Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2020 Dec 3;11:587886. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.587886

Using Smartphones When Eating Increases Caloric Intake in Young People: An Overview of the Literature

Marco La Marra 1,*,, Giorgio Caviglia 2,, Raffaella Perrella 2,
PMCID: PMC7744612  PMID: 33343462

Abstract

Recent literature highlights that the use of smartphones during meals increases the number of calories ingested in young people. Although the distraction interferes with physiological signals of hunger and satiety, a social facilitation effect has also been suggested. Cognition is a pivotal component in regulating food intake, and activities requiring high perceptual demands should be discouraged during meals.

Keywords: social facilitation, distraction, obesity, food intake, smartphone

Introduction

In recent decades, the use of smartphones has gradually increased worldwide. Currently, more than three billion people in the world own a smartphone with an expected increase in the next few years of several hundred million (O’Dea, 2020).

The mobile device has become an essential tool in everyday life, especially among young people, where one in four teenagers claims to use their smartphone constantly (Kabali et al., 2015; Lenhart, 2015).

In the United States, cell phone access in young people has increased from 40% in 2004 to 75% in 2013 (Rideout et al., 2010, 2013), and 53% report having a smartphone from the age of 11, with an increase of more than 80% to 14 years (Anderson and Jiang, 2018; Rideout and Robb, 2019). European figures also record a progressive rise, where 64% of young people have a smartphone at 15–16 years, 55% at 13–14, 40% at 11–12, and 20% at 9–10 years (Mascheroni and Cuman, 2014). Social media applications, in particular, are the most used, with 38% of young people claiming their use several times per hour and 16% interacting with them continuously (Rideout and Robb, 2018).

Linearly related to owning a mobile device, teenagers report a growing increase in media multitasking activities during their ordinary daily activities: in the bathroom, in bed, on the street, and especially during mealtimes (Webby Awards, 2015). This trend does not seem to slow down and could increase further.

Although technological development has led to some improvements across society (such as fast communication and content transmission facilities), certain negative aspects have also been highlighted. These include social isolation, addictive use behavior (Takao et al., 2009) and, more recently, interference with eating behavior, and the amount of calories ingested has been suggested.

Overall, current evidence reveals that the use of digital technologies referred to as “screen time” (devices using a screen) represents an obesity risk factor, especially for young people.

Investigations previously conducted using media devices, such as television and video games, showed a consistent correlation between time spent watching television, body mass index (BMI), and adiposity level (Coon and Tucker, 2002; Janz et al., 2002; Staiano et al., 2013). It was pointed out that people increased their food intake when they were allowed to eat snacks while playing video games or watching television (Temple et al., 2007; Chaput et al., 2011).

More recently, some preliminary studies extend a similar effect to smartphone use (Kenney and Gortmaker, 2017). However, unlike studies conducted with other multimedia devices, research investigating the relationship between food intake and smartphone use is at an early stage. Nevertheless, the American Academy of Pediatrics (2016) expands the effects of television and video games to mobile phones. This is reasonable in the discussion on weight management, since smartphone use—like other forms of screen time device—is also a sedentary activity (Lanningham-Foster et al., 2006). Furthermore, the compact size and one-handed operation make the smartphone the most popular multimedia device currently used during meals.

It has been highlighted that media multitasking via smartphone provides a distracting effect for tasks including reading an article and crossing the street (Stavrinos et al., 2009; Chen and Yan, 2016). Drawing on this observations, in our review, we will analyze the first experimental evidence that smartphone use, similar to other technological devices (Bellisle et al., 2004; Brunstrom and Mitchell, 2006; Hetherington et al., 2006; Robinson et al., 2013), promotes food intake by distracting users from eating behavior.

Smartphone Distraction and Eating Behavior

Research identifying distraction as something that can promote food intake has been ongoing for quite a few years now. Eating in competition with other tasks has been shown to increase food intake, especially when the tasks are cognitively demanding (Hetherington et al., 2006). For instance, ambient music and the social context in which people consume their meals affect the number of calories ingested (Van der Bilt, 2011; Chapman et al., 2014; Higgs, 2015; Marsh et al., 2015). In a similar vein, experimental evidence indicates that several factors can distract from eating including listening to a story (Bellisle and Dalix, 2001; Long et al., 2011), background music (Stroebele and de Castro, 2006), playing a computer game (Oldham-Cooper et al., 2011), and engaging in a counting task (Boon et al., 2002). This effect was linearly related to the distraction provided by concomitant activities which affected the ability to correctly record the actual amount of food ingested (Higgs and Woodward, 2009; Marsh et al., 2015). Therefore, engaging in a task that diverts attention away from food could interfere with eating behavior and lead to greater caloric intake.

Similarly, studies using multimedia devices show that the distraction produced by watching a television program interferes with the amount of food consumed (Hetherington et al., 2006; Bellissimo et al., 2007; Patel et al., 2011; Marsh et al., 2013; Ogden et al., 2017). In particular, television content affects attention influencing eating behavior (Chapman et al., 2014), regardless of appetite level (Blass et al., 2006). In addition, Moray et al. (2007) found that the amounts of food ingested were less accurate if participants watched television while eating.

The distraction provided by smartphone use has also been highlighted in recent years (Kenney and Gortmaker, 2017). In a study conducted among 62 university volunteers between 18 and 28 years of both sexes, da Mata Gonçalves et al. (2019) evaluated the distraction produced by smartphone use during meals on both the total caloric intake and the type of calories ingested. During the trial sessions, participants were introduced to a snack task in which they were asked to eat under three different experimental conditions: no distraction, using their smartphones, or reading a printed text. After each session, the total calorie intake and nutritional composition of the food ingested were measured. The results showed that eating in the presence of distractors (smartphone/reading of printed articles) increased the total calorie intake by 15% with higher lipid ingestion. These results showed that smartphone use during meals, as well as reading a printed text, significantly affects the number of calories ingested.

Further, taking into account the external factors influencing eating behavior, coupled to the most recent observations concerning distraction effects on the number of calories ingested, Lopez et al. (2019a) tested the hypothesis by which children engaged in smartphone media multitasking activities (MMT) would be more driven by environmental stimuli. In their study, they verified the relationship between media multitasking—the use and switching from unrelated forms of digital media—and obesity risk. The authors recruited a sample of 179 pre-adolescent children aged 9–11 years and investigated the relationship between media multitasking and BMI. Their results showed a positive association between the frequency of children’s MMT behavior and BMI, regardless of physical activity, suggesting that the use of screen time technologies affects food intake by diverting attention to external stimuli. In line with previous findings, individuals eat more as a result of reduced cognitive control about the amount of food ingested (Ogden et al., 2013; Dohle et al., 2017).

Psychological research examined in more detail the role of attention during meals. Through experimental paradigms, it has been proved that eating distractedly increases both the current food intake and the amount of food consumed at subsequent meals (Higgs and Woodward, 2009; Higgs and Donohoe, 2011; Mittal et al., 2011; Oldham-Cooper et al., 2011; Ogden et al., 2017). Distractors significantly affect dietary memory formation, preventing the proper awareness of food ingested and interfering with hunger and satiety signals. Satiety is a key component of appetite control and refers to the feeling of fullness which suppresses additional intake (Blundell and Tremblay, 1995; Morris et al., 2020). It is the result of physiological processes that tend to split into early cognitive and sensory influences and, subsequently, into post-ingestive effects (Blundell and Tremblay, 1995; Bellisle and Blundell, 2013). More recent cognitive models of eating behavior suggested that satiety is partly cognitively constructed and memory dependent (Higgs et al., 2017). These models are supported by consistent evidence that reducing memory for food consumed by interfering with attention at the time of consumption increases subsequent food intake (Higgs and Woodward, 2009; Mittal et al., 2011; Oldham-Cooper et al., 2011; Robinson et al., 2013; Higgs, 2015). Besides, the impact of distraction on calorie intake could be related to the way that different types of dietary contexts influence an individual. According to Ogden et al. (2013), a possible explanation lies in the multidimensional nature of the distraction, which could affect the link between hunger and changes in the desire to eat. From this perspective, she proposed two forms of distraction: distraction away from hunger and distraction away from eating. Once external factors are distracted from internal stimuli, such as hunger and satiety, the individual eats mindlessly and food intake would not be encoded to affect their desire to eat. Nevertheless, food intake requires a certain cognitive effort in itself, and if too distracted, the subject will have insufficient cognitive resources to engage in eating behavior. To date, the influence of attention and memory on eating behavior is well known (Chieffi et al., 2011a,b, 2015; Robinson et al., 2014a; Higgs and Spetter, 2018), and neuropsychological evidence shows that memory deterioration, or amnesia, corresponds to increased food intake (Rozin et al., 1998; Higgs et al., 2008; Chieffi et al., 2017). Furthermore, experimental evidence shows that increased awareness of the calories ingested during previous meals reduced the number of calories ingested subsequently in both normal and overweight subjects (Higgs et al., 2008; Higgs and Donohoe, 2011; Robinson et al., 2014b; Seguias and Tapper, 2018). Similarly, weight reduction treatments, which limited the time spent watching television or playing video games during meals, produced a greater decrease in BMI (Robinson, 1999). However, no correlation between digital device use and caloric intake has also been reported (Whitelock et al., 2018; Whitelock and Robinson, 2018) and further investigations are needed.

Smartphone and Social Facilitation

The main difference between smartphones and traditional digital technologies is that the phone provides an easier way to enjoy intrinsically social activities. Studies investigating mobile phone usage models reveal that social interaction and peer chat features are the most used, and teenagers are estimated to send over 110 messages per day (Lenhart, 2015; Smith and Page, 2015; Teo et al., 2018).

Smartphone use implies that adolescents engaging in multitasking activities during meals interact with friends and family in a distinctly different way than other screen time devices. It has been observed that individuals tend to overeat in the presence of friends and family, so social activity with their smartphones during meals could play a significant role in eating behavior.

The construction of “social facilitation” implies that people tend to change their behavior according to others’ behavior (Herman, 2015). Concerning eating behavior, eaters tend to activate food intake in people who do not eat (de Castro and de Castro, 1989; de Castro, 1997). In the same way, the presence of passive individuals who do not eat will make eaters more aware of their behavior, inducing them to decrease food consumption (Herman, 2015).

It has been shown that simple companionship can increase food intake by about 44% (de Castro and de Castro, 1989; de Castro, 1997), exerting a facilitating effect that manifests itself independently of real food needs (de Castro and de Castro, 1989). Moreover, this effect is cross-cultural (Herman, 2015).

Several studies indicated that social influence is so pervasive that even a simple online presence through digital technologies is enough to trigger facilitation effects in a variety of activities ranging from labyrinth resolution and arithmetic tasks to physical exercise (Park and Catrambone, 2007; Anderson-Hanley et al., 2011; Snyder et al., 2012).

Extending these results, Teo et al. (2018) tested the hypothesis by which the virtual presence of friends and parents—interconnected through a telephone messaging service—exerts a social facilitation effect on eating behavior. In a study of 50 Singaporean male adolescents, they examined whether social activity via smartphone could affect the number of calories ingested. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the following telephone activities: (i) sending and receiving messages (social activity) or (ii) reading a neutral article (non-social activity). Their results showed that participants consumed more calories when interacting virtually than those reading the article.

These findings seem to confirm the scientific literature highlighting the role of social influences on food intake (de Castro, 1997; Herman, 2015) and suggested that different ways of smartphone use may influence individuals to eat more than required. However, as suggested by the authors, virtual social facilitation is an emerging concept which needs further investigations.

Using mobile phones, people interacting via messaging service cannot be considered as eating co-actors. Analogously, people receiving messages are not aware of the amount of food their counterpart eats, minimizing the importance of maintaining an impression through food intake.

To date, virtual social facilitation cannot be described in the same way of a group exerting co-action or passive audience effects, and future research will have to investigate whether the impact of social facilitation can also apply to the digital realm.

Discussion

In recent years, smartphone use has progressively increased in the youth population, especially during meals (Kabali et al., 2015).

As reported in our review, preliminary findings suggest that distraction and social facilitation can be taken into account to explain the link between smartphone use and food intake. However, although a social facilitation effect has been observed even in a virtual context, further investigations should exclude any alternative cognitive explanations (Teo et al., 2018). Indeed, participants engaged in a social interaction activity via smartphone messaging service could eat more because they were more distracted (Bellisle et al., 2004; Brunstrom and Mitchell, 2006; Robinson et al., 2013) rather than because the act of messaging was social.

As reported in the above sections, smartphone use affects food intake by diverting attention from eating behavior; with reduced cognitive resources, the user engages in “mindless eating” and consumes more food (Ogden et al., 2013; Dohle et al., 2017).

Some potential mechanisms have been proposed to explain the role of distraction in eating behavior (La Marra et al., 2009; Robinson et al., 2014a; Higgs, 2015; Higgs and Spetter, 2018). However, more recently, it has been highlighted that the perceptual load theory could be successfully applied to the study of ingestive behavior (Morris et al., 2020). The perceptual load theory is a key theory in the literature on selective attention and implies that the extent to which task-irrelevant stimuli are processed is regulated by attention availability (Lavie, 2005, 2010). It is a passive process carried out automatically by the perceptual system at an early stage of selection and is determined by whether the primary task leaves adequate spare perceptual capacity (Lavie, 2005, 2010). Similarly, it has been suggested that appetite control based on satiety could be altered when attention is absorbed in a perceptually demanding task (Morris et al., 2020). A reliable satiety response is provided by cognitive and physiological stimuli integration (Yeomans and Chambers, 2011; Chambers et al., 2013; Camps et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2016; McCrickerd et al., 2020). Therefore, physiological signals can be altered by the perceptual load, affecting appetite control during consumption. These results are based on existing models of appetite regulation (Bellisle and Blundell, 2013), which emphasize the role of cognitive influences on satiety.

Although the role of biological, environmental, and cultural factors in determining dietary behavior is widely recognized (Monda et al., 2017; Qasim et al., 2018), recent experimental evidence also supports the role of cognition in satiety (Higgs et al., 2017), which could be altered by a high perceptual load. Therefore, factors acting on satiety (such as post-ingestive stimuli derived from nutrients) may also depend on the availability of basic perceptual capacity. The perceptual load is known to substantially interfere with the processing of information, from the early stages of perceptual elaboration to the encoding of memory, as indexed by both behavioral and neural measurements (Lavie, 2005, 2010).

A further interpretation points to the role of dopaminergic pathways. These are mainly implied in the “reward dependence” mechanisms. These mechanisms are related to the activity of projections to the limbic areas, mainly exerting facilitation, and controlled by the prefrontal cortex which inhibits (DLPFC) or stops (orbitofrontal cortex) food assumption, in particular as regards its compulsive-like behaviors (Lopez et al., 2019b). On the other hand, the same areas are heavily implied in attentional processes (Supervisory Attentional System—SAS; Shallice et al., 1989), in the cognitive estimation of stimuli of the environment (including the food) and the consequences of eating behaviors. It is possible that the allocation of resources to stimuli other than food (the smartphone) could divert the frontal areas from exerting executive control on food assumption.

Furthermore, another neurobiological mechanism could be ascribed to the role of serotonergic pathways. These are mainly related to “harm avoidance” behaviors. According to the theory of serotonin/dopamine balance (Cools et al., 2011), increased activity of dopaminergic pathways entails reduced activity of serotonergic ones. Attempting to include all these insights in a coherent pathophysiological framework, we would suggest the following cascade of events: (1) The reward dependence activates dopaminergic extrafrontal pathways (in particular mesolimbic); (2) the interfering stimuli prevent frontal areas to exert normal control on the cognitive estimation of food assumption and/or to stop the calorie intake; and (3) the imbalance of dopaminergic/serotonergic mechanisms led to acting worrying food-related behaviors. Finally, the well-known neurobiological mechanisms of long-term potentiation and neural plasticity may give rise to a stable pattern of eating behaviors and to increase the risk of affective disorders like depression.

This evidence seems to be particularly worrying considering that mobile phone overuse in some cases represents a risk behavior comparable to addiction (Domoff et al., 2020).

Our review underlines that the use of mobile devices during meals interferes with eating behavior contributing to calorie increase in a segment of the population for which the international scientific community is particularly concerned. This knowledge could help to inform cognitive dietary interventions about the importance of encouraging participants to pay attention to food intake.

Author Contributions

All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our very great appreciation to Prof. Alessandro Iavarone for his valuable and constructive suggestions during the planning and development of this review work. His willingness to give his time so generously has been very much appreciated.

References

  1. American Academy of Pediatrics (2016). More Time on Digital Devices Means Kids Less Likely to Finish Homework. Available online at: https://www.healthychildren.org/English/news/Pages/More-Time-on-Digital-Devices-Means-Kids-Less-Likely-to-Finish-Homework.aspx (accessed March 19, 2020). [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson M., Jiang J. (2018). Teens, Social Media & Technology 2018. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson-Hanley C., Snyder A. L., Nimon J. P., Arciero P. J. (2011). Social facilitation in virtual reality-enhanced exercise: competitiveness moderates exercise effort of older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 6 275–280. 10.2147/cia.s25337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bellisle F., Blundell J. E. (eds). (2013). “Satiation, satiety: concepts and organisation of behaviour,” in Satiation, Satiety and the Control of Food Intake, (Sawston: Woodhead Publishing; ), 3–11. 10.1533/9780857098719.1.3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bellisle F., Dalix A.-M. (2001). Cognitive restraint can be offset by distraction, leading to increased meal intake in women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 74 197–200. 10.1093/ajcn/74.2.197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bellisle F., Dalix A.-M., Slama G. (2004). Non food-related environmental stimuli induce increased meal intake in healthy women: comparison of television viewing versus listening to a recorded story in laboratory settings. Appetite 43 175–180. 10.1016/j.appet.2004.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bellissimo N., Pencharz P. B., Thomas S. G., Anderson G. H. (2007). Effect of television viewing at mealtime on food intake after a glucose preload in boys. Pediatr. Res. 61 745–749. 10.1203/pdr.0b013e3180536591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blass E. M., Anderson D. R., Kirkorian H. L., Pempek T. A., Price I., Koleini M. F. (2006). On the road to obesity. Television viewing increases intake of high – density foods. Physiol. Behav. 88 597–604. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blundell J. E., Tremblay A. (1995). Appetite control and energy (fuel) balance. Nutr. Res. Rev. 8 225–242. 10.1079/NRR19950014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boon B., Stroebe W., Schut H., Ijntema R. (2002). Ironic processes in the eating behaviour of restrained eaters. Br. J. Health Psychol. 7 1–10. 10.1348/135910702169303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brunstrom J. M., Mitchell G. L. (2006). Effects of distraction on the development of satiety. Br. J. Nutr. 96 761–769. 10.1079/BJN20061880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Camps G., Mars M., De Graaf C., Smeets P. A. (2016). Empty calories and phantom fullness: a randomized trial studying the relative effects of energy density and viscosity on gastric emptying determined by MRI and satiety. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 104 73–80. 10.3945/ajcn.115.129064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chambers L., Ells H., Yeomans M. R. (2013). Can the satiating power of a high energy beverage be improved by manipulating sensory characteristics and label information? Food Qual. Prefer. 28 271–278. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.08.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chapman C. D., Nilsson V. C., Thune H. Å., Cedernaes J., Le Grevès M., Hogenkamp P. S., et al. (2014). Watching TV and food intake: the role of content. PLoS One 9:e100602. 10.1371/journal.pone.0100602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chaput J.-P., Visby T., Nyby S., Klingenberg L., Gregersen N. T., Tremblay A., et al. (2011). Video game playing increases food intake in adolescents: a randomized crossover study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 93 1196–1203. 10.3945/ajcn.110.008680 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chen Q., Yan Z. (2016). Does multitasking with mobile phones affect learning? A review. Comput. Hum. Behav. 54 34–42. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chen Y., Lin Y. C., Zimmerman C. A., Essner R. A., Knight Z. A. (2016). Hunger neurons drive feeding through a sustained, positive reinforcement signal. eLife 5:e18640. 10.7554/eLife.18640 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chieffi S., Iavarone A., La Marra M., Messina G., Villano I., Ranucci S., et al. (2015). Memory for proprioceptive targets in bulimia nervosa. J. Psychiatry 18:297 10.4172/2378-5756.1000297 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chieffi S., La Marra M., Messina G., De Luca V., Messina A., Monda M. (2011a). β-amyloid peptide in pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Top. Pept. Protein Res. 12 65–69. [Google Scholar]
  20. Chieffi S., La Marra M., Viggiano A., Messina G., De Luca V., Monda M. (2011b). Caffeine protection against β-amyloid peptide toxicity in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Top. Pept. Protein Res. 12 71–75. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chieffi S., Messina A., Villano I., Valenzano A. A., Nigro E., La Marra M., et al. (2017). The use of velocity information in movement reproduction. Front. Psychol. 8:983. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00983 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cools R., Nakamura K., Daw N. D. (2011). Serotonin and dopamine: unifying affective, activational, and decision functions. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36 98–113. 10.1038/npp.2010.121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Coon K. A., Tucker K. L. (2002). Television and children’s consumption patterns. A review of the literature. Minerva Pediatr. 54 423–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. da Mata Gonçalves R. F., de Almeida Barreto D., Monteiro P. I., Zangeronimo M. G., Castelo P. M., van der Bilt A., et al. (2019). Smartphone use while eating increases caloric ingestion. Physiol. Behav. 204 93–99. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.02.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. de Castro J. M. (1997). Socio-cultural determinants of meal size and frequency. Br. J. Nutr. 77(Suppl. 1), S39–S55. 10.1079/BJN19970103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. de Castro J. M., de Castro E. S. (1989). Spontaneous meal patterns of humans: influence of the presence of other people. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 50 237–247. 10.1093/ajcn/50.2.237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dohle S., Diel K., Hofmann W. (2017). Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: a review. Appetite 124 4–9. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Domoff S. E., Sutherland E. Q., Yokum S., Gearhardt A. N. (2020). Adolescents’ addictive phone use: associations with eating behaviors and adiposity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2861. 10.3390/ijerph17082861 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Herman C. P. (2015). The social facilitation of eating. A review. Appetite 86 61–73. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hetherington M. M., Anderson A. S., Norton G. N. M., Newson L. (2006). Situational effects on meal intake: a comparison of eating alone and eating with others. Physiol. Behav. 88 498–505. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Higgs S. (2015). Manipulations of attention during eating and their effects on later snack intake. Appetite 92 287–294. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Higgs S., Donohoe J. E. (2011). Focusing on food during lunch enhances lunch memory and decreases later snack intake. Appetite 57 202–206. 10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Higgs S., Spetter M. S. (2018). Cognitive control of eating: the role of memory in appetite and weight gain. Curr. Obes. Rep. 7 50–59. 10.1007/s13679-018-0296-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Higgs S., Spetter M. S., Thomas J. M., Rotshtein P., Lee M., Hallschmid M., et al. (2017). Interactions between metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control: implications for novel weight management therapies. J. Psychopharmacol. 31 1460–1474. 10.1177/0269881117736917 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Higgs S., Williamson A. C., Attwood A. S. (2008). Recall of recent lunch and its effect on subsequent snack intake. Physiol. Behav. 94 454–462. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Higgs S., Woodward M. (2009). Television watching during lunch increases afternoon snack intake of young women. Appetite 52 39–43. 10.1016/j.appet.2008.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Janz K. F., Levy S. M., Burns T. L., Torner J. C., Willing M. C., Warren J. J. (2002). Fatness, physical activity, and television viewing in children during the adiposity rebound period: the Iowa bone development study. Prev. Med. 35 563–571. 10.1006/pmed.2002.1113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kabali H. K., Irigoyen M. M., Nunez-Davis R., Budacki J. G., Mohanty S. H., Leister K. P., et al. (2015). Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics 136 1044–1050. 10.1542/peds.2015-2151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kenney E. L., Gortmaker S. L. (2017). United States adolescents’ television, computer, videogame, smartphone, and tablet use: associations with sugary drinks, sleep, physical activity, and obesity. J. Pediatr. 182 144–149. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. La Marra M., Sapuppo W., Caviglia G. (2009). Dissociazione e alessitimia in un campione di pazienti con Disturbo del Comportamento Alimentare. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 12 6–20. 10.4081/ripppo.2009.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lanningham-Foster L., Jensen T. B., Foster R. C., Redmond A. B., Walker B. A., Heinz D., et al. (2006). Energy expenditure of sedentary screen time compared with active screen time for children. Pediatrics 118 e1831–e1835. 10.1542/peds.2006-1087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lavie N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9 75–82. 10.1016/J.TICS.2004.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lavie N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19 143–148. 10.1177/0963721410370295 26728138 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lenhart A. (2015). Teens, Social Media & Technology Overview. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  45. Long S., Meyer C., Leung N., Wallis D. J. (2011). Effects of distraction and focused attention on actual and perceived food intake in females with non-clinical eating psychopathology. Appetite 56 350–356. 10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lopez R. B., Brand J., Gilbert-Diamond D. (2019a). Media multitasking is associated with higher body mass index in pre-adolescent children. Front. Psychol. 10:2534. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lopez R. B., Courtney A. L., Wagner D. D. (2019b). Recruitment of cognitive control regions during effortful self-control is associated with altered brain activity in control and reward systems in dieters during subsequent exposure to food commercials. PeerJ 7:e6550. 10.7717/peerj.6550 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Marsh S., Mhurchu C. N., Jiang Y., Maddison R. (2015). Modern screen-use behaviors: the effects of single-and multi-screen use on energy intake. J. Adolesc. Health 56 543–549. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.01.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Marsh S., Mhurchu C. N., Maddison R. (2013). The non-advertising effects of screen-based sedentary activities on acute eating behaviours in children, adolescents, and young adults. A systematic review. Appetite 71 259–273. 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mascheroni G., Cuman A. (2014). Net Children go Mobile: Final Report (with Country Fact Sheets). Milan: Educatt. [Google Scholar]
  51. McCrickerd K., Tay P. P. S., Tang C. S., Forde C. G. (2020). Using sensory cues to optimise the satiety value of a reduced-calorie product labelled ‘healthier choice’. Nutrients 12:107. 10.3390/nu12010107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mittal D., Stevenson R. J., Oaten M. J., Miller L. A. (2011). Snacking while watching TV impairs food recall and promotes food intake on a later TV free test meal. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 25 871–877. 10.1002/acp.1760 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Monda V., La Marra M., Perrella R., Caviglia G., Iavarone A., Chieffi S., et al. (2017). Obesity and brain illness: from cognitive and psychological evidences to obesity paradox. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Obes. 10 473–479. 10.2147/DMSO.S148392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moray J., Fu A., Brill K., Mayoral M. S. (2007). Viewing television while eating impairs the ability to accurately estimate total amount of food consumed. Bariatr. Nurs. Surg. Patient Care 2 71–76. 10.1089/bar.2006.9991 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Morris J., Vi C. T., Obrist M., Forster S., Yeomans M. R. (2020). Ingested but not perceived: response to satiety cues disrupted by perceptual load. Appetite 155:104813. 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. O’Dea S. (2020). Smartphone Users Worldwide 2016-2021. Hamburg: Statista. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ogden J., Coop N., Cousins C., Crump R., Field L., Hughes S., et al. (2013). Distraction, the desire to eat and food intake. Towards an expanded model of mindless eating. Appetite 62 119–126. 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ogden J., Oikonomou E., Alemany G. (2017). Distraction, restrained eating and disinhibition: an experimental study of food intake and the impact of “eating on the go”. J. Health Psychol. 22 39–50. 10.1177/1359105315595119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Oldham-Cooper R. E., Hardman C. A., Nicoll C. E., Rogers P. J., Brunstrom J. M. (2011). Playing a computer game during lunch affects fullness, memory for lunch, and later snack intake. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 93 308–313. 10.3945/ajcn.110.004580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Park S., Catrambone R. (2007). Social facilitation effects of virtual humans. Hum. Fact. 49 1054–1060. 10.1518/001872007X249910 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Patel B. P., Bellissimo N., Thomas S. G., Hamilton J. K., Anderson G. H. (2011). Television viewing at mealtime reduces caloric compensation in peripubertal, but not postpubertal, girls. Pediatr. Res. 70 513–517. 10.1203/pdr.0b013e31822d783e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Qasim A., Turcotte M., De Souza R. J., Samaan M. C., Champredon D., Dushoff J., et al. (2018). On the origin of obesity: identifying the biological, environmental and cultural drivers of genetic risk among human populations. Obes. Rev. 19 121–149. 10.1111/obr.12625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rideout V., Robb M. B. (2018). Social Media, Social Life: Teens Reveal their Experiences. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media. [Google Scholar]
  64. Rideout V., Saphir M., Tsang V., Bozdech B. (2013). Zero to Eight: Children’s Media use in America. San Francisco, CA. [Google Scholar]
  65. Rideout V. J., Foehr U. G., Roberts D. F. (2010). Generation M 2: Media in the Lives of 8-to 18-Year-Olds. San Francisco, CA: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  66. Rideout V. J., Robb M. B. (2019). The Common Sense Census: Media use by Tweens and Teens. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media. [Google Scholar]
  67. Robinson E., Aveyard P., Daley A., Jolly K., Lewis A., Lycett D., et al. (2013). Eating attentively: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of food intake memory and awareness on eating. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 97 728–742. 10.3945/ajcn.112.045245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Robinson E., Kersbergen I., Brunstrom J. M., Field M. (2014a). I’m watching you. Awareness that food consumption is being monitored is a demand characteristic in eating-behaviour experiments. Appetite 83 19–25. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.07.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Robinson E., Kersbergen I., Higgs S. (2014b). Eating ‘attentively’ reduces later energy consumption in overweight and obese females. Br. J. Nutr. 112 657–661. 10.1017/S000711451400141X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Robinson T. N. (1999). Reducing children’s television viewing to prevent obesity: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 282 1561–1567. 10.1001/jama.282.16.1561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rozin P., Dow S., Moscovitch M., Rajaram S. (1998). What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychol. Sci. 9 392–396. 10.1111/1467-9280.00073 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Seguias L., Tapper K. (2018). The effect of mindful eating on subsequent intake of a high calorie snack. Appetite 121 93–100. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.10.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Shallice T., Burgess P. W., Schon F., Baxter D. M. (1989). The origins of utilization behaviour. Brain 112 1587–1598. 10.1093/brain/112.6.1587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Smith A., Page D. (2015). US Smartphone use in 2015. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  75. Snyder A. L., Anderson-Hanley C., Arciero P. J. (2012). Virtual and live social facilitation while exergaming: competitiveness moderates exercise intensity. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34 252–259. 10.1123/jsep.34.2.252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Staiano A. E., Harrington D. M., Broyles S. T., Gupta A. K., Katzmarzyk P. T. (2013). Television, adiposity, and cardiometabolic risk in children and adolescents. Am. J. Prev. Med. 44 40–47. 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.09.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Stavrinos D., Byington K. W., Schwebel D. C. (2009). Effect of cell phone distraction on pediatric pedestrian injury risk. Pediatrics 123 e179–e185. 10.1542/peds.2008-1382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Stroebele N., de Castro J. M. (2006). Listening to music while eating is related to increases in people’s food intake and meal duration. Appetite 47 285–289. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Takao M., Takahashi S., Kitamura M. (2009). Addictive personality and problematic mobile phone use. Cyber Psychol. Behav. 12 501–507. 10.1089/cpb.2009.0022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Temple J. L., Giacomelli A. M., Kent K. M., Roemmich J. N., Epstein L. H. (2007). Television watching increases motivated responding for food and energy intake in children. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 85 355–361. 10.1093/ajcn/85.2.355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Teo E., Goh D., Vijayakumar K. M., Liu J. C. (2018). To message or browse? Exploring the impact of phone use patterns on male adolescents’ consumption of palatable snacks. Front. Psychol. 8:2298. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Van der Bilt A. (2011). Assessment of mastication with implications for oral rehabilitation: a review. J. Oral Rehabil. 38 754–780. 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2010.02197.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Webby Awards (2015). 2015 Annual Trend Report. Ashland, TN: International Academy of Digital Arts and Sciences. [Google Scholar]
  84. Whitelock V., Higgs S., Brunstrom J. M., Halford J. C., Robinson E. (2018). No effect of focused attention whilst eating on later snack food intake: two laboratory experiments. Appetite 128 188–196. 10.1016/j.appet.2018.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Whitelock V., Robinson E. (2018). Remembered meal satisfaction, satiety, and later snack food intake: a laboratory study. Nutrients 10:1883. 10.3390/nu10121883 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yeomans M. R., Chambers L. (2011). Satiety-relevant sensory qualities enhance the satiating effects of mixed carbohydrate-protein preloads. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 94 1410–1417. 10.3945/ajcn.111.011650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES