Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2021 Aug;33(3):792–802. doi: 10.1017/S095457942000005X

Different Factors Predict Adolescent Substance Use vs. Adult Substance Abuse: Lessons from A Social-developmental Approach

Joseph P Allen 1, Emily L Loeb 1, Rachel K Narr 1, Meghan A Costello 1
PMCID: PMC7755752  NIHMSID: NIHMS1547620  PMID: 32638695

Abstract

This 17-year prospective study applied a social-developmental lens to the challenge of distinguishing predictors of adolescent-era substance use from predictors of longer-term adult substance use problems. A diverse community sample of 168 individuals was repeatedly assessed from age 13 to age 30 using test, self-, parent-, and peer- report methods. As hypothesized, substance use within adolescence was linked to a range of likely transient social and developmental factors that are particularly salient during the adolescent-era, including popularity with peers, peer substance use, parent-adolescent conflict, and broader patterns of deviant behavior. Substance abuse problems at ages 27–30 were best predicted, even after accounting for levels of substance use in adolescence, by adolescent-era markers of underlying deficits, including lack of social skills and poor self-concept. The factors that best predicted levels of adolescent-era substance use were not generally predictive of adult substance abuse problems in multivariate models (either with or without accounting for baseline levels of use). Results are interpreted as suggesting that recognizing the developmental nature of adolescent-era substance use may be crucial to distinguishing factors that predict socially-driven and/or relatively transient use during adolescence from factors that predict long-term problems with substance abuse that extend well into adulthood.


A core challenge in the field of developmental psychopathology is the identification of risk factors and competencies that explain not only concurrent difficulties, but underlying developmental issues linked to long-term risk and resilience. The present study seeks to distinguish short- vs. long-term predictors of substance use and abuse. Although a large body of developmentally-focused research has examined predictors of alcohol and substance use in adolescence, the challenge of determining which teens will vs. will not develop substance abuse problems in the longer-term, beyond the early twenties transitional period, has received far less attention. Identifying unique predictors of adolescent use vs. adult substance abuse problems from within early- and mid-adolescence has immense potential value in focusing prevention and intervention efforts.

With only a few exceptions noted below, much of the research that exists on adolescent-era predictors of substance use beyond adolescence extends to predictions only up through the early twenties. Yet, in the early twenties substance use levels remain notably high and still may be significantly driven by transient developmental and cultural factors (e.g., peer pressure, college drinking, etc.) (Masten, Faden, Zucker, & Spear, 2008; Nelson, Van Ryzin, & Dishion, 2014; Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002). In contrast, it is longer-term use that accounts for the most problems, with those abusing in their late twenties considered to be at a very high risk for lifelong patterns of substance abuse (Brook, Brook, Zhang, Cohen, & Whiteman, 2002).

Moffitt’s recognition that adolescence-limited criminal behavior is in large part driven by social and developmental pressures unique to adolescence seems particularly relevant to distinguishing long- vs. short-term substance use and abuse (Moffitt, 2017). Although Moffitt’s analysis was applied to criminal behavior, factors she identifies, such as peer approval of illicit behavior, desire to seek out new experiences, and desire to establish autonomy by taking on ‘adult-like’ behavior, all seem likely to also drive substance use during the adolescent/early adult period. If at least some adolescent criminal activity can arguably be considered as a partially adaptive response to normative social-developmental pressures (Moffitt, 2017), then this argument likely also applies to substance use as well; indeed, some adolescent substance use has even been linked to otherwise positive traits such as popularity (Allen, Porter, McFarland, Marsh, & McElhaney, 2005; Mayeux, Sandstrom, & Cillessen, 2008). Even beyond normative pressures in adolescence, some teens are likely to be exposed to specific stressors (e.g., high levels of parent-adolescent conflict) that, although non-normative, are unlikely to continue with the same intensity in adulthood. Taken together, these factors make it likely that adolescent substance users will differ in significant ways from adults with substance abuse problems.

This study employed a social-developmental perspective to distinguish factors associated with adolescent-era substance use from those which have long-term predictive import with regard to adult substance abuse difficulties (Masten et al., 2008; Zucker, 2008). This perspective is based upon distinguishing transient, age-specific risk factors in adolescence from those likely to reflect underlying difficulty in meeting key social developmental tasks. Several factors are proposed as likely to be important in explaining adolescent substance use, and yet be likely to be relatively transient both in duration and impact and thus much less likely to explain adult-era substance abuse difficulties. Among these potentially transient factors, peer influences supporting substance use are particularly prominent (Nash, McQueen, & Bray, 2005; Patrick & Schulenberg, 2014). Teen substance use has been linked to close friend use, overall popularity, and broader patterns of social contagion (Allen et al., 2005; Ennett et al., 2006; Li, Barrera, Hops, & Fisher, 2002; Mayeux et al., 2008; Patrick & Schulenberg, 2010). All of these links are likely to be especially salient in adolescence given the intense adolescent focus upon establishing status within the peer group (Dijkstra, Cillessen, Lindenberg, & Veenstra, 2010).

Similarly, criminal activity within adolescence has long been linked to adolescent substance use (Donovan & Jessor, 1985; Donovan, Jessor, & Costa, 1988); yet, in early- and mid-adolescence, many of the same transient factors noted above from Moffitt’s framework are likely to be driving both behaviors, and early- and mid-adolescent criminal behavior may be more likely to be normative than to be an indicator of dysfunction (Moffitt, 1993). Although late adolescent criminal behavior may have more significant implications (D’Amico, Ellickson, Collins, Martino, & Klein, 2005; Merline, Jager, & Schulenberg, 2008), criminal behavior across the bulk of the adolescent era, which is to some degree normative (Moffitt, 2017), appears relatively less likely to reliably mark a long-term risk for substance abuse.

Finally, normative adolescent autonomy struggles may lead in some cases to high levels of parent-adolescent harsh conflict, creating substantial distress—a known driver of substance use (Nash et al., 2005; Ryan, Jorm, & Lubman, 2010). Family conflict has also been related to early initiation of substance use (King & Chassin, 2007). Whether such conflict is a transient vs. enduring predictor is less clear however, with some research suggesting that it is unrelated to late adolescent outcomes when other factors are taken into account (King & Chassin, 2007), while other research suggests a more enduring relationship (Van Ryzin, Fosco, & Dishion, 2012). From a developmental perspective, when levels of exposure to family conflict decline as the adolescent moves into adulthood, we might expect corresponding levels of substance use to decline as well. In contrast, one of the few studies following individuals from early adolescence up through age 27 (Herrenkohl, Lee, Kosterman, & Hawkins, 2012) found that adolescent-era family conflict was a strong predictor of age 27 substance use disorder symptoms, although the study was somewhat limited by reliance upon self-report data at both ages.

Each of the factors above is likely to be salient primarily within the adolescent and early adult era, which may explain why many adolescents ultimately ‘mature out’ of patterns of elevated substance use (Masten et al., 2008; Sher & Gotham, 1999). Yet, a developmental perspective suggests that the import of some or all of these factors is likely to change as development progresses (Zucker, 2008). As social and developmental contingencies change, normal, healthy youth are likely to respond adaptively and reduce levels of deviant behavior (Moffitt, 2017). Indeed, a developmental perspective would suggest a gradual shift from late adolescence into adulthood in both patterns and predictors of use. In the early twenties, levels of use are still likely to be largely socially driven, though perhaps not as much as in early adolescence. The effects of family factors (e.g., family conflict), however, might be expected to fade by this point. In the longer term, a different pattern is likely to emerge. Substance use is unlikely to cease completely later in adulthood, given the degree to which it is socially normative. However, the factors that predict adolescent substance use (and which are often targets of intervention) seem likely to be less useful in predicting non-normative patterns of substance abuse as individuals transition through the early twenties and into adulthood.

In contrast to the transient and developmentally-driven factors noted above, indices of poor underlying overall adaptation within adolescence are likely to be far better predictors of long-term abuse. Substance abuse is distinguished from substance use by the presence not only of uncontrolled levels of use but also of significant deleterious external consequences of use, including legal difficulties, interpersonal problems, career problems, and risky behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Presley, Meilman, & Lyerla, 1994). This study examined two such indicators: poor social skills and poor self-concept. Unlike the more transient factors described above, a lack of underlying social skills, as seen in capacity to assert oneself successfully with peers in a way that still maintains relationships, is known to have long-term import across a variety of functional domains (Allen, Narr, Kansky, & Szwedo, in press; Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth, & Tellegen, 2004). Social skills in this domain are distinct from short-term popularity, which can be driven by factors ranging from physical attractiveness and athletic skill to a tendency toward aggressive behavior, and which does not necessarily predict longer-term adaptation (Narr, Allen, Tan, & Loeb, 2019). Indeed, in the short-term, poor social skills might even buffer teens from substance use by reducing their access to a peer culture supporting this use. In the longer term, however, poor social skills, which often result from difficulties with undercontrol and impulsivity (Zucker, 2008) are likely to lead to a host of risk factors for later substance abuse, from heightened levels of ongoing conflict to a lack of self-efficacy in social situations, both of which have long been linked to substance abuse (Allen, Leadbeater, & Aber, 1990; Herrenkohl et al., 2012).

Similarly, adolescence is a period during which cognitions surrounding identity and a sense of self-esteem consolidate. If this process goes poorly, leaving the adolescent with a negative self-concept, features of adolescent neurological development appear to create a sensitive period that would make such cognitions particularly likely to endure (Dahl & Hariri, 2005; Nelson, Jarcho, & Guyer, 2016). Indeed, longitudinal research has now identified links between poor adolescent self-esteem and a range of adult problems (Trzesniewski et al., 2006). Within adolescence, the links between self-esteem and substance use are mixed, suggesting that adolescents across the full range of levels of self-esteem are at-risk for substance use. For example, low self-esteem may create distress leading to substance use, yet high self-esteem can lead to greater contact with peers and their influences and (over)confidence in taking risks, thus also leading to substance use. This is one potential example of what Zucker (2008) has referred to as the changing developmental saliency of factors related to alcohol and substance abuse.

By early adulthood, the chronic distress created by a poor self-concept has been directly linked to alcohol-related problems (Tomaka, Morales-Monks, & Shamaley, 2013). Further, a poor self-concept appears to limit ability to cope with external stressors (i.e., negative relationships with parents in early adulthood) in a way that has also been linked to alcohol-related problems (Backer-Fulghum, Patock-Peckham, King, Roufa, & Hagen, 2012). Finally, links from a poor self-concept to poor overall mental health also increase the likelihood that low self-esteem will create an enduring risk for substance abuse (D’Amico et al., 2005; Jackson & Sher, 2003). Together, factors such as poor social skills and poor self-concept in adolescence appear likely to reflect enduring developmental deficits that will be more likely to be linked to long-term problems with substance abuse than the transient factors that are viewed as more likely to drive adolescent-era use. To date, however, no research has examined this proposition.

This prospective, multi-method study, utilizing a diverse community sample followed over a 17-year period, assessed a developmental model to distinguish predictors of adolescent substance use that has limited long-term implications from those that predict long-term patterns of substance abuse. The following specific hypotheses were tested:

First, primary predictors of adolescent-era substance use were expected to include peer factors, including those which reflect adaptation to adolescent norms (e.g., popularity and peer substance use), as well as contemporaneous social stressors (e.g., exposure to parent-adolescent conflict). Given the socially-driven nature of adolescent substance use, underlying risk factors that would be likely to lead to lower levels of peer interaction in adolescence (i.e., lack of social skill and poor self-concept) were not hypothesized to be linked to substance use during the adolescent era.

Second, problems associated with substance use by ages 27–30 were expected to be predicted not by transient stressors and peer influence factors in adolescence, but rather by adolescent-era markers of enduring social and emotional difficulty (i.e., poor self-concept and poor social skills) that can nevertheless be identified within adolescence. This pattern was expected to begin emerging by the early twenties (ages 20–22).

Third, given the societally approved nature of alcohol and marijuana use within adulthood, predictions from enduring markers of difficulty are hypothesized to be stronger for problems linked to substance use in adulthood than for simple levels of use.

Methods

Participants

This report is drawn from a larger longitudinal investigation of adolescent social development in familial and peer contexts. Participants included 184 seventh and eighth graders (86 male and 98 female) followed over an 18-year period from ages 13 to 30, along with collateral data collected from close friends, classmates and parents of these adolescents. The sample was racially/ethnically and socioeconomically diverse: 107 adolescents (58%) identified themselves as Caucasian, 53 (29%) as African American, 15 (8%) as of mixed race/ethnicity and 9 (5%) as being from other minority groups. Adolescents’ parents reported a median family income in the $40,000 - $59,999 range at the initial assessment. During this period data were also obtained from participants’ parents and close friends.

Adolescents were initially recruited from the 7th and 8th grades of a public middle school drawing from suburban and urban populations in the Southeastern United States. Students were recruited via an initial mailing to all parents of students in the school along with follow-up contact efforts at school lunches. Families of adolescents who indicated they were interested in the study were contacted by telephone. Of all students eligible for participation, 63% agreed to participate either as either target participants or as peers providing collateral information. All participants provided informed assent before each interview session, and parents provided informed consent. Initial interviews took place in private offices within a university academic building. Follow-up assessments were conducted in the same setting, or for participants’ living at a distance, were conducted either in local settings (e.g., hotel conference rooms), or via mail.

Participants were first assessed annually over a five-year period across adolescence (at ages 13.35 (SD = .64); 14.27 (SD = .77); 15.21 (SD = .81); 16.35 (SD = .87); and 17.32 (SD = .88). At each age, adolescents also nominated their closest friend to be included in the study as well as an additional two peers from within their extended circle of friends and acquaintances. Close friends came in during a visit along with the target adolescent participant. Additional friends came in in separate visits. Friends were close in age to participants (i.e., their ages differed on average by less than a month from target adolescents’ ages). Close friends were specified to be same-gender friends, but the same friend need not be specified across different waves. Close friends reported that they had known the target adolescents on average for periods ranging from a low of 4.01 years (SD = 2.90) at the age 13 assessment to high of 5.91 years (SD = 3.86) at the age 17 assessment.

Data were also obtained at two points from the adolescents’ parents (at adolescent ages 13 and 16).

For the adult follow-up assessments, data were obtained from participants in the early twenties (ages 20.83 (SD=.98), 21.68 (SD=.95, 22.79 (SD=.96)) and in adulthood (ages 26.63 (SD = 1.01); 27.67 (SD = .99); 28.59 (SD = 1.01); and 30.07 (SD = .90)).

Attrition Analyses

Adult follow-up data were obtained from 91.3% of the original sample (N = 168). Attrition analyses comparing those participants with vs. without follow-up data at ages 27–30 revealed no significant differences on any baseline variables, nor on any variables at ages 20–22, with the exception of participant gender (2% attrition among females vs. 16% among males, p < .001).

To best address any potential biases due to attrition in longitudinal analyses, full information maximum likelihood (FIML) methods were used with analyses including all variables that were linked to future missing data (i.e., where data were not missing completely at random). Because these procedures have been found to yield the least biased estimates when all available data are used for longitudinal analyses (vs. listwise deletion of missing data) (Arbuckle, 1996), the entire original sample of 184 was utilized for these analyses. This full sample thus provides the best possible estimates of variances and covariances in measures of interest and was least likely to be biased by missing data.

Procedure

In the initial introduction and throughout all sessions, confidentiality was assured to all study participants and adolescents were told that their parents and friends would not be informed of any of the answers they provided. Participants’ data were protected by a Confidentiality Certificate issued by the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, which protected information from subpoena by federal, state, and local courts. Transportation and childcare were provided if necessary. Adolescent/adult participants, their parents, and their peers were all paid for participation.

Measures

Popularity (Age 13). Adolescents’ capacity to establish themselves as preferred social companions with a range of their peers was assessed using a limited nomination procedure. Each adolescent, their closest friend, and two other target peers were asked to nominate up to 10 peers in their grade with whom they would “most like to spend time on a Saturday night.” The raw number of ‘like’ nominations each teen received was standardized within grade level as a measure of desirability as a social companion in the broader peer group following the procedure described in Coie et al (1982). This approach to assessing social acceptance has been previously found to be relatively stable over time and related to adolescent attachment security, qualities of positive parental and peer interactions, and short-term changes in levels of deviant behavior (Allen et al., 2005; Allen, Porter, McFarland, McElhaney, & Marsh, 2007; McElhaney, Antonishak, & Allen, 2008).

Participant and Close Peer Alcohol and Marijuana Use Frequency (Ages 13–17) and Participant Alcohol and Marijuana Use Amount (Ages 20–22 and 27–30) were assessed with the alcohol and drug use questionnaire from the Monitoring the Future surveys (Johnston, O’Malley, & Bachman, 1987). This self-report measure was separately administered to participants and to their closest friend each year during adolescence and included items assessing the frequency of participant use of alcohol and/or marijuana in the past 30 days (1 = no use, 2 = 1–2 times, 3 = 3–5 times, 4 = 6–9 times, 5 = more than 10 times). Johnston and colleagues (1987) found high reliability from year to year and consistency between related measures within the same questionnaire administration. Scores for alcohol and marijuana use were averaged together each year. Data from each year from ages 13–17 was then summed and averaged to produce a measure frequency of alcohol and marijuana use across adolescence. In the early twenties and in adulthood the same approach was used, but for alcohol use, a second item asked how many drinks on average participants had on each occasion of drinking. This was combined with the measure of number of days drinking to yield a measure of total amount (i.e., frequency and volume) of alcohol use. This measure and the ‘times per 30 days’ measure for marijuana use were both standardized and summed to yield a measure for amount of adult alcohol and marijuana use.

Problems Due to Alcohol and Substance Use (Ages 27–30) were assessed with the Core Alcohol and Drug Survey (Presley et al., 1994). This survey asks respondents to note whether they’ve experienced any of 20 different problems due to drinking or drug use during the past year, ranging from having a hangover to being hurt or injured, to being arrested for driving under the influence. A total problems score is created as the sum of responses to these 20 dichotomous items. It has previously been administered nationally to more than 50,000 college-age students each year (Presley et al., 1994). The number of problems reported in each year from age 27–30 was summed and averaged to produce a total problems score, and this summation was itself quite reliable (Cronbach’s α = .85).

Age of First Alcohol Use

Beginning with the age 13 assessment participants who acknowledged having drunk alcohol (outside of family events) were asked the age at which they first drank. For participants who had not begun drinking at age 13, this question was repeated at each succeeding age up through age 18. Participants who had not begun drinking by age 18 were assigned the maximum possible score (age 18).

Participant Criminal Behavior (Ages 13–17) was measured as the total number of times youths reported engaging in each of 37 non-overlapping classes of illegal behavior (designed to assess all significant classes of criminal behavior, except for drug use) during the previous six months, using the Self Report of Delinquency measure (Elliott, Huizinga, & Menard, 1989; Huizinga & Elliott, 1986). When obtained by sensitive interviewers who have first established rapport with interviewees, self-reports of problem behaviors have long been found: a) to correlate significantly with reports obtained from independent observers and official records; b) to be adequately reliable; and c) to eliminate systemic biases present in official records of deviant behavior (Elliott et al., 1989; Huizinga & Elliott, 1986). Cronbach’s α’s across years ranged from .66 to .91 (M = .77).

Perceived Self-worth (Ages 13–17)

Participants completed the four-item global self-worth scale from the Adolescent Self-Perception Profile each year from 13 to 17 (Harter, 1988). Scores for ages 13–17 were averaged to yield a measure of self-worth across adolescence and this sum displayed good reliability (Cronbach’s α = .77).

Parent-adolescent Conflict (Ages 13, 16)

Use of harsh conflict tactics in parent-adolescent interactions were reported by each parent using the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS; Straus, 1979). This study used the 6-item verbal aggression scale (e.g., insults, threats, etc.) and 11-item physical aggression scale (e.g., slap, push, etc.) of the Conflict Tactics Scale as indicators of harsh conflict. Each parent reported on both their own behavior toward the adolescent and on their adolescents’ behavior toward them. At the first assessment parents were asked to report how often each behavior had occurred over their child’s lifetime. The second assessment covered behavior across the past year. A 4-point scale (1 = never, 2 = once or twice, 3 = several times, 4 = many times) was used for each item. Scores were summed across the two scales for each parent and then averaged across all of these assessments (reliability for the final measure combining these scales across the two time points and two raters was good (Cronbach’s α = .85).

Social Skills (Ages 13–17)

The test-based Adolescent Problem Inventory (API) (Freedman, Rosenthal, Donahoe, Schlundt, & McFall, 1978; Gaffney & McFall, 1981) was used to assess adolescents’ social problem-solving skill. Adolescents were asked to describe their most likely response to a series of five problematic hypothetical situations. These responses were then rated by coders who were unaware of the hypotheses of the study using a 0 to 10 scale, rating competence in resolving the situation at hand in a way that also makes future problematic situations less likely. Situations included conflicts with peers, romantic partners, and teachers, and situations in which adolescents might be tempted to engage in delinquent behaviors. For example, one situation asks how the participant might deal with a situation in which a gym teacher unfairly calls the student out and tells him/her to do extra push-ups in front of the class. Another asks how the participant might deal with being harshly confronted by a parent about clothing choices just before heading out to a party. This measure was obtained repeatedly at each of the first 5 waves of data collection using different prompt situations at each wave. Interrater reliabilities calculated using the intraclass correlation coefficient, ranged from r = .86 − .96, all of which are considered in the “excellent” range for this statistic (Cicchetti & Sparrow, 1981). The mean of scores across these five waves was used in analyses and this mean demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .75).

Analytic Plan

Initial analyses examined the role of gender and early adolescent family income on the primary measures examined in the study. Several variables of substantive interest in the study were related to both adolescent gender and income in the adolescent’s family of origin, hence these factors were considered as covariates in analyses below. No moderating effects of gender and income on any of the primary analyses below were found.

Primary analyses were conducted via hierarchical regressions using full information maximum likelihood handling of missing data. Socio-demographic factors (gender and baseline family income) were entered first, followed by adolescent age at first alcohol use, followed by hypothesized transient adolescent-era risks (adolescent deviant behavior, peer factors, family conflict), followed by hypothesized developmental risk factors likely to have enduring effects (poor social skills and self-concept). All analyses were performed using SAS Proc CALIS, Version 9.4 (Sas Institute, 2015). Tabled primary results include both simple correlations with the outcome of interest, for descriptive purposes, as well as results of regression analyses.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Means and standard deviations for all substantive variables and intercorrelations among predictor variables are presented in Tables 1 and 2 respectively. Means reflect growth in usage primarily from adolescence to the early twenties, with the average adolescent reporting use of alcohol or marijuana between zero and 1–2 times in the past month across all of adolescence. By the early twenties, participants reported drinking an average of 7 drinks per week and using marijuana between 0 and 1–2 times per week. In adulthood participants reported drinking an average of 6 drinks per week and using marijuana between 0 and 1–2 times per week.

Table 1.

Means and Standard Deviations of Primary Measures and Demographic Variables

Mean s.d.
Adolescent Alcohol & Marijuana Use Frequency (Ages 13–17) 1.6 0.98
Age of First U of Alcohol 15.1 2.4
Problems with Substance Use (Ages 20–22) 2.93 2.93
Use of Alcohol & Marijuana (standardized, Ages 20–22) 3.92 3.57
Adult Problems with Substance Use (Ages 27–30) 2.17 2.36
Adult Use of Alcohol & Marijuana (Ages 27–30) 4.06 3.83
Parent-adolescent conflict (Ages 13, 16) 0.4 0.74
Popularity with Peers (Age 13) 0.96 1.35
Adol. Criminal Behavior (Ages 13–17) 18.3 3.13
Close Peer Substance Use (Ages 13–17) 0.70 1.10
Social Skills (Ages 13–17) −0.03 0.75
Self-concept (Ages 13–17) 13.3 1.9
N / % N / %
Adolescent Gender Males: 86 / 46.7% Females: 98 / 53.3%
Adolescent Racial/Ethnic Minority Status Minority: 71 / 38.6% Non-Minority: 113 / 61.4%

Note: Untransformed mean is reported for adolescent use of alcohol and marijuana

Table 2.

Intercorrelations Among Predictor Variables

2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
1. Age of first use −13 −07 −41*** −21** 28*** 20** 14 −06
2. Family conflict -- −25*** 22** 06 −15* 05 −08 −14
3. Popularity with Peers -- −08 28 13 −03 04 32***
4. Adol. Criminal Behavior -- 17* −36*** −16* −20** −06
5. Close Peer Use -- −16* −10 −24*** −20**
6. Social Skills -- 23** 31*** −03
7. Self-concept -- 08 −04
8. Adolescent Gender (1-M;2-F) -- −11
9. Family Income --
***

p < .001.

**

p < .01.

*

p < .05.

Primary Analyses

Hypothesis 1: Predictors of adolescent-era substance use will include peer factors, including those which reflect adaptation to adolescent norms, as well as contemporaneous social stressors. Lack of social skill is not expected to be a predictor.

As hypothesized, an array of adolescent-era factors was linked to adolescent substance use, including peers’ reports of their own use levels, adolescents’ greater popularity with peers, high levels of parent-adolescent conflict, and adolescent criminal behavior. Notably, each of these factors contributed unique variance to explaining total levels of use across adolescence (see Table 3). Age at first alcohol use was correlated with total levels of use across adolescence, but did not add to prediction in final models. Of the two developmental risks with potentially enduring effects, a lower level of social problem-solving skills was also predictive of higher frequency of use over and above all of the other factors examined, though self-worth was not. Effects of gender and family income observed in simple correlations became non-significant in regression models that also accounted for psychosocial factors.

Table 3.

Adolescent Alcohol and Marijuana Use Frequency (Ages 13–17)

Adolescent Alcohol and Marijuana Use Frequency (Ages 13–17)
r β ΔR2 R2
Step I. Demographic Factors
Gender (1-M; 2=F) −.18* .03
Total Family Income (13) .20** .07
Statistics for Step .068** .068**
Step II. Baseline Assessments
Age of first use −.31*** −.04
Statistics for Step .106*** .174
Step III. Adolescent-era Risks
Family conflict (13, 16) .15* .12*
Popularity with Peers (13) .35*** .32***
Adol. Criminal Behavior (13–17) .42*** .30***
Close Peer Use Frequency (13–17) .50*** .31***
Statistics for Step .263*** .437
Step IV. Developmental Risks
Social Skills (13–17) −.17* −.20***
Self-concept (13–17) −.10 .04
Statistics for Step .055** .492

Note.

***

p < .001.

**

p < .01.

*

p < .05.

β weights are from final model. Age of assessments are included in parentheses.

Hypothesis 2: Problems associated with substance use by ages 27–30 were expected to be predicted not by transient stressors and peer-influence factors in adolescence, but by adolescent-era markers of potentially enduring social and emotional difficulty (i.e., poor self-concept and poor social skills). This pattern was expected to begin emerging by the early twenties.

For these analyses, we considered all of the same predictors examined previously, but we also added in the baseline measure of adolescent alcohol and marijuana use. As shown in Table 4 and as hypothesized, a quite different picture from the adolescent-era predictions described above emerges in predictions to early twenties and adult substance use problems. Continuity from adolescent levels of use was observed as expected. However, in predictions to early twenties (ages 20–22) substance use problems, the block of adolescent-era risk factors was no longer significant and social skills became a highly significant inverse predictor.

Table 4.

Early Twenties (Ages 20–22) and Adult (Ages 27–30) Substance Use Problems

Early Twenties Substance Use Problems (Ages 20–22) Adult Substance Use Problems (Ages 27–30)
r β ΔR2 R2 r β ΔR2 R2
Step I. Demographic Factors
Gender (1-M; 2=F) −.19* −.02 .09 .03
Total Family Income (13) .23** .14* .12 .04
Statistics for Step .080*** .080*** .022 .022
Step II. Baseline Assessments
Age of first use −.24** −.01 −.22** −.03
Total Frequency of Substance Use (13–17) .44*** .25** .35*** .26**
Statistics for Step .140*** .220*** .126*** .148***
Step III. Adolescent-era Risks
Family conflict (13,16) .06 −.00 .09 −.17*
Popularity with Peers (13) .16* .09 .11 .02
Adol. Criminal Behavior (13–17) .22** −.01 .20* −.00
Close Peer Use (13–17) .21** −.04 .14 −.05
Statistics for Step .003 .223*** .002 .150***
Step IV. Developmental Risks
Social Skills (13–17) −.48*** −.39*** −.28*** −.32***
Self-concept (13–17) −.17* −.04 −.25** −.13*
Statistics for Step .110*** .333*** .125*** .275***

Note.

***

p < .001.

**

p < .01.

*

p < .05.

β weights are from final model.

In predictions to adult (ages 27–30) substance use problems, after accounting for continuity from adolescent use levels, none of the adolescent peer, family, or behavioral factors predicted adult problems, with the exception of the prior presence of high levels of parent-adolescent conflict, which predicted relatively lower levels of substance use problems at ages 27–30. This latter finding indicates that adolescents whose adolescent-era use was linked to (presumably transient) conflict, go on to have relatively fewer problems as adults than might have been expected from solely observing their levels of baseline substance use.

In contrast to the decreasing role of these family, peer, and behavioral factors, the two adolescent factors that were believed to reflect enduring developmental risks—poor social skills and low self-esteem—became strongly predictive of adult-era problems. Together they added 12.5% to the explained variance in adult problems over and above baseline levels of use, and adolescent-era factors.

To assess whether the observed differences in prediction between adolescent and later assessments was significant, nested models were tested in which predictions were made simultaneously to both adolescent use and later substance use problems (separate models were examined for both early twenties problems and for adult problems). Estimated effects of predictors to the different time periods were first allowed to vary across time period (as in the models above), and then were fixed to be equal. The resulting difference between the two nested models is a test of whether predictions are significantly different. For each of the two models comparing effects of adolescent risk factors on early twenties problems or on adult problems, results indicated no significant difference in the magnitude of the estimates for the adolescent risk factors considered as a block (although predictions from parent-adolescent conflict did differ from adolescence to adulthood when examined individually (χ2(1) = 8.2, p = .004)). For both of the two models comparing effects of the developmental risk factors (to early twenties problems and to adult problems), significant differences from predictions to adolescent-era use were detected (χ2(2) = 27.9, p < .001; and χ2(2) = 19.8, p < .001, respectively).

Hypothesis 3: Predictions from markers of potentially enduring difficulty are hypothesized to be stronger when made to adult problems linked to substance use than when made to simple adult levels of use.

Predictions to overall frequency and amount of adult use of alcohol and marijuana displayed a somewhat mixed pattern of results, as hypothesized and as shown in Table 5. As with problems related to use, continuity from adolescent-era levels of use was observed in both the early twenties and adult eras. Only social skills emerged as a significant predictor of usage in the early twenties. In adulthood, the block of social skills and self-concept was significant, though the individual predictors within that block were not. To explore whether this reflected the effects of the covariation of these two factors, each of these two predictors was examined separately, in isolation, as the last step in the model in Table 5. These analyses (not shown) indicated that the covariation of the two was somewhat obscuring their effects, in that poor social skills was a significant predictor when examined in the absence of self-concept (β =.16, p = .04), although the effect of self-concept was found only at a non-significant trend level (β = −.11 p = .09). In zero-order correlations at both eras, adolescent peer factors, both greater popularity with peers and greater peer use predicted adult use, suggesting that these social factors were linked to later substance use, just not to later problems with use. In these correlations, use at ages 20–22 was also predicted by adolescent gender (greater use among males) and family income. Use at ages 27–30 was also predicted by adolescent gender.

Table 5.

Early Twenties (Ages 20–22) and Adult (Ages 27–30) Substance Use Levels

Early Twenties Substance Use Levels (Ages 20–22) Adult Substance Use Levels (Ages 27–30)
r β ΔR2 R2 r β ΔR2 R2
Step I. Demographic Factors
Gender (1-M; 2=F) −.26*** −.13* −.29*** −.22**
Total Family Income (13) .29*** .13* .136*** .136*** .20* .06
Statistics for Step .114*** .114***
Step II. Baseline Assessments
Age of first use −.26*** −.10 −.24** −.07
Total Frequency of Substance Use (13–17) .48*** .37*** .48*** .44***
Statistics for Step .159*** .295*** .101*** .286***
Step III. Adolescent-era Risks
Family conflict (13,16) −.06 −.11 −.00 −.05
Popularity with Peers (13) .24** .04 .19* .04
Adol. Criminal Behavior (13–17) .07 −.22** .14 −.14
Close Peer Use (13–17) .31*** .03 .20** −.11
Statistics for Step .042* .337*** .010 .304***
Step IV. Developmental Risks
Social Skills (13–17) −.38*** −.26*** −.31*** −.14
Self-concept (13–17) −.16* −.05 −.17* −.09
Statistics for Step .075*** .391*** .027* .331***

Note.

***

p < .001.

**

p < .01.

*

p < .05.

β weights are from final model.

To assess whether the observed differences in prediction to adolescent substance use vs. later substance use as seen in Tables 3 and 5 were significant, nested models were tested in which predictions were made simultaneously to both adolescent and adult use, similar to those described regarding Hypothesis 2. With regard to differences in predictions to adolescent use vs. early adult (ages 20–22) use, results indicated that the prediction estimates for adolescent family conflict and criminal behavior differed depending upon whether adolescent substance use or early adult substance use were the dependent variable (χ2(1) = 9.79, p = .002; and χ2(1) = 10.0, p = .002). With regard to differences in predictions to adolescent use vs. later adult use (ages 27–30), results indicated that the prediction estimates for adolescent criminal behavior and peer substance use in adolescence differed depending upon whether adolescent substance use or early adult substance use were the dependent variable (χ2(1) = 6.0, p = .014; and χ2(1) = 19.0, p < .001).

Post-hoc Analyses

Models with Adolescent Use Not Covaried

To fully explore issues of continuity and discontinuity, we examined the possibility that including levels of adolescent substance use in multivariate models predicting adult problems was potentially obscuring the effects of adolescent-era predictors (i.e., if those effects were mediated via use that began in adolescence). To examine this, we assessed whether transient predictors of substance use in adolescence were related to future substance abuse problems in adulthood in multivariate models identical to those in Table 4, but without covarying adolescent-levels of use. No relation was observed for any of the transient predictors (i.e., popularity, close friend use, criminal behavior, or family conflict; not tabled). This means that covarying adolescent substance use in the models above was not obscuring long-term effects of these adolescent-era predictors and that the adolescent-era factors did not have later effects that were mediated via adolescent levels of use.

Substance-specific Processes

We also explored whether the primary results above would differ substantially if we examined alcohol and marijuana use separately. We focused on adulthood as our primary outcome period for these post-hoc analyses. Only very minor differences were found in these analyses. Comparing the above model predicting adolescent substance use to models just predicting adolescent alcohol use, all four adolescent-era predictors remained significant; in addition, social skill was now also a significant predictor, whereas it was previously linked in zero-order correlations and part of a significant block of predictors but not significant in final regression models. Comparing the above model predicting adolescent substance use to models just predicting adolescent marijuana use, all predictors remained significant except for age of first alcohol use and family conflict which dropped out as predictors. When predicting adult substance use problems from either adolescent alcohol use or adolescent marijuana use, all predictions from the models presented above remained either significant or non-significant consistent with their status above, except for self-worth, which was predictive at the trend level (p = .06) in models predicting adult problems from adolescent alcohol use.

Discussion

Findings of this study supported the overarching hypothesis that factors that predict alcohol and marijuana use within adolescence can be distinguished from factors that predict the presence of longer-term problems with substance use beyond the adolescent/early-adult transition. As hypothesized, factors that were peer-oriented and that were likely to exist primarily within the adolescent/early adult period were most closely associated with adolescent-era substance use, whereas adolescent-era markers of difficulties in underlying social-emotional adaptation were the primary predictors of long-term substance abuse problems.

Factors that were linked to substance use across adolescence were, as expected, largely consistent with those found in prior research on the social and emotional correlates of such use, and included age of first use, greater popularity with peers, close peer level of substance use, high levels of parent-adolescent conflict and adolescent deviance proneness as reflected in criminal behavior. Perhaps most striking, however, was the finding that, after accounting for adolescents’ overall level of substance use, these risk factors were of relatively little utility in explaining adult substance use. This was true regardless of whether the outcome considered was adult levels of substance use, or adult problems with use. It was also true regardless of whether baseline levels of substance use were entered into models, making it unlikely that predictions were being obscured by including this covariate.

In contrast to the apparently more transient effects of these adolescennt environmental factors, two markers of underlying psychosocial adaptation in adolescence—poor social skills and poor self-concept—were predictive of longer-term problems with substance use, with predictions spanning as much as 17-years and appearing even after accounting for predictions from robust measures of substance use obtained across a broad span of adolescence. These findings are fully consistent with findings in adult substance abuse research suggesting that the distress created by poor self-concept as well as levels of adult conflict that likely result from poor social skills can be significant drivers of substance abuse (Frone, 1999; Jackson & Sher, 2003). This study suggests these factors can be identified in adolescence in a way that potentially allows us to distinguish them from transient factors that primarily explain adolescent and early adult use.

Examination of multivariate predictions to the intervening early twenties period (ages 20–22) revealed an almost identical pattern of results to predictions to the later adult period. The only exception being that self-concept only emerged as a significant predictor of fewer substance use problems by adulthood. When examining univariate correlations, unsurprisingly, several of the adolescent-era risk factors were linked to problems in the early twenties but not adulthood, suggesting that the impact of these predictors fades across this period. Taken together, these findings are consistent with the idea that a developmental transition is occurring relatively rapidly across this period: For most youth, the early twenties are indeed a waypoint between adolescence and adulthood in terms of substance use, but in terms of patterns of predictions from adolescence, this period more closely resembles later adulthood than it does the earlier adolescent era.

Taken together, these findings suggest that, similar to prior research on criminal behavior (Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt, 2017), there appears to be a theoretical basis for distinguishing predictors of adolescent substance use from predictors of adult problems with substance use. With criminal behavior, however, it has thus far not been possible to distinguish among these two phenomena by looking solely within adolescence—it has only been by looking to whether behavior began prior to adolescence that distinguishing who is at risk for longer term criminal behavior has been possible (Moffitt, 2017). The results of this study are thus particularly promising in this regard in showing fairly distinct sets of predictors linked to each phenomenon that can be observed within adolescence.

Although not predicted a priori, findings regarding parent-adolescent conflict were particularly striking in this regard. Parent-adolescent conflict, as expected, predicted higher levels of substance use in adolescence. After accounting for this higher baseline use, however, parent-adolescent conflict predicted relatively lower levels of substance abuse problems by the late twenties. One explanation is that conflict within the family of origin seems likely to be the risk factor easiest for adolescents to ultimately escape. To be clear, the finding is not that conflict predicted lower absolute levels of substance abuse problems, but levels that were lower relative to what would be predicted from adolescent levels of use. This is important because without recognizing that this is a prediction of substance abuse that fully accounted for adolescent-era use, these findings appear to contradict prior research findings that parent-adolescent conflict predicted substance abuse to age 27 (Herrenkohl et al., 2012). This prior research stopped at a slightly younger age than the present study (which may be relevant given the extent to which the twenties are a period during which individuals often move beyond family of origin conflicts (Arnett, 2001)), used adolescent self-reports of conflict, and did not focus on harsh conflict as this study did. Perhaps the biggest difference between the two studies, however, is that the prior study only covaried a measure of substance use obtained at an early age (13 to 14) when a truly robust covariate might not yet exist. Further research is clearly needed with respect to the role of conflict, but findings point in the direction of suggesting the need to be highly sensitive to developmental issues in assessing both conflict and substance use.

Taken together, these findings highlight the unique social and developmental nature of substance use across this span. Several of the factors that predict adolescent use, such as popularity among peers and peers’ level of use, likely reflect natural socialization processes. Indeed, Moffitt has argued that these socialization processes may even reflect adaptive socialization, even though some of the results of these socialization processes carry significant potential for real harm.

When examining total levels of adult alcohol and marijuana use (as opposed to problems associated with that use), a slightly different pattern was observed. Continuity was also observed with adolescent-era use and lower levels of adolescent social skills, though self-concept was unrelated to adult levels of substance use. Of course, in adulthood, some level of substance use is relatively normal and indeed encouraged via many social rituals. Hence, the distinction between use and abuse appears critical to keep in mind. Relatedly, both male gender and higher baseline family income were positively correlated with adolescent use and found to be predictive of levels of adult use, with males and individuals from higher income families exhibiting higher usage levels. These demographic factors were not predictive of substance use problems in adulthood, however.

Several limitations to this study also warrant note. First, because this study was non-experimental in nature, causal conclusions cannot be drawn regarding predictors of substance use problems. Specifically, it was entirely possible that unmeasured ‘third variables’ drove some of the relationships observed. For example, extensive work has been done on undercontrol/disinhibition as a predictor of substance problems (Zucker, Heitzeg, & Nigg, 2011), and, though not measured in this study, undercontrol may have partly driven the lack of social skills we observed. Also, this study specifically focused on just one set of potential long-term predictors, social skills and self-concept, considered likely to be linked to substance use and abuse—yet a wide range of other factors from genetic propensities to internalizing symptoms to broader sociocultural factors would all be important for future research to consider. These factors might also differentially predict to alcohol use vs. marijuana use in ways that the current predictors did not. The current findings should thus not be interpreted as identifying the key predictors of long-term use, but rather as indicating that it is possible to distinguish predictors of adolescent use from predictors of long-term problems with abuse and as pointing toward some factors likely to aid in making such distinctions. Similarly, this study found that transient factors, such as concurrent peer use, in adolescence were not predictive of long-term outcomes. This does not mean, however, that these factors would be unimportant if examined concurrently in adulthood, but only that they are not predictive from adolescence into adulthood. Even this conclusion must be qualified by the possibility that predictions were simply not large enough to be detectable given the modest statistical power of the study.

It should further be noted that a failure to reject the null hypothesis regarding effects (i.e., from transient adolescent predictors to adult substance use problems) does not establish the null hypothesis as true. We can, however, at least say that with regard to the developmental risk factors, these were significantly stronger as predictors to adult problems than they were to adolescent substance use levels. Beyond that, regarding the transient adolescent predictors, we can say that they were not useful in understanding adult substance use problems in this study.

This study also focused on a community sample of adolescents, rather than a sample pre-identified to be at high risk of substance abuse; hence results cannot be generalized to riskier populations. This also limited consideration of variables, such as use of harder drugs which occurred at such low rates as to make their inclusion statistically problematic within this sample. Nonetheless, it is precisely within more normative samples that making the distinction between adolescence-limited and life-course persistent issues with substance use and abuse is likely to be most relevant.

Finally, and perhaps most importantly, nothing about these results should in any way be taken as suggesting that substance use in adolescence is not a significant cause for concern. From its links to accidental death, violence, and other forms of criminal behavior, to its potential to start biologically vulnerable teens on a path toward lifelong problems with addiction, the reasons for concern are many and self-evident. In terms of longer-term prevention efforts, however, the present findings suggest potential value in particularly targeting risk factors that are most likely to be enduring and to move with individuals as they traverse social-environmental contexts. Although adolescent substance use may remain highly problematic, to the extent that the factors that predict it are not the same as those that predict longer-term difficulties, identifying those will be critical to longer-term prevention efforts. Indeed, given the similarities in developmental course between substance use and other problematic behaviors of adolescence (e.g., criminal behavior, risk-taking behavior, etc.) future research might do well to consider whether a developmental model as suggested in this study could apply more broadly to those problems as well.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the National Institute of Mental Health (5R37HD058305-23, R01 HD058305-16A1, R01-MH58066).

References

  1. Allen JP, Leadbeater BJ, & Aber JL (1990). The relationship of adolescents’ expectations and values to delinquency, hard drug use, and unprotected sexual intercourse. Development & Psychopathology, 2(1), 85–98. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen JP, Narr RK, Kansky J, & Szwedo DE (in press). Adolescent Peer Relationship Qualities as Predictors of Long-term Romantic Life Satisfaction. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen JP, Porter MR, McFarland CF, Marsh PA, & McElhaney KB (2005). The two faces of adolescents’ success with peers: Adolescent popularity, social adaptation, and deviant behavior. Child Development, 76, 747–760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Allen JP, Porter MR, McFarland FC, McElhaney KB, & Marsh PA (2007). The relation of attachment security to adolescents’ paternal and peer relationships, depression, and externalizing behavior. Child Development, 78(4), 1222–1239. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01062.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  6. Arbuckle JL (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In Schumaker GAMRE (Ed.), Advanced structural modeling: Issues and Techniques (pp. 243–277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  7. Arnett JJ (2001). Conceptions of the transition to adulthood: Perspectives from adolescence through midlife. Journal of Adult Development, 8(2), 133. [Google Scholar]
  8. Backer-Fulghum LM, Patock-Peckham JA, King KM, Roufa L, & Hagen L (2012). The stress-response dampening hypothesis: How self-esteem and stress act as mechanisms between negative parental bonds and alcohol-related problems in emerging adulthood. Addictive Behaviors, 37(4), 477–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brook DW, Brook JS, Zhang C, Cohen P, & Whiteman M (2002). Drug use and the risk of major depressive disorder, alcohol dependence, and substance use disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(11), 1039–1044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cicchetti DV, & Sparrow SA (1981). Developing criteria for establishing interrater reliability of specific items: Applications to assessment of adaptive behavior. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86(2), 127–137. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7315877 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Coie JD, Dodge KA, & Coppotelli H (1982). Dimensions and types of social status: A cross age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 121–132. [Google Scholar]
  12. D’Amico EJ, Ellickson PL, Collins RL, Martino S, & Klein DJ (2005). Processes linking adolescent problems to substance-use problems in late young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66(6), 766–775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dahl RE, & Hariri AR (2005). Lessons from G. Stanley Hall: Connecting New Research in Biological Sciences to the Study of Adolescent Development. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15(4), 367–382. doi:doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00102.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dijkstra JK, Cillessen AHN, Lindenberg S, & Veenstra R (2010). Basking in Reflected Glory and Its Limits: Why Adolescents Hang Out With Popular Peers. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(4), 942–958. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00671.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Donovan JE, & Jessor R (1985). Structure of problem behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 890–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Donovan JE, Jessor R, & Costa FM (1988). Syndrome of problem behavior in adolescence: A replication. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 762–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Elliott DS, Huizinga D, & Menard S (1989). Multiple problem youth: Delinquency, substance use, and mental health problems. New York: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  18. Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Hussong A, Faris R, Foshee VA, Cai L, & DuRant RH (2006). The Peer Context of Adolescent Substance Use: Findings from Social Network Analysis. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16(2), 159–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00127.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Freedman BJ, Rosenthal L, Donahoe CP, Schlundt DG, & McFall RM (1978). A social-behavioral analysis of skill deficits in delinquent and nondelinquent adolescent boys. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 46(6), 1448–1462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frone MR (1999). Work stress and alcohol use. Alcohol research and health, 23(4), 284–291. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gaffney LR, & McFall RM (1981). A comparison of social skills in delinquent and nondelinquent adolescent girls using a behavioral role-playing inventory. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 49(6), 959–967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Harter S (1988). Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for adolescents. Denver, Colorado: University of Denver. [Google Scholar]
  23. Herrenkohl TI, Lee JO, Kosterman R, & Hawkins JD (2012). Family influences related to adult substance use and mental health problems: a developmental analysis of child and adolescent predictors. The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 51(2), 129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Huizinga D, & Elliott DS (1986). Reassessing the reliability and validity of self-report delinquency measures. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 2, 293–327. [Google Scholar]
  25. Jackson KM, & Sher KJ (2003). Alcohol use disorders and psychological distress: a prospective state-trait analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, & Bachman JG (1987). Psychotherapeutic, licit, and illicit use of drugs among adolescents: An epidemiological perspective. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 8(1), 36–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. King KM, & Chassin L (2007). A prospective study of the effects of age of initiation of alcohol and drug use on young adult substance dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 68(2), 256–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Li F, Barrera M, Hops H, & Fisher KJ (2002). The longitudinal influence of peers on the development of alcohol use in late adolescence: A growth mixture analysis. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25(3), 293–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Masten AS, Faden VB, Zucker RA, & Spear LP (2008). Underage drinking: a developmental framework. Pediatrics, 121 Suppl 4, S235–251. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mayeux L, Sandstrom MJ, & Cillessen AH (2008). Is being popular a risky proposition? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18(1), 49–74. [Google Scholar]
  31. McElhaney KB, Antonishak J, & Allen JP (2008). They like me, they like me not: Popularity and adolescents’ perceptions of acceptance predicting changing social functioning over time. . Child Development, 79, 720–731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Merline A, Jager J, & Schulenberg JE (2008). Adolescent risk factors for adult alcohol use and abuse: stability and change of predictive value across early and middle adulthood. Addiction, 103 Suppl 1, 84–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02178.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Moffitt TE (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100(4), 674–701. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Moffitt TE (2017). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. In Biosocial Theories of Crime (pp. 69–96): Routledge. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Narr RK, Allen JP, Tan JS, & Loeb EL (2019). Close Friendship Strength and Broader Peer Group Desirability as Differential Predictors of Adult Mental Health. Child Development, 90(1), 298–313. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12905 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nash SG, McQueen A, & Bray JH (2005). Pathways to adolescent alcohol use: Family environment, peer influence, and parental expectations. Journal of Adolescent Health, 37(1), 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nelson EE, Jarcho JM, & Guyer AE (2016). Social re-orientation and brain development: An expanded and updated view. Dev Cogn Neurosci, 17, 118–127. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nelson SE, Van Ryzin MJ, & Dishion TJ (2014). Alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco use trajectories from age 12 to 24 years: Demographic correlates and young adult substance use problems. Development and Psychopathology, 27(01), 253–277. doi: 10.1017/s0954579414000650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Patrick ME, & Schulenberg JE (2010). Alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking prevalence and predictors among national samples of American eighth-and tenth-grade students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71(1), 41–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Patrick ME, & Schulenberg JE (2014). Prevalence and predictors of adolescent alcohol use and binge drinking in the United States. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews, 35(2), 193. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Presley CA, Meilman PW, & Lyerla R (1994). Development of the Core Alcohol and Drug Survey: Initial findings and future directions. Journal of American College Health, 42, 248–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Roisman GI, Masten AS, Coatsworth JD, & Tellegen A (2004). Salient and emerging developmental tasks in the transition to adulthood. Child Development, 75(1), 123–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ryan SM, Jorm AF, & Lubman DI (2010). Parenting factors associated with reduced adolescent alcohol use: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44(9), 774–783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sas Institute. (2015). SAS, Version 9.4. Cary, NC: Author. [Google Scholar]
  45. Schulenberg JE, & Maggs JL (2002). A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Suppl14, 54–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sher KJ, & Gotham HJ (1999). Pathological alcohol involvement: A developmental disorder of young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 11(4), 933–956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tomaka J, Morales-Monks S, & Shamaley AG (2013). Stress and coping mediate relationships between contingent and global self-esteem and alcohol-related problems among college drinkers. Stress and Health, 29(3), 205–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan MB, Moffitt TE, Robins RW, Poulton R, & Caspi A (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 42(2), 381–390. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Van Ryzin MJ, Fosco GM, & Dishion TJ (2012). Family and peer predictors of substance use from early adolescence to early adulthood: An 11-year prospective analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 37(12), 1314–1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Zucker RA (2008). Anticipating problem alcohol use developmentally from childhood into middle adulthood: what have we learned? Addiction, 103, 100–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Zucker RA, Heitzeg MM, & Nigg JT (2011). Parsing the undercontrol–disinhibition pathway to substance use disorders: A multilevel developmental problem. Child Development Perspectives, 5(4), 248–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES