Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Aug 20.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2021 Aug;33(3):1072–1084. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420000358

Dyadic Synchrony and Repair Processes are Related to Preschool Children’s Risk Exposure and Self-Control

Carolyn M Scholtes 1, Emma R Lyons 1, Elizabeth A Skowron 2
PMCID: PMC7728658  NIHMSID: NIHMS1569922  PMID: 32524930

Abstract

We examined associations between preschool children’s cumulative risk exposure, dyadic interaction patterns, and self-control abilities in 238 mother-child dyads. Positive interactive synchrony, relationship ruptures, and latency to repair were micro-coded during a three to five-minute joint challenge task. Children’s self-control was assessed via two laboratory tasks and by parent report. Structural equation modeling and mediation analyses were utilized to examine the direct and indirect effects of cumulative risk on children’s observed and parent-reported self-control abilities. Parent-child interactive processes of dyadic synchrony and latency to repair ruptures in synchrony were examined as mediators. Dyadic synchrony and latency to repair ruptures were found to mediate associations between cumulative risk exposure and children’s behavioral and parent-reported self-control. Children exposed to more cumulative risk engaged in less dyadic synchrony and experienced longer latencies to repair ruptures with their caregiver, which in turn was associated with lower child self-control. Though cross-sectional, findings suggest dyadic synchrony and repair processes may represent viable mechanistic pathways linking cumulative risk exposure and deficits in child self-control. However, independent replications using longitudinal and experimental intervention designs are needed to determine causal pathways and inform new approaches for targeting the effects of early risk exposure through a focus on two-generational interventions.

Keywords: cumulative risk, parent-child interaction, self-control, dyadic synchrony, rupture-repair


The development of self-regulation – a set of intrinsic processes involved in managing and expressing emotions, directing attention, and initiating and inhibiting behavior, (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010) – is a critical component of successful social, emotional, and cognitive functioning across the lifespan (Best & Miller, 2010; Blair & Diamond, 2008; Cole, Ram, & English, 2018; Vohs & Baumeister, 2016). Self-regulation skills enable children to focus their attention, recognize and correct mistakes, make decisions, solve problems, and control impulses (Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University, 2011). Disruption in the development of self-regulation in early childhood can result in psychopathology, behavioral misconduct, substance abuse, and school failure, the effects of which often continue into adolescence and adulthood (Blair & Raver, 2015; Cole et al., 2018; Posner & Rothbart, 2000). Early exposure to multiple risk factors has been shown to compromise self-regulation development and leave children at risk for poor academic and socioemotional outcomes (Cipriano, Skowron, & Gatzke-Kopp, 2011; Kim & Brody, 2005; Kim, Brody, & McBride Murry, 2003; Lengua et al., 2015; Pears, Fisher, Bruce, Kim, & Yoerger, 2010). Likewise, links between parenting quality and children’s regulatory development are well established, with numerous studies documenting the role of sensitive, responsive, autonomy-promoting parenting in fostering children’s regulatory control (Grolnick & Farkas, 2002; Kopp, 1982; McCabe, Rebello-Britto, Hernandez, & Brooks-Gunn, 2004; Sroufe, 1996; Thompson, 1994).

Effortful control is one component of self-regulation that involves attentional shifting, attention focusing, inhibitory control, and perceptual sensitivity (Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). Effortful control is defined as one’s ability to inhibit a dominant response and instead perform a subdominant response, as well as detect errors and engage in planning (Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). Effortful control is linked to a variety of adaptive developmental skills, including sympathy/empathy (Eisenberg et al., 2007; Valiente et al., 2004), conscience and moral development (Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003), social competence (Eisenberg et al., 2003; Fabes et al., 1999; Spinrad et al., 2006; Spinrad et al., 2007), and school competence (Fabes, Martin, Hanish, Anders, & Madden-Derdich, 2003; Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, & Castro, 2007; Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson, & Reiser, 2008). Additionally, higher levels of effortful control have been linked to lower rates of internalizing and externalizing disorders (Eisenberg et al., 2001; Eisenberg, Sadovsky, et al., 2005; Eisenberg, Zhou, et al., 2005; Eisenberg et al., 2009).

Inhibitory control represents another key component of self-regulation that develops rapidly between 2 and 5 years of age (Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001; Kochanska et al., 2000). Inhibitory control is defined as the ability to inhibit a pre-potent response and engage in non-dominant, less automatic responding (e.g., raising one’s hand in class before speaking; Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Kochanska et al., 2000; Kochanska et al., 2001). Inhibitory control skills are essential for successful school adjustment, positive peer relations, and socioemotional functioning (Bell & Deater-Deckard, 2007; Blair, 2002; Coie, Lochman, Terry, & Hyman, 1992; Crandall, Deater-Deckard, & Riley, 2015; Lewit & Baker, 1995; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005). Inhibitory control and effortful control both represent key aspects of self-control for children. Thus, self-control will be referred to throughout the present study when referring to these aspects of regulatory functioning concurrently.

The development of self-control skills is fostered or compromised in the early years, when children are particularly sensitive to the influence of environmental stressors and supports (Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University, 2011). Numerous studies show that early exposure to risk factors (i.e., poverty, harsh, unresponsive parenting, parent psychopathology, and chaotic home life) has a direct, negative effect on children’s developing self-control skills (e.g., Blair & Raver, 2015; Hostinar, Stellern, Schaefer, Carlson, & Gunnar, 2012; Lengua, Honorado, & Bush, 2007; McDermott, Westerlund, Zeanah, Nelson, & Fox, 2012; Pears et al., 2010; Skowron, Cipriano-Essel, Gatzke-Kopp, Teti, & Ammerman, 2014). When conceptualizing exposure to multiple overlapping and independent risk factors, the cumulative risk framework is a particularly effective measure of developmental risk exposure and a better predictor of adverse developmental outcomes than singular risk exposures (Evans, Li, & Whipple, 2013; Evans & Kim, 2007; Rutter, 1979; Sameroff & Rosenblum, 2006). Cumulative risk captures exposure to multiple, overlapping environmental or individual risk factors that often lead to a host of negative outcomes later in life (i.e., Rutter, 1979, 1981), and is conceptualized in this study to include sociodemographic risk, psychosocial risk, and child maltreatment risk. Further, cumulative risk confers a dose-response effect, such that as exposure to risk factors increases, so too does the severity of adverse outcomes (Evans et al., 2013).

A growing body of research is focusing on elucidating proximal candidate pathways through which cumulative risk exerts its negative effects on children’s outcomes. These include child characteristics such as temperament (Cipriano-Essel, Skowron, Stifter, & Teti, 2013; Kochanska & Kim, 2013), parenting beliefs and practices (Candelaria, Teti, & Black, 2011; Kim & Brody, 2005; Kim et al., 2003; Lengua et al., 2007; Mistry, Benner, Biesanz, Clark, & Howes., 2010), and the home environment (Brody & Flor, 1998; Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997; Obradović, Yousafzai, Finch, & Rasheed, 2016). Given the malleability of parent-child interactive processes, we view the parent-child relationship as a particularly promising candidate pathway for understanding how cumulative risk exposure effects children’s outcomes.

Dyadic Synchrony, Rupture, and Repair Processes

Dyadic Synchrony

There is increasing recognition that parent-child relationships are built and maintained through bidirectional processes that are reciprocally shaped by a parent and their child, beginning very early in a child’s life (Combs-Ronto, Olson, Lunkenheimer, & Sameroff, 2009; Sameroff & MacKenzie, 2003). Studied primarily among mother-child dyads, positive dyadic synchrony represents prosocial coordination characterized by an observable pattern of mutually regulated, reciprocal, and harmonious interaction between parent and child (Cohn & Tronick, 1989; Harrist & Waugh, 2002; Skowron, Kozlowski, & Pincus 2010). During positive synchronous interactions, parents and children may engage in reciprocal non-verbal behaviors, such as laughing and smiling, as well as verbal behaviors including expression of positive affect and positive verbal appraisals of their dyadic partner (Harrist & Waugh, 2002). Research suggests that children’s experience of positive synchrony is critical in promoting healthy development across several domains. Specifically, children who experience regular synchronous states with their caregiver show greater interpersonal skills with peers (Kennedy & Bakeman, 1984) and have better behavioral and academic adjustment (Harrist, Pettit, Dodge, & Bates, 1994; Pettit & Mize, 1993). Children’s experiences of positive synchrony with caregivers are also thought to support self-control development beginning in infancy, when parents foster basic security, bonding, and homeostatic regulation (DiCorcia & Tronick, 2011; Feldman, 2007; Kopp, 1982; Skowron, 2015; Tronick, 1989; Beeghly & Tronick, 2011). Positive interactive synchrony has been linked with improved self-control outcomes for children across development, such that children who experience more mutually positive and prosocial interactions with their caregivers demonstrate better behavioral and emotional control (Davis, Bilms, & Suveg, 2017; Kim & Kochanska, 2012; Lindsey, Cremeens, Colwell, & Caldera, 2009; Olson & Lunkenheimer, 2009; Sameroff, 2009). For example, dyadic synchrony observed during mother-toddler play was associated with the toddlers’ ability to delay gratification and engage in self-control strategies (Raver, 1996). Similarly, positive mother-infant synchrony has been found to predict children’s self-control two years later (Feldman, Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999).

The present study aims to replicate previous findings linking dyadic synchrony to children’s self-control by examining this dyadic process in relation to children’s behavioral inhibitory control and parent-reported effortful control. We posit that positive dyadic synchrony functions to support a child’s developing capacity for self-control by providing the child with safety and support for his or her emerging efforts to manage arousal and maintain relational engagement (Harrist & Waugh, 2002). Conversely, lower rates of positive parent-child synchrony are thought to impede the development of children’s self-control skills, as relationships characterized by frequent negative arousal and caregiver disengagement undermine adaptive efforts by the child to assume control of their own behavior (Harrist & Waugh, 2002). We aim to study the association between dyadic synchrony and children’s self-control in a sample of families in which children have significant exposure to early life risk. Positive dyadic synchrony may be a particularly important mechanism for supporting children’s self-control among dyads with significant risk exposure due to the negative impact that cumulative risk has consistently shown on children’s self-control development (Cipriano et al., 2011; Kim & Brody, 2005; Lengua et al., 2007; Lengua et al., 2015; Pears et al., 2010). Further, positive dyadic synchrony has previously demonstrated important associations with early life risk. For example, mother-child dyads with a history of maltreatment have been shown to exhibit decreases in positive synchrony over the course of a single interactive exchange (Giuliano, Skowron, & Berkman, 2015). Thus, it is particularly important to examine these associations in a high-risk sample.

Rupture-Repair Processes

While the experience of positive interactive synchrony provides important benefits for children’s development, we posit that rupture and repair processes may also be associated with self-control skills in early childhood. Research demonstrates that positive synchrony occurs less than 50% of the time in parent-infant and parent-preschooler exchanges (e.g. Cohn & Tronick, 1989; Harrist & Waugh, 2002), and that ruptures in synchrony are quite common, even in healthy mother-child dyads (Skowron et al., 2010). Ruptures occur when one member of the dyad interrupts a sequence of positive interaction with a negative verbalization, such as attacking, blaming, controlling, or ignoring the other member of the dyad (Skowron et al., 2010). Such ruptures are repaired when the dyad enters back into a mutually positive synchronous state, where both members of the dyad are engaging in the interaction in a positive and affirming manner (Skowron et al., 2010). We theorize that relationship ruptures are manageable to the extent that they are repaired quickly. When ruptures in positive synchrony with one’s caregiver are relatively brief, they may provide young children with age-appropriate opportunities to manage themselves in the midst of interactive stress. Together, we reasoned that young children’s experiences with brief ruptures in dyadic synchrony that are soon repaired may provide children with opportunities to experience negative affect in brief doses, which subsides following returns to positive relational engagement with their caregiver. These repeated experiences in caregiver-child synchrony-rupture-repair transactions help to support a growing child’s confidence in their ability to cope, trust in their caregiver’s availability, and over time, internalize the self-control functions that caregivers previously served (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Skowron, 2015; Tronick, 1989).

Though fewer studies have examined dyadic rupture and repair processes and their links to children’s self-control, there is some evidence supporting the healthy nature of rupture-repair processes in parent-child dyads. Shorter latencies to repair ruptures in mother-infant synchrony predicted better infant cortisol regulation (Müller, Zietlow, Tronick, & Reck, 2015), and higher rates of interactive repair observed in mother-preschooler dyads correspond to better child emotion regulation and fewer behavior problems (Kemp, Lunkenheimer, Albrecht, & Chen, 2016). Further, there is some prior research which suggests that when ruptures in positive synchrony do occur, successful repairs and quicker returns to dyadic synchrony serve as important interactive processes for fostering children’s self-control development (Kemp et al., 2016; Skowron et al., 2010; Tronick, 2007). In regard to early adversity, research suggest that broad parenting skills may help to explain whether and how cumulative risk adversely impacts children’s developing self-control skills (Evans et al., 2013; Best & Miller, 2010; Cipriano-Essel et al., 2013; Harrist & Waugh, 2002; Kochanska, Aksan, Penney, & Boldt 2007). Additionally, some research suggests that greater risk may be associated with maladaptive rupture-repair processes. For example, families at higher risk for maltreatment show patterns of greater dyadic discord with more ruptures and fewer successful repairs, as compared to families with lower levels of risk (Skowron et al., 2010). However, it is unknown whether cumulative risk is associated with deficits in children’s self-control skills through disruptions in parent-child co-created dyadic synchrony and rupture-repair processes. To our knowledge, little work to date has examined whether dyadic rupture-repair experiences are associated with child self-control skills in a high-risk sample of families.

Thus, in the current study, we sought to test the mediating effects of dyadic synchrony and rupture-repair processes on links between cumulative risk exposure and children’s self-control skills in a cross-sectional sample of high-risk families. We reasoned that (a) dyadic interactive synchrony and (b) latency to repair following a rupture each represent theoretically grounded pathways through which exposure to cumulative risk is associated with deficits in children’s self-control. First, we predicted direct associations between cumulative risk and lower child self-control, evidenced in performance on two inhibitory control tasks and parent-reported effortful control. Second, we tested the mediating effects of observations of dyadic interactive synchrony and latencies to repair on links between children’s cumulative risk exposure and self-control. We predicted the linkage between cumulative risk and lower child self-control skills would be mediated by lower positive dyadic synchrony and lengthier latencies to repair following interactive rupture.

Method

Participants

Participants were 238 mother-child dyads recruited from five rural counties in the mid-Atlantic region of the United States. Approximately half of the families in the present study (58.2%) had prior involvement with CPS (Child Protective Services), in which participating mothers were documented perpetrators of maltreatment. Study children were between the ages of 3 and 5 (M = 3.74) and 51.5% female. Study mothers ranged in age from 19 to 45 (M = 29.25, SD = 6.42). The majority (71.2%) of mothers had an annual income of $30,000 or less, were married or in a committed relationship (63.7%), and had a obtained a high school education or less (72.7%). Mothers identified as White (90.0%), Bi- or Multi-Racial (3.2%), Hispanic/Latina (3.2%), African American (2.4%), and Asian (0.4%). Mothers identified their children as White (78.9%), Bi- or Multi-Racial (16.7%), African American (2.0%), and Hispanic/Latino (1.6%).

Procedure

Mothers and children completed two home visits and one 2.5-hour laboratory visit over a 2–3-week period. During the lab visit, parent-child processes were assessed during a three to five-minute joint-challenge task (Hoffman, Crnic, & Baker, 2006), during which children were instructed to build a replica 3-D block figurine and mothers were instructed to assist their child as they might typically do at home (though without physically handling the blocks). Mothers provided demographic information and completed several questionnaires used to assess their child’s exposure to risk factors while their children participated in two solo lab tasks to assess self-control skills. Families received $150 for their participation, were compensated for transportation, and received a small gift for the participating child.

Measures

Cumulative risk

Cumulative risk was operationalized as a composite variable consisting of eight individually coded dichotomous risk factors. Factors were coded into three categories – sociodemographic, psychosocial, and child maltreatment risk – and each factor within these categories was given a score of one when present or a score of zero when absent.

Sociodemographic risk

Sociodemographic risk factors included annual low income (< $30,000 annually), single-parent status, and maternal high-school dropout.

Psychosocial risk

Psychosocial risk factors included family turmoil and community violence exposure. Family turmoil was assessed using the 60-item Life Experience Survey (LES; Sarason, Johnson, & Siegel, 1978). Scores within the top quartile of negative events experienced within the past year (endorsing four or more items) were given a score of one on the cumulative risk index, while scores of three or fewer negative life events within the previous year were given a score of zero. Children’s exposure to community violence was assessed using the 54-item Child Exposure to Community Violence (CECV) survey (Richters & Saltzman, 1990), which assesses the frequency with which a child has been a victim of or experienced community violence and related activities. Scores within the top quartile (endorsing eight or more items) were given a score of one on the composite cumulative risk index, while scores of seven or fewer received a score of zero.

Child maltreatment risk

Child maltreatment risk included maltreatment risk presence, maltreatment risk severity, and child separation from the home due to foster care placement. CPS record data was coded using the Maltreatment Classification System (MCS; Barnett, Manly, & Cicchetti, 1993) to determine maltreatment risk presence and severity. Children who had any previous experience with child welfare documented by CPS, including previous abuse, neglect, or involvement with child welfare without a codable instance of maltreatment, received a score of one. No information was available on whether maltreatment instances were substantiated. Thus, this variable is best characterized as maltreatment risk rather than maltreatment experience, as it encompasses a wide variety of maltreatment risk exposures, ranging from a single unsubstantiated report to multiple substantiated reports. Children who had no documented history with child welfare received a score of zero on this variable. Children with a documented maltreatment experience received an additional point on the cumulative risk index if the severity of the maltreatment they had experienced received a score of three or higher on a five-point maltreatment severity scale, with five indicating the most severe maltreatment experiences. Finally, children who had been placed in foster care during their lifetime received a score of one, while those who had no foster care placement received a score of zero. Composite scores on the cumulative risk index comprised of all eight risk factors were available for 227 participants (M = 2.70, SD = 1.83; range = 0–7). Descriptive statistics of cumulative risk responses are presented in Table 1, including the prevalence of each individual cumulative risk factor in the present sample.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics for Components of Cumulative Risk

Cumulative risk component N (%) M SD
Sociodemographic risk
 Low income (<$30,000) 141 (71.2) 0.71 0.45
 Single-parent status 68 (34.3) 0.34 0.48
 Maternal high-school dropout 23 (11.6) 0.12 0.32
Psychosocial risk
 Family turmoil (LES; upper quartile) 62 (31.3) 2.87 2.47
 Community violence (CECV; upper quartile) 52 (26.3) 5.19 5.03
Child maltreatment risk (MCS)
 Maltreatment risk presence 146 (58.2) 0.51 0.50
 Maltreatment severity (>3) 71 (35.9) 2.33 1.55
 History of foster care placement 14 (7.1) 0.07 0.26

Note. For the frequency and percentage values reported, all variables are dichotomously coded, 1 = present, 0 = absent. Family turmoil was considered to be present for families who scored within the top quartile of negative events experienced the last year on the Life Experience Survey (LES). Community violence exposure was considered to be present for families who scored within the top quartile of the Child Exposure to Community Violence (CECV) survey. Low income was considered to be present for families with an annual income of $30,000 or less. Child maltreatment severity was considered to be present when children had a maltreatment history coded as 3, 4, or 5 using the Maltreatment Classification System (MCS), indicating moderately severe to the most severe maltreatment. For variables which were initially continuous (e.g., LES, CECV, and maltreatment severity), the mean and standard deviation reflect true variable values. For all other variables, the mean and standard deviation are reflective of the dichotomous variable values.

Children’s self-control

Children’s self-control was assessed using two behavioral measures of inhibitory control and one parent-report measure of effortful control, described below.

Behavioral inhibitory control

Children’s behavioral inhibitory control was measured using two Stroop-like tasks administered by a trained research assistant. The Shapes task (Kochanska, Murray, & Coy, 1997) consists of children first being shown pictures of three large fruits and three small fruits and then being shown pictures of three small fruits embedded within a different larger fruit (e.g., a small orange embedded in a large banana). Children were instructed to point to each small fruit embedded within the larger fruit, thus being asked to inhibit their dominant response of pointing to the large fruit and instead engaging in a sub-dominant response to point to the smaller fruit. Each trial was scored as a zero (incorrect response), one (spontaneous self-correct), or two (correct initial response). Final scores for the Shapes task ranged from zero to six (M = 4.78, SD = 1.77). The Day/Night task (Gerstadt, Hong, & Diamond, 1994) consists of children being instructed to say “day” when shown a card with a moon and stars and say “night” when presented with a card with a sun. Children completed 16 trials and scores for each trial were again scored as zero (incorrect response), one (spontaneous self-correct), or two (correct initial response). Final scores for the Day/Night task ranged from zero to 32 (M = 16.12, SD = 10.13). Scores from the Shapes and Day/Night tasks were found to be significantly correlated, r(195) = .26, p < .001, and thus were converted to z-scores and added together to create a composite measure of participating children’s behavioral inhibitory control (M = −.05, SD = 0.86; range = −2.86–1.44).

Parent-reported effortful control

Mother-reported effortful control was assessed using the effortful control factor of the Child Behavior Questionnaire short-form (CBQ; Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001). The CBQ was administered to each mother by a trained research assistant. Items are scored on a seven-point Likert scale and range from one (extremely untrue of my child) to seven (extremely true of my child). Items on the CBQ contributing to the Effortful Control factor assess areas of attentional focusing, inhibitory control, low-intensity pleasure, and perceptual sensitivity. Scores for the effortful control factor are computed by averaging the scale scores for each of these sub-scales. Scores for the Effortful Control factor ranged from 3.40 to 6.83 (M = 5.01, SD = 0.65).

Dyadic Processes

The Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (SASB; Benjamin, 1996) assessment methodology includes an observational coding system (Benjamin & Cushing, 2000) for dyadic interpersonal behavior, along the orthogonal dimensions of affiliation and interdependence to yield eight behavioral blends along a circumplex. Affiliation describes communications on a continuum ranging from loving to hostile, and Interdependence describes communications on a continuum ranging from differentiated (i.e., autonomy-granting) to enmeshed (i.e., controlling/submitting). Communications that are transitive focus on the other and are prototypically parent-like, whereas communications that focus on self in reaction to the other are intransitive in nature, and prototypically child-like. The SASB has been used to meaningfully code brief segments of interaction in parent-child relationships.

We used the SASB observational coding system to code mother-child interactions during the standard joint Duplo task, assessing two dyadic process codes: (a) the extent of dyadic interactive synchrony, and (b) latency to repair ruptures in synchrony. The process of SASB coding a unit of behavior involves three steps (Benjamin & Cushing, 2000): determining focus, degree of warmth/affiliation, and degree of interdependence. As shown in Figure 1, transitive and intransitive behaviors in Clusters 2, 3, and 4 were identified to form a constellation of positive behaviors (i.e., Affirm/Understand. Disclose/Express; Love/Approach, Joyfully Connect; and Nurture/Protect, Trust/Rely). In contrast, transitive and intransitive behaviors in Clusters 6, 7, and 8 (i.e., Blame/Criticize, Sulk/Appease; Attack/Reject, Protest/Recoil; and Ignore/Neglect, Wall-Off/Avoid) form the set of negative behaviors.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

The Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (SASB) simplified cluster model (Benjamin, 1996). Bold labels represent transitive behaviors, underlined labels represent intransitive behaviors, and italicized labels represent introject behaviors.

Mothers’ and children’s videotaped interactions during the Duplo task were transcribed, unitized, and then subjected to SASB observational coding by a team of trained coders with over 80 hours of training. Verbatim transcripts of video-recorded tasks were prepared. Individual speaking turns were further unitized into separate units on the transcript to facilitate coding of discrete speech acts. For example, a mother might praise her child for handling the blocks gently then provide guidance to try the red block next, all in a single speaking turn. Unitization allowed each of these discrete speech acts to be coded separately. Using the unitized transcript and video recording, SASB coding began with the first codable event (i.e., first utterance by mother or child) in the sequence of mother-child interactions and ended with the last codable behavior that occurred in the task. Mother and child transactions were assigned SASB cluster codes and interrater reliability was calculated on 15% of tapes, with weighted kappas ranging from .73 to .84. SASB codes from proximal speaking turns were reviewed to identify all sequences of dyadic interactive synchrony and latency to repair.

Positive Dyadic Synchrony

We defined positive dyadic synchrony as a sequence of three or more positive mother-child behaviors, that is (M+, C+, M+ and so on) or (C+ M+, C+, and so on) from SASB Clusters 2,3, and 4 (see Figure 1). Thus, the lower bound for dyadic synchrony scores was 3 contiguous speaking turns up to and until positive synchrony was interrupted by a rupture or the end of task. Ruptures were defined as a single SASB-coded negative behavior, that is, M− or C− expressed by mother or child, interrupting a sequence of positive synchrony. Raw dyadic synchrony scores equaled the total number of M+ and C+ contiguous speaking turns during the task. Total speaking turns in positive dyadic synchrony was divided by the total number of speaking turns in the task to obtain a proportion score to control for between dyad differences in rates of speech. Thus, positive dyadic synchrony scores were calculated as the proportion of the total number of speaking turns in which a dyad was in dyadic synchrony, from 0.0 to 1.0.

Latency to Repair

Relationship repairs were defined as a three-step sequence of SASB-coded contiguous positive mother and child speaking turns (M+, C+, M+ or C+, M+, C+) that occurred after a relationship rupture, initiated by mother or child, (i.e., M− or C− from SASB Cluster 6, 7, or 8: criticize, sulk; attack, recoil; ignore, wall off). In other words, a relationship repair reflects a return to positive synchrony following a relationship rupture. Notably, dyads ranged in the amount of time following a rupture that they began repair efforts. We calculated latency to repair scores as the number of total speaking turns that elapsed following a rupture in synchrony to successful repair. Latency to repair scores were averaged for dyad who experienced multiple synchrony-rupture-repair sequences during the task, and then log transformed.

Results

Child age was significantly associated with behavioral inhibitory control (r(211) = 0.39, p < .001), but not with parent-reported effortful control (r(218) = 0.12, p = 0.09). Lower cumulative risk scores were significantly associated with higher behavioral inhibitory control scores on the Day/Night and Shapes task composite (r(201) = −0.19, p < .01) and greater parent-reported effortful control scores on the CBQ-EC scale (r(209) = −0.20, p < .01). Cumulative risk was still significantly associated with the child inhibitory composite after controlling for child age (r(175) = −0.17, p = 0.02). Lower cumulative risk scores were also significantly associated with higher positive dyadic synchrony scores (r(210) = −0.20, p < .01) and shorter latencies to repair (r(173) = 0.17, p = 0.03). Greater positive dyadic synchrony was significantly associated with both inhibitory control (r(209) = 0.31, p < .001) and effortful control (r(216) = 0.24, p < .001) scores. The relationship between dyadic synchrony and inhibitory control was still significant after controlling for child age (r(209) = 0.19, p = .005). Shorter latencies to repair were associated with higher inhibitory control (r(172) = −0.27, p < .001) and effortful control scores (r(176) = −0.17, p = .03). The relation between latency to repair and children’s inhibitory control was still significant after controlling for child age (r(172) = −0.20, p = .007). Correlations between all variables of interest are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Correlations between Cumulative Risk, Dyadic Processes, and Child Self-Control

Variable 1 2 3 4 5
1. Child age
2. Cumulative risk −.11
3. Positive dyadic synchrony .36** −.20*
4. Latency to repair −.20* .17* −.70**
5. Child inhibitory control .39** −.19* .31** −.27**
6. Child effortful control .12 −.20* .24** −.17* .11

Note. Positive dyadic synchrony was recorded as a proportion of the total number of speaking turns during a 5-minute joint problem-solving task. Latency to repair was recorded as the average number of speaking turns a dyad took to enter back into positive synchrony after one member ruptured a positive synchronous state. The latency to repair variable was log transformed. The child inhibitory control score is represented by a z-score composite of child performance on the Day/Night and Shapes tasks, with higher scores indicating greater inhibitory control. The Effortful Control factor from the Child Behavior Questionnaires (CBQ) was used to represent parent-report of child effortful control, with higher scores indicating better effortful control abilities.

*

p < .05

**

p < .001.

Analytic Strategy

After confirming expected associations between all variables of interest, a model was specified and tested using structural equation modeling (SEM) in AMOS version 23 (Arbuckle, 2014). Little’s test for missing completely at random (MCAR) was used to examine all variables included in the present analyses and revealed that data did not meet criteria for missing completely at random (Little’s MCAR χ2 [42] = 60.24, p = .03). Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) estimation was used in estimating model parameters in order to account for missing data. FIML maximizes the likelihood of missing data values based on observed data values, resulting in more statistically reliable standard errors when compared list-wise deletion, pair-wise deletion, and mean-imputation (Wothke, 1998). A latent variable of dyadic process was created with loadings onto both positive dyadic synchrony and latency to repair ruptures in synchrony in order to represent these processes as a single construct. Due to low latent factor loadings, children’s behavioral and parent-reported self-control were examined as separate outcomes in the model. Child age was controlled for in all analyses. Model fit was assessed using the χ2 fit statistic, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Values consistent with good fit include non-significant χ2 values, CFI values > .95, and RMSEA values within or below the .05–.08 range (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

Model associations were further examined using mediation analyses (MacKinnon, 2008). Mediation requires a direct effect of cumulative risk on both dyadic process and each self-control outcome. Additionally, dyadic processes must be related to each self-control outcome and result in a reduction in significance of the relationship between cumulative risk and dyadic processes. Monte Carlo simulated confidence intervals calculated in RMediation were utilized to estimate significance of indirect effects, as recommended (Tofighi & MacKinnon, 2011).

Mediation Analyses

The present model examined dyadic process as a partial mediator of the relationship between cumulative risk and children’s observed and parent-reported self-control abilities, controlling for child age. Results are shown in Figure 2. Findings suggest that partial mediation, in which cumulative risk shows direct and indirect associations with children’s self-control abilities through dyadic processes, demonstrates excellent model fit (χ2 = 3.86, df = 4, p = .43, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00). Model estimates showed that greater cumulative risk was associated with less positive dyadic interactions and longer ruptures (b = −.19, p = .005). Additionally, dyadic processes were significantly associated with both children’s observed (b = .21, p = .006) and parent-reported (b = .23, p = .005) inibitory control. Model predictors accounted for 21.7% of the variance in children’s observed inhibitory control and 9.1% of the variance in parent-reported effortful control. The indirect effect of cumulative risk on children’s observed inhibitory control was significant (Cumulative Risk ➔ Dyadic Process ➔ Behavioral Inhibitory Control) = −.04, p < .05; 95% CIs [−.02, −.005], as was the indirect effective of cumulative risk on children’s parent- reported effortful control (Cumulative Risk ➔ Dyadic Process ➔ Parent-Reported Effortful Control) = −.04, p < .05; 95% CIs [−.03, −.003]. The direct associations between cumulative risk and parent-reported effortful control remained significant (p = .04), suggesting both direct and indirect effects of cumulative risk on this aspect of children’s self-control abilities. In contrast, the direct association between cumulative risk and behavioral inhibitory control was non-significant (p = .07), suggesting behavioral inhibitory control was primarily associated with cumulative risk exposure through dyadic processes in the present model.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Structural equation path model examining direct and indirect effects of the mediation hypothesis. Paths are standardized estimates. Model fit: χ2(238) = 3.86, df = 4, p = .43; CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00. Standardized indirect effects of cumulative risk on children’s observed inhibitory control (Cumulative Risk → Dyadic Process → Behavioral Inhibitory Control) = −.04, p < .05; 95% CIs [−.02, −.005], and parent-reported effortful control (Cumulative Risk → Dyadic Process → Parent-Reported Effortful Control) = −.04, p < .05; 95% CIs [−.03, −.003] were significant. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Discussion

In the current study, we sought to clarify pathways through which cumulative risk is associated with children’s self-control. We tested whether dyadic coordination patterns in parent-preschooler interactions mediated the association between cumulative risk and children’s self-control skills. Results confirmed that the relation between risk exposure and children’s self-control skills varied by (a) the extent of positive interactive synchrony that parent-child dyads exhibited during a standard lab task, and (b) the length of time it took dyads to repair ruptures in synchrony. Specifically, greater cumulative risk exposure was associated with less time spent in positive dyadic synchrony and longer latencies to repair. Additionally, dyadic processes, including less dyadic synchrony and lengthier ruptures, were related to lower self-control scores for children, whether those skills were assessed via child performance or parent-report methods.

The present findings are consistent with prior research demonstrating that exposure to contextual risk is negatively associated with children’s self-control skills (Berthelsen, Hayes, White, & Williams, 2017; Blair & Raver, 2012, 2015; Brown, Ackerman, & Moore, 2013; Lengua et al., 2015; Perry, Braren, & Blair, 2018; Tomalski et al., 2017), and supports the role of dyadic synchrony and rupture-repair processes as dyadic processes closely associated with children’s self-control in early childhood (Blair et al., 2011; Lengua et al., 2007; Lengua et al., 2015; Merz, Landry, Montroy, & Williams, 2017; Ruberry, Klein, Kiff, Thompson, & Lengua, 2018). Findings contribute to the growing body of evidence suggesting that quality of parent-child interactions (Davis et al., 2017; Kim & Kochanska, 2012; Lindsey et al., 2009; Olson & Lunkenheimer, 2009; Sameroff, 2009), including repair processes (Kemp et al., 2016; Skowron et al., 2010; Tronick, 2007) are associated with the development of child self-control. The current results offer new evidence that early risk may be negatively associated with children’s self-control development through their experiences with briefer periods of positive synchrony and lengthier ruptures or breakdowns in those positive synchronous states between caregiver and child. Thus, findings contribute to previous literature examining the self-control skills of children exposed to significant early life adversity (Gach, Ip, Sameroff, & Olson, 2018; Labella, Narayan, McCormick, Desjardins, & Masten, 2017; Song, Miller, Leung, Lumeng, & Rosenblum, 2018). Specifically, the present study: a) utilizes a cumulative risk model to examine the association of a variety of early risk factors with study outcomes, b) examines specific dyadic transactional processes rather than examining parenting behaviors alone, and c) explores self- control processes specifically in contrast to broader social-emotional adjustment or the development of internalizing/externalizing behavior disorders. Thus, the constellation of specific variables examined in the present study provides cross-sectional evidence that experiences with greater interactive synchrony and more efficient repair of relational ruptures may reflect a set of mechanistic pathways through which early adversity exposure bears on children’s self-control skills.

In the sample studied here, parents and children exposed to greater cumulative risk spent less time in positive interactive synchrony, and those same children showed greater deficits in their self-control abilities. In early childhood, positive relational patterns with one’s caregiver are especially important due to age-related increases in exploration and independence, which require increased parental monitoring and limit-setting (Shaw, Bell, & Gilliom, 2000; Trentacosta et al., 2008). As warm, nurturing parenting behavior may be inherently challenging at times when parenting one’s preschooler, the presence of cumulative risk may further exacerbate a parent’s ability to manage difficult parent-child interactions. In addition, research suggests that children exposed to greater cumulative risk often show more externalizing behaviors and behavioral problems (Appleyard, Egeland, van Dulmen, & Sroufe, 2005; Blanz, Schmidt, & Esser, 1991; Jones, Forehand, Brody, & Armistad, 2002), which may further contribute to reduced time that children and parents spend in harmonious, reciprocal transactions. Thus, these findings suggest that the presence or absence of cumulative risk are associated with parents’ abilities to co-create a relational context that may support or impedes the development of children’s self-control skills. Positive dyadic synchrony has previously emerged as a factor associated with self-control development in childhood (Beeghly, Perry, & Tronick, 2016; Feldman, 2007, 2009), and the current findings extend this knowledge by documenting the role of bidirectional parent-child interactions for self-control in early childhood among families exposed to high levels of cumulative risk. More specifically, warm, responsive parent-child relationships may contribute to improved child self-control by providing a context for children to experience developmentally appropriate successes and failures in a growth-promoting manner, although longitudinal evidence is needed to confirm the directionality of this association. As environments characterized by adversity are often marked by fear and hypervigilance that create lasting changes in the brain areas responsible for self-control skills (Brown et al., 2013), our results suggest that the experience of non-harmonious parent-child relationships are also correlated with deficits in children’s self-control.

In the present study, parents and children exposed to greater cumulative risk also showed lengthier times between relational ruptures and repairs, which was associated with deficits in child self-control. In addition to being associated with a parent’s ability to maintain positive interactive synchrony with their child, greater contextual risk also was also related to a parent’s ability to quickly repair challenging parent-child interactions. Longer times spent between relational ruptures and repairs prolong states of negative affect, arousal, and conflict that children experience, de facto reducing the time spent with their caregiver in positive interactive synchrony. Our results suggest that shorter time to repair after a relationship rupture is related to child self-control development in the face of adversity, where children are already at risk for deficits in self-control. Longitudinal and intervention research is necessary to identify the directionality of these associations and confirm hypotheses postulated in this theory-driven work. For example, parents who facilitate quicker interactive repairs following a rupture (i.e., speedier returns to dyadic synchrony) may be strengthening their children’s self-control development while supporting and gaining trust of their child. These opportunities for quick repairs after ruptures may enable children to learn to manage their negative arousal and behaviors, and thus provide a context where they can begin to internalize self-control abilities initially performed by their caregivers (Beeghly et al., 2016, Feldman, 2007, 2009; Skowron et al., 2010; Tronick, 1989). Among children who experience multiple risk factors, lengthy ruptures may serve to extend time children spend experiencing strong negative affect, further impacting their ability to regulate their arousal and leaving them at risk for emotional and behavioral dysregulation. Longer episodes between ruptures and subsequent repairs could leave children in a state of uncertainty, unpredictability, and feeling overwhelmed. Thus, such extended episodes may result in children managing negative arousal without support and could provide children fewer opportunities with caregiver scaffolding, depriving them of modeling that supports self-control skill development. These questions await further investigations using longitudinal designs to strengthen directional, causal claims that extend beyond conclusions that can be drawn from the current study.

Taken together, these results highlight positive dyadic synchrony and prompt repairs of ruptures as important areas for further study and potential targets for early intervention in families with exposure to significant early life risk. Although previous research has demonstrated that general parenting skills are related to cumulative risk, including caregiver responsiveness and warmth, the present study expands on these findings by documenting specific, bidirectional processes between parents and children that are linked with cumulative risk exposure and variations in children’s self-control development. Interventions that target parent and child mutual interaction patterns rather than parenting behaviors alone may represent an effective approach for strengthening children’s self-control.

While these results provide new insights into how parents and children co-create optimal relationships related to the development of self-control in the context of adversity, it is important to note several limitations of the present study. First, participants were predominantly White, lower-income, and living in rural communities, perhaps limiting the generalizability of these findings to more diverse samples. However, given that rural samples are often understudied, the present study may offer an important contribution in examining the intersection of risk, dyadic process, and self-control in this population. Though we found support for the hypothesized model (i.e., dyadic synchrony and repair processes mediate links between cumulative risk exposure and deficits in children’s self-control), the directionality of associations between study variables cannot be assumed due to the cross-sectional nature of the data. It is plausible that children raised in low-risk contexts are better regulated and are thus better able to contribute to creating more mutually positive interactions with their caregivers and help to repair ruptures when they occur. Further longitudinal research is needed to confirm if cumulative risk exerts a negative causal impact on dyadic interactional processes, which then results in impaired self-control skills among children. Because levels of cumulative risk and positive dyadic interaction cannot be randomly assigned, experimental intervention designs can be employed to test causal associations between these variables by randomly assigning participants to receive an intervention that strengthens interactive synchrony and promotes quicker repair processes. Researchers can then assess whether interventions that strengthen dyadic coordination between a parent and their child lead to stronger self-control in children. Alternatively, longitudinal studies repeatedly assessing both dyadic synchrony and children’s self-control abilities as children develop these skills between the ages of 2 and 5 years would provide valuable information for confirming the present findings in the absence of intervention. Finally, although the cumulative risk model utilized in the present analyses has many strengths, there are also some limitations to this approach. These include lack of information on intensity or impact of individual risk factors and interactions between risk factors. For example, it is possible that experiencing severe maltreatment has a much greater impact on dyadic processes and subsequent self-control skills compared to living in a single-parent household. Therefore, despite the strengths of the cumulative risk approach, it may also be important for risk factors to be examined as individual predictors of dyadic interaction and self-control outcomes in future research.

A variety of interventions exist that have already been shown to improve self-control among children exposed to early life adversity. Such interventions include high-quality Montessori programs, Tools of the Mind, Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies (PATHS), and the Chicago School Readiness Project (CSRP; e.g., Diamond & Lee, 2011). However, given the present findings and body of prior literature highlighting the importance of parenting and dyadic interaction for children’s self-control outcomes, parent-child interventions present themselves as an important area for further examination, particularly among populations experiencing high levels of early adversity. Programs such as Parent Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT; Eyberg, 1988) may be deserving of closer inquiry given their focus on promoting mutually positive dyadic interaction and practice of coaching parents to mastery on skills that promote such interactions (Chaffin et al., 2004; Goldfine, Wagner, Branstetter, & McNeil, 2008; Thomas & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2011). Given that parenting is a proximal factor that influences children’s development in an ongoing manner, such programs may be uniquely equipped to promote the development of self-control among children with early adverse life experiences. For instance, PCIT targets parenting behavior through live, individualized coaching and teaches caregivers to generate nurturing, positive interactions with their child while also reinforcing warm, positive child behaviors in return. Throughout PCIT, parents and children reinforce warm interactions with each other, thus co-creating positive interactive synchrony and perhaps fostering children’s self-control skills. PCIT also facilitates a healthy rupture-repair process by teaching parents to impose predictable, consistent limits and age-appropriate consequences, and encouraging quick returns to synchrony after expected ruptures occur. These aspects of PCIT may strengthen children’s self-control skills by giving them opportunities to manage their negative arousal in the context of positive special time play and supporting caregiver-facilitated repairs to ruptures in dyadic synchrony. However, research is needed to examine if PCIT enhances self-control skills through these processes. Thus, randomized clinical trials of PCIT and other parenting interventions will help to identify whether increased positive dyadic interaction and ability to repair ruptures promoted by these interventions is responsible for improving self-control outcomes in families where children are exposed to significant early life risk.

Future research should also continue to explore how specific aspects of dyadic synchrony and rupture-repair processes vary from family to family and how they support or hinder the development of self-control. It will be particularly important to identify interaction sequences that enable children and parents to return to positive synchrony following a rupture and to investigate what aspects of these sequences enable dyads to repair ruptures quickly and efficiently. Alternatively, exploring the nature of ruptures (i.e., interactions that lead to longer amounts of time between repairs; how and why ruptures go unrepaired) reflects another important next step. Further study of dyadic repair mechanisms underlying successful and unsuccessful post-rupture returns to mutually positive interactions will be important to identify individual and dyad-level characteristics that shape these interactive processes and whether these vary based on a family’s exposure to cumulative risk. For example, in dyads with lower risk exposure, parents may be more likely to repair interruptions to positive dyadic interactions, while in higher-risk families this responsibility may more often fall on the child (Skowron, et al., 2010). Identifying thresholds that differentiate shorter latencies associated with healthier developmental outcomes from longer latencies that are more strongly associated with problematic child outcomes may also help to characterize at what point rupture lengths become problematic for long-term child outcomes. Although higher rates of repairing interactive ruptures have been linked to better emotion regulation in children (Kemp et al., 2016), other aspects of interactive rupture and repair, such as onset and timing of repair-initiation and caregiver vs. child-specific roles in repair processes could be examined to continue clarifying impacts on children’s developing self-control. Given that weaker self-control is associated with lower academic achievement, difficulties forming positive peer relationships, and deficits in social-emotional functioning, further research should explore the ways in which interactive coordination helps to support healthy developmental outcomes for children (Bell & Deater-Deckard, 2007; Blair, 2002; Coie et al., 1992; Crandall et al., 2015; Lewit & Baker, 1995; Rueda et al., 2005).

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH Research Grant R01 MH079328 to Dr. Skowron and funded by the National Institute of Mental Health and Administration for Children and Families/Children’s Bureau of the Administration on Children, Youth and Families as part of the Federal Child Neglect Research Consortium. We would like to thank Dr. Dave DeGarmo for his consultation to support our statistical analyses, and Rose Jeffries for her careful copyediting of this manuscript.

References

  1. Appleyard K, Egeland B, van Dulmen MH., & Sroufe LA (2005). When more is not better: The role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 235–245. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00351.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arbuckle JL (2014). Amos (Version 23.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago: IBM SPSS. [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnett D, Manly JT and Cicchetti D (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The interface between policy and research. In: Cicchetti D and Toth SL (Eds.), Child abuse, child development and social policy: Advances in applied developmental psychology (8th ed., pp. 7–73). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corp. [Google Scholar]
  4. Beeghly M, Perry BD, & Tronick E (2016). Self-regulatory processes in early development. In: Maltzman S (Ed.), The oxford handbook of treatment processes and outcomes in psychology: A multidisciplinary, biopsychosocial approach (pp. 42–54). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beeghly M, & Tronick E (2011). Early resilience in the context of parent-infant relationships: A social developmental perspective. Current Problems in Pediatric and Adolescent Health Care, 41(7), 197–201. 10.1016/j.cppeds.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bell MA, & Deater-Deckard K (2007). Biological systems and the development of self-regulation: Integrating behavior, genetics, and psychophysiology. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 28(5), 409–420. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181131fc7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Benjamin LS (1996). A clinician-friendly version of the Interpersonal Circumplex: Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (SASB). Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(2), 248–266. 10.1207/s15327752jpa6602_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamin LS, & Cushing G (2000). Manual for coding social interactions in terms of structural analysis of social behavior. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah. [Google Scholar]
  9. Berthelsen D, Hayes N, White SL, & Williams KE (2017). Executive function in adolescence: Associations with child and family risk factors and self-regulation in early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 903. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Best JR, & Miller PH (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blair C (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57(2), 111–127. 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blair C, & Diamond A (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 899–911. 10.1017/S0954579408000436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT, … Fortunato CK (2011). Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development, 82(6), 1970–1984. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blair C, & Raver CC (2012). Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist, 67(4), 309–318. 10.1037/a0027493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blair C, & Raver CC (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711–731. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Blanz B, Schmidt MH, & Esser G (1991). Familial adversities and child psychiatric disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(6). 939–950. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb01921.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brody GH, & Flor DL (1998). Maternal resources, parenting practices, and child competence in rural, single-parent African American families. Child Development, 69(3), 803–816. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06244.x\ [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brooks-Gunn J, & Duncan GJ (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children, 7(2), 55–71. 10.2307/1602387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brown ED, Ackerman BP, & Moore CA (2013). Family adversity and inhibitory control for economically disadvantaged children: Preschool relations and associations with school readiness. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(3), 443–452. 10.1037/a0032886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Candelaria M, Teti DM, & Black MM (2011). Multi-risk infants: Predicting attachment security from sociodemographic, psychosocial, and health risk among African-American preterm infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(8), 870–877. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02361.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University (2011). Building the brain’s “air traffic control” system: How early experiences shape the development of executive function: Working paper no. 11. Retrieved from https://www.developingchild.harvard.edu
  22. Chaffin M, Silovsky JF, Funderburk B, Valle LA, Brestan EV, Balachova T, … Bonner BL (2004). Parent-Child Interaction Therapy with physically abusive parents: Efficacy for reducing future abuse reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(3), 500–510. 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cipriano EA, Skowron EA, & Gatzke-Kopp (2011). Preschool children’s cardiac reactivity moderates relations between exposure to family violence and emotional adjustment. Child Maltreatment, 6, 205–215. 10.1177/1077559511408887 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cipriano-Essel E, Skowron EA, Stifter CA, & Teti DM (2013). Heterogeneity in maltreated and non-maltreated preschool children’s inhibitory control: The interplay between parenting quality and child temperament. Infant and Child Development, 22(5), 501–522. 10.1002/icd.1801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cohn JF, & Tronick E (1989). Specificity of infants’ response to mothers’ affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(2), 242–248. 10.1097/00004583-198903000-00016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Coie JD, Lochman JE, Terry R, & Hyman C (1992). Predicting early adolescent disorder from childhood aggression and peer rejection. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(5), 783–792. 10.1037/0022-006X.60.5.783 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cole PM, Ram N, & English MS (2018). Toward a unifying model of self-regulation: A developmental approach. Child Development Perspectives, 13(2), 91–96. 10.1111/cdep.12316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Combs-Ronto LA, Olson SL, Lunkenheimer ES, & Sameroff AJ (2009). Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(8), 1151–1163. 10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Crandall A, Deater-Deckard K, & Riley AW (2015). Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 36, 105–126. 10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Davis M, Bilms J, & Suveg C (2017). In sync and in control: A meta-analysis of parent-child positive behavioral synchrony and youth self-regulation. Family Process, 56(4), 962–980. 10.1111/famp.12259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Diamond A, & Lee K (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964. 10.1126/science.1204529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Diamond A, & Taylor C (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to “Do as I say, not as I do”. Developmental Psychobiology, 29(4), 315–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. DiCorcia JA, & Tronick ED (2011). Quotidian resilience: Exploring mechanisms that drive resilience from a perspective of everyday stress and coping. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(7), 1593–1602. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL, Fabes RA, Shepard S, Reiser M, … & Guthrie IK (2001). The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development, 72(4), 1112–1134. 10.1111/1467-8624.00337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Eisenberg N, Michalik N, Spinrad TL, Hofer C, Kupfer A, Valiente C, … & Reiser M (2007). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children’s sympathy: A longitudinal study. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 544–567. 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Eisenberg N, Sadovsky A, Spinrad DL, Fabes RA, Losoya SH, Valiente C, … & Shepard SA (2005). The relations of problem behavior status to children’s negative emotionality, effortful control, and impulsivity: Concurrent relations and prediction of change. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 193–211. 10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, & Eggum ND (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495–525. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Fabes RA, Smith CL, Reiser M, Shepard SA, … & Cumberland AJ (2003). The relations of effortful control and ego control to children’s resiliency and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 761–776. 10.1036/0012-1649.34.9.761 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Cumberland A, Liew J, Resier M, … & Losoya SH (2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 988–1008. 10.1037/a0016213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Eisenberg N, Zhou Q, Spinrad TL, Valiente C, Fabes RA, & Liew J (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76(5), 1055–1071. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00897.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Evans GW, & Kim P (2007). Childhood poverty and health: Cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychological Science, 18(11), 953–957. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02008.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Evans GW, Li D, & Whipple SS (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 1342–1396. 10.1037/a0031808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Eyberg S (1988). Parent-Child Interaction Therapy: Integration of traditional and behavioral concerns. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 10(1), 33–46. 10.1300/J019v10n01_04 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Fabes RA, Eisenberg N, Jones S, Smith M, Guthrie I, Poulin R, … & Friedman J. (1999). Regulation, emotionality, and preschoolers’ socially competent peer interactions. Child Development, 70(2), 432–442. 10.1111/1467-8624.00031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fabes RA, Martin CL, Hanish LD, Anders MC, & Madden-Derdich DA (2003). Early school competence: The roles of sex-segregated play and effortful control. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 848–858. 10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Feldman R (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; Physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3-4), 329–354. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Feldman R (2009). The development of regulatory function from birth to 5 years: Insights from premature infants. Child Development, 80(2), 544–561. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01278.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Feldman R, Greenbaum CW, & Yirmiya N (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 223–231. 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gach EJ, Ip KI, Sameroff AJ, & Olson SL (2018). Early cumulative risk predicts externalizing behaviors at age 10: The mediating role of adverse parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 32(1), 92–102. 10.1037/fam0000360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gerstadt CL, Hong YJ, & Diamond A (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1/2–7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Giuliano RJ, Skowron EA, & Berkman ET (2015). Growth models of dyadic synchrony and mother-child vagal tone in the context of parenting at-risk. Biological Psychology, 29(8), 29–36. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Goldfine ME, Wagner SM, Branstetter SA, & McNeil CB (2008). Parent-Child Interaction Therapy: An examination of cost-effectiveness. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 5(1), 119–141. 10.1037/h0100414 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Grolnick WS, & Farkas M (2002). Parenting and the development of children’s self-regulation. In: Bornstein MH (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Practical issues in parenting (5th ed., pp. 89–110). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  54. Harrist AW, Pettit GS, Dodge KA, & Bates JE (1994). Dyadic synchrony in mother child interaction: Relation with children’s subsequent kindergarten adjustment. Family Relations, 43(4), 417–424. 10.2307/585373 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Harrist AW, & Waugh RM (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children’s development. Developmental Review, 22(4), 555–592. 10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00500-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hoffman C, Crnic KA, & Baker JK (2006). Maternal depression and parenting: Implications for children’s emergent emotion regulation and behavioral functioning. Parenting Science and Practice, 6(4), 271–295. 10.1207/s15327922par0604_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hostinar CE, Stellern SA, Schaefer C, Carlson SM, & Gunnar MR (2012). Associations between early life adversity and executive function in children adopted internationally from orphanages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Supplement 2), 17208–17212. 10.1073/pnas.1121246109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hu LT, & Bentler PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Jones DJ, Forehand R, Brody G, & Armistad L (2002). Psychosocial adjustment of African American children in single-mother families: A test of three risk models. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(1), 105–115. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00105.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kemp CJ, Lunkenheimer E, Albrecht EC, & Chen D (2016). Can we fix this? Parent–child repair processes and preschoolers’ regulatory skills. Family Relations, 65(4), 576–590. 10.1111/fare.12213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kennedy JH, & Bakeman R (1984). The early mother-infant relationship and social competence with peers and adults at three years. The Journal of Psychology, 116(1), 23–34. 10.1080/00223980.1984.9923612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kim S, & Brody GH (2005). Longitudinal pathways to psychological adjustment among Black youth living in single-parent households. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 305–313. 10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kim S, Brody GH, & McBride Murry V (2003). Longitudinal links between contextual risks, parenting, and youth outcomes in rural African American families. Journal of Black Psychology, 29(4), 359–377. 10.1177/0095798403256887 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kim S, & Kochanska G (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent-child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83(4), 1275–1289. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01778.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kochanska G, Aksan N, Penney SJ, & Boldt LJ (2007). Parental personality as an inner resource that moderates the impact of ecological adversity on parenting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 136–150. 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kochanska G, Coy KC, & Murray KT (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091–1111. 10.1111/1467-8624.00336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kochanska G, & Kim S (2013). Difficult temperament moderates links between maternal responsiveness and children’s compliance and behavior problems in low-income families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(3), 323–332. 10.1111/jcpp.12002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kochanska G, & Knaack A (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71(6), 1087–1112. 10.1111/1467-6494.7106008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kochanska G, Murray K, & Coy KC (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68(2), 263–277. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01939.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kochanska G, Murray KT, & Harlan ET (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220–232. 10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kopp CB (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: a developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 199–214. 10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Labella MH, Narayan AJ, McCormick CM, Desjardins CD, & Masten AS (2017). Risk and adversity, parenting quality, and children’s social-emotional adjustment in families experiencing homelessness. Child Development, 90(1), 227–244. 10.1111/cdev.12894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lengua LJ, Honorado E, & Bush NR (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 40–55. 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Lengua LJ, Moran L, Zalewski M, Ruberry E, Kiff C, & Thompson S (2015). Relations of growth in effortful control to family income, cumulative risk, and adjustment in preschool-age children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(4), 705–720. 10.1007/s10802-014-9941-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lewit EM, & Baker LS (1995). School readiness. The Future of Children, 5(2), 128–139. 10.2307/1602361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lindsey EW, Cremeens PR, Colwell MJ, & Caldera YM (2009). The structure of parent-child dyadic synchrony in toddlerhood and children’s communication competence and self-control. Social Development, 18(2), 375–396. 10.1111/j.14679507.2008.00489.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mackinnon DP (2008). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  78. McCabe LA, Rebello-Britto P, Hernandez M, & Brooks-Gunn J (2004). Games children play: Observing young children’s self-regulation across laboratory, home, and school settings. In DelCarmen-Wiggins R & Carter A (Eds.), Handbook of infant, toddler, and preschool mental health assessment (pp. 491–521). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  79. McDermott JM, Westerlund A, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA, & Fox NA (2012). Early adversity and neural correlates of executive function: Implications for academic adjustment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, S59–S66. 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Merz EC, Landry SH, Montroy JJ, & Williams JM (2017). Bidirectional associations between parental responsiveness and executive functioning during early childhood. Social Development, 26(3), 591–609. 10.1111/sode.12204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mistry RS, Benner AD, Biesanz JC, Clark SL, & Howes C (2010). Family and social risk, and parental investments during the early childhood years as predictors of low-income children’s school readiness outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), 432–449. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.01.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Müller M, Zietlow AL, Tronick E, & Reck C (2015). What dyadic reparation is meant to do: An association with infant cortisol reactivity. Psychopathology, 48(6), 386–399. 10.1159/000439225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Obradović J, Yousafzai AK, Finch JE, & Rasheed MA (2016). Maternal scaffolding and home stimulation: Key mediators of early intervention effects on children’s cognitive development. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1409–1421. 10.1037/dev0000182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Olson SL, & Lunkenheimer ES (2009). Expanding concepts of self-regulation to social relationships: Transactional processes in the development of early behavioral adjustment. In: Sameroff A (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp.3–21). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  85. Pears KC, Fisher PA, Bruce J, Kim HK, & Yoerger K (2010). Early elementary school adjustment of maltreated children in foster care: The roles of inhibitory control and caregiver involvement. Child Development, 81(5), 1550–1564. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01491.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Perry RE, Braren SH, & Blair C (2018). Socioeconomic risk and school readiness: Longitudinal mediation through children’s social competence and executive function. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1544. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01544 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pettit GS, & Mize J (1993). Substance and style: Understanding the ways in which parents teach children about social relationships. In Duck S (Ed.), Understanding relationship processes series: Learning about relationships (2nd ed., pp. 118–151). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  88. Posner MI, & Rothbart MK (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 427–441. 10.1017/S0954579400003096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Raver CC (1996). Relations between social contingency in mother-child interaction and 2-year-olds’ social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 850–859. 10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.850 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  90. Richters JE, & Saltzman W (1990). Survey of exposure to community violence: Self-report version. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health. [Google Scholar]
  91. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, & Fisher P (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72(5), 1394–1408. 10.1111/1467-8624.00355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Rothbart MK, & Rueda MR (2005). The development of effortful control. In Mayr U, rotAwh E, & Keele S (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner (pp. 167–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  93. Ruberry EJ, Klein MR, Kiff CJ, Thompson SF, & Lengua LJ (2018). Parenting as a moderator of the effects of cumulative risk on children’s social-emotional adjustment and academic readiness. Infant and Child Development, 27(3), 1. 10.1002/icd.2071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Rueda MR, Posner MI, & Rothbart MK (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 573–594. 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Rutter M (1979). Protective factors in children’s responses to stress and disadvantage. Annals of the Academy of Medicine, 8(3), 324–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Rutter M (1981). Stress, coping, and development: Some issues and some questions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22(4), 323–356. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1981.tb00560.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Ryan RM, & Deci EL (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. https://doi.org/10.1037110003-066X.55.1.68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sameroff A (2009). The transactional model. In: Sameroff A (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp.3–21). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  99. Sameroff AJ, & MacKenzie MJ (2003). Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 613–640. 10.1017/S0954579403000312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Sameroff AJ, & Rosenblum KL (2006). Psychosocial constraints on the development of resilience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094(1), 116–124. 10.1196/annals.1376.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sarason IG, Johnson JH, & Siegel JM (1978). Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the Life Experiences Survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(5), 932–946. 10.1037/0022-006X.46.5.932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Shaw DS, Bell RQ, & Gilliom M (2000). A truly early starter model of antisocial behavior revisited. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3(3), 155–172. 10.1023/A:1009599208790 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Skowron EA (2015). Understanding autonomic physiology and relationship processes in high-risk families. In Noone RJ & Papero DV (Eds.), The family emotional system: An integrative concept for theory, science, and practice (pp. 237–256). Lanham, MD: Lexington Books. [Google Scholar]
  104. Skowron EA, Cipriano-Essel E, Gatzke-Kopp LM, Teti DM, & Ammerman RT (2014). Early adversity, RSA, and inhibitory control: Evidence of children’s neurobiological sensitivity to social context. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 964–978. 10.1002/dev.21175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Skowron EA, Kozlowski JM, & Pincus AL (2010). Differentiation, self–other representations, and rupture–repair processes: Predicting child maltreatment risk. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57(3), 304–316. 10.1037/a0020030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Song J, Miller AL, Leung CY, Lumeng JC, & Rosenblum KL (2018). Positive parenting moderates the association between temperament and self-regulation in low income toddlers. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(7), 2354–2364. 10.1007/s10826-018-1066-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Fabes RA, Valiente C, Shepard SA, … & Guthrie IK (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children’s social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 6(3), 498–510. 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Gaertner B, Popp T, Smith CL, Kupfer A, … & Hofer C (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1170–1186. 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Sroufe AL (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  110. Thompson RA (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 25–52. 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Thomas R, & Zimmer-Gembeck MJ (2011). Accumulating evidence for Parent-Child Interaction Therapy in the prevention of child maltreatment. Child Development, 82(1), 177–192. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01548.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Tofighi D, & MacKinnon DP (2011). RMedation: An R package for mediation analysis confidence intervals. Bheavior Research Methods, 43(3), 692–700. 10.3758/s13428-011-0076-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Tomalski P, Marczuk K, Pisula E, Malinowska A, Kawa R, & Niedźwiecka A (2017). Chaotic home environment is associated with reduced infant processing speed under high task demands. Infant Behavior and Development, 48, 124–133. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Trentacosta CJ, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Gardner F, & Wilson M (2008). The relations among cumulative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1211–1219. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01941.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Tronick EZ (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44(2), 112–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Tronick EZ (2007). Interactive mismatch and repair: Challenges to the coping infant. In: Tronick EZ (Ed.), The neurobehavioral and social-emotional development of infants and children (pp.155–163). New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  117. Valiente C, Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Shepard SA, Cumberland AJ, & Losoya SH (2004). Prediction of children’s empathy-related responding from their effortful controls and parents’ expressivity. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 911–926. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, & Castro KS (2007). Children’s effortful control and academic competence. Mediation through school liking. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53(1), 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  119. Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Swanson J, & Reiser M (2008). Prediction of children’s academic competence from their effortful control, relationships, and classroom participation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 67–77. 10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.67 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Vohs KD, & Baumeister RF (Eds.). (2016). Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  121. Wothke W (1998). Longitudinal and multi-group modeling with missing data. In Little TD, Schnabel KU, & Baumert J (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal and multiple group data: Practical issues, applied approaches, and specific examples. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES