Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Dec 31;15(12):e0244612. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244612

C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study

Miguel Arce Rentería 1, Sarah R Gillett 2, Leslie A McClure 3, Virginia G Wadley 4, Stephen P Glasser 4, Virginia J Howard 5, Brett M Kissela 6, Frederick W Unverzagt 7, Nancy S Jenny 8, Jennifer J Manly 1, Mary Cushman 9,*
Editor: Stephen D Ginsberg10
PMCID: PMC7774911  PMID: 33382815

Abstract

Markers of systemic inflammation are associated with increased risk of cognitive impairment, but it is unclear if they are associated with a faster rate of cognitive decline and whether this relationship differs by race. Our objective was to examine the association of baseline C-reaction protein (CRP) with cognitive decline among a large racially diverse cohort of older adults. Participants included 21,782 adults aged 45 and older (36% were Black, Mean age at baseline 64) from the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study. CRP was measured at baseline and used as a continuous variable or a dichotomous grouping based on race-specific 90th percentile cutoffs. Cognitive measures of memory and verbal fluency were administered every 2 years for up to 12 years. Latent growth curve models evaluated the association of CRP on cognitive trajectories, adjusting for relevant demographic and health factors. We found that higher CRP was associated with worse memory (B = -.039, 95% CI [-.065,-.014]) and verbal fluency at baseline (B = -.195, 95% CI [-.219,-.170]), but not with rate of cognitive decline. After covariate adjustment, the association of CRP on memory was attenuated (B = -.005, 95% CI [-.031,-.021]). The association with verbal fluency at baseline, but not over time, remained (B = -.042, 95% CI [-.067,-.017]). Race did not modify the association between CRP and cognition. Findings suggest that levels of CRP at age 45+, are a marker of cognitive impairment but may not be suitable for risk prediction for cognitive decline.

1. Introduction

As global population aging increases, the prevalence of cognitive impairment and neurocognitive disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders, will increase dramatically [1, 2]. Higher prevalence of cognitive impairment among aging adults is a public health concern, as it is associated with increased rates of disability [3], larger health care costs [4], and increase risk of dementia [5]. As such, identification of those at highest risk for cognitive decline might allow targeted prevention efforts.

Inflammation may be an important mechanism underlying risk for cognitive impairment and dementia [68]. C-reactive protein (CRP) is a marker of acute inflammation in acute illness, but low level inflammation in healthy people is captured by high sensitivity assays and is related to a variety of disease outcomes [9]. A number of studies suggest that CRP might be associated with cognitive impairment [1013], with some evidence of increased risk of cognitive decline [1416]. While some prospective studies have found higher rates of cognitive decline among individuals with higher CRP [1719], the majority of these studies were among highly selected clinic or cohort samples of adults. In order to generalize the relationship between CRP and cognitive outcomes, a national, longitudinal, population-based sample of adults is needed.

Associations between CRP and cognitive decline may be moderated by race. Generally, Black Americans are at higher risk of cognitive impairment compared to White Americans [20, 21]. Similarly, they have higher levels of CRP than their White counterparts [2224]. There is some evidence to suggest that Black people may respond to inflammatory stimuli differently than White people [25, 26]. Gene variants that up-regulate proinflammatory cytokines are also more common in Black than White Americans [26]. Given these findings, it can be hypothesized that Black Americans may be at higher risk for inflammation-related cognitive decline.

The objective of this study was to examine the association between CRP and cognitive trajectories in a national, population-based cohort of Black and White American adults. We hypothesized that higher baseline CRP concentration would be associated with worse cognitive functioning at baseline and a steeper rate of cognitive decline over time, independent of other risk factors. Additionally, we evaluated whether any association of CRP with cognition would be moderated by race such that the association would be stronger among Black compared to White people.

2. Methods

2.1. Design and procedures

The REasons for Geographical and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study is a national, population-based prospective cohort study of Black and White Americans aged ≥45 years at baseline [27]. The cohort includes 30,239 participants, 45% men and 55% women, 58% White and 42% Black, 56% residing in the southeastern Stroke Belt region of United States (Alabama, Arkansas, Georgia, Louisiana, Mississippi, North Carolina, South Carolina, and Tennessee) and 44% in the remaining 40 contiguous United States. The Stroke Belt region has well-documented higher rates of stroke than the rest of the United States [28]. Participants were recruited from commercially available lists of U.S. residents using mail and telephone contact. Enrollment occurred between January 2003 and October 2007. Interviewers were trained to identify participants answering questions in a manner suggesting lack of comprehension, and such participants were not included further. Baseline demographic information, medical history, and health status were collected by computer-assisted telephone interview (CATI) with follow-ups occurring every 6 months (maximum follow-up up to 12 years). Trained health care professionals collected blood and urine samples, electrocardiogram, blood pressure, height, and weight during an in-home visit at baseline. Further methodological details are available elsewhere [27], but in brief, blood pressure quality control was monitored by central examination of digit preference, height was measured once utilizing an 8-foot metal tape measure and a square, and weight (without shoes) was measured once using a standard 300-lb calibrated scale.

As shown in Fig 1, for the current project, participants were excluded if they reported a history of stroke at baseline, cognitive impairment at baseline based on the Six Item Screener (SIS score ≤4) [29], or if missing CRP data. The resulting sample size for analysis was 21,782.

Fig 1. Flow diagram for identifying participants.

Fig 1

CA = Cognitive Assessment.

REGARDS was approved by Institutional Review Boards of all participating institutions and all participants provided written informed consent. Potential participants who were able to respond to telephone questions provided verbal consent, which was followed by written consent at an in-home visit. The current study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Columbia University Medical Center and Larner College of Medicine at the University of Vermont.

2.2. Inflammatory biomarkers and laboratory analysis

At the in-home visit, blood was collected by trained personnel using standardized procedures after a 10–12 hour fast and centrifuged within 2 hours of collection. Plasma and serum were separated and shipped overnight on gel ice packs to a central laboratory. Samples were re-centrifuged and stored for batch processing [27]. Lipid profile and glucose were measured using the Ortho Vitros Clinical Chemistry System 950IRC instrument. CRP was measured in plasma with a high-sensitivity, particle enhanced immunonephelometric assay (N High Sensitivity CRP, Dade Behring Inc., Deerfield, IL; interassay CVs 2.1–5.7%). Validity of results using this blood collection method was confirmed using a paired samples technique [30] and the assay has reasonable within person variability [31]. In addition to evaluating CRP as a continuous variable (with log transformation to correct for skewness), all participants were categorized as having either having CRP “above” or “below” race-specific 90th percentile cutoffs (≥ 8 mg/L for White adults and ≥ 12.3 mg/L for Black adults). Although studies suggest that a CRP ≥3 mg/L indicates levels of inflammation important to disease risk prediction [32, 33], we chose a race-specific higher cutoff for elevated CRP because differences in CRP are partially driven by genetic ancestry, and those with African ancestry have higher values than all other groups, suggesting that a single threshold value may not be appropriate [22]. CRP ≥3 mg/L was also not associated with risk of future stroke in Black Americans [34]. We chose race-specific 90th percentile in order to select the highest possible range of CRP for given racial group.

2.3. Cognitive assessment

Starting in 2006 and repeated every two years, participants completed cognitive measures over the phone administered by trained interviewers [35]. The cognitive measures assessed the cognitive domains of memory and verbal fluency/executive functioning. Episodic memory was assessed through the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) Word List [36]. Participants are asked to recall a list of 10 unrelated words across three learning trials, and after a 5-minute delay, they are asked to recall as many words as possible from the list. Total words recalled across the three learning trials were summed to create an immediate recall score. The immediate and delayed recall scores were converted to z-scores using the entire sample’s means and standard deviation at the initial cognitive assessment. An episodic memory composite score was derived as the average of the immediate and delayed recalled z-scores at each visit. Verbal fluency and executive functioning were assessed with tests of letter and semantic fluency. Verbal fluency tests are considered to measure aspects of executive functioning such as organization, initiation and maintenance [37]. Participants were asked to generate as many words that begin with the letter “F” or names of animals in 60 seconds, respectively. Letter and semantic fluency scores were converted to z-scores using the entire sample’s means and standard deviation at the initial cognitive assessment. A verbal fluency composite score was created by averaging the semantic and letter fluency z-scores at each visit. These cognitive measures have been validated for reliable administration over CATI [38, 39].

2.4. Covariates

Covariates included demographic, health behavior, and vascular risk variables collected during the baseline CATI or in-home visit. Demographic covariates included self-reported age, race, and sex, education level, yearly income, and region of residence. Participants reported their education level (< high school, high school degree, some college, ≥ college degree), and yearly income (<$20,000, $20,000–34,000, $35,000–74,000, ≥$75,000 or unwilling to report). Region was categorized as residence in the stroke belt or non-stroke belt. Health behavior and vascular factors covariates included smoking status, alcohol use, exercise level, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease. Smoking status was categorized as never, former or current, and alcohol use as none, moderate (≤4 drinks/week for men, ≤3 drinks/week for women) or heavy (> 4 drinks on any day or >14 drinks/week for men, > 3 drinks on any day or > 7 drinks/week for women). Exercise level was categorized as 4 or more times per week, 1 to 3 times per week, and none. Diabetes was defined as fasting glucose ≥126 mg/dL, nonfasting glucose ≥200 mg/dL, or self-reported use of diabetes medications. Dyslipidemia was defined as total cholesterol ≥6.22 mmol/L (240 mg/dl), low density lipoproteins ≥4.14 mmol/L (160 mg/dl), high density lipoproteins ≤1.04 mmol/L (40 mg/dl) or use of self-reported anti-hyperlipidemic medications. Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure ≥140 mm Hg or diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mm Hg (average of two blood pressure measurements), or self-reported use of hypertension medications. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated from height and weight measurements. BMI was computed as kg/m2 and categorized according to Centers for Disease Control and Prevention guidelines: underweight (<18.5 kg/m2), normal (18.5–24.9 kg/m2), overweight (25–29.9 kg/m2), or obese (≥30 kg/m2). Prevalent cardiovascular disease was defined as self-reported coronary bypass, percutaneous coronary intervention, myocardial infarction, or myocardial infarction on electrocardiogram.

2.5. Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics of the sample were calculated in SPSS 26. Longitudinal data were analyzed using latent growth curve models in Mplus version 7 [40] to determine the relationship of CRP to initial levels and rate of change in episodic memory and verbal fluency over time. Time was parametrized as years from baseline. In total, 5 visits (follow-up up to 12 years) were analyzed to maximize covariance coverage. We evaluated model fit by the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) [41]. Missing data were managed with full information maximum likelihood using all available data at each occasion. All models included CRP (logCRP or CRP > 90th percentile) as the primary predictor. Initially, separate latent growth curve models were estimated for each cognitive domain (episodic memory, verbal fluency) adjusting only for baseline age. Allowing linear versus curvilinear (age squared) change was compared in each of these models. To assess for evidence of practice effects, a spline modeling retest effects was included [42]. The best fitting models were retained for subsequent analyses that included evaluating the association of CRP and covariates. Compared with models allowing only linear change, fit was improved by allowing both linear and curvilinear change. Fit did not improve by modeling a spline for retest effects for each cognitive domain. Therefore, models estimating retest effects were not used. Subsequently, we built adjusted models in three steps, Step 1 included all demographic covariates, Step 2 added the health behavior covariates, and Step 3 added vascular risk factors. While cardiovascular disease and vascular risk factors (i.e., hypertension, BMI) may be considered mediators between CRP and cognition, we included these covariates in our final models to determine if there was an impact of CRP even after accounting for cardiovascular disease and vascular risk factors. The covariate-adjusted models fit the data well as noted by the improved model fit for each cognitive trajectory as demonstrated by smaller BICs (episodic memory BICs 127001.522, 103072.774; verbal fluency BICs 137901.434, 111822.316).

Multiple-group modeling was used to compare the magnitude of associations between CRP and cognitive trajectory between Black and White people in the unadjusted and adjusted models. Furthermore, given prior studies that report that the association between CRP and cognitive decline is stronger in midlife [19] we conducted post-hoc multiple-group modeling to evaluate whether the association between CRP and cognitive trajectory differed by age groups (midlife < 65 years of age vs late life ≥ 65).

3. Results

Table 1 shows the demographic and health characteristics of the overall sample and by race. Black participants were younger, had lower income, fewer years of education, were more likely to be overweight/obese, and were more likely to have hypertension and diabetes compared to White participants. White participants were more likely to be male and have cardiovascular disease and dyslipidemia. Black participants had higher median CRP than White participants.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics by race.

All (N = 21,782) Black (n = 7,974) White (n = 13,808) p-value
Demographics
Male % (n) 44.0 (9,583) 36.0 (2,873) 48.6 (6,710) <0.001
Stroke Belt % (n) 55.8 (12,155) 51.0 (4,069) 58.6 (8,086) <0.001
Age, mean (SD) 64.1 (9.1) 63.1 (8.8) 64.7 (9.2) <0.001
Education* N = 21,773 n = 7,970 n = 13,803 <0.001
 < HS, % (n) 9.5 (2,072) 15.6 (1,241) 6.0 (831)
 HS grad % (n) 25.0 (5,454) 27.2 (2,165) 23.8 (3,289)
 Some college % (n) 27.3 (5,944) 28.1 (2,243) 26.8 (3,701)
 ≥ College grad % (n) 38.1 (8,303) 29.1 (2,321) 43.3 (5,982)
Income, $1,000/year, % (n) <0.001
 < 20 14.8 (3,233) 22.9 (1,824) 10.2 (1,409)
 20–34 23.3 (5,077) 26.2 (2,088) 21.6 (2,989)
 35–74 32.1 (6,998) 29.0 (2,316) 33.9 (4,682)
 ≥75 18.1 (3,945) 10.8 (860) 22.3 (3085)
 Refused 11.6 (2,529) 11.1 (886) 11.9 (1,643)
Health Behaviors
Alcohol amount % (n)* N = 21,394 n = 7,785 n = 13,609 <0.001
 None 60.3 (12,911) 70.1 (5,457) 54.8 (7,454)
 Moderate 35.5 (7,592) 27.5 (2,137) 40.1 (5,455)
 Heavy 4.2 (891) 2.5 (191) 5.1 (700)
Smoking frequency % (n)* N = 21,701 n = 7,939 n = 13,762 <0.001
 Never/Past 86.7 (18,819) 84.1 (6,677) 88.2 (12,142)
 Current 13.3 (2,882) 15.9 (1,262) 11.8 (1,620)
No weekly exercise % (n)* 32.0 (6,872) 35.0 (2,756) 30.2 (4,116) <0.001
N = 21,488 n = 7,871 n = 13,617
Vascular Risk Factors
Diabetes % (n)* 18.7 (4,043) 26.7 (2,120) 14.0 (1,923) <0.001
N = 21,649 n = 7,927 n = 13,722
Prevalent CVD % (n)* 15.7 (3,368) 13.0 (1,019) 17.3 (2,349) <0.001
N = 21,436 n = 7,834 n = 13,602
Dyslipidemia % (n)* 57.9 (12,485) 53.4 (4,212) 60.5 (8,273) <0.001
N = 21,559 n = 7,888 n = 13,671
Hypertension % (n)* 71.9 (15,625) 83.1 (6,619) 65.4 (9,006) <0.001
N = 21,736 n = 7,964 n = 13,772
BMI Category % (n)* N = 21,653 n = 7,913 n = 13,740 <0.001
 Underweight/Normal 24.6 (5,327) 16.7 (1,323) 29.1 (4,004)
 Overweight/Obese 75.4 (16,326) 83.3 (6,590) 70.9 (9,736)
CRP mg/L, median (IQR) 2.1 (0.9–4.8) 2.8 (1.2–6.3) 1.8 (0.8–4.1) <0.001

Note. CVD = cardiovascular disease, BMI = body mass index, CRP = C-reactive protein.

3.1. Cognitive trajectories

Table 2 shows the unadjusted and adjusted models for the association of logCRP with episodic memory and verbal fluency. For memory and verbal fluency, higher CRP was associated with worse initial scores but not with change over time. After adjusting for covariates (Step 3), there was no independent association of CRP with memory, whereas the association of CRP with baseline verbal fluency remained. Similar patterns were observed when evaluating the association of elevated CRP (>90th percentile; (Table 2) on cognitive trajectories. Elevated CRP was associated with worse initial memory and verbal fluency scores but did not influence slope. After adjusting for covariates, the association between elevated CRP and baseline cognition was partly attenuated, although it remained statistically significant. Multiple group models (Tables 3 and 4) revealed that the association between CRP (both continuous and 90th percentile status) and cognitive trajectories did not differ reliably by race (all ps > .05).

Table 2. Associations of CRP with memory and verbal fluency trajectories.

logCRP
Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Initial Level Slope Initial Level Slope Initial Level Slope Initial Level Slope
Memory -.039(.013)** .001(.002) -.036(.012)** .001(.002) -.027(.013)* .002(.002) -.005(.013) .001(.002)
Fluency -.195(.013)*** .004(.002)* -.077(.012)*** .002(.002) -.061(.012)*** .002(.002) -.042(.013)** .003(.002)
90th Percentile CRP
Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Initial Level Slope Initial Level Slope Initial Level Slope Initial Level Slope
Memory -.026(.022) .001(.003) -.077(.021)*** -.001(.004) -.066(.021)** -.001(.004) -.045(.022)* -.001(.004)
Fluency -.132(.021)*** -.001(.003) -.089(.020)*** -.003(.003) -071(.020)*** -.003(.003) -.052(.021)* -.003(.003)

Note. Values reflect unstandardized parameter estimates (standard error); adjusted model 1 includes all demographic covariates; adjusted model 2 includes the health behavior covariates; adjusted model 3 added vascular risk factors;

*p < .05,

**p < .01,

***p < .001. CRP = C reactive protein.

Table 3. Multiple group comparisons of associations of logCRP with memory and verbal fluency by race.

Memory
Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
White -.005(.016) .000(.002) -.030(.015) .001(.003) -.019(.016) .001(.003) .003(.016) .001(.003)
Black .020(.022) .002(.003) -.047(.021)* .001(.004) -.042(.022) .002(.004) -.021(.023) .001(.004)
White vs Black -.025(.028) -.002(.004) .017(.026) .000(.004) .023(.027) -.001(.005) .024(.028) .000(.005)
Verbal Fluency
Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
White -.155(.016)*** .002(.002) -.090(.016)*** .001(.002) -.072(.016)*** .002(.002) -.055(.017)** .003(.003)
Black -.086(.019)*** .004(.003) -.049(.019)** .004(.003) -.037(.019) .004(.003) -.019(.020) .005(.003)
White vs Black -.070(.025)** -.002(.003) -.041(.024) -.003(.004) -.035(.025) -.002(.004) -.036(.026) -.002(.004)

Note. Values reflect unstandardized parameter estimates (standard error); adjusted model 1 includes all demographic covariates; adjusted model 2 includes the health behavior covariates; adjusted model 3 added vascular risk factors;

*p < .05,

**p < .01,

***p < .001. CRP = C reactive protein.

Table 4. Multiple group comparisons of associations of CRP above the 90th percentile with memory and language by race.

Memory
Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
White -.034(.027) .001(.004) -.070(.025)** -.003(.004) -.058(.025)* -.003(.004) -.037(.026) -.003(.005)
Black -.009(.038) .002(.006) -.089(.036) .002(.006) -.081(.037) .003(.006) -.061(.038) .003(.006)
White vs Black -.026(.047) -.001(.007) .019(.044) -.005(.008) .023(.045) -.006(.008) .024(.046) -.006(.008)
Language
Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
White -.148(.026)*** -.002(.004) -.095(.026)*** -.005(.004) -.075(.026) -.004(.004) -.061(.027)* -.004(.004)
Black -.096(.033)** .000(.004) -.071(.032)* -.001(.004) -.055(.032) -.002(.004) -.029(.032) -.003(.005)
White vs Black -.052(.042) -.002(.006) -.024(.041) -.004(.006) -.020(.041) -.002(.006) -.033(.042) .000(.006)

Note. Values reflect unstandardized parameter estimates (standard error); adjusted model 1 includes all demographic covariates; adjusted model 2 includes the health behavior covariates; adjusted model 3 added vascular risk factors;

*p < .05,

**p < .01,

***p < .001. CRP = C reactive protein.

Post-hoc multiple group models by age group revealed distinct associations between CRP and cognitive trajectories (Table 5). For memory, higher CRP was associated with worse initial scores among participants in midlife compared with older participants, and this interaction remained after adjusting for covariates. There was no association of CRP on memory slope and this did not differ by age group (all ps > .05). For verbal fluency, higher CRP was associated with worse initial scores and this did not differ by age group. There was a counterintuitive association between CRP and change in fluency scores over time such that among older adults higher CRP was associated with greater stability of trajectories, whereas there was no association between CRP and slope among participants in midlife (p = .02). The association of CRP with verbal fluency slope by age group was attenuated after adjusting for covariates. None of the previously mentioned results remained when evaluating the association of elevated CRP (>90th percentile; Table 6) on cognitive trajectories.

Table 5. Multiple group comparisons of associations of logCRP with memory and verbal fluency by age.

Memory
Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
Midlife -.066(.016)*** -.002(.003) -.055(.016)** -.001(.003) -.031(.017) -.001(.003)
Late life .002(.020) .005(.004) .006(.020) .006(.004) -.023(.021) .005(.004)
Midlife vs Late life -.068(.026)** -.006(.005) -.062(.026)* -.007(.005) -.055(.027)* -.006(.005)
Verbal Fluency
Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
Midlife -.085(.016)*** -.001(.002) -.067(.017)*** -.001(.002) -.043(.018)* .002(.002)
Late life -.072(.018)*** .007(.003)* -.061(.018)** .008(.003)** -.050(.018)** .008(.003)**
Midlife vs Late life -.014(.024) -.008(.004)* -.006(.025) -.008(.004)* -.007(.026) -.007(.004)

Note. Values reflect unstandardized parameter estimates (standard error); adjusted model 1 includes all demographic covariates; adjusted model 2 includes the health behavior covariates; adjusted model 3 added vascular risk factors;

*p < .05,

**p < .01,

***p < .001. CRP = C reactive protein.

Table 6. Multiple group comparisons of associations of CRP above the 90th percentile with memory and verbal fluency by age.

Memory
Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
Midlife -.102(.027)*** -.001(.004) -.091(.026)** -.001(.004) -.068(.028)* .000(.005)
Late life -.043(.033) .000(.006) -.033(.034) .000(.006) -.015(.034) -.001(.007)
Midlife vs Late life -.058(.043) -.001(.008) -.058(.043) .000(.008) -.053(.044) .001(.008)
Verbal Fluency
Adjusted Model 1 Adjusted Model 2 Adjusted Model 3
Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Intercept Slope
Midlife -.098(.027)*** -.007(.004) -.081(.028)** -.007(.004) -.055(.029) -.005(.004)
Late life -.081(.030)** .003(.005) -.064(.030)* .003(.005) -.056(.030) .002(.005)
Midlife vs Late life -.017(.040) -.010(.006) -.017(.041) -.010(.006) .001(.042) -.007(.006)

Note. Values reflect unstandardized parameter estimates (standard error); adjusted model 1 includes all demographic covariates; adjusted model 2 includes the health behavior covariates; adjusted model 3 added vascular risk factors;

*p < .05,

**p < .01,

***p < .001. CRP = C reactive protein.

4. Discussion

In this nationally representative, population-based sample of White and Black American adults aged ≥45 years at baseline, higher CRP was associated with worse memory and verbal fluency at baseline but not with rate of cognitive decline over a span of 12 years. While the associations of continuous increments of CRP on baseline memory was attenuated after adjusting for demographic and vascular risk factors, the association remained significant among individuals with elevated CRP (>90th percentile). Higher CRP was more consistently associated with worse baseline verbal fluency with and without covariate adjustment. Furthermore, the association of CRP on cognition did not differ by race.

Our large cohort of Black and White adults followed longitudinally allowed us to assess the influence of CRP on cognitive trajectories. While the association of CRP on baseline cognition was robust, our hypothesis that elevated CRP would increase rate of cognitive decline was not supported. There are a few possible explanations for this discrepancy, and our null findings compared to some literature. First, prior studies among older adults that reported an association between CRP and cognitive decline have largely used brief cognitive screeners (i.e., Mini-Mental State Examination) [43], defined cognitive decline based on a single follow-up [16], or had brief follow-up periods (≤ 10 years) [17]. In contrast, our longitudinal study, focused on discrete cognitive domains (memory, verbal fluency), used measures that were well validated for use in this cohort [38, 39], and leveraged up to 5 follow-ups over the span of 12 years to characterize rate of cognitive decline. Second, previous studies generally accounted for few potentially confounding demographic and health variables. For instance, most studies would only include basic demographics (i.e., age, sex/gender, education) [16, 43, 44], a few others controlled for relevant health behaviors (i.e., alcohol use, smoking) [7, 11] associated with elevated CRP, or medical comorbidities (i.e., cardiovascular disease) [6, 11, 19] that may lie in the casual pathway between inflammation and cognitive decline. Third, we used race-specific cut-offs to determine elevated CRP, which may have influenced results. However, given the variability in CRP concentrations due to genetic ancestry, we believe our approach allows for robust evaluation of CRP across racial/ethnic groups. Moreover, the association between CRP measured continuously on rate of decline did not differ substantially from the associations using cut-offs, which suggests that a different or lower cut-off level for CRP would yield similar null results. As such, given these methodological differences, it may be possible that concentrations of CRP only affect cognitive level and not rate of decline among older adults. Fourth, another possible explanation was that the effect of CRP on rate of decline may take place before the age of 65, given various studies suggesting that age-related cognitive decline accelerates during midlife [45], and that higher levels of CRP during midlife are associated with a steeper rate of cognitive decline [19], white matter disease [46, 47], and higher risk of dementia in late-life [6]. While we did not find a faster rate of cognitive decline among participants in midlife in our post-hoc analyses, we did find that among older adults higher CRP was associated with greater stability in verbal fluency trajectories (less decline). Prior studies have reported this inverse relationship between CRP and cognition among older adults, which may be due to successful cognitive aging and survival bias [48, 49]. Differences in mid- and late life associations were only observed when evaluating CRP continuously but largely attenuated when adjusting for covariates and not present when evaluating individuals with elevated CRP (>90th percentile). Lastly, while we evaluated change in cognition over up to 12 years, this may not be sufficient time to detect cognitive decline, even given our large sample size. For instance, a recent study found an association between midlife CRP and 20-year cognitive decline that was robust to adjustment for multiple covariates [19].

The availability of cognitive data in two domains (episodic memory and verbal fluency) allowed for investigation in domain-specific associations of CRP. Our results indicate that verbal fluency may be more susceptible to the effects of inflammation than episodic memory. Verbal fluency involves skills related to executive functioning (i.e., updating, inhibition) [37, 50] and is associated with integrity of frontal cortical structures, for letter fluency, and frontal and temporal-parietal regions, for semantic fluency [51]. As CRP is considered a marker of vascular disease, these results are in line with research that links vascular risk factors and markers of cerebrovascular disease with impairments in executive functioning [52, 53]. Similarly, a recent study reported that among older adults, higher baseline CRP was associated with reduced blood flow in frontal regions and the anterior cingulate cortex which are both regions associated with executive functioning [54]. Inflammation may lead to impaired endothelial functioning which is associated with white matter hyperintensities [55, 56]. In contrast, while studies have reported an association between elevated levels of CRP and worse performance and declines in episodic memory [19, 57, 58], we only found a robust association with worse memory at CRP levels above the 90th percentile. Inflammation may impact cognition through different mechanisms; it may lead to impaired endothelial functioning which is associated with white matter hyperintensities [55, 56], and with smaller brain volume [59]. Structures important for memory, such as the hippocampus, have high concentrations of pro-inflammatory cytokines and receptors which may be vulnerable to systemic inflammation. Given these domain-specific associations even after adjusting for vascular risk factors and cardiovascular disease, there may be different neurologic pathways through which inflammation may impact cognition.

We did not find that race modified the association between inflammation and cognitive decline. Given prior evidence for race differences in peripheral immune function [25, 26] and studies that have found a stronger association between markers of cerebrovascular disease (i.e., white matter hyperintensities) [53] and metabolic disorders (i.e., diabetes) [60] with cognition among Black compared to White people, we expected to find differences in the relationship between inflammation and cognition by race in the current study. In fact, a recent study from the Health Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study found an association between higher CRP and worse cognitive performance among Black compared to White Americans [61]. However, participants in the HANDLS study were on average in midlife at baseline whereas our sample was older, HANDLS only included 2 cognitive assessments, and the association of CRP with cognition was only found on a test of attention but not on measures of episodic memory or verbal fluency, which mirrors our findings. Moreover, our results are in line with a recent study that did not find race differences in the association between midlife levels of CRP and cognitive decline in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study [19]. While the ARIC Study evaluated CRP ≥ 3 mg/L [32, 33], we expanded on these results by additionally evaluating CRP using race-specific higher cutoffs (≥ 8 mg/L for White adults and ≥ 12.3 mg/L for Black adults) given that CRP levels differ by race due to genetic ancestry [22]. Taken together, most results suggest that among older adults, the association of CRP with cognitive trajectories does not differ by race.

There are several strengths and weaknesses of this study. Strengths include the use of a large, national biracial cohort with serial cognitive assessments in two domains for up to 12 years, the use of structural equation modeling to estimate latent growth curve cognitive trajectories, and the inclusion of potentially confounding demographic and medical factors. A limitation is that it is unknown if findings are generalizable to individuals of other racial/ethnic backgrounds, given that our cohort only included White and Black adults. Another limitation was that given the resource limitations of a large-scale national study, we relied on telephone-based cognitive assessments. Although prior studies have validated telephone administration of cognitive measures and reported that they are comparable to face-to-face evaluations [38, 62], future studies should include a more extensive cognitive battery and in-person evaluations in order to improve sensitivity to cognitive decline. Another limitation is that we measured CRP only once. CRP level can increase in response to injury, infection, and inflammation [9, 63]. Although several studies indicate that CRP >3 mg/L indicates low grade-inflammation [64], and that CRP plays an important role in systemic inflammation [9], studies have shown that CRP varies within individuals over time and that at least three measurements are required to reliably establish the true mean [65]. It is possible that some participants had acute illness at the time of the CRP measurement, which would bias results towards the null hypothesis. However, the CRP distribution in our cohort was in line with what is expected based on other studies in the United States [66]. In addition, an increase in CRP over time has been associated with cognitive decline in two studies [67, 68], so longitudinal measurement may further help clarify the role of CRP on risk of cognitive impairment. Another similar potential limitation is that we only measured several covariates (i.e., height and weight) at baseline. Like most epidemiologic studies, for reasons of pragmatism, all baseline variables cannot be measured twice, which would improve precision of risk estimates. The impact of this limitation would be a bias to the null hypothesis. Lastly, we did not evaluate the association of CRP in conjunction with other inflammation biomarkers (i.e., IL-6). For instance, a summary index score of inflammation biomarkers including CRP, higher white blood cell counts, and lower serum albumin increased risk prediction of all-cause mortality, even after controlling for various demographic, sociocultural, and medical confounders [69]. Whether similar associations exist for rate of cognitive decline is unknown.

In conclusion, the current study with a large sample size (~20,000 adults) and well-characterized cohort in terms of demographics and medical factors, provides strong evidence that inflammation as measured with CRP at one time point is associated with cognitive level but may not increase rate of cognitive decline among older adults. Based on our findings, CRP may be used as a marker of cognitive impairment among older adults but may not be suitable for risk prediction for early cognitive decline. Further investigations are warranted to disentangle the association of this and other inflammatory markers on cognitive decline from the effects of other demographic and sociocultural risk factors.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the other investigators, staff, and participants of the REGARDS study for their valuable contributions. A full list of participating REGARDS investigators and institutions can be found at https://www.uab.edu/soph/regardsstudy/.

Data Availability

The data underlying the findings include potentially identifying participant information, and cannot be made publicly available due to ethical/legal restrictions. However, data including statistical code from this manuscript are available to researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Data can be obtained upon request through the University of Alabama at Birmingham at regardsadmin@uab.edu.

Funding Statement

This research project is supported by cooperative agreement U01 NS041588 co-funded by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) and the National Institute on Aging (NIA), National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Service. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NINDS or the NIA. Representatives of the NINDS were involved in the review of the manuscript but were not directly involved in the collection, management, analysis or interpretation of the data. Additional funding was provided by National Heart Lung and Blood Institute T32 HL07594-24 and National Institute of General Medical Sciences P20 GM135007.

References

  • 1.Sloane PD, Zimmerman S, Suchindran C, Reed P, Wang L, Boustani M, et al. The public health impact of Alzheimer's disease, 2000–2050: potential implication of treatment advances. Annual review of public health. 2002;23:213–31. 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimer's & dementia: the journal of the Alzheimer's Association. 2013;9(1):63–75. e2. 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Freedman VA, Martin LG, Schoeni RF. Recent trends in disability and functioning among older adults in the United States: a systematic review. Jama. 2002;288(24):3137–46. 10.1001/jama.288.24.3137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Albert SM, Glied S, Andrews H, Stern Y, Mayeux R. Primary care expenditures before the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2002;59(4):573–8. 10.1212/wnl.59.4.573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Li J-Q, Tan L, Wang H-F, Tan M-S, Tan L, Xu W, et al. Risk factors for predicting progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2016;87(5):476–84. 10.1136/jnnp-2014-310095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schmidt R, Schmidt H, Curb JD, Masaki K, White LR, Launer LJ. Early inflammation and dementia: A 25‐year follow‐up of the Honolulu‐Asia aging study. Annals of neurology. 2002;52(2):168–74. 10.1002/ana.10265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Engelhart MJ, Geerlings MI, Meijer J, Kiliaan A, Ruitenberg A, van Swieten JC, et al. Inflammatory proteins in plasma and the risk of dementia: the rotterdam study. Archives of neurology. 2004;61(5):668–72. 10.1001/archneur.61.5.668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gorelick PB. Role of inflammation in cognitive impairment: results of observational epidemiological studies and clinical trials. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1207(1):155–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sproston NR, Ashworth JJ. Role of C-reactive protein at sites of inflammation and infection. Frontiers in immunology. 2018;9:754 10.3389/fimmu.2018.00754 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Noble JM, Manly JJ, Schupf N, Tang MX, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Association of C-reactive protein with cognitive impairment. Archives of neurology. 2010;67(1):87–92. 10.1001/archneurol.2009.308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, Simonsick EM, Pahor M, Kritchevsky S, et al. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African-American and white elders. Neurology. 2003;61(1):76–80. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000073620.42047.d7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Christianson TJ, Pankratz VS, et al. Association of C-reactive protein with mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5(5):398–405. 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.01.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bahrami A, Bahrami-Taghanaki H, Khorasanchi Z, Tayefi M, Ferns GA, Sadeghnia HR, et al. The Association Between Neuropsychological Function with Serum Vitamins A, D, and E and hs-CRP Concentrations. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience. 2019;68(2):243–50. 10.1007/s12031-019-01288-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Alley DE, Crimmins EM, Karlamangla A, Hu P, Seeman TE. Inflammation and rate of cognitive change in high-functioning older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2008;63(1):50–5. 10.1093/gerona/63.1.50 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Weinstein G, Lutski M, Goldbourt U, Tanne D. C-reactive protein is related to future cognitive impairment and decline in elderly individuals with cardiovascular disease. Archives of gerontology and geriatrics. 2017;69:31–7. 10.1016/j.archger.2016.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Yang J, Fan C, Pan L, Xie M, He Q, Li D, et al. C‐reactive protein plays a marginal role in cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2015;30(2):156–65. 10.1002/gps.4236 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K, Simonsick EM, Harris T, Shorr RI, et al. The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. Jama. 2004;292(18):2237–42. 10.1001/jama.292.18.2237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Singh-Manoux A, Dugravot A, Brunner E, Kumari M, Shipley M, Elbaz A, et al. Interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein as predictors of cognitive decline in late midlife. Neurology. 2014;83(6):486–93. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Walker KA, Gottesman RF, Wu A, Knopman DS, Gross AL, Mosley TH, et al. Systemic inflammation during midlife and cognitive change over 20 years: The ARIC Study. Neurology. 2019;92(11):e1256–e67. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wilson RS, Capuano AW, Sytsma J, Bennett DA, Barnes LL. Cognitive aging in older Black and White persons. Psychology and aging. 2015;30(2):279 10.1037/pag0000024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Díaz‐Venegas C, Downer B, Langa KM, Wong R. Racial and ethnic differences in cognitive function among older adults in the USA. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2016;31(9):1004–12. 10.1002/gps.4410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Shah T, Newcombe P, Smeeth L, Addo J, Casas JP, Whittaker J, et al. Ancestry as a determinant of mean population C-reactive protein values: implications for cardiovascular risk prediction. Circulation. 2010;3(5):436–44. 10.1161/CIRCGENETICS.110.957431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kelley-Hedgepeth A, Lloyd-Jones DM, Colvin A, Matthews KA, Johnston J, Sowers MR, et al. Ethnic differences in C-reactive protein concentrations. Clinical chemistry. 2008;54(6):1027–37. 10.1373/clinchem.2007.098996 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Cushman M, McClure LA, Howard VJ, Jenny NS, Lakoski SG, Howard G. Implications of increased C-reactive protein for cardiovascular risk stratification in black and white men and women in the US. Clinical chemistry. 2009;55(9):1627–36. 10.1373/clinchem.2008.122093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pan Y, Jackson RT. Ethnic difference in the relationship between acute inflammation and serum ferritin in US adult males. Epidemiology & Infection. 2008;136(3):421–31. 10.1017/S095026880700831X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ness RB, Haggerty CL, Harger G, Ferrell R. Differential distribution of allelic variants in cytokine genes among African Americans and White Americans. American journal of epidemiology. 2004;160(11):1033–8. 10.1093/aje/kwh325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Howard VJ, Cushman M, Pulley L, Gomez CR, Go RC, Prineas RJ, et al. The reasons for geographic and racial differences in stroke study: objectives and design. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25(3):135–43. 10.1159/000086678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Howard G. Why do we have a stroke belt in the southeastern United States? A review of unlikely and uninvestigated potential causes. The American journal of the medical sciences. 1999;317(3):160–7. 10.1097/00000441-199903000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Callahan CM, Unverzagt FW, Hui SL, Perkins AJ, Hendrie HC. Six-item screener to identify cognitive impairment among potential subjects for clinical research. Medical care. 2002;40(9):771–81. 10.1097/00005650-200209000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gillett SR, Boyle RH, Zakai NA, McClure LA, Jenny NS, Cushman M. Validating laboratory results in a national observational cohort study without field centers: the Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke cohort. Clin Biochem. 2014;47(16–17):243–6. 10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2014.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sakkinen PA, Macy EM, Callas PW, Cornell ES, Hayes TE, Kuller LH, et al. Analytical and biologic variability in measures of hemostasis, fibrinolysis, and inflammation: assessment and implications for epidemiology. American journal of epidemiology. 1999;149(3):261–7. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Castoldi G, Galimberti S, Riva C, Papagna R, Querci F, Casati M, et al. Association between serum values of C-reactive protein and cytokine production in whole blood of patients with type 2 diabetes. Clinical science. 2007;113(2):103–8. 10.1042/CS20060338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Nyström T. C-reactive protein: a marker or a player? Clinical Science. 2007;113(2):79–81. 10.1042/CS20070121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Evans CR, Long DL, Howard G, McClure LA, Zakai NA, Jenny NS, et al. C-reactive protein and stroke risk in blacks and whites: The REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke cohort. American heart journal. 2019;217:94–100. 10.1016/j.ahj.2019.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gillett SR, Thacker EL, Letter AJ, McClure LA, Wadley VG, Unverzagt FW, et al. Correlates of incident cognitive impairment in the REasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) Study. The Clinical neuropsychologist. 2015;29(4):466–86. 10.1080/13854046.2015.1042524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159–65. 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Barry D, Bates ME, Labouvie E. FAS and CFL forms of verbal fluency differ in difficulty: a meta-analytic study. Applied neuropsychology. 2008;15(2):97–106. 10.1080/09084280802083863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Unverzagt FW, Monahan PO, Moser LR, Zhao Q, Carpenter JS, Sledge GW, et al. The Indiana University telephone-based assessment of neuropsychological status: a new method for large scale neuropsychological assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2007;13(5):799–806. 10.1017/S1355617707071020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rapp SR, Legault C, Espeland MA, Resnick SM, Hogan PE, Coker LH, et al. Validation of a cognitive assessment battery administered over the telephone. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2012;60(9):1616–23. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04111.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Muthén L. Mplus user’s guide 7th ed Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 1998. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. The annals of statistics. 1978;6(2):461–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.McArdle JJ, Ferrer-Caja E, Hamagami F, Woodcock RW. Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. Developmental psychology. 2002;38(1):115 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tilvis RS, Kähönen-Väre MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE. Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2004;59(3):M268–M74. 10.1093/gerona/59.3.m268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Teunissen C, Van Boxtel M, Bosma H, Bosmans E, Delanghe J, De Bruijn C, et al. Inflammation markers in relation to cognition in a healthy aging population. Journal of neuroimmunology. 2003;134(1):142–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Salthouse TA. Continuity of cognitive change across adulthood. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2016;23(3):932–9. 10.3758/s13423-015-0910-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Walker KA, Windham BG, Power MC, Hoogeveen RC, Folsom AR, Ballantyne CM, et al. The association of mid-to late-life systemic inflammation with white matter structure in older adults: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Neurobiology of aging. 2018;68:26–33. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.03.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Walker KA, Power MC, Hoogeveen RC, Folsom AR, Ballantyne CM, Knopman DS, et al. Midlife systemic inflammation, late-life white matter integrity, and cerebral small vessel disease: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Stroke. 2017;48(12):3196–202. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.018675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Silverman JM, Beeri MS, Schmeidler J, Rosendorff C, Angelo G, Mavris RS, et al. C-reactive protein and memory function suggest antagonistic pleiotropy in very old nondemented subjects. Age and ageing. 2009;38(2):237–41. 10.1093/ageing/afn278 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Silverman JM, Schmeidler J, Beeri MS, Rosendorff C, Sano M, Grossman HT, et al. C-reactive protein and familial risk for dementia: a phenotype for successful cognitive aging. Neurology. 2012;79(11):1116–23. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182698c89 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in psychology. 2014;5:772 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vonk JM, Rizvi B, Lao PJ, Budge M, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, et al. Letter and category fluency performance correlates with distinct patterns of cortical thickness in older adults. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29(6):2694–700. 10.1093/cercor/bhy138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Vicario A, Martinez CD, Baretto D, Casale AD, Nicolosi L. Hypertension and cognitive decline: impact on executive function. The Journal of Clinical Hypertension. 2005;7(10):598–604. 10.1111/j.1524-6175.2005.04498.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.B Zahodne L, J Manly J, Narkhede A, Y Griffith E, DeCarli C, S Schupf N, et al. Structural MRI predictors of late-life cognition differ across African Americans, Hispanics, and Whites. Current Alzheimer Research. 2015;12(7):632–9. 10.2174/1567205012666150530203214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Warren KN, Beason-Held LL, Carlson O, Egan JM, An Y, Doshi J, et al. Elevated markers of inflammation are associated with longitudinal changes in brain function in older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A. 2018;73(6):770–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hoth KF, Tate DF, Poppas A, Forman DE, Gunstad J, Moser DJ, et al. Endothelial function and white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Stroke. 2007;38(2):308–12. 10.1161/01.STR.0000254517.04275.3f [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Markus HS, Hunt B, Palmer K, Enzinger C, Schmidt H, Schmidt R. Markers of endothelial and hemostatic activation and progression of cerebral white matter hyperintensities: longitudinal results of the Austrian Stroke Prevention Study. Stroke. 2005;36(7):1410–4. 10.1161/01.STR.0000169924.60783.d4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Marsland AL, Gianaros PJ, Kuan DC-H, Sheu LK, Krajina K, Manuck SB. Brain morphology links systemic inflammation to cognitive function in midlife adults. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2015;48:195–204. 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.03.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Zahodne LB, Kraal AZ, Sharifian N, Zaheed AB, Sol K. Inflammatory mechanisms underlying the effects of everyday discrimination on age-related memory decline. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2019;75:149–54. 10.1016/j.bbi.2018.10.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gu Y, Vorburger R, Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Manly JJ, Schupf N, et al. Circulating inflammatory biomarkers in relation to brain structural measurements in a non-demented elderly population. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2017;65:150–60. 10.1016/j.bbi.2017.04.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Mayeda ER, Karter AJ, Huang ES, Moffet HH, Haan MN, Whitmer RA. Racial/ethnic differences in dementia risk among older type 2 diabetic patients: the diabetes and aging study. Diabetes care. 2014;37(4):1009–15. 10.2337/dc13-0215 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Beydoun MA, Dore GA, Canas J-A, Liang H, Beydoun HA, Evans MK, et al. Systemic inflammation is associated with longitudinal changes in cognitive performance among urban adults. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2018;10:313 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Castanho TC, Amorim L, Zihl J, Palha JA, Sousa N, Santos NC. Telephone-based screening tools for mild cognitive impairment and dementia in aging studies: a review of validated instruments. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2014;6:16 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Du Clos TW, Mold C. C-reactive protein. Immunologic research. 2004;30(3):261–77. 10.1385/IR:30:3:261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Ridker PM, Stampfer MJ, Rifai N. Novel risk factors for systemic atherosclerosis: a comparison of C-reactive protein, fibrinogen, homocysteine, lipoprotein (a), and standard cholesterol screening as predictors of peripheral arterial disease. Jama. 2001;285(19):2481–5. 10.1001/jama.285.19.2481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Koenig W, Sund M, Fröhlich M, Löwel H, Hutchinson WL, Pepys MB. Refinement of the association of serum C-reactive protein concentration and coronary heart disease risk by correction for within-subject variation over time: the MONICA Augsburg studies, 1984 and 1987. American journal of epidemiology. 2003;158(4):357–64. 10.1093/aje/kwg135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Woloshin S, Schwartz LM. Distribution of C-reactive protein values in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;352(15):1611–3. 10.1056/NEJM200504143521525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Metti AL, Yaffe K, Boudreau RM, Simonsick EM, Carnahan RM, Satterfield S, et al. Trajectories of inflammatory markers and cognitive decline over 10 years. Neurobiology of aging. 2014;35(12):2785–90. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Jenny NS, French B, Arnold AM, Strotmeyer ES, Cushman M, Chaves PH, et al. Long-term assessment of inflammation and healthy aging in late life: the Cardiovascular Health Study All Stars. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2012;67(9):970–6. 10.1093/gerona/glr261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kabagambe EK, Judd SE, Howard VJ, Zakai NA, Jenny NS, Hsieh M, et al. Inflammation biomarkers and risk of all-cause mortality in the reasons for geographic and racial differences in stroke cohort. American journal of epidemiology. 2011;174(3):284–92. 10.1093/aje/kwr085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Stephen D Ginsberg

21 Aug 2020

PONE-D-20-23675

C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Cushman,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration by 2 Reviewers and an Academic Editor, all of the critiques of both Reviewers must be addressed in detail in a revision to determine publication status. If you are prepared to undertake the work required, I would be pleased to reconsider my decision, but revision of the original submission without directly addressing the critiques of the 2 Reviewers does not guarantee acceptance for publication in PLOS ONE. If the authors do not feel that the queries can be addressed, please consider submitting to another publication medium. A revised submission will be sent out for re-review. The authors are urged to have the manuscript given a hard copyedit for syntax and grammar.

==============================

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. 

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? 

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The association between CRP and cognitive function has been extensively studied. Yet, findings are conflicting and the current study has the advantage of a large national, biracial sample that was followed up for a relatively long duration.

I have several comments:

Abstract:

1. please add values to the reported results. It seems like subheadings were omitted in line with the journal formatting but the sentences were not revised accordingly (line 3- "To examine...", results should start with words such as "We found that..." (line 39). In addition, the sentence in line 43 ("Findings suggest...") is redundant.

Methods:

2. Why did the authors exclude participants who had one or more errors on the Six-Item Screener? This may limit external validity. Please explain.

3. Why was the 90th percentile chosen as CRP cutoff? Did the authors identify a threshold there? Please provide a rationale.

Results:

4. Line 203 "adjusting for covariates did not change the results". Although p-value remained significant, the effect sizes did become smaller. Please revise the sentence accordingly.

Discussion

Some important literature seems to be missing. For example, Boydoun et at (PMID 30356710) explored the association between CRP and cognitive decline in Whites and African Americans. In addition, there are multiple studies demonstrating an association between CRP and brain function and structure (e.g. PMID 29304217). These may help explaining the underlying mechanisms.

minor comments:

Line 109 "After fully adjustment..." please rephrase as other confounders may exist that the authors weren't aware of or were not available for this cohort.

Line 243: rate decline (change to "rate of decline")

Reviewer #2: The manuscript “C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study” concerns an interesting theme related to aging. However, some alterations are needed in order to improve the quality of the manuscript. English use and writing are acceptable, but could still be refined. For example, past and present tense are mixed throughout the text (past tense is preferable). The use of a single measure of CRP is an important limitation of the study, given that authors say several times that this would be a marker of “systemic inflammation”, what is not true. This should be carefully revised, as also the affirmation that “effects” were found (longitudinal analyses provided no significant findings, and cross-sectional associations cannot infer causality). Specific comments to each section are given below.

Introduction

1. The topic of the study is not only relevant to the population of the United States, but worldwide. By starting the introductory paragraph of the manuscript specifically mentioning the United States, the authors seem to restrict their own interest to this country. I suggest editing the sentence to make it more general.

2. One important point about the relationship between inflammation and cognitive decline is that, mainly, it has been shown within the status of low-grade inflammation, which may have very different metabolic implications compared to acute inflammation. CRP may be used as a marker of low-grade inflammation, if repeated measures are used. This was not the case of the present study. So, in fact, authors have used a very general marker of non-specific inflammation to investigate such potential pathway affecting cognitive function. This has to be clearly stated in the manuscript since the beginning, and also recognized as a limitation of the study.

Methods

3. The topic “Design and procedures” should mention the maximum duration of follow-up.

4. Lines 83-84: Details about how the Stroke Belt is defined could be briefly presented. Again, authors need to consider that their manuscript will reach readers from many countries, not only from the USA.

5. Why classifying CRP as “low” or “elevated”? Naming those categories like this give the idea that both would be “not ideal”. If authors test the hypothesis that increased CRP would be associated with cognitive decline, then “elevated” (or “high”) could be one category, compared to “normal” values.

6. In the Methods, authors recognize that literature used a distinct cutoff for CRP, for what can be defined as “chronic systemic inflammation” (or low-grade inflammation). By using a very distinct cutoff based on other criteria (genetic ancestry affecting CRP in different races), authors in fact distance themselves from exploring low-grade inflammation (what would also demand repeated CRP measures, by the way). So, again, the fact that chronic systemic inflammation is not being investigated needs to be made very clear throughout the manuscript.

7. Lines 146-151: If fasting glucose, nonfasting glucose, total cholesterol, low density lipoproteins and high density lipoproteins were measured by the investigators, their analytical procedures should be reported, as done for CRP. Detailed information should be provided for blood pressure, weight and height assessments.

8. Were body weight and height also measured in duplicate as done for blood pressure? This information should be made clear. If they were not, this should also be reported as a limitation of the study.

9. Please check with the Editor if the Journal has any specific orientation about how to mention race / skin color categories. I suggest avoiding the use of the terms “blacks” and “whites”.

Results

10. Line 192. If categories of nutritional status based on BMI are given in the results, they should have been previously defined in the Methods.

11. Line 194. The word “greater” may suggest the idea of something that is positive, good. I suggest using “higher” instead of “greater” (here and all over the text) to avoid this misinterpretation.

Discussion

12. In the first paragraph, please indicate the population of the study (adults aged ≥45 years).

13. Line 221-223: Significant findings with baseline data do not infer causality, so the word “effect” should not be used. “Strong” could also be avoided. I suggest replacing “While the effect of continuous increments of CRP on baseline memory” by “While the associations between continuous increments of CRP and baseline memory”, and replacing “the effect remained strong” by “the association remained significant”.

14. Lines 227-228: Authors say that “the effect of CRP on baseline cognition was robust”, but cross-sectional associations did not test effect.

15. Lines 244-245: Avoid repeating “midlife” in the same sentence: “take place before the age of 65, in midlife given various studies suggesting that age-related cognitive accelerates during midlife”.

16. Authors did not discuss the fact that their single measure of CRP may have influenced the findings. As said before, a unique measure does not allow inferring the state of low-grade inflammation.

17. Another point that may have contributed to distinct findings compared to literature was the use of different cutoffs.

18. Line 260: Authors mention “systemic inflammation”, but again, they did not measured systemic inflammation. Please revise.

19. Lines 309-310: I do not agree with the conclusion, since authors again insist in saying that “systemic inflammation” was measured, but it was not. Also, the findings of the present study are not “strong evidence that systemic inflammation as measured with CRP has a deleterious effect on cognitive level”. Effect (and so, causality) is measured in longitudinal analysis, and your longitudinal analyses did not provide significant results to support this affirmation. The significant cross-sectional associations cannot infer any causal relationship, so authors should not call it “deleterious effect”.

20. Only 8 out of the 56 references of the manuscript are recent (published after 2016). I suggest including more recent publications about the topic.

Tables and figures

21. Table 1. I suggest including the “n” to all cells in which only the % is presented (categorical variables).

22. Table 1. Were all variables available to all 21,782 participants? If not, variables with missing values should present their exact “n”.

23. Table 1. The differences in baseline characteristics according to race should be given (either if there were, or in the footnote in case there were not).

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by February, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Stephen D. Ginsberg, Ph.D.

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for including your ethics statement: 

"REGARDS was approved by Institutional Review Boards of all participating institutions and all participants provided written informed consent. Interviewers were trained to identify participants answering questions in a manner suggesting lack of comprehension, and such participants were not included further. Potential participants who were able to respond to telephone questions provided verbal consent, which was followed by written consent at an in home visit.".   

a. Please amend your current ethics statement to include the full name of the ethics committee/institutional review board(s) that approved your specific study.

b. Once you have amended this/these statement(s) in the Methods section of the manuscript, please add the same text to the “Ethics Statement” field of the submission form (via “Edit Submission”).

For additional information about PLOS ONE ethical requirements for human subjects research, please refer to http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. Please see http://www.bmj.com/content/340/bmj.c181.long for guidelines on how to de-identify and prepare clinical data for publication. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

PLoS One. 2020 Dec 31;15(12):e0244612. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244612.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


13 Nov 2020

Dear Dr. Ginsberg,

We appreciate the careful review of our manuscript and are delighted that you will consider a revised version for publication. We have included a tracked changes version of the manuscript. We hope that our changes have adequately addressed the suggestions and that the revision will be suitable for publication in PLOS ONE.

Reviewer #1: The association between CRP and cognitive function has been extensively studied. Yet, findings are conflicting, and the current study has the advantage of a large national, biracial sample that was followed up for a relatively long duration. I have several comments:

1. Abstract:

Please add values to the reported results. It seems like subheadings were omitted in line with the journal formatting, but the sentences were not revised accordingly (line 3- "To examine...", results should start with words such as "We found that..." (line 39). In addition, the sentence in line 43 ("Findings suggest...") is redundant.

We have added values throughout the abstract and made the recommendation changes to the text.

2. Methods:

Why did the authors exclude participants who had one or more errors on the Six-Item Screener? This may limit external validity. Please explain.

In order to exclude people with baseline impairment to allow study of potential causal relationships, we excluded participants with a score of 4 or fewer at baseline, which would indicate 2 or more errors on the Six-Item Screener (SIS). This cut-off is largely cited as indicating severe cognitive impairment and excluded 6.7% of participants. An indication of a participant experiencing severe cognitive impairment at baseline might also suggest other underlying medical factors (i.e., a neurodegenerative disease) which may in turn limit our ability to detect an association between CRP and cognition. Given our aim is to evaluate whether CRP would be helpful in predicting cognitive decline as to identify individuals at higher risk of cognitive decline and dementia, excluding participants with cognitive impairment at baseline does not limit our external validity. We updated the text in the methods section to clarify this.

3. Why was the 90th percentile chosen as CRP cutoff? Did the authors identify a threshold there? Please provide a rationale.

We chose a race-specific 90th percentile cut-off to determine “elevated” CRP given recent work that differences in CRP concentration among racially/ethnically diverse populations may be partially driven by genetic ancestry, suggesting that a single threshold value of CRP may not be appropriate. We chose the 90th percentile to select the highest possible range in given racial group. In addition, in another REGARDS report, there was no association of the typically studied CRP cutoff of 3 mg/L with stroke in Black people in REGARDS. This rationale has been clarified in the methods section.

4. Results:

Line 203 "adjusting for covariates did not change the results". Although p-value remained significant, the effect sizes did become smaller. Please revise the sentence accordingly.

We did not intend to be misleading and updated the text accordingly.

5. Discussion:

Some important literature seems to be missing. For example, Boydoun et at (PMID 30356710) explored the association between CRP and cognitive decline in Whites and African Americans. In addition, there are multiple studies demonstrating an association between CRP and brain function and structure (e.g. PMID 29304217). These may help explaining the underlying mechanisms.

We have included these articles into our discussion, specifically relating how the Boydoun study differs from ours. We also included the study of CRP and blood brain flow as further evidence of potential mechanisms through which CRP impacts cognitive functioning.

6. Minor comments:

Line 109 "After fully adjustment..." please rephrase as other confounders may exist that the authors weren't aware of or were not available for this cohort.

We have removed “fully” from that and similar sentences.

7. Line 243: rate decline (change to "rate of decline")

We have updated the text accordingly.

Reviewer #2:

The manuscript “C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study” concerns an interesting theme related to aging. However, some alterations are needed in order to improve the quality of the manuscript. English use and writing are acceptable, but could still be refined. For example, past and present tense are mixed throughout the text (past tense is preferable). The use of a single measure of CRP is an important limitation of the study, given that authors say several times that this would be a marker of “systemic inflammation”, what is not true. This should be carefully revised, as also the affirmation that “effects” were found (longitudinal analyses provided no significant findings, and cross-sectional associations cannot infer causality).

Specific comments to each section are given below.

Introduction

8. The topic of the study is not only relevant to the population of the United States, but worldwide. By starting the introductory paragraph of the manuscript specifically mentioning the United States, the authors seem to restrict their own interest to this country. I suggest editing the sentence to make it more general.

We updated the text accordingly to reflect the increase in global population aging.

9. One important point about the relationship between inflammation and cognitive decline is that, mainly, it has been shown within the status of low-grade inflammation, which may have very different metabolic implications compared to acute inflammation. CRP may be used as a marker of low-grade inflammation, if repeated measures are used. This was not the case of the present study. So, in fact, authors have used a very general marker of non-specific inflammation to investigate such potential pathway affecting cognitive function. This has to be clearly stated in the manuscript since the beginning, and also recognized as a limitation of the study.

While we agree that multiple measurements of CRP over time would be ideal to establish chronic inflammation, nearly all articles on CRP as a risk factor for chronic diseases in initially healthy populations, including outcomes like mortality, heart disease, stroke, and cognitive impairment, measured CRP only once. The first commercially used assay for CRP for cardiovascular risk prediction (which was used in this study) was developed using the REGARDS central laboratory-developed ELISA assay as a gold standard in the 1990’s, and use of this test for cardiovascular risk prediction is supported in a number of guidelines, including the 2019 AHA/ACC guideline for primary prevention of cardiovascular disease (https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1161/CIR.0000000000000677). The high sensitivity CRP assay also has reasonable within person variability as a marker of chronic low grade inflammation (Sakkinen et al., 1999 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9927222/). It has been intensely studied in this context. We also previously showed that free-living people with levels of CRP in the range indicating acute inflammation (values >10 mg/L) are at increased cardiovascular risk (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15983251/). Any participants who might have had acute illness around the time of the first visit would have spuriously high levels and their presence in the study would bias results to the null hypothesis. The CRP distributions in REGARDS were in line with what is expected from other studies, and participants would likely have rescheduled their baseline in-home visit if they were ill, so we do not expect there to be undue bias in this regard. To respond to the concerns raised, we have included additional information in our introduction regarding CRP as a marker of acute inflammation, and heavily edited the limitations paragraph regarding CRP as potential marker of non-specific inflammation and on the benefit of longitudinal CRP measurements.

Methods

10. The topic “Design and procedures” should mention the maximum duration of follow-up.

We have indicated that maximum follow-up was up to 12 years in the methods.

11. Lines 83-84: Details about how the Stroke Belt is defined could be briefly presented. Again, authors need to consider that their manuscript will reach readers from many countries, not only from the USA.

In the methods section, under design and procedures, we have included information indicating that a large southeastern region of the United States is referred to as the stroke belt given its well documented high rates of stroke observed in this region compared to the rest of the United States.

12. Why classifying CRP as “low” or “elevated”? Naming those categories like this give the idea that both would be “not ideal”. If authors test the hypothesis that increased CRP would be associated with cognitive decline, then “elevated” (or “high”) could be one category, compared to “normal” values.

We have relabeled the CRP groups. Instead of using “low” and “elevated”, we use below and above the 90th percentile for our groupings in the methods section. However, for ease of readability, throughout the results we refer to above the 90th percentile as “elevated” CRP. We opted for not using the label of normal for values below the 90th percentile as they may still be above published cut-offs.

13. In the Methods, authors recognize that literature used a distinct cutoff for CRP, for what can be defined as “chronic systemic inflammation” (or low-grade inflammation). By using a very distinct cutoff based on other criteria (genetic ancestry affecting CRP in different races), authors in fact distance themselves from exploring low-grade inflammation (what would also demand repeated CRP measures, by the way). So, again, the fact that chronic systemic inflammation is not being investigated needs to be made very clear throughout the manuscript.

We addressed these concerns in the reviewers previous comment. We agree that multiple CRP measurements would be ideal to determine chronic inflammation, but as with other epidemiological studies, it is unfeasible to have repeated CRP measurements. We do not believe that using race-specific cut-offs distances us from evaluating CRP as an indicator of systemic inflammation as several studies demonstrate that one-time assessment of CRP is associated with greater risk of worse health outcomes. As mentioned previously, we included in the limitations sections greater discussion regarding the role of CRP as a marker of inflammation and the need for longitudinal CRP assessment. To avoid confusion we removed the one instance of usage of the word “chronic” from the paper in favor of referring to levels of CRP relevant to disease risk prediction.

14. Lines 146-151: If fasting glucose, nonfasting glucose, total cholesterol, low density lipoproteins and high-density lipoproteins were measured by the investigators, their analytical procedures should be reported, as done for CRP. Detailed information should be provided for blood pressure, weight and height assessments.

We provided additional information regarding how these data were collected in the methods section.

15. Were body weight and height also measured in duplicate as done for blood pressure? This information should be made clear. If they were not, this should also be reported as a limitation of the study.

Body weight and height were measured once during the in-home visit. We added this to the methods.

16. Please check with the Editor if the Journal has any specific orientation about how to mention race / skin color categories. I suggest avoiding the use of the terms “blacks” and “whites”.

We agree that these terms should be avoided, did not mean to infer we classified people by skin color (participants self-identified as reported), and changed all instances to Black people / participants and White people / participants.

Results

17. Line 192. If categories of nutritional status based on BMI are given in the results, they should have been previously defined in the Methods.

We have included information regarding the BMI categories in the covariates section in the methods.

18. Line 194. The word “greater” may suggest the idea of something that is positive, good. I suggest using “higher” instead of “greater” (here and all over the text) to avoid this misinterpretation.

We have updated the word greater for higher throughout the manuscript.

Discussion.

19. In the first paragraph, please indicate the population of the study (adults aged ≥45 years).

We have included additional descriptive information regarding the population in the first paragraph of the discussion.

20. Line 221-223: Significant findings with baseline data do not infer causality, so the word “effect” should not be used. “Strong” could also be avoided. I suggest replacing “While the effect of continuous increments of CRP on baseline memory” by “While the associations between continuous increments of CRP and baseline memory” and replacing “the effect remained strong” by “the association remained significant”.

We have updated our text throughout the discussion to remove any words indicative of causality.

21. Lines 227-228: Authors say that “the effect of CRP on baseline cognition was robust”, but cross-sectional associations did not test effect.

Similarly, we have removed the word “effect” for “association”.

22. Lines 244-245: Avoid repeating “midlife” in the same sentence: “take place before the age of 65, in midlife given various studies suggesting that age-related cognitive accelerates during midlife”.

We removed the first instance of midlife in that sentence.

23. Authors did not discuss the fact that their single measure of CRP may have influenced the findings. As said before, a unique measure does not allow inferring the state of low-grade inflammation.

As mentioned previously, we have addressed this issue with greater information in the limitations paragraph of the discussion.

24. Another point that may have contributed to distinct findings compared to literature was the use of different cutoffs.

We included this as a third point for the discrepancy in our findings from the literature in our second paragraph in the discussion, but the very null findings for CRP as a continuous variable argue against a missed association or that it represents a causal risk factor.

25. Line 260: Authors mention “systemic inflammation”, but again, they did not measured systemic inflammation. Please revise.

We responded to this comment above.

26. Lines 309-310: I do not agree with the conclusion, since authors again insist in saying that “systemic inflammation” was measured, but it was not. Also, the findings of the present study are not “strong evidence that systemic inflammation as measured with CRP has a deleterious effect on cognitive level”. Effect (and so, causality) is measured in longitudinal analysis, and your longitudinal analyses did not provide significant results to support this affirmation. The significant cross-sectional associations cannot infer any causal relationship, so authors should not call it “deleterious effect”.

We remove the term systemic and effect from our concluding paragraph.

27. Only 8 out of the 56 references of the manuscript are recent (published after 2016). I suggest including more recent publications about the topic.

We have included additional references throughout the document and now have 15 citations published on or after 2016.

Tables and figures.

28. Table 1. I suggest including the “n” to all cells in which only the % is presented (categorical variables).

To all categorical variables, we included now the n.

29. Table 1. Were all variables available to all 21,782 participants? If not, variables with missing values should present their exact “n”.

We have indicated which variables had missing data with an asterisk and included the exact n in the table.

30. Table 1. The differences in baseline characteristics according to race should be given (either if there were, or in the footnote in case there were not).

We had described the differences in baseline characteristics by racial group in the beginning of the results section, but we have now included a column that indicates the p-value to denote the differences we described in the text.

Attachment

Submitted filename: CRP PlosOne_Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Stephen D Ginsberg

3 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-23675R1

C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Cushman,

Thank you for resubmitting your work to PLOS ONE. Please make the corrections posed by Reviewer #2 so I can render a decision on this manuscript.

==============================

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. 

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? 

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: The quality of the manuscript was improved. Please find some minor comments below.

Line 74. The word “in” appears twice. Please delete one.

Please include in the limitations of the study the fact that weight and height were not measured in duplicate.

There are still several sentences using “greater”, for which “higher” would be more appropriate.

There are still some sentences in which the word "effect" must be avoided, because of describing cross-sectional findings (example: line 241).

Line 267-269: Authors say that “recent studies have reported (…) greater risk of dementia in later life [6]”. However, reference 6 is not “recent”, since it was published in 2002.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by February, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Stephen D. Ginsberg, Ph.D.

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

PLoS One. 2020 Dec 31;15(12):e0244612. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244612.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


7 Dec 2020

Dear Dr. Ginsberg,

We greatly appreciate the careful second review of our manuscript and are delighted that you will consider a revised version for publication. We have included a tracked changes version of the manuscript with changed text highlighted in yellow for convenience. We hope that our changes have adequately addressed the suggestions and that the revision will be suitable for publication in PLOS ONE.

Reviewer #2: The quality of the manuscript was improved. Please find some minor comments below.

1. Line 74. The word “in” appears twice. Please delete one.

We have removed this typo.

2. Please include in the limitations of the study the fact that weight and height were not measured in duplicate.

While we agree it would be ideal to measure everything in duplicate, due to the inherent design of a large epidemiological study such as REGARDS, it is not feasible to accomplish that. We are not concerned that this lack of duplicate measurement for weight and height is a notable limitation to the aims of our particular study. While lack of a duplicate height and weight measurement may lead to measurement error in a confounder (BMI), we are able to overcome this potential measurement error due to the statistical power provided by our large sample size. Moreover, this potential issue would lead to BMI further attenuating the relationship of CRP with our cognitive outcomes thus biasing to the null hypothesis. Regardless, we have added the following statement in our limitations section:

“Another similar potential limitation is that we only measured several covariates (i.e., height and weight) at baseline. Like most epidemiologic studies, for reasons of pragmatism, all baseline variables cannot be measured twice, which would improve precision of risk estimates. The impact of this limitation would be a bias to the null hypothesis.”

3. There are still several sentences using “greater”, for which “higher” would be more appropriate.

We have removed the word “greater” throughout the manuscript and updated it for “higher” or “increase” where appropriate.

4. There are still some sentences in which the word "effect" must be avoided, because of describing cross-sectional findings (example: line 241).

We have switched “effect” for “association” as appropriate throughout the manuscript.

5. Line 267-269: Authors say that “recent studies have reported (…) greater risk of dementia in later life [6]”. However, reference 6 is not “recent”, since it was published in 2002.

We removed “recent” from that sentence.

Lastly, an additional comment to address was that Reviewer #2 answered “No” to the following question “Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?” and we would just like to reiterate that we addressed this in our previous cover letter as suggested by editor with the following information:

“The data underlying the findings include potentially identifying participant information and cannot be made publicly available due to ethical/legal restrictions. However, data including statistical code from this manuscript are available to researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Data can be obtained upon request through the University of Alabama at Birmingham at regardsadmin@uab.edu.”

Attachment

Submitted filename: CRP PlosOne_2nd Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 2

Stephen D Ginsberg

14 Dec 2020

C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study

PONE-D-20-23675R2

Dear Dr. Cushman,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Stephen D. Ginsberg, Ph.D.

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Stephen D Ginsberg

18 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-23675R2

C-reactive protein and risk of cognitive decline: The REGARDS study

Dear Dr. Cushman:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Stephen D. Ginsberg

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: CRP PlosOne_Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: CRP PlosOne_2nd Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The data underlying the findings include potentially identifying participant information, and cannot be made publicly available due to ethical/legal restrictions. However, data including statistical code from this manuscript are available to researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Data can be obtained upon request through the University of Alabama at Birmingham at regardsadmin@uab.edu.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES