Visual Abstract
Keywords: acoustic startle, dominance, dopamine receptor, raphe, serotonin, sex differences
Abstract
Brain networks underlying states of social and sensory alertness are normally adaptive, influenced by serotonin and dopamine (DA), and abnormal in neuropsychiatric disorders, often with sex-specific manifestations. Underlying circuits, cells, and molecules are just beginning to be delineated. Implicated is a subtype of serotonergic neuron denoted Drd2-Pet1, distinguished by expression of the type-2 DA receptor (Drd2) gene, inhibited cell-autonomously by DRD2 agonism in slice, and, when constitutively silenced in male mice, affects levels of defensive and exploratory behaviors (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Unknown has been whether DRD2 signaling in these Pet1 neurons contributes to their capacity for shaping defensive behaviors. To address this, we generated mice in which Drd2 gene sequences were deleted selectively in Pet1 neurons. We found that Drd2Pet1-CKO males, but not females, demonstrated increased winning against sex-matched controls in a social dominance assay. Drd2Pet1-CKO females, but not males, exhibited blunting of the acoustic startle response, a protective, defensive reflex. Indistinguishable from controls were auditory brainstem responses (ABRs), locomotion, cognition, and anxiety-like and depression-like behaviors. Analyzing wild-type Drd2-Pet1 neurons, we found sex-specific differences in the proportional distribution of axonal collaterals, in action potential (AP) duration, and in transcript levels of Gad2, important for GABA synthesis. Drd2Pet1-CKO cells displayed sex-specific differences in the percentage of cells harboring Gad2 transcripts. Our results suggest that DRD2 function in Drd2-Pet1 neurons is required for normal defensive/protective behaviors in a sex-specific manner, which may be influenced by the identified sex-specific molecular and cellular features. Related behaviors in humans too show sex differences, suggesting translational relevance.
Significance Statement
A subtype of dorsal raphe (DR) serotonergic neuron, denoted Drd2-Pet1, is poised for regulation by dopamine (DA) via type-2 DA receptor (DRD2) expression. Functional removal of DRD2 in these cells through a conditional knockout (CKO) mouse strategy resulted in sex-specific behavioral abnormalities: Drd2Pet1-CKO females exhibited reduced acoustic startle while males showed increased social dominance. Drd2-Pet1 neurons were similar in number and distribution in males versus females but exhibited sex-specific differences in neurotransmission-related mRNAs, action potential (AP) duration, and relative distribution of collaterals. Abnormalities in sensory processing and social behaviors akin to those reported here manifest in autism, schizophrenia, and posttraumatic stress disorder, in sex-specific ways. Our findings, thus, may point to novel circuits and modulatory pathways relevant to human neuropsychiatric conditions.
Introduction
The serotonergic and dopaminergic neurotransmitter systems are known for their influence on and maladaptation in neuropsychiatric disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder, autism spectrum disorder, and schizophrenia. Clinical and animal studies implicate serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine; 5-HT) and dopamine (DA) in modulation of endophenotypes common to neuropsychiatric disorders, such as altered social interaction and sensory processing (Geyer and Braff, 1987; Meincke et al., 2004; Takahashi and Kamio, 2018). Transcriptome data coupled with structure-function maps in mice show that the serotonergic and dopaminergic neuronal systems are themselves heterogeneous, comprised of functionally specialized neuronal subtypes, manifesting distinct mRNA profiles, efferent projections, electrophysiological properties, and functions (Jensen et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2009; Crawford et al., 2013; Lammel et al., 2014; Spaethling et al., 2014; Okaty et al., 2015; Deneris and Gaspar, 2018; Poulin et al., 2018, 2020; Huang et al., 2019; Ren et al., 2019; Okaty et al., 2020). An important subtype of serotonergic neuron as relates to social and defensive behaviors is denoted Drd2-Pet1 (Niederkofler et al., 2016), identified by expression of the type-2 DA receptor (Drd2) gene and the serotonergic transcription factor gene Pet1 (aka Fev). DRD2 agonism in slice preparation drove outward (inhibitory) currents cell-autonomously in Drd2-Pet1 neurons, suppressing their excitability; and when these cells were constitutively silenced in male mice, i.e., exocytic neurotransmitter release was cell autonomously blocked, defensive, aggressive, and exploratory behaviors increased (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Here, we query whether Drd2 expression in Drd2-Pet1 cells contributes to the modulation of defensive, exploratory behaviors.
While Drd2 is expressed in many cell types throughout the midbrain and basal forebrain, expression in serotonergic neurons is restricted to a small subset of cells resident in the dorsal raphe (DR) nucleus. In these serotonergic neurons, Drd2 expression initiates around adolescence and continues through adulthood, at which point, Drd2 transcripts are the major DA receptor mRNA detected (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Thus, Drd2-Pet1 neurons come under DRD2 and presumably DA regulation during the developmental transition to sexual maturity. Drd2-Pet1 neurons project to brain regions involved in sensory processing, defensive, and mating behaviors including auditory brainstem regions and the sexually dimorphic medial preoptic area (mPOA; Niederkofler et al., 2016). These findings led us to hypothesize that DRD2 signaling in Drd2-Pet1 neurons contributes to social and sensory alertness and defensive behavior in a sex-specific manner.
Indeed, serotonergic and dopaminergic perturbations affect social and defensive behaviors differently in male versus female rodents. Decreases in serotonergic tone associate with increased levels of aggression in males (Brown et al., 1982; Hendricks et al., 2003; Yu et al., 2014; Niederkofler et al., 2016). By contrast, lesions of the serotonergic DR in female rats decreased maternal aggression (Holschbach et al., 2018), while DR serotonergic neuron activity in female, but not male, hamsters associates with social dominance (Terranova et al., 2016). The acoustic startle reflex (ASR), an evolutionarily-conserved, defensive reflex to loud, potentially threatening stimuli (Davis et al., 1982), also shows sex-specific differences within the context of altered 5-HT levels. Reduction in 5-HT levels enhanced ASR in female but not male rats (Pettersson et al., 2016). With respect to DA, deletion of the DA re-uptake transporter gene (Dat) altered ASR only in male mice (Ralph et al., 2001). Genetic removal of the soluble form of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), important for degradation of DA, enhanced the ASR and dominance behaviors in both sexes, but ASR especially in males (Tammimäki et al., 2010). Thus, serotonergic and dopaminergic neuronal systems influence social behaviors and sensory processing in sex-specific ways.
Here, we queried whether Drd2 conditional deletion in serotonergic neurons would alter aggression and social dominance behavior in males. Further, we sought to examine the role of Drd2 expression in serotonergic neurons in females with the hypothesis that other sensory or defensive behaviors would be affected, given typical lack of aggression in female mice (Lonstein and Gammie, 2002). We undertook a phenotypic analysis of mice in which we engineered Drd2 gene deletion selectively in ePet1-cre-expressing serotonergic neurons (Drd2Pet1-CKO mice). Here, we report that Drd2Pet1-CKO males exhibited increased social dominance whereas females displayed a robust decrease in ASR. We also investigated sex differences in Drd2-Pet1 neurons at the molecular, cellular, and circuit levels, identifying differences in candidate mRNA levels, electrophysiological properties, and relative distribution densities of axonal collaterals.
Materials and Methods
Ethical approval
All experimental protocols were approved by Harvard University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (IACUC) and were in accordance with the animal care guidelines of the National Institutes of Health.
Experimental animals
Mice were housed in a temperature-controlled environment on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to standard mouse chow and water. All experimental animals were virgins. For conditional knockout of Drd2, double transgenic mice of the genotype ePet-Cre;Drd2loxP/loxP (referred to as Drd2Pet1-CKO) were generated by crossing BAC transgenic ePet-Cre (Scott et al., 2005; Jax #012712) males to homozygous Drd2loxP/loxP (Bello et al., 2011; Jax #020631) females. From these crosses, ePet-Cre;Drd2loxP/wild-type males were then bred to homozygous Drd2loxP/loxP females for ePet-Cre;Drd2loxP/loxP male and female offspring used for experiments. Experimental controls were littermates with the Drd2loxP/loxP genotype thus negative for Cre but of comparable genetic background (C57BL/6J, Jax #000664). For Drd2-Pet1 neuron cell counts, triple transgenic Drd2-Cre;Pet1-Flpe;RC-FrePe (Gong et al., 2007; Jensen et al., 2008; Brust et al., 2014; RC-FrePe Jax #029486) were generated by crossing Drd2-Cre females to Pet1-Flpe;RC-FrePe double transgenic males. Likewise for axonal projection mapping, Drd2-Cre; Pet1-Flpe; RC-FPSit (RC-FPSit Jax #030206) triple transgenic mice were generated by crossing Drd2-Cre females to Pet1-Flpe;RC-FPSit double transgenic males. For both RC-FrePe and RC-FPSit crosses, all animals of each sex were from separate litters, though males and females from the same litter were used when possible. Genotypes were determined as previously described (Brust et al., 2014). Number of animals used for each assay is listed under the description for each assay.
Immunohistochemistry
Mice were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane and immediately perfused intracardially with PBS followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were extracted, postfixed in 4% PFA overnight at 4°C, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose/PBS for 48 h, and embedded in OCT compound (Tissue-Tek). Coronal sections were cryosectioned as 30-μm free-floating sections then rinsed three times with PBS for 10 min, blocked in 5% normal donkey serum (NDS; Jackson ImmunoResearch) and permeabilized with 0.1% Triton X-100 in PBS for 1 h at room temperature. Sections were incubated for 24–48 h in primary antibodies in the same blocking buffer at 4°C. Primary antibodies used were goat polyclonal anti-5-HT (1:1000, catalog #ab66047; Abcam), chicken polyclonal anti-GFP (1:2000, RRID: AB_2307313; AVES), rabbit polyclonal anti-DsRed (1:1000; catalog #632496; Clontech), and rabbit anti-GABA (1:500, catalog #A2052; Sigma). Following primary antibody incubation, sections were rinsed three times with PBS for 10 min and incubated in secondary antibody (Alexa Fluor 488 donkey anti-chicken IgY, 703-545-155, Jackson ImmunoResearch; Alexa Fluor 546 donkey anti-rabbit IgG, A10040, Invitrogen; Alexa Fluor 647 donkey anti-goat IgG, A-21447, Invitrogen) for 1 h at room temperature, rinsed three times with PBS for 10 min, then mounted using ProLong Gold Antifade Mountant (P36930, LifeTechnologies). For Drd2-Pet1 neuron cell counts, GFP+ cells were counted in every sixth section. The resulting number was multiplied by 6 to obtain the number of Drd2-Pet1 cells per animal.
Dual immunohistochemistry and fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH)
For dual in situ hybridization with immunostaining for GFP+ Drd2-Pet1 neuron cell bodies, PFA-perfused brain tissue from adult Drd2-Cre;Pet1-Flpe;RC-FrePe mice was collected as described above but cryosectioned at 20 μm onto slides (Superfrost Plus, catalog #48311-703, VWR), slides were warmed on a slide warmer set to 45°C for 30 min, and processed with RNAscope Multiplex Fluorescent Assay kit (Advanced Cell Diagnostics) following manufacturer’s protocol with the exception that at the end of the protocol, tissue was stained for anti-GFP, as described above, similar to Shrestha et al. (2018). The following probes were used for the dual protocol: Dmd (catalog #561551-C3), Drd2-E2 (catalog #486571-C2), Gad2 (catalog #439371-C2), and Serpini1 (catalog #501441). Cell nuclei were visualized with 4’,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI).
FISH
For FISH validation of Drd2 conditional knockout and Gad2 expression analysis, adult Drd2Pet1-CKO or control brain tissue was fresh frozen in OCT (TissueTek) and cryosectioned at 16 μm onto slides (Superfrost Plus, catalog #48311-703, VWR) and then processed with RNAscope Multiplex Fluorescent Assay kit (Advanced Cell Diagonstics) following manufacturer’s protocol for fresh frozen tissue. The following probes were used: Drd2-E2 (catalog #486571-C2), Drd2-O4 (Exon7/8; catalog #534241), Fev (Pet1) (catalog #413241-C3), Gad2 (catalog #439371-C2), Tph2 (catalog #318691), and cre (catalog #312281). Cell nuclei were visualized with DAPI.
Image collection
All images were acquired on a Nikon Ti inverted spinning disk confocal microscope with 488-, 561-, 647-nm laser lines and Andor Zyla 4.2 Plus sCMOS monochrome camera. Images were acquired with Nikon Elements Acquisition software AR 5.02. For RNA quantification and Drd2Pet1-CKO validation experiments, four images were taken of brain slices containing the DR: the first directly ventral to the aqueduct then one field of view below and to the left and right to capture each lateral wing.
FISH quantification
Quantification was conducted blind to sex and genotype. For Drd2Pet1-CKO validation, all Pet1+ (serotonergic) neurons within each image were identified, then the viewer outlined the DAPI-stained nuclei of each Pet1+ neuron and scored the presence of Drd2 puncta as “positive” (having puncta) or “negative” (no puncta).The total number of Drd2+ Pet1+ neurons was then divided by the total number of Pet1+ neurons to yield the “% Drd2+Pet1+ neurons.”
For quantification of Dmd, Drd2, Gad2, and Serpini1 manual counting of each mRNA punctum per cell was conducted by a trained viewer. All cells counted fit the criteria of GFP+ with a DAPI+ nucleus. The viewer outlined the GFP+ cell body in FIJI (https://Fiji.sc/; Schindelin et al., 2012) while only viewing that channel and then counted the number of distinct RNA puncta within that cell outline. Brain sections sampled were from five males and five female animals.
For quantification of Drd2-Exon7/8 and Gad2 puncta in Drd2Pet1-CKO tissue, DR sections corresponded to interaural −0.80 to 1.04 mm and bregma −4.60 to – 4.84 mm based on DAPI staining and anatomic landmarks (Franklin and Paxinos, 2008), where Drd2-Pet1 neurons are most enriched. A series of custom FIJI scripts and a CellProfiler (McQuin et al., 2018) pipeline were used to process and analyze confocal images of RNAscope FISH signal in a semi-automatic manner. Analysis was performed in 2D on maximum intensity projections of 6-µm-thick z-stacks. First, a (step 1) preprocessing FIJI script separated channels and preprocessed them for (step 2) CellProfiler to use as input to segment nuclei. The DAPI-stained channel was preprocessed by a Gaussian blur with a diameter of 18 before segmenting with the IdentifyPrimaryObjects module with a diameter range 30–100 pixels using a minimum cross entropy global thresholding strategy. Objects outside of the diameter range or those on the edges were excluded. A threshold smoothing scale of 1.3488 was used and the image was automatically declumped based on intensity values. Finally, holes were filled in the resulting label map image, which was exported for use in FIJI (step 3). In FIJI, the user manually excluded misidentified objects or added additional nuclei that were missed by the automatic detection pipeline. A highly similar script was recently published (Okaty et al., 2020), though this current script performs additional difference of Gaussian (Marr and Hildreth, 1980) based filtering for each FISH channel. For each FISH probe, after background subtraction with a rolling ball radius of 50 pixels, the image was duplicated and a Gaussian blur was performed at two different σ levels, one which obscured small background pixels but preserved mRNA puncta, and a more extreme blur that only retained larger diffuse background puncta. The difference of these two images was then calculated and puncta localized using the Find Maxima function. To find appropriate settings for each FISH channel, we compared the performance of several sets of parameters to automatically detect puncta versus a hand count of puncta. We were able to achieve excellent concordance between the hand count and automatic puncta detection. Table 1 summarizes our settings and performance in a linear regression against the hand count for each FISH probe (statistics calculated in GraphPad Prism v8.4.3 and Microsoft Excel v2002).
Table 1.
Probe | S1 | S2 | Prominence | R2 | RMSE | MAE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
E2 | 0.25 | 1 | 175 | 0.8696 | 0.5957 | 0.2458 |
E7/8 | 0.5 | 1 | 100 | 0.9421 | 0.8054 | 0.35 |
Cre | 0.5 | 1 | 100 | 0.9679 | 3.829 | 2.2244 |
Fev | 0.25 | 2 | 75 | 0.9555 | 4.414 | 3.7047 |
Gad2 | 0.25 | 16 | 150 | 0.8568 | 2.804 | 1.7973 |
Summary of settings and performance in a linear regression for semi-automated protocol versus hand counts for each FISH probe.
Behavioral assays
All assays, except the resident-intruder assay, were conducted in an initial cohort of 15 control (eight males, seven females) and 11 Drd2Pet1-CKO (six males, five females) mice. All behavioral assays were conducted at postnatal day (P)90 or later. The run order for the initial cohort was open field, elevated plus maze, tail suspension test, forced swim test, social interaction, acoustic startle response, prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle, water T-maze, contextual fear conditioning, tube test of social dominance and rotarod. An additional cohort of 16 controls (seven males, nine females) Drd2Pet1-CKO(six males, 10 females) was run for acoustic startle response. Resident-intruder assay of aggression was conducted in three separate cohorts of mice totaling 24 control and 26 Drd2Pet1-CKO males. The tube test of social dominance was run in the initial cohort and in the second (eight control and Drd2Pet1-CKO males) and third (11 control and Drd2Pet1-CKO males) aggression cohorts for a total of 24 control and Drd2Pet1-CKO males and a separate cohort of 16 control and 18 Drd2Pet1-CKO females. The rotarod assay was also repeated in a separate cohort of males (seven controls, six Drd2Pet1-CKO). Experiments were conducted between zeitgeber time (ZT)6 and ZT10, with interspersion of control and experimental animals, and assays were run and analyzed by a trained experimenter blinded to genotype. The open field test, elevated plus maze, tail suspension test, forced swim test, social interaction, prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle, water T-maze, and contextual fear conditioning were performed as previously described (Niederkofler et al., 2016). All other behavioral assays are described in detail below.
Rotarod
The rotarod apparatus (Stoelting; Ugo Basile Apparatus) contains a rotating rod set to an accelerating speed. Mice are placed onto the rod and rotation of the rod begins. When a mouse loses its balance and falls, the apparatus automatically stops and measures the latency and rotating speed at which the mouse fell. Training consisted of exposing the mice to the apparatus for 5 min at a constant speed of 4 rpm. Mice that fall during the training session are placed back on the apparatus until the training session time has elapsed. An hour following the training session, mice are placed back on the rod for a 2-min session in which speed increases steadily over 2 min from 5 to 40 rpm. If a mouse does not fall during the 2 min, the trial ends at 2 min. Each animal was tested over 3 d and the latency to fall was averaged for each mouse. This assay was conducted in 21 control mice (14 males, seven females) and 18 Drd2Pet1-CKO (13 males, five females).
Acoustic startle response
Mice were placed in a perforated holder (acrylic cylinder with 3.2-cm internal diameter) that allowed movement to be monitored. Animal holders were placed on top of a transducer platform, measuring the active response to both weak and startle stimuli, adjacent to a speaker, within an individual acoustic chamber (Med Associates). Each session consisted of a 5 min acclimation period followed by 10 blocks of 11 trials each with white noise acoustic stimuli (20–120 dB). Each startle stimulus (20–120 dB, in 10-dB increments) was played once per block, in a quasi-random order with a variable intertrial interval of 10–20 s (average of 15 s). The duration of the stimulus was 40 ms. Responses were recorded for 150 ms from startle onset and are sampled every ms. Mice were placed back into the home cage immediately after testing. Males and female were run on different days. This assay was conducted in 30 control mice (14 males, 16 females) and 28 Drd2Pet1-CKO (13 males, 15 females), as two separate cohorts per sex.
Tube test of social dominance
Two age-matched (∼P90), weight-matched mice of the same sex are introduced into opposite ends of a clear PVC tube (30.5 cm in length with an internal diameter of 2.5 cm) allowing them to interact in the middle but not pass each other within the tube. The subordinate mouse will back out allowing the dominant mouse to pass through (Lindzey et al., 1961). For each pair, five consecutive trials were run with a maximum time of 2 min per trial. Trials ended when one mouse backed out of the tube such that all four limbs are outside of the tube which was then recorded as a “backout” for that mouse. Matches lasting >2 min were excluded from analysis and scored as a draw. Side of introduction to the tube were alternated between trials and the tube was cleaned with ethanol between each trial. Opponents were from different litters and had never been housed together. This assay was conducted in 24 Drd2Pet1-CKO males versus 24 control males and 23 Drd2Pet1-CKO females versus 23 control females, conducted across three cohorts of animals.
Resident-intruder assay
Drd2Pet1-CKO or control mice were group-housed with male siblings until adulthood (P90) when they were single-housed for one night in the test cage to establish territorial residency. On day 1, a five-week-old Swiss Webster (Charles River) male, the “intruder,” was introduced to the cage divided with a clear perforated divider for 5 min. After 5 min, the perforated divider was removed, and the mice could interact for 5 min, in which the encounter was video recorded. Number of attack bites were counted by a trained, blinded viewer. The intruder mouse was introduced for 3 d to obtain an average number of attack bites per day. The intruder mouse had a lower body weight than the resident male. This assay was conducted only in males, as female laboratory-reared mice do not display territorial aggression (Palanza, 2001; Lonstein and Gammie, 2002) This assay was conducted in 26 Drd2Pet1-CKO and 24 controls.
Auditory brainstem response (ABR)
ABRs were recorded in a separate cohort of adult mice (males: 10 control and seven Drd2Pet1-CKO; females: eight control and seven Drd2Pet1-CKO) aged P71–P102 to correspond to the age of animals in other assays. ABRs were conducted similar to (Maison et al., 2013). Mice were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (7.5 mg/kg) and placed in a soundproof chamber on a heating pad. Acoustic stimuli were delivered using EPL Cochlear Function Test Suite (CFTS) software and analyzed using ABR peak analysis software [1.1.1.9, Massachusetts Eye and Ear (MEE)]. All ABR thresholds, amplitudes, and latencies were read by an investigator blind to mouse genotype.
Electrophysiology
Slice preparation and whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were conducted as previously described (Rood et al., 2014; Niederkofler et al., 2016). Briefly, to assess membrane and action potential (AP) characteristics a protocol of repeated sweeps of 500-ms current injections stepping in 20-pA steps from −80 to 180 pA was administered to cells in current clamp. Data were analyzed using Clampfit (Molecular Devices). Some cells included in cell property analyses were also used to generate data on the function of DRD2 receptors in the DR (Niederkofler et al., 2016). However, the intrinsic cell properties data we present in this article have not been previously published and include cells not part of the Niederkofler et al. (2016) dataset.
Projection mapping
Brain tissue from six females and five males from different litters, but with a female and male from the same litter where possible, were collected at P90 and processed as previously described (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Target region identification was based on anatomic landmarks identified by DAPI staining, anti-choline acetyltransferase (goat polyclonal anti-ChAT,1:500, AB144P; EMD234 Millipore) staining, and/or anti-tyrosine hydroxylase (rabbit anti-TH, 1:1000, AB152, Millipore) staining. Staining and imaging protocols were identical among the eleven samples analyzed.
Quantification of target innervation
Target innervation was quantified in a similar manner to (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Briefly, image stacks were acquired bilaterally per brain region analyzed for each animal using a Nikon Ti inverted spinning disk microscope with a Plan Fluor 40×/1.3 Oil DIC H/N2 objective, 488-, 561-, and 647-nm laser lines, and Andor Zyla 4.2 Plus sCMOS monochrome camera. Images were acquired with Nikon Elements Acquisition software AR 5.02. Image stacks (.nd2 files) were imported to FIJI for analysis of axon projection area. Each stack contained 21 optical slices of 0.3 μm. Innervation density was quantified by a FIJI macro, such that all images, were treated identically, including background subtraction, thresholding and particle counting as described in (Niederkofler et al., 2016). We then divided the total area occupied by the projection signal by the total area of the 21 optical slices to obtain the percent area occupied by projection signal. This was then averaged within images of the same brain region across male or female samples. Brain regions analyzed were either those previously described to be innervated by Drd2-Pet1 neurons in males only (Niederkofler et al., 2016) or those involved in auditory processing and ASR.
Statistical analyses
Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Statistical analyses were conducted in GraphPad Prism version 8.1. Statistical significance was determined by unpaired t test between control versus Drd2Pet1-CKO groups or male versus female groups except where noted: open field, forced swim test, acoustic startle response, and ABR statistical significance was determined using two-way ANOVA. For the resident-intruder assay, the tube test of social dominance, and rotarod, statistical significance was determined using the non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test. A result was considered significant if p < 0.05. Detailed statistical results are reported in Table 2.
Table 2.
Behavior/experiment | Line | Data structure (normality) |
Type of test | Power | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Comparison | F/df | p | ||||
Validation of Drd2 CKO | 1E | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 4.514, df = 10 | p = 0.0011 |
Open field distance | 2A | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,24) = 0.6405 | p = 0.4314 |
F2, time | F(11,264) = 47.99 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(11,264) = 0.8441 | p = 0.5960 | ||||
Open field % distance traveled | 2B | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 1.781, df = 24 | p = 0.0876 |
Rotarod | 2C | No | Mann–Whitney, two-tailed | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | M–W U = 142 | p = 0.1899 |
Elevated plus maze (% time in open arm) | 2D | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 1.250, df = 24 | p = 0.2234 |
Tail suspension test | 2E | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.3485, df = 24 | p = 0.7305 |
Forced swim test | 2F | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,24) = 0.2678 | p = 0.6095 |
F2, time | F(5,120) = 8.916 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(5,120) = 0.3090 | p = 0.9067 | ||||
Contextual fear conditioning (baseline freezing) | 2G | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.6682, df = 24 | p = 0.5104 |
Contextual fear conditioning (test freezing) | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.0127, df = 24 | p = 0.9900 | ||
Water T maze (%correct during acquisition) | 2H | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,24) = 0.08249 | p = 0.7764 |
F2, time | F(4,89) = 50.12 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(4,89) = 0.6698 | p = 0.6147 | ||||
Water T maze (%correct during reversal) | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,24) = 0.1631 | p = 0.6899 | |
F2, time | F(4,96) = 172.4 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(4,96) = 1.477 | p = 0.2153 | ||||
ASR (M) | 3C | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,25) = 0.0840 | p = 0.7745 |
F2, dB | F(10,250) = 28.99 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(10,250) = 0.3037 | p = 0.9798 | ||||
ASR habituation (M) | 3D | Yes | Pearson r correlation | Control trial number × startle response | r = −0.195 | p = 0.5893 |
Drd2Pet1-CKO trial number × startle response | r = 0.136 | p = 0.7079 | ||||
ASR latency (M) | 3E | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,25) = 2.425 | p = 0.1319 |
F2, dB | F(10,250) = 21.67 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(10,250) = 0.4722 | p = 0.9071 | ||||
ASR (F) | 3F | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,29) = 13.26 | p = 0.0011 |
F2, dB | F(10,29) = 35.29 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(10,290) = 7.475 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
ASR habituation (F) | 3G | Yes | Pearson r correlation | Control trial number × startle response | r = 0.1171 | p = 0.7473 |
Drd2Pet1-CKO trial number × startle response | r = 0.05165 | p = 0.8873 | ||||
ASR latency (F) | 3H | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,29) = 0.3748 | p = 0.5452 |
F2, dB | F(10,290) = 20.59 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(10,290) = 1.058 | p = 0.3953 | ||||
PPI (M) | 3I | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,12) = 0.6625 | p = 0.4315 |
F2, prepulse dB | F(2,24) = 42.86 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(2,24) = 4.104 | p = 0.0293 | ||||
PPI (F) | 3J | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,10) = 0.6526 | p = 0.4380 |
F2, prepulse dB | F(2,20) = 31.34 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(2,20) = 1.609 | p = 0.2249 | ||||
ABR amplitude (M) | 4C | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,15) = 1.770 | p = 0.2032 |
F2, peak | F(2,30) = 59.09 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(2,30) = 1.059 | p = 0.3595 | ||||
ABR latency (M) | 4D | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,15) = 3.515 | p = 0.0804 |
F2, peak | F(2,30) = 1171 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(2,30) = 3.121 | p = 0.0587 | ||||
ABR threshold (M) | 4E | |||||
5.6 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.9535, df = 14 | p = 0.3565 | |
8 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 1.894, df = 14 | p = 0.0791 | |
16 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 1.103, df = 14 | p = 0.2887 | |
32 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 2.129, df = 7 | p = 0.0708 | |
ABR amplitude (F) | 4F | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,13) = 2.489 | p = 0.1387 |
F2, peak | F(2,26) = 72.52 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(2,26) = 0.0487 | p = 0.9525 | ||||
ABR latency (F) | 4G | Yes | Repeated-measures ANOVA | F1, genotype | F(1,13) = 0.0053 | p = 0.9430 |
F2, peak | F(2,26) = 4360 | p < 0.0001 | ||||
(F1 × F2) | F(2,26) = 0.0822 | p = 0.9213 | ||||
ABR threshold (F) | 4H | |||||
5.6 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.1566, df = 13 | p = 0.8770 | |
8 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.1592, df = 14 | p = 0.8757 | |
16 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.9600, df = 14 | p = 0.3533 | |
32 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 1.644, df = 9 | p = 0.1346 | |
Social interaction (M, %time with stranger) | 5A | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.6283, df = 12 | p = 0.5415 |
Social interaction (F, %time with stranger) | 5B | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | t = 0.9598, df = 10 | p = 0.3598 |
Resident-intruder assay | 5C | No | Mann–Whitney, two-tailed | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | M–W U = 289.5 | p = 0.6649 |
Tube test of social dominance | 5E | |||||
Male | No | Mann–Whitney, two-tailed | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | M–W U = 166 | p = 0.0065 | |
Female | No | Mann–Whitney, two-tailed | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO | M–W U = 253 | p = 0.8123 | |
Drd2-Pet1 neuron count | 6A | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.8160, df = 12 | p = 0.4304 |
Soma size | 6C | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.021, df = 8 | p = 0.3372 |
Gene expression | 6D | |||||
Dmd | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.9581, df = 7 | p = 0.3699 | |
Drd2 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.514, df = 8 | p = 0.1686 | |
Gad2 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 2.498, df = 8 | p = 0.0370 | |
Serpini1 | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.459, df = 7 | p = 0.1879 | |
%Gad2+ Drd2-Pet1 neurons | 6E | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.876, df = 8 | p = 0.0975 |
% Drd2-Exon7/8+ | 7B | Yes | Unpaired t test | Control vs Drd2Pet1-CKO with Pet1 probe | t = 0.1291, df = 10 | p = 0.8998 |
Yes | One-way ANOVA | Control/Pet1 probe vs Drd2Pet1-CKO/ Pet1 probe vs Drd2Pet1-CKO /Cre probe |
F(2,19) = 0.1003 | p = 0.9051 | ||
% Gad2 in Cre+ neurons | 7D | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 3.057, df = 8 | p = 0.0157 |
Gad2 punctae per cell | 7E | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.768, df = 8 | p = 0.1151 |
Nucleus area | 7F | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.9931, df = 8 | p = 0.3497 |
Resting membrane potential | 8A | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.2113, df = 61 | p = 0.8334 |
Membrane resistance | 8B | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = −0.4084, df = 61 | p = 0.6844 |
AP threshold | 8B | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.8197, df = 61 | p = 0.0737 |
AP amplitude | 8D | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = −1.0474, df = 61 | p = 0.2990 |
AP duration | 8E | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = −2.2583, df = 61 | p = 0.0275 |
AHP amplitude | 8F | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.350, df = 61 | p = 0.1821 |
Innervation densities | 9C | |||||
DPGi | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 1.285, df = 9 | p = 0.2308 | |
PAG | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.2398, df = 9 | p = 0.8158 | |
mPOA | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.1978, df = 9 | p = 0.8476 | |
DLG | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.07798, df = 9 | p = 0.9395 | |
mHb | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.6732, df = 9 | p = 0.5178 | |
PnC | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.7901, df = 9 | p = 0.4498 | |
IC | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.5350, df = 9 | p = 0.6056 | |
LL | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.9100, df = 9 | p = 0.3865 | |
SOC | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.9282, df = 9 | p = 0.3775 | |
CNC | Yes | Unpaired t test | Male vs female | t = 0.2997, df = 9 | p = 0.7712 |
Statistical values are provided for behavioral analyses of Drd2Pet1-CKO mice and comparison of Drd2-Pet1 neuron properties in male versus female mice. Figure numbers are included to reference corresponding graphs. Statistical analyses were conducted in GraphPad Prism version 8.1.
Results
Visualization of Drd2-Pet1 serotonergic neurons and the loss of Drd2 gene expression in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice
As our first step, we confirmed the anatomic distribution of Drd2-Pet1 neurons in the mouse brainstem, observing cell soma distributed across the rostral and lateral regions of the DR nucleus (Fig. 1A) as previously reported (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Drd2-Pet1 cells were marked by GFP expression in triple transgenic Drd2-Cre;Pet1-Flpe;RC-Frepe (Gong et al., 2007; Jensen et al., 2008; Brust et al., 2014) mice in which cells positive for both Cre and Flpe activity – here those cells having expressed Drd2 and Pet1 – have recombined the RC-FrePe intersectional reporter allowing GFP expression; Flpe recombination alone configured RC-FrePe to drive mCherry expression, thus marking the remaining Pet1+ (Drd2-negative) serotonergic neurons (Fig. 1A,B). As expected (Niederkofler et al., 2016), GFP+ Drd2-Pet1 neurons showed detectable 5-HT by immunostaining and Drd2 mRNA by fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) (Fig. 1C).
To query the behavioral requirement for Drd2 gene expression in Drd2-Pet1 neurons, we deployed the ePet-cre driver (Scott et al., 2005) to delete floxed Drd2 gene sequences (Bello et al., 2011), creating a functional null Drd2 allele selectively in Pet1 neurons (Fig. 1D), and then subjected these Drd2Pet1-CKO mice to behavioral phenotyping. Cre-negative, Drd2flox/floxlittermates served as controls. To confirm loss of Drd2 gene expression in Pet1 neurons, we analyzed Drd2Pet1-CKO and control Drd2flox/flox brain tissue sections by mRNA in situ hybridization using a probe designed to detect exon 2-containing Drd2 mRNA, as exon 2 was the floxed gene portion to be excised by Cre recombination; concomitant identification of serotonergic neurons was by detection of Pet1 transcripts (Fig. 1F,G). Robust loss of Drd2 expression was observed in serotonergic neurons in both male and female mice [15.23 ± 2.41% of Pet1+ neurons in the DR express Drd2 transcripts in controls (n = 6), consistent with prior findings, compared with 3.87 ± 0.73% in Drd2Pet1-CKOs (n = 6), p = 0.0011, unpaired t test; Fig. 1E]. The few residual Pet1+ cells harboring Drd2 transcripts likely reflects a limitation in cell capture by the ePet-cre driver. Reliable immunodetection to confirm the expected parallel loss of DRD2 protein in PET1 cells remains unavailable.
Behavioral assessments in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice and the detection of sex-specific sensory, defensive, and social behaviors
Having validated effective loss of Drd2 expression specific to Pet1 neurons in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice, next, we screened these mice for behavioral alterations in comparison to sibling control Drd2flox/flox (Cre-negative) mice. Locomotor behaviors were explored first because they are known to be influenced by serotonergic and dopaminergic manipulations (Baik et al., 1995; Gainetdinov et al., 1999; Holmes et al., 2003; Seo et al., 2019), and because motor alterations can affect performance in and interpretation of subsequent behavioral assays. Notably, we found no differences between Drd2Pet1-CKO versus control mice (males or females) in the locomotor behaviors reflected in the open field and rotarod tests, such as distance traversed (Fig. 2A) and location within the field (Fig. 2B), vertical rearing, length of time on the rotating rod (Fig. 2C), which reflects balance, coordination, physical conditioning, and motor-planning. Next, we explored measures of depression-like and anxiety-like behaviors, as they are altered in various 5-HT-pathway or DA-pathway mouse models and pharmacological manipulation of these neurotransmitter systems show positive clinical effect. (Lucki, 1998; Hendricks et al., 2003; Holmes et al., 2003; Grace, 2016). We observed no differences in performance in the elevated plus maze (Fig. 2D), tail suspension test (Fig. 2E), or forced swim test (Fig. 2F) in Drd2Pet1-CKO males and females compared with littermate controls. Additionally, contextual fear conditioning (Fig. 2G) and water T-maze acquisition and reversal (Fig. 2H) were not affected, suggesting no impairment of memory and learning in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice.
Because the serotonergic and dopaminergic systems are implicated in modulating the ASR (Davis and Aghajanian, 1976; Davis et al., 1980; Meloni and Davis, 1999, 2000a,b), we explored that next. The ASR is an evolutionarily conserved reflex involving rapid contraction of facial and skeletal muscles into a protective posture in response to a loud, threatening stimulus. We hypothesized that Drd2-Pet1 neurons modulate this response, given their dense projections to auditory brain regions (Niederkofler et al., 2016) and the observation that following acoustic startle, the activity of certain serotonergic neurons increases in the lateral wings of the DR (Spannuth et al., 2011), a location in which we find Drd2-Pet1 neurons. We measured startle responses to weak and startling stimuli ranging from 20 to 120 dB presented in a randomized order (Fig. 3A,B). Female Drd2Pet1-CKO mice showed a significant decrease in ASR magnitude in response to startle stimuli (n = 15 Drd2Pet1-CKO, n = 16 control littermates, p = 0.0011, two-way ANOVA; Fig. 3F). By contrast, the male Drd2Pet1-CKO cohort was indistinguishable from their male littermate controls (n = 13 Drd2Pet1-CKO, n = 14 control littermates, p = 0.7745, two-way ANOVA; Fig. 3C). To prevent habituation to the startle stimuli, the different stimulus intensities were presented in a quasi-random order with varied intertrial intervals (see Materials and Methods), and indeed, startle responses in late as compared with early trials were indistinguishable (shown at 110 dB, trial number is not significantly correlated with startle magnitude, males: controls, r = −0.1950 and Drd2Pet1-CKO, r = 0.1360; females: controls, r = 0.1171 and Drd2Pet1-CKO, r = 0.0517, Pearson correlation; Fig. 3D,G). Further, we observed no differences in latency to startle in either females or males (Fig. 3E,H). Females were of similar mass (controls: 32.117 ± 3.15 g vs Drd2Pet1-CKO: 37.2 ± 2.427 g, unpaired t test, p = 0.2031) regardless of genotype, thus differences in weight and its relative impact on transduction of the startle response via the piezoelectric platform were not a confound.
While Drd2Pet1-CKO females showed diminished response magnitudes to startling acoustic stimuli, they nevertheless expressed normal acoustic prepulse inhibition (PPI) whereby even the diminished response to startling acoustic stimuli (e.g., 120-dB stimuli) was further blunted proportionately when immediately preceded by a weak, non-startling stimulus (e.g., 65-, 75-, or 85-dB stimuli; Fig. 3I,J). Thus, sensorimotor gating, as measured by acoustic PPI, appeared relatively intact; the acoustic dysfunction instead centered on the ASR itself.
Having observed attenuation of the ASR in female Drd2Pet1-CKO mice, we assessed whether hearing was broadly disrupted as revealed by ABRs evoked by sound stimuli (Zhou et al., 2006). ABRs were recorded in response to pure tone stimuli at 5.6, 8, 16, and 32 kHz (n = 8 control females, 7 Drd2Pet1-CKO females, and 10 control males, 7 Drd2Pet1-CKO males). Across all these frequencies, the measured ABR waveforms (averaged ABR waveforms shown at 16 kHz at 80-dB SPL; Fig. 4A,B), peak amplitudes [shown for peaks 1–3 at 16 kHz at 80-dB SPL for males (p = 0.2032, two-way ANOVA) and females (p = 0.1387, two-way ANOVA); Fig. 4C,F], and latencies to peaks [shown for peaks 1–3 at 16 kHz at 80-dB SPL for males (p = 0.0804, two-way ANOVA) and females (p = 0.9430, two-way ANOVA); Fig. 4D,G] were indistinguishable between Drd2Pet1-CKO mice and littermate controls. As well, the ABR threshold to elicit a waveform was not significantly different between Drd2Pet1-CKO and control mice at 5.6, 8, 16, or 32 kHz (p > 0.05 at all frequencies, unpaired t test) in males (Fig. 4E) or females (Fig. 4H). Thus, hearing overall, as measured by ABR, appeared largely unaffected in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice.
ABRs were conducted in adult mice (ages P71–P102) to align with the age at which the other behavioral assays were performed. However at such ages, C57BL/6 mice, the strain background here, exhibit some age-related hearing loss at higher frequencies (Kane et al., 2012), which we saw here at 32 kHz with two control and three Drd2Pet1-CKO females and five control and three Drd2Pet1-CKO males. At all other tested frequencies, the ABRs were effectively normal for both genotypes, with one exception being a Drd2Pet1-CKO female that exhibited undetectable ABRs at 5.6 kHz, but otherwise normal responses at all other frequencies tested including 32 kHz. These findings at 32 and 5.6 kHz are likely independent of the ASR phenotype observed in females because all animals had normal hearing at 8 and 16 kHz, frequencies included in the white noise startle stimulus of the ASR test.
Next, we examined social behavior in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice using the three-chambered test of sociability (Moy et al., 2004) that measures preference to investigate a social stimulus (a novel “stranger” mouse inside a holder) as compared with an object (an empty holder). Drd2Pet1-CKO mice showed no alterations in sociability compared with controls and both control and Drd2Pet1-CKO spent significantly more time investigating the stranger than the object (Fig. 5A,B). Females of both genotypes displayed preference toward the social stimuli only for the first 5 min of the assay (Fig. 5B, white bars), while males displayed this preference throughout the 10-min assay. Similar sex differences in sustained preference for the social stimulus have been described in C57BL/6J mice (Netser et al., 2017).
We assayed intermale, territorial aggression in a separate cohort of mice using a resident-intruder assay. Females were not tested, as they have been shown to display low or no aggression in most forms of this assay (Palanza, 2001; Lonstein and Gammie, 2002). We observed no statistically significant difference in number of attack bites delivered to the intruder mouse by Drd2Pet1-CKO males (n = 26) compared with number of attack bites delivered to the intruder by controls (n = 24; Drd2Pet1-CKO: 4.07 ± 1.50 bites, controls: 1.77 ± 0.39 bites, p = 0.6649, Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 5C) noting, however, that four Drd2Pet1-CKO males displayed high levels of aggression.
To assay social dominance, we performed the tube test, which has relevance in females as well as males (Lindzey et al., 1961; van den Berg et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2017). Two mice are simultaneously released into opposite ends of a clear tube of sufficiently narrow diameter that prevents mice from passing by each other and instead requires that one back out for the other, more dominant “winning” mouse, to move forward (Fig. 5D). Drd2Pet1-CKO males won a higher percentage of trials against non-sibling, weight-matched, and genetic background-matched opponent males (shown as percent of trials won, Drd2Pet1-CKO: 65.83 ± 9%, n = 24; controls: 34.17 ± 9%, n = 24; p = 0.0065, Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 5E). By contrast, we observed no difference in percent of trials won by female Drd2Pet1-CKO mice as compared with female sibling controls (Drd2Pet1-CKO: 48.7 ± 8%, n = 23; controls 51.3 ± 8%, n = 23; p = 0.8123, Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 5E).
Drd2-Pet1 neurons in males versus females exhibit differences in candidate molecular and biophysical properties but not in cell number
Given these sex-specific differences in behaviors observed in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice, next we looked for sex-specific differences in Drd2-Pet1 cellular properties, beginning with cell number. Analyzing triple transgenic Drd2-Cre; Pet1-Flpe; RC-FrePe males versus females, we found no difference in number of GFP+ Drd2-Pet1 neurons per brain (males: 410.40 ± 55.30 cells/brain, females: 313 ± 87.52 cells/brain, p = 0.4304, unpaired t test; Fig. 6A). Further, in both males and females, Drd2-Pet1 neurons distributed as expected across the rostral-caudal and medial-lateral axis of the DR.
To understand whether gene expression might differ between male and female Drd2-Pet1 neurons, we examined single-cell RNA sequencing data previously analyzed for expression of serotonergic pathway genes as validation that Drd2-Pet1 cells were indeed serotonergic (Niederkofler et al., 2016). Comparison across sex, albeit lacking statistical significance given the small sample size, highlighted four genes for further evaluation, Drd2, Dmd (encoding Dystrophin, a component of protein scaffolds in the CNS; Perronnet and Vaillend, 2010), Gad2 (encoding glutamate decarboxylase 2 involved in catalyzing the production of the neurotransmitter GABA), and Serpini1 (encoding the serine protease Neuroserpin, important for synapse formation and plasticity; Galliciotti and Sonderegger, 2006). Quantitative in situ mRNA detection using dual FISH with immunodetection on tissue sections from Drd2-Cre;Pet1-Flpe;RC-FrePe mice revealed greater abundance of average Gad2 transcripts (puncta) per cell in males versus females [Gad2: 20.46 ± 2.243 in males (n = 5) vs 12.20 ± 2.427 in females (n = 5), p = 0.0370, unpaired t test; Fig. 6D]. There was no difference in the percentage of Drd2-Pet1 neurons expressing Gad2 in male versus female mice (Fig. 6E). No difference in soma size (GFP-stained cell body) was observed between males and females suggesting that transcript differences were not because of larger soma volume measured (Fig. 6C). No significant differences in mRNA abundance were observed between males and females for Dmd, Drd2, or Serpini1 (see Table 2).
As a first step toward understanding whether sex-specific gene expression differences observed in wild-type mice persist or are altered in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice, we assessed Gad2 transcript levels in Drd2Pet1-CKO cells. In these cells, the floxed exon 2 of Drd2 is excised by Cre recombination. Therefore, to identify mutant Drd2 mRNA and thus the mutant Drd2Pet1-CKO cells, we used a multi-probe strategy involving one probe to intact downstream exons 7 and 8 (referred to here as Drd2-E7/8), another to exon 2 (referred to as Drd2-E2), and another to either cre or Pet1. We examined expression in the DR region most enriched with Drd2-Pet1 neurons. We found Drd2-E7/8+ puncta in Pet1+ cells in both controls and Drd2Pet1-CKO mice, whereas Drd2-E2+ puncta were detectable in control tissue but greatly reduced in Drd2Pet1-CKO as expected given the efficiency of Cre-mediated gene deletion (Fig. 7A; see Drd2-E2 quantification in Fig. 1E). Drd2-E7/8 puncta were detected in 35.97 ± 2.403% of Pet1+ cells in control mice (n = 6) compared with 36.53 ± 3.621% of Pet1+ cells in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice (n = 6; p = 0.8998, unpaired t test; Fig. 7B). Similarly, in a separate experiment using an in situ probe to cre mRNA, 34.91 ± 2.238% of cre+ cells expressed Drd2-E7/8 (n = 10 mice, one-way ANOVA compared with Pet1 control and Drd2Pet1-CKO cell expression, p = 0.9051; Fig. 7B). Next, we analyzed Gad2 mRNA transcript levels in Drd2Pet1-CKO cells (dual Drd2-E7/8+ and cre+ cells) in the DR (Fig. 7C). In males, we observed 87.44 ± 3.034% of Drd2Pet1-CKO cells were Gad2+, while this percentage was 75.76 ± 0.5862% in females (p = 0.0157, unpaired t test; Fig. 7D). In these Drd2Pet1-CKO cells, there were 14.25 ± 1.325 transcripts per cell in males and 10.13 ± 2.074 transcripts per cell in females (p = 0.1151, unpaired t test; Fig. 7E). Because of the tightly packed distribution of cells in the DR, puncta were measured only within cre+ DAPI-stained nuclei to ensure puncta were not assigned to more than one cell. The area of nuclei did not differ between males (114.9 ± 3.030 μm2) and females (110.9 ± 1.768 μm2, p = 0.3497, unpaired t test; Fig. 7F). Thus, in Drd2Pet1-CKO males as compared with Drd2Pet1-CKO females, a greater percentage of the Drd2-Pet1 cells harbored Gad2 transcripts; of these Gad2-expressing cells, however, transcript levels were not significantly different between males versus female Drd2Pet1-CKO mice.
To explore potential sex differences in electrophysiological properties characterizing Drd2-Pet1 neurons, we conducted whole-cell recordings from GFP-labeled Drd2-Pet1 neurons in brain slices from triple transgenic Drd2-Cre;Pet1-Flpe;RC-FrePe males and females. Examination of cell membrane characteristics revealed no sex differences in resting membrane potential (AP; Fig. 8A) or resistance (Fig. 8B). Analyses of AP characteristics revealed an increase in AP duration (Fig. 8E) in male Drd2-Pet1 cells as compared with female (2.847 ± 0.155 ms, n = 19 cells vs 2.54 ± 0.094 ms, n = 44 cells, respectively, p = 0.0275, unpaired t test], but no differences in AP threshold (Fig. 8C), amplitude (Fig. 8D), or afterhyperpolarization (AHP) amplitude (Fig. 8F).
Differing covariance in axonal collateral densities from Drd2-Pet1 neurons directed to auditory targets in males versus females
As a first step in exploring sex differences in Drd2-Pet1 neuron circuitry that may underlie the sex-specific behavioral phenotypes in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice, we compared relative innervation density to brain regions involved in sensory processing and social behavior in male and female mice. Boutons from Drd2-Pet1 neurons were selectively marked with a Synaptophysin-GFP fusion protein using triple transgenic Drd2-Cre;Pet1-Flpe;RC-FPsit mice (Fig. 9A,B; Niederkofler et al., 2016). At P90, the same age at which the behavioral assays were conducted, we collected brain tissue and quantified projections to the cochlear nucleus complex (CNC), superior olivary complex (SOC), lateral lemniscus (LL), inferior colliculus (IC), caudal pontine reticular nucleus (PNC; critical for ASR; Davis et al., 1982), dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN), mPOA, medial habenula (mHb), periaqueductal gray (PAG), and dorsal paragigantocellular nucleus (DPGi; shown as percentage of target area occupied by projections; Fig. 9C). We observed no significant sex differences in the cohort average for absolute innervation density to each of these 10 brain regions. However, because we observed considerable interanimal variability in bouton densities at targets, we next explored correlation of innervation density across brain regions (Weissbourd et al., 2014). Using pairwise correlations between auditory brain regions (Fig. 9D), we constructed a correlation matrix that shows positively correlated regions in green and negatively correlated regions in black (Fig. 9E). This visualization reveals that most auditory brain regions are positively correlated in males (SOC and LL, Pearson’s r = 0.89) with only the LL and cochlear nucleus being slightly negatively correlated (Pearson’s r = –0.28). Interestingly, a greater number of innervated regions were negatively correlated in females, including the CNC with both the SOC and the IC (r = −0.68 and r = −0.75, respectively), as well as PNC and IC (r = −0.67). The innervation of the PNC and SOC was significantly negatively correlated (r = −0.85, p = 0.033, two-tailed test). Further, we expanded analyses to include the dLGN, a region critical for visually-cued potentiation of the acoustic startle (Tischler and Davis, 1983), and found that in females innervation of the dLGN was not strongly correlated with innervation of auditory brain regions, while in males this dLGN innervation was highly negatively correlated with both the SOC (r = −0.91, p = 0.033, two-tailed test) and the IC (r = −0.91, p = 0.034, two-tailed test), indicating that Drd2-Pet1 neuron circuitry may be set up to modulate multisensory information differently in males compared with females.
GABA and 5-HT in Drd2-Pet1 neurons
Given detection of Gad2 mRNA in Drd2-Pet1 neurons, we probed for GABA versus 5-HT immunopositivity in cell soma versus axonal boutons in males versus females. Punctate GABA immunostaining was indeed detectable in some Drd2-Pet1 neuron soma (Fig. 10A) in both males and females. Yet, in all target brain regions examined, GABA was undetectable in the GFP-marked Drd2-Pet1 boutons. Shown are representative images from the SOC (Fig. 10B) and IC (Fig. 10C), noting a GABA-positive cell body in the IC (boxed) and GABA-positive staining in the corpus callosum serving as a positive control for GABA immunodetection (Fig. 10D). By contrast, 5-HT immunostaining in Drd2-Pet1 boutons was readily detectable (representative images from the SOC and IC; Fig. 9B).
Discussion
Strategy
We hypothesized that loss of Drd2 gene expression and associated DRD2 signaling normally observed in certain DR Pet1+ serotonergic neurons (Drd2-Pet1 neurons) could impair sensory, social, and/or defensive behaviors. We used the transgenic driver ePet-cre to delete functionally critical Drd2 gene sequences selectively in serotonergic neurons, thereby abolishing transcript and DRD2 protein function, which would normally initiate in Pet1 cells during adolescence. We validated these Drd2Pet1-CKO mice and examined behavioral responses. Further, we explored Drd2-Pet1 neurons themselves.
Main findings
Key findings include the following. (1) Sex-specific behavioral alterations were observed in Drd2Pet1-CKO mice. Females showed a dramatic diminution in the protective, defensive ASR as compared with Drd2flox/flox controls, while no differences were observed in males. (2) Drd2Pet1-CKO males, but not females, showed increased winning in the tube test of social dominance against sex-matched and age-matched controls. (3) No differences were observed in ABRs, in PPI of acoustic startle, locomotion, cognition, nor various affective behaviors. (4) No sex-specific differences were found in Drd2-Pet1 neuron number, soma distribution, nor in the set of efferent targets; however, within-animal correlations between efferent densities across target brain regions suggest differences by sex, thus hinting at sex-specific structural differences in Drd2-Pet1 neuronal circuitry. (5) Drd2-Pet1 cells in males as compared with females showed longer AP durations and higher levels of Gad2 transcripts (important for GABA synthesis); Drd2Pet1-CKO cells did not show a sex specific difference in Gad2 transcript levels, but the percentage of Drd2-Pet1 cells that were Gad2+ in Drd2Pet1-CKO males was slightly higher than in Drd2Pet1-CKO females. These findings, coupled with our prior work (Niederkofler et al., 2016) implicating Drd2-Pet1 neurons in setting levels of defensive aggressive and exploratory behaviors in male mice, suggest that Drd2-Pet1 neurons may serve as a specialized neuromodulatory interface whereby DRD2 signaling alters serotonergic neuronal activity to shape defensive, protective, and dominance behaviors in a sex-specific manner.
Protective ASR diminished in Drd2Pet1-CKO females
Defensive posturing in millisecond response to abrupt noise, be it a predator or other potential hazard, is a crucial evolutionarily conserved protective mechanism. Loss or blunting of this reflex can result in life-threatening exposure, while excessive enhancement can drive unnecessary, debilitating responses that preclude normal functioning. Thus, “tuning” of the ASR setpoint to social and environmental circumstances is likely critical for species survival and well-being. The observed ASR attenuation in female Drd2Pet1-CKO mice suggests that Drd2-Pet1 neurons and the regulation of their activity cell autonomously by DRD2 comprises a critical modulatory node for ASR in females. Further, this node appears separate functionally from that involved in acoustic sensorimotor gating, given that acoustic PPI appeared intact in Drd2Pet1-CKO females, and from hearing, given that ABRs were indistinguishable from controls. Thus, DRD2 signaling in Drd2-Pet1 neurons forms a functional circuit node specialized in female mice to influence startle to acoustic stimuli.
In rats, reduction of 5-HT through synthesis inhibition increases ASR in females, but not males (Pettersson et al., 2016). Predicted reciprocally is that elevated 5-HT levels might blunt ASR in females. Relating this to our findings, it is possible that Drd2-Pet1 neurons are more excitable in the absence of DRD2-mediated inhibition, resulting in increased 5-HT release, perhaps explaining the observed ASR blunting. In wild-type mice, this would predict that under conditions of DA elevation, for example through local DR DA neuron activity associated with arousal and vigilance (Cho et al., 2017), Drd2-Pet1 neuron activity would be inhibited, reducing 5-HT release and thereby tuning a more sensitive ASR, conferring a protective advantage.
The ASR circuit follows from cochlea to CNC to PNC to spinal motoneurons (Davis et al., 1982; Koch et al., 1992), and receives inputs from auditory centers such as the SOC, IC, and SC (Lauer et al., 2017). Drd2-Pet1 neurons innervate each of these areas and the PNC, and thus may impart modulation at multiple levels.
Tube test wins increased in Drd2Pet1-CKO males
The increased winning by Drd2Pet1-CKO males in the tube test suggests that loss of DRD2 results in an increase in or favoring of dominance behaviors, at least under these forced, one-on-one interaction conditions. We did not observe significant differences in levels of aggressive attack behaviors by Drd2Pet1-CKO males in a resident-intruder assay. Together, these findings suggest that in wild-type mice, DRD2 signaling in Drd2-Pet1 neurons contributes to tempering certain dominance behaviors under particular conditions.
Understanding how the present results align with our prior work remains a pursuit. In earlier studies using a resident-intruder assay, we observed an increase in various aggressive behaviors in mice in which Drd2-Pet1 neurons were constitutively silenced, which suggested to us that Drd2-Pet1 neuron excitation and neurotransmitter release would normally temper such behaviors (i.e., favor non-confrontational, even submissive behaviors). Because canonical DRD2 signaling is inhibitory and, as well, appears largely inhibitory in Drd2-Pet1 neurons in slice, we predicted that loss of DRD2 signaling would enhance Drd2-Pet1 cell excitability and neurotransmitter release probability, and thus would suppress or at least not enhance dominance behaviors. Yet Drd2Pet1-CKO males exhibited enhanced winning in the tube test. Perhaps DRD2 signaling in Drd2-Pet1 neurons results in cellular activity changes that ultimately lead to a tempering of one-on-one social dominance under some conditions, while extreme, constitutive Drd2-Pet1 neuron silencing is required to prompt the opposite, in the form of aggression escalation to an intruder. Indeed, other findings also support this notion that dominance by tube test does not necessarily correlate with aggression in a resident-intruder assay (Tammimäki et al., 2010). Differences might also be explained by whether the input conditions trigger Drd2-Pet1 neurons to release 5-HT versus GABA, should the latter prove a capability, noting that Drd2-Pet1 cells express Gad2, albeit we were unable to show GABA in Drd2-Pet1 boutons, only their soma.
Interestingly, a subset of Drd2Pet1-CKO males (four out of 26) did display increased levels of aggressive behaviors as compared with other Drd2Pet1-CKO mice and controls, suggesting there may be other influencing variables, yet unknown. This is plausible given that mice deficient for the long isoform of DRD2 (D2LR) are reported to show anxiety-like and depressive-like behaviors only following a stress-exposure paradigm (Shioda et al., 2019). Moreover, these stress-induced affective phenotypes in D2LR knock-out mice were abrogated by driving D2LR expression in DR Pet1+ serotonergic neurons (Shioda et al., 2019). Together these findings suggest that the behavioral role of Drd2 expression in Drd2-Pet1 neurons may be influenced by environmental factors.
Sex-specific differences in Drd2-Pet1 neuron properties
The observed sex-specific differences in Gad2 transcript levels in Drd2-Pet1 neurons may contribute to the sex-specific behavioral alterations exhibited by Drd2Pet1-CKO mice. Gad2 expression in Drd2-Pet1 neurons is in line with prior reports showing Gad2 expression more generally in the serotonergic DR (Nanopoulos et al., 1982; Calizo et al., 2011; Shikanai et al., 2012). It may be that Drd2-Pet1 neurons can release GABA as well as or instead of 5-HT under certain conditions or at particular targets. This capacity may differ in males versus females, given our observation that in males, Drd2-Pet1 neurons harbor higher levels of Gad2 mRNA. Interestingly, Drd2Pet1-CKO cells did not display this sex specific difference in Gad2 transcript, suggesting that Drd2 expression, or more broadly dopaminergic signaling in Drd2-Pet1 neurons, may affect Gad2 transcript levels. One potential model to be tested is if DRD2 signaling, in turn, alters levels of Gad2 expression to allow for neuronal release of GABA in addition to or instead of serotonin when behavioral or environmental conditions necessitate. Indeed, there is precedent for the differential usage of serotonin and glutamate by raphe serotonergic neurons (Liu et al., 2014; Kapoor et al., 2016; Sengupta et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2019), although GABAergic and serotonergic co-release has not been reported.
AP duration measured ex vivo was longer in male versus female Drd2-Pet1 cells; this may also confer neurotransmitter release properties that could contribute to behavioral differences. Additional studies are needed to determine how Drd2Pet1-CKO affects Drd2-Pet1 neuron electrophysiology, gene expression, or efferent targets. Such experiments may be achieved through crossing Drd2Pet1-CKO (ePet1-Cre;Drd2f/f) mice to Drd2-Flpo mice (The Jackson Laboratory strain #034419 provided by Bernardo Sabatini) along with an intersectional reporter transgene which would allow for dual Cre-mediated and Flp-mediated fluorescent labeling of mutant Drd2Pet1-CKO cells. While complex genetics, this strategy would enable mutant cell visualization for electrophysiology, single-cell RNA sequencing, and analysis of axonal projections.
In both males and females, Drd2-Pet1 neurons densely innervate auditory brainstem regions, likely modulating auditory-related processes at one or multiple of these sites. In examining Drd2-Pet1 efferents, we observed interanimal variability in regional innervation density. We speculate this may arise from subgroups within the Drd2-Pet1 neuron population that target different downstream structures. For example, some Drd2-Pet1 neurons may project specifically to the SOC while others might project specifically to the LL. If some animals have more of one subgroup than the other, averaging absolute innervation densities for each target region across all males and females may hide meaningful circuit structure. Covariance analysis of projection targets in each animal thus might hint at which brain regions come under shared regulation by Drd2-Pet1 neurons. In males, the high correlation between auditory region efferent densities suggests shared input from the same Drd2-Pet1 neurons. In females, the CNC/SOC, CNC/IC, SOC/PNC, LL/PNC, and IC/PNC combinations were more negatively correlated, suggesting there might exist a subgroup of Drd2-Pet1 neurons that targets the PNC and a different subgroup, the SOC. We speculate that in males, Drd2-Pet1 neurons contribute to a general level of serotonergic tone across the auditory brainstem, while in females, certain Drd2-Pet1 neurons selectively target and modulate specific nuclei.
In conclusion, we found that Drd2 gene expression in a specialized subset of Pet1 serotonergic neurons is required for certain defensive, dominance, and protective behaviors, involving auditory processing in a sex-specific manner. Deficits in sensory processing such as altered acoustic startle and impaired social communication and dominance behaviors manifest in human disorders including autism spectrum disorder, schizophrenia, and posttraumatic stress disorder, often in sex-specific ways (King et al., 2013; Steel et al., 2014; Matsuo et al., 2016; Thye et al., 2018) and with sex-specific differences in therapeutic outcomes (Franconi et al., 2007). The presented findings, thus, may point to novel circuit nodes of relevance to human neuropsychiatric disease.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank all S.M.D. laboratory members, including G. Maddaloni, B. Okaty, R. Dosumu-Johnson, K. Lehigh, and T. Asher, for discussions; O. Alekseyenko for discussion and behavioral assay support; K. Blandino, B. Menjivar, M. Rice, and J. Mai for technical support; B. Shrestha for auditory brainstem response assistance; and the Microscopy Resources on the North Quad (MicRoN) core at Harvard Medical School for microscopy support. Graphical abstract and tube test diagram created with BioRender.com. Most behavioral work was carried out in the NeuroBehavior Laboratory Core at Harvard Medical School, we thank the support of Director Dr. Barbara Caldarone.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Christie Fowler, University of California, Irvine
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: David Dietz, Mary (Molly) Heyer. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.
The authors have done an excellent job addressing the reviewers’ comments, and they should be commended for their detailed attention to statistical and imaging analyses. The discussion section has also been improved by their thoughtful additions.
Author Response
Re: Resubmission of manuscript eN-TNWR-0202-20X: Sex-specific role for dopamine receptor D2 in dorsal raphe serotonergic neuron modulation of defensive acoustic startle and dominance behavior.
We have uploaded a revised version of our manuscript eN-TNWR-0202-20x. The reviews were insightful and thoughtful; our addressing of each point has indeed raised substantially the manuscript’s scholarly level. We appreciate the opportunity to submit this revision, including the below detailed explanation of our responses.
Comments are addressed in the order by which they appeared in the eNeuro decision letter.
1. While the significance statement contained valid statements, it is not written grammatically correct and thus needs revision
Response: Thank you, the significance statement has been rewritten.
2. Synthesis Statement: The main concern pertains to the indication that tools are not available to examine sex-specific differences following Drd2 deletion in Drd2-Pet1 cells. However, a custom RNAscope probe specific to the mRNA with the deleted exon could be used for in situ colocalization studies - this could be done in a reasonable time frame with minimal additional animal usage. While other approaches (e.g., single cell sequencing, alternate breeding strategies) were discussed by the reviewers as having the potential to provide more enriched data sets, they recognized that these approaches would require much time/effort and could just be highlighted in the discussion as a potential future experiment.
Response: We appreciate the ideas and way of thinking embodied in the Synthesis Statement. Accordingly, we have followed up with additions - experimentally and in the text - to our revision that are in line with these points. Greater detail follows, point-by-point below.
3. As currently written, this manuscript appears to lack a mechanistic central hypothesis. This is not to say the data are not worthy of publication or that there is an inherent issue, but currently the overall importance of these findings is lost in what seems like a phenotyping endeavor. Indeed, I suspect that based on similar publications in 2016 that the authors had more of concept of why these findings fit well.
Response: Revisions now highlight the hypothesis more clearly; we thank the reviewers for their careful attention and suggestion.
4. The only methodical concern appears to be the use of genetically identical mice in the tube test. The appropriate measure would have been to utilize a control non- genetic mutant as one of the test subjects. Having said that, the data from this pairing is interesting and should not be excluded.
Response: Within the literature, there are multiple methods reported for conducting the tube test. We conducted ours in accordance with the Harvard NeuroBehavior Core’s instructions and similar to other papers that use wild-type littermate controls against a mutant mouse (in our case, a control mouse harboring the floxed Drd2 allele but not expressing Cre paired against the experimental Drd2Pet1-CKO opponent - noting that Drd2flox/flox mice do not have altered social dominance1). References 2 and 3 below use similar tube test methodology as employed here.
1. Daniela Noaín, M. Inés Pérez-Millán, Estefanía P. Bello, Guillermina M. Luque, Rodrigo Casas Cordero, Diego M. Gelman, Marcela Peper, Isabel García Tornadu, Malcolm J. Low, Damasia Becú-Villalobos, Marcelo Rubinstein. (2013). Central Dopamine D2 Receptors Regulate Growth-Hormone-Dependent Body Growth and Pheromone Signaling to Conspecific Males. Journal of Neuroscience, 33 (13) 5834-5842; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5673-12.2013
2. Zhou, Y., Kaiser, T., Monteiro, P., Zhang, X., Van der Goes, M. S., Wang, D., Barak, B., Zeng, M., Li, C., Lu, C., Wells, M., Amaya, A., Nguyen, S., Lewis, M., Sanjana, N., Zhou, Y., Zhang, M., Zhang, F., Fu, Z., & Feng, G. (2016). Mice with Shank3 Mutations Associated with ASD and Schizophrenia Display Both Shared and Distinct Defects. Neuron, 89(1), 147-162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.023
3. Wei-Hsiang Huang, David C. Wang, William E. Allen, Matthew Klope, Hailan Hu, Mehrdad Shamloo, Liqun Luo. (2018). Early adolescent Rai1 reactivation reverses transcriptional and social interaction deficits in a mouse model of Smith-Magenis syndrome. PNAS, 115 (42) 10744-10749; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1806796115
5. The n for some of the molecular endpoints is very low (i.e. 3)
Response: The N for RNAscope studies in wildtype tissue has been increased from 3 to 5.
6. Given the very large n for some of the behavioral experiments--did the authors note any influence on cycle in female mice as has been reported previously?
Response: We did not examine estrous cycle in females given that published findings using C57BL/6J (the genetic background of mice used in our studies) indicate: no effect of estrous cycle in C57BL/6J on the acoustic startle reflex (Meziane et al., 2007); no effect on open field, rotarod, and pre-pulse inhibition of acoustic startle, noting that tail suspension test did show increased immobility in the metestrus phase (Meziane et al., 2007); see also Palanza et al., 2001; Romeo et al., 2003 found no effect of estrous cycle in open field; Plappert et al., 2005 found no effect of estrous cycle for acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition.
7. Please include the Dorsal Raphe in the title.
Response: Thank you, the title has been changed to ‘Sex-specific role for dopamine receptor D2 in dorsal raphe serotonergic neuron modulation of defensive acoustic startle and dominance behavior’
8. The main weakness of the paper is that the role of Drd2 in serotonergic neurons is only explored in the context of sex-specific behavioral differences following conditional Drd2 deletion. Gene expression, electrophysiology, and connectivity data from this population of neurons are only studied in wild type mice, such that any findings cannot be directly linked to the sex-specific effects of Drd2 deletion. There are a wide range of sex-specific behavioral differences in WT mice, so the usefulness of these data as pertains to the behaviors in question is unclear
Response: Please see below (point #9).
9. The authors state that tools are not available to examine sex-specific differences following Drd2 deletion in Drd2-Pet1 cells, as the ePet-Cre driver used for deletion of Drd2 would activate reporter/effector/indicator alleles in all Pet1+ neurons. However, single cell sequencing could be performed after Drd2 deletion in this cell type, which would be demarcated by the mutated Drd2 mRNA missing the floxed exon, as long as the mRNA is not degraded/destabilized. Similarly, a custom RNAscope probe specific to the mRNA with deleted exon could be used for in situ colocalization studies. In addition, if the authors were to swap their original genetic strategy for reporter/effector expression in Drd2-Pet1+ neurons by using the Drd2-Flp line and Pet1-Cre line, reporter expression AND Drd2 deletion could be achieved in the same mice, thus allowing for more thorough analysis of sex-dependent differences in this population that could be linked directly to the sex differences in behavior.
Response: We thank the reviewers for their insights into probing gene expression in Drd2Pet1-CKO cells. In line with the synthesis statement, we pursued the use of a combination of RNAscope probes to identify Drd2Pet1-CKO (knockout) cells. Cre-mediated deletion removes exon 2 from the Drd2 gene and subsequent transcript, leaving more distal exons, such as 7 and 8, intact. Assuming little degradation of the generated mutant Drd2 transcript, we tested the strategy that CKO cells would be identifiable as hybridization-positive for probes for Drd2-Exon7/8 and for cre mRNA but not Drd2-Exon2; by contrast, wild-type cells would be positive for Drd2-Exon7/8 and Drd2-Exon2 probes. After rigorous validation of a Drd2-Exon7/8 RNAscope probe in both control and knockout tissue, we paired this probe with a cre-specific probe to identify ‘CKO cells’ in Pet1Drd2-CKO mice. We then analyzed CKO cells for altered levels of Gad2 mRNA, given our finding of increased expression in wild-type male versus female Drd2-Pet1 neurons.
The Reviewer suggestions involving single-cell RNA sequencing and alternative breeding strategies are valuable, creative, and elegant, have not eluded us, and are discussed in this revision as future directions, in line with the editor recommendation found in the Synthesis statement. These experiments involve extensive time, labor, and cost, putting them outside the scope of this submission. Entailed would be additional transgenes and entirely new breeding strategies. For example, single-cell RNA sequencing experiments would likely require the addition of another transgene (a Cre-dependent fluorescent reporter) to the Drd2Pet1-CKO genotype to allow for selecting serotonergic neurons, which otherwise would be poorly represented among brainstem cells given that they are few in number (see recent yields by Huang et al., eLife 2019). Moreover, within such a pool of manually- or On-Chip-sorted fluorescently marked serotonergic neurons (Okaty et al., 2020 eLife), those of the Drd2-Pet1 subtype are even fewer (∼400-500 neurons per animal). As well, deep sequencing is required to detect Drd2 transcripts, let alone mutant Drd2 transcripts needed as the key cell identifier.
Regarding the reverse recombinase strategy involving the Drd2-Flpo transgenic, we have indeed been working with that line, validating its specificity and thus appropriateness for the suggested experiments. Jackson Laboratories describes this line as, “Specificity is described as ‘imperfect’ by the donating laboratory.’ We look forward to continuing along this avenue of investigation, but at present, feasibility remains to be demonstrated.
10. The figure legend for Figure 5 has incorrect letters for the corresponding panel (E should be D, etc). Also, in panel E, the data points at the 100% mark appear to be cropped.
Response: Thank you, this figure has been updated.
References
- Baik JH, Picetti R, Saiardi A, Thiriet G, Dierich A, Depaulis A, Le Meur M, Borrelli E (1995) Parkinsonian-like locomotor impairment in mice lacking dopamine D2 receptors. Nature 377:424–428. 10.1038/377424a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bello EP, Mateo Y, Gelman DM, Noaín D, Shin JH, Low MJ, Alvarez VA, Lovinger DM, Rubinstein M (2011) Cocaine supersensitivity and enhanced motivation for reward in mice lacking dopamine D2 autoreceptors. Nat Neurosci 14:1033–1038. 10.1038/nn.2862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown GL, Ebert MH, Goyer PF, Jimerson DC, Klein WJ, Bunney WE, Goodwin FK (1982) Aggression, suicide, and serotonin: relationships to CSF amine metabolites. Am J Psychiatry 139:741–746. 10.1176/ajp.139.6.741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brust RD, Corcoran AE, Richerson GB, Nattie E, Dymecki SM (2014) Functional and developmental identification of a molecular subtype of brain serotonergic neuron specialized to regulate breathing dynamics. Cell Rep 9:2152–2165. 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.11.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calizo LH, Akanwa A, Ma X, Pan YZ, Lemos JC, Craige C, Heemstra LA, Beck SG (2011) Raphe serotonin neurons are not homogenous: electrophysiological, morphological and neurochemical evidence. Neuropharmacology 61:524–543. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho JR, Treweek JB, Robinson JE, Xiao C, Bremner LR, Greenbaum A, Gradinaru V (2017) Dorsal raphe dopamine neurons modulate arousal and promote wakefulness by salient stimuli. Neuron 94:1205–1219.e8. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawford LK, Rahman SF, Beck SG (2013) Social stress alters inhibitory synaptic input to distinct subpopulations of raphe serotonin neurons. ACS Chem Neurosci 4:200–209. 10.1021/cn300238j [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M, Aghajanian GK (1976) Effects of apomorphine and haloperidol on the acoustic startle response in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 47:217–223. 10.1007/BF00427605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M, Strachan DI, Kass E (1980) Excitatory and inhibitory effects of serotonin on sensorimotor reactivity measured with acoustic startle. Science 209:521–523. 10.1126/science.7394520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M, Gendelman DS, Tischler MD, Gendelman PM (1982) A primary acoustic startle circuit: lesion and stimulation studies. J Neurosci 2:791–805. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-06-00791.1982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deneris E, Gaspar P (2018) Serotonin neuron development: shaping molecular and structural identities. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Dev Biol 7:10.1002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franconi F, Brunelleschi S, Steardo L, Cuomo V (2007) Gender differences in drug responses. Pharmacol Res 55:81–95. 10.1016/j.phrs.2006.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G (2008) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates, Vol 3 New York: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Gainetdinov RR, Wetsel WC, Jones SR, Levin ED, Jaber M, Caron MG (1999) Role of serotonin in the paradoxical calming effect of psychostimulants on hyperactivity. Science 283:397–401. 10.1126/science.283.5400.397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galliciotti G, Sonderegger P (2006) Neuroserpin. Front Biosci 11:33–45. 10.2741/1778 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Braff DL (1987) Startle habituation and sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and related animal models. Schizophr Bull 13:643–668. 10.1093/schbul/13.4.643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gong S, Doughty M, Harbaugh CR, Cummins A, Hatten ME, Heintz N, Gerfen CR (2007) Targeting Cre recombinase to specific neuron populations with bacterial artificial chromosome constructs. J Neurosci 27:9817–9823. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2707-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace AA (2016) Dysregulation of the dopamine system in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and depression. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:524–532. 10.1038/nrn.2016.57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendricks TJ, Fyodorov DV, Wegman LJ, Lelutiu NB, Pehek EA, Yamamoto B, Silver J, Weeber EJ, Sweatt JD, Deneris ES (2003) Pet-1 ETS gene plays a critical role in 5-HT neuron development and is required for normal anxiety-like and aggressive behavior. Neuron 37:233–247. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01167-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes A, Yang RJ, Lesch KP, Crawley JN, Murphy DL (2003) Mice lacking the serotonin transporter exhibit 5-HT(1A) receptor-mediated abnormalities in tests for anxiety-like behavior. Neuropsychopharmacol 28:2077–2088. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holschbach MA, Vitale EM, Lonstein JS (2018) Serotonin-specific lesions of the dorsal raphe disrupt maternal aggression and caregiving in postpartum rats. Behav Brain Res 348:53–64. 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang KW, Ochandarena NE, Philson AC, Hyun M, Birnbaum JE, Cicconet M, Sabatini BL (2019) Molecular and anatomical organization of the dorsal raphe nucleus. Elife 8:e46464 10.7554/eLife.46464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen P, Farago AF, Awatramani RB, Scott MM, Deneris ES, Dymecki SM (2008) Redefining the serotonergic system by genetic lineage. Nat Neurosci 11:417–419. 10.1038/nn2050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane KL, Longo-Guess CM, Gagnon LH, Ding D, Salvi RJ, Johnson KR (2012) Genetic background effects on age-related hearing loss associated with Cdh23 variants in mice. Hear Res 283:80–88. 10.1016/j.heares.2011.11.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapoor V, Provost AC, Agarwal P, Murthy VN (2016) Activation of raphe nuclei triggers rapid and distinct effects on parallel olfactory bulb output channels. Nat Neurosci 19:271–282. 10.1038/nn.4219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JC, Cook MN, Carey MR, Shen C, Regehr WG, Dymecki SM (2009) Linking genetically defined neurons to behavior through a broadly applicable silencing allele. Neuron 63:305–315. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King MW, Street AE, Gradus JL, Vogt DS, Resick PA (2013) Gender differences in posttraumatic stress symptoms among OEF/OIF veterans: an item response theory analysis. J Trauma Stress 26:175–183. 10.1002/jts.21802 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch M, Lingenhöhl K, Pilz PK (1992) Loss of the acoustic startle response following neurotoxic lesions of the caudal pontine reticular formation: possible role of giant neurons. Neuroscience 49:617–625. 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90231-P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lammel S, Lim BK, Malenka RC (2014) Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system. Neuropharmacology 76 [Pt B]:351–359. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lauer AM, Behrens D, Klump G (2017) Acoustic startle modification as a tool for evaluating auditory function of the mouse: progress, pitfalls, and potential. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 77:194–208. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindzey G, Winston H, Manosevitz M (1961) Social dominance in inbred mouse strains. Nature 191:474–476. 10.1038/191474a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Z, Zhou J, Li Y, Hu F, Lu Y, Ma M, Feng Q, Zhang JE, Wang D, Zeng J, Bao J, Kim JY, Chen ZF, El Mestikawy S, Luo M (2014) Dorsal raphe neurons signal reward through 5-HT and glutamate. Neuron 81:1360–1374. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonstein JS, Gammie SC (2002) Sensory, hormonal, and neural control of maternal aggression in laboratory rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:869–888. 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00087-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucki I (1998) The spectrum of behaviors influenced by serotonin. Biol Psychiatry 44:151–162. 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00139-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maison SF, Usubuchi H, Liberman MC (2013) Efferent feedback minimizes cochlear neuropathy from moderate noise exposure. J Neurosci 33:5542–5552. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5027-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr D, Hildreth E (1980) Theory of edge detection. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 207:187–217. 10.1098/rspb.1980.0020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuo J, Ota M, Hori H, Hidese S, Teraishi T, Ishida I, Hiraishi M, Kunugi H (2016) A large single ethnicity study of prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia: separate analysis by sex focusing on effect of symptoms. J Psychiatr Res 82:155–162. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.07.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McQuin C, Goodman A, Chernyshev V, Kamentsky L, Cimini BA, Karhohs KW, Doan M, Ding L, Rafelski SM, Thirstrup D, Wiegraebe W, Singh S, Becker T, Caicedo JC, Carpenter AE (2018) CellProfiler 3.0: next-generation image processing for biology. PLoS Biol 16:e2005970. 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005970 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meincke U, Light GA, Geyer MA, Braff DL, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E (2004) Sensitization and habituation of the acoustic startle reflex in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 126:51–61. 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meloni EG, Davis M (1999) Enhancement of the acoustic startle response in rats by the dopamine D1 receptor agonist SKF 82958. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 144:373–380. 10.1007/s002130051020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meloni EG, Davis M (2000a) Enhancement of the acoustic startle response by dopamine agonists after 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the substantia nigra pars compacta: corresponding changes in c-Fos expression in the caudate-putamen. Brain Res 879:93–104. 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02753-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meloni EG, Davis M (2000b) Synergistic enhancement of the acoustic startle reflex by dopamine D1 and 5-HT1A agonists and corresponding changes in c-Fos expression in the dorsal raphe of rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 151:359–367. 10.1007/s002130000474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Perez A, Barbaro RP, Johns JM, Magnuson TR, Piven J, Crawley JN (2004) Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav 3:287–302. 10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nanopoulos D, Belin MF, Maitre M, Vincendon G, Pujol JF (1982) Immunocytochemical evidence for the existence of GABAergic neurons in the nucleus raphe dorsalis. Possible existence of neurons containing serotonin and GABA. Brain Res 232:375–389. 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90281-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Netser S, Haskal S, Magalnik H, Wagner S (2017) A novel system for tracking social preference dynamics in mice reveals sex- and strain-specific characteristics. Mol Autism 8:53 10.1186/s13229-017-0169-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niederkofler V, Asher TE, Okaty BW, Rood BD, Narayan A, Hwa LS, Beck SG, Miczek KA, Dymecki SM (2016) Identification of serotonergic neuronal modules that affect aggressive behavior. Cell Rep 17:1934–1949. 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.10.063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okaty BW, Freret ME, Rood BD, Brust RD, Hennessy ML, deBairos D, Kim JC, Cook MN, Dymecki SM (2015) Multi-scale molecular deconstruction of the serotonin. Neuron 88:774–791. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okaty BW, Sturrock N, Escobedo Lozoya Y, Chang Y, Senft RA, Lyon KA, Alekseyenko OV, Dymecki SM (2020) A single-cell transcriptomic and anatomic atlas of mouse dorsal raphe Pet1 neurons. Elife 9:e55523 10.7554/eLife.55523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palanza P (2001) Animal models of anxiety and depression: how are females different? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:219–233. 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00010-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perronnet C, Vaillend C (2010) Dystrophins, utrophins, and associated scaffolding complexes: role in mammalian brain and implications for therapeutic strategies. J Biomed Biotechnol 2010:849426. 10.1155/2010/849426 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettersson R, Hagsäter SM, Eriksson E (2016) Serotonin depletion eliminates sex differences with respect to context-conditioned immobility in rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 233:1513–1521. 10.1007/s00213-016-4246-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin JF, Caronia G, Hofer C, Cui Q, Helm B, Ramakrishnan C, Chan CS, Dombeck DA, Deisseroth K, Awatramani R (2018) Mapping projections of molecularly defined dopamine neuron subtypes using intersectional genetic approaches. Nat Neurosci 21:1260–1271. 10.1038/s41593-018-0203-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin JF, Gaertner Z, Moreno-Ramos OA, Awatramani R (2020) Classification of midbrain dopamine neurons using single-cell gene expression profiling approaches. Trends Neurosci 43:155–169. 10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralph RJ, Paulus MP, Fumagalli F, Caron MG, Geyer MA (2001) Prepulse inhibition deficits and perseverative motor patterns in dopamine transporter knock-out mice: differential effects of D1 and D2 receptor antagonists. J Neurosci 21:305–313. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-01-00305.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ren J, Isakova A, Friedmann D, Zeng J, Grutzner SM, Pun A, Zhao GQ, Kolluru SS, Wang R, Lin R, Li P, Li A, Raymond JL, Luo Q, Luo M, Quake SR, Luo L (2019) Single-cell transcriptomes and whole-brain projections of serotonin neurons in the mouse dorsal and median raphe nuclei. Elife 8:e49424 10.7554/eLife.49424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rood BD, Calizo LH, Piel D, Spangler ZP, Campbell K, Beck SG (2014) Dorsal raphe serotonin neurons in mice: immature hyperexcitability transitions to adult state during first three postnatal weeks suggesting sensitive period for environmental perturbation. J Neurosci 34:4809–4821. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1498-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A (2012) Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods 9:676–682. 10.1038/nmeth.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott MM, Wylie CJ, Lerch JK, Murphy R, Lobur K, Herlitze S, Jiang W, Conlon RA, Strowbridge BW, Deneris ES (2005) A genetic approach to access serotonin neurons for in vivo and in vitro studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:16472–16477. 10.1073/pnas.0504510102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sengupta A, Bocchio M, Bannerman DM, Sharp T, Capogna M (2017) Control of amygdala circuits by 5-HT neurons via 5-HT and glutamate cotransmission. J Neurosci 37:1785–1796. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2238-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seo C, Guru A, Jin M, Ito B, Sleezer BJ, Ho YY, Wang E, Boada C, Krupa NA, Kullakanda DS, Shen CX, Warden MR (2019) Intense threat switches dorsal raphe serotonin neurons to a paradoxical operational mode. Science 363:538–542. 10.1126/science.aau8722 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shikanai H, Yoshida T, Konno K, Yamasaki M, Izumi T, Ohmura Y, Watanabe M, Yoshioka M (2012) Distinct neurochemical and functional properties of GAD67-containing 5-HT neurons in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus. J Neurosci 32:14415–14426. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5929-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shioda N, Imai Y, Yabuki Y, Sugimoto W, Yamaguchi K, Wang Y, Hikida T, Sasaoka T, Mieda M, Fukunaga K (2019) Dopamine D2L receptor deficiency causes stress vulnerability through 5-HT1A receptor dysfunction in serotonergic neurons. J Neurosci 39:7551–7563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shrestha BR, Chia C, Wu L, Kujawa SG, Liberman MC, Goodrich LV (2018) Sensory neuron diversity in the inner ear is shaped by activity. Cell 174:1229–1246.e17. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spaethling JM, Piel D, Dueck H, Buckley PT, Morris JF, Fisher SA, Lee J, Sul JY, Kim J, Bartfai T, Beck SG, Eberwine JH (2014) Serotonergic neuron regulation informed by in vivo single-cell transcriptomics. FASEB J 28:771–780. 10.1096/fj.13-240267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spannuth BM, Hale MW, Evans AK, Lukkes JL, Campeau S, Lowry CA (2011) Investigation of a central nucleus of the amygdala/dorsal raphe nucleus serotonergic circuit implicated in fear-potentiated startle. Neuroscience 179:104–119. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steel Z, Marnane C, Iranpour C, Chey T, Jackson JW, Patel V, Silove D (2014) The global prevalence of common mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis 1980-2013. Int J Epidemiol 43:476–493. 10.1093/ije/dyu038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi H, Kamio Y (2018) Acoustic startle response and its modulation in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder in Asian subjects. Schizophr Res 198:16–20. 10.1016/j.schres.2017.05.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tammimäki A, Käenmäki M, Kambur O, Kulesskaya N, Keisala T, Karvonen E, García-Horsman JA, Rauvala H, Männistö PT (2010) Effect of S-COMT deficiency on behavior and extracellular brain dopamine concentrations in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 211:389–401. 10.1007/s00213-010-1944-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terranova JI, Song Z, Larkin TE 2nd, Hardcastle N, Norvelle A, Riaz A, Albers HE (2016) Serotonin and arginine-vasopressin mediate sex differences in the regulation of dominance and aggression by the social brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:13233–13238. 10.1073/pnas.1610446113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thye MD, Bednarz HM, Herringshaw AJ, Sartin EB, Kana RK (2018) The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci 29:151–167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tischler MD, Davis M (1983) A visual pathway that mediates fear-conditioned enhancement of acoustic startle. Brain Res 276:55–71. 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90548-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Berg WE, Lamballais S, Kushner SA (2015) Sex-specific mechanism of social hierarchy in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:1364–1372. 10.1038/npp.2014.319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang HL, Zhang S, Qi J, Wang H, Cachope R, Mejias-Aponte CA, Gomez JA, Mateo-Semidey GE, Beaudoin GMJ, Paladini CA, Cheer JF, Morales M (2019) Dorsal raphe dual serotonin-glutamate neurons drive reward by establishing excitatory synapses on VTA mesoaccumbens dopamine neurons. Cell Rep 26:1128–1142.e7. 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissbourd B, Ren J, DeLoach KE, Guenthner CJ, Miyamichi K, Luo L (2014) Presynaptic partners of dorsal raphe serotonergic and GABAergic neurons. Neuron 83:645–662. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu Q, Teixeira CM, Mahadevia D, Huang Y, Balsam D, Mann JJ, Gingrich JA, Ansorge MS (2014) Dopamine and serotonin signaling during two sensitive developmental periods differentially impact adult aggressive and affective behaviors in mice. Mol Psychiatry 19:688–698. 10.1038/mp.2014.10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou T, Zhu H, Fan Z, Wang F, Chen Y, Liang H, Yang Z, Zhang L, Lin L, Zhan Y, Wang Z, Hu H (2017) History of winning remodels thalamo-PFC circuit to reinforce social dominance. Science 357:162–168. 10.1126/science.aak9726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou X, Jen PH, Seburn KL, Frankel WN, Zheng QY (2006) Auditory brainstem responses in 10 inbred strains of mice. Brain Res 1091:16–26. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]