Abstract
Aim
To determine the prevalence of atypical general movements (GMs) in the general population, to examine its time trend and associated factors.
Methods
Participants consisted of 300 infants born in 2016‐2018 (current cohort; gestational age 39.4 weeks (27‐42); 162 boys), representative of the Dutch population. GMs were assessed at 2‐4 months corrected age in terms of GM‐complexity (definitely abnormal (DA) or not) and fidgety movements (present or absent). GM‐complexity data from a cohort of 455 Dutch infants born in 2001‐2002 were used to investigate the time trend.
Results
In the current cohort, 10 infants (3%) showed DA GM‐complexity and 8 (3%) absent fidgety movements. Only one infant had both GM‐impairments (0.3%). The prevalence of DA GM‐complexity did not differ from that in the 2001‐2002 cohort (adjusted odds ratio (OR) = 1.47 [0.53, 4.06]). DA GM‐complexity was associated with maternal smoking (adjusted OR = 3.59 [1.56, 8.28]) and marginally with prematurity (adjusted OR = 2.78 [1.00, 7.74]); absence of fidgety movements was curvilinearly associated with assessment age only (OR = 1.06 [1.01, 1.12]).
Conclusion
In the general population, the prevalence of DA GM‐complexity and absent fidgety movements is 3%. The finding that they rarely co‐occur and are associated with different factors indicates that GM‐assessment needs to address both aspects.
Keywords: general movements, general population, neurological function, prevalence, risk factors
Abbreviations
- CA
corrected age
- CP
cerebral palsy
- CI
confidence interval
- DA
definitely abnormal
- GM
general movement
- GMA
general movements assessment
- IMP
infant motor profile
- MA
mildly abnormal
- OR
odds ratio
- SINDA
Standardized Infant NeuroDevelopment Assessment
- UMCG
University Medical Center Groningen
Key notes.
This study reports the prevalence of atypical features of general movements (GMs) in Dutch infants representative of the general population, therewith providing reference data for high‐risk infants.
Atypical GM‐complexity and absent fidgety movements are prevalent in 3% of infants aged 2‐4 months, but these impairments rarely co‐occur (0.3%).
Atypical GM‐complexity is associated with maternal smoking and marginally with prematurity, whereas absence of fidgety movements is only associated with assessment age.
1. INTRODUCTION
General movements (GMs) are the spontaneous movements involving all parts of the body in the foetus and young infant. Accumulating evidence suggests that the assessment of the quality of GMs evaluates early neurological function and identifies infants at high risk of developmental problems, including cerebral palsy (CP). 1 , 2 The predictive power of GMs assessment (GMA) is particularly high when it is performed at 2 to 5 months corrected age (CA; so‐called fidgety phase). 3 GMA in the fidgety phase focuses on two different but interrelated aspects: (a) the basic parameter of GM‐quality typically present at all GM‐ages, that is movement complexity and variation (in short: movement complexity) and (b) the age‐dependent presence of fidgety movements. 1 The former refers to the spatial and temporal variation of the movements; the latter are tiny movements occurring irregularly over the body. Currently, there are two variants of GMA: Prechtl's method 4 and the Hadders‐Algra classification. 1 They assess the same construct of GMs, that is movement complexity and fidgety movements, but focus in their description on either of these aspects. Prechtl's method emphasises the absence of fidgety movements, whereas Hadders‐Algra's classification stresses the marked reduction in movement complexity in GMs classified as definitely abnormal (DA).
GMA has been applied extensively in high‐risk infants. In these infants, the prevalence of clinically relevant atypical GMs, that is DA GM‐complexity and/or absent fidgety movements, is well documented. The prevalence of these atypical GMs in infants born very preterm is 22%‐26% 5 , 6 ; that in infants with a brain lesion 34%‐53%. 7 , 8 In contrast, relatively little attention has been paid to GMs in typically developing infants. Most studies on low‐risk infants reported on small samples (from 21 to 84 infants; 0%‐11% atypical) 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 or more or less selective samples: healthy full‐terms participating in a study on infant nutrition (0% atypical) 13 ; infants born to subfertile couples (1% atypical) 14 and healthy full‐terms matched to a cohort of extremely preterm infants (3% atypical). 15 Only the Dutch study of Bouwstra et al 16 studied the prevalence of atypical GMs in the general population born in 2001‐2002. The study reported a prevalence of 4%.
Over the years, changes in obstetric and neonatal practices led to decreasing rates in perinatal mortality and morbidity, also in low‐risk infants. 17 This may have resulted in a lower prevalence of atypical GMs in present times compared with earlier periods. In addition, previous studies reported the prevalence of atypical GMs either in terms of DA GM‐complexity 5 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 16 or by means of absent fidgety movements. 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 15 We know that in high‐risk infants DA GM‐complexity and absent fidgety movements are highly but not perfectly interrelated. We hypothesise that both characteristics of GM‐impairment may be less coupled in low‐risk populations, as their underlying neurobiological substrate presumably differs. 1 In addition, information on the prevalence of both GM‐impairments in the general population may serve as a reference for studies in high‐risk populations.
The aim of this study is threefold, (a) to report the prevalence of atypical GMs both in terms of DA GM‐complexity and in terms of absent fidgety movements, in a cohort representative of the general Dutch population; (b) to examine if the prevalence of atypical GMs decreased over the last 15 years; and (c) to investigate which prenatal, perinatal and socio‐economic characteristics are associated with atypical GMs. Our representative sample includes infants aged 2 to 4 months. The period of 2 to 4 months covers the age range of the fidgety phase. We did not include infants aged 5 months as 5‐months‐olds are mostly involved in goal‐directed movements and spend little time with fidgety GMs anymore. 18 We hypothesised that (a) the group of infants with GMs with DA GM‐complexity would only partially overlap with the group of infants with GMs lacking fidgety movements, as both phenomenon presumably are based on different pathophysiological mechanisms 1 ; (b) the prevalence of atypical GMs in the current cohort is lower than that in the cohort of Bouwstra et al 16 ; and (c) prenatal and perinatal complications, including preterm birth, and low socio‐economic status are associated with atypical GMs.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
This study is based on the IMP‐SINDA project, a study performed to collect norm data for the Infant Motor Profile (IMP) 19 and the Standardized Infant NeuroDevelopmental Assessment (SINDA). 20 The project aimed to recruit a cross‐sectional sample of infants that was representative of the Dutch population in terms of maternal education and ethnicity, with 100 infants per month of age. Inclusion criteria were 2 to 18 months CA, living in the northern part of the Netherlands (ie in the provinces of Groningen, Friesland and Drenthe) and having caregivers with sufficient understanding of the Dutch language to give informed consent. Infants were only excluded if they were too ill to be assessed (eg severe congenital cardiac disorder with insufficient oxygen saturation). Infants were recruited via well‐baby clinics and advertisements, between January 2017 and March 2019. The 300 infants aged 2, 3 and 4 months CA were included in the current GM‐study. All children were assessed once. The Medical Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen (UMCG) approved the study design (METc 2016/294), and the study was registered in the Dutch trial register (NL58069.042.16). All caregivers provided written informed consent. Caregivers filled out a standardised questionnaire on prenatal, perinatal and neonatal (in short: perinatal) and socio‐economic history. If the questionnaire revealed complications, medical records from the concerning hospital were consulted.
For the evaluation of a time trend, data of the 455 infants of the cohort of Bouwstra et al 16 were used. These infants had been born in 2001‐2002 and were assessed at 3 months CA as part of their routine health check at one of the well‐baby clinics in the northern part of the Netherlands. Infants whose parents had an insufficient understanding of the Dutch language were not eligible for inclusion. The prenatal, perinatal and socio‐economic data of participants were collected by means of standardised forms and from medical records. For detailed information see Bouwstra et al. 16
2.2. GMA
GMs were recorded in a similar way in both cohorts. The infants were placed in supine position on a mattress, dressed in a diaper and a bodysuit. Spontaneous movements in an actively awake state without interacting with other persons or toys were videotaped for at least 3 minutes. The recordings in the IMP‐SINDA cohort were based on the first three minutes of the IMP‐assessment, which in young infants consists of a recording of spontaneous movements in supine. The recordings were performed at home, at well‐baby clinics, or at the baby‐laboratory of Developmental Neurology of the UMCG, depending on the parents’ preference. GMA scoring was performed offline by MHA, who has worked on GMA for more than 25 years and was masked for the infant's perinatal history and social background.
GMA consisted of both the evaluation of GM‐complexity and fidgety movements. GM‐complexity was classified into four classes: normal‐optimal (abundant complexity and fluency), normal‐suboptimal (sufficient complexity, no fluency), mildly abnormal (MA, insufficient complexity, no fluency) and definitely abnormal (DA, very limited or absent complexity, no fluency). 1 Fidgety movements were classified according to the following categories: continually present (frequent occurrence in whole body with very short pauses), intermittently present (occurrence in whole body with prolonged pauses), sporadic (isolated occurrence in a few body parts with long pauses) and absent (no fidgety movements). 4 DA GM‐complexity and absent fidgety movements are the clinically relevant forms of atypical GMs, as they are associated with development problems, including CP. 1 , 4
The GMs recordings in the Bouwstra cohort were performed at the well‐baby clinics. Two assessors, HB and MHA (inter‐rater reliability: kappa = 0.82), performed GMA in terms of GM‐complexity. 16 The degree of fidgety movements was not recorded and the old videotapes did not allow for re‐assessment.
2.3. Statistical analyses
The background characteristics of the IMP‐SINDA and Bouwstra cohorts were compared using Mann‐Whitney tests for continuous data and Fisher's exact tests for categorical data. The time trend of DA GM‐complexity was examined by an univariable logistic analysis and a multivariable logistic analysis with adjustment for the unbalanced background characteristics. To investigate which factors were associated with atypical GMs, the association of individual background characteristic with atypical GMs was first tested with an univariable logistic regression analysis. Significant associated factors (P < .05 in univariable analyses) were then entered into a multivariable regression analysis to investigate the major determinants. We ran two separate analyses: one on DA‐GM complexity and one on absent fidgety movements. For the former analysis, we pooled the IMP‐SINDA and the Bouwstra cohorts; for the latter analysis, we only could use the IMP‐SINDA cohort. For the evaluation of the association between the infant's age at GMA and atypical GMs, CA in weeks was used, both for infants born at term and preterm, as it is known that the developmental changes in GMs are stronger associated with CA than with postnatal age. 21 The strength of associations was presented by the odds ratio (OR) with 95% confidence interval (CI). A P value lower than .05 was considered to be statistically significant. All analyses were conducted with SPSS package version 23 (SPSS InC.).
3. RESULTS
3.1. Cohort characteristics
Table 1 describes the background characteristics of the IMP‐SINDA and Bouwstra cohorts. Compared to the Bouwstra cohort, the IMP‐SINDA cohort had a younger maternal age (P < .001), a higher proportion of non‐native Dutch parents (mothers: P < .001, fathers: P = .006), a lower rate of maternal substance use (P < .001) but a higher rate of maternal medication use (P = .001) during pregnancy, a lower birthweight (P = .003) with a higher percentage of infants small for gestational age (P = .019) and a younger assessment age (P < .001).
TABLE 1.
Basic characteristics of the study populations
IMP‐SINDA cohort (assessed 2017‐2019) (n = 300) |
Bouwstra cohort (assessed 2001‐2002) (n = 455) |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
n of data | median (range) or n (%) | n of data | median (range) or n (%) | |
Prenatal and perinatal characteristics | ||||
Pre‐pregnancy maternal BMI in kg/m2 | 298 | 24.1 (17.4‐45.0) | — a | |
Obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2), n (%) | 298 | 54 (18%) | ||
Overweight (25 ≤ BMI ≤29.9 kg/m2) | 298 | 76 (26%) | ||
Assisted reproduction | 297 | 23 (8%) | — a | |
Maternal smoking | 299 | 35 (12%) | 450 | 61 (14%) |
Substance exposure (alcohol or drugs) | 299 | 5 (2%) | 452 | 37 (8%) |
Maternal medication b | 299 | 33 (11%) | 451 | 20 (4%) |
Maternal hypertension | 299 | 38 (13%) | — a | |
Maternal diabetes | 299 | 24 (8%) | — a | |
Maternal thyroid disease | 299 | 9 (3%) | — a | |
Instrumental delivery | 300 | 63 (21%) | 447 | 110 (25%) |
Sex: Male | 300 | 162 (54%) | 446 | 214 (48%) |
Twin | 300 | 10 (3%) | 455 | 15 (3%) |
Birthweight in grams | 300 | 3374 (1120‐4875) | 447 | 3500 (850‐5432) |
Small for gestational age c | 300 | 45 (15%) | 441 | 41 (9%) |
Gestational age in weeks | 300 | 39.4 (27.3‐42.1) | 452 | 40.0 (29.0‐42.0) |
Preterm birth (<37 wk) | 300 | 26 (8%) | 452 | 32 (7%) |
Meconium in amniotic fluid | 298 | 38 (13%) | — a | |
Hyperbilirubinemia requiring phototherapy | 298 | 18 (6%) | — a | |
Socio‐economic characteristics | ||||
Maternal age in years | 300 | 30 (16‐44) | 405 | 31 (15‐46) |
Advanced maternal age (Age ≥35 y) | 300 | 48 (16%) | 405 | 86 (21%) |
Paternal age in years | 293 | 32 (21‐68) | 403 | 33 (16‐63) |
Advanced paternal age (Age ≥40 y) | 293 | 37 (13%) | 403 | 40 (10%) |
Maternal educational level: High d | 300 | 119 (40%) | 402 | 146 (36%) |
Paternal educational level: High d | 289 | 116 (40%) | 394 | 153 (39%) |
Maternal ethnicity: Non‐native Dutch | 300 | 33 (11%) | 407 | 15 (4%) |
Paternal ethnicity: Non‐native Dutch | 294 | 27 (9%) | 406 | 16 (4%) |
Assessment age in weeks e | 300 | 12 (6‐20) | 449 | 13 (4‐22) |
Bold type indicates group difference between the IMP‐SINDA and Bouwstra cohort (P < .05).
Abbreviation: BMI, body mass index.
Data were unavailable in the Bouwstra cohort.
Maternal medication indicates the use of at least one of the following: insulin, antihypertensive medication, thyroid‐stimulating medication, antidepressants/antipsychotics/benzodiazepines, anti‐epileptic medication.
Small for gestational age: birth weight for gestational age <10th percentile of Dutch growth curves.
High educational level: vocational college and university.
Corrected age is used in all infants, estimated from the expected birth date (term age) and assessment date, rounded down to weeks.
3.2. Prevalence of atypical GMs and time trend
The assessment in the IMP‐SINDA cohort revealed that 10 (3%) infants had DA GM‐complexity (Figure 1A) and 8 (3%) showed absent fidgety movements (Figure 1B). Only one infant (0.3%) showed GMs with both impairments. This infant was born preterm, and his cranial ultrasound had shown small cysts in left periventricular area. His GMs had been assessed at 8 weeks CA.
FIGURE 1.
Prevalence of atypical general movements (GMs) in the current cohort. Percentage of atypical GMs in terms of (A) GM‐complexity and (B) fidgety movements in the IMP‐SINDA cohort at various corrected ages (CA) in weeks. Typical GM‐complexity includes the classifications of GM‐complexity as normal optimal, normal suboptimal, and mildly abnormal. Present fidgety movements include the categories of continually present, intermittently present, and sporadic fidgety movements. The numbers above the bars indicate the numbers of infants assessed at each week CA
The prevalence of DA GM‐complexity seemed slightly lower in the IMP‐SINDA cohort (3%) than in the Bouwstra cohort (4%). 16 However, the likelihood of having DA GM‐complexity in the two cohorts did not differ significantly when examined without (OR = 0.89 [0.40, 1.97], P = .771) or with adjustments for the unbalanced background characteristics (adjusted OR = 1.47 [0.53, 4.06], P = .461).
3.3. Factors associated with atypical GMs
Univariable analysis indicated that maternal smoking (OR = 3.65 [1.59, 8.38], P = .002) and prematurity (OR = 2.88 [1.05, 7.92], P = .04) were associated with DA GM‐complexity (Table 2). Multivariable analysis revealed that DA GM‐complexity was significantly associated with maternal smoking (adjusted OR = 3.59 [1.56, 8.28], P = .003) and marginally with prematurity (adjusted OR = 2.78 [1.00, 7.74], P = .051).
TABLE 2.
Associations of background characteristics with atypical GMs
Factor in univariable analysis | Outcome | |
---|---|---|
GM‐complexity: Definitely abnormal |
Fidgety movements: Absent |
|
Prenatal and perinatal characteristics | ||
Maternal overweight or obesity | OR = 0.86 [0.24, 3.10] a | OR = 1.30 [0.32‐5.31] a |
Assisted reproduction | OR = 1.34 [0.16, 11.05] a | — b |
Maternal smoking | OR = 3.65 [1.59, 8.38] | — b |
Substance exposure | OR = 1.37 [0.31, 5.98] | — b |
Maternal medication | OR = 1.68 [0.49, 5.78] | — b |
Maternal hypertension | OR = 1.76 [0.36, 8.60] a | OR = 0.98 [0.12, 8.20] a |
Maternal diabetes | — b | — b |
Maternal thyroid disease | — b | — b |
Instrumental delivery | OR = 1.50 [0.64, 3.51] | OR = 2.32 [0.54, 9.98] a |
Sex: Male | OR = 0.81 [0.37, 1.75] | OR = 0.70 [0.16, 2.97] a |
Twin | OR = 1.13 [0.15, 8.66] | — b |
Small for gestational age | OR = 2.51 [0.97, 6.47] | OR = 0.81 [0.10, 6.71] a |
Prematurity | OR = 2.88 [1.05, 7.92] | OR = 1.53 [0.18, 12.90] a |
Meconium in amniotic fluid | OR = 1.75 [0.36, 8.57] a | OR = 0.98 [0.12, 8.17] a |
Jaundice requiring phototherapy | OR = 1.77 [0.21, 14.81] a | OR = 2.30 [0.27, 19.80 a |
Socio‐economic characteristics | ||
Advanced maternal age | OR = 1.63 [0.62, 4.24] | — b |
Advanced paternal age | OR = 2.46 [0.88, 6.87] | — b |
Maternal educational level: High | OR = 0.61 [0.24, 1.58] | OR = 0.50 [0.10, 2.51] a |
Paternal educational level: High | OR = 0.77 [0.29, 1.77] | OR = 0.24 [0.03, 2.04] a |
Maternal ethnicity: non‐native Dutch | OR = 0.65 [0.09, 4.91] | OR = 1.16 [0.14, 9.74] a |
Paternal ethnicity: non‐native Dutch | OR = 0.72 [0.10, 5.49] | — b |
Assessment age | ||
Corrected age (quadratic function) | OR = 1.00 [0.97, 1.04] | OR = 1.06 [1.01, 1.12] a |
Data are presented as odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval in brackets. Bold type indicates a significant association (P < .05).
The OR was based on the IMP‐SINDA cohort only, as data were unavailable in the CB cohort.
OR cannot be estimated because the prevalence in study sample was low (2%‐13%) and it did not occur in infants with DA GM‐complexity (n = 10) or infants with absent fidgety movements (n = 8).
Absent fidgety movements were only associated with assessment age in weeks CA (quadratic logistic regression analyses: age‐square, OR = 1.06 [1.01, 1.12], P = .034) (Table 2), indicating an U‐shaped relationship (Figure 1B).
4. DISCUSSION
The current prevalence of atypical GMs, either in terms of very limited movement complexity or in terms of absent fidgety movements, was 3% in 2 to 4 months old infants representative of the general Dutch population. The prevalence had not changed significantly over the last 15 years. However, in this low‐risk population impairment in movement complexity did not automatically imply impairment in fidgety movements and vice versa. Only one infant (0.3%) showed impairments in both GM‐features. The results reinforce the notion that movement complexity and fidgety movements are two dimensions of GMs based on different neurobiological mechanisms. It has been hypothesised that GM‐complexity is brought about by activity in cortical‐subcortical networks, in which initially the subplate plays a central role and—at fidgety GM‐age—the cortical plate of the primary sensorimotor cortices. The fidgety GMs are the result of general maturational processes in the cortical networks. 1
The idea of two different neurobiological mechanisms was further supported by the finding that atypical GM‐complexity and atypical fidgety movements were associated with different background factors. Maternal smoking and—marginally—preterm birth were risk factors of DA GM‐complexity, whereas assessment age was associated specifically with absent fidgety movements. The association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and DA GM‐complexity has been reported previously. 16 Interestingly, Chang et al 22 showed that prenatal tobacco exposure is associated with altered microstructures in the thalamus and internal capsule in early infancy, alterations which also are associated with reduced movement complexity. 23 We found just a modest association between preterm birth and atypical GM‐complexity and no association between preterm birth and absent fidgety movements, whereas it is well known that preterm infants, especially very preterm infants, are at high risk of DA GM‐complexity 5 and absent fidgety movements. 4 Our findings reflect the low‐risk nature of our group in which only a few infants were born very preterm (<32 weeks, n = 6).
DA GM‐complexity and absent fidgety movements were both prevalent in 3% of the general population. In high‐risk infants, atypical GMs in either format are clearly associated with later diagnosis of CP, 1 but in low‐risk infants the isolated presence of very limited GM‐complexity or absent fidgety movements is especially associated with milder forms of neurological dysfunction. 12 Follow‐up of the Bouwstra cohort at 3 years and 9 months revealed that DA GM‐complexity was also associated with an increased risk of major neurodevelopmental disability, including CP. 24 However, predictive values were lower than those reported in groups of high‐risk infants (Bouwstra cohort: sensitivity to predict CP 67%, specificity 97%). The relatively low sensitivity for CP was brought about by the fact that one of the three children who had developed CP, had shown GMs with typical complexity and typical fidgety movements. He was diagnosed with a unilateral spastic CP. 24
In high‐risk infants, the combination of DA GM‐complexity and no fidgety movements is associated with the highest risk of CP. Hamer et al 25 reported that half of the infants with the combination of these two GM‐impairments were later diagnosed with CP. In our study, the prevalence of the combination of DA GM‐complexity and absent fidgety movements was 0.3%, a prevalence which is twice the prevalence of CP in the general European population (0.18%). 26 This finding and the data of Hamer et al 25 may imply that low‐risk infants with the combination of the two GM‐impairments are at a similar high risk of CP as high‐risk infants with DA GM‐complexity and no fidgety movements. Future studies are needed to further determine the clinical utility of GMA in the general population. For the IMP‐SINDA cohort, follow‐up at the age of 4‐5 years is planned.
The major strength of this study is the representativeness of the study cohort. Apart from maternal education and ethnicity, which were the selection criteria to achieve a representative sample, perinatal and socio‐economic characteristics were comparable to those in Dutch national data (IMP‐SINDA cohort vs national data): maternal overweight or obesity (44% vs 37%), assisted reproduction (8% vs 7%), instrumental delivery (21% vs 24%), male (54% vs52%), twin (3% vs 3%), preterm birth (8% vs 7%), small for gestational age (15% vs 11%), maternal age (30 vs 31 years), paternal age (32 vs 34 years) and high paternal educational level (40% vs 40%). 27 , 28 The representativeness allows generalisation of our results to the general population and other low‐risk populations in high‐income countries, that is populations with similar prevalence of CP, the Netherlands, 0.2%, 29 Europe in general (0.18%), 26 the United States (0.18%) 30 and Australia (0.20%). 31 Another strength of our study is that the GMAs in both the IMP‐SINDA and Bouwstra cohorts were mostly conducted by one experienced assessor. On the other hand, relying on mainly one assessor could also function as a disadvantage, as Gima et al showed that scoring GM‐details in a low‐risk population was associated with substantial interrater variation. 32 The presence of the same assessor for both cohorts supported the reliability of the time trend analysis. The study's major limitation is the single assessment of GMs. As seen in Figure 1B, atypical fidgety movements were observed at 7 to 9 weeks and 15 to 17 weeks CA. We do not know whether the absence of fidgety movements in these children was due to deviant neurological development or typical developmental trends or both. Longitudinal studies in low‐risk infants indicated that the absence of fidgety movements is age‐related; it occurs in particular at the age margins of the fidgety period. 9 , 20 Practically this may imply that the presence or absence of fidgety movements should be assessed longitudinally in the fidgety phase or—when relying on a single assessment—it should be performed in the restricted time window of 10 to 14 weeks CA.
5. CONCLUSION
In the general population, the prevalence of atypical GMs was 3%, which has been stable over the last 15 years. However, the two dimensions of atypical GMs were associated with different risk factors: DA GM‐complexity with maternal smoking and prematurity; absent fidgety movement with not any of the perinatal and social risk factors. The absence of fidgety movement was only associated with the infant's assessment age in weeks CA. Both GM‐impairments infrequently co‐occurred. Our results support the idea that GM‐complexity and fidgety movements are two important aspects of GMs and both need to be assessed.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
Prof. dr Mijna Hadders‐Algra has provided courses on the assessment of GMs since 1993. The honorarium of the courses flows into the Research Fund of Developmental Neurology. She did not get a honorarium, grant or other form of payment to produce the manuscript. Other authors declare no conflict of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The assistance of the medical students and the research assistants of the Kinder Academie Groningen in recruiting the participants and filming the assessments is gratefully acknowledged.
Wu Y‐C, Bouwstra H, Heineman KR, Hadders‐Algra M. Atypical general movements in the general population: Prevalence over the last 15 years and associated factors. Acta Paediatr. 2020;109:2762–2769. 10.1111/apa.15329
Funding information
This project was financially supported by the Cornelia Stichting and the Stichting Ontwikkelingsneurofysiologie Groningen in the Netherlands, and the Postdoctoral Research Abroad Program of Ministry of Science and Technology in Taiwan (108‐2917‐I‐564‐001).
REFERENCES
- 1. Hadders‐Algra M. Neural substrate and clinical significance of general movements: An update. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60:39‐46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Novak I, Morgan C, Adde L, et al. Early, accurate diagnosis and early intervention in cerebral palsy: advances in diagnosis and treatment. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171:897‐907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Bosanquet M, Copeland L, Ware R, Boyd R. A systematic review of tests to predict cerebral palsy in young children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55:418‐426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, Bos AF, Ferrari F, Cioni G. Prechtl's method on the qualitative assessment of general movements in preterm, term and young infants. London: Mac Keith Press; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. De Bock F, Will H, Behrenbeck U, Jarczok MN, Hadders‐Algra M, Philippi H. Predictive value of general movement assessment for preterm infants' development at 2 years ‐ implementation in clinical routine in a non‐academic setting. Res Dev Disabil. 2017;62:69‐80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Spittle AJ, Spencer‐Smith MM, Cheong JL, et al. General movements in very preterm children and neurodevelopment at 2 and 4 years. Pediatrics. 2013;132:e452‐e458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Ferrari F, Todeschini A, Guidotti I, et al. General movements in full‐term infants with perinatal asphyxia are related to basal ganglia and thalamic lesions. J Pediatr. 2011;158:904‐911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Prechtl HF, Einspieler C, Cioni G, Bos AF, Ferrari F, Sontheimer D. An early marker for neurological deficits after perinatal brain lesions. Lancet. 1997;349:1361‐1363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Ferrari F, Frassoldati R, Berardi A, et al. The ontogeny of fidgety movements from 4 to 20 weeks post‐term age in healthy full‐term infants. Early Hum Dev. 2016;103:219‐224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Hitzert MM, Roze E, Van Braeckel KN, Bos AF. Motor development in 3‐month‐old healthy term‐born infants is associated with cognitive and behavioural outcomes at early school age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2014;56:869‐876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Salavati S, Einspieler C, Vagelli G, et al. The association between the early motor repertoire and language development in term children born after normal pregnancy. Early Hum Dev. 2017;111:30‐35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. van Iersel PAM, Bakker SCM, Jonker AJH, Hadders‐Algra M. Does general movements quality in term infants predict cerebral palsy and milder forms of limited mobility at 6 years? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58:1310‐1316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Bouwstra H, Dijck‐Brouwer DA, Wildeman JA, et al. Long‐chain polyunsaturated fatty acids have a positive effect on the quality of general movements of healthy term infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;78:313‐318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Middelburg KJ, Haadsma ML, Heineman MJ, Bos AF, Hadders‐Algra M. Ovarian hyperstimulation and the in vitro fertilization procedure do not influence early neuromotor development; a history of subfertility does. Fertil Steril. 2010;93:544‐553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Kwong AKL, Olsen JE, Eeles AL, et al. Occurrence of and temporal trends in fidgety general movements in infants born extremely preterm/extremely low birthweight and term‐born controls. Early Hum Dev. 2019;135:11‐15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Bouwstra H, Dijk‐Stigter GR, Grooten HMJ, et al. Prevalence of abnormal general movements in three‐month‐old infants. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85:399‐403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ensing S, Abu‐Hanna A, Schaaf JM, Mol BJ, Ravelli ACJ. Trends in birth asphyxia, obstetric interventions and perinatal mortality among term singletons: a nationwide cohort study. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2015;28:632‐637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Hadders‐Algra M. Early human development: from variation to the ability to vary and adapt. Neurosci Biobehav Rew. 2018;90:411‐427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Heineman KR, Bos AF, Hadders‐Algra M. The infant motor profile: A standardized and qualitative method to assess motor behaviour in infancy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008;50:275‐282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Hadders‐Algra M, Tacke U, Pietz J, Rupp A, Philippi H. Reliability and predictive validity of the standardized infant neurodevelopmental assessment neurological scale. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2019;61:654‐660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hadders‐Algra M, Prechtl HF. Developmental course of general movements in early infancy. I. Descriptive analysis of change in form. Early Hum Dev. 1992;28:201‐213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Chang L, Oishi K, Skranes J, et al. Sex‐specific alterations of white matter developmental trajectories in infants with prenatal exposure to methamphetamine and tobacco. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:1217‐1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Guzzetta A, Mercuri E, Rapisardi G, et al. General movements detect early signs of hemiplegia in term infants with neonatal cerebral infarction. Neuropediatrics. 2003;34:61‐66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Bouwstra H, Dijk‐Stigter GR, Grooten HM, et al. Predictive value of definitely abnormal general movements in the general population. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;52:456‐461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Hamer EG, Bos AF, Hadders‐Algra M. Assessment of specific characteristics of abnormal general movements: Does it enhance the prediction of cerebral palsy? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2011;53:751‐756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Sellier E, Platt MJ, Andersen GL, et al. Decreasing prevalence in cerebral palsy: a multi‐site european population‐based study, 1980 to 2003. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58:85‐92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. StatLine databank: Statistics Netherlands Web Site. Available from: https://opendata.cbs.nl/statline/#/CBS/nl/, accessed 18‐08‐2019.
- 28. Perinatale zorg in Nederland: Jaarboek 2016–2017 Stichting Perinatale Registratie Nederland. Available from: https://www.perined.nl/producten/publicaties/jaarboeken, accessed 18‐08‐2019.
- 29. Becher J, Gorter JW, Vemeulenm J, van Schie P, Wiegerink D. Cerebrale parese In: Hadders‐Algra M, Maathuis K, Pangalila RF, Becher J, de Moor J, eds. Kinder revalidatie. Assen: Koninklijke Van Gorcum BV; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Braun KV, Doernberg N, Schieve L, Christensen D, Goodman A, Yeargin‐Allsopp M. Birth prevalence of cerebral palsy: a population‐based study. Pediatrics. 2016;137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Smithers‐Sheedy H, McIntyre S, Gibson C, et al. A special supplement: Findings from the australian cerebral palsy register, birth years 1993 to 2006. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58:5‐10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. GIma H, Shimatani K, Nakano H, Watanabe H, Taga G. Evaluation of fidgety movements of infants based on Gestalt perception reflects differences in limb movement trajectory curvature. Phys Ther. 2019;99:701‐710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]