Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Dec 29;15(12):e0243083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243083

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

Jessica Turgeon 1,*, Annie Bérubé 2, Caroline Blais 2, Annie Lemieux 2, Amélie Fournier 1
Editor: Zezhi Li3
PMCID: PMC7771866  PMID: 33373377

Abstract

Several studies have shown that child maltreatment is associated with both positive and negative effects on the recognition of facial emotions. Research has provided little evidence of a relation between maltreatment during childhood and young adults’ ability to recognize facial displays of emotion in children, an essential skill for a sensitive parental response. In this study, we examined the consequences of different forms of maltreatment experienced in childhood on emotion recognition during parenthood. Participants included sixty-three mothers of children aged 2 to 5 years. Retrospective self-reports of childhood maltreatment were assessed using the short form of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ). Emotion recognition was measured using a morphed facial emotion identification task of all six basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise). A Path Analysis via Structural Equation Model revealed that a history of physical abuse is related to a decreased ability to recognize both fear and sadness in children, whereas emotional abuse and sexual abuse are related to a decreased ability to recognize anger in children. In addition, emotional neglect is associated with an increased ability to recognize anger, whereas physical neglect is associated with less accuracy in recognizing happiness in children’s facial emotional expressions. These findings have important clinical implications and expand current understanding of the consequences of childhood maltreatment on parents’ ability to detect children’s needs.

Introduction

Parental sensitivity refers to a parent’s ability to interpret child signals correctly and to offer an appropriate response [1]. This allows the establishment of a secure attachment relationship [1, 2] and promotes the healthy development of young children [3, 4]. Among other things, parental sensitivity is associated with positive socioemotional functioning and academic outcomes [5, 6]. Conversely, a misinterpretation of the child’s needs or avoidance of caregiving responsibilities leads to important developmental consequences, as the child is unable to use his caregiver as a secure base to explore his environment and seek contact in case of distress [1, 7, 8].

The inability to meet children’s primary needs, causing either potential or actual harm, is referred to as child maltreatment [9]. A recent meta-analysis of the global prevalence of child maltreatment suggests prevalence rates between 12% and 36% for abuse (sexual, physical, emotional) and neglect (physical, emotional) [10]. In the United States, researchers have found that 37.4% of children will be reported to authorities for suspected abuse or neglect before the age of 18 [11]. Billions of dollars are invested annually in response to this social phenomenon [12]. From a developmental perspective, damaging effects in the cognitive, social, verbal, physical, and emotional spheres of the child have been reported in several studies. Among other things, child maltreatment is associated with insecure and disorganized attachment relationships [13], motor delays [14], learning difficulties [15, 16], internalized behavior problems [17], and externalized behavior problems such as aggressivity and opposition [18]. Long-term consequences include an increased risk of delinquency [19], substance use disorder [20, 21], and parenting difficulties [22].

One of the most documented risk factors for child maltreatment is the caregiver’s own history of maltreatment during childhood [23, 24]. Parents who experienced abuse or neglect during their childhood are more likely to perpetrate maltreatment themselves [2527]. Researchers have suggested that facial emotion recognition may have a key role in the intergenerational continuity of maltreatment [28].

Emotions

In general, individuals are largely skilled at recognizing others’ emotional expressions [29]. Studies have found that the ability to discriminate emotional facial expressions increases throughout development [30]. Richoz and colleagues [31] determined that the overall recognition ability of the six basic emotions (i.e. anger, disgust, fear, happiness, surprise, and sadness) peaks during adulthood, except happiness that is mastered at a younger age. Studies have shown that a child’s living environment can influence the development of perceptual abilities. Children who have secure attachment relationships and more discussions about emotions are better at recognizing emotions [32]. Conversely, children who experience abuse or neglect differ from non-maltreated children in their ability to understand and distinguish emotional facial expressions [33, 34].

Emotion recognition in maltreated children

Most studies examining the relationship between maltreatment and emotion recognition have been conducted by Dr. Seth Pollak. In 2000, Pollak and colleagues [34] found that neglected children, compared to physically abused and non-maltreated children, had more difficulty distinguishing emotional expressions as well as a tendency to generalize sadness to other negative emotions. Shipman and colleagues [35] also revealed that, compared to non-maltreated peers, neglected children had lower abilities in both emotion regulation and emotion understanding of negative emotions, such as anger and sadness. Other studies have found that children who are physically abused have an increased sensitivity to facial expressions of anger in comparison to other basic emotions [36, 37]. Several studies have found that maltreated children are better to identify expressions of anger [3841] and fear [42] compared to controls. Conversely, studies have found that maltreated children require more sensory information to recognize facial expressions of sadness and are less accurate at processing happy faces compared to non-maltreated peers [37, 43]. Sexual abuse, on the other hand, is associated with lower understanding of emotions as well as deficits in emotion regulation [44]. Together, these findings suggest that maltreatment influences children’s recognition and processing of emotions.

Emotion recognition in adults reporting childhood maltreatment

Few studies have examined the effect of childhood maltreatment on the recognition of facial displays of emotions in adults, especially in parents. Gibb and colleagues [45] assessed undergraduate students’ interpretation of facial emotions by exposing them to morphed facial stimuli at different levels of intensity. Their study revealed that participants reporting a history of childhood maltreatment had an increased sensitivity to anger. They perceived this expression when the emotion was presented at lower intensity. Similarly, English, and colleagues [46] reported hypervigilance to fear in women who experienced emotional maltreatment as children when completing a facial emotion recognition task requiring a high cognitive load. Another study conducted by Germine and colleagues [47] found no significant association between adults’ emotion discrimination abilities and childhood experiences of maltreatment. However, rather than assessing abuse and neglect alone, the authors included many other adversities such as exposure to parental substance abuse, mental illness or criminal behavior.

The relationship between childhood maltreatment and facial emotion discrimination in adults is thus an emerging topic. Moreover, very few studies have examined this relationship using child facial expressions. Arteche and colleagues [48] investigated how postnatal depression and anxiety affects the way mothers process infant emotions and found that mothers were better at recognizing happy faces compared to sad faces when the expressions were at lower levels of intensity. Dayton and colleagues [49] documented perceptions of facial emotions among a sample of pregnant mothers, some of whom experienced childhood maltreatment and/or domestic violence. They found that mothers who experienced both forms of maltreatment perceived ambiguous infant facial expressions more negatively than other mothers.

Altogether, these studies have attempted to investigate the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition in adulthood. However, very few have looked at parents’ recognition of child facial expressions. Most protocols have used infant pictures, although children won’t express the full range of the six basic emotions before the age of two [50]. In a previous study, we found that mothers who had higher scores on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) scales had lower performance scores on the emotion recognition task [51]. Overall scores were created and used in analyses, for both childhood maltreatment experiences and emotion recognition performances. The current study seeks to further explore these results, by drawing conclusions about the differential effects of five maltreatment subtypes on mothers’ ability to recognize six emotions expressed on child faces. To our knowledge, no studies have examined associations between the processing of the six basic facial emotional expressions and the five most documented forms of maltreatment (physical, emotional and sexual abuse, physical and emotional neglect). Thus, the main objective of this study is to examine facial expression recognition ability using child facial expressions of emotions among mothers reporting a history of child maltreatment.

Materials and methods

The Ethical Committee of the University of Québec in Outaouais approved the present study (UQO CER #2518-B). The preliminary analyses for this work were the subject of a master thesis by the first author, which has been accepted and archived (Turgeon, 2019; http://di.uqo.ca/id/eprint/1125). All tasks were performed according to the university’s guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to their participation.

Participants

Participation in the study was voluntary. Participants were recruited from two community organizations in the Outaouais region, as well as from advertising on Facebook and recruitment posters displayed on university walls. Different recruitment environments were selected to ensure considerable variability in individual exposure to maltreatment. Sixty-three mothers of a biological 2 to 5-year-old child participated in the study. Mothers’ mean age was 32 years (S.D. = 5.29, range = 22 to 45). More than half of subjects (53%) reported a family income of less than 24, 000$ a year. Most mothers had completed post-secondary education (53%). However, 42% reported being either unemployed or a stay-at-home parent. Finally, most mothers were of White-European descent (80%) and had either two (44%) or three children (21%) in their care.

Although path analysis is a relatively simple model, we will respect Rex Kline's statistical power criteria [52]. Kline suggests that there are ideally 15 subjects per estimator, 10 is common practice, and at least 5 subjects is required for a simple model. With a sample size of sixty-three, a maximum of 12 estimators will be tested in the final model.

Procedure

Subjects were met by a research assistant in the community organization they attended or at the University for families who were recruited through Facebook and advertising posters. Prior to performing tasks, all subjects completed an initial assessment of socio-demographic information. Mothers then performed a computerized task to assess their ability to categorize facial expressions of basic emotions. Subsequently, the research assistant administered a questionnaire to mothers to assess adverse childhood experiences.

Measures

Adverse childhood experiences

The French brief screening version of the CTQ developed by Paquette and colleagues [53] was used to assess experiences of childhood abuse or neglect. It is a self-administered and self-reported questionnaire and consists of 28 questions on a 5-point Likert-type scale. For each item, mothers must indicate if the affirmation is 1 (never true), 2 (rarely true), 3 (sometimes true), 4 (often true) or 5 (very often true) [54]. The items are categorized into five scales: emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect. Scores on each scale can range from 5 to 25, the latter being the most severe. The brief screening version of the CTQ has demonstrated excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha varying between 0.79 and 0.94), as well as excellent temporal stability (varying between 0.76 and 0.96) [52].

Recognition of children’s facial expressions of emotion

To assess mothers’ ability to recognize facial expressions of emotions, we used a computerized task resembling the Facial Expression Megamix Task, which is used to reveal deficits in emotion recognition [55]. Faces from two children of White-European descent were selected in the Child Affective Facial Expression set (CAFE) database [56]. Mothers were exposed to different combinations of emotional expressions, morphed together using FantaMorph software. A total of 15 combinations of expressions was created, each including 5 levels of intensity (20%, 35%, 50%, 65%, 80). The entire task consisted of 450 trials, divided into three blocks, and presented in random order. Mothers were instructed to identify which of the six basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise) was dominant in each trial. A research assistant entered the mother’s response by pressing the corresponding key on the computer’s keyboard. To reduce any possible bias, there was no communication between the research assistant and the parent during the task, and photographs were presented in grayscale only. To calculate mothers’ performance, arcsine-transformed unbiased hit rates for each emotion were computed [57]. This measure was designed to account for potential response biases in emotion recognition tasks, such as if the participant uses one specific emotion category more frequently than the others or chooses the correct category by chance. This task is identical to the one used in a previously published article [51].

Statistical analyses

First, pairwise comparisons were computed between each pair of emotions to compare the level of ability across the sixty-three mothers. Bonferroni correction for multiple tests was applied and a p-value = .003 was used to assess significant results between the comparisons. Second, a confusion matrix was constructed to measure the percentage of time an emotion presented at 50% or more in an image was confused with another emotion. This matrix gives information on participants’ interpretive biases. For example, a score of .3 on the intersection between anger (horizontally) and disgust (vertically) would mean that participants confuse anger with disgust 30% of the time when anger is presented at 50% or more in the picture.

Third, descriptive and univariate correlational analyses were performed for all variables involved in the analysis. Finally, a path model via Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was performed using MPlus 7.4 to assess the link between maltreatment and emotion recognition, while controlling for ethnicity, mother’s education, and the number of children in the family. The full model with all estimate paths was not tested because of the relatively small sample size in the current study and the risk for multicollinearity. Only univariate correlations with p-values smaller than .10 were integrated into the first tested model (control variables and types of maltreatment with emotion recognition). This criterion is based on the work of Bendel & Afifi [58] and Mickey & Greenland [59] who show that a p-value cut-off of .05 often fails to identify important variables. We used a backward elimination strategy to reach a parsimonious model. Each non-significant (p-value >.10) relationship was eliminated from the model in a decreasing order based on the p-value. At each elimination step, the modification indices were verified to ensure that a previously removed link did not become significant (p-value < .10). Hu and Bentler’s [60] statistical fits recommendations were used to validate the final model (non-significant Chi-Square, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) < .05; comparative fit index (CFI) >.95). Due to the small sample size, an examination of the residual values was made to ensure good validation of the model. Small residual values (close to zero) represent a good adjustment of the model.

Results

Paired t-tests

Controlling for multiple tests (Bonferroni corrected critical p-value = .003), mean accuracy was significantly lower for disgust and fear compared with all other emotions (p < .001), apart from the non-significant relationship between disgust and fear (p = .028). Contrarily, accuracy was significantly higher for happiness compared with all other emotions. Anger was the second emotion with the highest accuracy scores, followed by sadness and surprise, respectively. A full list of pair-tested comparisons is presented in Table 1. Mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion are presented in Fig 1.

Table 1. Results of paired-samples t-tests for each combination of emotions (n = 63).

t(62) Cohen d 95% CI (low; high) p
Anger-Disgust -25.44 -3.20 -(0.47; -0.55) < .001
Anger-Fear -21.68 -2.73 -(0.40; -0.48) < .001
Anger-Happiness -17.18 -2.16 (-0.30; -0.24) < .001
Anger-Sadness -07.61 -0.96 -(0.09; -0.15) < .001
Anger-Surprise -14.79 -1.86 -(0.21; -0.27) < .001
Disgust-Fear 0–2.25 -0.28 (-0.12; -0.01) .028
Disgust-Happiness -32.52 -4.10 (-0.83; -0.73) < .001
Disgust-Sadness -16.75 -2.11 (-0.43; -0.34) < .001
Disgust-Surprise -10.31 -1.30 (-0.32; -0.21) < .001
Fear-Happiness -30.33 -3.82 (-0.76; -0.67) < .001
Fear-Sadness -13.49 -1.70 (-0.37; -0.28) < .001
Fear-Surprise 0–8.81 -1.11 (-0.25; -0.16) < .001
Happiness-Sadness -22.36 -2.82 -(0.36; -0.43) < .001
Happiness-Surprise -27.77 -3.50 -(0.48; -0.55) < .001
Sadness-Surprise -05.54 -0.70 -(0.08; -0.17) < .001

Note. Bonferroni corrected critical p-value = 0.003.

CI, confidence interval (lower and upper bounds of the mean difference).

Fig 1. Mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion.

Fig 1

Confusion matrix

Table 2 shows patterns in the confusions that participants made during the emotion recognition task. Our data revealed that the largest misinterpretation in recognition made by participants was fear being mistaken for surprise (50%). The inverse error (surprise being interpreted as fear) also occurred at 24%. Moreover, mothers most often confuse disgust for anger (32%) and anger for disgust (14%). A tendency to confuse disgust and sadness (26%) was also detected, as well as a confusion between sadness and fear (14%).

Table 2. Confusion matrix using total performances (hit rates) on the emotion task (n = 63).

Anger Disgust Fear Happiness Sadness Surprise
Anger 0.82 0.14 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01
Disgust 0.32 0.29 0.05 0.05 0.26 0.03
Fear 0.02 0.07 0.37 0.01 0.03 0.50
Happiness 0.01 0.02 0.00 0.93 0.00 0.03
Sadness 0.01 0.11 0.14 0.03 0.68 0.03
Surprise 0.00 0.02 0.24 0.05 0.04 0.64

Descriptive and correlational analyses

Table 3 depicts the mean, standard deviation, minimum, maximum, and distribution (skewness and kurtosis) for each variable involved in the analysis, as well as the bivariate correlations across all variables. Bivariate correlations revealed a negative significant association between the recognition of anger and the three types of abuse (physical, emotional, and sexual). The more severe the abuse, the less accurate mothers were at correctly recognizing the emotion when it was presented at 50% or more in the stimulus. The same relationship was found for fear and all types of abuse, as well as emotional neglect. Furthermore, physical neglect was negatively correlated with the recognition of happiness. The more mothers reported having a severe history of physical neglect, the less they were accurate at recognizing children’s facial expressions of happiness (see Table 3). Finally, ethnicity was negatively correlated with the recognition of sadness, mother’s education was positively correlated with anger, disgust, and fear, and the number of children in the family was significantly associated with less ability in fear recognition. These significant associations were included in the path model.

Table 3. Bivariate correlations, means, and standard deviations among variables in the path model (n = 63).

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14.
1. Ethnicity - -.26*** -.15*** -.09* -.14 .06 -.03 -.26* -.03 -.12 -.09 -.02 -.10 -.00
(0 = White-European descent, 1 = other)
2. Mother’s education - -.26*** -.29* .34** .36** -.02 -.13 -.01 -.39** -.36** -.28* -.41** -.40**
3. Number of children - -.15* -.01 -.35** -.09 -.05 -.08 -.33** -.26* -.28* .38** .37**
4. Anger (UAT) - .50*** .34** -.12 -.38** -.13 -.32** -.34** -.29* -.17 -.18
5. Disgust (UAT) - .21 -.20 -.34** -.09 -.16 -.05 -.17 -.11 -.10
6. Fear (UAT) - -.12 -.20 -.20 -.36** -.28* -.21 -.34** -.20
7. Happiness (UAT) - -.27* -.01 -.05 -.20 -.17 -.20 -.25*
8. Sadness (UAT) - -.12 -.17 -.12 -.14 -.07 -.05
9. Surprise (UAT) - -.06 -.02 -.04 -.07 -.07
10. Physical abuse - -.72*** -.72*** -.69*** -.65***
11. Emotional abuse - -.52*** -.78*** -.65***
12. Sexual abuse - -.54*** -.62***
13. Emotional neglect - -.72***
14. Physical neglect -
Mean 0.16 3.21 2.48 0.86 0.35 0.41 1.13 0.74 0.62 7.19 9.44 6.98 9.83 7.67
SD 0.37 1.69 1.09 0.09 0.18 0.17 0.10 0.13 0.11 4.71 5.84 4.40 4.86 3.86
Min 0.00 1.00 1.00 0.65 0.03 0.04 0.92 0.37 0.41 5.00 5.00 5.00 5.00 5.00
Max 1.00 5.00 5.00 1.07 0.77 0.94 1.35 1.03 0.97 25.00 25.00 25.00 24.00 19.00
Skewness 1.91 -0.13 0.87 0.43 0.12 0.24 -0.08 -0.42 0.16 2.48 1.43 2.77 1.20 1.46
Kurtosis 1.72 -1.74 0.31 0.49 -0.67 0.42 -0.67 0.41 0.55 5.66 0.96 7.50 0.92 1.24

UAT = unbiased/arcsin transformed.

p < 0.1.

* p < 0.05

** p < 0.01

*** p < 0.001.

Path model—SEM

The retained model showed excellent fits (Chi-Square = 56.231, df = 63, p-value = 0.714; RMSEA = 0.00; CFI = 1.00) and all residual values were close to zero (|value| < 1). All significant paths were in the expected directions (see Fig 2). Controlling for shared variance between types of maltreatment, a history of physical abuse is related to a decreased ability to recognize fear (β = -.26, p < .001) and sadness (β = -.23, p < .010) in children. Emotional abuse is related to a decreased ability to recognize anger in children (β = -.57, p < .001). Sexual abuse is also related to a decreased ability to recognize anger but with a marginal effect (β = -.15, p < .100), while emotional neglect is positively associated with the recognition of anger (β = .35, p < .050). Physical neglect is associated with less accuracy in recognizing happiness in child facial emotional expressions (β = -.25, p < .001).

Fig 2. Path model results using unbiased hit rates.

Fig 2

Discussion

Our study examined the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition during parenthood. We exposed mothers to child facial expressions, and we examined the relationship between the six basic emotions and the five most documented forms of maltreatment, which yields new understanding of the differential long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment. Strictly in terms of facial expression recognition, results from the current study suggest that mothers, in general, were less accurate in recognizing facial expressions of disgust and fear, than emotions of anger, happiness, sadness, and surprise. These results align with previous studies in facial expression recognition performed on same-age groups [61]. Although adults are better at recognizing adult faces compared with faces of newborns and children [62], the present study indicates that they keep the same pattern of performance in recognition ability. It is important to note that there are certain interpretive biases in emotion recognition, such as the happy face advantage, which refers to the general tendency of adults to better recognize happiness and to categorize happy faces at a faster rate than negatively-valenced emotions [63, 64].

Regarding the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition, our results indicate that a history of maltreatment influences the ability of mothers to recognize and discriminate the emotions expressed on children’s faces. Specifically, we found that a history of physical abuse is related to a lower recognition ability in fear and sadness. Moreover, mothers who experienced emotional abuse in their childhoods show a reduced ability to recognize anger, whereas emotional neglect is related to better accuracy in the detection of anger on children’s faces. Finally, mothers with a history of physical neglect are less accurate in recognizing expressions of happiness in children. Our results follow the work of Pollak and his colleagues [34] who revealed that children who have experienced maltreatment recognize emotions differently than non-maltreated controls. Twenty years later, these children may have become parents. We now know that their history of maltreatment will have detrimental effects on their parental role as they will have more difficulty recognizing children’s emotions.

Our findings reveal that anger, fear, and sadness are the negative emotions that differentiate those who have a history of childhood maltreatment from those who have not experienced maltreatment during their childhood. Studies have argued that abused children have an enhanced sensitivity to negative facial expressions (i.e., anger, and fear) as a consequence of living in environments where they are exposed to more cases of hostility and threat [37]. Certain authors believe this hypervigilance helps protect children. By identifying signs of anger more rapidly, these children may be able to prevent further abuse [42, 65]. This hypervigilance could decrease as the child becomes an adult and has fewer interactions with his or her maltreating caregivers [46]. Moreover, all the resources maltreated children must dedicate to their survival are allocated to the detriment of their development [66]. Chronic stress experienced by these children affects the development of their brain, particularly the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala, both involved in emotion recognition [67]. Overall, children who have experienced maltreatment show alterations in the neural structures involved in the recognition of facial affect, leading to important deficits in emotional processing [68].

The developmental challenges children growing up in maltreating environments must face could explain a particular developmental trajectory in emotion recognition. As of now, we only have a glimpse of this trajectory. Most research on these children as they reach adulthood has looked at their ability to recognize emotions in adult faces. For example, Hartling and colleagues [69] showed that adults with a history of childhood maltreatment are less accurate in the perception of emotions expressed on adult faces, strictly if they are carriers of a more stress-responsive genetic profile. On the other hand, Gibb and colleagues [45] found that individuals reporting childhood maltreatment are better at perceiving anger in adult faces when presented at lower levels of intensity (morphed at 20 to 40%) but showed similar performances at higher levels of intensity.

Given that deficits in emotion recognition might change between childhood and adulthood, accuracy should also be expected to vary with child and adult facial stimuli. For instance, Olsavsky and colleagues [70] exposed mothers with and without a history of maltreatment to pictures of infants and adults depicting different emotions. They found a difference in brain activation between both groups, strictly in response to infant faces. Results from our previous work using child facial stimuli revealed that a history of maltreatment not only affects emotion recognition, but also mother-child interactions. Specifically, we found that mothers who had higher performances on the emotion recognition task, combined with a severe history of childhood maltreatment, demonstrated fewer sensitive behaviors towards their child during interactions [51]. Our current findings expand these results by helping us recognize how each form of maltreatment interacts with the way mothers perceive and interpret their child’s signals. These findings may lead to the development of specific interventions that address the consequences of each form of maltreatment on emotion recognition. The results of our previous and current studies highlight the specific needs of parents with a history of maltreatment. General emotion recognition interventions may not be appropriate for this population. Rather, our results encourage a more specific approach that focuses on individual difficulties in recognizing and responding to emotions. For instance, Kolijn and colleagues [71] showed that video-feedback, an individualized attachment-based intervention aimed at enhancing the recognition of child emotional expressions and sensitive parenting, may improve mothers’ ability to process children’s emotions, by reducing the neural effort associated with this task. These results are encouraging as they suggest that the detrimental, long-lasting effects of maltreatment on emotion recognition can be reversed.

Limitations and future directions

This study is one of the first to document the effect of early maltreatment on mothers’ ability to recognize children’s emotional expressions. It is also, to our knowledge, the first study to include the six basic emotions in addition to five subtypes of maltreatment. Nevertheless, the findings of the present study must be interpreted in light of certain limitations. First, the relatively small sample size in the current study decreases statistical power. Small effect sizes could be revealed by a larger sample. Therefore, non-significant links should not be interpreted as nonexistent. The small sample size also warrants further studies to ensure the replicability of the present findings. Second, the use of a retrospective self-report instrument to assess mother’s exposure to childhood maltreatment may have led certain participants to underestimate or overestimate the prevalence of these experiences. However, the short form of the CTQ is often used in assessing childhood maltreatment experiences in both clinical and nonclinical samples, as it provides a brief, valid, and reliable screening of childhood maltreatment experiences. Third, we conducted a path analysis on cross-sectional data using variables collected at a single time-point. Our results must be interpreted as correlations and do not imply causation. Finally, we examined the relationship between childhood maltreatment and emotion processing in adulthood, without considering other factors such as mental illness. Studies have shown that mental health problems in adulthood, such as major depression or bipolar disorder, can lead to biases in emotion processing [72, 73]. However, Johnson and colleagues [74] found that these biases remained statistically significant after controlling for adult’s past and current mental illness.

Conclusion

Childhood physical abuse, emotional abuse, and physical neglect are related to mothers’ decreased ability to recognize facial emotions in children, whereas emotional neglect is related to increased accuracy in the recognition of anger. These findings highlight the need to consider the parent’s past experiences when working with families referred to services for childhood maltreatment. Although several interventions already target these parents, those aimed more specifically at increasing their ability to interpret and respond appropriately to the child signals might not rely on an accurate understanding of the mechanisms underlying this difficulty. Our research provides some answers as to which emotions are more challenging to some parents.

Supporting information

S1 File. Database.

(SAV)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the families who devoted their time to participate in this research, as well as the Centre de pédiatrie sociale de Gatineau and the Carrefour de la Miséricorde who welcomed us and introduced us to these families.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The study was funded by the Centre de recherche universitaire sur les jeunes et les familles (CRUJeF) https://www.crujef.ca/, (JT) and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) https://www.sshrc-crsh.gc.ca/, 430-2016-00521, (AB). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Ainsworth M. D. S., Bell S. M., & Stayton D. F. (1974). Infant-mother attachment and social development: Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Swain J. E., Kim P., Spicer J., Ho S., Dayton C. J., et al. (2014). Approaching the biology of human parental attachment: Brain imaging, oxytocin and coordinated assessments of mothers and fathers. Brain Research, 1580, 78–101. 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.03.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kerig P. K., & Becker S. P. (2015). Early Abuse and Neglect as Risk Factors for the Development of Criminal and Antisocial Behavior In The development of criminal and antisocial behavior (pp. 181–199): Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Leerkes E. M., Blankson A. N., & O’Brien M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social‐emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762–775. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Grossmann K., Grossmann K. E., Kindler H., & Waters E. (2005). Early care and the roots of attachment and partnership representations. Attachment from infancy to adulthood: The major longitudinal studies, 98–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Moss E., & St-Laurent D. (2001). Attachment at school age and academic performance. Developmental psychology, 37(6), 863 10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.863 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Stirling J., & Amaya-Jackson L. (2008). Understanding the behavioral and emotional consequences of child abuse. Pediatrics, 122(3), 667–673. 10.1542/peds.2008-1885 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Toth S. L., & Manly J. T. (2019). Developmental Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect: Implications for Intervention. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 59–64. 10.1111/cdep.12317 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Manly J. T. (2005). Advances in research definitions of child maltreatment. Child abuse & neglect, 29(5), 425–439. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Stoltenborgh M., Bakermans‐Kranenburg M. J., Alink L. R., & van IJzendoorn M. H. (2015). The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: Review of a series of meta‐analyses. Child Abuse Review, 24(1), 37–50. 10.1002/car.2353 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kim H., Wildeman C., Jonson-Reid M., & Drake B. (2017). Lifetime prevalence of investigating child maltreatment among US children. American journal of public health, 107(2), 274–280. 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303545 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Peterson C., Florence C., & Klevens J. (2018). The economic burden of child maltreatment in the United States, 2015. Child abuse & neglect, 86, 178–183. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.09.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Grady M. D., Levenson J. S., & Bolder T. (2017). Linking adverse childhood effects and attachment: A theory of etiology for sexual offending. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 18(4), 433–444. 10.1177/1524838015627147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wade T. J., Bowden J., & Sites H. J. (2018). Child maltreatment and motor coordination deficits among preschool children. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 11(2), 159–162. 10.1007/s40653-017-0186-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Elklit A., Michelsen L., & Murphy S. (2018). Childhood Maltreatment and School Problems: A Danish National Study. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 62(1), 150–159. 10.1080/00313831.2016.1253608 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Trocmé N. M., Fallon B., MacLaurin B., Daciuk J., Felstiner C., et al. (2005). Étude canadienne sur l'incidence des signalements de cas de violence et de négligence envers les enfants, 2003: données principales: Agence de santé publique du Canada, Centre national d'information sur la violence dans la famille. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Romano E., Babchishin L., Marquis R., & Fréchette S. (2015). Childhood maltreatment and educational outcomes. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 16(4), 418–437. 10.1177/1524838014537908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Evans C. B., & Burton D. L. (2013). Five types of child maltreatment and subsequent delinquency: Physical neglect as the most significant predictor. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 6(4), 231–245. 10.1080/19361521.2013.837567 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bland V. J., Lambie I., & Best C. (2018). Does childhood neglect contribute to violent behavior in adulthood? A review of possible links. Clinical psychology review, 60, 126–135. 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hagborg J. M., Thorvaldsson V., & Fahlke C. (2020). Child maltreatment and substance-use–related negative consequences: Longitudinal trajectories from early to mid adolescence. Addictive behaviors, 106 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lo C. C., & Cheng T. C. (2007). The impact of childhood maltreatment on young adults' substance abuse. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 33(1), 139–146. 10.1080/00952990601091119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Savage L.-É., Tarabulsy G. M., Pearson J., Collin-Vézina D., & Gagné L.-M. (2019). Maternal history of childhood maltreatment and later parenting behavior: a meta-analysis. Development and psychopathology, 31(1), 9–21. 10.1017/S0954579418001542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Buisman R. S., Pittner K., Tollenaar M. S., Lindenberg J., van den Berg L. J., et al. (2020). Intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment using a multi-informant multi-generation family design. PLoS One, 15(3). 10.1371/journal.pone.0225839 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Thornberry T. P., Knight K. E., & Lovegrove P. J. (2012). Does maltreatment beget maltreatment? A systematic review of the intergenerational literature. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 13(3), 135–152. 10.1177/1524838012447697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bartlett J. D., Kotake C., Fauth R., & Easterbrooks M. A. (2017). Intergenerational transmission of child abuse and neglect: Do maltreatment type, perpetrator, and substantiation status matter? Child abuse & neglect, 63, 84–94. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dixon L., Hamilton‐Giachritsis C., & Browne K. (2005). Attributions and behaviours of parents abused as children: A mediational analysis of the intergenerational continuity of child maltreatment (Part II). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(1), 58–68. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00340.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Madigan S., Cyr C., Eirich R., Fearon R. P., Ly A., Rash C., et al. (2019). Testing the cycle of maltreatment hypothesis: Meta-analytic evidence of the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 23–51. 10.1017/S0954579418001700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Perrault I., & Beaudoin G. (2008). La négligence envers les enfants: bilan des connaissances: Centre de liaison sur l'intervention et la prévention psychosociales en collaboration avec le Groupe de recherche et d'intervention en négligence (GRIN) de l'Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nelson C. A. (1987). The recognition of facial expressions in the first two years of life: Mechanisms of development. Child Development, 889–909. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Herba C. M., Landau S., Russell T., Ecker C., & Phillips M. L. (2006). The development of emotion‐processing in children: Effects of age, emotion, and intensity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(11), 1098–1106. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01652.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Richoz A.-R., Lao J., Pascalis O., & Caldara R. (2018). Tracking the recognition of static and dynamic facial expressions of emotion across the life span. Journal of vision, 18(9), 5–5. 10.1167/18.9.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Raikes H. A., & Thompson R. A. (2006). Family emotional climate, attachment security and young children's emotion knowledge in a high risk sample. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 89–104. 10.1348/026151005X70427 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pears K. C., & Fisher P. A. (2005). Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: Evidence of deficits. Development and psychopathology, 17(1), 47–65. 10.1017/s0954579405050030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pollak S. D., Cicchetti D., Hornung K., & Reed A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental psychology, 36(5), 679 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Shipman K., Edwards A., Brown A., Swisher L., & Jennings E. (2005). Managing emotion in a maltreating context: A pilot study examining child neglect. Child abuse & neglect, 29(9), 1015–1029. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Pollak S. D., Messner M., Kistler D. J., & Cohn J. F. (2009). Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition. Cognition, 110(2), 242–247. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pollak S. D., & Sinha P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children's recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental psychology, 38(5), 784 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Curtis W. J., & Cicchetti D. (2011). Affective facial expression processing in young children who have experienced maltreatment during the first year of life: An event-related potential study. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 373–395. 10.1017/S0954579411000125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Curtis W. J., & Cicchetti D. (2013). Affective facial expression processing in 15-month-old infants who have experienced maltreatment: An event-related potential study. Child Maltreatment, 18(3), 140–154. 10.1177/1077559513487944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Leist T., & Dadds M. R. (2009). Adolescents' ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clinical child psychology and psychiatry, 14(2), 237–250. 10.1177/1359104508100887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Pollak S. D., Klorman R., Thatcher J. E., & Cicchetti D. (2001). P3b reflects maltreated children's reactions to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology, 38(2), 267–274. 10.1111/1469-8986.3820267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Masten C. L., Guyer A. E., Hodgdon H. B., McClure E. B., Charney D. S., et al. (2008). Recognition of facial emotions among maltreated children with high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder. Child abuse & neglect, 32(1), 139–153. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pollak S. D., & Tolley-Schell S. A. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of abnormal psychology, 112(3), 323 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Shipman K., Zeman J., Penza S., & Champion K. (2000). Emotion management skills in sexually maltreated and nonmaltreated girls: A developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and psychopathology, 12(1), 47–62. 10.1017/s0954579400001036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gibb B. E., Schofield C. A., & Coles M. E. (2009). Reported history of childhood abuse and young adults' information-processing biases for facial displays of emotion. Child Maltreatment, 14(2), 148–156. 10.1177/1077559508326358 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.English L. H., Wisener M., & Bailey H. N. (2018). Childhood emotional maltreatment, anxiety, attachment, and mindfulness: Associations with facial emotion recognition. Child abuse & neglect, 80, 146–160. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Germine L., Dunn E. C., McLaughlin K. A., & Smoller J. W. (2015). Childhood adversity is associated with adult theory of mind and social affiliation, but not face processing. PloS one, 10(6). 10.1371/journal.pone.0129612 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Arteche A., Joormann J., Harvey A., Craske M., Gotlib I. H., et al. (2011). The effects of postnatal maternal depression and anxiety on the processing of infant faces. Journal of affective disorders, 133(1), 197–203. 10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dayton C. J., Huth-Bocks A. C., & Busuito A. (2016). The influence of interpersonal aggression on maternal perceptions of infant emotions: Associations with early parenting quality. Emotion, 16(4), 436 10.1037/emo0000114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Izard C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bérubé A., Blais C., Fournier A., Turgeon J., Forget H., Coutu S., et al. (2020). Childhood maltreatment moderates the relationship between emotion recognition and maternal sensitive behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 102 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kline R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, third In: New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Paquette D., Laporte L., Bigras M., & Zoccolillo M. (2004). Validation of the French version of the CTQ and prevalence of the history of maltreatment. Santé mentale au Québec, 29(1), 201–220. 10.7202/008831ar [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bernstein D. P., Stein J. A., Newcomb M. D., Walker E., Pogge D., et al. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child abuse & neglect, 27(2), 169–190. 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Young A. W., Rowland D., Calder A. J., Etcoff N. L., Seth A., & Perrett D. I. (1997). Facial expression megamix: Tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition, 63(3), 271–313. 10.1016/s0010-0277(97)00003-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.LoBue V., & Thrasher C. (2015). The Child Affective Facial Expression (CAFE) set: Validity and reliability from untrained adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1532 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01532 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wagner H. L. (1993). On measuring performance in category judgment studies of nonverbal behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17(1), 3–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bendel R. B., & Afifi A. A. (1977). Comparison of stopping rules in forward “stepwise” regression. Journal of the American Statistical association, 72(357), 46–53. 10.1080/01621459.1977.10479905 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Mickey R. M., & Greenland S. (1989). The impact of confounder selection criteria on effect estimation. American journal of epidemiology, 129(1), 125–137. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hu L. T., & Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal, 6(1), 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Ebner N. C., & Johnson M. K. (2009). Young and older emotional faces: are there age group differences in expression identification and memory? Emotion, 9(3), 329 10.1037/a0015179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kuefner D., Macchi Cassia V., Picozzi M., & Bricolo E. (2008). Do all kids look alike? Evidence for an other-age effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 811 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Leppänen J. M., Tenhunen M., & Hietanen J. K. (2003). Faster choice-reaction times to positive than to negative facial expressions: The role of cognitive and motor processes. Journal of Psychophysiology, 17(3), 113 10.1027/0269-8803.17.3.113 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Lipp O. V., Craig B. M., & Dat M. C. (2015). A happy face advantage with male caucasian faces: It depends on the company you keep. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(1), 109–115. 10.1177/1948550614546047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pollak S. D., Vardi S., Putzer Bechner A. M., & Curtin J. J. (2005). Physically abused children's regulation of attention in response to hostility. Child development, 76(5), 968–977. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00890.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Twardosz S., & Lutzker J. R. (2010). Child maltreatment and the developing brain: A review of neuroscience perspectives. Aggression and violent behavior, 15(1), 59–68. 10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Beers S. R., & De Bellis M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological function in children with maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(3), 483–486. 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.McCrory E., De Brito S. A., & Viding E. (2010). Research review: the neurobiology and genetics of maltreatment and adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(10), 1079–1095. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02271.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hartling C., Fan Y., Weigand A., Trilla I., Gartner M., et al. (2019). Interaction of HPA axis genetics and early life stress shapes emotion recognition in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 99, 28–37. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Olsavsky A. K., Stoddard J., Erhart A., Tribble R., & Kim P. (2019). Neural Processing of Infant and Adult Face Emotion and Maternal Exposure to Childhood Maltreatment. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 10.1093/scan/nsz069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kolijn L., Huffmeijer R., Van Den Bulk B. G., Vrijhof C. I., Van Ijzendoorn M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J. (2020). Effects of the Video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting and sensitive discipline on mothers’ neural responses to child faces: A randomized controlled ERP study including pre-and post-intervention measures. Social Neuroscience, 15(1), 108–122. 10.1080/17470919.2019.1660709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Veague H. B., & Hooley J. M. (2014). Enhanced sensitivity and response bias for male anger in women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry research (Print), 215(3), 687–693. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Suzuki A., Poon L., Kumari V., & Cleare A. J. (2015). Fear Biases in Emotional Face Processing Following Childhood Trauma as a Marker of Resilience and Vulnerability to Depression. Child Maltreat, 20(4), 240–250. 10.1177/1077559515600781 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Johnson A. L., Gibb B. E., & McGeary J. (2010). Reports of Childhood Physical Abuse, 5-HTTLPR Genotype, and Women's Attentional Biases for Angry Faces. Cognitive therapy and research, 34(4), 380–387. 10.1007/s10608-009-9269-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Zezhi Li

22 Sep 2020

PONE-D-20-13201

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Turgeon,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 06 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Zezhi Li, Ph.D., M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for including a copy of your related published article and discussing it in your manuscript. Please remove blinded references to this other study and give the full reference for ref. 49 in your manuscript since review is not blinded in this journal.

3. We note that Figure 1 includes an image of a participant in the study. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual.

4. We note that Figure 1 in your submission contain copyrighted images. All PLOS content is published under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which means that the manuscript, images, and Supporting Information files will be freely available online, and any third party is permitted to access, download, copy, distribute, and use these materials in any way, even commercially, with proper attribution. For more information, see our copyright guidelines: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright.

We require you to either (1) present written permission from the copyright holder to publish these figures specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license, or (2) remove the figures from your submission:

4.1.         You may seek permission from the original copyright holder of Figure(s) [#] to publish the content specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license.

We recommend that you contact the original copyright holder with the Content Permission Form (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=7c09/content-permission-form.pdf) and the following text:

“I request permission for the open-access journal PLOS ONE to publish XXX under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CCAL) CC BY 4.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Please be aware that this license allows unrestricted use and distribution, even commercially, by third parties. Please reply and provide explicit written permission to publish XXX under a CC BY license and complete the attached form.”

Please upload the completed Content Permission Form or other proof of granted permissions as an "Other" file with your submission. 

In the figure caption of the copyrighted figure, please include the following text: “Reprinted from [ref] under a CC BY license, with permission from [name of publisher], original copyright [original copyright year].”

4.2.    If you are unable to obtain permission from the original copyright holder to publish these figures under the CC BY 4.0 license or if the copyright holder’s requirements are incompatible with the CC BY 4.0 license, please either i) remove the figure or ii) supply a replacement figure that complies with the CC BY 4.0 license. Please check copyright information on all replacement figures and update the figure caption with source information. If applicable, please specify in the figure caption text when a figure is similar but not identical to the original image and is therefore for illustrative purposes only.

5. We noted in your submission details that a portion of your manuscript may have been presented or published elsewhere.

"Our manuscript is part of a larger research project that allowed us to publish an article in Child Abuse & Neglect, which we uploaded in the Attach Files. The results related to the manuscript we are submitting to PLOS ONE are original. They have not been published and are not under consideration for publication elsewhere. Please find the details of its specific contributions in our cover letter, in which we explain why our manuscript does not constitute dual publication. "

Please clarify whether thispublication was peer-reviewed and formally published. If this work was previously peer-reviewed and published, in the cover letter please provide the reason that this work does not constitute dual publication and should be included in the current manuscript.

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for allowing me to review this interesting manuscript.

Overall, I found it be well written and topical. There is clear attention to detail.

I only have minor comments:

- The topic is important and will be of interest to the readership.

- The manuscript could use some greater detail regarding the implications of this research. What could these results mean for identifying and treating mothers who have experienced trauma and have difficulty with emotion recognition? Are there specific treatments for this deficit?

- The analysis and methodology generally seems appropriate, however, I defer to the other reviewers and editors on this point. Highlight that a path analysis where all variables are collected at a single time-point has limitations.

- It may be worth mentioning the “happy face advantage” (references below) in interpreting some of these results and biases. This is a common bias in facial recognition:

Leppanen, J., Tenhunen, M., & Hietanen, J. (2003). Faster choice reaction times to positive than negative facial expressions: The role of cognitive and motor processes. Emotion, 3, 315–326.

Lipp, O., Craig, B., & Dat, M. (2015). A happy face advantage with male caucasian faces: It depends on the company you keep. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 109–115.

Minor points:

- The first paragraph could possibly be stronger and longer.

- Consider deleting second last sentence of page six: “This paper aims…”

Reviewer #2: The objective of the research entitled " Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment" is to examine the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition during parenthood. The study is quite interesting and offered new understanding of the differential long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment. I only have a few questions. Below are several suggestions for a revision.

Points to address:

1. Regarding the results of the Paired t-test section, a full list of pair-tested comparisons was presented in Table 1. However, the results state that “mean accuracy was significantly lower for disgust and fear compared with all other emotions” and “anger was the second emotion with the highest accuracy scores, followed by sadness and surprise, respectively”. The t-test table tells which pair comparison is significant, from the t-values and Cohen’s d the reader can infer which emotion have the relative higher or lower accuracy, but it is not straightforward. It would be great if the authors can show the mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion in a table or bar plot.

2. Is there a reason why only univariate correlations with p-values smaller than .10 were integrated into the first tested model?

3. For controlling for multiple tests, p-value = .003 was used, I wonder what happens if a different threshold? Are the main results still holds?

4. In line 123, abbreviation CTQ used before first define it.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Dec 29;15(12):e0243083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243083.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


22 Oct 2020

Enclosed please find our response to reviewers concerning our manuscript entitled “Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment”, which we are resubmitting to PLOS ONE with great hopes of achieving publication as an original contribution.

We first would like to thank the reviewers for their suggestions that have helped clarify and improve the paper. We would also like to thank the editor for giving us the opportunity to resubmit what we believe are important results, as they highlight how childhood maltreatment interferes with mothers’ ability to discriminate child facial expressions of emotion. For clarity, we first state the comments verbatim (in italics), followed by our response. The sections in red indicate changes that were made in the manuscript.

Reviews from the Edition Team

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.

Authors’ reply: We have made the necessary changes to ensure that our manuscript meets PLOS ONE’s style requirements.

2. Thank you for including a copy of your related published article and discussing it in your manuscript. Please remove blinded references to this other study and give the full reference for ref. 49 in your manuscript since review is not blinded in this journal.

Authors’ reply: We have removed blinded references and have added the full reference for ref. 49 in our manuscript (which is now ref. 51, after revisions).

3. We note that Figure 1 includes an image of a participant in the study and 4. We note that Figure 1 in your submission contain copyrighted images.

Authors’ reply: We have removed the Figure from our submission as it is not essential to the reader’s understanding of our paper. The figure provided an illustration of the images to which participants were exposed, but the description in text provides sufficient information and is usually found in other similar papers (Gibb et al., 2009).

5. We noted in your submission details that a portion of your manuscript may have been presented or published elsewhere.

Please clarify whether this publication was peer-reviewed and formally published. If this work was previously peer-reviewed and published, in the cover letter please provide the reason that this work does not constitute dual publication and should be included in the current manuscript.

Authors’ reply: Our publication in Child Abuse & Neglect was peer-reviewed and formally published [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104432]. Our cover letter addresses how our current manuscript does not constitute dual publication, as it extends our previous findings. We have added a sentence in our manuscript to clarify the difference between both manuscripts, see lines 125-128:

In a previous study, we found that mothers who had higher scores on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) scales had lower performance scores on the emotion recognition task [51]. Overall scores were created and used in analyses, for both childhood maltreatment experiences and emotion recognition performances. The current study seeks to further explore these results, by drawing conclusions about the differential effects of five maltreatment subtypes on mothers’ ability to recognize six emotions expressed on child faces. To our knowledge, no studies have examined associations between the processing of the six basic facial emotional expressions and the five most documented forms of maltreatment (physical, emotional and sexual abuse, physical and emotional neglect).

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly.

Authors’ reply: We have included captions for our Supporting Information files at the end of our manuscript.

Reviewer #1:

Thank you for allowing me to review this interesting manuscript.

Overall, I found it be well written and topical. There is clear attention to detail.

I only have minor comments:

- The topic is important and will be of interest to the readership.

Authors’ reply: Thank you, we appreciate this comment.

- The manuscript could use some greater detail regarding the implications of this research. What could these results mean for identifying and treating mothers who have experienced trauma and have difficulty with emotion recognition? Are there specific treatments for this deficit?

Authors’ reply: In response to this comment, we have added sentences in the Discussion section to clarify the implications of our research. The last paragraph now reads:

Our current findings expand these results by helping us recognize how each form of maltreatment interacts with the way mothers perceive and interpret their child’s signals. These findings may lead to the development of specific interventions that address the consequences of each form of maltreatment on emotion recognition. The results of our previous and current studies highlight the specific needs of parents with a history of maltreatment. General emotion recognition interventions may not be appropriate for this population. Rather, our results encourage a more specific approach that focuses on individual difficulties in recognizing and responding to emotions. For instance, Kolijn and colleagues [73] showed that video-feedback, an individualised attachment-based intervention aimed at enhancing the recognition of child emotional expressions and sensitive parenting, may improve mothers’ ability to process children’s emotions, by reducing the neural effort associated with this task. These results are encouraging as they suggest that the detrimental, long-lasting effects of maltreatment on emotion recognition can be reversed.

- The analysis and methodology generally seems appropriate, however, I defer to the other reviewers and editors on this point. Highlight that a path analysis where all variables are collected at a single time-point has limitations.

Authors’ reply: A path analysis on cross-sectional data has limits, particularly regarding data interpretation. Historically, a path analysis was used to test causal effects and therefore a longitudinal data estimate was required. However, this type of analysis is now widely used with cross-sectional data, which makes it possible to discuss correlations between variables in the model. One limitation would be that our conclusions are "correlational" rather than "causal". We have added the sentence below to highlight this limit, see lines 360-362.

Third, we conducted a path analysis on cross-sectional data using variables collected at a single time-point. Our results must be interpreted as correlations and do not imply causation.

- It may be worth mentioning the “happy face advantage” (references below) in interpreting some of these results and biases. This is a common bias in facial recognition:

Leppanen, J., Tenhunen, M., & Hietanen, J. (2003). Faster choice reaction times to positive than negative facial expressions: The role of cognitive and motor processes. Emotion, 3, 315–326.

Lipp, O., Craig, B., & Dat, M. (2015). A happy face advantage with male caucasian faces: It depends on the company you keep. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 109–115.

Authors’ reply: Thank you for this advice. We have mentioned the “happy face advantage” in our manuscript and have added both references to facilitate the interpretation of our results, see lines 288-291.

Minor points:

- The first paragraph could possibly be stronger and longer.

Authors’ reply: Thank you for this advice. We have made changes to the paragraph to make it stronger. The paragraph now reads:

Parental sensitivity refers to a parent’s ability to interpret child signals correctly and to offer an appropriate response [1]. This allows the establishment of a secure attachment relationship [1,2] and promotes the healthy development of young children [3,4]. Among other things, parental sensitivity is associated with positive socioemotional functioning and academic outcomes [5-6]. Conversely, a misinterpretation of the child’s needs or avoidance of caregiving responsibilities leads to important developmental consequences, as the child is unable to use his caregiver as a secure base to explore his environment and seek contact in case of distress [1,7,8].

- Consider deleting second last sentence of page six: “This paper aims…”

Authors’ reply: This sentence was removed.

Reviewer #2:

The objective of the research entitled " Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment" is to examine the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition during parenthood. The study is quite interesting and offered new understanding of the differential long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment. I only have a few questions. Below are several suggestions for a revision.

Points to address:

1. Regarding the results of the Paired t-test section, a full list of pair-tested comparisons was presented in Table 1. However, the results state that “mean accuracy was significantly lower for disgust and fear compared with all other emotions” and “anger was the second emotion with the highest accuracy scores, followed by sadness and surprise, respectively”. The t-test table tells which pair comparison is significant, from the t-values and Cohen’s d the reader can infer which emotion have the relative higher or lower accuracy, but it is not straightforward. It would be great if the authors can show the mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion in a table or bar plot.

Authors’ reply: In line with this comment, we have added a bar plot with the mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion (see fig. 1).

Fig 1. Mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion.

2. Is there a reason why only univariate correlations with p-values smaller than .10 were integrated into the first tested model?

Authors’ reply: This criterion is based on the work of Bendel & Afifi (1977) and Mickey & Greenland (1989) who show that a p-value cut-off of .05 often fails to identify important variables (see references below). We have added this sentence in our manuscript, see lines 215-216.

Bendel, R. B., & Afifi, A. A. (1977). Comparison of stopping rules in forward “stepwise” regression. Journal of the American Statistical association, 72(357), 46-53. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1977.10479905

Mickey, R. M., & Greenland, S. (1989). The impact of confounder selection criteria on effect estimation. American journal of epidemiology, 129(1), 125-137. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115101

3. For controlling for multiple tests, p-value = .003 was used, I wonder what happens if a different threshold? Are the main results still holds?

Authors’ reply: The p-value = .003 corresponds to the Bonferroni correction we applied, as mentioned on p.10. Only one paired comparison test has a p-value > 0.003 [Disgust-Fear p=.028]. All other results reach the conservative p-value < 0.003, which we used, and would therefore be retained in the absence of a Bonferroni correction.

4. In line 123, abbreviation CTQ used before first define it.

Authors’ reply: We have defined the abbreviation before using it.

We would like to thank both reviewers for their time in reading and reviewing our manuscript.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Zezhi Li

16 Nov 2020

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

PONE-D-20-13201R1

Dear Dr. Turgeon,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Zezhi Li, Ph.D., M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper is well written. The authors have addressed my queries adequately. I believe the paper makes a worthwhile contribution to the literature.

Reviewer #2: The authors have satisfactorily responded to all my questions and made the necessary changes to the manuscript. The revised version of the manuscript appears to be good. I do not have any other questions.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Zezhi Li

15 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-13201R1

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

Dear Dr. Turgeon:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Zezhi Li

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES