Abstract
The orbitofrontal cortex in primates including humans is the key brain area in emotion, and in the representation of reward value and in non-reward, that is not obtaining an expected reward. Cortical processing before the orbitofrontal cortex is about the identity of stimuli, i.e. ‘what’ is present, and not about reward value. There is evidence that this holds for taste, visual, somatosensory and olfactory stimuli. The human medial orbitofrontal cortex represents many different types of reward, and the lateral orbitofrontal cortex represents non-reward and punishment. Not obtaining an expected reward can lead to sadness, and feeling depressed. The concept is advanced that an important brain region in depression is the orbitofrontal cortex, with depression related to over-responsiveness and over-connectedness of the non-reward-related lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and to under-responsiveness and under-connectivity of the reward-related medial orbitofrontal cortex. Evidence from large-scale voxel-level studies and supported by an activation study is described that provides support for this hypothesis. Increased functional connectivity of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex with brain areas that include the precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex and angular gyrus is found in patients with depression and is reduced towards the levels in controls when treated with medication. Decreased functional connectivity of the medial orbitofrontal cortex with medial temporal lobe areas involved in memory is found in patients with depression. Some treatments for depression may act by reducing activity or connectivity of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. New treatments that increase the activity or connectivity of the medial orbitofrontal cortex may be useful for depression. These concepts, and that of increased activity in non-reward attractor networks, have potential for advancing our understanding and treatment of depression. The focus is on the orbitofrontal cortex in primates including humans, because of differences of operation of the orbitofrontal cortex, and indeed of reward systems, in rodents. Finally, the hypothesis is developed that the orbitofrontal cortex has a special role in emotion and decision-making in part because as a cortical area it can implement attractor networks useful in maintaining reward and emotional states online, and in decision-making.
Keywords: depression, orbitofrontal cortex, reward, emotion, decision-making
The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in emotion, reward value and reward-related decision-making. Rolls et al. describe how it implements these functions, and the effects of damage. In depression, the lateral orbitofrontal cortex has increased functional connectivity and sensitivity to non-reward; and the medial orbitofrontal cortex has decreased connectivity and sensitivity to reward.
Graphical Abstract
Introduction
Advances in our understanding of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) are described here, and include the following. First, the OFC includes the medial OFC areas 13 and 11, and the lateral OFC area 12, as shown in Fig. 1. Conclusions about the functions of the OFC cannot be established by considering only one part of it, the medial OFC areas 13 and 11 (Rudebeck et al., 2013). Furthermore, some authors have termed a region that includes the lateral OFC area 12 the ‘ventrolateral prefrontal cortex’ (Rudebeck et al., 2017), and that terminology has clouded the issue in such studies of the functions of the lateral OFC, area 12. Dividing the OFC into a medial part (areas 13 and 11) and a lateral part (area 12) (based on its architectonics, Fig. 1) is useful, for connectivity-based parcellations show differences in the connectivity of these parts in humans (Hsu et al., 2020; Du et al., 2020a).
A second key point made is that this distinction between the human medial and lateral OFC is important functionally: the lateral OFC (area 12, Fig. 1) is implicated in the effects of aversive and subjectively unpleasant stimuli, and in not receiving expected rewards (termed ‘non-reward’) when a reward choice must be reversed and has increased functional connectivity in depression. In contrast, the medial OFC (areas 13 and 11, Fig. 1) is activated by rewarding and subjectively pleasant stimuli and has reduced functional connectivity in depression. This difference is supported by recent investigations in, for example, a monetary Win versus NoWin task (Xie et al., 2020).
A third key advance is in understanding the connectivity of the human OFC using both tractography (Hsu et al., 2020) and functional connectivity (Du et al., 2020a), showing, for example, that the medial OFC is connected especially with the reward-related pregenual anterior cingulate cortex, and the lateral OFC and its closely related orbitofrontal part of the inferior frontal gyrus is connected especially with the non-reward and punishment-related supracallosal part of the anterior cingulate cortex. This is relevant to increasing understanding that the orbitofrontal is a key brain region involved in reward value and its rapid updating in even one trial when the reward value changes, and sends this information to the anterior cingulate cortex for actions to be learned guided by the reward or non-reward outcomes received (Rolls, 2019b).
A fourth key advance included here is that the anterior cingulate cortex can be conceptualized as receiving information about reward outcomes from the OFC, and the posterior cingulate cortex as receiving information from the parietal cortex about actions just performed, providing the signals needed for action–outcome goal-related learning, and sending outputs to premotor areas from the mid-cingulate cortex (Rolls, 2019b).
A fifth set of key advances described here is in understanding the functions of the OFC in mental disorders including depression by using voxel-level analyses of functional connectivity in many recent large-scale studies. Indeed, it is a key aim of this article to highlight the potential importance of the OFC, as a key brain region involved in emotion, in understanding and treating depression.
A sixth key feature of this article is the increasing evidence that the human and non-human primate OFC, with its importance for reward representations and very rapid updating of these that is important to social behaviour is very different from the rodent OFC, for the rodent OFC, and the whole organization of reward processing in the rodent brain, is very different (Rolls, 2019c, 2021a). Partly for this reason, a feature of this article is that it focusses on the evidence from both humans and foundational studies in non-human primates, because of similarities in their OFC systems, and their differences from the processing in rodents.
This article refers to some of the key concepts developed in The Orbitofrontal Cortex (Rolls, 2019c), and has a different focus to an earlier article published in 2017 (Rolls, 2019d).
We start by describing new evidence on the connections of the OFC and its functions in reward and emotion and then consider new evidence on how differences in OFC function are related to depression.
The orbitofrontal cortex is a key brain region in reward value, mood and emotion
The connections of the orbitofrontal cortex
The OFC cytoarchitectonic areas of the human brain are shown in Fig. 1 (left). The medial OFC includes areas 13 and 11 (Öngür et al., 2003). The lateral OFC includes area 12 (sometimes in humans termed 12/47). The anterior cingulate cortex includes the parts shown in Fig. 1 (right) of areas 32, 25 (subgenual cingulate) and 24. The ventromedial prefrontal cortex includes areas 14 (gyrus rectus), 10m and 10r.
Some of the main connections of the OFC in primates are shown schematically in Fig. 2 (Carmichael and Price, 1994, 1995; Barbas, 1995; Petrides and Pandya, 1995; Pandya and Yeterian, 1996; Ongür and Price, 2000; Price, 2006, 2007; Barbas, 2007; Saleem et al., 2008; Mackey and Petrides, 2010; Petrides et al., 2012; Saleem et al., 2014; Henssen et al., 2016; Rolls, 2017, 2019c,d). The OFC receives inputs from the ends of the ventral cortical streams that process the identity of visual, taste, olfactory, somatosensory and auditory stimuli (Rolls, 2019c). At the ends of each of these cortical processing streams, the identity of the stimulus is represented independently of its reward value. This is shown by neuronal recordings in primates (Rolls, 2019c). For example, the inferior temporal cortex represents objects and faces independently of their reward value as shown by visual discrimination reversal and devaluation of reward tests by feeding to satiety (Rolls et al., 1977; Rolls, 2012a, 2016b, 2019c). Similarly, the insular primary taste cortex represents what the taste is independently of its reward value (Yaxley et al., 1988; Rolls, 2015, 2016c, 2019c).
Outputs of the OFC reach the anterior cingulate cortex (Rolls, 2019c), the striatum, the insula and the inferior frontal gyrus, and enable the reward value representations in the OFC to influence behaviour (Fig. 2, green). The OFC projects reward value information to the anterior cingulate cortex, where it is used to provide the reward outcomes for action–outcome learning (Rushworth et al., 2012; Rolls, 2019a,c). The OFC projects reward-related information to the ventral striatum (Williams et al., 1993), and this provides a route, in part via the habenula, for reward-related information to reach the dopamine neurons (Rolls, 2017), which respond inter alia to positive reward prediction error (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; Schultz, 2016a, 2017). The striatal/basal ganglia route is used for stimulus–response, habit, learning (Everitt and Robbins, 2013; Rolls, 2014), with dopamine used to provide reward prediction error in reinforcement learning (Schultz, 2016b; Cox and Witten, 2019). As that system uses dopamine in reinforcement learning of stimulus–response habits, it is much less fast to learn than the OFC (reward or punishment outcome) with anterior cingulate cortex (action) system for action–outcome goal-based learning, and for emotion (Rolls, 2021a). The OFC may also have direct connections to the ventral tegmental area in mice where dopamine neurons are located (Namboodiri et al., 2019). The OFC outputs to the insula include a projection to the viscero-autonomic cortex in the antero-ventral insula (Hassanpour et al., 2018) that helps to account for the reason why the insula is activated in some tasks in which the OFC is involved (Rolls, 2016c, 2019c). The lateral OFC also projects to the inferior frontal gyrus (Hsu et al., 2020; Du et al., 2020a), to a region that on the right is implicated in stopping behaviour (Aron et al., 2014), and to a region on the left that includes Broca’s area (Hsu et al., 2020; Rolls, 2021a).
New evidence on the connections of the OFC in humans is shown in Fig. 3, based on resting-state functional connectivity in 654 participants (Du et al., 2020a). First, it is shown that a parcellation based on the voxel-wise functional connectivity of OFC voxels with other brain areas reveals sub-divisions (Fig. 3) that are very similar to the cytoarchitectural divisions of the human OFC shown in Fig. 1. Second, the lateral OFC (parcels 5 and 6, Fig. 3) has connectivity with language-related areas not only in the inferior frontal gyrus (Broca’s area) but also with the angular and supramarginal gyri. Parts of the medial OFC (parcels 2–4, Fig. 3) have connectivity with the parahippocampal gyrus, hippocampus, the temporal cortex and fusiform gyrus, the insula and the cingulate cortex. These connectivities, as shown below, are altered in opposite directions in depression.
The connectivity of the OFC analysed in humans with functional connectivity is likely to include trans-synaptic effects, but direct connections have been investigated with diffusion tractography imaging in 50 participants (Hsu et al., 2020). The medial OFC and ventromedial prefrontal cortex have direct connections with the pregenual and subgenual parts of the anterior cingulate cortex, all of which are reward-related areas. The lateral OFC and its closely connected right inferior frontal gyrus have direct connections with the supracallosal anterior cingulate cortex, all of which are punishment or non-reward-related areas (Hsu et al., 2020). This confirms the findings based on functional connectivity for connections between the medial OFC and the pregenual cingulate cortex; and the lateral OFC and related right inferior frontal gyrus with the supracallosal anterior cingulate cortex (Rolls et al., 2019; Du et al., 2020a). The lateral OFC and right inferior frontal gyrus also have direct connections with the right supramarginal gyrus and inferior parietal cortex, and with some premotor cortical areas, which may provide outputs for the lateral OFC and right inferior frontal gyrus. Direct connections of the human OFC and inferior frontal gyrus with the temporal lobe were especially with the temporal pole (Hsu et al., 2020).
The medial orbitofrontal cortex represents reward value; and the lateral orbitofrontal cortex represents punishers and non-reward
The primate including human OFC is the first stage of cortical processing that represents reward value (red, Fig. 2) (Rolls, 2019c). For example, in devaluation experiments, taste, olfactory, visual and oral texture neurons in the macaque orbitofrontal respond to food when hunger is present, and not after feeding to satiety when the food is no longer rewarding (Rolls et al., 1989; Critchley and Rolls, 1996). In visual discrimination reversal experiments, neurons in the macaque OFC reverse the visual stimulus in as little as one trial when the reward versus punishment taste received as an outcome for the choice reverses (Thorpe et al., 1983; Rolls et al., 1996). This is rule-based reversal, in that after a previously rewarded visual stimulus is no longer rewarded, the macaques choose the other stimulus on the very next trial, although its previous reward association was with punishment, as shown in Fig. 4 which illustrates a non-reward neuron active at the time of the reversal (Thorpe et al., 1983). (Non-reward refers here to not obtaining an expected reward.) This capability requires a rule to be held in memory and reversed by non-reward (Deco and Rolls, 2004; Rolls and Deco, 2016), is very appropriate for primates which in social situations may benefit from being very responsive to non-reward versus reward signals, and may not occur in rodents (Rolls, 2019c, 2021a; Hervig et al., 2020). The human lateral OFC is activated in this one-trial rule-based non-associative reversal (Rolls et al., 2020b). The macaque OFC contains neurons that reflect face expression and face identity (both necessary to decode value and important in social behaviour) (Thorpe et al., 1983; Rolls et al., 2006), and also social categories such as young faces (Barat et al., 2018) and position in the social hierarchy (Munuera et al., 2018). Economic value is represented in the OFC, in that, for example, single neurons reflect the trade-off between the quality of a reward and the amount that is available (Padoa-Schioppa and Conen, 2017). These investigations show that some OFC neurons respond to outcome value (e.g. the taste of food), and others to expected value (or future rewards), and interestingly humans with ADHD have increased sensitivity to these future rewards (Tegelbeckers et al., 2018). The expected value neurons are not positive reward prediction error neurons, for they keep responding to the expected reward even when there is no prediction error (Rolls, 2021a). Consistent with this neurophysiological evidence, lesions of the macaque medial OFC areas 13 and 11 make the animals less sensitive to reward value, as tested in devaluation experiments in which the animal is fed to satiety (Rudebeck et al., 2017). (These were described as OFC lesions, but in fact, included primarily the medial OFC areas 13 and 11 shown in Fig. 1.)
Neuroimaging experiments in humans (‘Technical Note’) produce consistent evidence about reward value representations (de Araujo et al., 2003; Kringelbach and Rolls, 2003; Kringelbach et al., 2003; Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2008; Grabenhorst et al., 2008a), and allow the types of reward to be extended to include monetary reward (O’Doherty et al., 2001; Xie et al., 2020), face expressions (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2003) and face beauty (O’Doherty et al., 2003). In humans, the medial OFC is activated by many rewarding stimuli and reflects their subjective pleasantness (Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2011; Rolls, 2019c). This is found for odours (Rolls et al., 2003a), flavour (de Araujo et al., 2003; Kringelbach et al., 2003), pleasant touch (Rolls et al., 2003b; McCabe et al., 2008), monetary reward (O’Doherty et al., 2001; Xie et al., 2020) and amphetamine (Völlm et al., 2004). A recent study with 1877 participants emphasizes these points, by showing that the medial OFC is activated by reward (such as winning money or candies) and that the lateral OFC is activated by not winning (Fig. 5) (Xie et al., 2020). Humans with OFC lesions may also be less sensitive to reward, as shown by their reduced subjective emotional feelings and altered social and emotional behaviour and problems with face and voice expression processing (Rolls et al., 1994; Hornak et al., 1996, 2003; Rolls, 2021b).
The macaque OFC has neurons that respond when an expected reward is not received (Thorpe et al., 1983), and these have been termed non-reward neurons (Rolls, 2014, 2019c) (see example in Fig. 4C). They can be described as negative reward prediction error neurons, in that they respond when a reward outcome is less than was expected (Rolls, 2019c). These neurons do not respond to expected punishers [e.g. the discriminative stimulus for saline in Fig. 4C; (Thorpe et al., 1983)], but other neurons do respond to expected punishers (Rolls et al., 1996), showing that non-reward and punishment are represented by different neurons in the OFC. The finding of non-reward neurons is robust, in that 18/494 (3.6%) of the neurons in the original study responded to non-reward (Thorpe et al., 1983), consistent results were found in different tasks in a complementary study (10/140 non-reward neurons in the OFC or 7.1%) (Rosenkilde et al., 1981), and an fMRI study has shown that the macaque lateral OFC is activated when an expected reward is not obtained during reversal (Chau et al., 2015) (Fig. 4D). The hypothesis is that these non-reward neurons are computed in the OFC, because this is the first brain region in primates at which expected value and outcome value are represented, and these two signals are those required to compute non-reward, that is, reward outcome < expected value (as shown in Fig. 2) and with the evidence set out fully by Rolls (2019c, 2021a). Corresponding to this, the human lateral OFC is activated when a reward is not obtained in a visual discrimination reversal task (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2003) (Fig. 4A), and when money is not received in a monetary reward task (O’Doherty et al., 2001; Xie et al., 2020), and in a one-trial rule-based reward reversal task (Rolls et al., 2020b). Consistent with this, the human lateral OFC is also activated by punishing, subjectively unpleasant, stimuli (Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2011; Rolls, 2019c). Examples include unpleasant odours (Rolls et al., 2003a), pain (Rolls et al., 2003b), losing money (O’Doherty et al., 2001) and receiving an angry face expression, indicating that behaviour should change in a reversal (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2003). The human right lateral OFC/inferior frontal gyrus is also activated when behavioural correction is required in the stop-signal task (Fig. 4B) (Deng et al., 2017). These discoveries show that one way in which the OFC is involved in decision-making is by representing rewards, punishers and errors made during decision-making. This is supported by the problems that OFC damage produces in decision-making, which include failing to respond correctly to non-reward, as described next.
Consistent with this neurophysiological and neuroimaging evidence, lesions of the OFC can impair behavioural changes to non-reward. For example, reward reversal learning is impaired during decision-making in humans, who continue responding to the previously rewarded, now non-rewarded, stimulus (Rolls et al., 1994; Hornak et al., 2004; Fellows, 2011). The change in contingency between the stimulus and the reward versus non-reward is not processed correctly. In macaques, damage to the lateral OFC impairs reversal and extinction (Butter, 1969; Iversen and Mishkin, 1970). It has been a problem in some studies of the orbitofrontal cortex in macaques that the OFC lesions have been incomplete, with for, example, the lesions including only the medial areas 13 and 11, and not the lateral OFC area 12, with one study reporting no reversal learning deficit after such a medial lesion, and suggesting that the OFC was not involved in reversal (Rudebeck et al., 2013). In the light of the above, that does not address the role of the OFC in reversal learning, as including the lateral OFC area 12 is highly relevant. In a more recent study, damage to the lateral OFC (mainly area 12 as shown in Fig. 1, and extending around the inferior convexity, but described as VLPFC—ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) was found to impair the ability to make choices based on whether reward versus non-reward had been received (Rudebeck et al., 2017; Murray and Rudebeck, 2018), which is the type of contingency learning in which this brain region is implicated (Rolls, 2019c; Rolls et al., 2020b). (Unfortunately, the one-trial, rule based, reversal learning in which the OFC is implicated (Rolls, 2019c), was not tested in that study (Rudebeck et al., 2017).) Further evidence that the lateral OFC is involved in learning contingencies between stimuli and reward versus non-reward is that in humans, lateral OFC damage impaired this type of ‘credit assignment’ (Noonan et al., 2017). This type of flexibility of behaviour is important in primate including human social interactions (Rolls, 2018a, 2019c).
The ventromedial prefrontal cortex and reward-related decision-making
The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC, which can be taken to include the gyrus rectus area 14 and parts of 10m and 10r, Fig. 1) receives inputs from the OFC and has distinct connectivity [with strong functional connectivity with the superior medial prefrontal cortex, cingulate cortex and angular gyrus (Du et al., 2020a)]. The VMPFC has long been implicated in reward-related decision-making (Bechara et al., 1997, 2005; Glascher et al., 2012), this region is activated during decision-making contrasted with reward valuation (Rolls and Grabenhorst, 2008; Grabenhorst et al., 2008b), and it has the signature of a decision-making region of increasing its activation in proportion to the difference in the decision variables, which correlates with decision confidence (Rolls et al., 2010a,b; Rolls, 2019c). Consistently, single neurons in the macaque ventromedial prefrontal cortex signal the value of the chosen offer, suggesting that the network produces a choice (Strait et al., 2014), also consistent with the attractor model of decision-making (Rolls et al., 2010a,b; Rolls, 2014, 2016b, 2021a). The attractor model of decision-making is a neuronal network with associatively modifiable recurrent collateral synapses between the neurons of the type prototypical of the cerebral cortex (Wang, 2002; Rolls and Deco, 2010; Rolls, 2021a). The decision variables are applied simultaneously, and the network, after previous training with these decision variables, reaches a state where the population of neurons representing one of the decision variables has a high firing rate (Rolls and Deco, 2010; Deco et al., 2013; Rolls, 2016b; 2021a).
The orbitofrontal cortex and emotion
One of the major theories of emotion is that emotions are states elicited by rewards and punishers, which are instrumental reinforcers (Rolls, 2000, 2013b, 2014, 2018a) (Fig. 6). The evidence described above shows that the OFC is involved in representing the reward value of stimuli (with an emphasis on the medial and mid-OFC areas 11 and 13); and is involved in learning associations between stimuli and rewards, and rapidly correcting these (with an emphasis on the lateral OFC area 12 and the closely connected orbital part of the inferior frontal gyrus) (Rolls, 2018a, 2019c). In this context, the theory of emotion holds that the role of the OFC in emotion is to decode the reward/punishment goals for action, by representing reward value, and by learning about stimuli with reward versus non-reward contingencies, and then to transmit the resulting representations to further brain regions (such as the cingulate cortex) which implement the learning of actions to obtain the reward outcomes signalled by the OFC (Rolls, 2019a,b; Rolls, 2021a). In accordance with this, the rewarding and punishing stimuli described above are all affective stimuli, and activate the OFC; and OFC damage impairs subjective emotional states (Hornak et al., 2003, Rolls, 2019c), emotional responses to stimuli such as face and voice expression (Hornak et al., 1996, 2003), and emotional and social behaviour, with the neurological evidence described in more detail elsewhere (Rolls, 2019c, 2021b). Furthermore, activations in the OFC are linearly related to the subjective pleasantness (i.e. affective experience) of stimuli, as described above, and elsewhere in more detail (Rolls, 2019c). The brain bases of subjective experience are a topic of considerable current interest, not only with higher order thought theories (Rosenthal, 2004; Brown et al., 2019) but also with the higher order syntactic thought theory of consciousness (Rolls, 2007, 2012b, 2014, 2016b, 2018a, 2020) which is more computationally specific and addresses the adaptive value of the type of processing related to consciousness. The point made here is that the OFC is at least on the route to human subjective experience of emotion and affective value (Rolls, 2019c).
Although the amygdala has many of the same connections as the OFC (Fig. 2), it is an evolutionarily old brain region, and appears to be overshadowed by the OFC in humans, in that the effects of damage to the human amygdala on emotion and emotional experience are much more subtle (Whalen and Phelps, 2009; Delgado et al., 2011; LeDoux and Pine, 2016; LeDoux et al., 2018) than that of damage to the OFC (Rolls et al., 1994; Hornak et al., 1996, 2003, 2004; Camille et al., 2011; Fellows, 2011; Rolls, 2019c). Indeed, LeDoux and colleagues have emphasized the evidence that the human amygdala is rather little involved in subjective emotional experience (LeDoux and Pine, 2016; LeDoux and Brown, 2017; LeDoux et al., 2018). That is in strong contrast to the OFC, which is involved in subjective emotional experience, as shown by the evidence just cited. The OFC provides the answer to LeDoux’s conundrum: if not the amygdala for subjective emotional experience, then what? Furthermore, consistent with the poor rapid reversal learning found by amygdala neurons (Sanghera et al., 1979; Rolls, 2014) compared to OFC neurons, it has been found that neuronal responses to reinforcement predictive cues in classical conditioning update more rapidly in the macaque OFC than amygdala, and activity in the OFC but not the amygdala was modulated by recent reward history (Saez et al., 2017). Neurons that are sensitive to the rank of the individual being viewed in the social hierarchy are found not only in the macaque amygdala, but also in the closely connected OFC, and anterior cingulate cortex (Munuera et al., 2018). In addition, it has been shown in the macaque that amygdala neurons are involved in social, observational learning in a reversal-learning task, and that some neurons even predicted the choices of the partner monkey (Grabenhorst et al., 2019). These processes—assessing the social rank of individuals, learning from social partners, anticipating their behaviour—are critical for social life. However, the balance may shift towards the OFC in humans, in that it is OFC damage in humans that produces profound changes in social and emotional behaviour, and subjective emotional experience, as well as in reward reversal learning (Rolls et al., 1994; Hornak et al., 1996; 2003; Berlin et al., 2004, 2005; Hornak et al., 2004; Rolls, 2019c).
The rodent orbitofrontal cortex and reward systems
The focus in this article is on evidence from primates including humans. The reason for this focus is that the rodent OFC (Schoenbaum et al., 2009; Wilson et al., 2014; Sharpe et al., 2015; Izquierdo, 2017; Sharpe et al., 2019), and the whole operation of reward systems in rodents, appears to be somewhat different from that in primates including humans (Rolls, 2019c, 2021a), as follows.
First, the rodent OFC contains only agranular cortex, which corresponds to only a small region of the primate OFC, posteriorly (Wise, 2008; Passingham and Wise, 2012; Passingham, 2021), and these authors provide evidence that there is no equivalent in rodents of most of the primate OFC.
Second, the connectivity of the rodent reward systems including the OFC is so different from that of primates that the principles of operation appear to be very different. One example is the taste system, which in primates proceeds mainly via thalamo-cortical processing through a primary taste cortex in the insula to the OFC, whereas instead, rodents have a pontine taste area which projects taste information to many subcortical areas (Scott and Small, 2009; Small and Scott, 2009; Rolls, 2015, 2016c, 2019c). A second example is that with the great development of the temporal lobe in primates, visual processing becomes highly elaborated and transmits information about face identity and face expression to the OFC, where it can be used in emotional and social behaviour appropriate for different individuals, given the face expression and gestures (including face view) of each individual (Rolls, 2016c; 2019c). A third example is that because the visual representation in primates includes processing to the level of view-invariant representations of objects and faces, reward value-related learning in the OFC is efficient, for after a value association is made to one view, it generalizes to other views or transforms (Rolls, 2012a, 2016c, 2021a).
Third, in rodents, reward value as indicated in devaluation (satiety) studies involves reward processing even far peripherally in the first central relay, the nucleus of the solitary tract for taste (Giza and Scott, 1983, 1987; Giza et al., 1992), and the olfactory bulb for odour (Pager et al., 1972), making these complex systems, as reward and identity processing about taste and odour are entangled throughout the system with each other. In another example, in reward reversal in mice, the OFC has reward-related top-down effects on the primary somatosensory cortex (Banerjee et al., 2020). This makes reward processing in rodents difficult to analyse. In contrast, in primates and humans, there is a clear separation between perceptual representations (Tier 1, Fig. 2), and reward value representations in the OFC and amygdala (Tier 2, Fig. 2) (Rolls, 2015, 2016b, 2019c, 2021a).
Fourth, although reward value is represented in the rodent (agranular) OFC, so also apparently are behavioural responses (Schoenbaum et al., 2009; Wilson et al., 2014; Sharpe et al., 2015, 2019; Izquierdo, 2017), which makes the rodent OFC very different to the primate OFC. The primate OFC, in contrast, appears to specialize in reward (and of course punishment and non-reward) value representations, but not in interfacing these value representations to actions [which occurs in the primate cingulate cortex (Rolls, 2019b,2021a)] or to responses (which occurs in the striatum and other parts of the basal ganglia), for actions and responses are poorly if at all represented in the primate OFC (Thorpe et al., 1983; Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006; Grattan and Glimcher, 2014; Rolls, 2019c). In contrast, in rodents, the OFC seems much more heterogeneous with behavioural responses also represented in it (Wilson et al., 2014; Sharpe et al., 2015, 2019), and again, the system is more complex because different computations are apparently intermingled in the same brain region. Although it is interesting that in the mouse, neurons in LO represent the values of individual options, the binary choice outcome and the chosen value, this is in the context of spatial responses (Kuwabara et al., 2020), not of the value of goods as in primates.
Fifth, although reward reversal learning is studied in rodents (Hervig et al., 2020), it does not so far appear to be of the same powerful type as the rule-based system present in primates, which allows switching to a different stimulus even previously associated with punishment when no reward is received when it was expected by a behavioural choice on a single trial (Thorpe et al., 1983; Rolls et al., 1996, 2020b; Rolls, 2019c). This type of rapid, rule-based, reversal provides a foundation for rapid changes in social behaviour whenever feedback is received, and a similar rule-based system is not known to be present in rodents. This is consistent with the great development of cortical processing for these functions provided by the primate OFC, given that the cortex provides a computational basis in its attractor networks for holding information online, and therefore producing behaviour that depends on ‘hidden’ internal states, rather than being more dominated by sensory input (Rolls, 2016b, 2021a). However, it is of interest that neurons in the rodent lateral OFC respond in reward reversal and that silencing these neurons impairs the reversal (Banerjee et al., 2020).
Because we wish the advances described here to be relevant to understanding the functions of the OFC in humans, we focus here on the findings in primates including humans, but further evidence on research in rodents is provided elsewhere (Izquierdo, 2017; Rolls, 2019c, 2021a).
A theory of depression
Better understanding of the functions of the OFC in major depressive disorder is important, for it is ranked by the World Health Organization as the leading cause of years-of-life lived with disability (Drevets, 2007; Gotlib and Hammen, 2009; Hamilton et al., 2013). Moreover, the economic cost of depression is enormous, with an estimated 350 million people affected globally. For example, the cost to Europe of work-related depression was estimated to be Euro 617 billion annually in 2013 and rising (Matrix, 2013).
A theory of depression has been developed based on our understanding of the brain processes involved in emotion, reward and non-reward described above (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a). Given that not receiving expected rewards is a reinforcement contingency that can lead to sadness, or in the extreme case such as the loss of a loved one, depression, the theory was proposed that the lateral OFC, implicated in non-reward and learning contingencies between stimuli and reward versus non-reward, over-responds to non-reward in people with depression; and that a major non-reward event that activated the lateral OFC might lead to depression (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a). Because non-reward neurons in the lateral OFC can maintain their activity for at least many seconds (Fig. 4) (Thorpe et al., 1983), and because this persistent activity is needed to ensure that after non-reward, the behaviour changes even if the same stimuli are not received for some time, the theory is that there is a non-reward attractor network in the lateral OFC, and that this is more sensitive or persistent in depression (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a). It is postulated that the effects of the non-reward can be prolonged by rumination of sad thoughts which is supported by a long loop attractor involving language areas in the angular gyrus and related regions, which receive inputs from the lateral OFC, and project back to it. The theory thus is that some aspects of depression may be related to over-responsiveness of the lateral orbitofrontal to non-reward and punishment (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a, 2021a). Consistent with this, increased sensitivity to non-reward (not winning in a monetary incentive delay task) of the lateral OFC is associated with the severity of depressive symptoms measured in hundreds of adolescents (Xie et al., 2020,).
Given that the activations of the lateral and medial OFC often appear to be reciprocally related (O’Doherty et al., 2001; Rolls et al., 2003a; Xie et al., 2020), the other part of the theory is that in depression there may be underactivity, under-sensitivity or under-connectivity of the (reward-related) medial OFC in depression (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a). The theory is further that under-responsiveness of the medial OFC could contribute to other aspects of depression, such as anhedonia.
There is now much evidence that supports this theory of depression, as described in the following sections. This approach based on advances in understanding the functions of the primate including human OFC also provides an approach to understanding how other brain systems such as the subgenual/subcallosal anterior cingulate cortex are implicated in depression (Mayberg et al., 2016), in that they receive inputs from the OFC. This approach also adds to previous approaches relating to reward and punishment systems (Eshel and Roiser, 2010; McCabe et al., 2012; Satterthwaite et al., 2015), by being based on the theory of emotion described above and on the understanding of brain systems involved in emotion in primates including humans described above. Other contributing factors to depression are considered elsewhere (Rolls, 2018a).
Increased functional connectivity of the non-reward related lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and decreased functional connectivity of the reward-related medial orbitofrontal cortex, in depression
This section describes many new large-scale voxel-level analyses of differences in the connectivity of the OFC in depression, and this is the first review in which this new evidence is brought together. An overview of many of the new discoveries is shown in Fig. 8.
There is considerable interest in functional connectivity differences between people with major depressive disorder and controls, for increased functional connectivity could reflect increased communication between brain regions, and decreased connectivity the opposite. Functional connectivity studies have typically been performed with resting-state fMRI, and the functional connectivity is measured by the correlation of the BOLD signals between pairs of brain regions. A number of studies have provided evidence for different functional connectivities that may include the OFC, anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala and hippocampus, but many of these studies have involved relatively small numbers of participants, and whole-brain regions (Helm et al., 2018). Indeed, power analyses show that resting-state functional connectivity for individual links in people is not robust with small samples (Button et al., 2013; Poldrack, 2019), especially in populations with psychiatric disorders, as we also have found. The studies described next have therefore focused on large numbers of participants to provide robust results, and on voxel-level analysis to enable separation of connectivity of nearby brain areas such as the medial and lateral OFC. In addition, the studies considered next also have the advantage that in several it was possible to perform neuroimaging in a large sample of unmedicated patients and to compare the functional connectivity with that in medicated patients. Descriptions of some of the studies in which this has been possible are provided next.
A brain-wide analysis of voxel-level differences in functional connectivity implicates the lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex in depression
In the first brain-wide voxel-level resting-state functional connectivity neuroimaging analysis of depression (with 421 patients with major depressive disorder and 488 controls), we found that one major circuit with altered functional connectivity involved the medial OFC BA 13, which had reduced functional connectivity in depression with memory systems in the parahippocampal gyrus and medial temporal lobe (Cheng et al., 2016) (Fig. 7). The lateral OFC BA 47/12, involved in non-reward and punishing events, did not have this reduced functional connectivity with memory systems, so that there is an imbalance in depression towards decreased reward-related connectivity with the medial temporal lobe memory system.
A second major circuit change was that the lateral OFC area BA 47/12 had increased functional connectivity with the precuneus, the angular gyrus and the temporal visual cortex BA 21 (Cheng et al., 2016) (Fig. 7). This enhanced functional connectivity of the non-reward/punishment system (BA 47/12) with the precuneus [involved in the sense of self and agency (Rolls, 2021a)], and the angular gyrus [involved in language (Rolls, 2021a)] is thus related to the explicit affectively negative sense of the self, and of self-esteem, in depression.
The differences in orbitofrontal connectivity with these brain regions were related to the depression by evidence that the symptoms of depression were correlated with these differences of functional connectivity; and that the lateral OFC functional connectivity links described were less high if the patients were receiving antidepressant medication (Cheng et al., 2016).
Because the lateral OFC responds to many punishing and non-rewarding stimuli that are likely to elicit autonomic/visceral responses via the anteroventral insula (see above), and in view of connections from these areas to the anterior insula which is implicated in autonomic/visceral function (Critchley and Harrison, 2013; Rolls, 2016c), the anterior insula would also be expected to be overactive in depression, which it is (Drevets, 2007; Hamilton et al., 2013; Ma, 2015).
These advances were made possible because we performed whole-brain voxel-level functional connectivity, enabling clear separation and localization of differences between the lateral and medial OFC. Further analyses which focused instead on voxel-level functional connectivity of particular brain systems has revealed much more about the different systems involved, as described next, and have provided cross-validation in a cohort from the USA (Cheng et al., 2018d).
Precuneus: higher connectivity with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex
The precuneus is a medial parietal cortex region implicated in the sense of self and agency, autobiographical memory, spatial function. and navigation (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Freton et al., 2014). The retrosplenial cortex provides connections and receives connections from the hippocampal system, connecting especially with the parahippocampal gyrus areas TF and TH, and with the subiculum (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003, 2007; Bubb et al., 2017). The precuneus provides access to the hippocampus for spatial information from the parietal cortex (given the rich connections between the precuneus and the parietal cortex) (Rolls and Wirth, 2018; Rolls, 2021a).
To further analyse the functioning of the precuneus in depression, resting-state functional connectivity was measured in 282 patients with major depressive disorder and 254 controls (Cheng et al., 2018c). In 125 patients not receiving medication, voxels in the precuneus had significantly higher functional connectivity with the lateral OFC (Fig. 8). In patients receiving medication, the functional connectivity between the lateral OFC and the precuneus was decreased back towards that in the controls (Cheng et al., 2018c). These findings support the theory that the non-reward system in the lateral OFC has increased effects on areas in which the self is represented including the precuneus, which could relate to the low self-esteem in depressed patients (Rolls, 2016a).
Parahippocampal gyrus/medial temporal lobe memory system, and temporal lobe visual cortex: lower connectivity with the medial orbitofrontal cortex
We found that voxels in the medial OFC had lower functional connectivity with the parahippocampal gyrus/medial temporal lobe memory system (Cheng et al., 2016) (Fig. 7), and interpreted this as resulting in fewer happy memories being recalled, as the medial OFC has activations that correlate with subjective pleasantness, as described above, and the parahippocampal gyrus is a pathway in the hippocampal episodic memory system (Kesner and Rolls, 2015; Rolls, 2016b, 2018b, 2019b, 2021a; Rolls and Wirth, 2018). The reduced connectivity with temporal cortex areas in which objects and faces are represented was interpreted as contributing to the reduced positive valuation of signals involved in emotion such as the sight of face expressions, and of people (Hasselmo et al., 1989; Critchley et al., 2000,).
In a further analysis that investigated the effects of antidepressant medication (Rolls et al., 2020a), medial OFC voxels had lower functional connectivity with temporal cortex areas, the parahippocampal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, and supplementary motor area, and medication did not result in these functional connectivities being closer to controls. This is consistent with the anhedonia of depression and reduced happy memories being related to these low functional connectivities of the medial OFC with temporal lobe and memory systems. What is especially interesting is that these low functional connectivities are not normalized by treatment with antidepressant drugs (Rolls et al., 2020a), suggesting that one goal of future treatment for depression might be to increase the functionality of the medial OFC.
Posterior cingulate cortex: higher functional connectivity with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex
The posterior cingulate cortex is a region with strong connectivity in primates with the entorhinal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus (areas TF and TH), and thus with the hippocampal memory system (Vogt, 2009; Bubb et al., 2017; Rolls and Wirth, 2018; Rolls, 2018b; 2019b,c, 2021a). The posterior cingulate cortex also has connections with the OFC (Vogt and Pandya, 1987; Vogt and Laureys, 2009), and the posterior cingulate cortex has high functional connectivity with the parahippocampal regions that are involved in memory (Cheng et al., 2018b; Rolls, 2019b). The posterior cingulate region (including the retrosplenial cortex) is consistently engaged by a range of tasks that examine episodic memory including autobiographical memory, and imagining the future; and also spatial navigation and scene processing (Auger and Maguire, 2013; Leech and Sharp, 2014). Self-reflection and self-imagery activate the ventral part of the posterior cingulate cortex (the part with which we will be mainly concerned here) (Kircher et al., 2000, 2002; Johnson et al., 2002; Sugiura et al., 2005).
A study with 15 patients and 15 controls had implicated functional connectivity of the posterior cingulate with the subgenual cingulate cortex to rumination in depression (Berman et al., 2011), but we find that functional connectivity in people with psychiatric disorders is not robust with small samples. Therefore, to analyse the functioning of the posterior cingulate cortex in depression, we performed a full voxel-level resting-state functional connectivity neuroimaging analysis of depression of the posterior cingulate cortex, with 336 patients with major depressive disorder and 350 controls (Cheng et al., 2018b). In depression, the posterior cingulate cortex had significantly higher functional connectivity with the lateral OFC (Fig. 8). In patients receiving medication, the functional connectivity between the lateral OFC and the posterior cingulate cortex was decreased back towards that in the controls. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the non-reward system in the lateral OFC has increased effects on memory systems, which contribute to the rumination about sad memories and events in depression (Cheng et al., 2018b).
Anterior cingulate cortex: reduced connectivity with the orbitofrontal cortex in depression
The OFC projects to the anterior cingulate cortex (Vogt, 2009, 2019; Rolls, 2019b). The supracallosal anterior cingulate cortex is activated by many aversive stimuli (Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2011, Rolls, 2014, 2019b), and has strong connectivity with the lateral OFC and adjoining part of the inferior frontal gyrus (Rolls et al., 2019; Hsu et al., 2020; Du et al., 2020a). The pregenual cingulate cortex is activated by many pleasant, rewarding, stimuli (Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2011; Rolls, 2014, 2019b), and has strong functional connectivity with the medial OFC (Rolls et al., 2019; Hsu et al., 2020; Du et al., 2020a). However, the anterior cingulate cortex appears to be involved in learning actions to obtain rewards (action–outcome learning), where the outcome refers to the reward or punisher for which an action is being learned (Rudebeck et al., 2008; Camille et al., 2011; Rushworth et al., 2011, 2012; Rolls, 2019b,c). In contrast, the medial OFC is involved in reward-related processing and learning, and the lateral OFC in non-reward and punishment-related processing and learning (Rolls, 2019c). These involve stimulus–stimulus associations, where the second stimulus is a reward (or its omission), or a punisher (Rolls, 2019c). Given that emotions can be considered as states elicited by rewarding and punishing stimuli, and that moods such as depression can arise from prolonged non-reward or punishment (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a, 2019c), the part of the brain that processes these stimulus–reward associations, the OFC, is more likely to be involved in depression than the action-related parts of the cingulate cortex. However, the action-related parts of the cingulate cortex, and other regions related to action such as the right inferior frontal gyrus, could contribute to the motor-related slowing, fatigue and decreased energy that are all symptoms commonly seen in depressed individuals (Rolls et al., 2020a).
The subgenual (or subcallosal) cingulate cortex has been implicated in depression, and electrical stimulation in that region may relieve depression (Mayberg, 2003; Hamani et al., 2009, 2011; Lozano et al., 2012; Laxton et al., 2013; Lujan et al., 2013) [although it has not been possible to confirm this in a double-blind study (Holtzheimer et al., 2017)]. However, the subgenual cingulate cortex is also implicated in autonomic function (Gabbott et al., 2003), and this could be related to some of the effects found in this area that are related to depression. Whether the subgenual cingulate cortex is activated because of inputs from the OFC, or performs separate computations, is not yet clear. Furthermore, the possibility is considered that electrical stimulation of the subcallosal region, which includes parts of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Laxton et al., 2013), that may relieve depression, may do so at least in part by activating connections involving the OFC, other parts of the anterior cingulate cortex, and the striatum (Johansen-Berg et al., 2008; Hamani et al., 2009; Lujan et al., 2013).
In a study of depression it was found in unmedicated patients that the lateral orbitofrontal cortex where it becomes adjacent with the anteroventral insular cortex had increased functional connectivity with the subgenual / subcallosal anterior cingulate cortex (Fig. 8) (see Fig. S2B of (Rolls et al., 2019)). This may reflect increased effects of unpleasant states represented in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex on autonomic output in which the anteroventral insula and subgenual cingulate cortex are implicated (Rolls, 2021). Increased functional connectivity was also found between the medial orbitofrontal cortex and a region including parts of the supracallosal anterior cingulate cortex (see Fig. S2A in Rolls et al., (2019) (Fig. 8)). This may reflect reward inputs reaching a supracallosal anterior cingulate cortex region typically involved in representing aversive stimuli.
Inferior frontal gyrus: increased connectivity with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex in depression
The lateral OFC projects to the inferior frontal gyrus, and, very interestingly, higher functional connectivity was found in depression of voxels in the right inferior frontal gyrus with voxels in the lateral and medial OFC, cingulate cortex, inferior and middle temporal gyrus and temporal pole, the angular gyrus, precuneus, hippocampus and mid- and superior frontal gyrus (Rolls et al., 2020a) (Fig. 8). In medicated patients, these functional connectivities of the inferior frontal gyrus were lower and towards those in controls.
The hypothesis was proposed that one way in which the OFC influences behaviour in depression is via the right inferior frontal gyrus, which projects in turn to premotor cortical areas (Du et al., 2020a). Consistent with the consequent hypothesis that the inferior frontal gyrus route may allow non-reward signals to have a too great effect to inhibit behaviour in depression, lesions of the right inferior frontal gyrus impair stopping in the stop-signal task, and produce impulsiveness (Aron et al., 2004, 2014). Also consistent with the hypothesis, successful stopping in the stop-signal task is associated with high activation of the inferior frontal gyrus and lateral OFC (Deng et al., 2017).
Amygdala: reduced connectivity with the orbitofrontal cortex in depression
The amygdala is involved in emotion, though as shown above it may be overshadowed in humans by the OFC (LeDoux and Pine, 2016; Rolls, 2019c). Some relatively small-scale studies had shown different functional connectivities of the amygdala in depression (Connolly et al., 2017; Helm et al., 2018). In a large-scale study of depression, amygdala voxels had decreased functional connectivity with the medial OFC (involved in reward); the lateral OFC (involved in non-reward and punishment); temporal lobe areas [involved in visual and auditory perception including face expression analysis (Perrett et al., 1982; Leonard et al., 1985; Rolls, 2011, 2012a)]; and the parahippocampal gyrus (involved in memory; Fig. 8) (Cheng et al., 2018a). This disconnectivity of the amygdala may contribute to the depression.
Sleep, depression and increased lateral orbitofrontal cortex connectivity
Sleep is frequently impaired in depression (Becker et al., 2017). To advance understanding of the brain regions involved in sleep and depression, the relation between functional connectivity, depressive symptoms (the Adult Self-Report Depressive Problems scores) and poor sleep quality was measured in 1017 participants from the general population in the Human Connectome Project (Cheng et al., 2018d). The brain areas with increased functional connectivity of these common links related to both sleep and depressive scores included the lateral OFC; the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex; the insula; the parahippocampal gyrus and hippocampus; the amygdala; the temporal cortex; and the precuneus. A mediation analysis showed that these functional connectivities in the brain contribute to the relation between depression and poor sleep quality.
Evidence was also found in this general population that the Depressive Problems scores were correlated with functional connectivities between areas that included the lateral OFC, cingulate cortex, precuneus, angular gyrus and temporal cortex (Cheng et al., 2018d). Part of the importance of this is that it provides strong support for a role of the lateral OFC in depression in a general population in the USA in which a tendency to have depressive problems could be assessed. This cross-validation in a completely different population and in people not selected to have depression (Cheng et al., 2018d) provides support for the theory that the lateral OFC is a key brain area that might be targeted in the search for treatments for depression (Rolls, 2016a). Low sleep duration and depression are also related to structural differences of the OFC (Cheng et al., 2020). In particular, higher depressive problems’ scores were associated with reduced cortical areas or volumes of brain regions that included the lateral and medial OFC, temporal cortex, precuneus, superior and middle frontal gyrus and superior medial frontal cortex, angular and supramarginal gyrus, and hippocampus (Cheng et al., 2020).
Effective connectivity in depression
Effective connectivity measures the effect of one brain region on another in a particular direction, and can, in principle, therefore provide information related to the causal processes that operate in brain function, that is, how one brain region influences another.
In a resting-state fMRI investigation, effective connectivity directed to the medial OFC from areas including the parahippocampal gyrus, temporal pole, inferior temporal gyrus and amygdala was decreased in depression (Rolls et al., 2018). This implies less strong positive driving influences of these input regions on the medial and middle OFC, regions implicated in reward, and thus helps to elucidate part of the decreased feelings of happy states in depression (Rolls, 2016a). The links from temporal cortical areas to the precuneus were increased in depression, and this may relate to representations of the sense of self (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), which become more negative in depression (Cheng et al., 2016; Rolls, 2016a). The lateral OFC, implicated in non-reward and punishment, had an increased level of activity as reflected in the model in the depressed group. In addition, activity in the model was also higher in the right and left hippocampus of patients with depression, implying heightened memory-related processing (Rolls et al., 2018).
Orbitofrontal cortex activations to reward and non-reward in depression
Reinforcement learning approaches
Depression has been investigated in the framework of reinforcement learning, using in particular the learning rate coefficient and the sensitivity to reward. In earlier studies, it was reported that depression reduces prediction errors during reinforcement learning (Kumar et al., 2008; Gradin et al., 2011), but Rutledge et al. (2017) found that prediction error was unchanged in the ventral striatum. On that basis, they suggested that depression does not affect the expression of dopaminergic reward prediction errors. Their study was consistent with evidence that reward sensitivity and not learning rates are reduced in anhedonic depression (Huys et al., 2013; Chen et al., 2015). Also within the reinforcement learning framework, it was found that positive reward prediction error in the medial OFC is reduced in depression, and was correlated with anhedonia, but the learning was intact, in 28 drug-naive patients with depression (Rothkirch et al., 2017).
Increased activations to non-reward of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and decreased sensitivity to reward of the medial orbitofrontal cortex are related to depression scores
In 1140 adolescents at age 19 and 1877 at age 14 in the monetary incentive delay task, the medial OFC had graded increases in activation as the reward (Win) value increased (Xie et al., 2020). The lateral OFC had graded increases of activation as the reward value dropped to zero (the No-Win condition) (Fig. 5).
In a subgroup with a high score on the Adolescent Depression Rating Scale at the age of 19 and 14, the medial OFC activations had reduced sensitivity to the different reward conditions; and the lateral OFC activation showed high activation to the No-Win (i.e. Non-reward) condition (Xie et al., 2020). These new findings provide support for the hypothesis that those with symptoms of depression have increased sensitivity to non-reward in the lateral OFC, and decreased sensitivity for differences in reward of the medial OFC. Moreover, these differences are evident at an age as early as 14 years old (Xie et al., 2020). This result thus supports the theory that depressive symptoms can be related to sensitivity to non-reward (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a), that is, to not winning in this monetary reward task.
Possible structural differences in the orbitofrontal cortex in depression
For completeness, we note that there is some evidence for altered structure and function of the lateral OFC in depression (Drevets, 2007; Price and Drevets, 2012; Ma, 2015). For example, reductions of grey-matter volume have been demonstrated specifically in the posterolateral OFC (BA 47, caudal BA 11 and the adjoining BA 45), and also in the subgenual cingulate cortex (BA 24, 25) (Nugent et al., 2006; Drevets, 2007; Grieve et al., 2013). Meta-analyses revealed that depressed patients showed large volume reductions in frontal regions, especially in the anterior cingulate and OFC (Koolschijn et al., 2009; Lorenzetti et al., 2009).
In recent large-scale studies with the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) data set, it has been found that a number of factors are associated with psychiatric problems including the depressive problems score, and with reduced brain volume. The brain regions with reduced volume include the OFC, hippocampus, temporal cortex and medial frontal cortex. The factors that are associated with these differences are low maternal age (Du et al., 2020b), problems in the family (Gong et al., 2020), severe nausea and vomiting in pregnancy (Wang et al., 2020) and low sleep duration (Cheng et al., 2020).
In depression, there is increased cerebral blood flow in areas that include the ventrolateral OFC (which is a prediction of the theory), and also in regions such as the subgenual cingulate cortex and amygdala, and these increases appear to be related to the mood change, in that they become more normal when the mood state remits (Drevets, 2007), but convergence across studies is not strong (Gray et al., 2020).
The orbitofrontal cortex and possible treatments for depression, including new areas for brain stimulation
In research stimulated by the theory (Rolls, 2016a), it has been reported that transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right lateral OFC, which may disrupt its activity, helps in the treatment of depression in a substantial proportion of patients (Feffer et al., 2018; Downar, 2019).
Treatment with antidepressant drugs decreases the activity (Ma, 2015) and functional connectivity (Cheng et al., 2016, 2018b,c; Rolls et al., 2019, 2020a) of the non-reward lateral OFC system. The research described suggests that a search for new treatments that would increase the connectivity of the reward-related medial OFC could be helpful because current medications do not ameliorate these reduced functional connectivities.
Deep brain stimulation of the OFC may also be useful in the treatment of mood disorders and depression. The macaque OFC is a key brain site at which deep brain electrical stimulation is rewarding (Rolls et al., 1980; Rolls, 2005, 2019c). Electrical stimulation of the human OFC can also produce reward and raise mood (Rao et al., 2018), and many of the sites were in the middle part of the OFC, areas 13 and 11, which are categorized as medial OFC, the area activated by rewards (Rolls, 2019c). It is likely that these medial OFC sites will produce better reward in humans than stimulation in the lateral OFC BA12/47, for these lateral sites are activated by unpleasant stimuli and by not obtaining expected rewards. The medial (/middle) OFC may, for the reasons described here and elsewhere (Rolls, 2019c), be a key area of interest for deep brain stimulation to help relieve depression.
The anterior cingulate cortex, including the subcallosal cingulate cortex, is a key brain region to which the OFC projects (Rolls, 2019b). It is possible that brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate cortex might be useful in the treatment of at least some patients with depression (Johansen-Berg et al., 2008; Lujan et al., 2013; Dunlop et al., 2017; Holtzheimer et al., 2017; Riva-Posse et al., 2018), and it is possible that the subcallosal cingulate stimulation affects pathways that connect with the OFC (Johansen-Berg et al., 2008; Lujan et al., 2013; Dunlop et al., 2017; Riva-Posse et al., 2018). Given that the anterior cingulate cortex is an output region of the OFC (Fig. 1), it may be that treatments of the OFC, where the emotion is implemented, may be a better target for potential treatments for depression.
The general approach to depression described here, that it relates to effects produced by increased non-reward or non-reward sensitivity, or decreased reward or reward sensitivity (Fig. 6), has implications for self-help and behavioural treatments for depression (Rolls, 2018a), as well as for medical interventions.
What are the special computational roles of the orbitofrontal cortex in reward, emotion and decision-making?
It is important in terms of our understanding of brain function to consider what is special about the primate including human OFC for reward, decision-making and emotion, compared to other brain regions. This invites an answer about what is special about the computations performed by the OFC compared to other brain regions (Rolls, 2016b, 2019c, 2021a).
First, the OFC, as a neocortical area, has highly developed recurrent collateral connections between its pyramidal cells, which together with associative synaptic plasticity, provide the basis for auto-association or attractor networks (Rolls, 2016b, 2021a). [The amygdala, in contrast, has little recurrent collateral connectivity (Millhouse and DeOlmos, 1983).]
These attractor networks provide the basis for short-term memory functions, by maintaining neuronal firing in a stable attractor (Rolls, 2021a). These attractor networks can hold on-line which stimuli (and this could be other individuals) are currently rewarding, which is important for social interactions and economic decisions. This memory capability is an important component of rule-based one-trial reversal, in which the current rule must be held in short-term memory (Deco and Rolls, 2005). Short-term memory is also potentially very useful for holding mood online for some time, so that if, for example, a reward is not received, the non-reward state of frustration can lead to continuing attempts to regain the reward. Similar short-term memory processes might enable one to remember the recent reinforcement history of individuals, and again can be important in decision-making. The short-term memory aspects of these attractor networks are also important for holding the expected value online, until the reward outcome is received, after which non-reward neurons may be activated in ways for which there is a computational model (Rolls and Deco, 2016), and such computations may also contribute to reward prediction error, defined as the reward outcome value minus the expected reward. The short-term memory also provides the biasing system for top-down attention to reward value (Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2008, 2010; Rolls et al., 2008; Ge et al., 2012; Luo et al., 2013), and attention by biased activation (Rolls, 2013a, 2021a).
These attractor networks in the OFC and ventromedial prefrontal cortex also provide the basis for reward-related decision-making, in which inputs to two competing attractor states in an attractor network lead to a bifurcation and to a decision (Rolls and Deco, 2010; Rolls et al., 2010a,b, Deco et al., 2013; Rolls, 2021a). Part of the utility of this approach to decision-making is that once the decision has been taken in the attractor network, the results of the decision are kept active in the decision-making attractor network to provide the goals for the selection of actions by the cingulate cortex to obtain the rewards (Rolls, 2019b,c, 2021a).
Second, the primate and human OFC as a neocortical area is beautifully connected anatomically to receive inputs from representations of ‘what’ stimulus is present from every sensory modality at the top of each sensory cortical hierarchy, independently of reward value (Fig. 2), and then to compute multimodal representations that are then represented in terms of their reward value. This is very different from the rodent, in which reward is represented throughout the processing systems (Rolls, 2019c, 2021a).
Third, the primate OFC specializes in reward value, rather than action. This separation allows the value of many stimuli in the high dimensional space of different rewards (Rolls, 2014) to be represented, and for competition between them to be useful for computing relative reward value (Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2009). Moreover, mood states can be maintained, independently of any actions being performed. In contrast, as described above, the rodent ‘OFC’ is also involved in motor responses and actions (Wilson et al., 2014), so it can be less specialized for representing reward value, and rapidly changing it.
Fourth, the primate OFC projects reward value representations to the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex, and punishment value representations to the supracallosal anterior cingulate cortex (Hsu et al., 2020; Du et al., 2020a), where they can be associated with actions performed recently, received via the posterior cingulate cortex from the parietal cortex, to implement action–outcome learning. Outputs are then directed from mid-cingulate areas to the premotor cortical areas (Rolls, 2019b). The amygdala does not have similar connectivity.
Fifth, the human lateral OFC has considerable connectivity with the inferior frontal gyrus areas 45 and 44 which in the left are Broca’s area, and this may be part of the route by which the OFC, especially laterally, becomes functionally connected with language areas in the posterior temporal and parietal areas (Hsu et al., 2020; Du et al., 2020a; Rolls, 2021a). This provides a route for top-down influences of language-related processing on emotional and social behaviour, and indeed it is part of the long-loop interactions between attractor networks that are proposed to contribute to increased rumination in depression (Cheng et al., 2016; Rolls, 2016a, 2021a).
Conclusions
The research described here provides evidence that the primate including human OFC is a key brain region in reward value representation and in emotion (Rolls, 2019c). The primate OFC represents values, but not actions. Instead, the OFC sends reward outcome information to the cingulate cortex for use in action–outcome goal-directed learning, both of which are therefore key to understanding emotions and goal-directed actions (Rolls, 2019b). The case is also made that the OFC is a source of reward-related information that reaches the dopamine neurons in the brainstem (Rolls, 2017; Namboodiri et al., 2019) (Fig. 2), which provide for reinforcement-based learning of stimulus–response habits in the basal ganglia.
This framework provides a foundation for the proposal that because of its importance in emotion, the OFC is likely to be a key brain region in emotional disorders such as depression. The new evidence from large-scale voxel-level neuroimaging studies described here shows that the reward-related medial OFC has reduced functional connectivity with a number of brain systems including the medial temporal lobe memory system. Interestingly, antidepressant medications do not normalize these functional connectivities, suggesting a new avenue for exploration for new treatments for depression. The new evidence also shows that the punishment/non-reward-related lateral OFC and its related nearby right inferior frontal gyrus areas have increased functional connectivity in depression, which may relate to increased non-reward processing in depression. These advances, and the way in which depression can be related to altered responsiveness to non-rewards, provide an approach to the better understanding and treatment of depression (Rolls, 2016a, 2018a, 2019c, 2021a). Moreover, these new approaches relate to ideas that the subgenual cingulate cortex is involved in depression, for it receives major inputs from the OFC.
The research described here provides a theory supported by much empirical evidence for why and how the OFC is involved in depression, and provides a complementary approach to the strong focus there has been on the subgenual or subcommissural cingulate cortex in depression (Mayberg, 2003; Hamani et al., 2011; Lozano et al., 2012; Mayberg et al., 2016; Dunlop et al., 2017; Riva-Posse et al., 2018). We hope that the new concepts and evidence presented here based on a fundamental understanding of the functions of the OFC in emotion will help in developments of better understanding and treatments for depression (Rolls, 2018a).
Technical note
The orbitofrontal cortex is a difficult brain region for fMRI as sometimes signal dropout and distortion can occur due to inhomogeneities in the magnetic field being produced by the air-filled bony sinuses. For many of our investigations, we utilized a protocol set-up very helpfully by Prof Peter Jezzard (FMRIB, Oxford) for imaging in the coronal plane with the head angle optimized for each participant, and with imaging parameters that were carefully selected, as described in our articles before 2012. Since then, we have successfully used imaging in approximately the plane of the Sylvian fissure, which enables imaging of both the OFC and the medial temporal lobe, as set out in the previously published articles (Deichmann et al., 2002, 2003; Rolls et al., 2015b, 2020b).
Funding
This work was supported as follows: J. F. was supported by National Key R&D Program of China (No. 2019YFA0709502 and 2018YFC1312904); the 111 Project (No. B18015); Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Major Project (No. 2018SHZDZX01); ZJLab; Shanghai Center for Brain Science and Brain-Inspired Technology; and National Key R&D Program of China (No 2018YFC1312900). W.C. was supported by grants from the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China (No. 82071997 and 81701773) and Natural Science Foundation of Shanghai (No. 18ZR1404400).
Conflict of interest
The authors report no competing interests.
Data availability
Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.
Glossary
- ADHD =
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
- BA =
Brodmann area
- BOLD =
blood oxygenation-level dependent
- fMRI =
functional magnetic resonance imaging
- OFC =
orbitofrontal cortex
- VLPFC =
ventrolateral prefrontal cortex
- VMPFC =
ventromedial prefrontal cortex
References
- Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA.. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2004; 8: 170–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA.. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci 2014; 18: 177–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auger SD, Maguire EA.. Assessing the mechanism of response in the retrosplenial cortex of good and poor navigators. Cortex 2013; 49: 2904–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee A, Parente G, Teutsch J, Lewis C, Voigt FF, Helmchen F.. Value-guided remapping of sensory cortex by lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Nature 2020; 585: 245–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barat E, Wirth S, Duhamel JR.. Face cells in orbitofrontal cortex represent social categories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2018; 115: E11158–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H. Anatomic basis of cognitive-emotional interactions in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995; 19: 499–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H. Specialized elements of orbitofrontal cortex in primates. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1121: 10–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR.. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997; 275: 1293–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR.. The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9: 159–62; discussion 62–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker NB, Jesus SN, Joao K, Viseu JN, Martins RIS.. Depression and sleep quality in older adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Health Med 2017; 22: 889–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin H, Rolls ET, Iversen SD.. Borderline Personality Disorder, impulsivity and the orbitofrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 2360–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin H, Rolls ET, Kischka U.. Impulsivity, time perception, emotion, and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain 2004; 127: 1108–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berman MG, Peltier S, Nee DE, Kross E, Deldin PJ, Jonides J.. Depression, rumination and the default network. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2011; 6: 548–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bromberg-Martin ES, Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O.. Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron 2010; 68: 815–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R, Lau H, LeDoux JE.. Understanding the higher-order approach to consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23: 754–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bubb EJ, Kinnavane L, Aggleton JP.. Hippocampal—diencephalic—cingulate networks for memory and emotion: an anatomical guide. Brain Neurosci Adv 2017; 1: 1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butter CM. Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective prefrontal ablations in Macaca mulatta. Physiol Behav 1969; 4: 163–71. [Google Scholar]
- Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013; 14: 365–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camille N, Tsuchida A, Fellows LK.. Double dissociation of stimulus-value and action-value learning in humans with orbitofrontal or anterior cingulate cortex damage. J Neurosci 2011; 31: 15048–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael ST, Price JL.. Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 1994; 346: 366–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael ST, Price JL.. Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1995; 363: 642–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanna AE, Trimble MR.. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 2006; 129: 564–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chau BK, Sallet J, Papageorgiou GK, Noonan MP, Bell AH, Walton ME, et al. Contrasting roles for orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala in credit assignment and learning in macaques. Neuron 2015; 87: 1106–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C, Takahashi T, Nakagawa S, Inoue T, Kusumi I.. Reinforcement learning in depression: a review of computational research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 55: 247–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng W, Rolls ET, Gong W, Du J, Zhang J, Zhang XY, et al. Sleep duration, brain structure, and psychiatric and cognitive problems in children. Mol Psychiatry 2020. doi:10.1038/s41380-020-0663-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng W, Rolls ET, Qiu J, Liu W, Tang Y, Huang CC, et al. Medial reward and lateral non-reward orbitofrontal cortex circuits change in opposite directions in depression. Brain 2016; 139: 3296–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng W, Rolls ET, Qiu J, Xie X, Lyu W, Li Y, et al. Functional connectivity of the human amygdala in health and in depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2018. a; 13: 557–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng W, Rolls ET, Qiu J, Xie X, Wei D, Huang CC, et al. Increased functional connectivity of the posterior cingulate cortex with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex in depression. Transl Psychiatry 2018. b; 8: 90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng W, Rolls ET, Qiu J, Yang D, Ruan H, Wei D, et al. Functional connectivity of the precuneus in unmedicated patients with depression. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2018. c; 3: 1040–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng W, Rolls ET, Ruan H, Feng J.. Functional connectivities in the brain that mediate the association between depressive problems and sleep quality. JAMA Psychiatry 2018. d; 75: 1052–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly CG, Ho TC, Blom EH, LeWinn KZ, Sacchet MD, Tymofiyeva O, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity of the amygdala and longitudinal changes in depression severity in adolescent depression. J Affect Disord 2017; 207: 86–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox J, Witten IB.. Striatal circuits for reward learning and decision-making. Nat Rev Neurosci 2019; 20: 482–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, et al. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum Brain Mapp 2000; 9: 93–105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Harrison NA.. Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron 2013; 77: 624–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Rolls ET.. Hunger and satiety modify the responses of olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 1996; 75: 1673–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Araujo IET, Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, McGlone F, Phillips N.. Taste-olfactory convergence, and the representation of the pleasantness of flavour, in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 2003; 18: 2059–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Rolls ET.. Synaptic and spiking dynamics underlying reward reversal in orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2005; 15: 15–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Rolls ET, Albantakis L, Romo R.. Brain mechanisms for perceptual and reward-related decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 2013; 103: 194–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deichmann R, Gottfried JA, Hutton C, Turner R.. Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroimage 2003; 19: 430–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deichmann R, Josephs O, Hutton C, Corfield DR, Turner R.. Compensation of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses in echo-planar fMRI imaging. Neuroimage 2002; 15: 120–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado MR, Jou RL, Phelps EA.. Neural systems underlying aversive conditioning in humans with primary and secondary reinforcers. Front Neurosci 2011; 5: 71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng WL, Rolls ET, Ji X, Robbins TW, Banaschewski T, Bokde ALW, et al. Separate neural systems for behavioral change and for emotional responses to failure during behavioral inhibition. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38: 3527–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downar J. Orbitofrontal cortex: a ‘non-rewarding’ new treatment target in depression? Curr Biol 2019; 29: R59–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drevets WC. Orbitofrontal cortex function and structure in depression. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1121: 499–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du J, Rolls ET, Cheng W, Li Y, Gong W, Qiu J, et al. Functional connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Cortex 2020. a; 123: 185–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du J, Rolls ET, Gong W, Cao M, Vatansever D, Cheng W, et al. 2020. b. Association between parental age, brain structure, and behavioral and cognitive problems in children. Am Acad Child Adolescent Psychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Dunlop BW, Rajendra JK, Craighead WE, Kelley ME, McGrath CL, Choi KS, et al. Functional connectivity of the subcallosal cingulate cortex and differential outcomes to treatment with cognitive-behavioral therapy or antidepressant medication for Major Depressive Disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174: 533–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eshel N, Roiser JP.. Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 68: 118–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Robbins TW.. From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37: 1946–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feffer K, Fettes P, Giacobbe P, Daskalakis ZJ, Blumberger DM, Downar J.. 1Hz rTMS of the right orbitofrontal cortex for major depression: Safety, tolerability and clinical outcomes. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2018; 28: 109–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellows LK. Orbitofrontal contributions to value-based decision making: evidence from humans with frontal lobe damage. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2011; 1239: 51–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freton M, Lemogne C, Bergouignan L, Delaveau P, Lehericy S, Fossati P.. The eye of the self: precuneus volume and visual perspective during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain Struct Funct 2014; 219: 959–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabbott PL, Warner TA, Jays PR, Bacon SJ.. Areal and synaptic interconnectivity of prelimbic (area 32), infralimbic (area 25) and insular cortices in the rat. Brain Res 2003; 993: 59–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ge T, Feng J, Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET.. Componential Granger causality, and its application to identifying the source and mechanisms of the top-down biased activation that controls attention to affective vs sensory processing. Neuroimage 2012; 59: 1846–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giza BK, Scott TR.. Blood glucose selectively affects taste-evoked activity in rat nucleus tractus solitarius. Physiol Behav 1983; 31: 643–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giza BK, Scott TR.. Intravenous insulin infusions in rats decrease gustatory-evoked responses to sugars. Am J Physiol 1987; 252: R994–1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giza BK, Scott TR, Vanderweele DA.. Administration of satiety factors and gustatory responsiveness in the nucleus tractus solitarius of the rat. Brain Res Bull 1992; 28: 637–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glascher J, Adolphs R, Damasio H, Bechara A, Rudrauf D, Calamia M, et al. Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109: 14681–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gong W, Rolls ET, Du J, Feng J, Cheng W.. Brain structure is linked to the association between family environment and psychiatric problems in children. Nat Commun 2020. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Gotlib IH, Hammen CL, editors. Handbook of Depression. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Baez-Mendoza R, Genest W, Deco G, Schultz W.. Primate amygdala neurons simulate decision processes of social partners. Cell 2019; 177: 986–98 e15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET.. Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. Eur J Neurosci 2008; 27: 723–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET.. Different representations of relative and absolute value in the human brain. Neuroimage 2009; 48: 258–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET.. Attentional modulation of affective vs sensory processing: functional connectivity and a top-down biased activation theory of selective attention. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104: 1649–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET.. Value, pleasure, and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2011; 15: 56–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET, Bilderbeck A.. How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: top down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cereb Cortex 2008. a; 18: 1549–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET, Parris BA.. From affective value to decision-making in the prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci 2008. b; 28: 1930–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gradin VB, Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Stickle C, Milders M, et al. Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain 2011; 134: 1751–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grattan LE, Glimcher PW.. Absence of spatial tuning in the orbitofrontal cortex. PLoS One 2014; 9: e112750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JP, Muller VI, Eickhoff SB, Fox PT.. Multimodal abnormalities of brain structure and function in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Am J Psychiatry 2020; 177: 422–434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grieve SM, Korgaonkar MS, Koslow SH, Gordon E, Williams LM.. Widespread reductions in gray matter volume in depression. Neuroimage Clin 2013; 3: 332–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamani C, Mayberg H, Snyder B, Giacobbe P, Kennedy S, Lozano AM.. Deep brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate gyrus for depression: anatomical location of active contacts in clinical responders and a suggested guideline for targeting. J Neurosurg 2009; 111: 1209–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamani C, Mayberg H, Stone S, Laxton A, Haber S, Lozano AM.. The subcallosal cingulate gyrus in the context of major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69: 301–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton JP, Chen MC, Gotlib IH.. Neural systems approaches to understanding major depressive disorder: an intrinsic functional organization perspective. Neurobiol Dis 2013; 52: 4–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassanpour MS, Simmons WK, Feinstein JS, Luo Q, Lapidus RC, Bodurka J, et al. The insular cortex dynamically maps changes in cardiorespiratory interoception. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018; 43: 426–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasselmo ME, Rolls ET, Baylis GC.. The role of expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the temporal visual cortex of the monkey. Behav Brain Res 1989; 32: 203–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helm K, Viol K, Weiger TM, Tass PA, Grefkes C, Del Monte D, et al. Neuronal connectivity in major depressive disorder: a systematic review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018; 14: 2715–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henssen A, Zilles K, Palomero-Gallagher N, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Gerboga F, et al. Cytoarchitecture and probability maps of the human medial orbitofrontal cortex. Cortex 2016; 75: 87–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hervig ME, Fiddian L, Piilgaard L, Bozic T, Blanco-Pozo M, Knudsen C, et al. Dissociable and paradoxical roles of rat medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex in visual serial reversal learning. Cereb Cortex 2020; 30: 1016–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtzheimer PE, Husain MM, Lisanby SH, Taylor SF, Whitworth LA, McClintock S, et al. Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: a multisite, randomised, sham-controlled trial. Lancet Psychiatry 2017; 4: 839–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornak J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, O'Doherty J, Bullock PR, et al. Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain 2003; 126: 1691–712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornak J, O'Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, Bullock PR, et al. Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16: 463–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornak J, Rolls ET, Wade D.. Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 1996; 34: 247–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu C-CH, Rolls ET, Huang C-C, Chong ST, Lo C-YZ, Feng J, et al. Connections of the human orbitofrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. Cereb Cortex 2020; 30: 5830–43. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhaa160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huys QJ, Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Dayan P.. Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2013; 3: 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iversen SD, Mishkin M.. Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Exp Brain Res 1970; 11: 376–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo A. Functional heterogeneity within rat orbitofrontal cortex in reward learning and decision making. J Neurosci 2017; 37: 10529–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansen-Berg H, Gutman DA, Behrens TE, Matthews PM, Rushworth MF, Katz E, et al. Anatomical connectivity of the subgenual cingulate region targeted with deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Cereb Cortex 2008; 18: 1374–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SC, Baxter LC, Wilder LS, Pipe JG, Heiserman JE, Prigatano GP.. Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain 2002; 125: 1808–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner RP, Rolls ET.. A computational theory of hippocampal function, and tests of the theory: new developments. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 48: 92–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kircher TT, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Simmons A, Bartels M, David AS.. The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self-processing. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 683–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kircher TT, Senior C, Phillips ML, Benson PJ, Bullmore ET, Brammer M, et al. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self-processing: effects of faces and words. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2000; 10: 133–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi Y, Amaral DG.. Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: II. Cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 2003; 466: 48–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi Y, Amaral DG.. Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: III. Cortical efferents. J Comp Neurol 2007; 502: 810–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koolschijn PC, van Haren NE, Lensvelt-Mulders GJ, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS.. Brain volume abnormalities in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2009; 30: 3719–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML, O’Doherty J, Rolls ET, Andrews C.. Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb Cortex 2003; 13: 1064–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET.. Neural correlates of rapid reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction. Neuroimage 2003; 20: 1371–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Milders M, Reid I, Steele JD.. Abnormal temporal difference reward-learning signals in major depression. Brain 2008; 131: 2084–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuwabara M, Kang N, Holy TE, Padoa-Schioppa C.. Neural mechanisms of economic choices in mice. Elife 2020; 9: e49669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laxton AW, Neimat JS, Davis KD, Womelsdorf T, Hutchison WD, Dostrovsky JO, et al. Neuronal coding of implicit emotion categories in the subcallosal cortex in patients with depression. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 74: 714–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE, Brown R.. A higher-order theory of emotional consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2017; 114: E2016–E25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J, Brown R, Pine D, Hofmann S.. Know thyself: well-being and subjective experience. Cerebrum 2018; 2018 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6353121/. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE, Pine DS.. Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety: a two-system framework. Am J Psychiatry 2016; 173: 1083–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leech R, Sharp DJ.. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 2014; 137: 12–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard CM, Rolls ET, Wilson FAW, Baylis GC.. Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. Behav Brain Res 1985; 15: 159–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzetti V, Allen NB, Fornito A, Yucel M.. Structural brain abnormalities in major depressive disorder: a selective review of recent MRI studies. J Affect Disord 2009; 117: 1–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozano AM, Giacobbe P, Hamani C, Rizvi SJ, Kennedy SH, Kolivakis TT, et al. A multicenter pilot study of subcallosal cingulate area deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. J Neurosurg 2012; 116: 315–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lujan JL, Chaturvedi A, Choi KS, Holtzheimer PE, Gross RE, Mayberg HS, et al. Tractography-activation models applied to subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation. Brain Stimul 2013; 6: 737–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo Q, Ge T, Grabenhorst F, Feng J, Rolls ET.. Attention-dependent modulation of cortical taste circuits revealed by Granger causality with signal-dependent noise. PLoS Comput Biol 2013; 9: e1003265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma Y. Neuropsychological mechanism underlying antidepressant effect: a systematic meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20: 311–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackey S, Petrides M.. Quantitative demonstration of comparable architectonic areas within the ventromedial and lateral orbital frontal cortex in the human and the macaque monkey brains. Eur J Neurosci 2010; 32: 1940–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matrix. 2013. Economic analysis of workplace mental health promotion and mental disorder prevention programmes and of their potential contribution to EU health, social and economic policy objectives. Executive Agency for Health and Consumers, Specific Request EAHC/2011/Health/19 for the Implementation of Framework Contract EAHC/2010/Health/01/Lot 2
- Mayberg HS. Positron emission tomography imaging in depression: a neural systems perspective. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 2003; 13: 805–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayberg HS, Riva-Posse P, Crowell AL.. Deep brain stimulation for depression: keeping an eye on a moving target. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73: 439–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe C, Rolls ET, Bilderbeck A, McGlone F.. Cognitive influences on the affective representation of touch and the sight of touch in the human brain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2008; 3: 97–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe C, Woffindale C, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ.. Neural processing of reward and punishment in young people at increased familial risk of depression. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 72: 588–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millhouse OE, DeOlmos J.. Neuronal configuration in lateral and basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 1983; 10: 1269–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munuera J, Rigotti M, Salzman CD.. Shared neural coding for social hierarchy and reward value in primate amygdala. Nat Neurosci 2018; 21: 415–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, Rudebeck PH.. Specializations for reward-guided decision-making in the primate ventral prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2018; 19: 404–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Namboodiri VMK, Otis JM, van Heeswijk K, Voets ES, Alghorazi RA, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, et al. Single-cell activity tracking reveals that orbitofrontal neurons acquire and maintain a long-term memory to guide behavioral adaptation. Nat Neurosci 2019; 22: 1110–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noonan MP, Chau BKH, Rushworth MFS, Fellows LK.. Contrasting effects of medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex lesions on credit assignment and decision-making in humans. J Neurosci 2017; 37: 7023–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nugent AC, Milham MP, Bain EE, Mah L, Cannon DM, Marrett S, et al. Cortical abnormalities in bipolar disorder investigated with MRI and voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 2006; 30: 485–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C.. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4: 95–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Doherty J, Winston J, Critchley H, Perrett D, Burt DM, Dolan RJ.. Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41: 147–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öngür D, Ferry AT, Price JL.. Architectonic division of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol 2003; 460: 425–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ongür D, Price JL.. The organisation of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10: 206–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa-Schioppa C, Assad JA.. Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 2006; 441: 223–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa-Schioppa C, Conen KE.. Orbitofrontal cortex: a neural circuit for economic decisions. Neuron 2017; 96: 736–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pager J, Giachetti I, Holley A, Le Magnen J.. A selective control of olfactory bulb electrical activity in relation to food deprivation and satiety in rats. Physiol Behav 1972; 9: 573–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandya DN, Yeterian EH.. Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1996; 351: 1423–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passingham RE, 2021. Understanding the prefrontal cortex: selective advantage, connectivity and neural operations. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Passingham REP, Wise SP, 2012. The neurobiology of the prefrontal cortex. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Perrett DI, Rolls ET, Caan W.. Visual neurons responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp Brain Res 1982; 47: 329–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN, Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and macaque frontal cortex In: Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science; 1995. p. 17–58. [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Tomaiuolo F, Yeterian EH, Pandya DN.. The prefrontal cortex: comparative architectonic organization in the human and the macaque monkey brains. Cortex 2012; 48: 46–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA. The costs of reproducibility. Neuron 2019; 101: 11–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JL. Connections of orbital cortex In: Zald DH, Rauch SL, editors. The orbitofrontal cortex. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. p. 39–55. [Google Scholar]
- Price JL. Definition of the orbital cortex in relation to specific connections with limbic and visceral structures and other cortical regions. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1121: 54–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JL, Drevets WC.. Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16: 61–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao VR, Sellers KK, Wallace DL, Lee MB, Bijanzadeh M, Sani OG, et al. Direct electrical stimulation of lateral orbitofrontal cortex acutely improves mood in individuals with symptoms of depression. Curr Biol 2018; 28: 3893–902 e4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riva-Posse P, Choi KS, Holtzheimer PE, Crowell AL, Garlow SJ, Rajendra JK, et al. A connectomic approach for subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation surgery: prospective targeting in treatment-resistant depression. Mol Psychiatry 2018; 23: 843–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Précis of the brain and emotion. Behav Brain Sci 2000; 23: 177–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Emotion explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. A computational neuroscience approach to consciousness. Neural Netw 2007; 20: 962–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Face neurons In: Calder AJ, Rhodes G, Johnson MH, Haxby JV, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. p. 51–75. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Invariant visual object and face recognition: neural and computational bases, and a model: VisNet . Front Comput Neurosci 2012. a; 635: 1–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Neuroculture On the implications of brain science. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. b. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. A biased activation theory of the cognitive and attentional modulation of emotion. Front Hum Neurosci 2013. a; 7: 74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. What are emotional states, and why do we have them? Emot Rev 2013. b; 5: 241–7. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Emotion and decision-making explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Taste, olfactory, and food reward value processing in the brain. Prog Neurobiol 2015; 127–128: 64–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. A non-reward attractor theory of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016. a; 68: 47–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Cerebral cortex: principles of operation. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2016. b. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Functions of the anterior insula in taste, autonomic, and related functions. Brain Cogn 2016. c; 110: 4–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The roles of the orbitofrontal cortex via the habenula in non-reward and depression, and in the responses of serotonin and dopamine neurons. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 75: 331–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The brain, emotion, and depression. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2018. a. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The storage and recall of memories in the hippocampo-cortical system. Cell Tissue Res 2018. b; 373: 577–604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for action, emotion, and memory In: Vogt BA, editor. Handbook of clinical neurology: cingulate cortex. Oxford: Elsevier; 2019. a. p. 23–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for emotion, action, and memory. Brain Struct Funct 2019. b; 224: 3001–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2019. c. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex and emotion in health and disease, including depression. Neuropsychologia 2019. d; 128: 14–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Neural computations underlying phenomenal consciousness: a Higher Order Syntactic Thought theory. Front Psychol 2020; 11: 655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. Brain computations: what and how. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2021. a. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, The neuroscience of emotional disorders In: Heilman KM, Nadeau S, editors. Handbook of clinical neurology: emotional disorders associated with neurological diseases. New York: Elsevier; 2021. b. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Burton MJ, Mora F.. Neurophysiological analysis of brain-stimulation reward in the monkey. Brain Res 1980; 194: 339–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Cheng W, Du J, Wei D, Qiu J, Dai D, et al. Functional connectivity of the right inferior frontal gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex in depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2020. a; 15: 75–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Cheng W, Gilson M, Qiu J, Hu Z, Ruan H, et al. Effective connectivity in depression. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2018; 3: 187–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Cheng W, Gong W, Qiu J, Zhou C, Zhang J, et al. Functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in depression and in health. Cereb Cortex 2019; 29: 3617–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Browning AS, Inoue K.. Face-selective and auditory neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Exp Brain Res 2006; 170: 74–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Mason R, Wakeman EA.. Orbitofrontal cortex neurons: role in olfactory and visual association learning. J Neurophysiol 1996; 75: 1970–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Deco G.. The noisy brain: stochastic dynamics as a principle of brain function Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Deco G.. Non-reward neural mechanisms in the orbitofrontal cortex. Cortex 2016; 83: 27–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F.. The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 2008; 86: 216–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F, Deco G.. Choice, difficulty, and confidence in the brain. Neuroimage 2010. a; 53: 694–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F, Deco G.. Decision-making, errors, and confidence in the brain. J Neurophysiol 2010. b; 104: 2359–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F, Margot C, da Silva MAAP, Velazco MI. Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes olfactory stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci 2008; 20: 1815–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J.. Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994; 57: 1518–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Joliot M, Tzourio-Mazoyer N.. Implementation of a new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the automated anatomical labeling atlas. Neuroimage 2015. a; 122: 1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Judge SJ, Sanghera M.. Activity of neurones in the inferotemporal cortex of the alert monkey. Brain Res 1977; 130: 229–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Kellerhals MB, Nichols TE.. Age differences in the brain mechanisms of good taste. Neuroimage 2015. b; 113: 298–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, de Araujo IET.. Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odors in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 2003. a; 18: 695–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Francis S, Bowtell R, McGlone F.. Representations of pleasant and painful touch in the human orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Cereb Cortex 2003. b; 13: 308–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ, Yaxley S.. Hunger modulates the responses to gustatory stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 1989; 1: 53–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Vatansever D, Li Y, Cheng W, Feng J.. Rapid rule-based reward reversal and the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex Communications doi: 10.1093/texcom/tgaa087. 2020. b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Wirth S.. Spatial representations in the primate hippocampus, and their functions in memory and navigation. Prog Neurobiol 2018; 171: 90–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenkilde CE, Bauer RH, Fuster JM.. Single unit activity in ventral prefrontal cortex in behaving monkeys. Brain Res 1981; 209: 375–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal DM. Varieties of higher-order theory In: Gennaro RJ, editor. Higher order theories of consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 2004. p. 17–44. [Google Scholar]
- Rothkirch M, Tonn J, Kohler S, Sterzer P.. Neural mechanisms of reinforcement learning in unmedicated patients with major depressive disorder. Brain 2017; 140: 1147–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudebeck PH, Behrens TE, Kennerley SW, Baxter MG, Buckley MJ, Walton ME, et al. Frontal cortex subregions play distinct roles in choices between actions and stimuli. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 13775–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudebeck PH, Saunders RC, Lundgren DA, Murray EA.. Specialized representations of value in the orbital and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: desirability versus availability of outcomes. Neuron 2017; 95: 1208–20 e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudebeck PH, Saunders RC, Prescott AT, Chau LS, Murray EA.. Prefrontal mechanisms of behavioral flexibility, emotion regulation and value updating. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16: 1140–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF, Kolling N, Sallet J, Mars RB.. Valuation and decision-making in frontal cortex: one or many serial or parallel systems? Curr Opin Neurobiol 2012; 22: 946–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF, Noonan MP, Boorman ED, Walton ME, Behrens TE.. Frontal cortex and reward-guided learning and decision-making. Neuron 2011; 70: 1054–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutledge RB, Moutoussis M, Smittenaar P, Zeidman P, Taylor T, Hrynkiewicz L, et al. Association of neural and emotional impacts of reward prediction errors with major depression. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74: 790–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saez RA, Saez A, Paton JJ, Lau B, Salzman CD.. Distinct roles for the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in representing the relative amount of expected reward. Neuron 2017; 95: 70–7 e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleem KS, Kondo H, Price JL.. Complementary circuits connecting the orbital and medial prefrontal networks with the temporal, insular, and opercular cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 2008; 506: 659–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleem KS, Miller B, Price JL.. Subdivisions and connectional networks of the lateral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 2014; 522: 1641–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanghera MK, Rolls ET, Roper-Hall A.. Visual responses of neurons in the dorsolateral amygdala of the alert monkey. Exp Neurol 1979; 63: 610–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satterthwaite TD, Kable JW, Vandekar L, Katchmar N, Bassett DS, Baldassano CF, et al. Common and dissociable dysfunction of the reward system in bipolar and unipolar depression. Neuropsychopharmacol 2015; 40: 2258–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA, Takahashi YK.. A new perspective on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in adaptive behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 885–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nat Rev Neurosci 2016. a; 17: 183–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Reward functions of the basal ganglia. J Neural Transm 2016. b; 123: 679–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Reward prediction error. Curr Biol 2017; 27: R369–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott TR, Small DM.. The role of the parabrachial nucleus in taste processing and feeding. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009; 1170: 372–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe MJ, Stalnaker T, Schuck NW, Killcross S, Schoenbaum G, Niv Y.. An integrated model of action selection: distinct modes of cortical control of striatal decision making. Annu Rev Psychol 2019; 70: 53–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe MJ, Wikenheiser AM, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G.. The state of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron 2015; 88: 1075–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small DM, Scott TR.. Symposium overview: what happens to the pontine processing? Repercussions of interspecies differences in pontine taste representation for tasting and feeding. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009; 1170: 343–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strait CE, Blanchard TC, Hayden BY.. Reward value comparison via mutual inhibition in ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 2014; 82: 1357–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugiura M, Watanabe J, Maeda Y, Matsue Y, Fukuda H, Kawashima R.. Cortical mechanisms of visual self-recognition. Neuroimage 2005; 24: 143–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tegelbeckers J, Kanowski M, Krauel K, Haynes JD, Breitling C, Flechtner HH, et al. Orbitofrontal signaling of future reward is associated with hyperactivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Neurosci 2018; 38: 6779–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe SJ, Rolls ET, Maddison S.. Neuronal activity in the orbitofrontal cortex of the behaving monkey. Exp Brain Res 1983; 49: 93–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, editor. Cingulate neurobiology and disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, editor. Handbook of clinical neurology: cingulate cortex. 3rd edn.Oxford: Elsevier; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Laureys S, The primate posterior cingulate gyrus: connections, sensorimotor orientation, gateway to limbic processing In: Vogt BA, editor. Cingulate neurobiology and disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. p. 275–308. [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Pandya DN.. Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: II. Cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 1987; 262: 271–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Völlm BA, de Araujo IET, Cowen PJ, Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, Smith KA, et al. Methamphetamine activates reward circuitry in drug naïve human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29: 1715–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H, Rolls ET, Du X, Du J, Yang D, Li J, et al. Severe nausea and vomiting in pregnancy: psychiatric and cognitive problems, and brain structure in children. BMC Med 2020; 18: 228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ. Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron 2002; 36: 955–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whalen PJ, Phelps EA, editors. The human amygdala. New York: Guilford; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Williams GV, Rolls ET, Leonard CM, Stern C.. Neuronal responses in the ventral striatum of the behaving macaque. Behav Brain Res 1993; 55: 243–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RC, Takahashi YK, Schoenbaum G, Niv Y.. Orbitofrontal cortex as a cognitive map of task space. Neuron 2014; 81: 267–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise SP. Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends Neurosci 2008; 31: 599–608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xie C, Jia T, Rolls ET, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ, Zhang J, et al. Reward vs non-reward sensitivity of the medial vs lateral orbitofrontal cortex relates to the severity of depressive symptoms. Biol Psychiatry 2020. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaxley S, Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ.. The responsiveness of neurons in the insular gustatory cortex of the macaque monkey is independent of hunger. Physiol Behav 1988; 42: 223–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.