Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Neuroscience
. 2020 Dec 3;14:601063. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.601063

Altered Gray Matter Volume and Functional Connectivity in Human Immunodeficiency Virus-Infected Adults

Dan Liu 1, Cui Zhao 2, Wei Wang 1, Yuanyuan Wang 3, Ruili Li 1, Jun Sun 1, Jiaojiao Liu 1, Mingming Liu 4, Xu Zhang 2,*, Ying Liang 2,*, Hongjun Li 1,5,*
PMCID: PMC7744568  PMID: 33343289

Abstract

People living with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) (PLWH) are at high risk of neurocognitive impairment. The pathogenesis of neurocognitive impairment remains unclear, and there is still no diagnostic biomarker. By coupling three-dimensional T1-weighted imaging and resting-state functional imaging, we explored structural and functional alterations in PLWH and examined whether such imaging alterations had the potential to denote neurocognitive function. A total of 98 PLWH and 47 seronegative controls aged 20–53 years were recruited. Structural alterations were first explored between HIV-negative controls and PLWH. Subsequently, brain regions showing gray matter alterations were used as seeds for separate whole-brain functional connectivity (FC) analysis. Finally, the relationships between imaging alterations and cognitive function were explored. PLWH suffered from thalamus, occipital lobe, and hippocampus/parahippocampus atrophy. Visual cortices in PLWH showed decreased anticorrelation with the posterior cingulate cortex and left angular gyrus of the default mode network. FC within the visual cortices (between the left calcarine and right calcarine) and in the thalamic prefrontal circuit and between the thalamus and somatosensory association cortex were also altered. In addition, FC between the left thalamus and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the cognitively impaired group was significantly different from that in the cognitively normal group in PLWH. Partial correlation analysis uncorrected for multiple comparisons suggested that some imaging alterations can be associated with neurocognition. Our study supports the presence of brain atrophy and functional reconfiguration in PLWH. Imaging alterations can be associated with neurocognitive function. We hold that neuroimaging is a promising approach in evaluating PLWH and might have the potential to clarify the pathogenesis of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder.

Keywords: human immunodeficiency virus, HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder, structural MRI, resting-state functional MRI, functional connectivity

Introduction

Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) enters the central nervous system (CNS) soon after infection (Davis et al., 1992) and causes brain damage, including inflammation, microglial/macrophage activation, oligodendrocyte dysfunction, neural degeneration, and even apoptosis of neural cells (Zayyad and Spudich, 2015; Jensen et al., 2019; Rojas-Celis et al., 2019). Approximately 15–50% of people living with HIV (PLWH) show HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) (Schouten et al., 2011). The prevalence varies according to the population studied and increases with age (Makinson et al., 2019). Methods used to define impairment also have an impact on the prevalence (Underwood et al., 2019). To date, the pathogenesis underlying HAND has not been fully characterized. It is believed that an indirect effect of chronic immune activation, inflammatory response (González-Scarano and Martín-García, 2005; Saylor et al., 2016) and a direct neurotoxic effect of viral protein (Nath, 2002), as well as vascular damage work together and may result in HAND in the combined antiretroviral therapy (cART) era (Zayyad and Spudich, 2015).

Neuroimaging is a promising approach to assess brain morphometric and functional changes that shows broad application prospects in the diagnosis and management of PLWH and may have the potential to shed light on the pathogenesis of HAND (Clifford and Ances, 2013; Saylor et al., 2016; Campbell et al., 2019). Previous structural MRI studies indicated that PLWH display widespread brain atrophy in the subcortical nucleus as well as the frontal, parietal, occipital, temporal, and cerebellar cortices and cortical thinning in the frontal and temporal lobes and cingulate cortex (Wilson et al., 2015; Underwood et al., 2017; Sanford et al., 2018a,b; Israel et al., 2019; Hassanzadeh-Behbahani et al., 2020). Diffuse abnormalities in white matter (WM) microstructure were also noticed, including reduced fractional anisotropy with increased mean and radial diffusivity (O’Connor et al., 2017; Underwood et al., 2017). Functional MRI (fMRI) indicated hyperactivation in task-related brain regions (Hakkers et al., 2017), disruption of functional connectivity (FC) within cortical networks, and corticostriatal networks in PLWH compared with controls (Thomas et al., 2013; Plessis et al., 2014; Ortega et al., 2015; Chaganti et al., 2017) and a reduction in resting cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in lenticular nuclei and the visual cortex (Ances et al., 2009). Positron emission tomography as molecular imaging provides valuable insight into the pathophysiology underlying HAND, and several studies reported perturbation of metabolism, neuroinflammation, abnormal function of neurotransmitters, and amyloid/tau protein deposition in HAND (Vera et al., 2016; Vera et al., 2017; Sinharay and Hammoud, 2019). In general, dopamine-modulated frontal-striatal circuit alteration was most consistently reported in previous studies and may play a part in the pathogenesis of HAND. Some researchers investigated the effects of cART using neuroimaging, and most of them reported improved characteristics in viral suppressed PLWH, including increased cortical thickness, no further structural deterioration, and improved FC (Ortega et al., 2015; Zhuang et al., 2017; Sanford et al., 2018a,b). The relationship between cognitive performance and neuroimaging is controversial. Whereas neuroimaging alterations in morphometry, diffusion, FC, and network topology were reported to be correlated with cognitive function (Wilson et al., 2015; Underwood et al., 2017; Haddow et al., 2019; Israel et al., 2019; Kato et al., 2020), there are also studies that failed to find any such associations (Thomas et al., 2013; Su et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2018).

In this study, by coupling voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis based on high-resolution T1-weighted MRI and seed-based whole-brain FC analysis based on resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI), we investigated brain morphometric and FC alterations in PLWH and further explored the association between imaging alterations and neurocognitive function.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 98 PLWH and 47 seronegative controls (HIV-negative controls) aged 20–53 years were recruited in our study. The inclusion criteria for both groups were as follows: right-handed and able to give informed consent. For the PLWH, only those infected by homosexual sex were included. The exclusion criteria for both groups were as follows: (1) medical or neuropsychiatric illnesses that might confound the study; (2) previous head injury with loss of consciousness longer than 30 min; (3) substance abuse; and (4) MR contraindication. Clinical data were obtained from medical records and are presented in Table 1. Among the 98 PLWH, 63 were tested for current viral load, 90 were tested for hepatitis B virus and hepatitis C virus, and 98 were tested for syphilis. This study was approved by the institutional review boards, and all the participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1.

Demographics, clinical information, and neurocognitive performance.

Variable Control (n = 46) GDS normal (n = 55) GDS impaired (n = 36) P-value
Age (mean ± SD) 34.4 ± 6.7 32.0 ± 7.2 30.8 ± 7.1 0.057a
Male, n (%) 42 (91.3%) 54 (98.2%) 36 (100.0%) 0.089d
Education years (mean ± SD) / 13.0 ± 3.7 14.3 ± 2.7 0.060b
CD4 + T (mean ± SD) / 473.9 ± 233.3 496.2 ± 208.3 0.643b
CD4/CD8 (mean ± SD) / 0.6 ± 0.4 0.6 ± 0.5 0.982b
TND, n (%) / 30 (85.7%) 18 (81.8%) 0.722d
BMI (mean ± SD) / 21.1 ± 2.7 22.4 ± 2.6 0.031b
Hepatitis B virus infection, n (%) / 13 (26.5%) 9 (26.5%) 1.000c
Hepatitis C virus infection, n (%) / 1 (2.0%) 0 (0.0%) 1.000d
Syphilis, n (%) / 19 (34.5%) 16 (44.4%) 0.383c
TIV (mean ± SD) 1,507.7 ± 132.8 1,528.2 ± 120.1 1,553.0 ± 100.9 0.240a
Neurocognitive performance T scores (mean ± SD)
Speed of information processing / 48.4 ± 7.2 40.8 ± 9.5 < 0.001b
Memory (learning and recall) / 46.8 ± 6.5 39.1 ± 7.7 < 0.001b
Abstraction/executive / 58.4 ± 9.1 51.9 ± 8.6 0.001b
Attention/working memory / 44.8 ± 7.2 38.2 ± 6.9 < 0.001b
Fine motor skills / 48.4 ± 7.7 41.2 ± 11.8 0.001b
verbal and language / 48.7 ± 7.0 39.8 ± 9.6 < 0.001b
GDS / 0.1 ± 0.1 0.8 ± 0.8 < 0.001b

TND, virus not detectable; BMI, body mass index; TIV, total intracranial volume; GDS, global deficit score.P-value for comparison among three groups (age, male%, TIV) and between GDS normal and GDS impaired subgroups.Statistical tests including: aone-way ANOVA; bt-test; cchi-square test; dFisher’s exact test.

Neurocognitive Tests

Six cognitive domains were assessed in PLWH using a set of neurocognitive tests normed for age, sex, education level, and scale of residence area (Shi et al., 2015), including (1) speed of information processing [trail-making test part A (TMT A)]; (2) memory, including learning and recall [Hopkins verbal learning test-revised (HVLT-R) and brief visuospatial memory test-revised (BVMT-R)]; (3) abstraction and executive function [Wisconsin Card Sorting Test 64-card version (WCST-64)]; (4) attention and working memory [continuous performance test-identical pairs (CPT-IP), Wechsler memory scale-III (WMS-III), and Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)]; (5) fine motor skills (Grooved Pegboard Test); and (6) verbal and language (animal naming test). The global deficit score (GDS) approach was used to determine cognitive status in PLWH, as it considers both the number and severity of cognitive deficits in performance throughout the overall test battery (Blackstone et al., 2012). A cutoff of 0.5 was applied to classify PLWH into GDS normal and GDS impaired subgroups according to previous studies (Carey et al., 2004).

Only PLWH were provided with neurocognitive tests; among them, four men and three women were not tested.

Neuroimaging Acquisition

All images were acquired using a Siemens Trio Tim 3T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. Foam pads were used to restrict head movement. All participants were asked to lie relaxed and not to think of any particular things with their eyes closed without falling asleep.

Structural images were collected using a sagittal magnetization prepared gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence [repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE)/inversion time (TI) = 1,900/2.52/900 ms; acquisition matrix = 256 246; field of view = 250 250; flip angle = 9 degrees; voxel size = 1 mm 1 mm 1 mm].

Whole-brain rs-fMRI data were collected utilizing a gradient echo single-shot echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR/TE = 2,000/30 ms; resolution matrix = 64 64; voxel size = 3.5 mm 3.5 mm 3.5 mm; flip angle = 90°) with 35 axial slices and 240 time points.

One HIV-negative control was excluded for inadequate imaging acquisition.

Image Preprocessing

All images were first reoriented using Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM12)1 to align images with standard space as closely as possible and set the origin to be close to the anterior commissure. T1-weighted images were preprocessed using computational anatomy toolbox 12 (CAT12)2 within SPM12 using MATLAB R2013b for voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis. As parts of the images showed inhomogeneity, in the segmentation module, we selected the strength of the SPM inhomogeneity correction as strong, SPM processing accuracy as high, and strength of the local adaptive segmentation as medium. Images were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 space using the diffeomorphic anatomical registration through exponentiated lie algebra (DARTEL) algorithm (Ashburner, 2007). Data quality was subsequently checked by running the check data quality module in CAT12. We visually inspected each normalized bias-corrected volume to remove volumes with artifacts and unsatisfactory orientations, and modulated normalized gray matter (GM) segments were checked to identify outliers as well as sample homogeneity. No participant was excluded. Total intracranial volume (TIV) was estimated. Finally, all the modulated normalized GM segments were smoothed with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) kernel.

Resting-state fMRI was preprocessed with Data Processing and Analysis for Brain Imaging (DPABI V4.2) (Yan et al., 2016). The first 10 timepoints were removed to guarantee signal stability. Slice timing correction was performed with the timing of the middle slice as a reference. The time series of each participant was realigned using a six-parameter (rigid body) linear transformation with a two-pass procedure (first realigned to the initial image and then to the mean of the image). After realignment, T1-weighted images were coregistered to mean functional images using 6 degrees of freedom linear transformation and subsequently segmented into GM, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). DARTEL was used to compute transformations from individual native space to MNI space. The Friston 24-parameter head motion model was used to regress out head motion effects (Friston et al., 1996; Yan et al., 2013). Mean framewise displacement (FD), which was derived from Jenkinson’s relative root mean square algorithm, was used to reduce the residual effects of motion in group analyses (Jenkinson et al., 2002). Linear trends were regressed to reduce drift in the signal, and CSF and WM signals were regressed as covariates through linear regression to reduce respiratory and cardiac effects. Global signal regression (GSR) was also applied because it can reduce unwanted global confounds, such as vascular effects, respiration, and movement; enhance the detection of system-specific correlations; and improve the correspondence between resting-state correlations and anatomy (Fox et al., 2009; Zhu et al., 2015; Murphy and Fox, 2017). The temporal bandpass filter (0.01∼0.1 Hz) was used to remove the low-frequency drifts and high-frequency physiological noise.

Nine HIV-negative controls and seven PLWH were excluded for excessive head motion (mean FD > 0.2 mm), and two PLWH were excluded for poor image registration. A group binary mask was created that allowed 90% of participants to have the voxel in each participant’s brain coverage for statistical analysis.

Cortical renderings were visualized with BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013).

Functional Connectivity Analysis

The bilateral thalamus, right calcarine, right cuneus, bilateral lingual, left hippocampus, left parahippocampus, and left fusiform, which exhibited GM alterations, were defined as regions of interest (ROI). These ROI masks were extracted from the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas. The average time series of voxels in each ROI was extracted. Pearson’s correlation analysis was used to calculate the temporal correlation between the ROI and each voxel of the brain. Fisher’s r- to -z transformation was used to improve data normality. An isotropic Gaussian kernel at 4 mm FWHM was used for spatial smoothing.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical tests conducted on demographics, clinical information, and neurocognitive function comparisons included one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) (age, TIV), two-sample t-test [education, CD4 + T cell count, CD4/CD8, body mass index (BMI), neurocognitive function in each of the six domains], chi-square test (hepatitis B virus, syphilis), and Fisher’s exact test (sex, percentage of viral load not detectable, hepatitis C virus). Group differences (PLWH and HIV-negative controls) in brain volume and FC were identified using a two-sample t-test. Correction for multiple comparisons used Gaussian random field (GRF) correction, with statistical significance defined by voxel level P < 0.001 and cluster level P < 0.05 (two-tailed). Brain volume and FC differences among HIV-negative controls, GDS normal groups, and GDS impaired groups were identified using a univariate general linear model, and post hoc multiple comparisons were conducted using the least significant difference (LSD) test. Partial correlation was applied to explore the relationship between each cognitive domain, GDS, and imaging alterations, with a false discovery rate (FDR) of P < 0.05 after correction for multiple comparisons.

Statistical analyses were performed using IBM Statistics version 22. A P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Participants

Demographics and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the PLWH subgroups in education, current CD4 + T cell count, CD4/CD8, percentage of undetectable viral load, or percentage of hepatitis B virus/hepatitis C virus/syphilis coinfection. The GDS impaired group showed a higher BMI than the GDS normal group (T = −2.2, P = 0.031). Fisher’s exact test showed no significant difference in sex. One-way ANOVA indicated no age and TIV differences among the HIV-negative controls, GDS normal, and GDS impaired groups (F = 2.9, P = 0.057; F = 1.4, P = 0.240).

In our study, 39.6% of PLWH were neurocognitively impaired according to the GDS approach. Compared with GDS normal participants, GDS impaired participants showed significantly decreased T scores in all six cognitive domains and GDS, as shown in Table 1.

Difference in Gray Matter Volume Between HIV-Negative Controls and People Living With HIV

We compared GM volume (GMV) between HIV-negative controls and PLWH (Table 2 and Figure 1). The GMV of the bilateral thalamus, right calcarine (extended to the right lingual), right cuneus, left lingual, left hippocampus, and left parahippocampus (extended to the left lingual, left fusiform) in PLWH were significantly reduced compared with HIV-negative controls after controlling for age, sex, and TIV (GRF corrected, voxel level P < 0.001, cluster level P < 0.05, two-tailed).

TABLE 2.

Gray matter volume differences between HIV-negative controls and PLWH.

Structure L/R Volumes (mm3) Coordinates
T-value P-value
X Y Z
Cluster 1 Thalamus R 756.0 8 −14 −3 5.9 <0.001
Thalamus L −5 −14 −5 5.2 <0.001
Cluster 2 Calcarine R 7,519.5 12 −74 3 5.5 <0.001
Cuneus R 17 −74 38 4.9 <0.001
Lingual L −9 −68 −6 4.5 <0.001
Cluster 3 Parahippocampus L 2,575.1 −21 −38 −11 5.4 <0.001
Hippocampus L −18 −36 0 4.3 <0.001

Coordinates (X, Y, Z) refer to the peak MNI coordinates of brain regions with peak intensity. Corrected for multiple comparisons (GRF correction, voxel level P < 0.001, cluster level P < 0.05, two-tailed).GRF, Gaussian random field; MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; PLWH, people living with HIV.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Gray matter volume differences between HIV-negative controls and people living with HIV (PLWH) based on voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis. Brain regions with significantly reduced gray matter colored by T-statistic (red/yellow). Corrected for multiple comparisons [Gaussian random field (GRF) correction, voxel level P < 0.001, cluster level P < 0.05, two-tailed].

Differences in Seed-Based Whole-Brain Functional Connectivity

FC was calculated and compared between HIV-negative controls and PLWH after controlling for age, sex, and head motion. Reduction of anticorrelation was noticed among the left lingual, right calcarine, right lingual, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). FC between the right calcarine, right cuneus, and left angular (BA39) was also reduced. The PCC and left angular detected in our study were located in the default mode network (DMN). The positive correlation within the visual network (between the right calcarine and left calcarine) was also reduced. The left thalamus showed decreased FC in the ipsilateral precuneus/superior parietal lobe (BA5, BA7) and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), whereas the right thalamus showed decreased FC in the right DLPFC in PLWH compared with HIV-negative controls. No significant HIV effect was observed in the FC of the left hippocampus/parahippocampus and left fusiform (GRF correction, voxel level P < 0.001, cluster level P < 0.05, two-tailed), as shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Functional connectivity (FC) differences between HIV-negative controls and people living with HIV (PLWH). Seeds were extracted according to the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas. THA.L, left thalamus (purple); LING.L, left lingual (yellow); CUN.R, right cuneus (orange); CAL.R, right calcarine (red); LING.R, right lingual (green); and THA.R, right thalamus (cyan). Corrected for multiple comparisons [Gaussian random field (GRF) correction, voxel level P < 0.001, cluster level P < 0.05, two-tailed]. Color bar indicates corrected T-values.

Relationship Between Cognitive Function and Imaging Alterations

Total GMV, WM volume (WMV), CSF volume, and clusters showed that differences in GMV and FC due to serostatus were further compared to explore whether cognition status had an effect on these imaging parameters. No significant differences were detected between PLWH subgroups after controlling for age, sex, TIV (only for GMV comparisons), or head motion (only for FC comparisons), except that the anticorrelation between the left thalamus and right DLPFC was significantly increased in the GDS impaired group compared with the GDS normal group (Figure 3). Partial correlation analysis conducted between specific cognitive domains, GDS, and imaging alterations indicated that right cuneus-left angular FC was negatively correlated with GDS (r = −0.230, P = 0.041). Speed of information processing was positively correlated with left thalamus-DLPFC FC (r = 0.243, P = 0.030). Attention and working memory were positively correlated with right cuneus-left angular FC (r = 0.271, P = 0.015). Fine motor skills were positively related with total WMV (r = 0.360, P < 0.001), GMV of the cluster in the occipital lobe (r = 0.232, P = 0.03), right lingual-left angular FC (r = 0.256, P = 0.022), right cuneus-left angular FC (r = 0.367, P < 0.001), right calcarine-angular FC (r = 0.321, P = 0.004), and right calcarine-PCC FC (r = 0.224, P = 0.045), whereas total CSF volume showed a negative correlation (r = −0.276, P = 0.009) (Figure 4). However, after correction for multiple comparisons with a FDR of P < 0.05, these findings were not significant.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Volume (ml) and functional connectivity (FC) (Z-score) differences among HIV-negative controls, global deficit score (GDS) normal, and GDS impaired groups. For voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis, the left Y-axis is the volume of gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), and the right Y-axis is the volume of thalamus (THA), occipital, and left hippocampus/parahippocampus (HIP/PHG.L). P < 0.05; ∗∗P < 0.01; ∗∗∗P < 0.001. Least significant difference (LSD) for multiple comparison correction. LING.L, left lingual; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; CAL.R, right calcarine; CAL.L, left calcarine; CUN.R, right cuneus; ANG.L, left angular; LING.R, right lingual; DLPFC, dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex; THA.L, left thalamus; THA.R, right thalamus; SA.L, left somatosensory association cortex.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

partial correlation between specific cognitive domains, global deficit score (GDS), and imaging alterations. The areas between two dotted curves indicate the 95% confidence interval. All variables shown above were unstandardized residuals plus their own mean value.

Discussion

A combination of VBM analysis and resting-state FC in our study indicated that PLWH suffered from thalamus, occipital lobe, and hippocampus/parahippocampus atrophy. The visual cortices in PLWH showed decreased anticorrelation with the PCC and left angular of the DMN. The FC within the visual network was also reduced. Alterations in FC in the thalamic prefrontal circuit between the thalamus and somatosensory association cortex were also detected. In addition, FC between the left thalamus and right DLPFC in the GDS impaired group was significantly different from that in the GDS normal group. Partial correlation analysis uncorrected for multiple comparisons suggested that certain imaging alterations can be associated with neurocognition. However, after correction for multiple comparisons, these results were not significant. We discussed partial analysis without FDR correlation in the following sections to discern all the possible relationships between imaging alterations and cognitive function.

Glutamate is the most abundant neurotransmitter in the nervous system. Aberrant metabolism of this neurotransmitter plays an important role in the development of HAND (Ernst et al., 2010; Nedelcovych et al., 2019). Previous studies suggested that macrophages and activated astrocytes can produce tumor necrosis factor (TNF) and other inflammatory cytokines, resulting in increased glutamate release and reduced glutamate uptake by astrocytes. The subsequently high levels of glutamate resulted in uncontrolled Ca2+ influx, which led to neuron degeneration and apoptosis (Zayyad and Spudich, 2015). Neuron loss is rare in the cART era but not in degeneration, and synaptodendritic degenerative changes have been reported to be associated with HAND (Guha et al., 2018). We presume that the injury to glutamate neurons might account for the scattered brain atrophy and decreased neural synchronous activity detected in our study.

The thalamus is a central relay station of sensory signals and the integration center of the CNS. Several previous studies delineated thalamus atrophy in PLWH (Castillo et al., 2018; Sanford et al., 2018a,b). In our study, thalamus atrophy was mainly located in the medial nucleus (e.g., mediodorsal nucleus, MD), which contributes principal subcortical input to the DLPFC (Popken et al., 2000). Moreover, a growing body of evidence suggests a causal relationship between MD-PFC abnormalities and cognitive deficits, including working memory, behavioral flexibility, goal-directed behavior, and recollection (Danet et al., 2017; Parnaudeau et al., 2018). In our study, FC between the left thalamus and right DLPFC in the GDS impaired group was significantly different from that in the GDS normal group and positively correlated with the speed of information processing. We speculate that neuroimaging alterations can be associated with neurocognition.

The ventral posterolateral nucleus and MD of the thalamus also participate in pain processing (Fenton et al., 2015). The same is true for somatosensory association cortex (Witting et al., 2001). It is worth noting that HIV-associated sensory neuropathy, a common neuropathy, is characterized by neuropathic pain, sensory loss, and minimal weakness (Aziz-Donnelly and Harrison, 2017). Both the periphery and CNS are involved in this neuropathy. A study evaluated brain morphometry abnormalities within the pain matrix and detected smaller surface areas in the prefrontal pars triangularis, anterior cingulate cortex, smaller thalamus, and putamen in PLWH (Castillo et al., 2018). Further research is needed to test whether the abnormalities delineated in our study contribute to the pathogenesis of this condition.

GM atrophy and FC reduction within the visual network were reported in PLWH (Wang et al., 2011; Underwood et al., 2017; Egbert et al., 2018). In addition, poorer fine motor skills were reported to correlate with reduced occipital cortex volume, lower CBF in the occipital cortex, and smaller supratentorial WMV (Haddow et al., 2019; Kato et al., 2020). Neuroinflammation, rCBF reduction, and lower electroencephalogram (EEG) rhythms were also reported in the occipital lobe in PLWH (Babiloni et al., 2014; Wiesman et al., 2018; Haddow et al., 2019). Evidence from neurocognitive assessment suggested episodic memory, visual attention, visuoperception, and visual working memory deficits in PLWH (Woods et al., 2009), which might indicate abnormalities in the occipital lobe. A cytopathology study examined brain tissue in the temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes and found that the calcarine cortex (primary visual area) had the greatest neuronal loss of 30% (Everall et al., 1993).

Reduced FC between the cortices of the visual network and PCC and the left angular of the DMN is relevant to other studies reporting FC reduction within the DMN and between the DMN and other networks in PLWH (Thomas et al., 2013; Ortega et al., 2015; Ann et al., 2016; Samboju et al., 2018). Moreover, the detected correlation with fine motor skills, attention and working memory, and GDS might suggest that compromised internetwork integration and communication impair cognitive function. Reduced FC between the visual network and DMN was also previously reported in elderly people (with a mean age of 60.2 ± 12.8 years) (Onoda et al., 2012). As our participants were relatively young (32.5 ± 7.0 years) and there was no significant difference across groups, the FC reduction observed in our study is unlikely due to aging. Whether the internetwork FC reduction suggests premature brain aging still requires further study.

The lower left hippocampus/parahippocampus volume detected in our cohort is consistent with other studies (Wilson et al., 2015). An autopsy study focused on the basal ganglia and hippocampus showed that neuroinflammation continued in cART-treated patients, particularly in the hippocampus (Anthony et al., 2005). In another neuropathological study, Sá et al. (2000) found that the total number of neurons in the hippocampus was not different from that in controls, but cell atrophy was detected in patients with AIDS, which supports the findings of our study from a pathologic perspective. Our study also suggested the decoupling of FC and GM atrophy in the hippocampus/parahippocampus. A similar phenomenon was reported in studies coupling WMV and diffusion in PLWH (Underwood et al., 2017) as well as studies focused on other disease models (Bernhardt et al., 2016; Peer et al., 2017).

There are several limitations in our current study. First, the lack of more detailed clinical and neurocognitive measures in seronegative controls limited its comparison to PLWH. In addition, we only orally asked for any medical history of mental disorders as an exclusion criterion, and it would be more rigorous to collect psychiatric symptoms based on neuropsychiatric testing. Second, some antiretroviral drugs have been reported to have neurotoxicity and cause neurodegenerative changes, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and brain volume reductions (O’Halloran et al., 2019). In our study, a combination of two nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors (NRTIs) (usually tenofovir disoproxil fumarate and lamivudine) and one non-nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitor (NNRTI) (usually efavirenz) made up a free cART regimen. Additional caution should be taken as efavirenz has been proven to be neurotoxic (Ciccarelli et al., 2011). Moreover, the exact regimes and whether regimes were switched in each PLWH were not clear. Thus, we cannot eliminate such confounding factors caused by drugs. Third, the nature of the cross-sectional design makes it subject to inherent bias, and a longitudinal study by our group is now in progress for a more powerful, objective analysis.

Our study indicates that PLWH suffered from brain atrophy and functional reconfiguration, and imaging alterations can be associated with neurocognition. We hold that neuroimaging is a promising approach in evaluating PLWH and helps to elucidate the pathogenesis of HAND. Future longitudinal studies with large samples, more detailed clinical information, and neurocognitive information aimed at PLWH (both cART naive and on cART) are needed to clarify existing confounds and further increase our understanding of the pathogenesis of HAND.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Beijing YouAn Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

HL and DL contributed to conception and design of the study. RL, WW, JL, YW, and ML recruited participants and organized the database. JS and CZ performed parts of the statistical analysis. DL collected parts of participants’ information, analyzed the statistics, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the participants who volunteered for our study and our team in Beijing Youan Hospital for recruiting, collecting these data.

Funding. This research was supported by the National Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 61936013 and 81771806).

References

  1. Ances B. M., Sisti D., Vaida F., Liang C. L., Leontiev O., Perthen J. E., et al. (2009). Resting cerebral blood flow: a potential biomarker of the effects of HIV in the brain. Neurology 73 702–708. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b59a97 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ann H. W., Jun S., Shin N., Han S., Ahn J. Y., Ahn M. Y., et al. (2016). Characteristics of resting-state functional connectivity in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. PLoS One 11:e0153493. 10.1371/journal.pone.0153493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anthony I. C., Ramage S. N., Carnie F. W., Simmonds P., Bell J. E. (2005). Influence of HAART on HIV-related CNS disease and neuroinflammation. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 64 529–536. 10.1093/jnen/64.6.529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashburner J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38 95–113. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aziz-Donnelly A., Harrison T. B. (2017). Update of HIV-associated sensory neuropathies. Curr. Treat. Option. Neurol. 19:36. 10.1007/s11940-017-0472-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Babiloni C., Buffo P., Vecchio F., Onorati P., Muratori C., Ferracuti S., et al. (2014). Cortical sources of resting-state EEG rhythms in “experienced” HIV subjects under antiretroviral therapy. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125 1792–1802. 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.01.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bernhardt B. C., Bernasconi A., Liu M., Hong S. J., Caldairou B., Goubran M., et al. (2016). The spectrum of structural and functional imaging abnormalities in temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann. Neurol. 80 142–153. 10.1002/ana.24691 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blackstone K., Moore D. J., Franklin D. R., Clifford D. B., Collier A. C., Marra C. M., et al. (2012). Defining neurocognitive impairment in HIV: deficit scores versus clinical ratings. Clin. Neuropsychol. 26 894–908. 10.1080/13854046.2012.694479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Campbell L. M., Fennema-Notestine C., Saloner R., Hussain M., Chen A., Franklin D., et al. (2019). Use of neuroimaging to inform optimal neurocognitive criteria for detecting HIV-associated brain abnormalities. J. Int. Neuropsych. Soc. 26 147–162. 10.1017/S1355617719000985 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carey C. L., Woods S. P., Gonzalez R., Conover E., Marcotte T. D., Grant I., et al. (2004). Predictive validity of global deficit scores in detecting neuropsychological impairment in HIV infection. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsyc. 26 307–319. 10.1080/13803390490510031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Castillo D., Ernst T., Cunningham E., Chang L. (2018). Altered associations between pain symptoms and brain morphometry in the pain matrix of HIV-seropositive individuals. J. Neuroimmune Pharm. 13 77–89. 10.1007/s11481-017-9762-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chaganti J. R., Heinecke A., Gates T. M., Moffat K. J., Brew B. J. (2017). Functional connectivity in virally suppressed patients with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder: a resting-state analysis. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 38 1623–1629. 10.3174/ajnr.A5246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ciccarelli N., Fabbiani M., Di Giambenedetto S., Fanti I., Baldonero E., Bracciale L., et al. (2011). Efavirenz associated with cognitive disorders in otherwise asymptomatic HIV-infected patients. Neurology 76 1403–1409. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821670fb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Clifford D. B., Ances B. M. (2013). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. Lancet Infect. Dis. 13 976–986. 10.1016/S1473-3099(13)70269-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Danet L., Pariente J., Eustache P., Raposo N., Sibon I., Albucher J., et al. (2017). Medial thalamic stroke and its impact on familiarity and recollection. eLife 6:e28141. 10.7554/eLife.28141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Davis L. E., Hjelle B. L., Miller V. E., Palmer D. L., Llewellyn A. L., Merlin T. L., et al. (1992). Early viral brain invasion in iatrogenic human immunodeficiency virus infection. Neurology 42 1736–1739. 10.1212/wnl.42.9.1736 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Egbert A. R., Biswal B., Karunakaran K., Gohel S., Pluta A., Wolak T., et al. (2018). Age and HIV effects on resting state of the brain in relationship to neurocognitive functioning. Behav. Brain Res. 344 20–27. 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ernst T., Jiang C. S., Nakama H., Buchthal S., Chang L. (2010). Lower brain glutamate is associated with cognitive deficits in HIV patients: a new mechanism for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 32 1045–1053. 10.1002/jmri.22366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Everall I. P., Luthert P. J., Lantos P. L. (1993). Neuronal number and volume alterations in the neocortex of HIV infected individuals. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 56 481–486. 10.1136/jnnp.56.5.481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fenton B. W., Shih E., Zolton J. (2015). The neurobiology of pain perception in normal and persistent pain. Pain Manag 5 297–317. 10.2217/pmt.15.27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fox M. D., Zhang D., Snyder A. Z., Raichle M. E. (2009). The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J. Neurophysiol. 101 3270–3283. 10.1152/jn.90777.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Friston K. J., Williams S., Howard R., Frackowiak R. S., Turner R. (1996). Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35 346–355. 10.1002/mrm.1910350312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. González-Scarano F., Martín-García J. (2005). The neuropathogenesis of AIDS. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 5 69–81. 10.1038/nri1527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Guha D., Wagner M., Ayyavoo V. (2018). Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1)-mediated neuroinflammation dysregulates neurogranin and induces synaptodendritic injury. J Neuroinflammation 15:126. 10.1186/s12974-018-1160-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Haddow L. J., Godi C., Sokolska M., Cardoso M. J., Oliver R., Winston A., et al. (2019). Brain Perfusion, Regional Volumes, and Cognitive Function in Human Immunodeficiency Virus–positive Patients Treated With Protease Inhibitor Monotherapy. Clin. Infect. Dis. 68 1031–1040. 10.1093/cid/ciy617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hakkers C. S., Arends J. E., Barth R. E., Du Plessis S., Hoepelman A. I., Vink M. (2017). Review of functional MRI in HIV: effects of aging and medication. J. Neurovirol. 23 20–32. 10.1007/s13365-016-0483-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hassanzadeh-Behbahani S., Shattuck K. F., Bronshteyn M., Dawson M., Diaz M., Kumar P., et al. (2020). Low CD4 nadir linked to widespread cortical thinning in adults living with HIV. Neuroimage Clin. 25:102155. 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Israel S. M., Hassanzadeh-Behbahani S., Turkeltaub P. E., Moore D. J., Ellis R. J., Jiang X. (2019). Different roles of frontal versus striatal atrophy in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40 3010–3026. 10.1002/hbm.24577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jenkinson M., Bannister P., Brady M., Smith S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17 825–841. 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jensen B. K., Roth L. M., Grinspan J. B., Jordan-Sciutto K. L. (2019). White matter loss and oligodendrocyte dysfunction in HIV: a consequence of the infection, the antiretroviral therapy or both? Brain Res. 1724:146397. 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kato T., Yoshihara Y., Watanabe D., Fukumoto M., Wada K., Nakakura T., et al. (2020). Neurocognitive impairment and gray matter volume reduction in HIV-infected patients. J. Neurovirol. 26 590–601. 10.1007/s13365-020-00865-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Makinson A., Dubois J., Eymard-Duvernay S., Leclercq P., Zaegel-Faucher O., Bernard L., et al. (2019). Increased prevalence of neurocognitive impairment in aging people living with human immunodeficiency virus: the ANRS EP58 HAND 55-70 study. Clin. Infect. Dis. 70 2641–2648. 10.1093/cid/ciz670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Murphy K., Fox M. D. (2017). Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 154 169–173. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Nath A. (2002). Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) proteins in neuropathogenesis of HIV dementia. J. Infect. Dis. 186(Suppl. 2), S193–S198. 10.1086/344528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nedelcovych M. T., Kim B., Zhu X., Lovell L. E., Manning A. A., Kelschenbach J., et al. (2019). Glutamine antagonist JHU083 normalizes aberrant glutamate production and cognitive deficits in the EcoHIV murine model of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. J. Neuroimmune Pharm. 14 391–400. 10.1007/s11481-019-09859-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. O’Halloran J. A., Cooley S. A., Strain J. F., Boerwinkle A., Paul R., Presti R. M., et al. (2019). Altered neuropsychological performance and reduced brain volumetrics in people living with HIV on integrase strand transfer inhibitors. AIDS 33 1477–1483. 10.1097/QAD.0000000000002236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. O’Connor E. E., Jaillard A., Renard F., Zeffiro T. A. (2017). Reliability of white matter microstructural changes in HIV infection: meta-analysis and confirmation. AJNR 38 1510–1519. 10.3174/ajnr.A5229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Onoda K., Ishihara M., Yamaguchi S. (2012). Decreased functional connectivity by aging is associated with cognitive decline. J. Cognitive Neurosci. 24 2186–2198. 10.1162/jocn_a_00269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ortega M., Brier M. R., Ances B. M. (2015). Effects of HIV and combination antiretroviral therapy on cortico-striatal functional connectivity. AIDS 29 703–712. 10.1097/QAD.0000000000000611 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Parnaudeau S., Bolkan S. S., Kellendonk C. (2018). The mediodorsal thalamus: an essential partner of the prefrontal cortex for cognition. Biol. Psychiatry 83 648–656. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Peer M., Prüss H., Ben-Dayan I., Paul F., Arzy S., Finke C. (2017). Functional connectivity of large-scale brain networks in patients with anti-NMDA receptor encephalitis: an observational study. Lancet Psychiatry 4 768–774. 10.1016/s2215-0366(17)30330-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Plessis S. D., Vink M., Joska J. A., Koutsilieri E., Stein D. J., Emsley R. (2014). HIV infection and the fronto-striatal system: a systematic review and meta-analysis of fMRI studies. AIDS 28 803–811. 10.1097/QAD.0000000000000151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Popken G. J., Bunney W. J., Potkin S. G., Jones E. G. (2000). Subnucleus-specific loss of neurons in medial thalamus of schizophrenics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97 9276–9280. 10.1073/pnas.150243397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rojas-Celis V., Valiente-Echeverría F., Soto-Rifo R., Toro-Ascuy D. (2019). New challenges of HIV-1 infection: how HIV-1 attacks and resides in the central nervous system. Cells 8:1245. 10.3390/cells8101245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sá M. J., Madeira M. D., Ruela C., Volk B., Mota-Miranda A., Lecour H., et al. (2000). AIDS does not alter the total number of neurons in the hippocampal formation but induces cell atrophy: a stereological study. Acta Neuropathol. 99 643–653. 10.1007/s004010051175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Samboju V., Philippi C. L., Chan P., Cobigo Y., Fletcher J. L. K., Robb M., et al. (2018). Structural and functional brain imaging in acute HIV. NeuroImage 20 327–335. 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.07.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sanford R., Ances B. M., Meyerhoff D. J., Price R. W., Fuchs D., Zetterberg H., et al. (2018a). Longitudinal trajectories of brain volume and cortical thickness in treated and untreated primary human immunodeficiency virus infection. Clin. Infect. Dis. 67 1697–1704. 10.1093/cid/ciy362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sanford R., Fellows L. K., Ances B. M., Collins D. L. (2018b). Association of brain structure changes and cognitive function with combination antiretroviral therapy in HIV-positive individuals. JAMA Neurol. 75 72–79. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.3036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Saylor D., Dickens A. M., Sacktor N., Haughey N., Slusher B., Pletnikov M., et al. (2016). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder - pathogenesis and prospects for treatment. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 12:309. 10.1038/nrneurol.2016.27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schouten J., Cinque P., Gisslen M., Reiss P., Portegies P. (2011). HIV-1 infection and cognitive impairment in the cART era: a review. AIDS 25 561–575. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e3283437f9a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shi C., Kang L., Yao S., Ma Y., Li T., Liang Y., et al. (2015). The matrics consensus cognitive battery (MCCB): co-norming and standardization in China. Schizophr. Res. 169 109–115. 10.1016/j.schres.2015.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sinharay S., Hammoud D. A. (2019). Brain PET imaging: value for understanding the pathophysiology of HIV-associated Neurocognitive disorder (HAND). Curr. HIV/AIDS Rep. 16 66–75. 10.1007/s11904-019-00419-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Su T., Caan M. W., Wit F. W., Schouten J., Geurtsen G. J., Cole J. H., et al. (2016). White matter structure alterations in HIV-1-infected men with sustained suppression of viraemia on treatment. AIDS 30 311–322. 10.1097/QAD.0000000000000945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Thomas J. B., Brier M. R., Snyder A. Z., Vaida F. F., Ances B. M. (2013). Pathways to neurodegeneration: effects of HIV and aging on resting-state functional connectivity. Neurology 80 1186–1193. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318288792b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Underwood J., Cole J. H., Caan M., De Francesco D., Leech R., van Zoest R. A., et al. (2017). Gray and white matter abnormalities in treated human immunodeficiency virus disease and their relationship to cognitive function. Clin. Infect. Dis. 65 422–432. 10.1093/cid/cix301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Underwood J., De Francesco D., Cole J. H., Caan M., van Zoest R. A., Schmand B. A., et al. (2019). Validation of a novel multivariate method of defining HIV-associated cognitive impairment. Open Forum Infect Dis 6:ofz198. 10.1093/ofid/ofz198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Vera J. H., Guo Q., Cole J. H., Boasso A., Greathead L., Kelleher P., et al. (2016). Neuroinflammation in treated HIV-positive individuals: a TSPO PET study. Neurology 86 1425–1432. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Vera J. H., Ridha B., Gilleece Y., Amlani A., Thorburn P., Dizdarevic S. (2017). PET brain imaging in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) in the era of combination antiretroviral therapy. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. I. 44 895–902. 10.1007/s00259-016-3602-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wang P., Li J., Wang X., Thapa D., Wu G. (2018). Asymptomatic human immunodeficiency virus vertical transmitted adolescents’ brain functional changes: based on resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. AIDS Res. Hum. Retrov. 34 699–704. 10.1089/aid.2017.0267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wang X., Foryt P., Ochs R., Chung J. H., Wu Y., Parrish T., et al. (2011). Abnormalities in resting-state functional connectivity in early human immunodeficiency virus infection. Brain Connect 1 207–217. 10.1089/brain.2011.0016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wiesman A. I., O Neill J., Mills M. S., Robertson K. R., Fox H. S., Swindells S., et al. (2018). Aberrant occipital dynamics differentiate HIV-infected patients with and without cognitive impairment. Brain 141 1678–1690. 10.1093/brain/awy097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wilson T. W., Heinrichs-Graham E., Becker K. M., Aloi J., Robertson K. R., Sandkovsky U., et al. (2015). Multimodal neuroimaging evidence of alterations in cortical structure and function in HIV-infected older adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36 897–910. 10.1002/hbm.22674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Witting N., Kupers R. C., Svensson P., Arendt-Nielsen L., Gjedde A., Jensen T. S. (2001). Experimental brush-evoked allodynia activates posterior parietal cortex. Neurology 57 1817–1824. 10.1212/wnl.57.10.1817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Woods S. P., Moore D. J., Weber E., Grant I. (2009). Cognitive neuropsychology of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neuropsychol. Rev. 19 152–168. 10.1007/s11065-009-9102-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Xia M., Wang J., He Y. (2013). BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8:e68910. 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Yan C., Cheung B., Kelly C., Colcombe S., Craddock R. C., Di Martino A., et al. (2013). A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional connectomics. Neuroimage 76 183–201. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yan C., Wang X., Zuo X., Zang Y. (2016). DPABI: data processing & analysis for (Resting-State) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics 14 339–351. 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Zayyad Z., Spudich S. (2015). Neuropathogenesis of HIV: from initial neuroinvasion to hiv-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND). Curr. HIV/AIDS Rep. 12 16–24. 10.1007/s11904-014-0255-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Zhu D. C., Tarumi T., Khan M. A., Zhang R. (2015). Vascular coupling in resting-state fMRI: evidence from multiple modalities. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 35 1910–1920. 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Zhuang Y., Qiu X., Wang L., Ma Q., Mapstone M., Luque A., et al. (2017). Combination antiretroviral therapy improves cognitive performance and functional connectivity in treatment-naive HIV-infected individuals. J. Neurovirol. 23 704–712. 10.1007/s13365-017-0553-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.


Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES