatory inputs were organized into a single population, subdivided into 16 signal groups of 200 excitatory afferents. Inhibitory inputs initially formed a single population, mirroring the excitatory subdivision, but with 50 afferents per group. Subsequently, we split the inhibitory inputs into two populations with 25 afferents per signal group (Fig. 2A; see Materials and Methods), allowing us to obtain two differently tuned populations (presumably types) of inhibition. Excitatory and inhibitory afferents belonging to the same group shared temporal fluctuations in firing rates, termed input patterns, even if they belonged to different populations. In our simulations, input patterns could either be natural or pulse. Natural inputs were generated through an inhomogeneous Poisson process based on a modified OU process (Fig. 2B,C), such that neurons of the same signal group also had temporally correlated firing patterns (Fig. 2C, top) (see Ujfalussy et al., 2018). The resulting long-tail distribution of ISIs (Fig. 2C, bottom) was similar to experimentally observed spike patterns in vivo (Reich et al., 2000; Hengen et al., 2013). We used this type of input to train inhibitory synapses via plasticity rules, and to quantify steady-state (average) postsynaptic responses.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Model details. A, Schematic of the input organization. An external signal (representing, e.g., sound) was delivered through three input populations (one excitatory and two inhibitory), with 16 input signals per population (representing, e.g., sound frequency). Each signal was simulated by 250 independent, but temporally correlated, spike trains (input afferents); 200 excitatory, and 50 inhibitory divided into two groups of 25. One postsynaptic neuron (black triangle) was the output of this system, simulated as a single-compartment LIF neuron. The firing rate of each of the inhibitory populations was modulated by a contextual cue (green and purple boxes). Excitatory and inhibitory input spike trains were generated as point processes (for details, see Materials and Methods). B, Natural input statistics. Raster plot (gray dots) of 800 neurons that take part in 4 signal groups (200 neurons per signal group), each with firing rate changing according to a modified OU process (colored lines; see Materials and Methods). C, Temporal autocorrelation (top) and distribution of the ISIs (bottom) of the presynaptic inputs. The autocorrelation of two groups are shown (green and pink), as well as the correlation between two different groups (black). Autocorrelation is computed as the Pearson coefficient with a delay (x axis). D, Pulse input schematic. A steplike increase in the firing rate of a given input group lasting 100 ms (top) with varying firing rates (grayscale). The postsynaptic response can be separated in phasic (first 50 ms), and tonic (last 50 ms), which reveals transient (middle) or persistent (bottom) types of response.

In the alternative pulse input regimen, we analyzed transient responses with 100-ms-long pulses of varying amplitudes (Vogels et al., 2011). Pulses were delivered through a single signal group of excitatory and inhibitory afferents, whereas all other groups remained at baseline firing rate (see Materials and Methods). Responses were quantified according to postsynaptic firing rates during the first (phasic) and last (tonic) 50 ms stimulation (Fig. 2D), averaged over 100 trials. Separating responses in phasic and tonic allowed us to discriminate changes in output because of the input onset (Fig. 2D, transient), when excitation outweighs inhibition (phasic period), and slower integration of the pulse (Fig. 2D, persistent), when excitation and inhibition are balanced (tonic period). The transient period is best captured by limiting the phasic window to 50 ms, and we thus set the tonic window to the same duration to keep both window sizes symmetric.

Learning was implemented via three distinct inhibitory plasticity rules (Fig. 3), in three different combinations. We first implemented a Hebbian rule, which potentiated synaptic weights for coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes and depressed them for sole presynaptic spikes (Vogels et al., 2011) (Fig. 3A), in one of the two inhibitory populations, while the synapses of the other inhibitory and the excitatory population remained fixed. This learning rule has previously been shown to generate inhibitory weight profiles that mirror the excitatory synaptic weight profiles of a postsynaptic neuron, imposing a firing rate fixed point (target; Fig. 3A) by balancing excitation and inhibition (Vogels et al., 2011), supporting similar experimental findings in mouse auditory cortex (D'amour and Froemke, 2015). Next, we implemented the Hebbian plasticity rule in one of them and a scaling plasticity rule (Fig. 3B) in the other population. The homeostatic scaling rule upregulates or downregulates the entire synapse population to reach a predetermined target firing rate. Notably, this plasticity rule was purely local, taking only synaptic weights and postsynaptic firing rate into account, similarly to the experimentally observed scaling of inhibitory synapses (Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006; Zhong et al., 2018). Finally, we also implemented an experimentally inspired (Woodin et al., 2003; Ormond and Woodin, 2009, 2011) anti-Hebbian rule in the second inhibitory population (Fig. 3C). Unlike its Hebbian counterpart, the anti-Hebbian rule leads to indefinite increases in the firing rate of the postsynaptic neuron because correlated activity decreases synaptic weights (only sole presynaptic spikes increase synaptic weights; see Materials and Methods). The anti-Hebbian plasticity rule is thus unstable (Fig. 3C, middle). We found that we could prevent catastrophe without incorporating additional, complex dynamics by using a variable learning rate for the anti-Hebbian rule. For simplicity, we decreased the learning rate exponentially over time (Fig. 3C, right), but this could also be achieved through top-down control (see Discussion).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Synaptic plasticity models. A, Hebbian plasticity rule. Left, Spike-timing dependency. Δw indicates level of synaptic change, and Δt indicates interval between presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes. Coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes elicit positive changes, whereas presynaptic spikes alone elicit negative changes in synaptic strength (Vogels et al., 2011). Right, Synaptic changes (Δw) as a function of postsynaptic firing rate. When the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate is above the target rate, inhibitory synapses increase in weight and, as a consequence, the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate decreases. The opposite happens for when the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate is lower than the target rate (Vogels et al., 2011) (see Materials and Methods). B, Synaptic scaling rule. Changes in synaptic strength (Δw) as a function of the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate. When the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate is lower than a lower bound threshold, inhibitory synapses decrease, proportionally to their current strength. When the postsynaptic neuron's firing rate is higher than an upper bound threshold, inhibitory synapses increase. Because of the lower and upper bounds, there is a region with no change around the target rate. C, Anti-Hebbian plasticity rule. Left, Spike-timing dependency. Presynaptic spikes elicit positive changes, whereas coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes elicit negative changes in synaptic weights. Middle, Changes in synaptic efficacy (Δw) as a function of the postsynaptic firing rate. The target rate of anti-Hebbian plasticity rule is unstable. Right, Evolution of the learning rate of the anti-Hebbian plasticity model. Because of its unstable nature, we set the learning rate to decay exponentially over time.

Shaping and modulating a single inhibitory population

To begin, we constructed a standard cortical circuit motif with one excitatory and one inhibitory population (Diesmann et al., 1999; Vogels and Abbott, 2009; Vogels et al., 2011; Clopath et al., 2016; Weber and Sprekeler, 2018) (Fig. 4A, top). We followed previous work showing that the Hebbian plasticity rule (Fig. 3A) changes inhibitory synapses to provide precisely balanced inputs (Vogels et al., 2011), such that both excitatory and inhibitory weight profiles are shaped according to previous experimental observations (Froemke et al., 2007) (Fig. 4A, bottom). Afferent synaptic weights of the excitatory population were set according to a receptive field tuning curve (see Materials and Methods) while the efficacy of inhibitory afferents was governed by a synaptic plasticity rule for a period of 30 min. Learning was established by a Hebbian plasticity rule that potentiated synapses for coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes and depressed synapses for sole presynaptic spikes (Vogels et al., 2011) (Fig. 3A). The plasticity rule was set to allow average postsynaptic firing rates of ∼5 Hz for natural inputs (Fig. 4B,C). We then tested the effect of changing the gain of all inhibitory afferents, while keeping their synaptic strengths fixed, by modulating their firing rates, from 50% to 150% of control rates. This change of input balance translated into changes in output rates (Fig. 4C, bottom) and spike patterns (Fig. 4B, middle and right). When inhibition was equal or larger than excitation, the output was largely uncorrelated to any given input signal (Fig. 4D, top). When inhibitory firing rates fell below 90% of the control condition, the output first began to correlate with the preferred input signal. When inhibition became even weaker, the correlations increased, and even nonpreferred signals were articulated in the postsynaptic firing patterns (Fig. 4D, bottom).

Transient presynaptic activity pulses caused strong phasic responses in the balance state when they were delivered through the afferents of the preferred inputs (Fig. 4E, top row). Stimuli from nonpreferred afferents were largely ignored. This discriminability between transients of low- or high-amplitude pulses decreased when inhibition was downregulated (Fig. 4E, middle row) such that pulse stimuli from all signal groups caused a response. Increased inhibition, on the other hand, completely abolished transient responses to nonpreferred afferents (Fig. 4E, bottom row). In all 3 cases (balanced control, weak and strong inhibition), the postsynaptic neuron elicited most of its spikes within the phasic period of the total 100 ms input step (Fig. 4E). This indicates that strong postsynaptic responses are mostly driven by the onset of the presynaptic stimulation rather than the stimulus being integrated slowly over time, a consequence of the precise balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Vogels et al., 2011).

Thus, a single inhibitory population, even with tuned weights, could not affect the postsynaptic receptive field via only the modulation of the inhibitory firing rate. To test whether an additional inhibitory population would allow for more sophisticated control of postsynaptic activity, we constructed a model with different plasticity rules, which were applied to two different populations of inhibitory inputs.

Plasticity shapes inhibitory weight profiles and receptive fields

To study how plasticity can shape the emergence of distinct synaptic weight profiles, we incorporated inhibitory synaptic plasticity mechanisms into a model with two inhibitory populations. We started with a symmetric Hebbian plasticity rule in one of the two inhibitory populations: coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes potentiated synapses, whereas sole presynaptic spikes depressed synapses (Vogels et al., 2011) (Fig. 3A). The synapses of the excitatory and the other inhibitory population remained fixed (Fig. 5). Simulations began with tuned excitatory synapses and flat inhibitory weight profiles in both inhibitory populations (Fig. 5A).

After 30 min of stimulation with natural inputs (compare Fig. 2B), inhibitory weights of the plastic population stabilized (Fig. 5D–G). Whether the target firing rate (Fig. 5B,C) was reached depended on the synaptic strength of the other, static population of inhibitory synapses. If the static weights were weak, the plastic synapses increased their strength until the target firing rate was reached (Fig. 5C). If the static population provided strong inhibition (and thus kept postsynaptic firing below the target rate), weights from the plastic population would eventually vanish, before the target firing rate could be reached (Fig. 5C,G). Consequently, the shape of the static population determined the shape of the plastic population (Fig. 5D,E). As expected, the input/output correlation of the postsynaptic responses followed the effective synaptic weight profile (Fig. 6A, compare Fig. 5E), with distinct input/output correlations for turning either of the populations off (Fig. 6B). The Hebbian plasticity rule, because of the strengthening of synapses for coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes, thus complemented additional inhibitory synaptic connectivity in establishing a state of detailed balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Postsynaptic response after stabilization of synapses from one population. A, Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and excitatory input firing rates with both inhibitory populations active. Color code as in Figure 5A. B, Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and excitatory input firing rates for different signals with both inhibitory populations active, Population 1 inactive, and Population 2 inactive. Three examples are shown.

Hebbian and scaling plasticity rules

Next, we introduced plasticity to the second population of inhibitory afferents. We tested two different rules, beginning with a homeostatic plasticity rule, which (multiplicatively) scaled synapses down and additively potentiated synapses so that a fixed point for the postsynaptic firing rates was reached (Fig. 3B; see Materials and Methods). With the homeostatic rule coactive, the Hebbian synapses, connections changing according to the Hebbian plasticity rule, developed a co-tuned profile from initially random weights (Fig. 7A, top; Fig. 7C, left), while the synapses following the scaling rule collapsed to a single value (Fig. 7A, bottom; Fig. 7C, right; for mathematical analysis, see Materials and Methods). Consequently, the postsynaptic neuron received precisely balanced inputs (Fig. 7B). The two plasticity rules cooperate to impose an average postsynaptic activity, and thus naturally work in harmony. Importantly, both plasticity rules are stable regarding their interaction with postsynaptic dynamics (see Materials and Methods), and as such, not sensitive to initial conditions, and relatively robust to parameter choices, as long as the postsynaptic activity fixed point (see Materials and Methods) imposed by both plasticity rules match.

We then studied the effects of differentially modulating the activity of the two inhibitory populations after their tuning curves had been established by the plasticity rules described above. First, we focused on the interaction of the connectivity created by Hebbian and the scaling plasticity rules (Fig. 8A, top; compare Fig. 7), that is, a co-tuned population and a flat population (Fig. 8A, bottom). We compared the output of the neuron in three scenarios: with both inhibitory populations active (control); with the co-tuned population inactive; and with the flat population inactive (Fig. 8B–E).

With both populations active, the input/output correlation was indistinguishable from a model with one, homogeneous inhibitory population (Fig. 8D, top; compare Fig. 4D, top), because the two populations (co-tuned and flat) created the same effect as the single (co-tuned) population. Deactivating either population had pronounced effects on postsynaptic responses. We increased the firing rate of the active inhibitory population to maintain the same average output firing rate of 5 Hz in the modulated conditions (Fig. 8C, top; where the green and purple lines cross the black horizontal line in Fig. 8C, bottom). Fluctuations in firing rate and membrane potential increased in both cases (Fig. 8B,C). When the co-tuned inhibitory population was turned off, the emerging imbalance of excitation and inhibition unmasked the excitatory tuning curve, thus increasing the chance of action potential generation when preferred signal populations were active (Fig. 8B, middle). The compensatory increase in the activity of the flat population further quenched nonpreferred excitatory signals, leading to anticorrelated responses for nonpreferred input signals (Fig. 8D, purple), reflecting the lack of postsynaptic firing during periods in which nonpreferred signals were active (Fig. 8B, middle). The opposite effect could be observed when the flat population was deactivated. In this case, the lack of inhibition for nonpreferred signals gave rise to input/output correlations for nonpreferred signals, while preferred signals saw no response (Fig. 8B, right; Fig. 8D, green).

Transient responses, when compared with the unmodulated control case (Fig. 8E, top), were substantially increased for preferred inputs when the co-tuned population was deactivated, and the response to nonpreferred signals was completely diminished (Fig. 8E, middle). When the flat population was deactivated, the postsynaptic neuron responded strongly to the nonpreferred inputs, but not to preferred inputs (Fig. 8E, bottom). Interestingly, modulating either of the inhibitory populations had similar effects on the postsynaptic response both in phasic and tonic periods, in contrast with the unmodulated control case, in which only phasic responses were postsynaptically elicited (Fig. 8E). Again, this reflects the state of balance between excitation and inhibition in the unmodulated control case, which only reveals transient input dynamics.

Hebbian and anti-Hebbian plasticity rules

Instead of a purely homeostatic scaling rule, we also tried an experimentally inspired (Woodin et al., 2003; Ormond and Woodin, 2009, 2011) anti-Hebbian rule in the second inhibitory population (Fig. 3C). The anti-Hebbian rule, unlike the Hebbian, decreases synaptic weights for correlated activity, and sole presynaptic spikes increase synaptic weights. Such a rule can only either indefinitely increase the firing rate of the postsynaptic neuron or decrease it to zero (see Materials and Methods). We accounted for the unstable nature of the anti-Hebbian plasticity rule (Fig. 3C, middle) by controlling its learning rate, such that it decreased exponentially over time (Fig. 3C, right). With both Hebbian and anti-Hebbian rules active, initially random weights evolved into co-tuned and counter-tuned synaptic weight profiles (Fig. 9A). As learning slowed down because of the decreasing learning rate, the anti-Hebbian synapses (connections changing according to the anti-Hebbian plasticity rule) stabilized, and Hebbian synapses ceased to change once the target firing rate was reached (Fig. 9B,C). The anti-Hebbian plasticity rule, differently from the Hebbian one, is sensitive to initial conditions and choice of parameters because of its intrinsic instability (see Materials and Methods). For example, different initialization of inhibitory weights requires distinct decay times for the learning rate (data not shown). Additional complexity in the formulation of the plasticity rule may solve these instability problems (Discussion).

Postsynaptic dynamics with two inhibitory populations with tuning that resulted from the combination of the Hebbian and the anti-Hebbian plasticity rules (Fig. 10A), that is, co-tuned and counter-tuned populations, were similar to that with co-tuned and flat inhibitory populations. In the unmodulated balanced state, output behavior is near identical to previous results (Fig. 10B–D, control). The main distinction between the models with counter-tuned or flat inhibitory profiles is how they complemented the co-tuned inhibitory currents: the flat inhibition produced currents that tracked the co-tuned inhibitory currents, whereas counter-tuned inhibition produced inhibitory currents that were largely uncorrelated to the co-tuned inhibitory currents (Fig. 10B, left; compare with Fig. 8B, left).

When either the co- or the counter-tuned inhibitory populations were inactivated, fluctuations in both firing rate and membrane potential increased considerably (Fig. 10B, middle and right). As before (Fig. 8), we adjusted the firing rate of the active inhibitory population so that the average output firing rate was 5 Hz in all conditions (Fig. 10C, top; crossing between green/purple line and the black horizontal line in Fig. 10C, bottom). Deactivation of the co-tuned population resulted in positive correlation between postsynaptic activity and preferred signals, and negative correlation between output and nonpreferred signals (Fig. 10D, purple). For transient stimulation, there was no discernible difference to the model with flat inhibition in the control state (Fig. 10E, top).

Turning off counter-tuned inhibition (Fig. 10B–E) also had similar results in the postsynaptic response as turning off the flat inhibition (compare Fig. 8B–E); that is, nonpreferred input produced output activity with positive correlation (Fig. 10D, green) and strong postsynaptic activity for transient activation (Fig. 10E, bottom). Unlike before, turning off co-tuned inhibition produced elevated firing rate responses also for transient stimuli from signals directly neighboring the preferred input (Fig. 10E, middle row, compare with Fig. 8E, middle).

Quantitative differences of inhibitory profiles

For a better understanding of the differences between the three conditions studied here (one inhibitory population, co-tuned & flat, and co- & counter-tuned), we compared different modulation schemes quantitatively. We introduced the parameter ΔC=0.5(CprefCnon-pref), that is, 50% of the difference in input/output correlation between preferred, Cpref, and nonpreferred, Cnon-pref, signals (see Materials and Methods). Ideally, the sensory system should present three distinct responses for the three different modulatory conditions, which are captured by different values of ΔC. With unmodulated input (control), the output neuron should present uncorrelated activity with all input groups, and thus ΔC0. Modulated inputs (by decreasing the activity of either of the inhibitory populations) should correlate preferred (for one inhibitory inactive) and nonpreferred (for the other population inactive) to the output activity. This results in ΔC > 0 for correlated output/preferred signals, and ΔC < 0 for correlated output/nonpreferred signals.

In the control condition, we observed similar ΔC0 in all models (Fig. 11A, gray), reflecting low levels of correlation between output and input signals (Fig. 11B, top). With downregulated inhibition, ΔC increased slightly in the model with one homogeneous inhibitory population. ΔC increased more considerably in a two-population model in which the co-tuned population was inactive (Fig. 11A, purple), confirming an increased correlation between preferred signal and output (Fig. 11B, middle). When the flat or the counter-tuned inhibitory populations were inactivated, we observed postsynaptic responses even to nonpreferred input signals (Fig. 11B, bottom), which led to negative ΔC (Fig. 11A, green). Inactivating the flat inhibitory population resulted in a slightly better discrimination: larger positive ΔC (Fig. 11A, purple) and larger negative ΔC (Fig. 11A, green).

Figure 11.

Figure 11.

Comparison of postsynaptic responses receiving co-tuned & flat or co-tuned & counter-tuned inhibitory populations. A, Performance index as the difference in input/output correlation between preferred and nonpreferred signal. Ideal outcome is ΔC = 0 for control case (gray), ΔC > 0 for co-tuned population inactive (purple), and ΔC < 0 for flat or counter-tuned populations inactive (green). We added the values for a single inhibitory population with control (gray), weak (purple), and strong (green) inhibitory inputs for comparison. B, Pearson correlation between postsynaptic firing rate and excitatory input firing rates for different signal indices from Figures 4D, 8D, and 10D, replotted for reference. C, Signals recovered in the pulse input paradigm. Signals represented are calculated as the percentage of signal afferents that activate the postsynaptic neuron with more than half the spikes of the maximum response for the 3 cases considered for the circuits analyzed together. D, Normalized phasic response to a pulse input of 40 Hz, from Figures 4E, 8E, and 10E, replotted for reference. Horizontal line indicates 50% of maximum response.

To compare pulse responses of the three models, we quantified which input signal groups elicited a substantial response to a pulse signal. We defined the number of signals recovered (Fig. 11C) as the number of responses with >50% of the maximum postsynaptic firing rate (Fig. 11D). The single inhibitory population model could only produce responses to preferred input signals, while co-modulation of two inhibitory populations could promote responses to nonpreferred input signals, as well. Counter-tuned population achieved better (i.e., broader) postsynaptic control than flat inhibition (Fig. 11C).

The addition of a second population of inhibitory inputs thus gives rise to a more flexible response to varying stimuli. In summary, our results shed light on the role of the many types of interneurons in cortical areas (Markram et al., 2004; Jiang et al., 2015; Gouwens et al., 2019), and show the benefits of combining different biologically inspired plasticity rules in neuronal networks.

Discussion

We investigated how several distinctly tuned inhibitory connectivity profiles emerge through biologically reasonable plasticity rules and how they interact with a tuned excitatory connectivity profile in a receptive field-like paradigm. We found that the two aspects of selective attention (enhancing response to targets and suppressing the response to distractors) were implemented in our model by two types of disinhibition. Our results indicate a simple neuronal mechanism to help disentangle (or bind) parallel sensory input streams and may represent a step toward understanding the neural basis of intricate behaviors, such as the “cocktail party effect,” focusing on a single voice in a crowded, cacophonous place.

Modulation of receptive field response

Our findings fit well with recent experimental results showing that pyramidal neurons in sensory areas of the cortex change their response to external stimuli depending on the context of the signal or attentional state (Bathellier et al., 2012; Ruff and Cohen, 2014, 2019; Benjamin et al., 2019; McClure and Polack, 2019). For example, principal neurons in macaque V4 respond to monochrome images of varying hues with variable response amplitude that is consistent with specific color tuning. However, the preferred color response of the neurons changes when naturally colored images are shown (Benjamin et al., 2019). In macaque V1, principal neurons can change the preferred orientation of visual stimuli when a pure tone is played alongside the visual stimulation (McClure and Polack, 2019). In the framework of our model, such a change in preference could be explained with differential input to the two inhibitory populations or by changes in their gains through contextual neuromodulation. Similarly, up to 20% of neurons in all areas of the mouse visual system (Billeh et al., 2019) were recently shown to change their preferred orientation according to the (spatial and temporal) frequency of the drifting gratings used in the experiments. In some cases, neuronal responses were shown to shift from their preferred to their nonpreferred input signals (a “flip” in response), similar to what we see in our simulations. These effects could also be explained by temporal fluctuations in the interaction of the two inhibitory populations, and the concurrent changes in transient responses of our model.

Neuron types

The architecture of our model maps easily onto the neocortical microcircuit (Markram et al., 2004; Jiang et al., 2015). Co-tuned inhibition, for example, may originate from PV+ interneurons. As the main source of inhibition to pyramidal cells, PV+ interneurons target postsynaptic neurons with similar preferred orientation (Wilson et al., 2012), and activation of these neurons leads to broadened selectivity (Wilson et al., 2012; but see Lee et al., 2012). Flat or counter-tuned inhibition may arrive from SOM+ interneurons with their less selective connectivity patterns (Wilson et al., 2012). This interpretation is also in line with recent evidence suggesting that top-down visual attention relies on local inhibitory circuitry in primary visual cortex (Zhou et al., 2014). In this scheme, PV+ and SOM+ neurons inhibit pyramidal cells, whereas vasoactive intestinal peptide-positive neurons suppress other inhibitory interneurons, acting as a source of disinhibition. Direct manipulation of SOM+, PV+, and vasoactive intestinal peptide-positive neurons confirms these respective roles in inhibition and disinhibition in both visual (Fu et al., 2014) and auditory cortices (Pi et al., 2013; Kuchibhotla et al., 2017). Additionally, Zhou et al. (2014) reported that vasoactive intestinal peptide-positive neurons received excitatory top-down inputs from the rodent cingulate cortex, leading to a narrow selectivity profile of pyramidal cells when cingulate inputs are active and broad tuning when cingulate cortex is silent. This is analogous to deactivating the co-tuned population in our simulations. Finally, blocking cortical inhibition reduces the stimulus selectivity of cortical neurons (Sillito, 1979; Sillito et al., 1980; but see Nelson et al., 1994).

Balance between excitatory and inhibitory inputs

In our model, we aimed for precise balance of excitation and inhibition, by way of a Hebbian-like inhibitory plasticity rule (Vogels et al., 2011), and accordant with evidence of excitatory and inhibitory co-tuning in cat visual cortex (Anderson et al., 2000), rodent auditory cortex (Wehr and Zador, 2003; Froemke et al., 2007; Zhou et al., 2014) and rodent hippocampus (Bhatia et al., 2019), and temporal correlations in neighboring excitatory and inhibitory synapses (Okun and Lampl, 2008). Consistent with earlier work, we could modulate the efficacy of a single inhibitory population to enhance the output correlation with the preferred input (Vogels and Abbott, 2009; Kremkow et al., 2010), but the flexibility of the control mechanism was very limited and nonpreferred signals never evoked faithful responses.

Inhibitory synaptic plasticity

To explore how different inhibitory synaptic populations could form and interact, we split the inhibitory afferents into two populations and implemented a Hebbian-like inhibitory plasticity rule (Vogels et al., 2011; D'amour and Froemke, 2015) in one population that was coactive with either a homeostatic scaling (Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006; Zhong et al., 2018) or an anti-Hebbian (Woodin et al., 2003; Ormond and Woodin, 2009, 2011) plasticity rule. Woodin et al. (2003) recorded postsynaptic currents with the postsynaptic neuron clamped at a voltage below the reversal potential of chloride, effectively making a GABAergic synapse excitatory. Plasticity at those synapses changed the reversal potential of chloride toward a more depolarized value, making them stronger in those artificial conditions. However, once the voltage clamp is released, the inhibitory currents would be weaker than before learning because of the more depolarized chloride reversal potential that is closer to the neuron's resting membrane potential and thus exerts a smaller driving force (Hennequin et al., 2017, their Fig. 1C). These results suggest a plasticity rule that is Hebbian if the postsynaptic neuron's voltage is artificially kept below the reversal potential of chloride, but anti-Hebbian in normal conditions. The scaling plasticity rule acted locally but squeezed the distribution of all synaptic strengths to a narrow regimen, providing a parsimonious explanation for the untuned, blanket inhibition often encountered in experiments (Karnani et al., 2014), and providing easy means for modulating postsynaptic responses independently of the presynaptically tuned weight profiles. The anti-Hebbian rule was naturally unstable; that is, it could lead to infinite strengthening of weights and thus silent networks. Our implementation reinforces this outcome because inhibitory inputs are always active.

It is unclear how biological circuits would avoid such catastrophe, but in our model we could balance the effect of the two opposing rules and remain at plausible levels of postsynaptic activity by including a modulatory term that controlled the learning rate of the anti-Hebbian plasticity rule. While this mimics some of the observed modulatory control of plasticity through other neuronal types (Abraham and Bear, 1996; Froemke et al., 2007; Abs et al., 2018; Aljadeff et al., 2019), the reality is likely more complex, and possibly relies on finely orchestrated interaction of several different plasticity rules (Zenke et al., 2015; Hennequin et al., 2017), and their learning rates. Additionally, if the inhibitory neurons are driven laterally by excitatory neurons that lack excitatory recurrence, a form of anti-Hebbian plasticity is also stable (Hendin et al., 1997). No matter what form the ultimate mechanism may take, it is unlikely that it will affect the generality of our results.

Parallels to artificial neural networks

Interestingly, artificial networks have been shown to develop similar receptive field profiles to the ones explored here when they are trained to solve multiple tasks (Yang et al., 2019). Yang et al. (2019) have shown that clusters of neurons can acquire co-tuned or flat connectivity, which are controlled by context-encoding signals. These results hint at the possibility that biological and artificial systems may use similar strategies to solve context-dependent filtering tasks.

Additional biological complexity

To explore the interaction between two distinct inhibitory plasticity rules without confounds, we made the simplifying assumption that excitatory synapses would remain fixed (but see Litwin-Kumar and Doiron, 2014; Zenke et al., 2015; Clopath et al., 2016). Obviously, inhibitory plasticity rules do interact with multiple additional rules and constraints (e.g., excitatory or modulatory synaptic plasticity). Similarly, our model only considered a single postsynaptic neuron, with no feedback or lateral connectivity, which is thought to play an important role in cortical feature selectivity (Ahmed et al., 1994), and was theoretically shown to provide the means for multiplicative and additive modulation of receptive fields, and surround suppression (Litwin-Kumar et al., 2016). Other common features of sensory areas are the adaptation of postsynaptic responses to repetitive stimuli presentation (Kohn and Movshon, 2004) and the quick reshape of postsynaptic responses toward the attended stimuli (Fritz et al., 2003). These features may be explored in our model by including, for example, short-term plasticity (Tsodyks and Markram, 1997) in both inhibitory and excitatory synapses. Additional types of activity modulation may also reveal new features of networks with multiple types of inhibition and synaptic plasticity, such as induction of phase locking after inhibitory plasticity, as seen in mouse barrel cortex (Lourenço et al., 2020). Finally, other possible functions beyond simple input filtering, such as multiplexing or amplifying temporally varying signal streams (Naud and Sprekeler, 2018; Hertäg and Sprekeler, 2019), one-shot learning (Nicola and Clopath, 2019) must be considered. Our work only lays the groundwork for studies of multiple distinct plasticity rules in larger networks, with more complex excitatory-inhibitory interaction (Holmgren and Zilberter, 2001; Haas et al., 2006; Hennequin et al., 2017; Lourenço et al., 2020).

In conclusion, we predict that various GABAergic interneurons in the same cortical region must obey a range of different inhibitory synaptic plasticity rules, to restore or alter neuronal stimulus selectivity as appropriate and necessary. Such evidence would inform the theoretical framework presented here, and in turn inspire future computational modeling.

Footnotes

This work was supported by Leverhulme Trust Research Project Grant RPG-2016-446 to E.J.A., Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society Sir Henry Dale Fellowship WT100000 to E.J.A. and T.P.V., Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellowship 214316/Z/18/Z to E.J.A. and T.P.V., ERC Consolidator Grant SYNAPSEEK to E.J.A. and T.P.V., and University of Oxford Claredon Scholarship to A.I.L. We thank the members of the Vogels group, and particularly Georgia Christodoulou and William Podlaski, for fruitful discussions.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Abraham WC, Bear MF (1996) Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci 19:126–130. 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)80018-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abs E, Poorthuis RB, Apelblat D, Muhammad K, Pardi MB, Enke L, Kushinsky D, Pu DL, Eizinger MF, Conzelmann KK, Spiegel I, Letzkus JJ (2018) Learning-related plasticity in dendrite-targeting layer 1 interneurons. Neuron 100:684–699. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ahmed B, Anderson JC, Douglas RJ, Martin KA, Nelson JC (1994) Polyneuronal innervation of spiny stellate neurons in cat visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 341:39–49. 10.1002/cne.903410105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aljadeff J, D'amour J, Field RE, Froemke RC, Clopath C (2019) Cortical credit assignment by Hebbian, neuromodulatory and inhibitory plasticity. arXiv:1911.00307. [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson JC, Kennedy H, Martin KA (2011) Pathways of attention: synaptic relationships of frontal eye field to V4, lateral intraparietal cortex, and area 46 in macaque monkey. J Neurosci 31:10872–10881. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0622-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anderson JS, Carandini M, Ferster D (2000) Orientation tuning of input conductance, excitation, and inhibition in cat primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 84:909–926. 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.909 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baluch F, Itti L (2011) Mechanisms of top-down attention. Trends Neurosci 34:210–224. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barron HC, Vogels TP, Emir UE, Makin TR, O'Shea J, Clare S, Jbabdi S, Dolan RJ, Behrens TE (2016) Unmasking latent inhibitory connections in human cortex to reveal dormant cortical memories. Neuron 90:191–203. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bathellier B, Ushakova L, Rumpel S (2012) Discrete neocortical dynamics predict behavioral categorization of sounds. Neuron 76:435–449. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Benchenane K, Tiesinga PH, Battaglia FP (2011) Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: a gateway to memory and attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:475–485. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Benjamin AS, Ramkumar P, Fernandes H, Smith MA, Kording KP (2019) Hue tuning curves in V4 change with visual context 780478. bioRxiv. 10.1101/780478. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bhatia A, Moza S, Bhalla US (2019) Precise excitation-inhibition balance controls gain and timing in the hippocampus. eLife 8:e43415 10.7554/eLife.43415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Billeh YN, Iyer R, Wahle IA, Caldejon S, Durand S, Groblewski PA, Larkin JD, Lecoq J, Williford A, Mihalas S, Arkhipov A, de Vries SE (2019) Widespread presence of direction-reversing neurons in the mouse visual system 826701. bioRxiv. 10.1101/826701. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chiu CQ, Barberis A, Higley MJ (2019) Preserving the balance: diverse forms of long-term GABAergic synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 20:272–281. 10.1038/s41583-019-0141-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Clopath C, Vogels TP, Froemke RC, Sprekeler H (2016) Receptive field formation by interacting excitatory and inhibitory synaptic plasticity 066589. bioRxiv. 10.1101/066589. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. D'amour JA, Froemke RC (2015) Inhibitory and excitatory spike-timing-dependent plasticity in the auditory cortex. Neuron 86:514–528. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Diesmann M, Gewaltig MO, Aertsen A (1999) Stable propagation of synchronous spiking in cortical neural networks. Nature 402:529–533. 10.1038/990101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Douglas RJ, Martin KA (1991) A functional microcircuit for cat visual cortex. J Physiol 440:735–769. 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Estebanez L, Férézou I, Ego-Stengel V, Shulz DE (2018) Representation of tactile scenes in the rodent barrel cortex. Neuroscience 368:81–94. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.08.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ferster D. (1986) Orientation selectivity of synaptic potentials in neurons of cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 6:1284–1301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fritz J, Shamma S, Elhilali M, Klein D (2003) Rapid task-related plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 6:1216–1223. 10.1038/nn1141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Froemke RC, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE (2007) A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive field plasticity. Nature 450:425–429. 10.1038/nature06289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fu Y, Tucciarone JM, Espinosa JS, Sheng N, Darcy DP, Nicoll RA, Huang ZJ, Stryker MP (2014) A cortical circuit for gain control by behavioral state. Cell 156:1139–1152. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.01.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gouwens NW, Sorensen SA, Berg J, Lee C, Jarsky T, Ting J, Sunkin SM, Feng D, Anastassiou CA, Barkan E, Bickley K, Blesie N, Braun T, Brouner K, Budzillo A, Caldejon S, Casper T, Castelli D, Chong P, Crichton K, et al. (2019) Classification of electrophysiological and morphological neuron types in the mouse visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 22:1182–1195. 10.1038/s41593-019-0417-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Haas JS, Nowotny T, Abarbanel HD (2006) Spike-timing-dependent plasticity of inhibitory synapses in the entorhinal cortex. J Neurophysiol 96:3305–3313. 10.1152/jn.00551.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Haley MS, Fontanini A, Maffei A (2016) Laminar-and target-specific amygdalar inputs in rat primary gustatory cortex. J Neurosci 36:2623–2637. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3224-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW (2009) Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature 461:941–946. 10.1038/nature08499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hendin O, Horn D, Tsodyks MV (1997) The role of inhibition in an associative memory model of the olfactory bulb. J Comput Neurosci 4:173–182. 10.1023/A:1008895429790 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hengen KB, Lambo ME, Van Hooser SD, Katz DB, Turrigiano GG (2013) Firing rate homeostasis in visual cortex of freely behaving rodents. Neuron 80:335–342. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hennequin G, Agnes EJ, Vogels TP (2017) Inhibitory plasticity: balance, control, and codependence. Annu Rev Neurosci 40:557–579. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hertäg L, Sprekeler H (2019) Amplifying the redistribution of somato-dendritic inhibition by the interplay of three interneuron types. PLoS Comput Biol 15:e1006999. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Holmgren CD, Zilberter Y (2001) Coincident spiking activity induces long-term changes in inhibition of neocortical pyramidal cells. J Neurosci 21:8270–8277. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-08270.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jiang X, Shen S, Cadwell CR, Berens P, Sinz F, Ecker AS, Patel S, Tolias AS (2015) Principles of connectivity among morphologically defined cell types in adult neocortex. Science 350:aac9462. 10.1126/science.aac9462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jouhanneau JS, Kremkow J, Poulet JF (2018) Single synaptic inputs drive high-precision action potentials in parvalbumin expressing GABA-ergic cortical neurons in vivo. Nat Commun 9:1540. 10.1038/s41467-018-03995-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Karnani MM, Agetsuma M, Yuste R (2014) A blanket of inhibition: functional inferences from dense inhibitory connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol 26:96–102. 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kohn A, Movshon JA (2004) Adaptation changes the direction tuning of macaque MT neurons. Nat Neurosci 7:764–772. 10.1038/nn1267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kremkow J, Aertsen A, Kumar A (2010) Gating of signal propagation in spiking neural networks by balanced and correlated excitation and inhibition. J Neurosci 30:15760–15768. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kuchibhotla KV, Gill JV, Lindsay GW, Papadoyannis ES, Field RE, Sten Hindmarsh TA, Miller KD, Froemke RC (2017) Parallel processing by cortical inhibition enables context-dependent behavior. Nat Neurosci 20:62–71. 10.1038/nn.4436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lee SH, Kwan AC, Zhang S, Phoumthipphavong V, Flannery JG, Masmanidis SC, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Zhang F, Boyden ES, Deisseroth K, Dan Y (2012) Activation of specific interneurons improves V1 feature selectivity and visual perception. Nature 488:379–383. 10.1038/nature11312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Letzkus JJ, Wolff SB, Lüthi A (2015) Disinhibition, a circuit mechanism for associative learning and memory. Neuron 88:264–276. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B (2014) Formation and maintenance of neuronal assemblies through synaptic plasticity. Nat Commun 5:5319. 10.1038/ncomms6319 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Litwin-Kumar A, Rosenbaum R, Doiron B (2016) Inhibitory stabilization and visual coding in cortical circuits with multiple interneuron subtypes. J Neurophysiol 115:1399–1409. 10.1152/jn.00732.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lourenço J, De Stasi AM, Deleuze C, Bigot M, Pazienti A, Aguirre A, Giugliano M, Ostojic S, Bacci A (2020) Modulation of coordinated activity across cortical layers by plasticity of inhibitory synapses. Cell Rep 30:630–641. 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.12.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Maffei A. (2017) Fifty shades of inhibition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 43:43–47. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Markram H, Toledo-Rodriguez M, Wang Y, Gupta A, Silberberg G, Wu C (2004) Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:793–807. 10.1038/nrn1519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McClure JP Jr, Polack PO (2019) Pure tones modulate the representation of orientation and direction in the primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 121:2202–2214. 10.1152/jn.00069.2019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Naud R, Sprekeler H (2018) Sparse bursts optimize information transmission in a multiplexed neural code. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:E6329–E6338. 10.1073/pnas.1720995115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nelson S, Toth L, Sheth B, Sur M (1994) Orientation selectivity of cortical neurons during intracellular blockade of inhibition. Science 265:774–777. 10.1126/science.8047882 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nicola W, Clopath C (2019) A diversity of interneurons and Hebbian plasticity facilitate rapid compressible learning in the hippocampus. Nat Neurosci 22:1168–1181. 10.1038/s41593-019-0415-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Noudoost B, Chang MH, Steinmetz NA, Moore T (2010) Top-down control of visual attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:183–190. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Okun M, Lampl I (2008) Instantaneous correlation of excitation and inhibition during ongoing and sensory-evoked activities. Nat Neurosci 11:535–537. 10.1038/nn.2105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ormond J, Woodin MA (2009) Disinhibition mediates a form of hippocampal long-term potentiation in area CA1. PLoS One 4:e7224. 10.1371/journal.pone.0007224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ormond J, Woodin MA (2011) Disinhibition-mediated LTP in the hippocampus is synapse specific. Front Cell Neurosci 5:17. 10.3389/fncel.2011.00017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pei X, Vidyasagar TR, Volgushev M, Creutzfeldt OD (1994) Receptive field analysis and orientation selectivity of postsynaptic potentials of simple cells in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci 14:7130–7140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pi HJ, Hangya B, Kvitsiani D, Sanders JI, Huang ZJ, Kepecs A (2013) Cortical interneurons that specialize in disinhibitory control. Nature 503:521–524. 10.1038/nature12676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Reich DS, Mechler F, Purpura KP, Victor JD (2000) Interspike intervals, receptive fields, and information encoding in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 20:1964–1974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ruff DA, Cohen MR (2014) Attention can either increase or decrease spike count correlations in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 17:1591–1597. 10.1038/nn.3835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ruff DA, Cohen MR (2019) Simultaneous multi-area recordings suggest that attention improves performance by reshaping stimulus representations. Nat Neurosci 22:1669–1676. 10.1038/s41593-019-0477-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sillito AM. (1979) Inhibitory mechanisms influencing complex cell orientation selectivity and their modification at high resting discharge levels. J Physiol 289:33–53. 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sillito AM, Kemp JA, Milson JA, Berardi N (1980) A re-evaluation of the mechanisms underlying simple cell orientation selectivity. Brain Res 194:517–520. 10.1016/0006-8993(80)91234-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Smith SL, Smith IT, Branco T, Häusser M (2013) Dendritic spikes enhance stimulus selectivity in cortical neurons in vivo. Nature 503:115–120. 10.1038/nature12600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sprekeler H. (2017) Functional consequences of inhibitory plasticity: homeostasis, the excitation-inhibition balance and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol 43:198–203. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stellwagen D, Malenka RC (2006) Synaptic scaling mediated by glial TNF-α. Nature 440:1054–1059. 10.1038/nature04671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Tsodyks MV, Markram H (1997) The neural code between neocortical pyramidal neurons depends on neurotransmitter release probability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:719–723. 10.1073/pnas.94.2.719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Ujfalussy BB, Makara JK, Lengyel M, Branco T (2018) Global and multiplexed dendritic computations under in vivo-like conditions. Neuron 100:579–592. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Vallentin D, Kosche G, Lipkind D, Long MA (2016) Inhibition protects acquired song segments during vocal learning in zebra finches. Science 351:267–271. 10.1126/science.aad3023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Van Vreeswijk C, Sompolinsky H (1996) Chaos in neuronal networks with balanced excitatory and inhibitory activity. Science 274:1724–1726. 10.1126/science.274.5293.1724 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Vogels TP, Abbott LF (2009) Gating multiple signals through detailed balance of excitation and inhibition in spiking networks. Nat Neurosci 12:483–491. 10.1038/nn.2276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Vogels TP, Sprekeler H, Zenke F, Clopath C, Gerstner W (2011) Inhibitory plasticity balances excitation and inhibition in sensory pathways and memory networks. Science 334:1569–1573. 10.1126/science.1211095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Volgushev M, Pei X, Vidyasagar TR, Creutzfeldt OD (1993) Excitation and inhibition in orientation selectivity of cat visual cortex neurons revealed by whole-cell recordings in vivo. Vis Neurosci 10:1151–1155. 10.1017/s0952523800010257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Weber SN, Sprekeler H (2018) Learning place cells, grid cells and invariances with excitatory and inhibitory plasticity. eLife 7:e34560 10.7554/eLife.34560 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wehr M, Zador AM (2003) Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature 426:442–446. 10.1038/nature02116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wilson NR, Runyan CA, Wang FL, Sur M (2012) Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitory networks in vivo. Nature 488:343–348. 10.1038/nature11347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Woodin MA, Ganguly K, Poo M (2003) Coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity modifies GABAergic synapses by postsynaptic changes in Cl transporter activity. Neuron 39:807–820. 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00507-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Yang GR, Joglekar MR, Song HF, Newsome WT, Wang XJ (2019) Task representations in neural networks trained to perform many cognitive tasks. Nat Neurosci 22:297–306. 10.1038/s41593-018-0310-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Zenke F, Agnes EJ, Gerstner W (2015) Diverse synaptic plasticity mechanisms orchestrated to form and retrieve memories in spiking neural networks. Nat Commun 6:6922. 10.1038/ncomms7922 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Zhang S, Xu M, Kamigaki T, Do JP, Chang WC, Jenvay S, Miyamichi K, Luo L, Dan Y (2014) Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science 345:660–665. 10.1126/science.1254126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Zhong LR, Chen X, Park E, Südhof TC, Chen L (2018) Retinoic acid receptor RARα-dependent synaptic signaling mediates homeostatic synaptic plasticity at the inhibitory synapses of mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci 38:10454–10466. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1133-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zhou M, Liang F, Xiong XR, Li L, Li H, Xiao Z, Tao HW, Zhang LI (2014) Scaling down of balanced excitation and inhibition by active behavioral states in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 17:841–850. 10.1038/nn.3701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES