Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Dec 31;15(12):e0240070. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240070

Nitrate and nitrite exposure leads to mild anxiogenic-like behavior and alters brain metabolomic profile in zebrafish

Manuel García-Jaramillo 1,2,3,4,*,#, Laura M Beaver 1,2,#, Lisa Truong 5, Elizabeth R Axton 2,6,¤a, Rosa M Keller 1, Mary C Prater 1,¤b, Kathy R Magnusson 2,7, Robyn L Tanguay 5, Jan F Stevens 2,6, Norman G Hord 1,8
Editor: Matthew Parker9
PMCID: PMC7774831  PMID: 33382700

Abstract

Dietary nitrate lowers blood pressure and improves athletic performance in humans, yet data supporting observations that it may increase cerebral blood flow and improve cognitive performance are mixed. We tested the hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite treatment would improve indicators of learning and cognitive performance in a zebrafish (Danio rerio) model. We utilized targeted and untargeted liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) analysis to examine the extent to which treatment resulted in changes in nitrate or nitrite concentrations in the brain and altered the brain metabolome. Fish were exposed to sodium nitrate (606.9 mg/L), sodium nitrite (19.5 mg/L), or control water for 2–4 weeks and free swim, startle response, and shuttle box assays were performed. Nitrate and nitrite treatment did not change fish weight, length, predator avoidance, or distance and velocity traveled in an unstressed environment. Nitrate- and nitrite-treated fish initially experienced more negative reinforcement and increased time to decision in the shuttle box assay, which is consistent with a decrease in associative learning or executive function however, over multiple trials, all treatment groups demonstrated behaviors associated with learning. Nitrate and nitrite treatment was associated with mild anxiogenic-like behavior but did not alter epinephrine, norepinephrine or dopamine levels. Targeted metabolomics analysis revealed no significant increase in brain nitrate or nitrite concentrations with treatment. Untargeted metabolomics analysis found 47 metabolites whose abundance was significantly altered in the brain with nitrate and nitrite treatment. Overall, the depletion in brain metabolites is plausibly associated with the regulation of neuronal activity including statistically significant reductions in the inhibitory neurotransmitter γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA; 18–19%), and its precursor, glutamine (17–22%). Nitrate treatment caused significant depletion in the brain concentration of fatty acids including linoleic acid (LA) by 50% and arachidonic acid (ARA) by 80%; nitrite treatment caused depletion of LA by ~90% and ARA by 60%, change which could alter the function of dopaminergic neurons and affect behavior. Nitrate and nitrite treatment did not adversely affect multiple parameters of zebrafish health. It is plausible that indirect NO-mediated mechanisms may be responsible for the nitrate and nitrite-mediated effects on the brain metabolome and behavior in zebrafish.

Introduction

Nitrate (NO3-), a component of leafy green and root vegetables, including beetroot juice (BRJ) and many green leafy vegetables, has blood pressuring-lowering and ergogenic effects in humans [1]. Nitrate supplementation (either as BRJ or sodium nitrate) has also demonstrated benefits pertaining to cardiovascular health [2], such as reducing blood pressure, enhancing blood flow, and elevating the driving pressure of O2 in the microcirculation to areas of hypoxia or exercising tissue [3, 4]. These findings are important to cardiovascular medicine and exercise physiology. Indeed, multiple studies support nitrate supplementation as an effective method to improve exercise performance [5, 6]. Additionally, it has been reported that dietary nitrate can modulate cerebral blood-flow (CBF), decrease reaction time in neuropsychological tests, improve cognitive performance and suggest one possible mechanism by which vegetable consumption may have beneficial effects on brain function in humans [7, 8]. In contrast, other recent studies have found no significant effect of nitrate or nitrite supplementation on cognitive function and this highlights the need for additional studies to clarify the effect of nitrate and nitrite treatment on cognitive function (reviewed in [9, 10]).

Nitric oxide (NO) is a gaseous, free radical signaling molecule produced via enzymatic and non-enzymatic pathways. The enzymatic pathways for NO synthesis are produced by three distinct families of nitric oxide synthase (NOS) enzymes in mammals that use L-arginine and numerous co-factors as substrates [11]. NO conveys essential signaling in the cardiovascular, central nervous, and immune systems [12]. NO, through formation of S-nitrosothiols and nitration of alkenes or other nitrated species, is also considered to have hormone-like properties that take part in different metabolic/endocrine disorders such as diabetes and dysglycemia, thyroid disorders, hypertension, heart failure, and obesity [13]. Furthermore, NO plays an important role in regulation of synaptogenesis and neurotransmission in the central and peripheral nervous system [14, 15]. NO can also be produced by a NO synthase-independent method through the nitrate-nitrite-nitric oxide pathway. Nitrate present in foods or water is reduced endogenously by lingual nitrate reductases in mammals to nitrite (NO2-) and, in the stomach, to nitric oxide (NO) before distribution via blood to tissues [16, 17]. Several endogenous enzymes, proteins, and chemical species can reduce nitrite to NO including deoxygenated hemoglobin, xanthine oxidoreductase, deoxymyoglobin, mitochondrial enzymes, ascorbic acid, etc. [18]. In spite of the vast amounts of research on NO production, NO-related signaling mechanisms, and the effects of nitrate supplementation on the cardiovascular system; there is still a gap in knowledge regarding whether dietary nitrate supplementation affects the brain metabolome, learning, and other brain functions.

In order to determine the physiological and cognitive effects derived from nitrate and nitrite exposure, we carried out a study with the aquatic model organism Danio Rerio (zebrafish). Zebrafish was chosen because it is a complex vertebrate organism that was originally established as a prime model for developmental studies and, is increasingly used for behavioral neuroscience research in part because of standardized and high throughput behavioral performance assays [1923]. Importantly, as in humans, the nitrate-nitrite-nitric oxide pathway and NOS enzymes play important roles in regulating NO levels, along with cardiac and blood vessel development in zebrafish [24]. In addition, high genetic homology exists between zebrafish and humans for genes associated with disease [25, 26]. Furthermore, we established that nitrate treatment in zebrafish improves the oxygen cost of exercise [27] as had been observed in humans. While conducting these experiments we also sought to test the hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite treatment would improve indicators of learning and cognitive performance. We also investigated the effects of nitrate and nitrite treatment on zebrafish behavior and the brain metabolome with the aim of elucidating mechanisms that may contribute to the potential improvement of cognitive performance. While these hypotheses are in keeping with nitrate literature in humans, there is also concern that nitrate and nitrite exposure from pollution has adverse effects on fish [28]. To this end, adult zebrafish were exposed to sodium nitrate, sodium nitrite, or control water and tested for changes in learning, memory, and behavior. Furthermore, we utilized targeted and untargeted liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) analysis to examine the extent to which treatment resulted in changed nitrate or nitrite concentrations in the brain and altered the brain metabolome.

Materials and methods

Fish husbandry

Wild type zebrafish (5D) were raised and maintained at the Sinnhuber Aquatic Research Laboratory (SARL) at Oregon State University on standard lab diet (Gemma Micro. Skretting, Tooele, France) in accordance with protocols approved by the Oregon State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). Adult fish were maintained at six fish per tank (3 male and 3 female) in 4-liter of aerated water in metal tanks. Experiments were conducted in several cohorts of healthy adult fish, fish from each cohort were all the same age and equally distributed between all treatment groups (cohorts ranged from 9–16 months in age, S1 Fig). Fish water was made with reverse-osmosis water supplemented with Instant Ocean® (Spectrum Brands Blacksburg, VA) at 1.4 g of salt/gallon of water and conductivity between 500–600 μS. Experiments contained three treatment groups which were treated for up to 31 days as 1) no treatment (control fish); 2) sodium nitrate-exposed fish (606.9 mg NaNO3 / L of water); and 3) sodium nitrite-exposed fish (19.5 mg NaNO2 / L of water). The nitrite dose was chosen because it increased blood nitrite levels and improved exercise performance, but was not associated with adverse effects at pathology with the exception of some mild irritation of gill epithelium [27, 29, 30]. For labeling experiments, a subset of fish was switched to water containing >99% stable isotopes of Na15NO3, or 100% Na15NO2 (Cambridge Isotope Laboratories, Tewksbury, MA) at day 28 for 3 days of treatment prior to collection. Nitrate and nitrite were dissolved in freshly prepared fish water and, unless otherwise indicated, chemicals were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO). The fish water and treatment exposure were replaced every 36 hours throughout the duration of the experiment to maintain low ammonia levels and consistent treatments; pH was held at 6.8–7, total ammonia levels to 0–2.0 ppm, and temperature at 27–29°C. Fish were fed a standard lab diet twice a day (Gemma Micro. Skretting, Westbrook, ME) at a volume of ~3% body weight/day. All efforts were made to ameliorate potential pain and distress including post-behavior assessments, tested zebrafish are housed together, and monitored for 24 hours for morphological (e.g. adhesions, bruising or abrasion to the skin) and abnormal swim defects. If any zebrafish exhibit these effects, they are kept in isolation and observed according to OSU Animal Care and Use Protocol. For sample collections fish were primarily euthanized with an overdose of the anesthesia drug tricaine mesylate, and all efforts were made to minimize suffering. Fish were then dried, weighed, measured for standard length, and brains were collected and snap frozen in liquid nitrogen. Fished used in hormone analysis were euthanized with rapid cooling, followed by the secondary method of cervical dislocation, and frozen in liquid nitrogen [31]. Samples were stored in -80°C until used for analysis.

Nitrate and nitrite quantification in water

Water was collected during the first week of the experiment and saved directly after a water change (designated as fresh), or 36 h post water change (designated as used). For nitrate measurements, fish water was snap frozen directly. For nitrite measurements, 1 mL fish water was mixed with 250 μL of a stop solution (containing potassium ferricyanide, N-ethylmaleimide, NP-40) as previously published [32]. Nitrate and nitrite concentrations were determined by ozone chemiluminescence as previously described on a Sievers Nitric Oxide Analyzer (NOA; Zysense, Frederick, CO) [30, 33].

Behavioral assays

Each zebrafish underwent behavioral assessments through a circuit of 1) swim behavior, 2) startle response and 3) learning and memory assays, where for each run, 4 fish per treatment were included. Multiple runs occurred over 4 days, between 14–17 days of treatment, and fish were within 4 h of being feed when assays were conducted. Swimming behavior and startle response was tested using a zebrafish visual imaging system (zVIS) as previously described in individual fish [34, 35]. Briefly, in the free swim assay fish were placed in a tank with 1.7L of water and the data from the first minute was ignored. The location of the fish was then analyzed by region of tank (top, middle, bottom) for the following 7 minutes (stressed, novel tank environment during minutes 1–8), and then during the last 7 minutes of the assay (minutes 11–18) speed and distance fish traveled was measured (unstressed environment). Habituation to an audio startle stimulus was tested in an array of 8 tanks (12cm × 12cm) filled with 750 mL of fish water [34]. Taps were generated by an electric solenoid below each tank. Following a 10-minute acclimation period, a total of five taps were delivered, with 20s following each tap, and the distance moved between taps was quantified.

Custom-built shuttle boxes were used to test learning with a modified protocol as previously described [34, 36]. The programmed protocol of this active avoidance conditioning test was designed to condition the zebrafish to leave the compartment with blue light (“reject side”) and swim to the dark side (“accept side”, also referred to as the correct side). The source of light is not at a fixed location in the box but rather depending on where the fish is in the box, the light opposite of the fish is turned on. There were a total of 30 trials; each trial consisted of giving the zebrafish 8 seconds to “seek” a dark side of the tank after the blue light came on to avoid a moderate shock. If the fish did not move to the correct side, the 16 second (s) shock period was initiated. A moderate pulse of 5 V was delivered at 1 s intervals, for a duration of 500 ms. Fish were removed from the assay when they did not swim to the correct side during 8 consecutive trials and these fish were counted as repeatedly failed. The statistical method remained as previously described [36], with the data fit using linear regression models to calculate the initial performance of the fish (intercept) and the rate of learning (slopes) for each recorded parameter including the period of time to decision and time shocked [36].

Epinephrine, norepinephrine, and dopamine quantification

Stress hormones epinephrine, norepinephrine, and their precursor dopamine were measured in fish (n = 12) using the 3-CAT ELISA (Rocky Mountain Diagnostics, Inc., Colorado Springs, CO) per manufacturer’s recommendations. Snap frozen whole zebrafish were ground in liquid nitrogen with mortar and pestle. To normalize variations in fish weight, the resulting whole fish powder was mixed with a buffer at a ratio of 100 mg fish powder to 500 μL HCL buffer solution, containing EDTA and sodium metabisulfite. Samples were centrifuged for 20 minutes at 10,000 × g at 4°C and supernatants collected. A standard curve was generated for each compound concentrations of 0.5, 1.5, 5, 20, and 80 ng/mL, for epinephrine and dopamine, and 0.2, 0.6, 2.0, 8.0, and 32.0 ng/mL for norepinephrine. Samples were diluted 1:1 to be in the range of the standard curve. A Spectramax® M2 plate reader (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA) was used to measure concentration at 450 nm.

Extraction of zebrafish brains for analysis

Twelve brains per treatment group were snap frozen using liquid nitrogen after four weeks of treatment and two brains were pooled together to compose each sample. Each sample was added into 2 mL pre-filled tubes containing 300 mg of RNAse and DNAse free zirconium oxide beads (0.5 mm diameter, ceria stabilized, Next Advance, Averill Park, NY). A mixture of 80:20 methanol: water at -80°C was used as the extraction solvent as previously described [37]. Brains were homogenized with a bullet blender (Precellys® 24-bead-based homogenizer for 2 minutes at 1350 rpm). Extracts were incubated at -20°C for 1 hour and then centrifuged at 13,000 rpm (Eppendorf, Hauppauge, NY) and 4°C for 10 min. The supernatant was split into three 1.5 mL Eppendorf tubes: 100 μL was aliquoted for nitrate and nitrite isotope targeted analysis by LC-MS/MS; 200 μL was aliquoted for untargeted metabolomics analysis, and the remainder (variable volume) was reserved and stored at -80°C.

Targeted LC-MS/MS analysis of nitrate and nitrite

In order to quantify nitrate and nitrite uptake into the brain, we used a previously described LC-MS/MS approach [38]. Assessing the percent enrichment (15N/(15N+14N) x 100%) allows us to determine the proportion of the nitrate and nitrite that was derived from exogenous sources (stable isotope treatment in water) versus endogenous source (nitrate oxidized from NO produced by NOS enzymes). This method utilizes 2,3-diaminonaphthalene (DAN) derivatization, which reacts with nitrite under acidic conditions to produce 2,3-naphthotriazole (NAT). The production NAT was measured with the previously described method [38] with minor modifications. Briefly, NAT was chromatographically separated on an InfinityLab Poroshell 120 HPH-C18 column (2.7 μm, 2.1 × 50 mm, Agilent, Santa Clara, CA), in a run time of 10 minutes, and detected using a multiple reaction monitoring (MRM) method on an ABSciex 3200 QTRAP mass spectrometer operated in positive ionization mode. Mass spectrometry allows for the quantification of 14N-NAT (m/z 170.1) and 15N-NAT (m/z 171.1). The percent enrichment (%) was calculated as: [15N/(15N+14N) × 100].

Untargeted LC-MS/MS metabolomics analysis

Aliquoted extracts were sonicated for 5 minutes and clarified by centrifugation at 13,000 rpm for 10 minutes. The supernatant was transferred to glass mass spectrometry vials and LC-MS/MS-based metabolomics was performed as previously described [27, 39]. Briefly, ultra-high-pressure liquid chromatography (UPLC) was performed on a Shimadzu Nexera system (Shimadzu, Columbia, MD) coupled to a quadrupole time-of-flight mass spectrometer (AB SCIEX TripleTOF 5600). Chromatographic separations were conducted on an Inertsil® Phenyl-3 column (4.6 × 150 mm, GL Sciences, Torrance, CA). Elution was achieved using a binary gradient employing as solvent A water, and solvent B methanol, both containing 0.1% formic acid (v/v), as described previously [39]. LC-MS/MS conditions were adapted from Kirkwood et al. (2012) [39] with some modifications. The gradient started with 5% B and was held for 1 min at 5% B, followed by a 11-min linear gradient from 5% to 30% B. The gradient was increased linearly to 100% B at 23 min, held for 5 min at 100% B and, finally, stepped back to 5% B to equilibrate the column. The flow rate was 0.4 mL/min. The auto-sampler temperature was held at 10°C, the column oven temperature at 50°C, and the injection volume was 5 μL. Time-of-flight (TOF) mass spectrometry (MS) was operated with an acquisition time of 0.25 s and a scan range of 70–1200 Da. Tandem mass spectrometry (MS/MS) acquisition was performed with collision energy set at 35 V and collision energy spread of 15 V. Each MS/MS scan had an accumulation time of 0.17 s and a range of 50–1250 Da using information-dependent MS/MS acquisition (IDA). Ion source gas 1 and 2 and curtain gas (all nitrogen) were set at 50, 40, and 25, respectively. The source temperature was set at 500°C and the ion spray voltage at 4.5 kV in positive ion mode. The mass calibration was automatically performed every 6 injections using an APCI positive/negative calibration solution (AB SCIEX) via a calibration delivery system (CDS). A separate quality control (QC) pool sample was prepared by combining 5 μL of each sample. Quality control was assured by: (i) randomization of the sequence, (ii) injection of QC pool samples at the beginning and the end of the sequence and between each 10 actual samples, (iii) procedure blank analysis.

Untargeted metabolomics data processing

Raw data was imported into PeakView with XIC Manager 1.2.0 (ABSciex, Framingham, MA) for peak picking, retention time correction, and peak alignment. Metabolite identities were assigned as previously described by matching with an in-house library consisting of IROA standards (IROA Technology, Bolton, MA) and other commercially available standards (650 total) [27]. The peak list was exported to MultiQuant 3.0.2 to integrate chromatograms to obtain peak area values for all of the assigned metabolites.

Statistical analysis

To determine significant differences between three treatment group data were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA with Tukey post hoc test (P-value < 0.05, statistically significant) with GraphPad Prism 4 software (La Jolla, CA). Significant differences were calculated with two-way ANOVA and Tukey post hoc test or a repeated measures two-way ANOVA and Tukey post-hoc test (P–value < 0.05, statistically significant) when both treatment and another condition, like water condition, zone of tank, or a behavioral stimulus, was present [40]. For the shuttle box assay a linear regression was fitted to the data for each treatment to generate initial time and rate of learning graphs, while a separate analysis of variance (AOV) followed by a Tukey’s statistical difference was used to calculate statistical significance amongst the groups [36]. For metabolomics data, annotated metabolites were used to conduct multivariate statistical analysis. Pathway analysis and partial least squares-discriminant analysis (PLS-DA), were generated with MetaboAnalyst 4.0 [41]. The significance of individual metabolites between the treatment groups was assessed with a one-way ANOVA followed by Fisher’s post-hoc analysis and Holm FDR-correction, with a P-value of < 0.05 and a q-value <0.1 indicating significance. If needed, data were logarithmically transformed to correct for unequal variance or non-normal distribution. No outliers were excluded from the statistical analyses. Figures were generated with Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA), PowerPoint 2016 (Microsoft, Redmond, WA), and MetaboAnalyst 4.0 [41].

Results

Effect of nitrate and nitrite treatment on health parameters and learning

Treatment increased nitrate or nitrite levels in the fresh and used fish water (Fig 1A and 1B). Furthermore, both nitrate and nitrite concentrations in control water were maintained at low levels throughout the treatment period (Fig 1A and 1B). Several parameters of fish health, including fish length and weight, were not significantly changed with nitrate or nitrite treatment (Fig 1C and 1D). Likewise, no significant differences were found between treatment groups for the distance and velocity fish traveled in an unstressed environment (Fig 1E and 1F, P = 0.2089 and 0.2088, respectively). No differences were detected in habituation to a startle with nitrate or nitrite treatment, but nitrate-treated fish had a slight reduction (10%) in startle response as measured by significantly less distance traveled (Fig 1G).

Fig 1. Nitrate and nitrite treatment did not adversely affect multiple parameters of health but nitrate treatment significantly decreased movement following a startle.

Fig 1

Adult zebrafish were treated with control water, sodium nitrate, or sodium nitrite and (A) nitrate and (B) nitrite concentrations were measured in newly treated fish water (fresh) and water at the end of 42-hour use (used) (n = 7–10). Fish (C) length and (D) weight was measured after 28–31 days of treatment (n = 18–33). At 14–17 days of treatment the (E) distance and (F) velocity zebrafish traveled was quantified in the voluntary swimming assay (n = 39–42) or (G) the response to five sequential acoustic startles (as taps against the fish tank) was quantified (n = 84–90). (A-G) Bars represent the mean ± SEM.

In order to address if nitrate and nitrite treatments altered learning, fish were tested in a learning and memory assay using custom-built shuttle box, where over 30 consecutive trials they learned to avoid an adverse event (mild shock) by moving when a light came on (Fig 2A). As seen from the linear regression calculated from the data, both nitrate and nitrite treated fish initially took longer to make a decision and were shocked longer (Fig 2B). Over subsequent trials, more nitrate- and nitrite-treated fish (5–7% of the population tested) had to be removed from the assay because they repeatedly failed to learn (Fig 2C). However, as the experiment was repeated over multiple trials both nitrate- and nitrite-treated fish were able to learn as seen in improvements in decision time, time shocked, and rate of learning over trials (Fig 2D). These observations from the linear regression hold when analyzing the data for statistical significance; overall, nitrate and nitrite treatment significantly increased the percentage of fish that failed to make a decision and were shocked (Fig 2E). When the population of fish analyzed was filtered to include only fish that could learn (i.e., completed the assay), the nitrite-treated fish were no longer significantly impaired but significant deficits were still present in nitrate-treated fish for time shocked and time to decision (Fig 2E).

Fig 2. Nitrate and nitrite treatment were associated with an initial decline in learning, but fish learned over repeated tests.

Fig 2

Adult zebrafish were treated with control water, sodium nitrate, or sodium nitrite for 14–17 days when learning and memory were tested in the shuttle box assay (n = 72–109). (A) Time-to-decision and time shocked was recorded for each fish and trial (dots) and linear regression of the data were calculated (lines). As calculated from the linear regression the bars indicate (B) initial periods of time fish spent for the indicated measure and (D) rates of learning as quantified by the slope from the linear regression. (C) Bars indicate the percentage of fish that were removed from the assay because they did not swim to the correct side during eight consecutive trials. (E) Statistical summary of shuttle box results as calculated by an analysis of variance (AOV) followed by a Tukey’s post-test where “All” indicates data from all fish analyzed, while “Completed trials” excludes data from fish that repeatedly failed.

Effect of nitrate and nitrite treatment on behavior and catecholamine levels

The effect of nitrate and nitrite exposure on behavior was tested in the free swim assay where fish were placed in a novel tank. As expected, control fish spent similar amounts of time at all three depths of the tank, balancing safety from predation and opportunity to find food (Fig 3A). In contrast, there was a significant difference between the time the nitrate- and nitrite-treated fish spent between the bottom and top zones (Fig 3A). Thus nitrate- and nitrite-treated fish spent 22–35% more time in the bottom zone, as compared to control fish, although there was not a significant treatment effect when just the bottom zone was considered. Nevertheless, the increase in bottom-dwelling is suggestive of mild anxiogenic-like behavior. Since anxiety can be associated with stress, we measured the levels of some stress hormones and found neither nitrate, nor nitrite treatment significantly increased epinephrine or norepinephrine levels (Fig 3B–3C). It appeared that nitrite-treated fish experienced lower concentrations of these hormones yet high variability between fish led to no significant differences being detected. Nitrate or nitrite treatment also did not significantly change dopamine concentrations which is the precursor for epinephrine and norepinephrine (Fig 3D).

Fig 3. Nitrate and nitrite treatment increased mild anxiogenic-like behavior in zebrafish.

Fig 3

Adult zebrafish were treated with control water, sodium nitrate, or sodium nitrite for 14–17 days. (A) Movement in a novel tank was recorded and the percent of time spent in the bottom, middle and top zones of tank are indicated (n = 83–89). (B-D) Concentrations of hormones were measured in whole fish by ELISA (n = 12). (A-D) Bars represent the mean ± SEM.

Nitrate and nitrite uptake into the brain

For the last three days of the experiment, a subset of fish was treated with 15N-nitrate or 15N-nitrite in order to study the uptake of nitrate and nitrite into brain tissue. The resulting percent enrichment results show the proportion of nitrate and nitrite derived from exogenous sources (the treatment in water) versus endogenous sources such as oxidation of NO from NOS-mediated production. We observed a low uptake of nitrate (14%) and almost no uptake of nitrite (0.1%) in the brain, which can be seen by comparing the fish that received labeled nitrate or nitrite as compared to the respective unlabeled nitrate or nitrite treatment conditions (Fig 4A and 4B). Furthermore, no significant changes in nitrate or nitrite concentrations were detected in the brain of animals treated with nitrate or nitrite (Fig 4A and 4B) when compared with the control group. Taken together, these results suggest that the behavioral changes observed with nitrate and nitrite exposure are likely due to indirect effects of treatment on brain metabolism, rather than a direct effect via influx of the nitrate or nitrite into the brain.

Fig 4. Little uptake of nitrate or nitrite from treatments was found in the brain.

Fig 4

The concentration of (A) nitrate or (B) nitrite was measured using targeted LC-MS/MS in control animals or animals treated with (A) unlabeled and 15N-labeled nitrate, or (B) unlabeled and 15N-labeled nitrite. (A-B) Zebrafish brains were collected on day 31 and percent enrichment (in boxes), indicates the relative amount of nitrate or nitrite in the brain that was derived from the treatment. Bars represent the mean concentration ± SEM (n = 6).

Metabolomics results

One hundred twenty-four (124) metabolites were annotated using our in-house library (S1 Table). Of these metabolites, 47 were significantly changed among at least one treatment group, as compared to the others and FDR-corrected P-values (q-values) for all significantly changed metabolites, between all treatment groups, are listed in S2 Table. For example, deoxyadenosine diphosphate (dADP) was significantly up-regulated (q = 0.018) in fish exposed to nitrate and nitrite, and desmosterol, the immediate precursor of cholesterol in the Bloch pathway of cholesterol biosynthesis, was significantly down-regulated (q = 0.018) in fish exposed to nitrate and nitrite.

Partial least squares discriminant analysis (PLS-DA) demonstrates spatial clustering and separation between treatment groups when considering all the annotated compounds (Fig 5A). There are two importance measures in PLS-DA: one is variable importance in projection (VIP) and the other is the weighted sum of absolute regression coefficients. The VIP graph of the most relevant 30 features (when considering the three treatments) is shown in Fig 5B. The colored boxes on the right indicate the relative concentrations of the corresponding metabolite in each group under study. Among the positively correlated metabolites with the highest VIP scores associated with the PLS-DA were dADP, desmosterol, linoleic acid, suberic acid, oleic acid and guanine.

Fig 5. Nitrate and nitrite treatment significantly altered the abundance of some brain metabolites.

Fig 5

Adult zebrafish were treated with control water, sodium nitrate, or sodium nitrite for 31 days and brain metabolites were measured using untargeted LC-MS/MS. (A) Partial least squares discriminant analysis (PLS-DA) scored plot demonstrates spatial clustering and separation between treatment groups when considering all the annotated compounds. (B) PLS-DA variable importance in projection (VIP) graph of the most relevant 30 features (when considering the three treatments). Colored boxes at right indicate the mean relative concentrations of the corresponding metabolite in each treatment group under study. Red color indicates higher abundance, while green color indicates lower abundance. The PLS-DA model display 95% confidence region. Abbreviations: dADP (deoxyadenosine diphosphate), NAM (N-acetyl-L-methionine), 3-Hydroxybenzo (3-Hydroxybenzoic acid), 1-Aminocyclopr (1-Aminocyclopropane-1-carboxylate), 3PG (3-Phosphoglyceric acid), NADP (Nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate), ARA (Arachidonic acid), NAD (Nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide), EPA (Eicosapentanoic acid), 12-Hydroxydode (12-Hydroxydodecanoic acid), MMA (Methylmalonic acid), 2-Hydroxybutyrate (2-Hydroxybutyric acid).

Notably, nitrate or nitrite treatment resulted in significant differences among multiple metabolites involved in purine metabolism like hypoxanthine, xanthine, inosine, guanine, guanosine, deoxyadenosine diphosphate (dADP) and cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP). Interestingly, we also observed a significant decline in nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate (NADP) and NAD+. We observed a significant depletion in the annotated fatty acids (linoleic acid (LA), eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), and arachidonic acid (ARA)) with nitrate and nitrite treatments. Remarkably, LA was depleted by 50% by nitrate treatment and by ~90% in the nitrite treatment when considering normalized peak intensity values. Similarly, ARA was depleted by 80 and 60% in the nitrate and nitrite treatments, respectively. We also observed lower abundances for some of the annotated TCA (tricarboxylic acid cycle) cycle intermediates (i.e., malate and succinic acid). A depletion in amino acids threonine, N-acetyl-L-methionine (NAM), and phosphoserine, was observed with nitrate and nitrite treatment. Notably, we observed that nitrate and nitrite treatment had an effect on γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA, Fig 6A), the chief inhibitory neurotransmitter in the developmentally mature central nervous system and its precursor, glutamine (Fig 6B) [42]. Nitrate treatment caused a significant 22% reduction in the abundance of glutamine and 19% reduction in GABA in zebrafish brains. Nitrite treatment also caused a significant 17% reduction in the abundance of glutamine and 18% reduction of GABA in zebrafish brains. Interestingly, no significant differences in the abundance of the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate were found with nitrate or nitrite treatments, thus GABA abundance changed in parallel with glutamine, but not glutamate levels.

Fig 6. Nitrate and nitrite treatment decrease gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and glutamine levels in zebrafish brain.

Fig 6

Adult zebrafish were treated with control water, sodium nitrate, or sodium nitrite for 31 days. Abundance of (A) GABA and (B) glutamine was measured in brain tissue by LC-MS/MS. Bars represent the mean peak area ± SEM (n = 6).

Discussion

We did not find evidence to support the hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite treatment would improve indicators of learning and cognitive performance in healthy zebrafish. While nitrate and nitrite treatment did not adversely affect multiple parameters of health, these treatments were associated with mild anxiogenic-like behavior and an initial deficit in learning, which was consistent with either decreased executive function or associative learning. The zebrafish in this study exhibited behavior we interpreted as mild anxiety, as evidenced by staying near the bottom of the novel tank. The mild anxiogenic-like behavior observed in nitrate- or nitrite-treated zebrafish was similar in scale to zebrafish that were not allowed to exercise [43].

Nitrate, but not nitrite treatment, caused a slight reduction in startle reactivity, but no difference was detected in habituation with treatment. We also observed an initial delay in zebrafish decision making following a light stimulus and increased time being shocked initially in the shuttle box task which could represent an initial deficit in associative learning and/or executive function (e.g., decision making). This is inconsistent with literature that showed nitrate, given as BRJ supplement, improved reaction time and cognitive performance [7, 4446]. A plausible mechanism underlying nitrate-induced cognitive improvements is increased NO-mediated vasodilation, yielding improved CBF [7, 9, 47, 48]. This is exemplified by a study in older adults where two days of consuming a high nitrate diet increased regional cerebral perfusion in frontal lobe white matter, particularly between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex [47]. These brain regions participate in executive function, which may have been affected by nitrate and nitrite treatment in our study. In contrast with our results, multiple studies show no significant association with foods containing nitrate and changes in cognitive function or mood [4955]. Possible factors contributing to conflicting cognitive responses reports and our own study include different routes of treatment (continual in water vs episodic in meals), different nitrate doses and lengths of treatment, food matrix effects, age and health status of participants, or unidentified species-specific effects. A significant limitation of this study is the use of whole zebrafish brains to derive metabolomics data, limiting our ability to draw inferences to specific functional structures within the brain, like the zebrafish equivalent of the prefrontal cortex [56].

While we have previously shown the nitrate and nitrite doses used here increased blood and whole-body nitrate and nitrite levels, the treatments were not associated with a significant increase in the concentration of nitrate or nitrate in the brain, and only a minor, or almost no uptake of these chemicals into the brain. Therefore, we propose that the observed changes in behavior due to nitrate and nitrite exposure are due to indirect effects of NO on cell signaling mechanisms in neurons, astrocytes or other cell types due to the observed changes in annotated metabolites. Namely, enhanced signaling through NO-dependent mechanisms, such as enhanced cGMP and NO-dependent S-nitrosylation are associated with changes in brain ion channel function [57]. Our data show that nitrate or nitrite treatment led to modulation of several important brain metabolites including neurotransmitters and their precursors, fatty acids, purine nucleotides and nucleosides, and amino acids, fatty acids. While the evidentiary basis is circumstantial, it is tempting to associate modulation of concentrations of these metabolites with changes in neuronal structure, function and signaling with the observed behavioral changes due to nitrate and nitrite.

The observed reduction of brain GABA and glutamine levels with nitrate and nitrite treatment is noteworthy because perturbations in the GABAergic system have been associated with anxiety and depression and thus may be central to the behavioral changes we observed [5860]. Glutamine is a substrate for GABA production and serves as an important energy source for the nervous system. We also observed changes in brain purine-related metabolites, which is consistent with the known relationship between exogenous and endogenous pathways that generate NO [18, 24, 27]. Notably, the nitrate- and nitrite-dependent decreases in nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate (NADP) and NAD+ to recapitulate changes observed in mitochondrial dysfunction and aging in the brain [61]. Physiologically, NAD+ depletion is also observed in response to excessive DNA damage due to free radical attack, resulting in significant poly(ADP-ribose) polymerase (PARP) activation and a high turnover and subsequent depletion of NAD+, as well as in chronic immune activation and inflammatory cytokine production resulting in accelerated CD38 activity and decline in NAD+ concentrations. If nitrate and nitrite exposure were mechanistically linked to decreased NAD+ levels, this could link oxidative damage to low NAD+ concentrations to inflammation and neurological dysfunction in the brain.

The decreased brain concentrations of fatty acids including LA, EPA, ARA observed with nitrate and nitrite treatment are similar to our previous observations in nitrite-treated whole fish [27]. We surmised that nitrite exposure in whole zebrafish led to the stimulation of fatty acid oxidation for energy production. It is unclear why nitrate, and especially nitrite, would selectively stimulate depletion of these fatty acids in brain. These diet-derived fatty acids are a major component of neuronal membranes which are important in brain development and function [62]. Low levels of very long chain PUFAs in the brain also affect the brain dopamine systems and influence risk, along with other genetic factors, of neurological disorders, including Parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Altering membrane PUFA composition in vitro has been shown to alter the function and/or signaling of a variety of receptors, including dopaminergic, cholinergic, and GABAergic receptors, as well as the Na+/K+ ATPase. Taken together with the observation that nitrate and nitrite exposure decreased the concentration of brain desmosterol, the precursor of cholesterol biosynthesis, it is biologically plausible that cell membrane composition of cholesterol, LA, EPA and ARA could lead to changes in neuron function. These structural changes in membrane composition in neurons could potentially interact with other observed changes in metabolites that could influence behavior, including altered concentrations of GABA and glutamate.

Besides the indirect mechanisms by which nitrate and nitrite could mediate the effects observed in these studies, NO exerts direct neurological effects including acting as an anterograde neurotransmitter, a retrograde neurotransmitter, regulator of presynaptic plasticity in GABAnergic and glutamatergic neurons, and effecting dendritic spine growth (reviewed in [63]). NO is directly involved in learning and memory, and NO modulators are being explored for the treatment of anxiety [64]. Manipulation of brain NO levels in rodents decreased anxiety when specific doses of L-arginine (NO precursor), L-NAME (NOS inhibitor), or sodium nitroprusside (NO donor) were given, but a high dose of L-NAME decreased locomotor activity, similar to our result [65, 66]. Consistent with the observed mild anxiogenic-like behavior, studies in mice showed anxiogenic effects of sildenafil (NO), or the combined treatment of sildenafil and ascorbic acid [6769]. As manipulation of NO levels in the brain can yield both anxiolytic or anxiogenic effects, it is possible that the changes in behavior we observed may be attributable to high NO levels, but we have not assessed surrogate markers of this phenomenon, such as nitrated tyrosine levels in brain tissue [64].

There is a substantial body of evidence linking intake of diets high in nitrate, as well as pharmaceutical exposure to NO donor drugs, to risk of headache. As such, we could speculate that NO-dependent mechanisms could contribute to the observed changes in zebrafish behavior due to nitrate and nitrite treatment. Organic nitrates, which serve as vasodilatory NO prodrugs, such as nitroglycerin, and sildenafil, a PDE inhibitor that inhibits NO degradation, are each associated with side effects that include headaches. In humans, animal models and fish, organic nitrates like nitroglycerin have been used to model migraines [70, 71]. Organic nitrates cause NO-mediated vasodilation of blood vessels in the brain, and “immediate” mild-to-medium severity headaches or “delayed” migraines are associated with increased cGMP or NO-dependent S-nitrosylation-mediated changes. Headache is also the most common side effect in patients taking sildenafil, which promotes blood flow to organs like the brain, through cGMP. Furthermore, consumption of high nitrite foods was associated with headaches in some people [72]. Migraines have also been correlated in humans with oral microbiomes that increased abundances of nitrate, nitrite, and NO reductase genes supporting that nitrite and NO could promote migraines [73]. Taken together, it is plausible to speculate that nitrate- or nitrite-induced production of NO in blood vessels stimulated vasodilation and caused a headache or migraine.

There are well-established adverse effects of nitrate pollution in aquatic ecosystems, referred to as eutrophication, that affect a variety of species (reviewed in [28]). Likewise, human consumption of nitrate- and nitrite-contaminated water or excessive intake from vegetables may also cause adverse effects [74]. An endocrine-disrupting role of nitrate and nitrite has been observed in various species, and the possible pathways of altering steroidogenesis have been proposed [75]. Both glutamate and GABA are involved in pituitary hormone release in fish. There is also good evidence for the involvement of GABA in luteinizing hormone release in fish [76]. Other studies have indicated that high nitrate and nitrite exposure from drinking water and diet may exert adverse effects on the development of the human nervous system [77, 78]. Nitrate and nitrite can also perturb the activity of dopaminergic (DA) neurons by acting through estrogen receptor (ER) in early development of zebrafish [79] at concentrations around the safety limit for drinking water recommended by the Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) and the World Health Organization (WHO) (10 mg/L NO3-N and 1 mg/L NO2-N, respectively) [80]. While many of these studies were conducted during embryonic development, and are different from own limited adult exposure, they highlight that nitrate and nitrite can have significant effects on the central nervous system.

As with all studies conducted in model organisms, there are some specific contextual factors that make comparison to humans difficult. While zebrafish are used to model complex brain disorders, including anxiety, limitations exist because we must infer pain, discomfort or other behaviors through observation [21, 22, 81, 82]. Another unique aspect of zebrafish exposure is ammonia in water. To address this potentially toxic metabolite, we regularly measured ammonia and found no effect of nitrate or nitrite treatment on water ammonia levels. Due to the large number of animals needed to conduct the study, we were limited in the number of doses we could test and thus focused on a nitrate dose and exposure duration associated with improvements in exercise performance [27]. Another limitation of our study is that it was conducted in healthy adult zebrafish thus it is not clear what effect nitrate or nitrite treatment would have in the presence of conditions like neurodegenerative disorders. More and larger studies are needed to delineate the potential benefits and risks associated with nitrate and/or nitrite treatment on CBF, mood, and cognitive function, particularly in populations of people with differing ages and underlying health status. Importantly, a study in humans is underway to look at the effect of increasing doses of nitrate on cognition-related outcomes [83]. We also cannot differentiate between the direct effects of nitrate or nitrite in the fish, or indirect effects that could be generated by increased NO availability. Nevertheless, we show that nitrate and nitrite treatment in a zebrafish model did not adversely affect multiple parameters of health but was associated with mild anxiogenic-like behavior, changes in brain metabolome, and an initial decrease in executive function or associative learning.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Experiment design summary.

(TIFF)

S1 Table. Metabolites annotated using our in-house library.

(XLSX)

S2 Table. Significantly changed metabolites, between all treatment groups.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Lindsey St. Mary, Eric Johnson, Carrie L. Barton, Sabrina Edwards, and Kimberly Hayward (Sinnhuber Aquatic Research Laboratory), Claudia S. Maier (Department of Chemistry and OSU Mass Spectrometry Center), and Jeffrey Morrè (Operational Manager, Oregon State University Mass Spectrometry Center) for technical assistance and advice.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported in part by Celia Strickland and G. Kenneth Austin III Endowment (NGH), the Oregon Agricultural Experimental Station and OSU College of Pharmacy Faculty Development Funds (JFS). It was also supported by National Institutes of Health grants 1S10RR027878-01 (JFS), NCCIH 2R90AT008924-06 (MGJ), and NIEHS Environmental Health Sciences P30 ES030287 (RLT). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Hord N. G., Tang Y. & Bryan N. S. Food sources of nitrates and nitrites: the physiologic context for potential health benefits. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 90, 1–10 (2009). 10.3945/ajcn.2008.27131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hord N. G. Dietary nitrates, nitrites, and cardiovascular disease. Curr. Atheroscler. Rep. 13, 484–492 (2011). 10.1007/s11883-011-0209-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Webb A. J. et al. Acute blood pressure lowering, vasoprotective, and antiplatelet properties of dietary nitrate via bioconversion to nitrite. Hypertension 51, 784–790 (2008). 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.107.103523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kapil et al. Inorganic Nitrate Supplementation Lowers Blood Pressure in Humans. Hypertension 56, 274–281 (2010). 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.110.153536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hoon M. W., Johnson N. A., Chapman P. G. & Burke L. M. The effect of nitrate supplementation on exercise performance in healthy individuals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Sport Nutr. Exerc. Metab. 23, 522–532 (2013). 10.1123/ijsnem.23.5.522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jones A. M. Dietary Nitrate Supplementation and Exercise Performance. 44, (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wightman E. L. et al. Dietary nitrate modulates cerebral blood flow parameters and cognitive performance in humans: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Physiol. Behav. 149, 149–158 (2015). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gilchrist M. et al. Nitric Oxide Dietary nitrate supplementation improves reaction time in type 2 diabetes: Development and application of a novel nitrate-depleted beetroot juice placebo. Nitric Oxide 40, 67–74 (2014). 10.1016/j.niox.2014.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Stanaway L., Rutherfurd-Markwick K., Page R. & Ali A. Performance and Health Benefits of Dietary Nitrate Supplementation in Older Adults: A Systematic Review. Nutrients 9, (2017). 10.3390/nu9111171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McDonagh S. T. J., Wylie L. J., Thompson C., Vanhatalo A. & Jones A. M. Potential benefits of dietary nitrate ingestion in healthy and clinical populations: A brief review. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 19, 15–29 (2019). 10.1080/17461391.2018.1445298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stuehr D. J. & Vasquez-Vivar J. Nitric oxide synthases-from genes to function. Nitric oxide: biology and chemistry 63, 29 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Garthwaite J. Glutamate, nitric oxide and cell-cell signalling in the nervous system. Trends Neurosci. 14, 60–67 (1991). 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90022-m [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ghasemi A. & Zahediasl S. Is nitric oxide a hormone? Iran. Biomed. J. 15, 59–65 (2011). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Godfrey E. W. & Schwarte R. C. The role of nitric oxide signaling in the formation of the neuromuscular junction. J. Neurocytol. 32, 591–602 (2003). 10.1023/B:NEUR.0000020612.87729.98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kiss J. P. Role of nitric oxide in the regulation of monoaminergic neurotransmission. Brain Res. Bull. 52, 459–466 (2000). 10.1016/s0361-9230(00)00282-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zweier J. L., Wang P., Samouilov A. & Kuppusamy P. Enzyme-independent formation of nitric oxide in biological tissues. Nat. Med. 1, 804–809 (1995). 10.1038/nm0895-804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Zweier J. L., Samouilov A. & Kuppusamy P. Non-enzymatic nitric oxide synthesis in biological systems. Biochim. Biophys. Acta—Bioenerg. 1411, 250–262 (1999). 10.1016/s0005-2728(99)00018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lundberg J. O., Weitzberg E. & Gladwin M. T. The nitrate-nitrite-nitric oxide pathway in physiology and therapeutics. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 7, 156–167 (2008). 10.1038/nrd2466 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bailey J., Oliveri A. & Levin E. D. Zebrafish model systems for developmental neurobehavioral toxicology. Birth Defects Res. C. Embryo Today 99, 14–23 (2013). 10.1002/bdrc.21027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bugel S. M., Tanguay R. L. & Planchart A. Zebrafish: A marvel of high-throughput biology for 21(st) century toxicology. Curr. Environ. Heal. reports 1, 341–352 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Müller, T. E. et al. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry Understanding the neurobiological e ff ects of drug abuse: Lessons from zebra fi sh models. 100, (2020). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Shams S., Rihel J., Ortiz J. G. & Gerlai R. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews The zebra fi sh as a promising tool for modeling human brain disorders: A review based upon an IBNS Symposium. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 85, 176–190 (2018). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Stewart A. M., Braubach O., Spitsbergen J., Gerlai R. & Kalueff A. V. Zebrafish models for translational neuroscience research: from tank to bedside. Trends Neurosci. 37, 264–278 (2014). 10.1016/j.tins.2014.02.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Jensen F. B. Nitric oxide formation from nitrite in zebrafish. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 3387 LP– 3394 (2007). 10.1242/jeb.008748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Howe K. et al. The zebrafish reference genome sequence and its relationship to the human genome. Nature 496, 498–503 (2013). 10.1038/nature12111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sykes B. G. et al. The Relationship between Estrogen and Nitric Oxide in the Prevention of Cardiac and Vascular Anomalies in the Developing Zebrafish (Danio Rerio). Brain Sci. 6, 51 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Axton E. R. et al. Treatment with Nitrate, but Not Nitrite, Lowers the Oxygen Cost of Exercise and Decreases Glycolytic Intermediates While Increasing Fatty Acid Metabolites in Exercised Zebrafish. J. Nutr. 149, 2120–2132 (2019). 10.1093/jn/nxz202 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Camargo J. A., Alonso A. & Salamanca A. Nitrate toxicity to aquatic animals: a review with new data for freshwater invertebrates. Chemosphere 58, 1255–1267 (2005). 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2004.10.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Learmonth C. & Carvalho P. Acute and Chronic Toxicity of Nitrate to Early Life Stages of Zebrafish—Setting Nitrate Safety Levels for Zebrafish Rearing. 12, 305–311 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Voslarova E., Pištěková V. & Svobodová Z. Nitrite Toxicity to Danio Rerio: Effects of Fish Age and Chloride Concentrations. Acta Vet. Brno. 75, 107–113 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wallace C. K. et al. Effectiveness of Rapid Cooling as a Method of Euthanasia for Young Zebrafish (Danio rerio). J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 57, 58–63 (2018). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Piknova B. & Schechter A. N. Measurement of nitrite in blood samples using the ferricyanide-based hemoglobin oxidation assay. Methods Mol. Biol. 704, 39–56 (2011). 10.1007/978-1-61737-964-2_4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Conley M. N., Roberts C., Sharpton T. J., Iwaniec U. T. & Hord N. G. Increasing dietary nitrate has no effect on cancellous bone loss or fecal microbiome in ovariectomized rats. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 61, (2017). 10.1002/mnfr.201600372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Knecht A. L. et al. Transgenerational inheritance of neurobehavioral and physiological de fi cits from developmental exposure to benzo [a] pyrene in zebra fi sh. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 329, 148–157 (2017). 10.1016/j.taap.2017.05.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Knecht A. L., Truong L., Simonich M. T. & Tanguay R. L. Developmental benzo[a]pyrene (B[a]P) exposure impacts larval behavior and impairs adult learning in zebrafish. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 59, 27–34 (2017). 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Truong L., Mandrell D., Mandrell R., Simonich M. & Tanguay R. L. NeuroToxicology A rapid throughput approach identifies cognitive deficits in adult zebrafish from developmental exposure to polybrominated flame retardants. Neurotoxicology 43, 134–142 (2014). 10.1016/j.neuro.2014.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Choi J. et al. Novel function of vitamin E in regulation of zebrafish (Danio rerio) brain lysophospholipids discovered using lipidomics. J. Lipid Res. 56, 1182–1190 (2015). 10.1194/jlr.M058941 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Axton E. R., Hardardt E. A. & Stevens J. F. Stable isotope-assisted LC-MS/MS monitoring of glyceryl trinitrate bioactivation in a cell culture model of nitrate tolerance. J. Chromatogr. B, Anal. Technol. Biomed. life Sci. 1019, 156–163 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kirkwood J. S. et al. Vitamin C deficiency activates the purine nucleotide cycle in zebrafish. J. Biol. Chem. 287, 3833–3841 (2012). 10.1074/jbc.M111.316018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Garcia G. R., Bugel S. M., Truong L., Spagnoli S. & Tanguay R. L. AHR2 required for normal behavioral responses and proper development of the skeletal and reproductive systems in zebrafish. PLOS One 13, e0193484 (2018). 10.1371/journal.pone.0193484 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chong J. et al. MetaboAnalyst 4.0: towards more transparent and integrative metabolomics analysis. Nucleic Acids Res. 46, W486–W494 (2018). 10.1093/nar/gky310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Monesson-Olson B. et al. Expression of the eight GABAA receptor α subunits in the developing zebrafish central nervous system. PLOS One 13, e0196083 (2018). 10.1371/journal.pone.0196083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.DePasquale C. & Leri J. The influence of exercise on anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Processes 157, 638–644 (2018). 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gilchrist M. et al. Dietary nitrate supplementation improves reaction time in type 2 diabetes: development and application of a novel nitrate-depleted beetroot juice placebo.Nitric oxide Biol. Chem. 40, 67–74 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Thompson C. et al. Dietary nitrate supplementation improves sprint and high-intensity intermittent running performance. Nitric oxide Biol. Chem. 61, 55–61 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Thompson C. et al. Dietary nitrate improves sprint performance and cognitive function during prolonged intermittent exercise. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 115, 1825–1834 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Presley T. D. et al. Acute effect of a high nitrate diet on brain perfusion in older adults. Nitric oxide Biol. Chem. 24, 34–42 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Joris P. J., Mensink R. P., Adam T. C. & Liu T. T. Cerebral Blood Flow Measurements in Adults: A Review on the Effects of Dietary Factors and Exercise. Nutrients 10, (2018). 10.3390/nu10050530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Clifford T. et al. Effects of inorganic nitrate and nitrite consumption on cognitive function and cerebral blood flow: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 59, 2400–2410 (2019). 10.1080/10408398.2018.1453779 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dobashi S., Koyama K., Endo J., Kiuchi M. & Horiuchi M. Impact of Dietary Nitrate Supplementation on Executive Function During Hypoxic Exercise. High Alt. Med. Biol. 20, 187–191 (2019). 10.1089/ham.2018.0114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bondonno C. P. et al. The acute effect of flavonoid-rich apples and nitrate-rich spinach on cognitive performance and mood in healthy men and women. Food Funct. 5, 849–858 (2014). 10.1039/c3fo60590f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kelly J. et al. Effects of short-term dietary nitrate supplementation on blood pressure, O2 uptake kinetics, and muscle and cognitive function in older adults. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 304, R73–83 (2013). 10.1152/ajpregu.00406.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Thompson K. G. et al. Influence of dietary nitrate supplementation on physiological and cognitive responses to incremental cycle exercise. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 193, 11–20 (2014). 10.1016/j.resp.2013.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Shannon O. M. et al. Effects of Dietary Nitrate Supplementation on Physiological Responses, Cognitive Function, and Exercise Performance at Moderate and Very-High Simulated Altitude. Frontiers in Physiology 8, 401 (2017). 10.3389/fphys.2017.00401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Lefferts W. K., Hughes W. E., White C. N., Brutsaert T. D. & Heffernan K. S. Effect of acute nitrate supplementation on neurovascular coupling and cognitive performance in hypoxia. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 41, 133–141 (2016). 10.1139/apnm-2015-0400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bloch S., Froc C., Pontiggia A. & Yamamoto K. Existence of working memory in teleosts: Establishment of the delayed matching-to-sample task in adult zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 370, 111924 (2019). 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bagdy G., Riba P., Kecskemeti V., Chase D. & Juhasz G. Headache-type adverse effects of NO donors: vasodilation and beyond. Br. J. Pharmacol. 160, 20–35 (2010). 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.00643.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pehrson A. L. & Sanchez C. Altered gamma-aminobutyric acid neurotransmission in major depressive disorder: a critical review of the supporting evidence and the influence of serotonergic antidepressants. Drug Des. Devel. Ther. 9, 603–624 (2015). 10.2147/DDDT.S62912 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Kantrowitz J., Citrome L. & Javitt D. GABA(B) receptors, schizophrenia and sleep dysfunction: a review of the relationship and its potential clinical and therapeutic implications. CNS Drugs 23, 681–691 (2009). 10.2165/00023210-200923080-00005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Greenfield L. J. J. Molecular mechanisms of antiseizure drug activity at GABAA receptors. Seizure 22, 589–600 (2013). 10.1016/j.seizure.2013.04.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Braidy N. et al. Role of Nicotinamide Adenine Dinucleotide and Related Precursors as Therapeutic Targets for Age-Related Degenerative Diseases: Rationale, Biochemistry, Pharmacokinetics, and Outcomes. Antioxid. Redox Signal. 30, 251–294 (2019). 10.1089/ars.2017.7269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Healy-Stoffel M. & Levant B. N-3 (Omega-3) Fatty Acids: Effects on Brain Dopamine Systems and Potential Role in the Etiology and Treatment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 17, 216–232 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Picón-Pagès P., Garcia-Buendia J. & Muñoz F. J. Functions and dysfunctions of nitric oxide in brain. Biochim. Biophys. Acta—Mol. Basis Dis. 1865, 1949–1967 (2019). 10.1016/j.bbadis.2018.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Pitsikas N. The role of nitric oxide (NO) donors in anxiety. Lights and shadows. Nitric oxide Biol. Chem. 77, 6–11 (2018). 10.1016/j.niox.2018.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Spiacci A., Kanamaru F., Guimarães F. & Oliveira R. Nitric oxide-mediated anxiolytic-like and antidepressant-like effects in animal models of anxiety and depression. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 88, 247–255 (2008). 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Papageorgoulis A., Fallon P., Mpalantes N., Papageorgouli D. & Pitsikas N. Repeated but not acute exposure with a low dose range of the nitric oxide (NO) donor sodium nitroprusside (SNP) induces anxiolytic-like behaviour in a dose-independent manner in two different rat models of anxiety. Nitric oxide Biol. Chem. 99, 1–6 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kurt M. et al. Effect of sildenafil on anxiety in the plus-maze test in mice. Pol. J. Pharmacol. 56, 353–357 (2004). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Shahidi S., Hashemi-Firouzi N. & Mahmoodi M. Modulation of Anxiety-Like Behavior in Sildenafil Citrate-Treated Mice Placed in an Elevated Plus-Maze. BCN 2, 53–57 (2011). [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Walia V., Garg C. & Garg M. Nitrergic signaling modulation by ascorbic acid treatment is responsible for anxiolysis in mouse model of anxiety. Behav. Brain Res. 364, 85–98 (2019). 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Demartini C. et al. Nitroglycerin as a comparative experimental model of migraine pain: From animal to human and back. Prog. Neurobiol. 177, 15–32 (2019). 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kovacic S. et al. Increased permeability of the blood-brain barrier following administration of glyceryl trinitrate in common carp (Cyprinus carpio L.). Coll. Antropol. 32 Suppl 1, 99–103 (2008). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Henderson W. R. & Raskin N. H. ‘Hot-dog’ headache: individual susceptibility to nitrite. Lancet 2, 1162–1163 (1972). 10.1016/s0140-6736(72)92591-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Gonzalez A. et al. Migraines Are Correlated with Higher Levels of Nitrate-, Nitrite-, and Nitric Oxide-Reducing Oral Microbes in the American Gut Project Cohort. mSystems 1, (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bryan N. & Loscalzo J. Nitrite and Nitrate in Human Health and Disease. (Humana Press, 2011). [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Guillette L. J. J. & Edwards T. M. Is nitrate an ecologically relevant endocrine disruptor in vertebrates? Integr. Comp. Biol. 45, 19–27 (2005). 10.1093/icb/45.1.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Trudeau V. L. et al. The role of amino acid neurotransmitters in the regulation of pituitary gonadotropin release in fish. Biochem. Cell Biol. 78, 241–259 (2000). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Arbuckle T. E., Sherman G. J., Corey P. N., Walters D. & Lo B. Water nitrates and CNS birth defects: a population-based case-control study. Arch. Environ. Health 43, 162–167 (1988). 10.1080/00039896.1988.9935846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Croen L. A., Todoroff K. & Shaw G. M. Maternal exposure to nitrate from drinking water and diet and risk for neural tube defects. Am. J. Epidemiol. 153, 325–331 (2001). 10.1093/aje/153.4.325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Jannat M., Fatimah R. & Kishida M. Nitrate (NO3−) and nitrite (NO2−) are endocrine disruptors to downregulate expression of tyrosine hydroxylase and motor behavior through conversion to nitric oxide in early development of zebrafish. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 452, 608–613 (2014). 10.1016/j.bbrc.2014.08.114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.World Health Organization. Nitrate and Nitrite in Drinking Water. Nitrate and Nitrite in Drinking Water (2015). 10.17226/9038 [DOI]
  • 81.Stewart A. et al. Modeling anxiety using adult zebrafish: a conceptual review. Neuropharmacology 62, 135–143 (2012). 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.07.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Maximino C. et al. Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: A critical review. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 157–171 (2010). 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Babateen A. M. et al. Protocol and recruitment results from a 13-week randomized controlled trial comparing the effects of different doses of nitrate-rich beetroot juice on cognition, cerebral blood flow and peripheral vascular function in overweight and obese older people. Contemp. Clin. trials Commun. 18, 100571 (2020). 10.1016/j.conctc.2020.100571 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Matthew Parker

20 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-27164

Nitrate and nitrite exposure increases anxiety-like behavior and alters brain metabolomic profile in zebrafish

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. García - Jaramillo,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. As you will see, the reviewers were generally supportive of the publication of this work. However, both reviewers raised several major concerns that preclude publication of the paper in its present form. In particular, one of the reviewers recommends re-considering whether to include all of the behavioural measures, which they found somewhat confusing and difficult to understand in the context of this study, in the final manuscript. Therefore, after careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. We invite you to submit asubstantially revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 04 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Matthew Parker

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. As part of your revisions, please discuss all methods undertaken to ameliorate potential pain and distress, e.g. proper monitoring of behavior, humane endpoints, etc.

3. We note that you have included the phrase “data not shown” in your manuscript. Unfortunately, this does not meet our data sharing requirements. PLOS does not permit references to inaccessible data. We require that authors provide all relevant data within the paper, Supporting Information files, or in an acceptable, public repository. Please add a citation to support this phrase or upload the data that corresponds with these findings to a stable repository (such as Figshare or Dryad) and provide and URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers that may be used to access these data. Or, if the data are not a core part of the research being presented in your study, we ask that you remove the phrase that refers to these data.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The manuscript entitles “Nitrate and nitrite exposure increases anxiety-like behavior and alters brain metabolomic profile in zebrafish” by García-Jaramillo is well-written and brings new information about how nitrate and nitrite affect learning and memory. Although the manuscript is interesting and new, there are few comments that need to be answered.

1. The authors used fish from a range of 9 to 16 months old. Zebrafish reach adult age around 3-4 months old, here the authors used fish from 9 to 16 months. It is known that aging affects cognitive performance and it is slightly worrying that the authors used a range of 7 months difference to this experiment. My question is why did the authors use fish from 9 to 16 months old? Were the ages equally distributed between groups?

2. Line 171. “Fish were fed a standard lab diet (Gemma Micro. Skretting, Westbrook, ME) at a volume of ~3% 172 body weight/day.” Did the authors feed the fish only once a day? If yes, why? Fish feeding regimen is really important and can modulate metabolism and behavior (doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5343).

3. Lines 173 – 175. Please explain the methods used to collect the fish body for further hormone analysis.

4. Lines 189 – 191 - The behavior was recorded between 14-17 days but it is not clear when each test was performed. I would recommend that the authors create an experiment design figure to make it clear for the readers.

5. In the figure legends, the N varies from 42 to 84 which is a big difference. Why for some groups is there almost double the sample size? Please describe in detail the sample size calculation and any criteria for exclusion. This is an important part of the research design and should always be decided prior. Also, the information about removal of outliers is essential for research transparency as well as how many fish were excluded.

6. The authors need to improve the result section, focusing on three points. More descriptive analysis is necessary, 1) the F and P values must be added in the result section since only on line 321 it was added; 2) The degrees of freedom and denominator values must be added; 3) Add the descriptive information about the negative results as well.

7. Lines 418 – 419 – The authors must discuss their data with care. Although they found that they disproved the hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite improve cognitive performance, they used healthy adult fish and no model of cognitive deficits was tested in this manuscript.

8. Although the discussion about the possible hypothesis of migraines being associated with behavioral response is interesting, I wonder if there is any behavioral characterization of migraine models? e.g. how nitroglycerin induced migraine’s affect fish behavior? It feels like there is a missing discussion about the specific link between behavior and migraine’s, since the authors only discussed about the physiological effects, but mention it as a possible factor that could affect behavior.

9. Please improve the term “mild anxiety-like behavior” because it does not indicate whether it is an anxiolytic or anxiogenic effect, this term is used in the abstract, discussion and conclusion.

Reviewer #2: This paper does have something to offer the field. The behavioral data are largely problematic, and while I have some suggestions for improving some of the measures, I think the simplest solution would be to exclude many of them from the paper. This will then require some reformulation of the discussion and conclusions, but ultimately I think that there are enough results of substance to merit publication. Therefore, my recommendation is to reconsider publication after revision.

General question – Upon reading the abstract and opening of the paper, my first question was one that was not addressed until the very end of the paper – that of known adverse effects of nitrate pollution, and concerns over ammonia exposure. The hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite exposure should enhance performance in fish was therefore surprising. While a full discussion of this issue could reasonably wait until the end, curious readers might appreciate an early acknowledgement of this apparent discrepancy when the question is introduced.

General Conclusions – The experiment primarily presents null results, used as evidence that nitrate and nitrite do not substantially affect health, learning or behavior. This can be useful, but extra precautions must be used (in terms of appropriate controls and experimental rigor) to ensure that the null results are not simply a failure of the experimental design to detect an effect. Without an especially high methodological standard, null results are not very informative. My suggestions below are geared at reducing the number of uninterpretable results stemming from less rigorous procedures, to allow greater focus on the positive and potentially meaningful results.

Habituation - The statistics seem to show that there is no difference in habituation, but simply that there is a slight reduction in startle response in the nitrate-treated fish.

*Suggested resolution – modify the wording of the discussion and interpretation to make clear that a difference in startle reactivity was observed, but that no difference was detected in habituation.

Avoidance - The avoidance procedure and results are not reported in enough detail to determine the validity of the results. For example, it doesn’t appear as though there were any controls in place to rule out differences in locomotor activity or sensitization induced by the shock, or sensory/attentional deficits (e.g. reduced attention to the blue light). If this is the case, while there might be mild behavioral differences between treatment groups, those differences cannot be attributed to learning or cognitive differences (they could simply be reactivity to or perception of the stimuli, such as the startle effects observed in the habituation data). I have looked up the reference (34) which explains the shuttlebox technique in more detail, but this also does not indicate whether an appropriate control was used (it seems not).

*Suggested Resolution – include results from appropriate controls, or remove this treatment completely from the manuscript. The results cannot be interpreted without the control groups, and may only provide a premise for future studies to use similar procedures.

Social/Predator - I am concerned about the validity of the social/predator test. If the animals were behaving as predicted, the ‘No stimulus’ condition should provide a neutral baseline from which the ‘social’ condition should show an increase, and the ‘predator’ condition should show a decrease in proximity. Instead, both stimulus conditions reflect a mild reduction in proximity compared to no stimulus, making it unclear how the fish perceive the video stimuli. The control treatment group did not show any significant response to the videos, making it especially difficult to interpret the changes observed in the treatment groups (which were strongest in the no-stimulus condition). In the absence of interpretable preferences by the control condition, the changes observed in the treatment condition are not very informative.

*Suggested Resolution – I would remove these data from the manuscript, again to avoid establishing a precedent of a procedure that doesn’t really measure what it is intended to measure. I think they could be left in with plenty of caveats and discussion of the results (since there was a clear effect of treatment), but it seems that this wouldn’t strengthen the paper, and instead simply distract from the more interesting effects.

Tank Location – This does seem to be the one relatively straightforward behavioral effect of treatment. However, it would be necessary to statistically compare ‘Time in Zone – bottom’ across the three treatment groups to determine whether the treatment does lead to significantly more time near the bottom. Because analysis revealed an interaction, a post-hoc test that confirms this difference would be warranted – I simply don’t see the results of such a test reported.

*Suggested Resolution – include post-hoc analysis for duration in the bottom zone across treatment groups. If this difference is not significant, adjust language in the discussion to (further) soften conclusions related to anxiety.

I am surprised that the authors tested catecholamines, but not cortisol, which would be a logical assay for stress or anxiety-related behaviors. I don’t see the inclusion of cortisol levels as necessary, but it would have enhanced the discussion and might be considered for next time.

Minor suggestions:

In many places when reporting the statistics, the wording seems stilted – usually around phrases involving statistical significance. Check these and see if they can be re-phrased.

Examples: Line 345: “a statistically significant more time close to the monitor”

line 338 "associated with a significant higher percentage of fish"

The discussion begins with a statement that “we disproved the hypothesis that nitrate, and nitrite treatment would improve indicators of learning and cognitive performance in a zebrafish model” This is too strong a statement for results that were largely null – instead, the study simply did not find evidence supporting the hypothesis (it similarly did not find evidence against the hypothesis). If the changes suggested above are made, all reference to learning/cognition should be carefully revised.

The final result, indicating that neither nitrate nor nitrite was taken into the brain in any great quantity, deserves more attention. For example, it seems odd to discuss at length the role of NO in learning and memory when the effects of treatment are likely to be through indirect mechanisms (as acknowledged near the end). Similarly, the section on migraines is a very interesting speculation, but again rather lengthy given the absence of direct evidence from this study (it might be a worthwhile future direction). The apparent effects of treatment on purine metabolism, annotated fatty acids, GABA and Glutamine are fairly straightforward, and the bulk of the discussion should focus on possible pathways and mechanisms for the effects that were observed.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Barbara D. Fontana

Reviewer #2: Yes: Rachel Blaser

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Dec 31;15(12):e0240070. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240070.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


3 Dec 2020

Response to reviewers’ comments

The comments of both reviewers on our manuscript were very helpful to us in clarifying our paper. We have carefully read each of these comments and will respond to them below. We would like to thank them for their positive comments. Furthermore, the suggestions made by the reviewers have been extremely helpful in the revision of this manuscript. To address these suggestions and provide further clarity in the text we have revised the manuscript to incorporate the reviewers’ suggestions. The changed text is marked by highlights and is found throughout the paper. Below you will find the detailed reviewers’ comments and our responses.

Reviewer #1: The manuscript entitles “Nitrate and nitrite exposure increases anxiety-like behavior and alters brain metabolomic profile in zebrafish” by García-Jaramillo is well-written and brings new information about how nitrate and nitrite affect learning and memory. Although the manuscript is interesting and new, there are few comments that need to be answered.

Response: Thank you for your helpful comments regarding our manuscript. We appreciate your time in careful review, and the positive encouragement. We have responded to each comment in the sections below and made the appropriate changes in our resubmission.

1. The authors used fish from a range of 9 to 16 months old. Zebrafish reach adult age around 3-4 months old, here the authors used fish from 9 to 16 months. It is known that aging affects cognitive performance and it is slightly worrying that the authors used a range of 7 months difference to this experiment. My question is why did the authors use fish from 9 to 16 months old? Were the ages equally distributed between groups?

Response: We agree that age can affect cognitive performance. When our experiments were performed, we took fish of the same age and equally distributed them between treatment groups in order to alleviate the potential impact of age on our results. We have added additional text to the methods section to clarify this. The reason for the age variation is that we ran the experiment in multiple cohorts using available fish. All fish used to conduct this study were healthy adults that still had reproductive capacity.

2. Line 171. “Fish were fed a standard lab diet (Gemma Micro. Skretting, Westbrook, ME) at a volume of ~3% 172 body weight/day.” Did the authors feed the fish only once a day? If yes, why? Fish feeding regimen is really important and can modulate metabolism and behavior (doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5343).

Response: We agree this is important. We fed them twice a day and fish were never more than 4h from a feeding when behavior experiments were conducted. We added further details about it to the methods section.

3. Lines 173 – 175. Please explain the methods used to collect the fish body for further hormone analysis.

Response: Thank you for pointing this out. We added text and a reference to the methods section in regard to this question. Briefly, for hormone analysis the animals were euthanized using rapid cooling directly prior to collecting the samples according to Wallace et al (2018).

CK Wallace, LA Bright, JO Marx, RP Andersen, MC Mullins, AJ Carty. Effectiveness of Rapid Cooling as a Method of Euthanasia for Young Zebrafish ( Danio rerio). J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2018 Jan 1;57(1):58-63. PMID: 29402353 PMCID: PMC5875099

4. Lines 189 – 191 - The behavior was recorded between 14-17 days but it is not clear when each test was performed. I would recommend that the authors create an experiment design figure to make it clear for the readers.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We have added additional text in the methods section to clarify when each test was performed. We have also included an experiment design summary in supplemental information (S1 Fig).

5. In the figure legends, the N varies from 42 to 84 which is a big difference. Why for some groups is there almost double the sample size? Please describe in detail the sample size calculation and any criteria for exclusion. This is an important part of the research design and should always be decided prior. Also, the information about removal of outliers is essential for research transparency as well as how many fish were excluded.

Response: Thank you for comment. We agree with the reviewer that it was not very clear in our previous version. With n=84 we meant that we had measured that parameter twice and thus had double the number of observations. Since Reviewer#2 also expressed some concerns about this, we have removed the additional data from the manuscript. To further answer the reviewers concern about outliers, we confirm that no outliers were excluded in our analysis.

6. The authors need to improve the result section, focusing on three points. More descriptive analysis is necessary, 1) the F and P values must be added in the result section since only on line 321 it was added; 2) The degrees of freedom and denominator values must be added; 3) Add the descriptive information about the negative results as well.

Response: Thank you for your comment. Following reviewer recommendation, we have excluded some of the behavioral measures from our final version, in order to better focus the results section and make it easier to understand. Following PLOS ONE’s guidelines, we have included F and P values within the figures; the degrees of freedom and denominator values are not requested within this Journal guidelines.

7. Lines 418 – 419 – The authors must discuss their data with care. Although they found that they disproved the hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite improve cognitive performance, they used healthy adult fish and no model of cognitive deficits was tested in this manuscript.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We agree with the reviewer and have changed this sentence to soften it and added the point about a limitation as it relates to only testing healthy fish in the discussion.

8. Although the discussion about the possible hypothesis of migraines being associated with behavioral response is interesting, I wonder if there is any behavioral characterization of migraine models? e.g. how nitroglycerin induced migraine’s affect fish behavior? It feels like there is a missing discussion about the specific link between behavior and migraine’s, since the authors only discussed about the physiological effects, but mention it as a possible factor that could affect behavior.

Response: We appreciate the reviewer’s question and did evaluate the available research on this topic. The group that has used nitroglycerin as a model for migraine published two papers on its effects in common carp (Cyprinus carpio L.). While they do show effects of treatment on blood brain barrier permeability and brain edema, they unfortunately, did not test for changes in behavior. We have edited this section of the discussion following reviewer’s recommendation.

9. Please improve the term “mild anxiety-like behavior” because it does not indicate whether it is an anxiolytic or anxiogenic effect, this term is used in the abstract, discussion and conclusion.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We have made the change per the reviewer’s suggestion from “mild anxiety-like behavior” to “mild anxiogenic-like behavior”.

Reviewer #2: This paper does have something to offer the field. The behavioral data are largely problematic, and while I have some suggestions for improving some of the measures, I think the simplest solution would be to exclude many of them from the paper. This will then require some reformulation of the discussion and conclusions, but ultimately I think that there are enough results of substance to merit publication. Therefore, my recommendation is to reconsider publication after revision.

Response: Thank you for your time in critical review and the positive recommendation. We have responded to each comment in the sections below and made the appropriate changes in our resubmission. We think our manuscript has been improved because of your careful review and suggestions.

General question – Upon reading the abstract and opening of the paper, my first question was one that was not addressed until the very end of the paper – that of known adverse effects of nitrate pollution, and concerns over ammonia exposure. The hypothesis that nitrate and nitrite exposure should enhance performance in fish was therefore surprising. While a full discussion of this issue could reasonably wait until the end, curious readers might appreciate an early acknowledgement of this apparent discrepancy when the question is introduced.

Response: Thank you this suggestion. We added a sentence to the introduction to address this concern.

General Conclusions – The experiment primarily presents null results, used as evidence that nitrate and nitrite do not substantially affect health, learning or behavior. This can be useful, but extra precautions must be used (in terms of appropriate controls and experimental rigor) to ensure that the null results are not simply a failure of the experimental design to detect an effect. Without an especially high methodological standard, null results are not very informative. My suggestions below are geared at reducing the number of uninterpretable results stemming from less rigorous procedures, to allow greater focus on the positive and potentially meaningful results.

Habituation - The statistics seem to show that there is no difference in habituation, but simply that there is a slight reduction in startle response in the nitrate-treated fish.

*Suggested resolution – modify the wording of the discussion and interpretation to make clear that a difference in startle reactivity was observed, but that no difference was detected in habituation.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We have made this change in the results and discussion sections.

Avoidance - The avoidance procedure and results are not reported in enough detail to determine the validity of the results. For example, it doesn’t appear as though there were any controls in place to rule out differences in locomotor activity or sensitization induced by the shock, or sensory/attentional deficits (e.g. reduced attention to the blue light). If this is the case, while there might be mild behavioral differences between treatment groups, those differences cannot be attributed to learning or cognitive differences (they could simply be reactivity to or perception of the stimuli, such as the startle effects observed in the habituation data). I have looked up the reference (34) which explains the shuttlebox technique in more detail, but this also does not indicate whether an appropriate control was used (it seems not).

*Suggested Resolution – include results from appropriate controls, or remove this treatment completely from the manuscript. The results cannot be interpreted without the control groups, and may only provide a premise for future studies to use similar procedures.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We apologize for the lack of details on the shuttlebox. The shuttlebox technique has an appropriate control where the stimuli (e.g. light) is not at a fixed location in the box. Depending on where the fish is in the box, the light opposite is turned on. This serves as a way to rule out reactivity or habituation per a fish. Additionally, there is 30 trials per fish, which reduces the probability of the behavioral response being attributed to cognitive differences, and more likely learning behavior.

Social/Predator - I am concerned about the validity of the social/predator test. If the animals were behaving as predicted, the ‘No stimulus’ condition should provide a neutral baseline from which the ‘social’ condition should show an increase, and the ‘predator’ condition should show a decrease in proximity. Instead, both stimulus conditions reflect a mild reduction in proximity compared to no stimulus, making it unclear how the fish perceive the video stimuli. The control treatment group did not show any significant response to the videos, making it especially difficult to interpret the changes observed in the treatment groups (which were strongest in the no-stimulus condition). In the absence of interpretable preferences by the control condition, the changes observed in the treatment condition are not very informative.

*Suggested Resolution – I would remove these data from the manuscript, again to avoid establishing a precedent of a procedure that doesn’t really measure what it is intended to measure. I think they could be left in with plenty of caveats and discussion of the results (since there was a clear effect of treatment), but it seems that this wouldn’t strengthen the paper, and instead simply distract from the more interesting effects.

Response: Thank you for your comment. Yes, we agree with the Reviewer and removed this data per your suggestion.

Tank Location – This does seem to be the one relatively straightforward behavioral effect of treatment. However, it would be necessary to statistically compare ‘Time in Zone – bottom’ across the three treatment groups to determine whether the treatment does lead to significantly more time near the bottom. Because analysis revealed an interaction, a post-hoc test that confirms this difference would be warranted – I simply don’t see the results of such a test reported.

*Suggested Resolution – include post-hoc analysis for duration in the bottom zone across treatment groups. If this difference is not significant, adjust language in the discussion to (further) soften conclusions related to anxiety.

Response: Thank you for your recommendation. We have completed post-hoc analysis for duration in the bottom zone across treatment groups and found no significant differences. For this reason, we have softened the language to point this out.

I am surprised that the authors tested catecholamines, but not cortisol, which would be a logical assay for stress or anxiety-related behaviors. I don’t see the inclusion of cortisol levels as necessary, but it would have enhanced the discussion and might be considered for next time.

Response: Thank you for your recommendation. We agree with the reviewer and will test cortisol in any future studies. Our analysis of the brain metabolomics did not allow us to identify cortisol; we will utilize a targeted approach in future studies to assist with identifying this important hormone in this model system.

Minor suggestions:

In many places when reporting the statistics, the wording seems stilted – usually around phrases involving statistical significance. Check these and see if they can be re-phrased.

Examples: Line 345: “a statistically significant more time close to the monitor”

line 338 "associated with a significant higher percentage of fish"

Response: Thank you for your comment. We have removed the language associated with line 345 and revised the paragraph that was stilted and contained line 338.

The discussion begins with a statement that “we disproved the hypothesis that nitrate, and nitrite treatment would improve indicators of learning and cognitive performance in a zebrafish model” This is too strong a statement for results that were largely null – instead, the study simply did not find evidence supporting the hypothesis (it similarly did not find evidence against the hypothesis). If the changes suggested above are made, all reference to learning/cognition should be carefully revised.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We made the change to the hypothesis statement per the reviewer’s suggestion.

The final result, indicating that neither nitrate nor nitrite was taken into the brain in any great quantity, deserves more attention. For example, it seems odd to discuss at length the role of NO in learning and memory when the effects of treatment are likely to be through indirect mechanisms (as acknowledged near the end). Similarly, the section on migraines is a very interesting speculation, but again rather lengthy given the absence of direct evidence from this study (it might be a worthwhile future direction). The apparent effects of treatment on purine metabolism, annotated fatty acids, GABA and Glutamine are fairly straightforward, and the bulk of the discussion should focus on possible pathways and mechanisms for the effects that were observed.

Response: Thank you for your comment. We have restructured the discussion to emphasize the indirect nature of the associations with nitrate and nitrite exposure on behavior. We explore the potential effects on neuronal function and behavior due to changes in these metabolites as suggested. The discussion now reflects a broader discussion of potential associations between nitrate and nitrite exposure based on the observed behavioral changes and alterations in brain metabolomic profile.

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Barbara D. Fontana

Reviewer #2: Yes: Rachel Blaser

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Matthew Parker

14 Dec 2020

Nitrate and nitrite exposure leads to mild anxiogenic-like behavior and alters brain metabolomic profile in zebrafish

PONE-D-20-27164R1

Dear Dr. García - Jaramillo,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Matthew Parker

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have significantly improved the manuscript and therefore I recommend the article to be accepted.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Acceptance letter

Matthew Parker

18 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-27164R1

Nitrate and nitrite exposure leads to mild anxiogenic-like behavior and alters brain metabolomic profile in zebrafish

Dear Dr. García-Jaramillo:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Matthew Parker

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Experiment design summary.

    (TIFF)

    S1 Table. Metabolites annotated using our in-house library.

    (XLSX)

    S2 Table. Significantly changed metabolites, between all treatment groups.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES