Abstract
Hippocampal synaptic plasticity includes both long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) of synaptic strength, and has been implicated in shaping place field representations that form upon initial exposure to a novel environment. However, direct evidence causally linking either LTP or LTD to place fields remains limited. Here, we show that hippocampal LTD regulates the acute formation and maintenance of place fields using electrophysiology and blocking specifically LTD in freely-moving rats. We also show that exploration of a novel environment produces a widespread and pathway specific de novo synaptic depression in the dorsal hippocampus. Furthermore, disruption of this pathway-specific synaptic depression alters both the dynamics of place field formation and the stability of the newly formed place fields, affecting spatial memory in rats. These results suggest that activity-dependent synaptic depression is required for the acquisition and maintenance of novel spatial information.
Subject terms: Hippocampus, Spatial memory, Long-term depression
LTP and LTD are involved in shaping hippocampal place field representations. Here, the authors show that de novo pathway-specific hippocampal LTD changes dynamics and stability of newly formed place fields, regulating acquisition and maintenance of novel spatial information in adult rats.
Introduction
The exploration and encoding of novel environments is a fundamental learning process that occurs on a relatively short time scale and serves as a useful model for studying how the hippocampus encodes and represents complex, arbitrary associations required for episodic memory. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, particularly long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), is thought to be an important subcellular substrate for spatial learning and memory1,2. Importantly, novelty exploration facilitates the induction of LTD3–5 and LTP4,6, the two principal forms of synaptic plasticity in area CA1 of the hippocampus7–10.
Pyramidal neurons in areas CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus consistently show place modulated firing patterns called “place fields”11, and individual “place cells” are considered prime substrates for hippocampal encoding of spatial environments during spatial learning12,13. Several lines of evidence indicate that stable place field firing location is necessary for this mnemonic role of the hippocampus. Place field locations are usually consistent across exposures to a consistent environment14–17, and spatial navigation deficits are associated with instability of place field locations in ambiguous environments11,18,19.
Importantly, interventions that affect synaptic plasticity have been shown also to affect the location stability of place cells2,20. However, these previous demonstrations of a role for synaptic plasticity in place field maintenance used interventions that generally block both LTP and LTD, such as NMDA receptor blockade20, reduced PKA expression21, or protein synthesis inhibition22. Therefore, the specific role of LTP and LTD in regulating place cell encoding dynamics and the subsequent consolidation of their stable place fields remain to be specified.
Prior attempts to specify the effect of synaptic plasticity inhibition on place field formation failed to find any acute effect on place field formation dynamics. Place fields are not immediately present in a novel environment, but develop over a time-course of minutes concurrent with exploration of these environments23–26. Therefore it is important to investigate whether changes in synaptic plasticity occur on the same time-scale during exposure to a novel environment and whether plasticity blockade at this time-scale affects dynamics of place field formation.
Here, we use multi-electrode electrophysiology to examine whether novelty exploration results in de novo changes in synaptic strength across multiple recording sites in stratum radiatum inputs to apical dendrites of hippocampal CA1 neurons, and if so, whether they are broadly observed across all sites, as reported for hippocampal-dependent aversive learning27. Historically, behaviorally driven changes in the strength of evoked field responses have been confounded by changes in body temperature and behavioral state28,29. To control for these and other unknown variables, we adopted an internal control by using a second stimulating electrode to activate dendritic inputs in the striatum oriens (Fig. 1A). These basal dendritic synapses also exhibit LTP and LTD30, but are not known to be affected by novelty or stress. We then observe the formation of place-cell fields in a novel environment and their stability across days in the presence of a specific pharmacological inhibitor of LTD expression to determine whether LTD participates in stable place representation in the hippocampus.
Results
Novelty triggers pathway-specific synaptic depression
Stimulation at both stratum radiatum and stratum oriens evoked canonical post-synaptic field potentials (fEPSPs, Fig. 1A) that were maintained for 1-h recording sessions across at least 3 days. Fig. 1B illustrates stable average responses in the final baseline session, after brief handling after 30 min to simulate environmental transfer. Individual channel responses were plotted every 5 stimulations for clarity. No change in the distribution of responses between channels was observed in the baseline session (Fig. 1C; Supplemental Table 1). On the exposure day rat fEPSPs were evoked once per minute (0.16 Hz) through a 30-min baseline period in the familiar recording chamber, followed by a subsequent 30-min period in a novel environment and a final 30-min returned to the familiar environment (Fig. 1F). The distribution of baseline normalized radiatum responses was significantly changed relative to oriens evoked responses during and after novel environment exploration (Fig. 1G; ks30–60 = 0.37, p < 0.01, ks60–90 = 0.47, p < 0.001), with nearly every channel showing a small decrease in amplitude over the duration of the recording session. While the distribution of responses were significantly shifted in the radiatum group relative to oriens, these results aggregated multiple recording locations from multiple animals and encompassed considerable variation in fEPSP change. Therefore we sought to determine whether a change in synaptic strength was evident on a per-animal basis. Previous research using multiple channels has used a 1 standard deviation (SD) threshold to examine deviations from baseline that do not depend on the absolute magnitude of the deviation or a homogenous shift on all channels27. Calculating an average proportion of channels above or below 1 SD on a per animal basis demonstrated that a specific decrease in a subset of radiatum evoked fEPSPs was a consistent feature across animals relative to oriens evoked fEPSPs after exposure to the novel environment (Fig. 1H, I; FEpoch(2,8) = 16.17, p < 0.001, FGroup(1,8) = 5.81, p = 0.04, FEpoch*Group(2,8) = 5.49, p = 0.02 interaction, follow-up pairwise comparisons oriens vs radiatum t0–30(8) = 0.55, p = 0.59, t30–60(8) = 1.89, p = 0.09, t60–90(8) = 2.67, p = 0.02). No such alteration was observed in the baseline recording session on the prior day (Fig. 1D, E; Supplementary Table 1). These results demonstrate that the novel environment induced a de novo and pathway-specific reduction in field potential magnitude at apical inputs to hippocampal CA1 neurons.
fEPSP depression is mediated by AMPA receptor endocytosis
To test whether the observed de novo fEPSP depression was mediated by a facilitation of endocytosis of postsynaptic AMPA receptors, a well-characterized mechanisms of LTD induced by electrical stimulation31,32, the GluA23Y (previously known as GluR23Y) interference peptide or a Scrambled control peptide was administered IV prior to the recording session on the novelty exposure day after baseline recording days (Supplementary Fig. 1) in a separate cohort of animals. Oriens evoked potentials in response to novelty exploration showed no alteration in the distribution of responses between GluA23Y and Scrambled treatments (Fig. 2A, B; Supplementary Table 1). In contrast, the de novo shift in fEPSP responses observed at radiatum sites was specifically prevented by GluA23Y relative to Scrambled control after novel environment exploration (Fig. 2E, F; ks30–60 = 0.27, p = 0.18, ks60–90 = 0.49, p < 0.001). This distribution change was reflected in the per-animal proportion of channels below 1 SD of baseline (Fig. 2G, H; FEpoch(2,7) = 3.21, p = 0.07, FGroup(1,7) = 8.11, p = 0.02, FEpoch*Group(2,7) = 3.60, p = 0.05; follow-up pairwise comparisons Scrambled vs GluA23Y t0–30(7) = 1.08, p = 0.32, t30–60(7) = 2.41, p = 0.047, t60–90(7) = 2.38, p = 0.049). No significant effect of drug treatment was observed in the oriens pathway on the exposure day (Fig. 2C, D; Supplementary Table 1). On the following day, rats were re-exposed to the same novel environment without drug infusions. In radiatum pathways, a transient alteration in the distribution fEPSP magnitude was observed with opposing directionalities between the novel environment and the post-novelty period (Supplementary Fig. 2, ks30–60 = 0.58, p < 0.001, ks60–90 = 0.32, p = 0.06) with no alteration in distribution in the oriens pathways (Supplementary Table 1). This was reflected in a marginal non-significant increase in the per-animal proportion of channels 1 SD higher than baseline in the Scrambled treated group relative to GluA23Y (Supplementary Fig. 2; FEpoch(2,7) = 4.20, p = 0.04, FGroup(1,7) = 0.59, p = 0.47, FEpoch*Group(2,7) = 3.74, p = 0.05). No significant effect of drug treatment was observed in the oriens pathway on the re-exposure day (Supplementary Fig. 2; Supplementary Table 1). Together these results show that novel environment exposure produces a de novo reduction in fEPSP magnitude in the apical inputs to CA1 that is mediated by activity-dependent AMPA receptor endocytosis, a well-characterized mechanism for LTD expression31,32. The novel environment elevated locomotion relative to the familiar environment in both GluA23Y and Scrambled peptide treated rats (Supplementary Fig. 7), with no difference between drug treatment groups. As locomotor activity itself could conceivably induce pathway-specific plasticity33,34, we compared activity levels during the first exploration of the novel environment to the re-exploration on the following day. Though fEPSP reductions were only observed on the first exploration, activity levels were comparable between exploration and the first re-exploration, suggesting that novelty itself is a necessary driver of the plasticity effect. Although some previous studies have reported fEPSP decreases triggered by exposure to novel environments3,27, it was unclear whether this effect was pathway-specific or mediated by synaptic plasticity mechanisms. We show here that a widespread synaptic change in response to environmental novelty is pathway-specific fEPSP depression mediated by AMPA receptor endocytosis. Our results suggest that while this reduction is widespread it is also heterogenous, with some channels undergoing a larger reduction than others.
New place field maintenance requires synaptic depression
The widespread de novo fEPSP reduction we observed suggests that rather than being a direct encoding mechanism for the new environment, this synaptic change may participate in a gain-control type function35. Accordingly, de novo fEPSP reduction may act in concert with other plasticity changes such as LTP in other hippocampal pathways to encode the new environment. To test this conjecture directly, we next investigated whether blocking AMPA receptor endocytosis during exposure to a novel environment would affect the hippocampal encoding of that environment. Place field formation occurs in a novel environment, and these newly formed place fields are specifically affected by NMDA receptor blockade, CAMKII deletion, and protein synthesis inhibition20,21,36–38. While these interventions likely block both synaptic LTP and LTD, LTP is thought to be the mediator of these effects39,40 and the specific role of blocking LTD has not been investigated. Measurement of place field formation dynamics is facilitated in a single dimensional maze where laps are completed more rapidly and with greater consistency. Accordingly, we employed a linear maze composed of reconfigurable sections that permitted multiple different environments (Supplementary Fig. 3) to examine the effect of LTD blockade. A total of 458 cells from seven rats were recorded during exploration of linear environments. Place field stability across days was achieved by the use of waveform characteristics (Supplementary Fig. 4) to identify matching cells41,42, and field stability was measured as a correlation between the firing maps produced in two different sessions from cells with matching waveform characteristics. As place fields can remap exclusively via either a rate change (rate remapping) or by changing field location (global remapping), either of which may account for reduced correlations, we additionally assessed place field stability in the subset of cells with stable firing rates, defined as a peak rate that changed between days by less than a factor of 2 (Fig. 3D, G). Place field firing was recorded in a highly familiar linear configuration on a baseline day. The subsequent day, GluA23Y or Scrambled control peptide was administered IV, and a novel maze configuration was presented after an initial exploration of the familiar environment. Following exposure to the novel configuration, rats were returned to the familiar environment. The same protocol was employed the following day in the absence of drug administration. Rats administered Scrambled peptide prior to novelty exposure showed primarily correlated place fields between days on the novel maze, both overall and in the subset of rate-stable cells (Fig. 3C, Supplementary Table 1). In contrast, following administration of the interference peptide GluA23Y, the novel environment place fields displayed significantly less conservation between days when compared as a distribution to Scrambled fields both overall (Fig. 3B, H; ks = 0.31, p = 0.003) and in the rate-stable subset of cells (Fig. 3I; ks = 0.40, p = 0.013). GluA23Y peptide treatment did not impair place field correlation distributions in the familiar environment overall (Fig. 3E) or in rate-stable cells (Fig. 3F). As correlations were skewed towards 1 in most groups, a non-parametric one-way ANOVA (Kruskal–Wallis) on four groups (Familiar/Novel Maze, Scrambled/GluA23Y treatment) was conducted on overall correlations and rate-stable correlations to investigate whether average place field correlations were affected by GluA23Y treatment. A significant difference between groups (overall, χ2(3) = 16.07, p = 0.0011; rate-stable, χ2(3) = 14.02, p = 0.0029) with follow-up targeted pairwise comparisons (Mann–Whitney U) showed that GluA23Y treatment reduced place field stability specifically in the novel environment (Fig. 3C, overall, UNovel = 2.59, p = 0.0096; rate-stable, UNovel = 2.63, p = 0.0085). These data suggest that reduced correlations are neither a generalized consequence of GluA23Y treatment nor specifically due to changes in firing rates. Therefore, we conclude that disruption of an LTD expression mechanism by GluA23Y induced aberrant global remapping of a novel environment. Similar results were obtained with exploration of a novel two dimensional square environment that matched the geometry used to observe LTD of evoked field potentials (Supplementary Fig. 5).
AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition alters field formation
Stable place fields form and consolidate over the first several minutes of exploration in a novel environment23,25,26,43. The time scale of these dynamics are consistent with the de novo fEPSP reduction we observed. Additionally, experiments that record field firing in mice exploring virtual environments44 report similar dynamics45,46 and implicate dendritic plateau potentials and other nonlinearities in the rapid recruitment of new place fields47. Importantly, these reports support the idea that an active synaptic plasticity process is required despite relatively short time spans, as the subthreshold voltages recorded before, during, and after recruitment change dramatically48. We hypothesized that AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition may interfere with the speed of field recruitment or the early stability of these fields by raising the noise level of synaptic inputs to CA1.
In our experimental design, rats traversed the entire maze more than once a minute, and therefore the linearized maze design permitted an analysis of firing field stability within a session (Supplementary Fig. 3). Firing rate maps were computed for each individual lap, and a correlation matrix was calculated for each cell, with the median correlation of each lap to each other lap taken as a similarity index. As individual rats completed varying numbers of laps within the fixed time recording session, the first eight laps were averaged for familiar configuration sessions (8-min sessions) and the first 16 laps were averaged for the novel configuration sessions (16-min sessions). Lap to lap similarly was uniformly high in the familiar environment (Supplementary Fig. 6). Consistent with prior reports, in the novel environment the correlations between average place cell firing maps increased over the first several laps, with significantly lower correlations until the fourth lap in control rats (Fig. 4A, B, E).
In contrast to our hypothesis, recruitment speed and field stability were not impaired by GluA23Y peptide treatment, however an alteration in dynamics was observed, with highly correlated place fields established by the second lap (Fig. 4C–E). A comparison of the distribution of correlations specifically on the first four laps demonstrated a rapid, single lap shift to a high correlation distribution in GluA23Y treated rats (Fig. 4G; ks-test vs lap 4: ksLap1 = 0.37, p < 0.001, all other laps p > 0.05), whereas a cumulative shift over four laps was evident in control rats (Fig. 4F; ks-test vs lap 4: ksLap1 = 0.52, p < 0.001; ksLap2 = 0.29, p = 0.0017; ksLap3 = 0.22, p = 0.042). These unexpected results indicate that AMPA receptor endocytosis blockade does not acutely diminish the lap to lap stability of place fields. Instead, this intervention appeared to accelerate the formation of a stable place field during novelty exposure. On a subsequent re-exposure day, place fields from Scrambled peptide treated rats were stable from the first lap of the re-exposed novel environment (Fig. 4H, I; Supplementary Table 1). In contrast, place fields were initially unstable in rats treated with GluA23Y peptide (Fig. 4H, J,p < 0.05 lap 1 vs 4), reminiscent of place field dynamics in a novel environment. Accordingly, AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition appears to exert two unique and opposing effects on the formation of new place fields in a novel environment. Specifically, following treatment with GluA23Y peptide, stable place fields form more rapidly within the session as compared with controls, however place fields are not maintained across days as is typically observed.
Synaptic depression is required for new contextual learning
These effects of GluA23Y on place field stability may suggest that AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition interferes with hippocampal-dependent memory such as contextual conditioning. However our previous research challenges this assertion, as LTD blockade by either GluA23Y peptide or GluN2B subtype NMDA receptor antagonism did not impair contextual fear conditioning49. Contextual fear conditioning is greatly affected by prior habituation to the training environment context50, and NMDA receptor antagonism in the hippocampus is less effective in blocking inhibitory avoidance in rats subject to context pre-exposure51,52. We hypothesized that GluA23Y peptide induced place field instability would impair contextual pre-exposure facilitation and therefore examined the effect of peptide administration during contextual pre-exposure on subsequent expression of inhibitory avoidance (Fig. 5A). Rats not exposed to contextual environmental cues a day prior to context-shock training showed impaired inhibitory avoidance, crossing rapidly into the dark chamber paired with footshock relative to rats that received contextual pre-exposure, who showed pronounced inhibitory avoidance (Fig. 5B; t(14) = 2.27, p = 0.039). Pre-exposed rats treated with Scrambled peptide also showed robust inhibitory avoidance, however the latency to enter the dark compartment was markedly impaired in GluA23Y treated rats (Fig. 5B; t(27) = 2.25, p = 0.033). It is important to note that GluA23Y peptide was administered only during pre-exposure to the environmental cues, leaving AMPA receptor endocytosis unperturbed during context-shock association 24 h later. To establish a locus of action for this systemic effect, GluA23Y or Scrambled control peptide (45 pmol/μl, 2.5 μl/hemisphere) was microinjected into the dorsal hippocampus prior to contextual pre-exposure. Similar to the systemic effect, GluA23Y markedly reduced inhibitory avoidance relative to Scrambled control treated rats (Fig. 5C; t(22) = 2.71, p = 0.013). Thus, AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition during contextual pre-exposure is sufficient to impair the subsequent context-shock association, supporting a critical role of LTD mechanisms during novel environment exposure in contextual learning.
Discussion
The present findings illuminate several features of hippocampal learning and support a role for synaptic LTD mechanisms in the acquisition of novel spatial information in the hippocampus. Inhibition of activity-dependent AMPA receptor endocytosis with the peptide inhibitor GluA23Y, a validated and specific inhibitor of LTD expression mechanisms, blocked a novelty induced decrease in evoked fEPSPs in the hippocampus, blocked facilitation of inhibitory avoidance by contextual exposure, and impaired the maintenance of place field locations on re-exposure to a novel environment. In addition, acute AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition altered the dynamics of place field formation in a novel environment. Although we demonstrated pathway-specific reduction in fEPSP magnitude in response to novelty exploration, we blocked AMPA receptor endocytosis throughout the brain while recording field potentials and CA1 place field activity. As the behavioral effect of systemic treatment on impaired inhibitory avoidance was recapitulated with hippocampus-specific inhibition of AMPA receptor endocytosis, our evidence suggests that mechanisms supporting LTD in the hippocampus are engaged during novel environment exposure to establish stable place field formation and memory. De novo fEPSP reduction specifically in synapses to apical dendrites of CA1 neurons in response to novelty exposure in the absence of changes in synapses targeting the basal dendrites of CA1 in the stratum oriens suggest an effect independent of global alterations such as brain state or temperature changes previously implicated in behaviorally driven alterations in synaptic strength28,34,53. Furthermore, we provide evidence that locomotor activity is not sufficient to produce this plasticity effect, as elevated locomotion on the second exploration of the novel environment did not produced fEPSP reductions seen on the first exploration.
We failed to observe evidence for LTP, reported to be facilitated under certain novelty conditions4,6, notably those that involve exploration of an empty environment in which the flooring has been altered. As this environmental change is similar to the novel environment we employed, we suggest two possible explanations for these differing results. First, LTP may only be observable with an induction stimulation protocol (i.e., high frequency stimulation). Accordingly, our data would suggest that environmental exploration creates a metaplastic environment that facilitates LTP while also inducing de novo synaptic depression. Considerable evidence54–56 supports the conjecture that metaplastic conditions that promote LTP can inhibit LTD and vice versa. Accordingly, this explanation would suggest a more complex interplay between LTP and LTD in reaction to novelty. A second explanation may be linked to important differences between the environmental exploration paradigm employed here as compared to those employed in previous studies. As our novel environment differed completely from baseline, rather than altering discrete elements of a familiar recording chamber4, the direction of metaplastic change may depend on some dimension of novelty magnitude. In this case our findings are consistent with prior reports confirming that facilitation of LTD can arise as a consequence of novelty exposure3,5.
Our observation that information transfer via inputs to basal and apical dendrites of CA1 pyramidal cells is differentially altered by novelty has implications for understanding how place fields form. Apical and basal inputs are physiologically isolated57,58, however both inputs contribute to the generation of the complex spiking in CA1 cells that codes for place in a freely moving animal59. It is important to note that apical inputs are adjacent to direct inputs from entorhinal cortex, and functional interactions between these inputs influence spiking behavior47 and plasticity in both pathways60,61. LTD in the apical Schaffer collateral input is therefore uniquely positioned to influence the likelihood that coincident inputs from the entorhinal cortex will drive cell spiking62.
We report an acute effect of plasticity blockade on the formation of place fields. Although these fields formed without apparent deficit, the dynamics of their formation were subtly altered, in that stable field firing formed more rapidly than expected. The initial absence and gradual formation of place fields in a novel environment has been well established23–26. Our results confirm these prior observations, with stable fields appearing after three to four laps of our linear maze, (i.e., the first 2–3 min). In the absence of AMPA receptor endocytosis, stability was apparent after a single lap. The regularity of established fields implies that without synaptic depression some pre-existing structure may predominate, and we suggest that LTD serves to constrain such structure. This conjecture provides a plausible explanation for the unexpected acceleration of place field formation in rats following administration of the GluA23Y peptide.
The present results are entirely consistent with prior reports of impaired maintenance of place field location after interference with synaptic plasticity20–22, raising the possibility they are related to effects on LTD, either exclusively or in combination with their effect on LTP. Importantly, we show that AMPA receptor endocytosis inhibition also impairs maintenance of novel spatial learning, suggesting that the effect of GluA23Y peptide on place field maintenance translates to impaired spatial memory. Most spatial and contextual learning paradigms are preceded by habituation to the training environment, and without such habituation, subsequent learning is severely impaired50–52,63. Our data confirm that this contextual pre-exposure facilitation effect is dependent on LTD, which we interpret as evidence that synaptic weakening is required for non-associative contextual learning. Thus although forming an association between an established context and an aversive consequence does not require LTD49, the initial formation of a contextual representation is LTD-dependent.
This work adds to an expanding corpus of evidence showing diverse roles for synaptic LTD in learning and memory64, including extinction learning49, behavioral flexibility65, time-dependent memory loss66,67 and impairment of recognition learning68,69. In the hippocampus, LTD is required for consolidation of spatial memories mediating performance in the Morris water maze70, and habituation to a novel object configuration5. These latter effects related to novelty suggest a core role for LTD in processing non-associative, highly novel information. Furthermore, exploration of a novel environment triggers behavioral and neurochemical responses that elicit LTD in the hippocampus. While it remains to be determined how this LTD impacts ongoing information processing in the hippocampus, we show here that disruption of LTD impairs consolidation of place fields, and alters their acute formation dynamics.
Methods
Animals
All experiments were approved by the University of British Columbia Animal Care Committee or the University of Calgary Animal Care Committee in accordance with the policies of the Canadian Council on Animal Care. Male Sprague-Dawley rats (Charles River) weighing 300–450 g were housed in pairs on a 12 h:12 h standard light cycle, or individually for freely moving electrophysiology experiments. Rats were provided food and water access ad libitum.
Drugs
The interference peptide Tat-GluA23Y (YGRKKRRQRRR-869YKEGYNVYG877) and a scrambled control Tat-Scrambled (YGRKKRRQRRR-VYKYGGYNE) were synthesized in house and dissolved in saline (2.25 µmol/kg) for intravenous bolus injection.
Probe construction
For evoked potential recordings, a probe containing 8 bundles of 4 electrodes (25 µm tungsten, California Fine Wire) was arranged in a 4 by 2 pattern. Within each bundle, electrode tips were cut at 300 µm spacing to span the strata of CA1, and bundles were separated by 635 µm. For CA1 spike recordings, microdrives with four to eight channels were constructed in order to independently adjust the depth of recording tetrodes. A single bundle of 30-gauge stainless steel guide tubes spaced the tetrodes by 300 µm. Tetrodes were made of four braided strands of 15 µm NiCr wire (California Fine Wire, USA) and sheathed in polyimide tubing (75 µm ID). Each actuator of the microdrive consisted of a 12.7 mm brass 00-90 hexagonal head machine screw (JI Morris, USA) coupled to a 23-gauge stainless steel drive tube. Tetrodes were threaded through the 30-gauge guide tubes and 23-gauge drive tubes, and individual wires were pinned to a 36 channel electronic interface board (EIB-36N, Neuralynx, USA). Prior to surgery, tetrode tips were precision cut and gold plated with a gold potassium cyanide solution to improve the recording of biopotentials by reducing electrical impedance and tissue reactivity.
Jugular vein catheterization surgery
Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction, 2% maintenance) and an incision made in the upper right quadrant of the thorax to expose the right jugular vein. An indwelling silastic catheter (Dow Corning Corp, USA) was inserted into the jugular vein, and the distal end was run subcutaneously to a port on the dorsal surface of the rat between the scapulae. In the case of rats undergoing electrophysiology recordings, the port was integrated into the headcap.
Intracranial cannulation
Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction, 2% maintenance) and skin excised to expose the skull. Bilateral guide cannulae (26 gauge, 5 mm spacing, Plastics One, USA) were inserted into the brain (AP −3.5 mm bregma, ML ±2.5 mm midline, VD −1.4 mm from dura). Guide cannulae were fixed to the skull with skull screws and dental cement. A microinjector (33 gauge) with a 1 mm extension was utilized to deliver drug.
Evoked potential electrophysiology surgery
Following jugular vein catheterization, anesthetized rats were placed in a stereotaxic frame, and a 3 by 2.5 mm cranial window (−1.75 to 4.75 mm AP, 47 0.5 to 3.0 mm ML) was drilled over the right dorsal hippocampus. Dura was excised to allow implant of the microwire array in the brain. Lateral to the implanted probe, two individually moveable stimulation electrodes (bipolar 50 µm stainless steel, AM systems) were implanted into the stratum radiatum and stratum oriens, respectively, using standard electrophysiological responses to confirm placement. Electrodes were fixed in place to the skull by dental cement and implanted skull screws, which also served as ground and reference points.
CA1 Pyramidal spiking electrophysiology surgery
Following jugular vein catheterization, anesthetized rats were placed in a stereotaxic frame, and a 2.0 by 2.0 mm cranial window (−2.3 to 4.3 mm AP, 1.0 to 3.0 mm ML) was drilled over the right dorsal hippocampus. Dura was excised to allow placement of the tip of the microdrive bundle above the cortex overlying the hippocampus. The microdrive was fixed to the skull by dental cement and implanted skull screws, which also served as ground and reference points. In order to record from populations of hippocampal neurons, tetrodes were lowered slowly through the overlying cortex over several weeks of daily sessions while filtered LFP activity (1–475 Hz) and high frequency unit activity (600–6000 Hz) were monitored. The hippocampal pyramidal layer was identified by the characteristic appearance of ripple activity (150–200 Hz) occurring irregularly during immobility, grooming, and sleep. Tetrodes were slowly lowered further until unit activity was observed during ripples, and subsequent tuning took place to maximize stable, separable units.
Evoked field potential recordings
Rats were acclimated to a recording chamber (40 × 40 × 60 cm) for several days after recovery from surgery. The baseline recording chamber was composed of four uniform dark walls with a removable blue corrugated plastic floor. A single white vertical strip polarized the box for orientation. Extramaze cues were available on all four walls in the recording room. Baseline evoked responses were then tested from 20 to 200 µA stimulation intensity (0.2 ms biphasic stimulation), and a stimulation magnitude evoking 50% of the maximal response was used for the remaining recordings. Baseline recordings were made for a minimum of three days (alternating oriens and radiatum test stimulations, 0.016 Hz per site), and the day prior to testing baseline responses were recorded for 1 h, with brief handling at 30 min. On the test day, drugs (saline, GluA23Y/Scrambled 2.25 µmol/kg) were administered 45 min prior to recording. Rats were randomly assigned to GluA23Y/Scrambled groups. After 30 min in the recording chamber, rats were transferred to a novel environment (60 × 60 × 60 cm polarized box). The novel environment was composed of three black walls and one white wall, with a removable black painted corrugated plastic floor. It was positioned adjacent to the baseline recording box in the same room. Extramaze cues within the room were unchanged. Rats were recorded for 30 min in the novel environment, with test stimulation continuing as before, and then transferred back to the baseline recording chamber for 30 min. On the following day, rats were re-exposed to the novel environment using the same testing protocol as above, with the exception that no drugs were administered prior to testing. Locomotor activity was tracked in 4 of 5 rats treated with Scrambled peptide and 4 of 5 rats treated with GluA23Y peptide via overhead camera using commercial behavioral tracking software (Ethovision 3.1, Noldus, USA).
CA1 Place field recording
For place field recordings in a box environment, rats were food restricted and trained to forage for sucrose pellets in a recording chamber (40 × 40 × 60 cm). Baseline recordings were collected during 15-min foraging sessions to confirm the appearance of place specific firing. On the test day, rats received IV infusions (GluA23Y/Scrambled 2.25 µmol/kg in 1 ml/kg saline) 45 min prior to recording. Rats first ran a 15-min foraging session in the familiar configuration, then were removed and placed in a novel environment (60 × 60 × 60 cm polarized box) to run a 30-min foraging session. Rats were then returned to the baseline recording chamber for an 15-min session.
For place field recordings in a linear maze, rats were food restricted and trained to shuttle for sucrose pellets delivered to both ends of a linear maze consisting of four 40 cm segments (width 12 cm, wall height 10 cm) linked by turns of 45°, 90°, and 135°. Rats were trained until a minimum of 8 laps were completed within an 8-min session. On the first exposure day, rats received IV infusions (GluA23Y/Scrambled 2.25 µmol/kg in 1 ml/kg saline) 45 min prior to recording. Rats were randomly assigned to GluA23Y/Scrambled groups. Rats first ran an 8-min shuttling session in the familiar maze configuration, then were removed and placed in a novel configuration to run a 16-min shuttling session. Rats were then returned to the baseline configuration for an 8-min recording session. On the subsequent re-exposure day, rats ran an 8-min shuttling session in the familiar maze configuration, then were removed and placed in the same, previously novel configuration to run a 16-min shuttling session. Rats were then returned to the baseline configuration for an 8-min recording session. Multiple configurations were possible by adjusting the position of the maze in space and the sequence and direction of turns from one end of the maze to the other. Five of seven rats underwent both treatment conditions, with exposure order randomized, while the two remaining animals underwent only one condition (GluA23Y/Scrambled, respectively).
Inhibitory avoidance
The inhibitory avoidance apparatus consisted of a light and dark chamber separated by a removable door. Each chamber was 35 × 30 × 35 cm, and the stainless steel flooring of the dark compartment was connected to a programmable scrambled shock generator (Colbourne Instruments, USA for systemic experiments, Harvard Apparatus for local injection experiments). Inhibitory avoidance was assessed over a three-day protocol designed to isolate the novel exposure to the recording chamber from the chamber-shock association learning71,72. On day one, rats received a contextual exposure to the chamber, in which they were placed in the light side of the box with the door open and allowed to freely explore both sides of box for 8 min. Prior to exploration, rats received bolus IV injection of saline, Scrambled peptide, or GluA23Y peptide (GluA23Y/Scrambled 2.25 µmol/kg) for systemic experiments, or bilateral microinjection of GluA23Y/Scrambled (45 pmol/µl, 2.5 µl/hemisphere) for local injection experiments 60 min prior to chamber exploration. On day 2, 24 h after contextual exposure, rats were placed directly in the dark chamber with the door closed, and received two brief footshocks 2 s after placement in the chamber (0.4 mA footshock, 0.5 s duration for systemic experiments, 0.55 mA footshock, 0.5 s duration for local injection experiments). Five seconds after termination of the footshock rats were removed from the chamber and returned to their homecage. No drugs were administered in the training session. On day 3, 24 h after training, rats were placed in the light compartment facing the far wall, and after 5 s the door to the dark chamber was opened. Latency to cross entirely into the dark compartment (all four paws within the compartment) was taken as a measure of inhibitory avoidance learning. A control group of rats was administered saline on day one and trained as above, with the exception that no contextual exposure was performed on day one. Drug administration (GluA23Y/Scrambled) or exposure condition were pseudorandomly assigned, with cagemates receiving opposing treatments. Testing on the third day was conducted blind to treatment condition.
Data acquisition and analysis
Cell spiking, local, and evoked field potentials were recorded using a 32-channel electrophysiology recording system with integrated headstage preamplifier (Digital Lynx 32, HS-36, Neuralynx, AZ). A native sampling rate of 32 kHz was broad-band filtered between 0.1 and 1000 Hz and downsampled to 6.4 kHz for analysis of field potentials. Stimulation (0.2 ms biphasic pulse) was delivered via AM 2100 stimulus isolators, controlled via computer generated TTL pulses. Data were analyzed offline using custom MATLAB analysis code. The amplitude of evoked field potentials was taken as a measure of synaptic strength. As evoked potentials within a recording tract were highly correlated and likely reflected the same population of evoked synapses, only the channel within the tract with the highest magnitude potential was used. This also avoided the analysis of complex waveforms recorded from channels intermediate between stratum oriens and stratum radiatum.
Analysis of standard deviation was adapted from previous work examining hippocampal fEPSPs recorded from multiple electrodes simultaneously27. For each channel, a standard deviation threshold was calculated from responses evoked in the baseline period. Responses exceeding this threshold in either direction were counted for each stimulation, resulting in a proportion of above and below threshold channels for each time point for each animal. Proportions were averaged across time for each epoch for analysis.
For spike recordings, positional information was recorded by tracking a colored LED mounted on the headstage preamplifier with an overhead camera (Neuralynx, AZ). Spike sorting was conducted offline using manual spike sorting software (Offline Sorter, Plexon). Putative spikes were preprocessed to remove noise and clusters were identified using 3d projections of spike characteristics. Only well-isolated clusters were selected for analysis. Entire recording sessions, whether containing a single or multiple environmental exposures were sorted collapsed over time. Sorted spike data and LFPs were analyzed using an open source MATLAB toolkit (FMAtoolbox, http://fmatoolbox.sourceforge.net/) and custom MATLAB analysis code. For each recording session, positional information was first filtered to remove target points occurring outside the arena. Tracking jitters were removed by setting a threshold movement speed. Missing points were interpolated from nearby samples and all position samples were smoothed over a ~2 s Gaussian window. Spikes were velocity filtered to remove firing during immobility, defined as movement below 2.5 cm/s over a smoothed 5-second window. For linear track data, two dimensional position samples were linearized to one dimension by collapsing along a single axis defined by a set of vertices corresponding to the end points and three interior corners of the linear maze. Laps were then identified using local extrema on a 25 s smoothed plot of the linearized data. Incomplete laps were removed and subsequent analysis separated inward and outward going laps. Spatial firing rate maps were generated for each isolated unit using a grid of 50 × 50 bins for 2 dimensional box data, while 1 dimensional linearized data used 50 bins.
Cells were identified across days based on previous methods41,42. For each cell on each day, the average waveform was calculated, and highly correlated waveforms (r > 0.97, taken as a 128 × 1 array of voltage samples, with 32 samples for each tetrode sequentially) recorded from the same tetrode across days in the same rat were taken as a training set of putative matched pairs. Two measures of waveform shape similarity (tolias distance) were calculated for each pair of cells from the same rat, with waveforms expressed as a 32 × 4 array of voltage samples, each column representing each channel of the tetrode. The first (D1) was a measure of shape similarity of waveforms normalized to minimize the sum of squares difference between the two waveforms on each channel. These scaling factors were used to calculate a normalized Euclidean distance between the waveforms on each channel, summing across channels to produce a single D1 value. A separate D2 value, meant to capture both differences between waveform sizes overall and differences between the scaling factor on each channel was also calculated. Plotted on two dimensions, the D1 and D2 values are both small for matching cell pairs, and larger for non-matching pairs. Using the training set, a Bayesian classifier was used to classify all cell pairs as matching or non-matching. A small minority of cells were positively classified to multiple cell on other days, in this case a linearized tolias distance was used to select only the most closely matched cell pair. Firing rate maps for pairs of cells were correlated as two linear arrays using Pearson’s product-moment correlation. The second exposure to the familiar environment in each session was used to compare familiar environmental representations across days.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) to Y.T.W. (FND-154286). Y.T.W. is the holder of the Heart and Stroke Foundation of British Columbia and Yukon Chair in Stroke Research. D.M.A. was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Canada Graduate Scholarship (CGS).
Source data
Author contributions
D.M.A. and Y.T.W. conceived of the study, D.M.A. conducted the research and analyzed the data. S.B.F., A.G.P., J.K.S., and Y.T.W. supervised the project. D.M.A., A.G.P., and Y.T.W. wrote the manuscript. D.M.A., A.M. A.G.P., J.K.S., and Y.T.W. edited the manuscript. A.M. and Y.T.W. provided materials and reagents.
Data availability
All data supporting the findings of this study are provided within the paper and its Supplementary information. All additional information will be made available upon reasonable request to the authors. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
The code that support the findings of this study are available at https://github.com/donovanashby/ltd-pc and used under GNU General Public License (GPL).
Competing interests
Y.T.W. and A.G.P. declare a patent related to glutamate receptor function (A Peptide that Specifically Blocks Regulated AMPA Receptor Endocytosis and Hippocampal CA1 Long-term Depression; European 04789721.0, and United States 13/066,700). A.G.P. also holds a US patent entitled “Tetrahydroprotoberbine Compounds and Uses Thereof” in the Treatment of Neurological, Psychiatric and Neurodegenerative Diseases. United States. US20150306092. A.G.P. is a shareholder in Resilience Biosciences Inc., Canada, focused on tetrahydroprotoberberine drug development. D.M.A., S.B.F., A.M., and J.K.S. declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41467-020-20317-7.
References
- 1.Morris RGM. Synaptic plasticity and learning: selective impairment of learning rats and blockade of long-term potentiation in vivo by the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist AP5. J. Neurosci. 1989;9:3040–3057. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-09-03040.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Morris RGM, Anderson EA, Lynch GS, Baudry M. Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5. Nature. 1986;319:774. doi: 10.1038/319774a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Manahan-Vaughan D, Braunewell K-H. Novelty acquisition is associated with induction of hippocampal long-term depression. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:8739–8744. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.15.8739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kemp A, Manahan-Vaughan D. Hippocampal long-term depression and long-term potentiation encode different aspects of novelty acquisition. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2004;101:8192–8197. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402650101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dong Z, et al. Mechanisms of hippocampal long-term depression are required for memory enhancement by novelty exploration. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:11980–11990. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0984-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Li S, Cullen WK, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Dopamine-dependent facilitation of LTP induction in hippocampal CA1 by exposure to spatial novelty. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:526–531. doi: 10.1038/nn1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bliss TVP, et al. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361:31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Malinow R, Malenka RC. AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;25:103–126. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Malenka RC, Nicoll RA. Long-term potentiation-a decade of progress? Science. 1999;285:1870–1874. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5435.1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Collingridge GL, Isaac JTR, Wang YT. Receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;5:952–962. doi: 10.1038/nrn1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.O’Keefe J, Speakman A. Single unit activity in the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory task. Exp. Brain Res. 1987;68:1–27. doi: 10.1007/BF00255230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971;34:171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Buzsaki G, Moser EI. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:130–138. doi: 10.1038/nn.3304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lever C, Wills T, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O’keefe J. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature. 2002;416:90. doi: 10.1038/416090a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Cacucci F, Wills TJ, Lever C, Giese KP, O’Keefe J. Experience-dependent increase in CA1 place cell spatial information, but not spatial reproducibility, is dependent on the autophosphorylation of the $α$-isoform of the calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:7854–7859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1704-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ziv Y, et al. Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:264. doi: 10.1038/nn.3329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Thompson LT, Best PJ. Long-term stability of the place-field activity of single units recorded from the dorsal hippocampus of freely behaving rats. Brain Res. 1990;509:299–308. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90555-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Markus EJ, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL, Gladden VL, Skaggs WE. Spatial information content and reliability of hippocampal CA1 neurons: effects of visual input. Hippocampus. 1994;4:410–421. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lenck-Santini P-P, Save E, Poucet B. Evidence for a relationship between place-cell spatial firing and spatial memory performance. Hippocampus. 2001;11:377–390. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kentros C, et al. Abolition of long-term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Science. 1998;280:2121–2126. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5372.2121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rotenberg A, Abel T, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Muller RU. Parallel instabilities of long-term potentiation, place cells, and learning caused by decreased protein kinase A activity. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:8096–8102. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-21-08096.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Agnihotri NT, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Kentros C. The long-term stability of new hippocampal place fields requires new protein synthesis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2004;101:3656–3661. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400385101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science. 1993;261:1055–1058. doi: 10.1126/science.8351520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tanila H, Shapiro M, Gallagher M, Eichenbaum H. Brain aging: changes in the nature of information coding by the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:5155–5166. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-13-05155.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Frank LM, Stanley GB, Brown EN. Hippocampal plasticity across multiple days of exposure to novel environments. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:7681–7689. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1958-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Monaco JD, Rao G, Roth ED, Knierim JJ. Attentive scanning behavior drives one-trial potentiation of hippocampal place fields. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:725. doi: 10.1038/nn.3687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF. Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science. 2006;313:1093–1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hargreaves EL, Cain DP, Vanderwolf CH. Learning and behavioral-long-term potentiation: importance of controlling for motor activity. J. Neurosci. 1990;10:1472–1478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-05-01472.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Moser EI. Learning-related changes in hippocampal field potentials. Behav. Brain Res. 1995;71:1–N1. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Leung LS, Shen B. Long-term potentiation at the apical and basal dendritic synapses of CA1 after local stimulation in behaving rats. J. Neurophysiol. 1995;73:1938–1946. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.5.1938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ahmadian G, et al. Tyrosine phosphorylation of GluR2 is required for insulin-stimulated AMPA receptor endocytosis and LTD. EMBO J. 2004;23:1040–1050. doi: 10.1038/sj.emboj.7600126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Brebner K, et al. Nucleus accumbens long-term depression and the expression of behavioral sensitization. Science. 2005;310:1340–1343. doi: 10.1126/science.1116894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Moser EI, Moser M-B, Andersen P. Potentiation of dentate synapses initiated by exploratory learning in rats: dissociation from brain temperature, motor activity, and arousal. Learn. Mem. 1994;1:55–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Moser E, Mathiesen I, Andersen P. Association between brain temperature and dentate field potentials in exploring and swimming rats. Science. 1993;259:1324–1326. doi: 10.1126/science.8446900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Abbott LF, Varela JA, Sen K, Nelson SB. Synaptic depression and cortical gain control. Science. 1997;275:221–224. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5297.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rotenberg A, Mayford M, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Muller RU. Mice expressing activated CaMKII lack low frequency LTP and do not form stable place cells in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Cell. 1996;87:1351–1361. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81829-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cho YH, Giese KP, Tanila H, Silva AJ, Eichenbaum H. Abnormal hippocampal spatial representations in αCaMKIIT286A and CREBαΔ-mice. Science. 1998;279:867–869. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5352.867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Agnihotri, N. T., Hawkins, R. D., Kandel, E. R., & Kentros, C. The long-term stability of new hippocampal place fields requires new protein synthesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101, 10, 3656-3661. (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 39.Morris RGM, et al. Elements of a neurobiological theory of the hippocampus: the role of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2003;358:773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Martin SJ, Grimwood PD, Morris RGM. Synaptic plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2000;23:649–711. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Powell NJ, Redish AD. Complex neural codes in rat prelimbic cortex are stable across days on a spatial decision task. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8:120. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tolias AS, et al. Recording chronically from the same neurons in awake, behaving primates. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3780–3790. doi: 10.1152/jn.00260.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tanila H, Perttu S, Shapiro M, Eichenbaum H. Brain aging: impaired coding of novel environmental cues. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:5167–5174. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-13-05167.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW. Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature. 2009;461:941. doi: 10.1038/nature08499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Epsztein J’er. Brecht, M. & Lee, A. K. Intracellular determinants of hippocampal CA1 place and silent cell activity in a novel environment. Neuron. 2011;70:109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lee D, Lin B-J, Lee AK. Hippocampal place fields emerge upon single-cell manipulation of excitability during behavior. Science. 2012;337:849–853. doi: 10.1126/science.1221489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bittner KC, et al. Conjunctive input processing drives feature selectivity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1133. doi: 10.1038/nn.4062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bittner KC, Milstein AD, Grienberger C, Romani S, Magee JC. Behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity underlies CA1 place fields. Science. 2017;357:1033–1036. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dalton GL, Wang YT, Floresco SB, Phillips AG. Disruption of AMPA receptor endocytosis impairs the extinction, but not acquisition of learned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2416. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rudy JW, Huff NC, Matus-Amat P. Understanding contextual fear conditioning: insights from a two-process model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2004;28:675–685. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Roesler R, et al. Intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA receptor antagonist AP5 impairs retention of an inhibitory avoidance task: protection from impairment by pretraining or preexposure to the task apparatus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1998;69:87–91. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Roesler R, et al. Differential involvement of hippocampal and amygdalar NMDA receptors in contextual and aversive aspects of inhibitory avoidance memory in rats. Brain Res. 2003;975:207–213. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)02656-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Leung LS. Behavior-dependent evoked potentials in the hippocampal CA1 region of the rat. I. Correlation with behavior and egg. Brain Res. 1980;198:95–117. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90347-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hagena H, Manahan-Vaughan D. Learning-facilitated long-term depression and long-term potentiation at mossy fiber—CA3 synapses requires activation of β-adrenergic receptors. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2012;6:23. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kemp A, Manahan-Vaughan D. Hippocampal long-term depression: master or minion in declarative memory processes? Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cooper LN, Bear MF. The BCM theory of synapse modification at 30: interaction of theory with experiment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:798. doi: 10.1038/nrn3353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sajikumar S, Navakkode S, Frey JU. Identification of compartment-and process-specific molecules required for “synaptic tagging” during long-term potentiation and long-term depression in hippocampal CA1. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:5068–5080. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4940-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Spruston N. Pyramidal neurons: dendritic structure and synaptic integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:206. doi: 10.1038/nrn2286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Grienberger C, Chen X, Konnerth A. NMDA receptor-dependent multidendrite Ca2+ spikes required for hippocampal burst firing in vivo. Neuron. 2014;81:1274–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Dudman JT, Tsay D, Siegelbaum SA. A role for synaptic inputs at distal dendrites: instructive signals for hippocampal long-term plasticity. Neuron. 2007;56:866–879. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Takahashi H, Magee JC. Pathway interactions and synaptic plasticity in the dendritic tuft regions of CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 2009;62:102–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jarsky T, Roxin A, Kath WL, Spruston N. Conditional dendritic spike propagation following distal synaptic activation of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:1667. doi: 10.1038/nn1599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Fanselow MS. Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: implications for the response selection rules governing species-specific defensive reactions. Learn. Motiv. 1986;17:16–39. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(86)90018-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Collingridge GL, Peineau S, Howland JG, Wang YT. Long-term depression in the CNS. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:459. doi: 10.1038/nrn2867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kim J-I, et al. PI3Kγ is required for NMDA receptor-dependent long-term depression and behavioral flexibility. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1447–1454. doi: 10.1038/nn.2937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Migues PV, et al. Blocking synaptic removal of GluA2-containing AMPA receptors prevents the natural forgetting of long-term memories. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:3481–3494. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3333-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hardt O, Nader K, Wang Y-T. GluA2-dependent AMPA receptor endocytosis and the decay of early and late long-term potentiation: possible mechanisms for forgetting of short-and long-term memories. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2014;369:20130141. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Griffiths S, et al. Expression of long-term depression underlies visual recognition memory. Neuron. 2008;58:186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Cazakoff BN, Howland JG. AMPA receptor endocytosis in rat perirhinal cortex underlies retrieval of object memory. Learn. Mem. 2011;18:688–692. doi: 10.1101/lm.2312711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Ge Y, et al. Hippocampal long-term depression is required for the consolidation of spatial memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:16697–16702. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008200107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Liang, K. C. Pre‐or post‐training injection of buspirone impaired retention in the inhibitory avoidance task: involvement of amygdala 5‐HT1A receptors. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 5, 1491–1500 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 72.Malin, E. L., & McGaugh, J. L. Differential involvement of the hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, and basolateral amygdala in memory for context and footshock. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 6, 1959–1963 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data supporting the findings of this study are provided within the paper and its Supplementary information. All additional information will be made available upon reasonable request to the authors. Source data are provided with this paper.
The code that support the findings of this study are available at https://github.com/donovanashby/ltd-pc and used under GNU General Public License (GPL).