Abstract
Several new technologies have emerged promising new Magnetoencephalography (MEG) systems in which the sensors can be placed close to the scalp. One such technology, Optically Pumped MEG (OP-MEG) allows for a scalp mounted system that provides measurements within millimetres of the scalp surface. A question that arises in developing on-scalp systems is: how many sensors are necessary to achieve adequate performance/spatial discrimination? There are many factors to consider in answering this question such as the signal to noise ratio (SNR), the locations and depths of the sources, density of spatial sampling, sensor gain errors (due to interference, subject movement, cross-talk, etc.) and, of course, the desired spatial discrimination. In this paper, we provide simulations which show the impact these factors have on designing sensor arrays for wearable MEG. While OP-MEG has the potential to provide high information content at dense spatial samplings, we find that adequate spatial discrimination of sources (< 1 cm) can be achieved with relatively few sensors (< 100) at coarse spatial samplings (~ 30 mm) at high SNR. After this point approximately 50 more sensors are required for every 1 mm improvement in spatial discrimination. Comparable discrimination for traditional cryogenic systems require more channels by these same metrics. We also show that sensor gain errors have the greatest impact on discrimination between deep sources at high SNR. Finally, we also examine the limitation that aliasing due to undersampling has on the effective SNR of on-scalp sensors.
Subject terms: Magnetoencephalography, Image processing
Introduction
Magnetoencephalography (MEG) has become a vital tool for studying human brain function in both basic and clinical settings1–4. Simulation studies show that MEG system performance continues to improve with an increase in channel count as long as channel noise is uncorrelated5. There is however a balance between system cost and channel redundancy. Based on work over the last 20 years5–8, the design of whole-head Superconducting Quantum Interference Device (SQUID) based systems stabilized to around 300 channels. While source localization performance has a strong dependence on channel count5, there is a complex interplay between the methods used to invert the lead-field matrix, selection of a forward model, sampling density, and the geometry of the source space6,9–15.
These issues have come back into focus with emergence of new MEG system technologies. These include the advent of high critical temperature SQUIDs (high Tc-SQUIDS), nitrogen vacancy magnetometers and optically pumped magnetometers (OPMs)16–21. These new sensors allow for the construction of MEG arrays that can be brought closer to the scalp (within a few mm) and in the case of OPMs, offer the flexibility to image human brain function during subject movement22–24 ( with the crucial addition of field nulling coils25–27). From here on we refer to these methods collectively as on-scalp MEG as opposed to typical helium-based cryogenic systems, which are often displaced some distance (~ 20 mm) from the scalp (off-scalp system).
As on-scalp sensors are a typically placed much closer to the scalp than off-scalp sensors, they measure a three to fivefold increase in signal15,28. In principle, this should offer the capability of resolving neuronal activity with a higher spatial discrimination as smaller spatial wavelengths can potentially be sampled6,29. Indeed recent work30 has shown that on-scalp systems would benefit from up to 300 spatial samples in order capture these small spatial wavelengths. An important point to note is that sparser arrays could result in under-sampling (of the high spatial frequency features) and this will give rise to unexplained noise due to signal aliasing.
Here we argue that for a wide variety of neuroscience applications, maximising spatial discrimination, although desirable, is not absolutely necessary. For instance, in paediatric epilepsy surgery, a crucial clinical application of MEG31–36 a large percentage of brain volume (~ 5–10%, equivalent to a 2–3 cm radius sphere) may be resected37. The clinical question does not require the spatial discrimination between sources at a millimetre scale. In the case of basic neuroscience applications that average results across subjects their spatial discrimination is limited by the functional and anatomical variability that exists between subjects38–41. Furthermore, in studies which investigate electrophysiological functional connectomes, the spatial discrimination required may be on the scale of 1–2 cm, as a single time course representing an entire atlas-defined parcel is often utilised42. Rather than strive to maximize spatial discrimination or information content, an alternative question is “given a desired discrimination what is the sampling density/ sensor number I require?” We approach the question of discrimination in a statistical framework by asking “at which sensor density one is able to confidently (p < 0.05) distinguish between two competing source models”43–45. By adopting this approach we can design MEG arrays around a given scientific question (or spatial discrimination criterion) and therefore minimize channel count. We note that the answer to this question will be conditional on Signal to Noise Ratio (SNR), which itself will be a function of any aliasing observed due to undersampling.
In this work we also consider the issue of sensor calibration. Unlike SQUID based systems, where the feedback electronics maintains a constant linear relationship to applied flux, optically pumped magnetometers are particularly vulnerable to external factors which give rise to a change in their gain. Perhaps the most pernicious of these errors is due to the inherent nonlinear response of the sensor21. When operating in the spin exchange relaxation Free (SERF) regime these sensors are only linear within a few nT of zero field22. These gain errors can be caused by subject movement through a non-zero background field or by the change in the ambient magnetic field over time26,27. Some form of active shielding27,46 or closed-loop feedback would typically be required to minimize these issues. Another issue is how the sensors interact with each other in multichannel systems. On-board coils that produce magnetic fields for field zeroing or amplitude modulation20,47 may change the gain of a nearby sensor. This problem is static (if the sensors do not move relative to one another) and deterministic but assumes that a suitable calibration procedure is in place to correct for these issues48. Additionally the use of custom designed on-board coils can reduce the impact of this issue49.
The paper proceeds as follows. First we outline the model comparison framework and the competing models used to define spatial discrimination. We examine these discrimination estimates for different sensor numbers under different SNR conditions for deep and shallow dipolar sources using on-scalp sensors. We use the same methods to investigate cryogenic sensors layouts and show, in accord with previous studies50, that comparable discrimination for on-scalp sensors is achievable with fewer sensors. We also examine the impact of sensor gain error21,22,26 on discrimination. Finally, we consider the effects of aliasing due to undersampling.
Methods
Simulating MEG data
All simulations and source reconstructions are based on fields generated by single dipolar sources. We simulate on-scalp MEG data based on sensors offset by 6.5 mm from the scalp. For comparison, we also simulate off-scalp MEG with identical simulation parameters except that the offset from the scalp is 20 mm. Each dipole source was given a 10 Hz sinusoidal profile and recorded for a single epoch of 1000 ms. The sensor level SNR of the response was manipulated by changing the source amplitude from 1 to 10 nAm to 100 nAm assuming a 100 fT noise standard deviation (uncorrelated across sensors) in all cases (corresponding to a noise floor in 100 Hz bandwidth).
The mesh used to generate these datasets was the MNI canonical mesh available in SPM12. The separation between vertices is approximately 3 mm on average. The orientation of the source is defined by the surface normal of the cortical mesh at that location. The forward model was the Nolte single shell model51 and sensors were assumed to be point magnetometers. A total of 50 different array designs were considered with inter sensor spacing varying between 10 and 60 mm (the method of array design is covered in “Array design” section).
For each of the 50 array designs, 40 different datasets were generated. Each dataset contained data from a different single dipolar source with added noise. The locations of these brain regions were chosen to vary in depth and location across the whole brain. To meet this aim we selected the 20 deepest sources and the 20 most superficial sources from the mesh. To ensure the sources varied in position around the brain, we ensured that no more than 1 source from each AAL atlas region52 was chosen (Fig. 3).
We also added 3 levels of gain error to the simulated data. The gain errors were assumed to unrelated for each sensor and drawn from a normal distribution of mean 0 and standard deviation 0% (zero gain error), 2.5% and 10% of the nominal gain. To limit computation time, the exploration of gain errors was examined at a single level of spatial sampling (~ 30 mm) with 30 repetitions.
In summary, the simulated data consisted of 6000 field topographies simulated from 40 different brain regions, for 50 different levels of spatial sampling, at 3 different SNRs for both on and off-scalp systems.
Source estimation, model comparison and spatial discrimination
Source estimation
Here we define a metric of spatial discrimination as the Euclidian distance at which we can confidently (p < 0.05) distinguish between the magnetic field patterns generated by two sources on the cortical mesh. We note that we could use other measures of distances such as geodesic distance. However, this may result in situations where one appears to be able to discriminate at short geodesic distances (high curvature area) but not at larger ones (either side of gyri where lead fields will be similar). Using Euclidean distance should therefore lead to a more conservative estimate of the minimum distance at which sources can be discriminated.
For estimating the sources we use an Empirical Bayesian Beamformer53 as implemented in SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). This method has been used in a similar context with real data to distinguish between models of cortical anatomy54 and models with missing or displaced hippocampal anatomy55. The advantage of this method is that gives us a model evidence (free energy) value to judge the quality of the data fit (and so can be used with real or simulated data). We note that there is a significant interaction between the choice of inversion method (or assumptions) and how the data are simulated (smoothness, correlation between sources, level of correlated interference etc.). Here we explicitly simulate data that conforms to the assumptions of the inversion method so as to factor out the choice of inversion method in the following analyses. It is also therefore a best-case scenario.
Model comparison and spatial discrimination
Here we use Bayesian model comparison56 to probabilistically formalise a metric of spatial discrimination. In each case we compare a base and a target model. The base model contains the source and all neighbouring potential sources (within a certain radius), the target model is identical to the source model but with a void (excluded sources) around the true source location. This void can be made progressively larger to establish the ability of distant sources to explain the data. It is possible to explain the MEG data using both models, however the current distribution on the target model must always be more complex or less accurate than the current estimate on the base model (as the generator source is not present). The increase in complexity and/or decrease in accuracy is quantified in the model evidence of the solution (as approximated by free energy). The free energy difference between any two models is how likely (on log scale) one model is over the other. Differences in log model evidence (or free energy) of greater than 3 suggest that one model is 20 times more likely than the other. The idea is to increase the radius of the void until the explanation of the data provided by the target model is significantly (20 times) less likely than that provided by the base model. This radius of the void at which we get a significant difference between models is our metric of spatial discrimination.
Here we begin with a ring of void radius 3 mm. We increase this distance within which vertices are excluded (4, 5, 6 mm etc.) from around the true source. We keep doing this until we reach a point where we can confidently discriminate the models. The target model that is 20 times (or log difference of 3) less likely than the base model defines the spatial discrimination of a given array. This process is then repeated for each level of spatial sampling (10–60 mm in 1 mm steps) for 3 levels of signal amplitude (1, 10 and 100 nAm). This process is repeated 40 times for different brain regions in the mesh. The final results are then averaged across brain regions to obtain an average relationship between spatial discrimination and spatial sampling as a function of signal amplitude.
Practically, when we implement this approach, we do not invert the data on to the entire cortical mesh. Instead we invert the data onto a limited source space which includes the true sources and all sources within 20 mm of the true sources. This results, on average, in 420 potential sources for each base model. This model is compared successively against models that have sources within X mm (3, 4, 5… 20 mm) excluded. These models effectively form rings around the true source (see Fig. 1). This is primarily to reduce computation time. It also makes the results slightly more conservative (the spatial discrimination is slightly better if more cortex is included in both models). The explicit steps in the approach can be broken down as follows:
(1) Different generative models of the same data are proposed. The “base model” includes a source and all vertices within 20 mm of the source (top-left panel Fig. 1). All other models do not include the source (or the lead-field elements mapping sensors to the simulated location) or any potential sources (or lead-field mappings) within a void surrounding the true source (see Fig. 1). The void radius is gradually increased in 1 mm steps up to 20 mm (top panels Fig. 1). (2) The base model is used to generate a single dataset. (3) Each candidate model is inverted (using the limited source space, 20 mm radius) to explain the source-generated dataset and a free energy (or log model evidence) value is obtained. Importantly, only the base-model includes the generating source. (4) The point at which the free energy drops to − 3 (the point at which distant vertices offer an explanation that is 20 times worse than the base model) defines the spatial discrimination of the array. Similar approaches have been used elsewhere to define the discriminability of different hippocampal43 and cortical mesh models57. A more thorough introduction to the use of free energy in source inversion is given elsewhere58.
Array design
We use a point packing algorithm (Fig. 2) to position sensors on the scalp surface at increasing densities (e.g. 20, 15, 10 mm separation). There are 5 steps to the algorithm and the approach is depicted graphically in Fig. 2. (1) Initially the bounding box of the surface is subdivided into squares of equal area with an edge length equal to the desired sampling density. (2) The corners of the squares are then projected onto the surface to initialise the algorithm at approximately the correct sampling density. (3) In the optimisation stage, at each loop iteration, a sensor is chosen at random and moved to a neighbouring vertex if this brings the sampling density closer to the target sampling density (measured with Euclidean distance). (4) If there are any vertices on the surface that are farther from the nearest sensor than the target spacing, a sensor is added to that vertex. (5) This process is repeated for a large (~ 10,000) number of iterations. For the purpose of the simulations performed here the lowest sensor is positioned at the same Z coordinate (inferior-superior axis) as the most inferior part of the brain. We note an alternate approach would be to use geodesic distance instead of Euclidean distance. We choose not to as the discrepancy between the methods is only noticeable, for smooth surfaces, at large inter sensor spacing (sensor spacing > > than radius of head). The code required to create these arrays is available via GitHub (https://github.com/tierneytim/OPM).
Considering the effects of aliasing
Here we simulate independent Gaussian noise on each sensor. However, in reality there will be noise variance due to brain activity unrelated to the experimental manipulation or signal of interest. These sources, in particular those proximal to the sensors59, have the potential to introduce noise due to the aliasing of (undersampled) higher spatial frequencies. This in turn will place a limit on the effective SNR for source reconstruction. We therefore characterise the effects of aliasing when one undersamples a given topography in two ways. First, we explore how much variance a given channel count can explain in the eigenmodes derived from a high density lead field covariance (700 sensors and 20,000 brain regions separated by approximately 3 mm). These eigenmodes are obtained by singular value decomposition of the high density lead field covariance matrix. Second, we characterise how much variance a given channel count can explain in each of the 20,000 lead fields.
For a given topography an available channel count is set. By way of example we will use 100 sensors as the channel count. To initialise the algorithm we randomly sample 100 points from the set of ~ 700 potential sensor locations defined by the high density lead field. The remaining 600 empty points are filled in by nearest neighbour interpolation. This provides us with a high density “reconstructed” lead field. The squared correlation coefficient between the reconstructed lead field and the true lead field defines the variance explained. The variance unexplained is an approximation for a worst-case aliased variance. To transform this variance explained to a worst case sensor-level SNR (in a regime limited by aliasing) we can calculate it as . This assumes the variance explained () is the signal and the variance unexplained is the noise () and the random sensor noise is minimal.
To optimise these initial random positions a single sensor position is randomly selected. It is then moved to a neighbouring position if that move increases the variance explained. This process is repeated until the method converges on a given channel layout that maximises variance explained/minimises aliasing. The results of this method are, by their very nature, stochastic and subject to finding local minima. As such, they can be considered worst case scenarios for considering the effects of aliasing (correlated, proximal and superficial sources). We also point out that using a nearest neighbour interpolation is by no means optimal but is useful for providing an approximate estimate of the expected aliasing quickly.
Use of experimental animals/and human participants
No experimental animals or human subjects participated in this study.
Results
Source locations and lead field rank
We first provide a descriptive account of the source models used in this study. We highlight the locations of the brain regions (Fig. 3A) used to generate the data used in these simulations (and the associated SNRs) as well as the variance explained as a function of lead-field rank (Fig. 3B) for our most densely sampled lead field (~ 700 sensors × 20,000 source locations). It is important to note that over 95% of the lead field variance can be explained with 50 spatial components and 99% of the variance can be explained with ~ 100 components. This same relationship has also been observed elsewhere30.
Depth, SNR, spatial sampling and spatial discrimination: on scalp and off scalp
The results of simulations described in “Source estimation, model comparison and spatial discrimination” section are graphically represented in Fig. 4. The white contour lines indicate the point at which nearby sources can be discriminated for each candidate array. For example, for shallow sources (left column) at moderate SNR (middle row) the ability to discriminate sources at less than 5 mm, reading from the F = 3 (or p < 0.05 contour line) would require more than 200 channels (spatial sampling 20 mm). However, if one wished to discriminate sources at 10 mm (for the same moderate SNR at approximately 30 mm spacing) only 87 sensors would be required. The same curves for typical cryogenic configurations (sensors offset from the scalp by 20 mm) are shown in Supplementary Fig. S1. In this case the trends are the same except that more channels are required in order to reach the same discrimination performance.
Number of sensors
We can graph the relationship between spatial discrimination and spatial sampling for on-scalp and off-scalp systems (displaced by 20 mm) at a given SNR (Fig. 5). Consistent with previous work50, we found that to achieve the same spatial discrimination on-scalp systems require fewer sensors than their off-scalp counterparts for a given signal level. For example, to achieve 10 mm spatial discrimination between sources, ~ 40 rather than ~ 70 channels are required for on-scalp vs. off-scalp sensors respectively at high SNR, while ~ 150 on scalp sensors are required for moderate SNR in comparison to more than 500 sensors for the off-scalp system. Interestingly for on-scalp systems (10,100 nAm conditions), we note that beyond 100 sensors, at least 50 sensors are required for every 1 mm improvement in discrimination. The purpose of this paper is not necessarily to highlight how good an on-scalp system could be (if one had infinite sensors) but to highlight the trade off in sensor number and discriminability, which is a relatively modest return (~ 50 sensors per mm improvement) after 100 sensors.
The impact of gain errors on spatial discrimination
From the previous section we can conclude that there was only a modest return as the number of scalp sensors increased beyond 100. In this section, we fix the sensor number at 100 and examine the effect of calibration or gain errors on spatial discrimination. The simulations include 30 repetitions of 3 different gain errors (0%, 2.5% and 10%) for 3 different signal levels (1 nAm, 10 nAm, 100 nAm with 100 fT standard deviation sensor level noise). Figure 6 shows the spatial discrimination that can be achieved for different levels of gain errors. It can be seen that the gain errors have the biggest impact at high SNR. Note the divergence between the 0% (stars) and 10% (dotted) gain error curves for 100 nAm (red). Interestingly the gain errors appear more detrimental for deeper sources. For deeper sources (right panel) at high SNR (red curves) achievable discrimination degrades from 10 to 16 mm at 10% gain error. At moderate and low levels of SNR the effects of gain errors are masked by the noise. These gain errors would be analogous to random sensor orientation errors of ~ 0, 13, and 26 degrees respectively when the field on the other axes is small (assuming a cosinusoidal dependence between orientation errors and gain errors, e.g. acos (0.975) × 180/pi = ~ 13 degrees).
The effects of aliasing
We have characterised the relationship between spatial sampling and spatial discrimination as a function of SNR to highlight a trade-off between spatial discrimination and channel count. This trade-off can lead to situations where brain activity is under-sampled and therefore aliases. This aliasing will manifest as noise that places an effective limit on the SNR. In Fig. 7A we can see that aliasing reaches approximately 4–5% for between 60 and 100 sensors for point sources. For cortical generators with some spatial extent this percentage decreases marginally. The marked decrease for generators of 50 mm FWHM is interesting but perhaps not physiologically reasonable. This 4–5% aliased variance corresponds to sensor level SNR (magnitude) of between 4.4 and 4.9 (~ 13–14 dB). We can conservatively extrapolate from Fig. 4 (10 nAm, shallow source corresponding to SNR of 8 dB) that in the aliased regime spatial discrimination would be limited to approximately 10–15 mm for 50–100 sensors. In Fig. 7B we decompose the aliasing into its regional contribution for our suggested channel count of 100. We can see that the vast majority of brain regions (95% interval) can be sampled with 100 sensors to have between 6 and 1% aliasing. This same information is represented in Fig. 7C on the brain’s surface. Unsurprisingly, the areas that contribute most to the aliasing are the more superficial sources (although these are few in number).
Discussion
We have presented a probabilistic model comparison framework to quantify spatial discrimination of competing sensor arrays. We show that spatial discrimination of 1 cm requires relatively few on-scalp sensors (< 100). This corresponds to an inter-sensor distance of approximately 3 cm. For this sensor layout, gain errors have the greatest impact discrimination at high SNR but its effects are masked at moderate or low SNR. We also explore the potential impact of noise due to aliasing on these sparse arrays and find that this could account for between 1 and 6% of signal variance.
Here we show that adequate discrimination performance for on-scalp sensors is achievable even though we are far from the theoretical limits of spatial discrimination for these sensors. Indeed recent studies30,60 have shown that, due to the higher spatial frequency fields now measurable, to fully exploit the potential of OPM systems one would require of the order 300 sensors. That being said, we show even when SNR is limited by a large degree of aliasing (e.g. 15% corresponding to ~ 8 dB SNR limit—Fig. 4, left column, second row) then spatial discrimination will be limited to be between 10 and 15 mm for between 50 and 100 sensors. This problem will be most acute when the sensor level noise (random noise) is small in comparison to the aliased signal. This can occur after averaging data arising from highly covariant sources or when the sensor has a much lower noise floor than the background spontaneous activity of the brain (e.g. in the 1–20 Hz band). In cases where this spontaneous activity is not correlated with experimental design this source of variability should reduce with averaging, assuming stationarity of the neural response61. A straightforward approach to mitigate this problem is to consider non-uniform sampling arrays specific to experimental or clinical regions of interest59.
We have made several idealistic assumptions. Practically, achievable discrimination performance may be bounded by other factors. For example it has been shown that forward modelling errors limit discrimination at high SNR50,62. Here, we assume that the source model is known and we have created and reconstructed the data under the same forward models. Other studies have looked at these models in more detail63,64 and we do not examine this factor directly. That said, increasing the channel number will not solve these issues. We had initially expected gain errors to have similar consequences to forward modelling errors but it appears these are much less pernicious. This is because the gain changes have been considered independent across sensors (rather than systematic for lead-field errors) and indeed we found that for practical OPM gain errors (± 2%) there were negligible effects on discrimination performance, similar to random coregistration errors65. However, there may be instances when gain changes have some correlated spatial structure across the array (for example as the subject moves through a magnetic field gradient) and this would most likely be more detrimental to the source modelling. We should point out that here we have focused on source reconstruction. However, some studies which operate in sensor space exclusively, for example studies using Multivariate Decoding66, will likely have different performance constraints.
The model comparison framework could be extended in a number of ways. For instance, it could be used to target specific brain regions or to compare other wearable brain imaging systems such as EEG. A more thorough comparison of EEG and on-scalp sensors has been performed elsewhere29,60. There are other ways to define spatial discrimination such as low correlation between sources15,50 or examination of point-spread functions considered resolved50. However, defining spatial discrimination is arguably a statistical problem. The proposed statistical framework has also been used extensively to model laminar sources44,67,68 in MEG as well as make probabilistic statements about deep structure activity43,69 where spatial precision is paramount. This probabilistic nature of variational Bayes is a strong motivation for adopting such a framework for assessing spatial discrimination. While the variational framework allows for approximate Bayesian inference in an efficient and accurate way56,70 it is important to recognise it does so under some key assumptions. The primary assumption is that the distribution of estimated parameters is well approximated by a Gaussian around its mode. This can be broken if the true underlying distribution has a mode that is not near the majority of the probability mass (heavy tailed distribution or multimodal distributions). In practice however it can be shown that in a large data limit this is of limited concern71.
We have provided a simulation-based framework for users of OPMs to establish how many sensors are required for neuroscientific applications to achieve a desired spatial discrimination. We find adequate discrimination (< 1 cm) can be obtained with relatively few sensors (< 100) at coarse spatial samplings (> 30 mm) at high SNR for both deep and superficial sources. Beyond this point, improving discrimination comes at a cost of nearly 50 sensors per mm improvement. Furthermore, aliasing due to undersampling (which is correlated with the experimental design) may place an effective limit on the SNR available for spatial discrimination. For between 50 and 100 sensors we expect this aliasing to typically be between 2 and 6% and rarely exceeding 10%. This limits the achievable discrimination to between 5 and 15 mm spatial discrimination for these channel counts. Finally, we also demonstrate that the dominant determinant of spatial discrimination in an OPM system is not gain changes/calibration errors but the magnitude of the physiological response and the depth of the source.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Wellcome collaborative award to GRB, MJB and RB (203257/Z/16/Z, 203257/B/16/Z). SM was funded through the EPSRC-funded UCL Centre for Doctoral Training in Medical Imaging (EP/L016478/1). The Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome (203147/Z/16/Z). The authors gratefully acknowledge funding from the Medical Research Council and Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council grant MR/K6010/86010/1, the Medical Research Council UKMEG Partnership Grant MR/K005464/1 and the BBSRC research grant BB/M009645/1.
Author contributions
The study was designed and conceived by T.M.T., G.R.B., S.M. and G.C.O.N. Analysis was performed by T.M.T. Interpretation of results was provided by M.J.B., R.B., N.H., E.B., J.L., G.R. and R.H. All Authors Contributed to the writing of the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-77589-8.
References
- 1.Hämäläinen MS, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV. Magnetoencephalography—theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasivee studies of the working human brain. Rev. Mod. Phys. 1993;65:413–505. doi: 10.1103/RevModPhys.65.413. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Baillet S. Magnetoencephalography for brain electrophysiology and imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:327–339. doi: 10.1038/nn.4504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bagic AI, Bowyer SM, Kirsch HE, Funke ME, Burgess RC. American clinical MEG society (ACMEGS) position statement #2: the value of magnetoencephalography (MEG)/magnetic source imaging (MSI) in noninvasive presurgical mapping of eloquent cortices of patients preparing for surgical interventions. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017;34:189–195. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rampp S, et al. Magnetoencephalography for epileptic focus localization in a series of 1000 cases. Brain. 2019 doi: 10.1093/brain/awz231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vrba J, Robinson SE. SQUID sensor array configurations for magnetoencephalography applications. Supercond. Sci. Technol. Top. 2002;15:51–89. doi: 10.1088/0953-2048/15/9/201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ahonen AI, et al. Sampling theory for neuromagnetic detector arrays. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1993;40:859–869. doi: 10.1109/10.245606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kemppainen PK, Ilmoniemi RJ. Channel capacity of multichannel magnetometers. In: Williamson SJ, Hoke M, Stroink G, Kotani M, editors. Advances in Biomagnetism. Boston: Springer; 1989. pp. 635–638. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Vrba J, Robinson SE, McCubbin J. How many channels are needed for MEG? Neurol. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;2004:99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hauk O, Stenroos M, Treder M. Towards an objective evaluation of EEG/MEG Source estimation methods: the linear tool kit Olaf. bioArxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/672956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Fawcett IP, Singh KD. Realistic spatial sampling for MEG beamformer images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2004;23:120–127. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brookes MJ, et al. Investigating spatial specificity and data averaging in MEG. Neuroimage. 2010;49:525–538. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hedrich T, Pellegrino G, Kobayashi E, Lina JM, Grova C. Comparison of the spatial resolution of source imaging techniques in high-density EEG and MEG. Neuroimage. 2017;157:531–544. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sekihara K, Sahani M, Nagarajan SS. Localization bias and spatial resolution of adaptive and non-adaptive spatial filters for MEG source reconstruction. Neuroimage. 2008 doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-79370-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hillebrand A, Barnes GR. A quantitative assessment of the sensitivity of whole-head MEG to activity in the adult human cortex. Neuroimage. 2002;16:638–650. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Iivanainen J, Stenroos M, Parkkonen L. Measuring MEG closer to the brain: performance of on-scalp sensor arrays. Neuroimage. 2017;147:542–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hingant T, et al. Magnetometry with nitrogen-vacancy defects in diamond. Rep. Prog. Phys. 2014;77:56503. doi: 10.1088/0034-4885/77/5/056503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Faley MI, et al. High-Tc DC SQUIDs for magnetoencephalography. IEEE Trans. Appl. Supercond. 2013;23:1600705–1600705. doi: 10.1109/TASC.2012.2229094. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schneiderman JF. Information content with low- vs. high-TcSQUID arrays in MEG recordings: the case for high-TcSQUID-based MEG. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014;222:42–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Allred JC, Lyman RN, Kornack TW, Romalis MV. High-sensitivity atomic magnetometer unaffected by spin-exchange relaxation. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2002;89:130801. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.130801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Osborne J, Orton J, Alem O, Shah V. Fully integrated, standalone zero field optically pumped magnetometer for biomagnetism. Steep Dispers. Eng. Opto-Atomic Precis. Metrol. XI. 2018 doi: 10.1117/12.2299197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Tierney TM, et al. Optically pumped magnetometers: from quantum origins to multi-channel magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage. 2019;199:598–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Boto E, et al. Moving brain imaging towards real-world applications using a wearable MEG system. Nature. 2018 doi: 10.1038/nature26147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tierney TM, et al. Cognitive neuroscience using wearable magnetometer arrays: non-invasive assessment of language function. Neuroimage. 2018;181:513–520. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Roberts G, et al. Towards magnetoencephalography in a virtual reality environment. Neuroimage. 2019;199:408–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Holmes N, et al. A bi-planar coil system for nulling background magnetic fields in scalp mounted magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage. 2018;181:760–774. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Iivanainen J, Zetter R, Grön M, Hakkarainen K, Parkkonen L. On-scalp MEG system utilizing an actively shielded array of optically-pumped magnetometers. Neuroimage. 2019;194:244–258. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Holmes N, et al. Balanced, bi-planar magnetic field and field gradient coils for field compensation in wearable magnetoencephalography. Sci. Rep. 2019 doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50697-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Boto E, et al. A new generation of magnetoencephalography: room temperature measurements using optically-pumped magnetometers. Neuroimage. 2016;149:404. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Boto E, et al. Wearable neuroimaging: combining and contrasting magnetoencephalography and electroencephalography. Neuroimage. 2019;201:116099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Iivanainen, J. et al. Sampling theory for spatial field sensing: Application to electro- and magnetoencephalography. arXiv (2019).
- 31.Sutherling W, Mamelak A, Thyerlei D, Maleeva T. Influence of magnetic source imaging for planning intracranial EEG in epilepsy. Neurology. 2008;71:990. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000326591.29858.1a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rosenow F, Lüders H. Presurgical evaluation of epilepsy. Brain. 2001;124:1683–1700. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.De Tiège X, et al. Clinical added value of magnetic source imaging in the presurgical evaluation of refractory focal epilepsy. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2012;83:417–423. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-301166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Doss RC, Zhang W, Risse GL, Dickens DL. Lateralizing language with magnetic source imaging: validation based on the Wada test. Epilepsia. 2009;50:2242–2248. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02242.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schneider F, et al. Magnetic source imaging and ictal SPECT in MRI-negative neocortical epilepsies: additional value and comparison with intracranial EEG. Epilepsia. 2013;54:359–369. doi: 10.1111/epi.12004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Englot DJ, et al. Epileptogenic zone localization using magnetoencephalography predicts seizure freedom in epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia. 2015 doi: 10.1111/epi.13002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Centeno M, et al. Combined electroencephalography–functional magnetic resonance imaging and electrical source imaging improves localization of pediatric focal epilepsy. Ann. Neurol. 2017;82:278–287. doi: 10.1002/ana.25003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Aquino KM, et al. Addressing challenges of high spatial resolution UHF fMRI for group analysis of higher-order cognitive tasks: an inter-sensory task directing attention between visual and somatosensory domains. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019;40:1298–1316. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sabuncu MR, et al. Function-based intersubject alignment of human cortical anatomy. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:130–140. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Geyer S, Turner R. Microstructural parcellation of the human cerebral cortex: From Brodmann’s post-mortem map to in vivo mapping with high-field magnetic resonance imaging. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2013;5:1–257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Devlin JT, Poldrack RA. praise of tedious anatomy. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1033–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.O’Neill GC, et al. Dynamics of large-scale electrophysiological networks: a technical review. NeuroImage. 2018;180:559–576. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Meyer SS, et al. Using generative models to make probabilistic statements about hippocampal engagement in MEG. Neuroimage. 2017;149:468–482. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Troebinger L, López JD, Lutti A, Bestmann S, Barnes G. Discrimination of cortical laminae using MEG. Neuroimage. 2014;102:885–893. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Henson RN, Mattout J, Phillips C, Friston KJ. Selecting forward models for MEG source-reconstruction using. Neuroimage. 2009;46:168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Iivanainen J, Zetter R, Groen M, Hakkarainen K, Parkkonen L. On-scalp MEG system utilizing an actively shielded array of optically-pumped magnetometers. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/471391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cohen-Tannoudji C, Dupont-Roc J, Haroche S, Laloë F. Diverses résonances de croisement de niveaux sur des atomes pompés optiquement en champ nul. I. Théorie. Rev. Phys. Appl. 1970;5:95–101. doi: 10.1051/rphysap:019700050109500. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Pfeiffer C, et al. Localizing on-scalp MEG sensors using an array of magnetic dipole coils. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0191111. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Nardelli NV, Krzyzewski SP, Knappe SA. Reducing crosstalk in optically-pumped magnetometer arrays. Phys. Med. Biol. 2019 doi: 10.1088/1361-6560/ab4c06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Boto E, et al. On the potential of a new generation of magnetometers for MEG: a beamformer simulation study. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:1–24. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Nolte G. The magnetic lead field theorem in the quasi-static approximation and its use for magnetoenchephalography forward calculation in realistic volume conductors. Phys. Med. Biol. 2003;48:3637–3652. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/48/22/002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Belardinelli P, Ortiz E, Barnes G, Noppeney U, Preissl H. Source reconstruction accuracy of MEG and EEG Bayesian inversion approaches. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e51985. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.López JD, Valencia F, Flandin G, Penny W, Barnes GR. Reconstructing anatomy from electro-physiological data. Neuroimage. 2017;163:480–486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tzovara A, et al. High-precision magnetoencephalography for reconstructing amygdalar and hippocampal oscillations during prediction of safety and threat. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019;40:4114–4129. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Friston K, Mattout J, Trujillo-Barreto N, Ashburner J, Penny W. Variational free energy and the Laplace approximation. Neuroimage. 2007;34:220–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bonaiuto JJ, et al. Non-invasive laminar inference with MEG: comparison of methods and source inversion algorithms. Neuroimage. 2018;167:372–383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lopez JD, Litvak V, Espinosa JJ, Friston K, Barnes GR. Algorithmic procedures for Bayesian MEG/EEG source reconstruction in SPM. Neuroimage. 2014;84:476–487. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Iivanainen, J. et al. Spatial sampling of MEG and EEG revisited: from spatial-frequency spectra to model-informed sampling. (2020).
- 60.Hämäläinen MS, Lundqvist D. MEG as an enabling tool in neuroscience: transcending boundaries with new analysis methods and devices. In: Supek S, Aine CJ, editors. Magnetoencephalography: From Signals to Dynamic Cortical Networks. Cham: Springer; 2019. pp. 1–37. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kaplan AY, Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Borisov SV, Darkhovsky BS. Nonstationary nature of the brain activity as revealed by EEG/MEG: methodological, practical and conceptual challenges. Signal Process. 2005;85:2190–2212. doi: 10.1016/j.sigpro.2005.07.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hillebrand A, Barnes GR. The use of anatomical constraints with MEG beamformers. Neuroimage. 2003;20:2302–2313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Stenroos M, Sarvas J. Bioelectromagnetic forward problem: isolated source approach revis(it)ed. Phys. Med. Biol. 2012;57:3517–3535. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/57/11/3517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Stenroos M, Hunold A, Haueisen J. Comparison of three-shell and simplified volume conductor models in magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage. 2014;94:337–348. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zetter R, Iivanainen J, Stenroos M, Parkkonen L. Requirements for coregistration accuracy in on-scalp MEG. Brain Topogr. 2018;31:1–18. doi: 10.1007/s10548-018-0656-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Liu Y, Dolan RJ, Kurth-Nelson Z, Behrens TEJ. Human replay spontaneously reorganizes experience. Cell. 2019;178:640–652. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bonaiuto JJ, et al. Non-invasive laminar inference with MEG: comparison of methods and source inversion algorithms. Neuroimage. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Bonaiuto JJ, et al. Lamina-specific cortical dynamics in human visual and sensorimotor cortices. Elife. 2018;7:1–32. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Khemka S, Barnes G, Dolan RJ, Bach DR. Dissecting the function of hippocampal oscillations in a human anxiety model. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:6869–6876. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1834-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Blei DM, Kucukelbir A, McAuliffe JD. Variational inference: a review for statisticians. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 2017;112:859–877. doi: 10.1080/01621459.2017.1285773. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Beal, M. J. & Ghahramani, Z. The variational Bayesian EM algorithm for incomplete data: with application to scoring graphical model structures. In Bayesian Stat 7 Proc. seventh Val Int Meet June 2–6, 2002, 453 (2003). 10.1152/jn.00664.2010.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.