Abstract
Mindfulness training is associated with improvements in psychological wellbeing and cognition, yet the specific underlying neurophysiological mechanisms underpinning these changes are uncertain. This study uses a novel brain-inspired artificial neural network to investigate the effect of mindfulness training on electroencephalographic function. Participants completed a 4-tone auditory oddball task (that included targets and physically similar distractors) at three assessment time points. In Group A (n = 10), these tasks were given immediately prior to 6-week mindfulness training, immediately after training and at a 3-week follow-up; in Group B (n = 10), these were during an intervention waitlist period (3 weeks prior to training), pre-mindfulness training and post-mindfulness training. Using a spiking neural network (SNN) model, we evaluated concurrent neural patterns generated across space and time from features of electroencephalographic data capturing the neural dynamics associated with the event-related potential (ERP). This technique capitalises on the temporal dynamics of the shifts in polarity throughout the ERP and spatially across electrodes. Findings support anteriorisation of connection weights in response to distractors relative to target stimuli. Right frontal connection weights to distractors were associated with trait mindfulness (positively) and depression (inversely). Moreover, mindfulness training was associated with an increase in connection weights to targets (bilateral frontal, left frontocentral, and temporal regions only) and distractors. SNN models were superior to other machine learning methods in the classification of brain states as a function of mindfulness training. Findings suggest SNN models can provide useful information that differentiates brain states based on distinct task demands and stimuli, as well as changes in brain states as a function of psychological intervention.
Keywords: mindfulness, oddball-paradigm event-related potential (ERP) data, target and distractor stimuli, dynamic spatiotemporal brain data, computational modelling, spiking neural network
1. Introduction
1.1. Mindfulness Training: The Neural Basis for Performance Monitoring
Mindfulness, derived from Buddhist traditions and adapted to Western secular contexts, is supported as an effective component in interventions for mood disorders (e.g., anxiety, depression, stress [1,2,3,4]), and sensory disorder (e.g., tinnitus [5]) and may influence several implicated neurocognitive domains (e.g., executive function, attention, emotional regulation, inhibition, and awareness) [6,7]. Improved cognitive performance following mindfulness training has been demonstrated using the Stroop [8,9,10], attentional switching [11], and sustained attention [12] tasks. Additionally, behavioural performance on the Go/No-go test (measuring sustained attention and inhibitory control) has been reported after a 3-month mindfulness retreat to reliably predict improved socioemotional function for up to 5 months [13].
Underpinning mechanisms of these changes have been investigated using neuroimaging [14,15] and electroencephalography (EEG) methods [16,17,18,19,20]. For example, alterations in resting-state EEG parallel improvement in mood following mindfulness intervention [21]. That is, normalisation of connectivity weights at frontal and temporal sites was seen following mindfulness training, particularly in a group that had the greatest reduction in depression. Resting-state functional connectivity changes with mindfulness practice have also been observed between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and default mode network areas (e.g., posterior cingulate cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate [22,23]), in line with findings of decreased mind-wandering and increased capacity for attention shifting in experienced meditators [15,24].
Further understanding of the functional significance (e.g., cognitive control and attention) of such changes in frontotemporal function is possible through the evaluation of task-related EEG activity, such as that measured by event-related potentials (ERPs). ERPs provide a direct measure of sensory/perceptual (before 200 ms e.g., P1, N100, P150) and higher-level (after 200 ms e.g., N200, P300) cognitive operations associated with stimulus presentation and/or task performance [25,26]. For example, previous work involving ERPs has reported that meditation can enhance the facility to efficiently and rapidly allocate and reallocate attention, with alterations seen at early information processing stages [27,28,29,30].
Other studies report mindfulness-associated alterations in later stages in information processing [31,32,33,34]. Two ERP components commonly investigated during ERP Go/No-go tasks (indexing cognitive control) include the N200 and P300. N200 occurs around 180–250 ms post-stimulus and has been associated with cognitive control, response inhibition, and conflict monitoring. It typically shows higher amplitude in No-Go compared to Go trials [35,36]. The P300 ERP component and late positive potential (LPP) have been proposed to reflect attentional, executive, and cognitive control processes [37]. Typically the P300 is observed between 280 and 400 ms after the presentation of the stimulus to salient and/or task-relevant stimuli [27,37]. It is sensitive to a wide range of stimulus characteristics and cognitive processes, ranging from task difficulty, stimulus category, and resource allocation [38]. N200 and P300 are composite waveforms comprising several subcomponents that vary in topography and latency. For example, subcomponents of the P300 include earlier anterior (P3a) responses to novelty and/or salience [39,40,41], mid-range posteriorly distributed (P3b) components associated with working-memory updating and target detection [41,42] and later anterior (P3c) responses, reflecting inhibition and behavioural modulation [43,44,45]. Subcomponents differentially respond to task demands. For example, P300 amplitude is generally higher and anteriorly distributed in No-Go, relative to GO trials and is related to the assessment of response inhibition in those trials [43,45].
The effect of meditation on ERPs depends on the type of mediation, processing stage investigated, and eliciting task used [31,46]. Some studies report increases in N200 amplitude [47], whilst both increases and decreases in P300 have been reported. Cahn and Polich [46], for example, suggest P3a (along with P200 and N100) amplitudes in response to white noise bursts is reduced following Vipasana meditation, whilst P3b may be increased. Others also report increased P3b with meditation (for review see Cahn and Polich, 2009). Moore et al. [47] report decreased P300 (centro-parietal sites alone were reported) to incongruent stimuli on a colour-word Stroop task in meditators compared with a nonmeditators group. In comparison, higher P300 amplitude was seen at frontocentral brain regions in response to target stimuli (frontal P3b) following meditation practice, suggesting an improvement in frontal attention that was related to subsequent memory processing [41,48,49]. Additionally, mind-wandering, which is often reduced during mindfulness, covaries with a reduction in P3b [50].
In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, meditators and nonmeditators of performance were assessed on impulse and attention control using a Stroop Task [51]. In comparison to meditators, nonmeditators demonstrated increased activity in the temporocentral, frontocentral, pre and postcentral gyri, and basal ganglia during incongruent conditions. These outcomes from neuroimaging studies suggest that meditation may improve efficiency, by enhancing the ability to control impulses [51] and sustain attention [52,53,54].
1.2. Computational Modelling of Data in a Brain-Inspired Spiking Neural Network Architecture
Recent studies have begun to use machine learning methods to integrate the dynamic patterns of spatiotemporal brain data contained in EEG and ERP data [55,56,57,58,59,60]. This poses a challenge, as the temporal component may display complicated interactions that dynamically alter over time [61]. Thus, spatial and temporal information is often separated for analysis, rather than incorporating these aspects into one unifying computational model. Such a model should be constrained by “biological plausibility” [62,63] (inspired by the function of neurological systems—Definition in Appendix A (1)) and be interpretable. The model’s interpretability should facilitate comprehension of why certain outcomes have resulted from the model, and discoverability of causal interactions that have resulted in the output. To this end, the current research applies a novel computational framework based on one of the most promising trends of artificial neural networks (ANN), called spiking neural networks (SNN). SNN models have been developed with a neurobiologically-plausible computational architecture that incorporates both spatial and temporal characteristics of data into one unifying model and can be applied for pattern recognition. Classification, and prediction of different mental and neurological cognitive states [64,65,66]. They are considered a suitable tool for the analysis of the spatiotemporal brain data (STBD), where both space and time components are crucial to be learnt [57,58,67,68,69,70].
The current research applies SNN to ERP data in order to investigate brain mechanisms of cognitive control in relation depression and mindfulness. It addresses the following aims to:
-
(i)
design a SNN architecture to model and understand brain activity patterns generated before and after mindfulness training for spatiotemporal relationships between continuous ERP data streams, while participants of two groups (experimental and waitlist) were responding to (1): target stimuli (2) and nontarget (distractor) stimuli.
-
(ii)
create computational models from ERP data to better scrutinize the temporal dynamic processes at different time points of stimulus presentation.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Ethics
Ethical approval was obtained from the Auckland University of Technology Ethics Committee (AUTEC), New Zealand. All experiments were performed under AUTEC guidelines and regulations. Before the assessment, each participant was fully advised of their rights and provided written informed consent. Data were collected at the Faculty of Health and Environmental Sciences, Auckland University of Technology, New Zealand.
2.2. Data and Software Availability
The software used for the implementation of the designed method can be found at http://www.kedri.aut.ac.nz/neucube/. Whilst current ethical approval does not allow us to make data available, the authors are currently applying to make data available in future.
2.3. Participants
Forty participants were recruited as part of a mindfulness intervention study [71] and underwent baseline (T1) assessments that included EEG/ERP, psychometrics (Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI) [72], Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS) [73], and Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) [74]), and blood measures [75] (not reported in the current study) (the definitions of the tests are in Appendix A (2), (3), (4)). Twenty people were selected for this study based on (i) having completed EEG assessments at all time-points (see below); (ii) shown an increase in mindfulness scores and (iii) the absence of severe depression. Of these n = 20, one group (experimental, n = 10; 3 male and 7 females; age = 19–57 years; mean = 29, SD = 10.58) had been enrolled into the mindfulness training programme following baseline assessments (T1) and were then assessed at 6 weeks following the mindfulness training programme (T2,) and 6 weeks follow-up after T2 (T3). Another group (waitlist, n = 10, 2 male and 8 females; age = 18–56 years; mean = 28.80, SD = 9.74) had been enrolled onto a waitlist (T1-T2, 6 weeks), and then underwent mindfulness training following which (6 weeks) they had a third assessment (T3).
2.4. Trait Mindfulness
Changes in mindfulness were calculated as the difference between post- and pre-training by the FFMQ [76,77] score. Table A1 of the Appendix B provides descriptive information of all participants and their FFMQ (Post-Pre) scores.
2.5. Mindfulness Intervention
The mindfulness intervention was revised from an educational program called “Pause, Breathe and Smile” [78]. Each mindfulness phase took 90–110 min and consisted of discussion and regular brief guided meditation exercises. A full explanation of the programme has been summarised in the previous research [71]. Figure 1a–e show the data collection protocol and SNN-based methodology for Spatiotemporal Brain Data (STBD) modelling, learning, visualisation, comparison, and classification. EEG data were recorded at three time points in each group (Figure 1a,b). In the experimental group, these included assessments at baseline, time point 1 (T1, PRE1), post 6 weeks mindfulness training (T2, POST1) and follow-up (T3, POST2). In the waitlist group, assessments were conducted at baseline (T1, PRE0), following 6 weeks on a waitlist (T2, PRE1) and following 6 weeks mindfulness training (T3, POST1), (shown in Figure 1b).
2.6. ERP Cognitive Task
Participants completed a 4-tone auditory oddball task undertaken in a quiet room. Stimuli were randomly presented through headphones (64 dB) across two blocks, each containing 90 standard tones (1000 Hz, sine, duration = 40 ms, attack/decay = 5 ms), 18 white noise bursts (duration = 40 ms), 18 targets, and 18 distractors. Thus, there were 288 stimuli in total with noise bursts, targets, and distractors each comprising 12.5% of stimuli. This is necessarily lower than the probability of target stimuli in a typical 2-tone oddball (often around 0.2), in order to balance requirements for maintaining a high frequency of standard tones (62.5% of trials in the current study), generate sufficient trials for all stimuli, and minimise duration of the whole task. Similarly, probabilities in Cahn and Polich (2009) were 0.10 for targets and white noise bursts. Targets and distractors were 1500 Hz sine wave tones attack/decay = 5 ms. Distractors were assigned the same frequency as targets in order to generate cognitive conflict, requiring activation of cognitive control and inhibitory mechanisms (P3c) in the case of the distractors which differed from targets on a second feature, duration. In block A, targets were 40 ms and distractors were 100 ms tones. In block B targets were 100 ms tones and distractors were 40 ms tones. The block order was randomly counterbalanced across participants. The noise bursts, which were designed to activate an orienting response (P3a), are not included in the current study. In all cases the interstimulus interval was 1100 with 100 ms jitter.
Participants were asked to click on a button with their right (dominant) hand as fast and accurately as possible once they heard the target sound, and to refrain from pressing any buttons to any other sound. Prior to commencing data collection, participants were presented three times with each stimulus and allowed sufficient practice to demonstrate that they understood instructions and could discriminate the target from distractor stimuli.
2.7. EEG Acquisition and Pre-Processing
During the task, a Synamps amplifier (NeuroScan; sampling rate = 1000 Hz; AFz ground) and quikcap (based on the 10–20 system) were used to record EEG data from 62 channels: FP1, FPZ, FP2, AF3, AF4, F7, F5, F3, F1, FZ, F2, F4, F6, F8, FT7, FC5, FC3, FC1, FCZ, FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8, T7, C5, C3, C1, CZ, C2, C4, C6, T8, TP7, CP5, CP3, CP1, CPZ, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8, P7, P5, P3, P1, PZ, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO7, PO5, PO3, POZ, PO4, PO6, PO8, CB1, O1, OZ, O2, CB2. Impedance was maintained below 10 kOhms. Curry 7.12 software was used to pre-process data off-line. The signal was re-referenced to linked A1/A2, corrected to baseline, and subject artefact removal. Initially, vertical ocular artefacts (e.g., blinks) were reduced by identifying activity in EOG and midline frontal electrodes that exceeded threshold (60 micoVolts). Principal components analysis was performed on data surrounding (−200–500 ms) the artefact peak to reduce the artefact. This was repeated using bilateral frontal electrodes to reduce any residual artefact. Data was then reduced to epochs of −100–900 ms surrounding stimulus onset. Epochs were scanned manually for excessive EMG artefact and if present the Epoch was removed from analysis.
The current study reports on target and distractor stimuli across 20 participants in two groups (experimental and waitlist) over three time points (T1, T2, and T3). Data collected at T1 and T2 was used for visualisation; T1, T2, and T3 for classification. The data was averaged to form ERPs for target and distractor stimuli. Twenty samples were created (10 for experimental and 10 for the waitlist group), each sample file contains data of one participant (1300 datapoints which represent the length of ERP epoch).
2.8. Method: Brain-Inspired Spiking Neural Networks
SNN models are neuro-computational units that are stimulated with respect to neural structure in the brain. According to several authors [70,79], the SNN follows a biologically plausible structure, in that:
-
-
A brain template is utilised to build a 3-dimensional space of spiking neurons (SNN model) that reserve spatial information of brain structure. Mapping of brain data variables into the SNN model.
-
-
Input spatiotemporal data (brain signals) are converted to a ‘spike train’ (series of binary events when the brain signal reaches a threshold value).
-
-
The SNN model is initialised using the brain-inspired small-world connectivity rule.
-
-
The initialised SNN model learns through a biologically plausible learning algorithm to adapt the model’s connectivity, resulting in long chains of connections.
In a SNN model, every “neuron” (more definition is in Appendix A (5)) is an information processing element, facilitated with a learning algorithm that extracts the relationship between the steaming data variables over time. That is, spiking neurons are connected by synapses, where the learning patterns are memorised. Compared with conventional machine learning methods [80,81,82], SNN models integrate the notion of time into the computation and thus are considered to be superior in biological plausibility in neural networks compared to previous models that do not account for temporal dynamics. Thus, SNNs are recognised as appropriate models for processing STBD [79].
In this study, the SNN connectivity captured the relationships between the ERP variables in a computational model rather than the precise organisation of the brain’s physical neural connectivity. The SNN architecture includes several modules: a data encoding procedure; a 3D SNN model that learns from data in an unsupervised mode; a layer of spiking neurons for supervised learning; output classification; optimisation; finally, interpretation of SNN models and knowledge extraction [79,83]. Figure 1c illustrates the designed methodology, containing segmentation of data based on the averaged ERP time points and ERP encoding into spike sequences; while Figure 1d,e present the computational modelling of data into a 3D space of artificial neurons, patterns recognition, and patterns classification.
In the following the steps for computational modelling of data will be explained.
2.8.1. Initialisation and Encoding of the ERP Data in the SNN Model
In the first phase of data modelling, the real-value ERP time series need to be encoded to train of spikes using an appropriate encoding method. The encoded spike trains reflect the significant changes in data over time. In this case, the Threshold-Based Representation (TBR) [84] was used as the encoding method. Using this technique, if the upward change in a signal’s amplitude is more than a threshold at a certain time, then a positive spike is produced. Conversely, negative spikes are created if the amplitude diminutions below the defined threshold. When none of these cases, then no spikes are generated.
The generated spike trains embody changes in the STBD that exceeded a threshold TBRthr. Figure 1c demonstrates an instance of encoded EEG signals into positive and negative spike trains generated from the raw EEG data. Figure 2 and Figure 3 depict the activated areas (based on the spiking activity) in the SNN models of target and distractor stimuli at different ERP latencies (253 and 313 ms post-stimulus) for an experimental participant at two time points (T1, T2). This confirmed the clear discrimination between the brain responses to target and distractor stimuli before and after the mindfulness training.
Figure 2a,b show that at T1 more spikes were activated around frontal, centroparietal, and occipitoparietal at 253 and 313 ms post target stimulus. However, after the mindfulness training (Figure 2c,d), a greater number of spikes were triggered around frontal and temporal areas compared with T1.
Figure 3 demonstrates that when participants responded to the distractor stimuli, the size of activated areas in SNN models was greater than the target stimuli across the scalp. Figure 3a,b show that more clusters of spikes activated at 313 ms post-stimulus across the scalp than 253 ms. The generated clusters were stronger (involved more activated neurons shown in red) around the frontal region at T2 (Figure 3c,d).
After spiking activity in the model, a model was then prestructured to represent the functional and structural information of the brain processes measured by spatiotemporal data (Figure 1d). The STBD data samples were mapped spatially into 3D artificial neural space where the spatial information of brain areas is topologically preserved concerning the (x, y, z) coordinates as positioned in brain Talairach atlas [85]. In the SNN model, after defining a biologically plausible 3D SNN, data were initialised with a Small-World Connectivity rule (SWC) [86] that defines a probability by which a neuron i can be linked to a neuron j with respect to their internal distance, the greater the distance between and the smaller in the connection probability. The generated initial connections were adapted during the unsupervised learning process which takes into account the temporal dynamics of input data (described in the following section).
2.8.2. Unsupervised Learning Mechanism in a SNN Model
The SNN model uses an unsupervised learning algorithm, called Spike Time-Dependent Plasticity (STDP) which allows the model to learn the spatiotemporal relationships in the input spikes [79,87,88]. This learning process modifies the neural connection strength with respect to the timing of pre to postsynaptic neurons. The connections between neurons were updated dynamically at each time point of the input data (e.g., at a millisecond scale), resulting in deep trajectories of connectivity learnt in the 3D SNN structure. Throughout the STDP learning process, if every neuron’s potential passes an activation threshold in time , then it produces an output spike. The spike is then transferred to other neurons linked to it. This neuron likewise keeps receiving spikes over time and, after passing a threshold, fires [89]. In this way, spikes are propagated inside the SNN model during the STDP learning and the ‘hidden’ spatiotemporal relationships between the data variables are captured in the shape of neural connectivity.
2.8.3. Supervised Learning and Pattern Classification in SNN Models
An output layer of spiking neurons is used and trained in a supervised mode to classify the spiking activity of the model identified with various stimuli (e.g., target, distractor) related to “before” and “after” the mindfulness training (two classes) to compare the two SNN models and to interpret the differences (Figure 1e).
An output classifier layer of dynamic evolving SNN (deSNN) [69,79,84] is built based on supervised learning to learn the relationship between the EEG data and their class labels. This classifier is evolving and allowing for new data to be introduced to the model and tested incrementally. Here, for every EEG sample (from the training dataset), one neuron is generated and positioned on the output layer which is fully connected to the whole SNN space. These initial connections between the SNN model and the output layer are initialised using rank-order rule and then modified when the input data were again passed into the model for supervised learning. In the validation phase, the EEG samples which were excluded from the training are used as testing data. The classification here is based on the k-nearest neighbours (kNN) method applied to the distance between the newly generated output neurons (testing samples) and that of any one of the already trained output neurons. When the output of a new testing sample was not known in advance, the class label of this sample was defined by the label of the majority of the output neurons [55,70] selected by the kNN method.
2.8.4. SNN Model Evaluation
To evaluate the level of significance in the trained SNN models, each experiment was performed with respect to the following criteria: for a mental activity (called class) of each individual data, one SNN model is created. Each class of data contains n samples that are used to train the SNN model through an iterative procedure of leave-one-out as follows (shown in Figure 4):
The SNN model is initialised.
The initialised SNN model is trained with (n − 1) samples (one sample is excluded from the training).
The average of the quantitative information (spatiotemporal connectivity) in the trained SNN model is calculated.
The hold-out sample is replaced by another sample, then it returns to step 1 until all the samples are excluded from the training set, one by one. This means that a set of SNN models are initialised and trained with different folds of samples.
The numerical information of the trained SNN models can be also statistically examined to evaluate the models’ significance. To this end, for every trained SNN model, an activation level was measured through computing the average value of its connection weights (definition is in Appendix A (6)). Here, we applied ANOVA (analysis of variance) to the activation levels of several trained SNN models to evaluate the significance of the models.
3. Results
3.1. Pattern Recognition, Visualisation, and Mapping
The results from the designed SNN-based methodology (that was fully explained in the methods section), is depicted graphically in Figure 1 and consists of the following steps:
-
(1)
Mapping, modelling, learning, classifying, and understanding of ERP data.
-
(2)
Statistical and quantitative analysis was performed on the SNN models to assess the model significance.
Firstly, a brain-inspired 3D SNN model was designed based on the Talairach brain atlas of 1471 neurons. Here, the term “neuron” is used to represent the centre co-ordinate of one cubic centimetre area from the 3D Talairach Atlas [85]. The SNN model input neurons are allocated to the 62 EEG channels to transfer their spike trains into the SNN model.
EEG channels were divided into five regions for their anatomical component (Figure 5): frontal, frontocentral, temporal, centroparietal, and occipitoparietal. Then, eight separate 3D models were trained with different ERP datasets related to target and distractor stimuli across experimental (T1, baseline; T2, post-mindfulness training) and waitlist (T1 baseline; T2, post-waitlist lead-in) groups (Figure 6, Figure 7, Figure 8 and Figure 9). Figure 6 and Figure 7 show the neuronal connections created for the experimental group in the brain-inspired SNN models reflect the functional connectivity in response to target (Figure 6) and distractor stimuli (Figure 7) before and after the mindfulness training; while Figure 8 and Figure 9 represent the neuronal connections created for the waitlist group in the brain-inspired SNN models reflect the functional connectivity in response to target (Figure 8) and distractor stimuli (Figure 9) before and after the mindfulness training.
As shown in Figure 6b, after the mindfulness training, stronger connectivity was generated around the frontal regions in the experimental group, compared with the waitlist group in response to target stimuli (Figure 7b). Distractor stimuli showed lower connectivity at T1 compared to T2 for both groups (Figure 7a and Figure 9b).
The differences between the SNN models of T1 and T2 sessions can be also studied by computing the amount of spatiotemporal network interactions between the EEG variables using a feature interaction network (FIN). In Figure A1 of the Appendix B, the total temporal spike interaction among 62 input neuronal areas (corresponding to 62 EEG channels) for experimental group towards target stimuli before and after the mindfulness training is shown in the FIN, where nodes represent the input neuronal areas (neuronal clusters) and each line, that links two nodes, corresponds to the amount of spike transmission between the clusters during the SNN learning model.
The numerical information of the trained SNN models can be statistically analysed to evaluate the models’ significance. To this end, for every trained SNN model, an activation level was measured through computing the average value of its connection weights.
Therefore, for every participant, one SNN model was developed at T1 and T2. The average connection weights for each developed individual SNN model were calculated as a function of group (experimental, waitlist) and time (T1, T2). The averaged connection weights of each developed SNN model for each group in response to target and distractor stimuli across all time of data collection are reported in Table 1 and were used for further statistical analysis.
Table 1.
The Averaged Connection Weight of SNN Models | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Site | Time Point | |||
T1 | T2 | ||||
Type of Stimuli | |||||
Target | Distractor | Target | Distractor | ||
Experimental | Frontal | 0.53 | 0.82 | 0.94 | 0.87 |
Temporal | 0.69 | 0.87 | 0.87 | 0.96 | |
Frontocentral | 0.72 | 0.80 | 0.77 | 0.81 | |
Centroparietal | 0.95 | 0.92 | 0.92 | 0.95 | |
Occipitoparietal | 1.18 | 0.92 | 0.93 | 0.99 | |
Total weight | 0.81 | 0.86 | 0.86 | 0.91 | |
Waitlist | Frontal | 0.55 | 0.94 | 0.73 | 0.87 |
Temporal | 0.74 | 0.94 | 0.88 | 0.95 | |
Frontocentral | 0.7 | 0.81 | 0.75 | 0.85 | |
Centroparietal | 0.99 | 0.92 | 0.92 | 0.95 | |
Occipitoparietal | 1.15 | 0.95 | 0.94 | 0.99 | |
Total weight | 0.82 | 0.91 | 0.84 | 0.92 |
3.2. Statistical Analysis of the SNN Models
The connection weights for each EEG channel were then divided into five sites for each hemisphere with their topographical features (as shown in Figure 5): frontal, temporal, frontocentral, centroparietal, and occipitoparietal. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to assess differences in functional activity between time and stimuli types (Table 2). Independent variables include Time (T1, T2), Hemisphere (left, right), Site (centroparietal, occipitoparietal, frontal, temporal, and frontocentral), Stimuli (target, distractor), and Group (experimental, waitlist). All violations of the assumption of sphericity were corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser corrections.
Table 2.
Variables | F-Value | Degrees of Freedom | p-Value | Partial-eta2 (ηp2) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Site | 18.432 | 4,72 | <0.001 | 0.506 |
Time | 24.468 | 1,18 | <0.001 | 0.576 |
Stimuli | 38.405 | 1,18 | <0.001 | 0.681 |
Time * Group | 14.962 | 1,18 | 0.001 | 0.454 |
Hemisphere * Stimuli | 9.815 | 1,18 | 0.006 | 0.353 |
Site * Time † | 44.631 | 2.761,49.702 | <0.001 | 0.713 |
Site * Stimuli | 40.842 | 2.135,38.434 | <0.001 | 0.694 |
Time * Stimuli | 5.786 | 1,18 | 0.027 | 0.243 |
Site * Time * Group | 5.931 | 2.761,49.702 | 0.002 | 202 |
The distribution of the connection weights can be seen in Figure 10 as a function of Site, Group, and Time. The SNN connection weights distribution related to “before” mindfulness (green colour) and six weekly mindfulness follow-ups (pink colour) across five sites of the brain in experimental and waitlist groups in response to target stimuli (Figure 10a,c) and distractor stimuli (Figure 10b,d) across the whole EEG epoch.
Firstly, the weights were examined at T1 in order to investigate differences between the stimuli pre-mindfulness training. There were significant Site * Stimuli [F (4, 72) = 85.225, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.826] and Site * Hemisphere * Stimuli [F (2.572, 46.302) = 13.254, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.424] interactions. These were due to lower weights over frontal [F (1, 19) = 129.832, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.872], and greater weights over parietooccipital [F (1, 19) = 63.047, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.768] sites bilaterally for targets compared to distractors. In addition, in the left hemisphere only, there were higher weights for distractors than targets at temporal [F (1, 19) = 37.291, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.662] and frontocentral [F (1, 19) = 22.678, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.544] sites. No differences were found between target and distractor stimuli over centroparietal regions [F (1, 19) = 0.419, p = 0.525, ηp2 = 0.022].
Separate ANOVAs were conducted for target and distractor stimuli to investigate the effects of mindfulness training. Within groups variables included Time and Site with Group as a between groups variable. These analyses only considered effects that included Time and Group interactions.
For target stimuli, there was a significant Site * Time * Group interaction [F (2.220, 39.956) = 61.419, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.204], which appeared due to a significant Time * Group interaction at frontal sites [F (1, 18) = 9.853, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.354]. Although there was an increase in weights between T1 and T2 for both experimental [F (1, 9) = 66.066, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.880] and waitlist [F (1, 9) = 13.577, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.601] groups, the effect was greater for the experimental group. Thus, whilst no difference was seen between groups at T1 [F (1, 18) = 0.048, p = 0.829, ηp2 = 0.003], at T2 the experimental group had greater weights than the waitlist group [F (1, 18) = 9.826, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.353]. For the distractor stimuli, there was a significant Group * Time interaction [F (2.884, 51.918) = 4.599, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.204], and this was due to a significant effect of Time (T2 > T1) in the experimental group [F (1, 9) = 40.843, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.819], which was not seen in the waitlist group [F (1, 9) = 1.112, p = 0.317, ηp2 = 0.111].
3.3. Correlations between Connections Weights, Mindfulness, and Psychometrics Data
A Kendall’s Tau correlation was computed to assess the relationship between the created connection weights in the SNN model across brain regions for both target and distractor stimuli at baseline (T1) and the psychometric scores including (BDI) [90], (DASS) [91], and the (FFMQ) [74].
Kendall’s Tau correlations showed right frontal connection weights were significantly positively associated with FFMQ (target rτ = 0.378, p = 0.033; distractor rτ = 0.43, p = 0.015). The response to distractors was also inversely associated with Depression as measured by the DASS (rτ = −515, p = 0.005) and BDI (rτ = −0.508, p = 0.004). Associations between right frontal connection weights in response to distractors and other DASS measures were on the threshold for significance (Anxiety rτ = −0.363, p = 0.05; Stress rτ = −0.348, p = 0.06).
3.4. Pattern Classification and Discrimination
After training the SNN models, a classifier was trained to classify the SNN model activity for the participants’ responses towards target stimuli at T1, T2, and T3, across two participant groups. In total, 30 samples were created and divided (10 samples per class = number of participants in each class) into three classes (class 1: response to target stimuli at T1; class 2: response to target stimuli at T2; class 3: response to target stimuli at T3). The same division of samples for the waitlist group was defined. Table 3 represents the classification results for the three classes. To perform a comparative analysis between the performance of the used dynamic evolving SNN method (deSNN) [69], and traditional machine learning methods, we used multilayer perceptron (MLP) [81,92], multiple linear regression (MLR) [82], and support vector machine (SVM) [63,93] for classification of EEG data as reported in Table 3. The classification procedure was performed using leave-one-out cross-validation. Table 3 reports the accuracy of classification resulted from the SNN models versus the conventional machine learning techniques. The confusion table here demonstrates the number of misclassified and correctly classified EEG samples.
Table 3.
SNN-Based Methodology (Experimental Group) | ||||||
ERP Data Classes |
Target Stimuli
at T1(C1) |
Target Stimuli
at T2 (C2) |
Target Stimuli
at T3 (C3) |
Accuracy
(%) |
Total Accuracy (%) |
F-Score
(%) |
Target Stimuli at T1(C1) | 7 | 1 | 2 | 70.00 | 73.00 | 79.00 |
Target Stimuli at T2 (C2) | 1 | 9 | 0 | 90.00 | ||
Target Stimuli at T3 (C3) | 2 | 2 | 6 | 60.00 | ||
Traditional Machine Learning Methods | ||||||
Methods
Accuracy in % |
MLP
50 |
SVM
58.50 |
MLR
48.50 |
|||
SNN-based methodology (waitlist Group) | ||||||
ERP data Classes |
Target Stimuli
at T1(C1) |
Target Stimuli
at T2 (C2) |
Target Stimuli at T3 (C3) | Accuracy (%) | Total accuracy % |
F-Score
(%) |
Target Stimuli at T1(C1) | 7 | 2 | 1 | 70.00 | 73.00 | 79.00 |
Target Stimuli at T2 (C2) | 1 | 7 | 2 | 70.00 | ||
Target Stimuli at T3 (C3) | 1 | 1 | 8 | 80.00 | ||
Traditional Machine Learning Methods | ||||||
Methods Accuracy in % |
MLP 59.50 |
SVM 51.50 |
MLR 55.50 |
4. Discussion
This is the first study to apply a brain-inspired SNN model, incorporating temporal and spatial components of EEG data, to investigate changes in brain function associated as a function of mindfulness training.
Several key findings have emerged from this work. In terms of using a SNN model to differentiate the EEG response to target compared to distractor stimuli at baseline, the connection weights were lower for targets than distractors over bilateral frontal, left frontocentral, and left temporal regions, but were greater for targets over left occipitoparietal regions. Right frontal connection weights in response to distractor stimuli at baseline were associated positively with FFMQ scores and inversely with Depression. Following mindfulness training, relative to the waitlist group, the experimental group showed a greater increase in frontal connection weights in response to targets. Thus, by follow-up, frontal connection weights were greater in the experimental group than the waitlist group. In response to distractors, a general increase across sites was seen in connection weights following mindfulness training in the experimental group, whilst the waitlist group showed no difference between T1 and T2. Finally, the accuracy of spatiotemporal data classification was higher when using SNN models compared with traditional machine learning methods (e.g., SVM, MLP, and MLR).
Previously, we applied SNN to resting-state EEG data to differentiate states of depression and the effect of mindfulness training on brain function [56]. That study reported mindfulness training increased resting-state connection weights, in parallel with improvements in depression; and that baseline connection weights predicted response to mindfulness training. Another study from our group applied SNN to ERP data in response to familiar and unfamiliar logos [57].
Traditionally, ERPs are measured according to amplitude and latency [27]. The current study applied a novel technique that capitalises on the temporal dynamics of the shifts in polarity throughout the ERP, spatially across electrodes, as inputs to the SNN (depending on the threshold). The SNN is then used to model the connectivity weights with respect to the (STDP) learning rule [87]. According to STDP rule, connectivity with greater weights reflects stronger spike transmission between neurons. Thus, the STDP rule develops neural connectivity among the spatially distributed neurons in the SNN model that represents the spatiotemporal relationships between the brain data variables.
4.1. Comparison of Stimuli (Target vs. Distractor)
Our findings are consistent with previous ERP amplitude research that has shown a more posterior maximum in response to target stimuli (e.g., P300), which may be underpinned by activity in several distributed regions including medial and lateral prefrontal regions, temporoparietal junction, and inferior parietal lobule [94,95]. In comparison, ERP anteriorisation is seen in response to distracting, stop and/or No-Go stimuli [43,96]. In the current study, this was supported by the bilateral increase in frontal connection weights in response to distractor (relative to the target) stimuli. Greater activation weights to distractors over left frontocentral and temporal sites might reflect the activity of the DLPFC which, along with the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), superior parietal cortex, and frontal eye field has been associated with a distractor network [97,98].
A similar network involving the DLPFC, orbitofrontal cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and amygdala is proposed to underpin deficits in cognitive control in depression [99]. Our current findings from the correlation analysis support this idea, at least for right frontal connection weights in response to distractor stimuli, which were associated with both depression and mindfulness and may reflect mechanisms involved in interference control. Thus, right frontal networks of cognitive control may represent a mechanism by which mindfulness training reduces depression. Further research will however need to extend these findings from the general population to clinical populations and investigate the clinical utility of applying SNN models to ERP data. For instance, in predicting response to interventions, as has been shown for resting-state data [56].
4.2. Effect of Mindfulness Training (T1 vs. T2)
As shown in Figure A1 of the Appendix B, broader interaction lines were formed between the 62 EEG data variables of target stimuli at T2 compared to the T1. In the case of the target stimuli, there were thicker interaction lines in the SNN model at T2, especially between the EEG channels positioned at frontal, frontocentral, and temporal areas, when compared with the models at time T1 (before training). The current findings suggest that mindfulness training results in an increase in connectivity, particularly in relation to frontal function and executive control. Previous studies have shown improved behavioural performance during cognitive distraction (e.g., Stroop task) in meditators. Jensen et al. [100,101] suggested the meditated group showed fewer errors in the commission of a Stroop task in contrast to the control group. Some research suggests that meditation training affects inhibition identified with verbal, as opposed to motor execution performance [102,103]. Conversely, other Stroop-based studies found no noteworthy performance differences among meditators and controls [104,105]. Nevertheless, Vega et al. [105] suggested that in the meditated group, a substantial error reduction was observed at follow-up which counteracted with longer response times, showing a speed–precision trade-off. Cahn and Polich [46] showed a decrease in the P3a ERP component, an orienting response to white noise bursts, following meditation. They interpreted this as reflecting improvement in cognitive control with mindfulness. Another study showed higher P300 amplitude at frontocentral brain regions in response to target stimuli (frontal P3b) following meditation practice, suggesting an improvement in frontal attention that was related to subsequent memory processing [41,106]. The current results for targets are in line with this latter study and extend understanding of the effects of mindfulness training on inhibitory aspects of the P300 (P3c); and more specifically show an increase in connection weights for P3c and frontal P3b. The weight of a connection reflects the amount of the signal input into the connection which passes between EEG channels and corresponds to the influence that the firing of one neuron has on another.
Other studies report reduced BOLD activation during mindfulness training [107,108]. Investigation of the precise relationship between connection weights, amplitude, and BOLD response warrants investigation and will be the focus of future research. Nevertheless, taken together, findings suggest that more efficient processing (reduced orientation to distractors, greater response to targets, increased inhibitory control) following mindfulness training might be underpinned by improvement in connectivity.
4.3. Pattern Classification (T1 vs. T2 vs. Post-Assessment)
In comparison to several conventional machine learning methods, the SNN models in the current study showed superior accuracy in classifying data from T1, T2, and T3. This may be due to several qualities inherent in the deSNN, compared to other models. With conventional machine learning techniques, the spatiotemporal features of every EEG sample are typically converted into one vector of features. Thus, the temporal connections cannot be obtained. These traditional methods can analyse the data in the form of static vectors and lack from exposing the spatiotemporal association. In comparison, the 3D brain-inspired SNN models allow the integration of dynamic interactions between underpinning brain functions during distinct psychological states. Moreover, rather than being a “black box”, deSNN allows visualisation of the changes in brain state as a function of group membership and experimental conditions.
SNN models can process time information alongside the spatial information while streaming the EEG data. Every training and testing sample includes the amplitude powers of all EEG channels within an entire time interval. Through the model training phase, the EEG temporal patterns are transferred into the SNN model via input neurons and the rest of the internal neurons in the model dynamically process these inputs. This is different from traditional machine learning techniques in which every data sample is presented as one data vector, where the integrated spatial and temporal information of the data is not preserved.
The brain-inspired SNN method is a generic architecture for precise analysis of various sorts of STBD (EEG and ERP). In comparison with conventional machine learning methods or deep learning techniques, SNN models have the following advantages: (1) they maintain both temporal and spatial components of the data in one model which is spatially regulated by a brain atlas; (2) they use a biologically plausible learning rule to detect spatiotemporal patterns from data; (3) they result in a better explanation of the spatiotemporal associations in the brain data variables. In this way, more accurate classification across assessment time points can be obtained.
4.4. Limitations and Future Research
For the current study, only EEG data of participants who had shown an increase in mindfulness scores (FFMQ) were analysed. Thus, no interpretation is made about the efficacy of mindfulness training, previously demonstrated in behavioural data. Furthermore, as with other ERP measures, the current scalp recorded data are limited in identifying precise cortical regions generating the activity. Given EEG data reflects activity from the superficial areas of the cortex, a more in-depth investigation of the fundamental brain regions needs to be undertaken.
The SNN models were trained on the whole EEG epoch for target and distractor stimuli (1300 ms post-stimulus). Future work should examine more closely smaller time windows around specific components. Although the present study has scrutinised the impacts of mindfulness training in response to target and distractors, our findings would not be limited to the defined stimuli. Therefore, further studies could examine whether similar impacts are detected in response to other types of stimuli (noise, background, etc.) followed by mindfulness training. Further studies are planned to model the deep patterns of connectivity learnt in the SNN models [70]. This will elicit an improved understanding of the individual effects of mindfulness for mental health and cognitive performance across population groups.
In summary, the designed brain-inspired SNN methodology for modelling brain data has provided several novel contributions to understanding ERPs. Current findings suggest that SNN models can provide useful information that differentiates brain states based on distinct task demands and stimuli, as well as changes in brain states as a function of psychological intervention. Thus, SNN-based methodology may provide a novel method for analysing EEG/ERP data that integrates temporal and scalp topographic information in understanding information processing.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge the support of the Auckland University of Technology (AUT) Strategic Research Investment Fund (SRIF) for funding this study.
Appendix A
Brain-Inspired: brain-inspired refers to the mathematical implementation of a method that gets inspiration from neuroscience research on brain activity.
Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI) measures the severity, depth, and intensity of depression with the Cronbach’s alpha of 0.90.
The DASS measures the three negative emotional states of anxiety, stress, and depression. Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.89, 0.78, and 0.81 for the subscales of depression, anxiety, and stress, respectively.
The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) is a psychometric measure of mindfulness that provides a five-domain profile of various aspects that are characteristic of mindfulness. However, scores may also be interpreted as a single overall score.
In spiking neural networks, an artificial neuron refers to a computational unit that mimics the behaviour of a biological neuron which receives the information, processes it, and produces an output.
Connection weights refer to the temporal relationship between two neurons in terms of the time of spike. The average weight of all neural connections (every single EEG channel) in each SNN model are extracted as a metric for comparison.
Appendix B
Table A1.
Participants | Id | Age | Gender | FFMQ Post-Pre |
---|---|---|---|---|
Experimental Group | S1 | 29 | M | 7.18 |
S2 | 29 | F | 6.82 | |
S3 | 40 | M | 0.81 | |
S4 | 47 | F | 10.92 | |
S5 | 19 | F | 2.01 | |
S6 | 26 | F | 15.38 | |
S7 | 23 | M | 6.37 | |
S8 | 19 | F | 6.37 | |
S9 | 19 | F | 2.43 | |
S10 | 37 | F | 6.54 | |
Mean (SD) | 28.80 (9.74) | 6.48 (4.32) | ||
Waitlist (Control) Group |
S#1 | 22 | F | 7.57 |
S#2 | 54 | F | 5.99 | |
S#3 | 30 | M | 10.99 | |
S#4 | 27 | M | 10.66 | |
S#5 | 21 | F | 1.34 | |
S#6 | 37 | F | 0.92 | |
S#7 | 33 | F | 7.98 | |
S#8 | 20 | F | 3.92 | |
S#9 | 27 | F | 2.03 | |
S#10 | 19 | F | 4.33 | |
Mean (SD) | 29 (10.58) | 5.57 (3.67) |
Author Contributions
Z.D. conducted the EEG data collection, designed and did the experiments, analysed and interpreted results, and wrote the manuscript. M.D. contributed to the implementation and experimentation of the computational models and also contributed to writing the manuscript. M.C.-R. contributed to statistical analysis, interpretation, and writing the manuscript. D.M. contributed to the interpretation of the statistical results and writing the manuscript. T.T. contributed to EEG data collection and performing the experiments, proofreading, and writing the manuscript. G.Y.W. contributed to EEG data collection, performing the experiments, and writing the manuscript. R.J.S. initiated the project funding and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. C.K. contributed to the design of the mindfulness intervention study and writing the manuscript. W.W. oversaw data collection for the mindfulness intervention study and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. N.K. proposed the overall design of the brain-inspired AI architecture based on SNN and the corresponding learning algorithms and also contributed to writing the manuscript. G.S. contributed a clinical perspective to the writing of the manuscript. A.S. contributed to designing the ERP cognitive task, performing the experiments, analysing both statistical and computational modelling of the data, interpretation of the data, and writing the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
Auckland University of Technology’s Strategic Research Investment Fund (SRIF) funded this research but did not influence on any part of the research process or preparation of this manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors have no competing interests as defined by Sensors Research, or other interests that might be perceived to influence the results and/or discussion reported in this paper.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Dawson A.F., Brown W.W., Anderson J., Datta B., Donald J.N., Hong K., Allan S., Mole T.B., Jones P.B., Galante J. Mindfulness-based interventions for university students: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being. 2019;12:384–410. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sevilla-Llewellyn-Jones J., Santesteban-Echarri O., Pryor I., McGorry P., Alvarez-Jimenez M., Firth J., Chadi N. Web-based mindfulness interventions for mental health treatment: Systematic review and meta-analysis. JMIR Ment. Health. 2018;5:e10278. doi: 10.2196/10278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schumer M.C., Lindsay E.K., Creswell J.D. Brief mindfulness training for negative affectivity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2018;86:569–583. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Goyal M., Singh S., Sibinga E., Gould N., Rowland-Seymour A., Sharma R., Berger Z., Sleicher D., Maron D., Shihab H., et al. Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern. Med. 2014;174:357–368. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rademaker M., Stegeman I., Ho-Kang-You K., Stokroos R., Smit D. The effect of mindfulness-based interventions on tinnitus burden. A systematic review. Front. Neurol. 2019;10:1135. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tang Y.Y., Hölzel B.K., Posner M.I. The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:213. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Creswell J.D. Mindfulness interventions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2017;68:491–516. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-042716-051139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Teper R., Inzlicht M. Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: The importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;8:85–92. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wenk-Sormaz H. Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 2005;11:42–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Moore A., Malinowski P. Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Conscious. Cogn. 2009;18:176–186. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Chambers R., Lo B.C.Y., Allen N.B. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2008;32:303–322. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Semple R.J. Does mindfulness meditation enhance attention? A randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness. 2010;1:121–130. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0017-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sahdra B.K., MacLean K.A., Ferrer E., Shaver P.R., Rosenberg E.L., Jacobs T.L., Zanesco A.P., King B.G., Aichele S., A Bridwell D., et al. Enhanced response inhibition during intensive meditation training predicts improvements in self-reported adaptive socioemotional functioning. Emotion. 2011;11:299–312. doi: 10.1037/a0022764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Young K.S., Van Der Velden A.M., Craske M.G., Pallesen K.J., Fjorback L., Roepstorff A., Parsons C.E. The impact of mindfulness-based interventions on brain activity: A systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;84:424–433. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Taren A.A., Gianaros P.J., Greco C.M., Lindsay E., Fairgrieve A., Brown K.W., Rosen R.K., Ferris J.L., Julson E., Marsland A.L., et al. Mindfulness meditation training and executive control network resting state functional connectivity: A randomized controlled trial. Psychosom. Med. 2017;79:674. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lomas T., Ivtzan I., Fu C.H. A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015;57:401–410. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cacioppo J.T., Tassinary L.G., Berntson G. Handbook of Psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Aftanas L.I., Golosheykin S. Impact of regular meditation practice on EEG activity at rest and during evoked negative emotions. Int. J. Neurosci. 2005;115:893–909. doi: 10.1080/00207450590897969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Farb N.A.S., Anderson A.K., Segal Z.V. The mindful brain and emotion regulation in mood disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry. 2012;57:70–77. doi: 10.1177/070674371205700203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bailey N.W., Freedman G., Raj K., Sullivan C.M., Rogasch N.C., Chung S.W., Hoy K.E., Chambers R., Hassed C., Van Dam N.T., et al. Mindfulness meditators show altered distributions of early and late neural activity markers of attention in a response inhibition task. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0203096. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shanok N.A., Reive C., Mize K.D., Jones N.A. Mindfulness meditation intervention alters neurophysiological symptoms of anxiety and depression in preadolescents. Psychophysiology. 2020;34:159–170. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000244. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Creswell J.D., Taren A.A., Lindsay E.K., Greco C.M., Gianaros P.J., Fairgrieve A., Marsland A.L., Brown K.W., Way B.M., Rosen R.K., et al. Alterations in resting-state functional connectivity link mindfulness meditation with reduced interleukin-6: A randomized controlled trial. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;80:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kim H.-C., Tegethoff M., Meinlschmidt G., Stalujanis E., Belardi A., Jo S., Lee J., Kim D.-Y., Yoo S.-S., Lee J.-H. Mediation analysis of triple networks revealed functional feature of mindfulness from real-time fMRI neurofeedback. NeuroImage. 2019;195:409–432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brewer J.A., Worhunsky P.D., Gray J.R., Tang Y.-Y., Weber J., Kober H. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:20254–20259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112029108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schroder H.S., Fisher M., Lin Y., Lo S.L., Danovitch J.H., Moser J.S. Neural evidence for enhanced attention to mistakes among school-aged children with a growth mindset. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;24:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Campos E., Hazlett C., Tan P., Truong H., Loo S., Distefano C., Jeste S., Şentürk D. Principle ERP reduction and analysis: Estimating and using principle ERP waveforms underlying ERPs across tasks, subjects and electrodes. NeuroImage. 2020;212:116630. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Luck S.J. An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Van Leeuwen S., Singer W., Melloni L. Meditation increases the depth of information processing and improves the allocation of attention in space. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:133. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Norris C.J., Creem D., Hendler R., Kober H. Brief mindfulness meditation improves attention in novices: Evidence from ERPs and moderation by neuroticism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018;12:315. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Klopsis A.L. Master’s Thesis. City University of New York; New York, NY, USA: 2020. The Impact of a Single Session of Mindfulness Meditation on the Attentional Blink in Non-Meditators. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sanger K.L., Dorjee D. Mindfulness training for adolescents: A neurodevelopmental perspective on investigating modifications in attention and emotion regulation using event-related brain potentials. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2015;15:696–711. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0354-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Zhang W., Ouyang Y., Tang F., Chen J., Li H. Breath-focused mindfulness alters early and late components during emotion regulation. Brain Cogn. 2019;135:103585. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.103585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Eddy M.D., Brunyé T.T., Tower-Richardi S., Mahoney C.R., Taylor H.A. The effect of a brief mindfulness induction on processing of emotional images: An ERP study. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:1391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Isbel B., Lagopoulos J., Hermens D., Stefanidis K., Summers M.J. Mindfulness improves attention resource allocation during response inhibition in older adults. Mindfulness. 2020;11:1500–1510. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01364-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Donkers F.C.L., Van Boxtel G.J. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cogn. 2004;56:165–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Falkenstein M. Inhibition, conflict and the Nogo-N2. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006;117:1638–1640. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Luck S.J., Kappenman E.S. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Linden D.E. The P300: Where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us? Neuroscience. 2005;11:563–576. doi: 10.1177/1073858405280524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Justo-Guillén E., Ricardo-Garcell J., Rodríguez-Camacho M., Rodríguez-Agudelo Y., de Larrea-Mancera E.S.L., Solís-Vivanco R. Auditory mismatch detection, distraction, and attentional reorientation (MMN-P3a-RON) in neurological and psychiatric disorders: A review. Intern. J. Psychophysiol. 2019;146:85–100. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gow R.V., Matsudaira T., Taylor E., Rubia K., Crawford M., Ghebremeskel K., Ibrahimovic A., Vallée-Tourangeau F., Williams L.M., Sumich A. Total red blood cell concentrations of ω-3 fatty acids are associated with emotion-elicited neural activity in adolescent boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fat. Acids. 2009;80:151–156. doi: 10.1016/j.plefa.2008.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Polich J. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007;118:2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wongupparaj P., Sumich A., Wickens M., Kumari V., Morris R.G. Individual differences in working memory and general intelligence indexed by P200 and P300: A latent variable model. Biol. Psychol. 2018;139:96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sumich A., Kumari V., Dodd P., Ettinger U., Hughes C., Zachariah E., Sharma T. N100 and P300 amplitude to Go and No–Go variants of the auditory oddball in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2008;98:265–277. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gow R.V., Rubia K., Taylor E., Vallée-Tourangeau F., Matsudaira T., Ibrahimovic A., Sumich A. Abnormal centroparietal ERP response in predominantly medication-naive adolescent boys with ADHD during both response inhibition and execution. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012;29:181–189. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e31824e1025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Huster R.J., Enriquez-Geppert S., Lavallee C.F., Falkenstein M., Herrmann C.S. Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013;87:217–233. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Cahn B.R., Polich J. Meditation (Vipassana) and the P3a event-related brain potential. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009;72:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Moore A.W., Egruber T., Ederose J., Malinowski P. Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:18. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Singh N., Telles S. Neurophysiological effects of meditation based on evoked and event related potential recordings. BioMed Res. Int. 2015;2015:406261. doi: 10.1155/2015/406261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kakumanu R.J., Nair A.K., Sasidharan A., John J.P., Mehrotra S., Panth R., Kutty B.M. State-trait influences of Vipassana meditation practice on P3 EEG dynamics. Prog. Brain Res. 2019;244:115–136. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Suskauer S.J., Simmonds D.J., Fotedar S., Blankner J.G., Pekar J.J., Denckla M.B., Mostofsky S.H. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for abnormalities in response selection in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Differences in activation associated with response inhibition but not habitual motor response. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008;20:478–493. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kozasa E.H., Sato J.R., Lacerda S.S., Barreiros M.A.M., Radvany J., Russell T.A., Sanches L.G., Mello L.E.A.M., Amaro E. Meditation training increases brain efficiency in an attention task. NeuroImage. 2012;59:745–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baerentsen K.B., Hartvig N.V., Stødkilde-Jørgensen H., Mammen J. Onset of meditation explored with fMRI. NeuroImage. 2001;13:297. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(01)91640-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lazar M., Weinstein D.M., Tsuruda J.S., Hasan K.M., Arfanakis K., Meyerand M., Badie B., Rowley H., Haughton V., Field A., et al. White matter tractography using diffusion tensor deflection. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2003;18:306–321. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lazar S.W., Kerr C.E., Wasserman R.H., Gray J.R., Greve D.N., Treadway M.T., McGarvey M., Quinn B.T., Dusek J.A., Benson H., et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. NeuroReport. 2005;16:1893–1897. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Doborjeh Z. Ph.D. Thesis. Auckland University of Technology; Auckland, Australia: 2019. Modelling of Spatiotemporal EEG and ERP Brain Data for Dynamic Pattern Recognition and Brain State Prediction using Spiking Neural Networks: Methods and Applications in Psychology. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Doborjeh Z., Doborjeh M., Taylor T., Kasabov N., Wang G.Y., Siegert R., Sumich A. Spiking neural network modelling approach reveals how mindfulness training rewires the brain. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42863-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Doborjeh Z., Kasabov N., Doborjeh M.G., Sumich A. Modelling peri-perceptual brain processes in a deep learning spiking neural network architecture. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27169-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Durai M., Sanders P., Doborjeh Z., Wendt A., Kasabov N., Searchfield G.D. Prediction of tinnitus masking benefit within a case series using a spiking neural network model. In: Waxman S., Stein D.G., Swaab D., Fields H., editors. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kasabov N., Doborjeh M.G., Doborjeh Z. Mapping, learning, visualization, classification, and understanding of fMRI data in the NeuCube evolving spatiotemporal data machine of spiking neural networks. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. Learn. Syst. 2016;28:887–899. doi: 10.1109/TNNLS.2016.2612890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Doborjeh Z.G., Doborjeh M.G., Kasabov N. Attentional bias pattern recognition in spiking neural networks from spatio-temporal EEG data. Cogn. Comput. 2018;10:35–48. doi: 10.1007/s12559-017-9517-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Moore M., Shafer A.T., Bakhtiari R., Dolcos F., Singhal A. Integration of spatio-temporal dynamics in emotion-cognition interactions: A simultaneous fMRI-ERP investigation using the emotional oddball task. NeuroImage. 2019;202:116078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Holzinger A. Machine Learning for Health Informatics: State-of-the-Art and Future Challenges. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ratsch G., Sonnenburg S., Schäfer C. Learning interpretable SVMs for biological sequence classification. BMC Bioinform. 2006;7:1–14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-7-S1-S9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ieracitano C., Adeel A., Morabito F.C., Hussain A. A novel statistical analysis and autoencoder driven intelligent intrusion detection approach. Neurocomputing. 2020;387:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2019.11.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mammone N., Ieracitano C., Morabito F.C. A deep CNN approach to decode motor preparation of upper limbs from time–frequency maps of EEG signals at source level. Neural Netw. 2020;124:357–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2020.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Capecci E., Doborjeh Z.G., Mammone N., La Foresta F., Morabito F.C., Kasabov N. Longitudinal study of alzheimer’s disease degeneration through EEG data analysis with a NeuCube spiking neural network model; Proceedings of the 2016 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN); Vancouver, BC, Canada. 24–29 July 2016; pp. 1360–1366. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Lotte F., Congedo M., Lecuyer A., Lamarche F., Arnaldi B. A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces. J. Neural Eng. 2007;4:R1–R13. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/4/2/R01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Izhikevich E.M. Polychronization: Computation with spikes. Neural Comput. 2006;18:245–282. doi: 10.1162/089976606775093882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kasabov N., Dhoble K., Nuntalid N., Indiveri G. Dynamic evolving spiking neural networks for on-line spatio- and spectro-temporal pattern recognition. Neural Netw. 2013;41:188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kasabov N.K. Time-Space, Spiking Neural Networks and Brain-Inspired Artificial Intelligence. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bharatharaj J., Medvedev O.N., Taylor T., Wrapson W., Rix G., Sumich A., Wang G.Y., Csako R., Anstiss D., Ranta J.T., et al. A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial for a Videoconference-Delivered Mindfulness-Based Group Intervention in a Nonclinical Setting. Mindfulness. 2018;10:700–711. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1024-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Lee E.H., Lee S.J., Hwang S.T., Hong S.H., Kim J.M. Reliability and validity of the Beck depression inventory-II among Korean adolescents. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14:30. doi: 10.4306/pi.2017.14.1.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Tran T.D., Tran T., Fisher J. Validation of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) 21 as a screening instrument for depression and anxiety in a rural community-based cohort of northern Vietnamese women. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;13:24. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Gu J., Strauss C., Crane C., Barnhofer T., Karl A., Cavanagh K., Kuyken W. Examining the factor structure of the 39-item and 15-item versions of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire before and after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for people with recurrent depression. Psychol. Assess. 2016;28:791. doi: 10.1037/pas0000263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Heym N., Heasman B.C., Hunter K., Blanco S.R., Wang G.Y., Siegert R., Cleare A., Gibson G.R., Kumari V., Sumich A.L. The role of microbiota and inflammation in self-judgement and empathy: Implications for understanding the brain-gut-microbiome axis in depression. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:1459–1470. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05230-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Baer R.A., Smith G.T., Hopkins J., Krietemeyer J., Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13:27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Medvedev O.N., Krägeloh C.U., Narayanan A., Siegert R.J. Measuring mindfulness: Applying generalizability theory to distinguish between state and trait. Mindfulness. 2017;8:1036–1046. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0679-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Devcich D.A., Rix G., Bernay R., Graham E. Effectiveness of a mindfulness-based program on school children’s self-reported well-being: A pilot study comparing effects with an emotional literacy program. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 2017;33:309–330. doi: 10.1080/15377903.2017.1316333. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kasabov N. NeuCube: A spiking neural network architecture for mapping, learning and understanding of spatio-temporal brain data. Neural Netw. 2014;52:62–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2014.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Hansen S.T., Hansen L.K. Spatio-temporal reconstruction of brain dynamics from EEG with a Markov prior. NeuroImage. 2017;148:274–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Murtagh F. Multilayer perceptrons for classification and regression. Neurocomputing. 1991;2:183–197. doi: 10.1016/0925-2312(91)90023-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Yao X.J., Panaye A., Doucet J.P., Zhang R.S., Chen H.F., Liu M.C., Hu Z.D., Fan B.T. Comparative study of QSAR/QSPR correlations using support vector machines, radial basis function neural networks, and multiple linear regression. J. Chem. Inf. Comput. Sci. 2004;44:1257–1266. doi: 10.1021/ci049965i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Bullmore E.T., Sporns O. Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Tu E., Kasabov N., Yang J. Mapping temporal variables into the NeuCube for improved pattern recognition, predictive modeling, and understanding of stream data. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. Learn. Syst. 2016;28:1305–1317. doi: 10.1109/TNNLS.2016.2536742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Talairach J., Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Thieme; New York, NY, USA: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Liao X., Vasilakos A.V., He Y. Small-world human brain networks: Perspectives and challenges. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;77:286–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Masquelier T., Guyonneau R., Thorpe S.J. Competitive STDP-based spike pattern learning. Neural Comput. 2009;21:1259–1276. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.06-08-804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Casula E.P., Pellicciari M.C., Picazio S., Caltagirone C., Koch G. Spike-timing-dependent plasticity in the human dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2016;143:204–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Van Schaik A., Liu S.-C. AER EAR: A Matched Silicon Cochlea Pair with Address Event Representation Interface. IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. I Regul. Pap. 2005;54:48–59. doi: 10.1109/iscas.2005.1465560. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Beck A.T., Alford B.A. Depression: Causes and Treatment. University of Pennsylvania Press; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 91.Beaufort I.N., Oene G.H.D.W.-V., Buwalda V.A., De Leeuw J.R.J., Goudriaan A.E. The depression, anxiety and stress scale (DASS-21) as a screener for depression in substance use disorder in patients: A pilot study. Eur. Addict. Res. 2017;23:260–268. doi: 10.1159/000485182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Gardner M.W., Dorling S.R. Artificial neural networks (the multilayer perceptron)—A review of applications in the atmospheric sciences. Atmos. Environ. 1998;32:2627–2636. doi: 10.1016/S1352-2310(97)00447-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Costantini G., Todisco M., Casali D., Carota M., Saggio G., Bianchi L., Abbafati M., Quitadamo L.R. SVM Classification of EEG Signals for Brain Computer Interface; Proceedings of the 19th Italian Workshop on Neural Nets, Vietri sul Mare; Salerno, Italy. 28–30 May 2009; pp. 229–233. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Singh-Curry V., Husain M. The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1434–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Bledowski C. Localizing P300 generators in visual target and distractor processing: A combined event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:9353–9360. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1897-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Salisbury D.F., Griggs C.B., Shenton M.E., McCarley R.W. The NoGo P300 ‘anteriorization’effect and response inhibition. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115:1550–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Goldstein A., Spencer K.M., Donchin E. The influence of stimulus deviance and novelty on the P300 and novelty P3. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:781–790. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3960781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Courchesne E., A Hillyard S., Galambos R. Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1975;39:131–143. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(75)90003-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Li B.-J., Friston K., Mody M., Wang H.-N., Lu H., Hu D.-W. A brain network model for depression: From symptom understanding to disease intervention. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 2018;24:1004–1019. doi: 10.1111/cns.12998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Lao S.-A., Kissane D., Meadows G. Cognitive effects of MBSR/MBCT: A systematic review of neuropsychological outcomes. Conscious. Cogn. 2016;45:109–123. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Jensen C.G., Vangkilde S., Frokjaer V., Hasselbalch S.G. Mindfulness training affects attention—Or is it attentional effort? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012;141:106–123. doi: 10.1037/a0024931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Gill L.-N., Renault R., Campbell E., Rainville P., Khoury B. Mindfulness induction and cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Conscious. Cogn. 2020;84:102991. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.102991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Sevinc G., Hölzel B.K., Hashmi J., Greenberg J., McCallister A., Treadway M., Schneider M.L., Dusek J.A., Carmody J., Lazar S.W. Common and dissociable neural activity after mindfulness-based stress reduction and relaxation response programs. Psychosom. Med. 2018;80:439–451. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Anderson N.D., Lau M.A., Segal Z.V., Bishop S.R. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2007;14:449–463. doi: 10.1002/cpp.544. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Vega B.R., Melero J., Pérez C.B., Cebolla S., Mira J., Valverde C., Fernández-Liria A. Impact of mindfulness training on attentional control and anger regulation processes for psychotherapists in training. Psychother. Res. 2013;24:202–213. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2013.838651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Delgado-Pastor L.C., Perakakis P., Subramanya P., Telles S., Vila J. Mindfulness (Vipassana) meditation: Effects on P3b event-related potential and heart rate variability. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013;90:207–214. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Garrison K.A., Scheinost D., Constable R.T., Brewer J.A. BOLD signal and functional connectivity associated with loving kindness meditation. Brain Behav. 2014;4:337–347. doi: 10.1002/brb3.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Zeidan F., Baumgartner J.N., Coghill R. The neural mechanisms of mindfulness-based pain relief: A functional magnetic resonance imaging-based review and primer. Pain Rep. 2019;4:e759. doi: 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]