Abstract
Objective
Social media and other digital technology use facilitate connection among adolescents, but also may reinforce norms and substance-related content from peers and advertisers. We use nationally representative data to examine the association between digital technology and past 30-day use of alcohol, cannabis, and vaping.
Methods
Data were drawn from the 2018 Monitoring the Future survey of US adolescents (N=44,482). Poisson regressions estimated the association between hours/day of technology use and past 30-day use of alcohol, cannabis, and vaping adjusting for grade, sociodemographics, and other past-year drug use.
Results
Across grades, mean hours of social media/day was 3.06 (standard deviation=2.90), past 30-day alcohol, cannabis, flavor vaping, cannabis vaping, and nicotine vaping were 15.7%, 12.6%, 10.6%, 4.9%, and 11.2%, respectively. Digital technology use that required interaction with others was associated with increased risk of past 30-day drinking, cannabis use, and vaping. For example, social media 3+ hours/day was associated with past 30-day drinking (adjusted relative risk [aRR]: 1.99, 95% CI: 1.65, 2.41). The magnitude of association was consistent across texting, phone calls, and video chatting, which were all more strongly associated with substance use than with activities that do not require interaction such as gaming and watching videos.
Conclusion
Digital technology that facilitates interaction among adolescents, such as texting and social media, is associated with past substance use. Magnitudes of association are consistent across substances, supporting the hypothesis that networks of adolescents are social drivers of substance use, rather than the technology itself.
Keywords: adolescents, youth, social media, marijuana, alcohol, vaping
1. Introduction
Smart phones and digital technology are ubiquitous among adolescents and facilitate new ways for adolescents to connect. For example, >90% of teens reported multi-daily engagement with social media (e.g. Instagram, Facebook, Snapchat) (Pew Research Center, 2018). While digital technology has potential benefits for social connection (Gross, 2009; Lee, 2009; Pew Research Center, 2018), they also provide a new vector to introduce and reinforce social norms around substance use (Cabrera-Nguyen et al., 2016; Frost and Rickwood, 2017; Miller et al., 2014), as well as expose adolescents to substance-related advertising (Camenga et al., 2018; Jernigan et al., 2017; Krauss et al., 2017; Pokhrel et al., 2018). Historically, manufacturers of addictive products such as alcohol and nicotine have targeted advertising to adolescents in an effort to gain a new market share (Chester et al., 2010; King III and Siegel, 1999; Pucci and Siegel, 1999). While regulation has reduced advertisers’ ability to market to adolescents using traditional media (magazines, TV, etc.), particularly alcohol (Anderson et al., 2009; Jernigan, 2011), new media types may offer other ways to display products to a young market.
Available evidence indicates that exposure to substance-related content, both from peers and advertisers, is common when engaging with digital media, and that involvement with wider social networks and friends engaged with substance-related content is associated with higher substance use (Cabrera-Nguyen et al., 2016; Frost and Rickwood, 2017; Hoffman et al., 2014; Huang et al., 2014). While existing research has predominately focused on alcohol, cannabis content is rapidly expanding given shifting legal markets, and social media depictions of use and users, as well as advertising, are becoming more frequent. Indeed, cannabis advertising is more common on social media that traditional media (Krauss et al., 2017), and cannabis posts by young people on social media are not only more frequent than older adults, but also largely positive in nature (Park and Holody, 2018) and associated with higher levels of cannabis use (Cabrera-Nguyen et al., 2016).
In addition to alcohol and cannabis, vaping and e-cigarette use have increased dramatically among youth in since 2016 (Miech et al., 2019), and social media advertising and peer content has followed. In addition to high levels of e-cigarette advertising in traditional sources such as stores (Margolis et al., 2018), exposure to e-cigarette content on social media is associated with future e-cigarette use (Camenga et al., 2018; Pokhrel et al., 2018), with variation based on social media site.
Many ofthe aforementioned studies focus on specific subsets of the population and, therefore, the total population Journal association between exposure to digital media and risk of alcohol, cannabis and e-cigarette use remains inadequately identified. Furthermore, the landscape of both digital technology use(Pew Research Center, 2018)as well as substance use (Miechetal., 2019)is rapidly changing among youth. Therefore, evaluation of the relationship between digital technology and substance use requires up-to-date data. We use 2018 Monitoring the Future (MTF) data to examine the association between adolescent exposure to traditional and newer forms of media and substance use, namely alcohol (past 2-week binge drinking and past 30-day use), cannabis(past 30-day use), and vaping(past 30-day use). Monitoring the Future is the only national study containing stream-lined, well-replicated measures of digital media engagement and substance use among US school-attending adolescents in 8th, 10th, and 12th grade, thus providing a large sample size and allowing for nationally representative inferences.
2. Methods
2.1. Sample
MTF includes annually administered cross-sectional surveys of 8th, 10th, and 12th grade students in the contiguous US. The survey is conducted at approximately 400 U.S. public and private high schools selected using a multi-stage random sampling design. Selected schools are invited to participate in MTF for 2 years, and schools that decline to participate are replaced with schools that have similar size, geographic location, and urbanicity. While the questionnaire is administered annually, the present study focuses on 2018 as this was the first year that hours per day of media use was queried. The 2018 survey had high school high participation rates: 89%, 86%, and 81% for 8th, 10th, and 12th grade, respectively (Miech et al., 2019). Almost all nonresponse was due to absenteeism; less than 1% of Pre-proof students refused to participate. A detailed description of MTF design and procedures are provided elsewhere (Bachman et al., 2015; Miech et al., 2019). We use sampling weights provided by MTF in all calculations to take into account selection probabilities and differences in sample sizes between schools.
MTF uses a self-administered questionnaire comprising a core form given to all participants that includes assessment of substance use and other key modules, and secondary forms randomly assigned to each participant that contain additional questions; we focus on students who received a form that included questions regarding entertainment, social media, and cell phone use, as well as substance use behaviors. These questions overlapped on 1–2 of 6 forms among 12th grade students (except vaping questions, which did not overlap with media questions), and all forms among 8th and 10th grade students. Form was included as a covariate in all analyses. The maximum sample size for this analysis was 44,482 (14,836 8th graders, 15,144 10th graders, and 14,502 12th graders). Missing data due to nonresponse ranged from 2.42% for the question pertaining to hours per day gaming to 8.80% for the question pertaining to past 30-day drinking.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Entertainment, Social Media, and Cell Phone Use
Students were asked “About how many hours on an average day do you spend: a. playing games on a computer, TV, phone, or other electronic device? b. texting? c. talking on the phone? d. on social networking Web sites like Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, etc.? e. video chatting (Skype, etc.)?” In addition, students were asked “How many hours do you estimate you watch Pre-provideo, TV,ormoviesonanelectronic device (such as a TV, computer, tablet or smartphone)? a. on an average weekday? b. on an average weekend day?” Scales for both sets of questions were: none, less than 1 hour, 1–2 hours, 3–4 hours, 5–6 hours, 7–8 hours, and 9 hours or more. For our study, we combine higher groups to “3 or more hours”. Students were additionally queried about hours spent shopping online and emailing; we did not include those analyses in this paper.
2.2.2. Substance Use
Frequency of substance use in the past 30 days was assessed as: 0, 1–2, 35,6–9,10–19,0–39, and 40+ occasions. We assessed any use (>=1 occasion) for alcohol, cannabis, flavor vaping, cannabis vaping, and nicotine vaping, as well as occasions of use of each as a Poisson distributed outcome. Number of occasions of consuming 5+ drinks (binge drinking) in the past two weeks was also assessed. For 12th graders, questions pertaining to vaping were asked on a different form than the entertainment, social media, and cell phone use questions, so we assessed relationships for 8th and 10th grade students only.
2.2.3. Other Behavioral and Parental Covariates
Two items measured sensation-seeking: “I get a real kick out of doing things that are a little dangerous” and “I like to test myself every now and then by doing something a little risky”. Three measures of internalizing symptoms are analyzed (low self-esteem, high self-derogation, and depressive affect). Four items measured self-esteem: “I take a positive attitude toward myself… I feel I am a person of worth, on an equal plane with others… I am able to do things as well as most other people… On the whole, I'm satisfied with myself.” Four items measured self-derogation: “I feel I do not have much to be proud of… Sometimes I think that I am no good at all… I feel that I canť do anything right… I feel that my life is not very useful.” Four items measured depressive affect: “Life often seems meaningless… The future often seems hopeless… I enjoy life as much as anyone… It feels good to be alive.” Response options for these statements were on a Likert scale (disagree, mostly disagree, neither, mostly agree, agree). Similarly, students were asked how often(never,rarely, sometimes, most times, always) these statements related to parental monitoring occurred: “My parents know where I am after school… When I go out at night, my parents know whom I am with… When I go out at night, my parents know where I am.” Items within each measure were averaged to create one item. Questions for the aforementioned measures (sensation-seeking, parental monitoring, and internalizing symptoms) were included on different forms of the survey and, thus, were asked of different participants. Therefore, we included each measure in separate analyses. Parental monitoring questions were also on a different form than vaping questions, precluding analyses of associations between these variables.
2.2.4. Sociodemographics
We included self-reported sociodemographic variables as control variables (percentages reported are for the entire sample): grade (8th grade, 10th grade, or 12th grade), sex, race/ethnicity (White, Black, Hispanic, multiple races, other) most recent grade point average (GPA) (‘B’ average or higher, or ‘less than B’ average) and highest level of parental education received (some high school, high school/some college, or college or higher).
2.3. Statistical Analysis
First, descriptive analyses graphically examined the association of number of hours spent per day on entertainment, social media, and cell phone use and substance use. For brevity, histograms for 10th grade students are provided in the main results, and histograms for 8th and 12th grade students are included in Supplemental Figures 1 and 2, respectively.
Second, we estimated Poisson regressions (Zou, 2004) of associations between hours/day on entertainment, social media, and cell phone use and each substance use outcome, adjusting for the sociodemographic variables (i.e. grade, race/ethnicity, most recent GPA, and highest level of parental education received), past-year substance Pre-puse,and assigned instrument form. We also performed sensitivity analyses in which we included hours/day using digital technology in the Poisson regression as a continuous variable to test whether using hours/day as a categorical variable might lead to loss of power, information loss, or removal of confounding control (Van Walraven and Hart, 2008). We then included measures of sensation seeking, parental monitoring, or internalizing symptoms in our Poisson regressions to determine whether any observed associations between digital technology and past substance use were spurious. Lastly, we estimated Poisson regressions of associations between hours/day on entertainment, social media, and cell phone use and occasions of substance use among users, adjusting for the aforementioned covariates.
We conducted data management and analysis in SAS, version 9.4. (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, North Carolina) (SAS Institute Inc., 2014). Graphs were generated in ggplot2 (Wickham and Chang, 2014) in R software (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
3. Results
Table 1 provides participant sociodemographic data. A majority of a sample comprised 8th (42.6%) and 10th graders (43.5%). The sample was balanced in terms of sex: 50.8% female and 49.2% male. A majority of participants self-identified as white (46.0%) and came from families where the maximum parental education was college or higher (55.4%). Spearman’s correlations between digital technology use variables were very weak to moderate, ranging from 0.08 (coefficient between hours spent video chatting and hours spent watching videos on the weekend) to 0.54 (coefficient between hours spent texting and hours spent social networking).
Table 1.
Unweighted N | Weighted % (SE)/ Mean (SE) | |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 32.759 | 15.05 (0.01) |
Grade | ||
8th | 14.836 | 42.62 (0.32) |
10th | 15.144 | 43.50 (0.33) |
12th | 4.823 | 13.88 (0.23) |
Female | 16.132 | 50.78 (0.34) |
Race/ethnicity | ||
White | 15.503 | 46.02 (0.34) |
Black | 3.940 | 12.18 (0.23) |
Hispanic | 9.240 | 29.80 (0.32) |
Multiple | 1.774 | 5.43 (0.15) |
Other | 2.222 | 6.57 (0.16) |
GPA lower than B | 9.501 | 29.92 (0.31) |
Maximum Parental Education | ||
<High school | 3.097 | 10.76 (0.22) |
Finished high school or some college | 9.819 | 33.89 (0.34) |
Finished college or graduate school | 17.140 | 55.35 (0.35) |
Past-year drinking | 10.278 | 31.51 (0.32) |
Past-year cannabis use | 7.133 | 21.31 (0.28) |
Past-year use of other drugs | 2. 631 | 8.19 (0.19) |
Figure 1 shows the prevalence of substance use by hours/day spent on social media, digital communication, and entertainment among 10th grade students (results for 8th and 12th grade students are Supplemental Figures 1 and 2, respectively). In general, there were positive and consistent relationships between use of each substance with use of technologies that require interaction: social media, texting, talking, and to a lesser extent, video chatting. For example, the prevalence of past 30-day drinking was 7.5% among students who report no hours of social media use, and 23.0% among students who report 3+ hours of social media use. Results were similar for texting; the prevalence of past 30-day drinking was 8.4% among students who report no hours texting/day, and 23.0% among students who report 3+ hours of texting/day. In contrast, a limited signal emerges for technologies that do not require interaction: gaming and watching videos on weekend days. For example, the prevalence of alcohol use among students who report no gaming hours was 20.8%, similar to the prevalence among students who report 3+ hours of gaming (18.7%). The exception to the pattern of substance use trends by whether the activity required interaction was for watching videos on weekdays, which was associated with most substances examined. Results were similar in direction for 8th and 12th grade students (see Supplemental Figures 1 and 2).
Table 2 shows Poisson regressions of the associations between social media and digital communication with past substance use, adjusting for grade, sex, race, GPA, highest level of parental education, past-year substance use, and assigned instrument form. Results confirmed that digital technology use was associated with past substance use, across many forms of technology including social media, texting, talking, and video chatting. Any amount of social networking per day significantly increased students’ risk of past 30-day drinking. Students who used social media 3+ hours per day had 1.99 (95% CI: 1.65, 2.41) times higher risk of past 30-day drinking. Any amount of texting, talking on the phone, or video chatting per day significantly also increased students’ risk of past 30-day drinking, adjusting for the aforementioned covariates. Any amount of talking on the phone or video chatting, compared with none, was associated with past 2-week binge drinking and past 30-day cannabis use, adjusting for the aforementioned covariates. Lastly, any amount of talking on the phone, compared with none, was associated with past 30-day flavor, cannabis, and nicotine vaping. In contrast, Table 2 shows associations for activities that do not require interaction with others and past substance use were of substantially lower magnitude. Poisson regressions run without adjusting for past-year substance use provided similar associations (see Supplemental Table 1), but results were less attenuated.
Table 2.
|
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Past 2-week binge drinkingb | Past 30-day drinkingb | Past 30-day cannabis usec | Past 30-day flavor vapingd | Past 30-day cannabis vapingc | Past 30-day nicotine vapingd | ||
N = 23071 | N = 23241 | N = 23258 | N = 6985 | N = 7020 | N = 6998 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent texting | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1.12(0.74, 1.69) | 1.45 (1.1, 1.9) | 1.32 (0.97, 1.8) | 1.13 (0.63, 2.05) | 1.41 (0.67, 3) | 1.36 (0.77, 2.43) | |
1–2 | 1.54 (1.02, 2.33) | 1.82 (1.38, 2.4) | 1.85 (1.36, 2.52) | 1.71 (0.95, 3.09) | 1.95 (0.93, 4.08) | 1.62 (0.91, 2.88) | |
3+ | 1.86 (1.24, 2.8) | 2.03 (1.54, 2.67) | 2.17 (1.6, 2.94) | 2.09 (1.17, 3.71) | 2.1 (1.03, 4.3) | 1.81 (1.02, 3.19) | |
| |||||||
N = 22943 | N = 23113 | N = 23130 | N = 6949 | N = 6983 | N = 6963 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent talking on cell phone | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1.45 (1.2, 1.77) | 1.32 (1.17, 1.47) | 1.15 (1.01, 1.31) | 1.45 (1.07, 1.97) | 1.96 (1.25, 3.09) | 1.27 (1, 1.63) | |
1–2 | 1.85 (1.49, 2.28) | 1.44 (1.27, 1.63) | 1.49 (1.29, 1.71) | 2.18 (1.59, 3) | 2.14 (1.31, 3.51) | 1.57 (1.2, 2.05) | |
3+ | 1.97 (1.58, 2.45) | 1.59 (1.4, 1.8) | 1.58 (1.37, 1.83) | 1.97 (1.42, 2.73) | 2.28 (1.38, 3.74) | 1.42 (1.08, 1.87) | |
| |||||||
N = 22911 | N = 23082 | N = 23100 | N = 6933 | N = 6968 | N = 6946 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent social networking | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1.33 (0.96, 1.83) | 1.42 (1.16, 1.74) | 1.12(0.87, 1.43) | 1.47 (0.89, 2.44) | 1.1 (0.53, 2.28) | 1.3 (0.81, 2.1) | |
1–2 | 1.45 (1.08, 1.96) | 1.67 (1.38, 2.02) | 1.55 (1.23, 1.96) | 1.66 (1.02, 2.69) | 1.47 (0.76, 2.85) | 1.75 (1.11, 2.75) | |
3+ | 1.85 (1.38, 2.49) | 1.99 (1.65, 2.41) | 1.94 (1.55, 2.44) | 2.48 (1.56, 3.96) | 1.85 (0.98, 3.49) | 2.06 (1.31, 3.21) | |
| |||||||
N = 22980 | N = 23150 | N = 23167 | N = 6967 | N = 7003 | N = 6980 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent video chatting | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1.44 (1.26, 1.64) | 1.35 (1.25, 1.47) | 1.15 (1.05, 1.27) | 1.26(1.05, 1.52) | 1.47 (1.11, 1.95) | 1.26 (1.07, 1.49) | |
1–2 | 1.38 (1.18, 1.62) | 1.34 (1.22, 1.47) | 1.37 (1.22, 1.54) | 1.25 (0.99, 1.59) | 1.47 (1.04, 2.07) | 1.37 (1.12, 1.68) | |
3+ | 1.28 (1.1, 1.5) | 1.3 (1.18, 1.44) | 1.42 (1.28, 1.58) | 1.59(1.28, 1.97) | 1.35 (0.94, 1.94) | 1.2(0.96, 1.49) | |
| |||||||
N = 23137 | N = 23308 | N = 23326 | N = 7005 | N = 7039 | N = 7017 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent gaming | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1 (0.81, 1.23) | 0.91 (0.8, 1.03) | 0.95 (0.81, 1.11) | 0.77 (0.55, 1.08) | 0.8 (0.5, 1.26) | 1 (0.75, 1.34) | |
1–2 | 0.92 (0.75, 1.13) | 0.9 (0.8, 1.02) | 0.92 (0.79, 1.07) | 0.89 (0.64, 1.24) | 0.67 (0.42, 1.07) | 0.93 (0.7, 1.25) | |
3+ | 0.96 (0.79, 1.16) | 0.94 (0.84. 1.05) | 1.02 (0.89, 1.17) | 1 (0.74, 1.36) | 0.7 (0.46, 1.06) | 0.95 (0.72, 1.25) | |
| |||||||
N = 24772 | N = 24966 | N = 24987 | N = 6989 | N = 7024 | N = 7002 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent watching videos: weekdays | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1.13 (0.84, 1.52) | 1.12(0.93, 1.34) | 0.92 (0.73, 1.14) | 1 (0.6, 1.68) | 1.88 (0.71, 4.98) | 1.47 (0.93, 2.32) | |
1–2 | 1.08 (0.82, 1.43) | 1.12(0.94, 1.33) | 0.95 (0.77, 1.17) | 0.96 (0.59, 1.56) | 2.24 (0.88, 5.67) | 1.48 (0.96, 2.28) | |
3+ | 1.18 (0.9, 1.54) | 1.17(0.99, 1.39) | 1.02 (0.83, 1.25) | 1.16(0.72, 1.87) | 1.94 (0.77, 4.87) | 1.31 (0.86, 2.02) | |
| |||||||
N = 24721 | N = 24915 | N = 24935 | N = 6984 | N = 7015 | N = 6995 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent watching videos: weekends | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 1.14(0.83, 1.57) | 1 (0.82, 1.23) | 0.94 (0.76, 1.18) | 1.28 (0.67, 2.44) | 1.11 (0.43, 2.84) | 1.45 (0.84, 2.5) | |
1–2 | 0.89 (0.65, 1.21) | 0.96 (0.79, 1.16) | 0.87 (0.71, 1.07) | 1.33 (0.74, 2.42) | 1 (0.42, 2.39) | 1.22 (0.72, 2.08) | |
3+ | 0.8 (0.6, 1.07) | 0.96 (0.8, 1.15) | 0.84 (0.69, 1.01) | 1.28 (0.72, 2.27) | 0.89 (0.39, 2.06) | 0.99 (0.59, 1.65) |
All data are presented as aRR (95% CI). All models adjusted for grade, race/ethnicity, most recent GPA, and highest level of parental education received), past-year substance use, and assigned instrument form.
Models with vaping outcomes only include 8th and 10th graders. Drugs included in “other drug” category vary by grade: for 12th graders we include LSD, other hallucinogens, cocaine, amphetamines, sedatives, tranquilizers, heroin, narcotics, crack, and other forms of cocaine. For 8th and 10th graders we do not include heroin, narcotics, or sedatives.
Adjusted for past-year cannabis use, and other past-year drug use.
Adjusted for past-year drinking, and other past-year drug use.
Adjusted for past-year drinking, past-year cannabis use, and other past-year drug use.
Sensitivity analyses in which hours/day of digital technology use were included as a continuous variable in the Poisson regressions yielded similar results, with some exceptions (Supplemental Table 2). Associations between talking on the phone and past 30-day cannabis and flavor vaping became non-significant. In addition, watching videos on weekends emerged as inversely associated with past 30-day nicotine vaping. For every one hour increase in watching videos on the weekend, students had 0.95 (95% CI: 0.93, 0.98) lower adjusted risk of past 30-day cannabis vaping than those who did not watch videos on weekends. The inclusion of other behavioral and parental covariates also yielded similar results, with some exceptions. After adjusting for sensation seeking, the association between 3+ hours of video chatting and past 2-week binge drinking was reduced in magnitude, and the association between <1 hour of video chatting and past 30-day cannabis use was reduced in magnitude Pre-proof (Supplemental Table 3). Similarly, associations between talking on the phone and video chatting and past substance use became more attenuated after controlling for parental monitoring (Supplemental Table 4). Lastly, after adjusting for internalizing symptoms, the association between <1 hour/day of talking on the phone and video chatting and past 30-day cannabis use was reduced in magnitude (Supplemental Table 5). However, any amount of video chatting was positively associated with past 30-day flavor vaping (Supplemental Table 5).
Table 3 shows Poisson regressions indicating that among past substance users, increases in hours spent social networking and communicating digitally were generally associated with increased occasions of substance use, except for past 30-day cannabis use and flavor-vaping across all forms of digital communication. For example, compared to students who don’t use social media, the expected log count for those who use social media for 3+ hours increased by 0.294 (95% CI: 0.087, 0.501) for past 2-week binge drinking and 0.145 (95% CI: 0.052, 0.239) for past 30-day drinking. Compared to students who don’t talk on the phone, the expected log count for those who talk for 3+ hours increased by 0.254 (95% CI: 0.171, 0.337) for past 30-day nicotine vaping. In contrast, the Poisson models indicated that among past substance users, increased hours spent gaming and watching videos were associated with decreased occasions of substance use across substances, except for past 30-day cannabis use by hours spent gaming (Table 3). Poisson regressions run without adjusting for past substance use provided similar associations (see Supplemental Table 6), but results were less attenuated.
Table 3.
|
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Past 2-week binge drinkingb | Past 30-day drinkingb | Past 30-day cannabis usec | Past 30-day flavor vapingd | Past 30-day cannabis vapingc | Past 30-day nicotine vapingd | ||
N = 1641 | N = 3683 | N = 2844 | N = 753 | N = 376 | N = 873 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent texting | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | −0.257 (−0.513, −0.001) | −0.435 (−0.551, −0.318) | −0.209 (−0.289, −0.129) | −0.202 (−0.354, −0.05) | 0.091 (−0.171, 0.354) | −0.324 (−0.46, −0.187) | |
1–2 | −0.132 (−0.385, 0.121) | −0.34 (−0.456, −0.225) | −0.305 (−0.385, −0.225) | −0.24 (−0.391, −0.088) | −0.075 (−0.341, 0.19) | −0.297 (−0.433, −0.16) | |
3+ | 0.036 (−0.21, 0.281) | −0.084 (−0.197, 0.028) | −0.268 (−0.345, −0.191) | −0.078 (−0.223, 0.067) | 0.004 (−0.25, 0.258) | −0.201 (−0.334, −0.069) | |
| |||||||
N = 1624 | N = 3657 | N = 2821 | N = 748 | N = 373 | N = 865 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent talking on cell phone | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 0.053 (−0.08, 0.187) | 0.15 (0.087, 0.213) | −0.129 (−0.168, −0.09) | −0.294 (−0.378, −0.211) | 0.008 (−0.141, 0.156) | 0.252 (0.178, 0.326) | |
1–2 | 0.188 (0.048, 0.327) | 0.23 (0.162, 0.299) | −0.077 (−0.119, −0.035) | −0.375 (−0.464, −0.285) | 0.079 (−0.072, 0.229) | 0.057 (−0.025, 0.139) | |
3+ | 0.288 (0.147, 0.428) | 0.446 (0.378, 0.514) | −0.05 (−0.093, −0.008) | 0.023 (−0.066, 0.111) | −0.105 (−0.259, 0.049) | 0.254 (0.171, 0.337) | |
| |||||||
N = 1628 | N = 3655 | N = 2818 | N = 746 | N = 375 | N = 864 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent social networking | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 0.125 (−0.104, 0.354) | −0.054 (−0.158, 0.05) | −0.044 (−0.113. 0.026) | −0.238 (−0.393, −0.082) | −0.011 (−0.228, 0.206) | −0.048 (−0.189, 0.093) | |
1–2 | 0.146 (−0.071, 0.363) | −0.093 (−0.191, 0.006) | −0.051 (−0.116, 0.014) | −0.111 (−0.254, 0.033) | 0.068 (−0.132, 0.268) | −0.15 (−0.284, −0.016) | |
3+ | 0.294 (0.087, 0.501) | 0.145 (0.052, 0.239) | −0.087 (−0.149, −0.025) | −0.005 (−0.141, 0.132) | −0.264 (−0.457, −0.071) | −0.026 (−0.155, 0.104) | |
| |||||||
N= 1638 | N = 3672 | N = 2823 | N = 751 | N = 376 | N = 871 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent video chatting | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | 0.056 (−0.031, 0.143) | 0.056 (0.012, 0.1) | 0.005 (−0.025, 0.035) | −0.066 (−0.126, −0.006) | 0.188 (0.1, 0.276) | −0.019 (−0.068, 0.03) | |
1–2 | 0.033 (−0.072, 0.137) | 0.013 (−0.04, 0.067) | −0.21 (−0.248, −0.172) | −0.132 (−0.207, −0.056) | −0.216 (−0.333, −0.099) | 0.143 (0.088, 0.198) | |
3+ | 0.189 (0.086, 0.291) | 0.304 (0.254, 0.354) | −0.004 (−0.038, 0.029) | 0.03 (−0.038, 0.097) | 0.22 (0.115, 0.325) | 0.037 (−0.027, 0.101) | |
| |||||||
N= 1650 | N = 3693 | N = 2857 | N = 757 | N = 377 | N = 875 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent gaming | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | −0.171 (−0.303, −0.038) | 0.012 (−0.055, 0.079) | −0.018 (−0.069, 0.033) | −0.18 (−0.293, −0.067) | −0.08 (−0.245, 0.085) | −0.375 (−0.452, −0.298) | |
1–2 | −0.226 (−0.357, −0.094) | −0 166 (−0.233, −0.099) | 0.133 (0.084, 0.182) | −0.145 (−0.251, −0.039) | 0.045 (−0.114, 0.204) | −0.369 (−0.446, −0.292) | |
3+ | −0.143 (−0.261, −0 026) | −0.023 (−0.083, 0.037) | 0.14 (0.096, 0.185) | 0.023 (−0.075, 0.12) | −0.171 (−0.321, −0.021) | −0.295 (−0.367, −0.224) | |
| |||||||
N = 1898 | N = 4232 | N = 3217 | N = 755 | N = 378 | N = 871 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent watching videos: weekdays | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | −0.136 (−0.331, 0.06) | −0.178 (−0.272, −0.084) | −0.141 (−0.202, −0.081) | −0.219 (−0.392, −0.045) | −0.231 (−0.505, 0.043) | −0.158 (−0.296, −0.019) | |
1–2 | 0.011 (−0.169, 0.19) | −0.132 (−0.218, −0.045) | −0.23 (−0.285, −0.174) | 0.019 (−0.141, 0.18) | −0.514 (−0.773, −0.256) | −0.099 (−0.23, 0.032) | |
3+ | 0.106 (−0.07, 0.281) | −0.014 (−0.099, 0.071) | −0.184 (−0.238, −0.13) | 0.144 (−0.013, 0.301) | −0.362 (−0.619, −0.106) | 0.138 (0.009, 0.267) | |
| |||||||
N = 1894 | N = 4219 | N = 3209 | N = 756 | N = 379 | N = 871 | ||
| |||||||
Hours spent watching videos: weekends | 0 | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF |
< 1 | −0.146 (−0.321, 0.028) | −0.214 (−0.302, −0.126) | −0.202 (−0.265, −0.139) | −0.522 (−0.736, −0.307) | −0.297 (−0.519, −0.074) | −0.584 (−0.716, −0.453) | |
1–2 | −0.357 (−0.523, −0.191) | −0.52 (−0.603, −0.438) | −0.25 (−0.307, −0.193) | −0.073 (−0.268, 0.123) | −0.518 (−0.723, −0.313) | −0.711 (−0.837, −0.586) | |
3+ | −0.248 (−0.399, −0.096) | −0.481 (−0.555, −0.406) | −0.278 (−0.33, −0.226) | −0.135 (−0.324, 0.054) | −0.527 (−0.719, −0.335) | −0.609 (−0.728, −0.49) |
All data are presented as expected log count (95% CI). All models adjusted for grade, race/ethnicity, most recent GPA, and highest level of parental education received), past-year substance use, and assigned instrument form.
Models with vaping outcomes only include 8th and 10th graders. Diugs included in “other drug” category vary by grade: for 12th graders we include LSD, other hallucinogens, cocaine, amphetamines, sedatives, tranquilizers, heroin, narcotics, crack, and other forms of cocaine. For 8th and 10th graders we do not include heroin, narcotics, or sedatives.
Adjusted for past-year cannabis use, and other past-year drug use.
Adjusted for past-year drinking, and other past-year drug use.
Adjusted for past-year drinking, past-year cannabis use, and other past-year drug use.
4. Discussion
This study is the first to estimate associations between digital technology and substance use using nationally representative data among the current cohort of US adolescents. We find that digital technology, including social media,isassociated with past substance use in adolescents. However, this association is present across many forms of digital communication, including texting, talking on the phone, and video chatting. In contrast, we generally did not observe consistent associations between gaming or watching videos and past substance use. In fact, we found some instances in which watching videos was associated with lower odds of past substance use. Sensitivity analyses in which hours/day of digital technology use were included as a continuous variable and in which behavioral and parental covariates were included generally yielded similar results. These differences support the hypothesis that social interactions drive substance use among adolescents, which is illustrated in how adolescents now more commonly communicate. Social networking, texting, and talking by default requires interaction with others, and digital communication is often used to directly interact with another individual. However, gaming and watching videos, although sometimes social activities, can easily be done solitarily, especially with the rise of single-player video games and streaming services such as Hulu and Netflix that make at-home movie watching easily accessible and inexpensive (Leonhardt, 2019). In summary, while social media may be exposing adolescents to advertising content and peer norms around addictive products, these results suggest that patterns of use are more consistent with social interactions in general, rather than social media in particular being a risk factor for substance use.
The role of peer interactions in substance use has been extensively documented for decades. Individuals who have wider peer groups are more likely to be exposed to peers that use substances (Bahr et al., 2005; Kandel, 1985), and to have opportunities to use (Crum et al., 1996; Lloyd and Anthony, 2003). As new ways for adolescents to engage with each other become available, increased frequency of digital communication may become a proxy for adolescents who are more socially engaged in general, thus, some part of the associations with peer-engagement and substance use may be reproduced via digital communication. We hypothesize that these adolescents are more social online and offline, increasing their opportunity risk and might enjoy spending unsupervised time with friends. Existing literature indicates that adolescents who use technologies that promote interaction, such as social media, are more likely to use substances (Brunborg et al., 2017; Brunborg and Andreas, 2019; Gommans et al., 2014; Gutierrez and Cooper, 2016; Ohannessian et al., 2017), underscoring that the results presented here are consistent with the broader literature.
While the associations observed here are consistent with a social interaction mechanism, it is worth underscoring that new modes of technology have the potential to expose adolescents to advertising aimed at increasing substance use (Anderson et al., 2009). Vaping is particularly concerning; recent data indicate a dramatic increase in adolescent use, especially of JUUL products (Fadus et al., 2019). JUUL accounted for about 40% of e-cigarette retail market shares for last quarter of 2017 (Huang et al., 2019) and often targets youth in their advertisements (Fadus et al., 2019). and adolescents who were exposed to advertising of e-cigarettes such as JUUL were more likely to experiment with e-cigarette use (Chen-Sankey et al., 2019). Additionally, recent evidence indicates that young adult cannabis users seek cannabis advertisements on social media, and it is now a larger source of cannabis advertising than traditional media (such as print, radio, and television) Pre-proof(Kraussetal., 2017).Indeed,the proportion of substance-related advertisements on social media platforms is alarming. An analysis of publicly available Instagram posts with cannabis-related hashtags found that 43% of posts had explicit content about cannabis use, of which 9% contained cannabis-related advertisement (Cavazos-Rehg et al., 2016).
Wider social networks, as indicated by more time on social media, texting, talking, and other forms of engagement also may increase substance use through peer modeling of substance-related behavior, both offline and online. For instance, exposure to pro-alcohol posts from friends on Twitter was associated with higher odds of current heavy episodic drinking compared to no exposure (Cabrera-Nguyen et al., 2016). Having more Facebook friends also predicted greater alcohol consumption (Frost and Rickwood, 2017). Interestingly, having more friends also predicted posting alcohol-related content on Facebook for the first time among those commencing college (Frost and Rickwood, 2017), thereby increasing one’s Facebook friends’ exposure to pro-alcohol content and perpetuating the cycle of observing posts and drinking. Similar to trends in alcohol use, exposure to social media content from peers is also linked to higher levels of cannabis use. In general, adolescents who report having friends who used cannabis had higher odds of ever using cannabis themselves (Roditis et al., 2016). The odds of current cannabis use were almost three times higher for those exposed to pro-cannabis posts from friends on Twitter compared to those with no exposure (Cabrera-Nguyen et al., 2016).
Although these aforementioned studies imply that youth exposure to advertisement and pro-substance content are associated with increased substance use, studies on restricting exposure and resulting substance use reduction have been Pre-mixed. A prospective observational study in Germany determined that parental restriction from watching movies rated for those 16 years and over decreased substance use risk among adolescents (Hanewinkel et al., 2008). However, a 2014 systematic literature review concluded that there is a lack of robust evidence to support or refute the impact of alcohol advertising restrictions on subsequent substance use (Siegfried et al., 2014). The lack of congruence among these studies indicates a need for more rigorous studies using nationally representative data containing indicators that specifically identify content exposure.
Our study has several limitations. Although our results support the hypothesis that adolescent social networks are drivers of substance use, rather than digital technology itself, it is possible that youth who engage in social interactions with others using digital media reflect different sub-popultions of youth, or different types of digital technology may expose youth to different substance-related content. Therefore, these associations are also not intended to be interpreted as causal estimates given that data are cross-sectional, and there may be uncontrolled confounding. The data are also self-reported and, thus, may be susceptible to recall and reporting bias. Furthermore, the available digital media measures in these data are limited. Since we have no direct measure of exposure to advertising, we use other exposures as a proxy and infer that advertising and pro-substance content from peers are the underlying mechanisms for increased substance use in youth. Although MTF is generalizable to school-attending adolescents, results in this study might not be relevant for non-school attending adolescents and non-US residents. Lastly, we do not have data for 12th graders regarding past vaping use.
Smart phones and digital communication are ubiquitous in adolescents’ lives. Assessing newer potential sources of exposure to pro-substance content is a vital part of monitoring the everchanging landscape of adolescent substance use. This study provides evidence that social media, texting, talking on the phone, and video chatting are associated with past substance use in adolescents, suggesting that social interactions drive adolescent substance use, with social networks and digital communication providing a medium for these interactions.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Social media and digital technology exposes youth to substance-related content
Adolescents who use social media are more likely to use substances
This relationship is also observed across other forms of digital communication
More socially connected adolescents are more likely to use substances
Acknowledgments
Role of Funding Source
Nothing declared
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
None
Supplementary material can be found by accessing the online version of this paper at https://urldefense.proofpoint.com/v2/url?u=http3A__dx.doi.org&d=DwICaQ&c=G2MiLlal7SXE3PeSnG8W6_JBU6FcdVjSsBSbw6gcR0U&r=GC3jPjfa01XKwW7DFQn1L0R2Y6ueekOxD9Q7trOx70&m=nWUm0LZxcyehguqua8QIW7tVatNt6P8yB1Qd0sQXYOY&s=KiCUfE8Y8FO0wJwJWIF1nIGNiVXO4Arpa5LLiPhBsCw&e=
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Anderson P, De Bruijn A, Angus K, Gordon R, Hastings G, 2009. Special issue: The message and the media: Impact of alcohol advertising and media exposure on adolescent alcohol use: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Alcohol Alcohol. 44, 229–243. 10.1093/alcalc/agn115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachman JG, Johnston LD, O’malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Miech RA, 2015. The Monitoring the Future Project After Four Decades: Design and Procedures.
- Bahr SJ, Hoffmann JP, Yang X, 2005. Parental and peer influences on the risk of adolescent drug use. J. Prim. Prev. 26, 529–551. 10.1007/s10935-005-0014-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunborg GS, Andreas JB, 2019. Increase in time spent on social media is associated with modest increase in depression, conduct problems, and episodic heavy drinking. J. Adolesc. 74, 201–209. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunborg GS, Andreas JB, Kvaavik E, 2017. Social Media Use and Episodic Heavy Drinking Among Adolescents. Psychol. Rep. 120, 475–490. 10.1177/0033294117697090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabrera-Nguyen E, Cavazos-Rehg P, Krauss M, Bierut L, Moreno M, 2016. Young adults’ exposure to alcohol- and marijuana-related content on Twitter. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 77, 349–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camenga D, Gutierrez KM, Kong G, Cavallo D, Simon P, Krishnan-Sarin S, 2018. E-cigarette advertising exposure in e-cigarette naïve adolescents and subsequent e-cigarette use: A longitudinal cohort study. Addict. Behav. 81, 78–83. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavazos-Rehg PA, Krauss MJ, Sowles SJ, Bierut LJ, 2016. Marijuana-related posts on Instagram. Prev. Sci. 17, 710–720. 10.1007/s11121-016-0669-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen-Sankey JC, Unger JB, Bansal-Travers M, Niederdeppe J, Bernat E, Choi K, 2019. E-cigarette Marketing Exposure and Subsequent Experimentation Among Youth and Young Adults. Pediatrics 144. 10.1542/peds.2019-1119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Chester J, Montgomery K, Dorfman L, 2010. Alcohol marketing in the digital age | Digital Ads. Berkeley Media Stud. Gr. [Google Scholar]
- Crum RM, Lillie-Blanton M, Anthony JC, 1996. Neighborhood environment and opportunity to use cocaine and other drugs in late childhood and early adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 43, 155–161. 10.1016/S0376-8716(96)01298-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fadus MC, Smith TT, Squeglia LM, 2019. The rise of e-cigarettes, pod mod devices, and JUUL among youth: Factors influencing use, health implications, and downstream effects. Drug Journal Alcohol Depend.201, 85–93. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.04.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frost RL, Rickwood DJ, 2017. A systematic review of the mental health outcomes associated with Facebook use. Comput. Human Behav. 76, 576–600. 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gommans R, Stevens GWJM, Finne E, Cillessen AHN, Boniel-Nissim M, ter Bogt TFM, 2014. Frequent electronic media communication with friends is associated with higher adolescent substance use. Int. J. Public Health 60, 167–177. 10.1007/s00038-014-0624-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross EF, 2009. Logging on, Bouncing Back: An Experimental Investigation of Online Communication Following Social Exclusion. Dev. Psychol. 45, 1787–1793. 10.1037/a0016541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutierrez KM, Cooper TV, 2016. The use of social networking sites: A risk factor for using alcohol, marijuana, and synthetic cannabinoids? Drug Alcohol Depend. 163, 247–250. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanewinkel R, Morgenstern M, Tanski SE, Sargent JD, 2008. Longitudinal study of parental movie restriction on teen smoking and drinking in Germany. Addiction 103, 1722–1730. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02308.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman EW, Pinkleton BE, Weintraub Austin E, Reyes-Velázquez W, 2014. Exploring college students use of general and alcohol-related social media and their associations with alcohol-related behaviors. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 62, 328–335. 10.1080/07448481.2014.902837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang GC, Unger JB, Soto D, Fujimoto K, Pentz MA, Jordan-Marsh M, Valente TW, 2014. Peer influences: The impact of online and offline friendship networks on adolescent smoking and alcohol use. J. Adolesc. Heal. 54, 508–514. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang J, Duan Z, Kwok J, Binns S, Vera LE, Kim Y, Szczypka G, Emery SL, 2019. Vaping versus JUULing: How the extraordinary growth and marketing of JUUL transformed the US retail e-cigarette market. Tob. Control 28, 146–151. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2018-054382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan D, Noel J, Landon J, Thornton N, Lobstein T, 2017. Alcohol marketing and youth alcohol consumption: a systematic review of longitudinal studies published since 2008. Addiction 112, 7–20. 10.1111/add.13591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan DH, 2011. Framing a public health debate over alcohol advertising: The Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth 2002–2008. J. Public Health Policy 32, 165–179. 10.1057/jphp.2011.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, 1985. On processes of peer influences in adolescent drug use: A developmental perspective. Adv. Alcohol Subst. Abuse. 10.1300/J251v04n03_07 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King C III, Siegel M, 1999. Brand-specific cigarette advertising in magazines in relation to youth and young adult readership, 1986–1994. Nicotine Tob. Res. 1, 331–340. 10.1080/14622299050011461 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krauss MJ, Sowles SJ, Sehi A, Spitznagel EL, Berg CJ, Bierut LJ, Cavazos-Rehg PA, 2017. Marijuana advertising exposure among current marijuana users in the U.S. Drug Alcohol Depend. 174, 192–200. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SJ, 2009. Online communication and adolescent social ties: who benefits more from internet use? J. Comput. Commun. 14, 509–531. 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2009.01451.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leonhardt M, 2019. Hulu just cut prices—here’s how it compares to other popular streaming services [WWW Document]. CNBC. URL https://www.cnbc.com/2019/02/26/cost-of-popular-streaming-sites-like-netflix-amazon-prime-and-hulu.html (accessed 11.21.19).
- Lloyd JJ, Anthony JC, 2003. Hanging Out with the Wrong Crowd: How Much Difference Can Parents Make in an Urban Environment? J. Urban Heal. 80, 383–399. 10.1093/jurban/jtg043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margolis KA, Donaldson EA, Portnoy DB, Robinson J, Neff LJ, Jamal A, 2018. E-cigarette openness, curiosity, harm perceptions and advertising exposure among U.S. middle and high school students. Prev. Med. (Baltim). 112, 119–125. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2018.04.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miech RA, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, Patrick ME, 2019. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2018: Volume I, Secondary school students 1. [Google Scholar]
- Miller J, Prichard I, Hutchinson A, Wilson C, 2014. The relationship between exposure to alcohol-related content on facebook and predictors of alcohol consumption among female emerging adults. Cyberpsychology, Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 735–741. 10.1089/cyber.2014.0337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohannessian CM, Vannucci A, Flannery KM, Khan S, 2017. Social Media Use and Substance Use During Emerging Adulthood. Emerg. Adulthood 5, 364–370. 10.1177/2167696816685232 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Park SY, Holody KJ, 2018. Content, Exposure, and Effects of Public Discourses about Marijuana: A Systematic Review. J. Health Commun. 23, 1036–1043. 10.1080/10810730.2018.1541369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pew Research Center, 2018. Teens, Social Media & Technology 2018.
- Pokhrel P, Fagan P, Herzog TA, Laestadius L, Buente W, Kawamoto CT, Lee HR, Unger JB, 2018. Social media e-cigarette exposure and e-cigarette expectancies and use among young adults. Addict. Behav. 78, 51–58. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.10.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pucci LG, Siegel M, 1999. Features of sales promotion in cigarette magazine advertisements, 1980–1993: An analysis of youth exposure in the United States. Tob. Control 8, 29–36. 10.1136/tc.8.1.29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roditis ML, Delucchi K, Chang A, Halpern-Felsher B, 2016. Perceptions of social norms and exposure to pro-marijuana messages are associated with adolescent marijuana use. Prev. Med. (Baltim). 93, 171–176. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.10.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SAS Institute Inc., 2014. SAS [computer program]. Version 9.4.
- Siegfried N, Pienaar DC, Ataguba JE, Volmink J, Kredo T, Jere M, Parry CDH, 2014. Restricting or banning of alcohol advertising to reduce alcohol consumption in adults and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev.Pre-proof 10.1002/14651858.CD010704.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Walraven C, Hart RG, 2008. Leave ’em alone - Why continuous variables should be analyzed as such. Neuroepidemiology 30, 138–139. 10.1159/000126908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H, Chang W, 2014. ggplot2 2.1.0. Documentation [WWW Document]. URL http://ggplot.yhathq.com/ [Google Scholar]
- Zou G, 2004. A Modified Poisson Regression Approach to Prospective Studies with Binary Data. Am. J. Epidemiol. 159, 702–706. 10.1093/aje/kwh090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.