Abstract
Emotion processing—including signals from facial expressions—is often altered in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The biological basis of this is poorly understood but may include neurochemically mediated differences in the responsivity of key ‘limbic’ regions (including amygdala, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and nucleus accumbens (NAc)). Emerging evidence also suggests that ASD may be a disorder of brain temporal dynamics. Moreover, serotonin (5-HT) has been shown to be a key regulator of both facial-emotion processing and brain dynamics, and 5-HT abnormalities have been consistently implicated in ASD. To date, however, no one has examined how 5-HT influences the dynamics of facial-emotion processing in ASD. Therefore, we compared the influence of 5-HT on the responsivity of brain dynamics during facial-emotion processing in individuals with and without ASD. Participants completed a facial-emotion processing fMRI task at least 8 days apart using a randomised double-blind crossover design. At each visit they received either a single 20-mg oral dose of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) citalopram or placebo. We found that citalopram (which increases levels of 5-HT) caused sustained activation in key limbic regions during processing of negative facial emotions in adults with ASD—but not in neurotypical adults. The neurotypical adults’ limbic response reverted more rapidly to baseline following a 5-HT-challenge. Our results suggest that serotonergic homoeostatic control of the temporal dynamics in limbic regions is altered in adults with ASD, and provide a fresh perspective on the biology of ASD.
Subject terms: Limbic system, Neurochemistry
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a worldwide prevalence rate of ~1% [1]. It is characterised by restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviour, impaired social interaction and communication, and atypical sensitivity to sensory stimuli [2]. Specifically, the impairment in social interaction is thought to be at least partly underpinned by altered processing of facial emotions [3].
Key components of the limbic system implicated in human social behaviour and emotion processing include the amygdala, the nucleus accumbens (NAc), and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [4]. For instance, in the neurotypical population, the amygdala has been robustly implicated in the processing of facial emotions and is closely linked to processing of fear, anger [5] and other emotional perceptions [6, 7] experienced explicitly or passively [8]. In contrast, the NAc—through its dense interconnections with amygdala [9]—facilitates motor behaviours driven by emotionally salient stimuli [10]. Furthermore, there is consensus that vmPFC connections to the amygdala and NAc have a regulatory role in emotion and social cognition [11]. Alterations in the function of these major limbic regions have been reported in ASD, however, findings have been inconsistent. For example, prior studies have described hypo- and hyper-activity (or even no change in activation) during facial-emotion processing within amygdala [12–18], striatum [13, 15] and vmPFC [14, 19]. Some of the inconsistency in findings is likely to be caused by differences between the experimental methods used and the sample characteristics (e.g., age).
In addition, however, previous work has mostly examined group differences in average brain activation across a fixed time period—and this may not optimally capture brain activity. The brain is in constant flux [20, 21] and so it may be better to capture fluctuations in brain activity over time. This may be especially important here as there is emerging evidence that the temporal dynamics of brain activity are different in ASD. For instance, during resting-state fMRI, overly ‘stable’ brain dynamics have been reported in individuals with ASD as compared with neurotypical individuals [22]. Therefore, the analysis of average activations across the entire time period of the fMRI contrast of interest may mask crucial group differences in temporal dynamics. Support for this hypothesis comes from preliminary studies of amygdala ‘habituation’ during fMRI tasks (defined as the progressive decrease in response to repeatedly presented stimuli). Habituation of amygdala activation in response to facial emotions has been reported to be altered in ASD [23] relative to neurotypical controls [24] during both explicit [25] and implicit [26, 27] fMRI paradigms. Yet, the biological basis of the habituation phenomenon in ASD is still largely unknown.
The regulation of brain temporal dynamics including habituation is likely to involve co-ordinated action of multiple neuro-signalling systems [28, 29]. Among these, the serotonin (5-HT) system is likely to be especially important. For instance, infusion of psilocybin, which has potent psycho-active effects at the 5-HT2A receptor, has been reported to impact on frontoparietal network dynamics in neurotypical individuals [30]. Stimulation of 5-HT2A receptors could increase the excitability of the pyramidal neurons where the receptors are highly expressed and subsequently modulate the widespread neural systems [31]. Moreover, differences in 5-HT pathways are some of the most consistent findings in ASD [32]. Elevated whole-blood serotonin has been recorded in around a third of children and adults with ASD [33, 34], and ASD has been strongly linked to 5-HT genetic polymorphisms and candidate genes within 5-HT pathways [35–37]. Also, there is preliminary evidence for differences in 5-HT2A receptor density in the ‘social brain’ (including anterior cingulate cortex) of adults with ASD [38]. Furthermore, using a tryptophan depletion protocol, we have previously found that lowering 5-HT levels has opposing effects on brain activation during processing of facial emotions in individuals with and without ASD [17]. In summary, 5-HT is a key regulator of facial-emotion processing and brain temporal dynamics [30, 39–42], which are both altered in ASD [3, 32, 33, 43]. However, nobody has directly examined how 5-HT influences the dynamics of facial-emotion processing in ASD.
Therefore, in this study we tested the hypothesis that the effects of 5-HT on the responsivity of brain dynamics of facial-emotion processing are different in individuals with and without ASD. Different aspects of brain dynamics, including the time-varying connectivity and time-frequency coherence [44], have been studied in ASD [22, 45, 46]. Here we were interested in the brain dynamics as measured by fMRI activations during processing of repeating facial-emotional stimuli [25–27, 47]. We conducted a single oral-dose pharmacological-fMRI study, and examined how the habituation pattern during baseline (i.e., placebo) would be changed by citalopram, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), administered in a double-blind, randomised order during a facial-emotion processing task. We focussed on the major limbic regions of interest (ROIs), namely the amygdala, vmPFC and NAc.
Materials and methods
Participants
We included 40 right-handed adult males (19 with ASD and 21 neurotypical controls, 18–60 years old), which the sample size is comparable to the previous fMRI study [17]. Participants with ASD were recruited through the National Adult Autism Service at the Maudsley Hospital and had a clinical diagnosis of ASD made by the multidisciplinary specialist team following ICD-10 research diagnostic criteria [48] and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [49] were completed for ASD participants where possible. For individuals with and without ASD, we only included individuals with no history of major medical disorders that could influence cognitive performance, other major mental illnesses, genetic disorders associated with ASD, alcohol or substance dependence, and who were not taking any medication affecting the 5-HT system. All participants underwent the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence test [50] to ensure that we only included those with an IQ of >70. Participants’ behaviours including social traits related to autism, namely ‘autistic traits’, were measured by autism-spectrum quotient [51]. Participants’ anxiety and depression were measured by Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAM-A) [52] and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) [53], respectively. All participants gave written, informed consent after receiving a complete description of the study. The study was ethically approved by the Stanmore NHS Research Ethics Committee (reference: 14/LO/0663).
Drug administration and study procedures
This study was conducted at the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience at De Crespigny Park, SE5 8AF, London, UK (December 2014 to December 2016). It adopted a placebo-controlled, randomised, double-blind, repeated-measures, crossover case-control study design as part of a larger investigation into the brain response to 5-HT medications in ASD (clinicaltrials.gov identifier: NCT04145076). Placebo and citalopram were allocated in a pseudo-randomised order, approximately half in each group attended a placebo visit before citalopram and the other half attended a citalopram visit before placebo. This random allocation was administered by GMM (https://www.random.org/) and both participants and researchers were blinded to the allocation. Participants were asked to complete two scanning visits, separated by at least 8 days to allow for complete washout of the drug. Participants were given either an acute single dose of encapsulated citalopram (20 mg), or encapsulated placebo (ascorbic acid) 3 h before each scanning session, as citalopram reaches its peak plasma level after ~3 h [54]. All the scanning sessions were completed within 1.5 h to ensure that citalopram did not reach half-life during scanning [55]. All participants received pre- and post-scan medical screening by a physician after administration of both drugs (placebo and citalopram).
Facial-emotion processing fMRI task
An adapted version of a face-matching task [5] was administered to participants during each scanning session. The task consisted of four blocks of faces and four blocks of geometric shapes, with six 5-s trials per block (Fig. 1). In each trial, participants were asked to indicate, by pressing buttons on a response box with their right hand, which of the two images in the lower panel were identical to the target image in the upper panel. Each trial was presented twice. Half of the faces were ‘angry’ and the other half were ‘fearful’. All faces were balanced for sex.
MRI data acquisition and preprocessing
The fMRI data were acquired on a 3T General Electric Signa HD × Twinspeed scanner (Milwaukee, Wisc.) fitted with a quadrature birdcage head coil: TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 192 × 192 mm, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 5 mm, flip angle = 80°, number of time points = 135. T1-weighted structural data was acquired sagittally: TR = 7.312 ms, TE = 3.016 ms, FOV = 270 × 270 mm, matrix size = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1.2 mm, flip angle = 11°.
Each participant’s fMRI images were first corrected for slice-timing and head movement with FSL [56, 57]. Volumes in each subject that had framewise displacement (FD) >0.5 mm [57] were identified using fsl_motion_outliers and were subsequently regressed out in the first-level general linear model (GLM) in FSL [56, 57]. A temporal high-pass filter at 128 s was applied and the fMRI data were smoothed by 8 mm full width at half-maximum kernel. One ASD participant had more than 30% volumes with FD > 0.5 mm and was removed from further analyses, giving us a final sample of 39 individuals.
ROI selection
We specifically focused on the amygdala, vmPFC and NAc based on hypotheses derived from previous work [4]. The amygdala ROIs were derived from FSL’s Juelich histological atlas [58] thresholding at 50%, and each voxel was assigned to one ROI, consistent to previous studies [59]. Our amygdala ROIs included the centromedial amygdala, basolateral amygdala (BLA) and the superficial amygdala (SFA). The vmPFC ROIs were derived from the vmPFC atlas of asymmetric and probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps [60], and included the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC), the rostral ACC (rACC), the ventral ACC (vACC) and the anterior vmPFC (avmPFC) [59] (Fig. 2). The NAc ROIs were defined according to FSL’s Harvard-Oxford subcortical structural atlas [61].
Habituation of functional activation to negative facial emotions
To investigate the habituation of neural response to facial emotions, the first-level GLM analysis for each subject was conducted at voxel-level across the whole brain with one regressor per block of faces or shapes in addition to the six motion regressors derived from the correction procedure of motion artefact (i.e., 14 regressors in total). The block regressors modelled the onset and duration of each block and were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Each block was contrasted to the mean of all remaining blocks to scale the output block-wise beta to the overall mean [23]. The output beta maps were then normalised to standard space through registering to their skull-stripped structural data. All the fMRI images were re-sampled to a voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm. The average block-wise beta of Faces>Shapes within each ROI in each subject for their two drug conditions were then extracted (Fig. 3a). As suggested by previous studies, an absolute habituation index is a reliable way to characterise the decrease in fMRI activation across blocks correcting for the initial activation [23]. This was obtained by regressing the block-wise beta on the logarithm of block numbers. Based on the regression,
the average beta of an ROI of each block in each participant (Y) was set up to be predicted by the logarithm of block numbers (X) (i.e., ln(1) = 0, ln(2) = 0.69, ln(3) = 1.10, ln(4) = 1.39). The logarithm of block numbers was adopted because habituation might not have a linear profile and logarithm transform has been reported to allow a better model fit [23, 62]. This also allows comparison with previous studies that used the same approach [23, 27]. As (b) (the change of activation of the ROI across blocks) was dependent on (a) (the estimated initial activation), an absolute habituation index of the ROI (b′) was further obtained,
by accounting for the slope (c) of (b) on (a) and the mean (ā) of (a).
Statistical analyses
All the analyses of demographics, autism traits, anxiety & depression levels and behavioural task performances were performed with t-tests and mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) using t.test and ezANOVA respectively in R (https://www.r-project.org). Significance was inferred when p < 0.05.
For the task-fMRI data, exploratory whole-brain voxel-wise analysis on the functional activations investigating group × drug interaction effects in the contrast Block 1Faces > Shapes > Block 4Faces > Shapes (i.e., difference in Faces > Shapes functional activations between block 1 and block 4) were performed to explore whether any differences in the habituation profile observed were a generalised ‘whole-brain’ phenomenon. Please refer to Supplementary Information for details and results.
We focused our investigation on the functional activation within our hypothesised ROIs. Group × drug interaction effects on the absolute habituation indices for the contrast Faces > Shapes were compared between individuals with and without ASD in the placebo and citalopram conditions, tested by linear mixed-effect models using lmer in R (https://www.r-project.org). We investigated whether the absolute habituation indices were modulated by group and drug conditions while accounting for the random intercept for each participant in the linear mixed-effect models fitted by maximum likelihood for model comparisons. As laterality was not the focus of the current study, hemisphere (left vs. right), in addition to age, IQ, anxiety and depression, were included as covariates in all the linear mixed-effect models. P values for effects were estimated by likelihood ratio tests between models and were corrected (pcorrected) for the eight subregions. Post-hoc pairwise tests corrected for multiple comparisons were performed. Significance was inferred when corrected p values < 0.05 and potential confounding effects of any differences in covariate measures were examined using regression.
We also collapsed the block-wise beta estimates across blocks into average beta estimates within each ROI and findings on the average beta estimates are reported in Supplementary Fig. 1.
Results
Demographics and clinical characteristics
There were no significant differences in age (t36 = 0.737, p = 0.466) and IQ (t36 = 0.774, p = 0.444) between individuals with ASD and neurotypical controls. As expected, individuals with ASD scored significantly higher in autism traits (t28.2 = 5.845, p < 0.001), anxiety (t28.5 = 2.863, p = 0.008) and depression levels (t28.4 = 3.281, p = 0.003) (Table 1). Only one individual with ASD and one control had anxiety scores ≥17, the clinical threshold of HAM-A and HAM-D. Both control and ASD groups had significantly lower mean scores than the clinical threshold of HAM-A and HAM-D, as revealed by one-sample t-tests (t ≥ 5.803, p < 0.001).
Table 1.
ASD | Controls | T | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 18) | (n = 21) | |||
Demographics | ||||
Age (years) | 29 (10) | 27 (9) | 0.737 | 0.466 |
Intelligence quotient | 111 (17) | 115 (10) | 0.744 | 0.444 |
Clinical characteristics | ||||
AQ | 31 (11) | 13 (7) | 5.845 | <0.001 |
HAM-A | 8 (7) | 3 (4) | 2.863 | 0.008 |
HAM-D | 7 (5) | 2 (3) | 3.281 | 0.003 |
ADOS-C | 3 (2) | – | ||
ADOS-S | 6 (2) | – |
The means are presented with standard deviations in parenthesis.
HAM-A Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; HAM-D Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, ADOS-C Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Communication domain, ADOS-S Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Reciprocal Social Interaction domain, AQ autism-spectrum quotient.
Task behavioural performance
A group × drug × stimuli factorial design was used to investigate the accuracy of the individuals’ responses during the task for the two drug conditions (placebo and citalopram), and no interaction effects were identified (p > 0.05). We only found a significant main effect of stimuli type—with individuals having higher accuracy in matching faces than shapes regardless of group (F1,35 = 45.893, p < 0.001) (Table 2). Similarly, no interaction effects were identified in the individuals’ response reaction time during the task (p > 0.05). A significant main effect of stimuli type was observed—with individuals matching faces slower than shapes regardless of group (F1,35 = 31.061, p < 0.001). Moreover, a significant main effect of group was observed where individuals with ASD responded more slowly than controls across all trials (F1,35 = 4.698, p = 0.037) (Table 2).
Table 2.
Stimuli | Drug | Group | Mean (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Accuracy (%) | ||||
Shapes | Placebo | Controls | 93.54 (6.550) | |
ASD | 94.85 (5.419) | |||
Citaloparm | Controls | 94.79 (5.036) | ||
ASD | 94.85 (4.780) | |||
Faces | Placebo | Controls | 97.92 (5.815) | |
ASD | 98.53 (4.151) | |||
Citaloparm | Controls | 98.96 (2.985) | ||
ASD | 99.51 (1.384) | |||
FStimuli | 45.893*** | |||
Fdrug | 1.377 | |||
Fgroup | 0.298 | |||
Fstimuli × drug | 0.123 | |||
Fstimuli × group | 0.007 | |||
Fdrug × group | 0.221 | |||
Fstimuli × drug × group | 0.291 | |||
Reaction time (s) | ||||
Shapes | Placebo | Controls | 0.9835 (0.19788) | |
ASD | 1.1374 (0.27225) | |||
Citaloparm | Controls | 0.9945 (0.23690) | ||
ASD | 1.0865 (0.27116) | |||
Faces | Placebo | Controls | 1.0358 (0.20316) | |
ASD | 1.2636 (0.25622) | |||
Citaloparm | Controls | 1.0803 (0.22423) | ||
ASD | 1.2636 (0.25622) | |||
Fstimuli | 31.061*** | |||
Fdrug | 0.048 | |||
Fgroup | 4.698* | |||
Fstimuli × drug | 1.831 | |||
Fstimuli × group | 3.539 | |||
Fdrug × group | 2.115 | |||
Fstimuli × drug × group | 0.027 |
The means, standard deviations (SD) and the statistics are presented.
*p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001.
Group differences in habituation in ROIs
Using one-sample t-test, we found that there was habituation to negative facial emotions in all the ROIs for both controls and individuals with ASD in the placebo condition (pcorrected < 0.05). However, only controls exhibited habituation in the citalopram condition (pcorrected < 0.05). Habituation to negative facial emotions was not evident in any ROI for individuals with ASD in the citalopram condition (pcorrected ≥ 0.657). Using the mean absolute habituation indices of controls in each condition (baseline and citalopram) as references, the number of autistic adults having higher or lower habituation at baseline (placebo) and post citalopram are also reported (Table 3; Supplementary Fig. 2).
Table 3.
Placebo | Citalopram | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Higher habituation | Lower habituation | Higher habituation | Lower habituation | |
n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | |
BLA | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 15 (83%) | 3 (17%) |
CMA | 10 (56%) | 8 (44%) | 16 (89%) | 2 (11%) |
SFA | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 16 (89%) | 2 (11%) |
NAc | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 14 (78%) | 4 (22%) |
sgACC | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 15 (83%) | 3 (17%) |
vACC | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 17 (94%) | 1 (6%) |
rACC | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 17 (94%) | 1 (6%) |
avmPFC | 11 (61%) | 7 (39%) | 17 (94%) | 1 (6%) |
BLA basolateral amygdala, CMA centromedial amygdala, SFA superficial amygdala, NAc nucleus accumbens, sgACC subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, vACC ventral ACC, rACC rostral ACC, avmPFC anterior vmPFC.
We also explored whether there were associations between habituation in the ROIs and the task behavioural performance in terms of the difference in accuracy and reaction time between matching faces and shapes, and found no significant associations (p ≥ 0.066).
To elucidate whether there was a significant difference between the responsivity to citalopram in individuals with and without ASD, we investigated whether group × drug interaction effects could be observed in our ROIs during habituation. All ROIs revealed significant interaction effects (b ≥ 31.285, χ2 ≥ 27.843, pcorrected < 0.001) (Fig. 3b). Post-hoc analyses revealed no group differences in placebo condition in any ROI (p > 0.05). On the contrary, significantly lower overall habituation (i.e., less negative indices) in ASD compared with controls was evident in the citalopram condition in avmPFC (b = 77.857, χ2 = 7.291, pcorrected = 0.042) and vACC (b = 84.339, χ2 = 7.064, pcorrected = 0.047). No differences between habituation in placebo and citalopram conditions were observed in controls (p > 0.05). Compared with the placebo condition, lower habituation (i.e., less negative indices) to negative facial emotions after citalopram intake was only detected in individuals with ASD in all ROIs (b ≥ 36.279, χ2 ≥ 90.001, pcorrected < 0.001).
Associations between covariates and 5-HT induced habituation change
Autism traits, anxiety and depression scores were not associated with the change in habituation in any ROI across groups (b ≤ 4.028, χ2 ≤ 1.285, p ≥ 0.257). Also, no significant interaction effects between autism traits, anxiety and depression scores and groups were observed in any ROI (b ≥ −10.783, χ2 ≤ 2.314, p ≥ 0.128). Furthermore, the ADOS subscores (communication and social interaction) were not associated with the change in habituation in any ROI in individuals with ASD (b ≤ 2.145, χ2 ≤ 2.286, p ≥ 0.131).
Discussion
We demonstrated (using a randomised double-blind crossover pharmacological-fMRI design) that the habituation of fMRI activation in key limbic regions during the processing of negative facial emotions was altered by 5-HT reuptake inhibition—but only in adults with ASD. In essence, in neurotypical adults, limbic response to emotional faces response reverted to baseline within 3 h following a 5-HT challenge but not in ASD. We suggest that this indicates altered homoeostatic control of limbic systems by 5-HT in ASD.
Previous fMRI investigations of amygdala habituation in response to sad [25], fearful [26] and neutral faces [25, 47] have generally reported that habituation (without pharmacological challenge) is reduced in ASD. Here we only observed a trend towards lower habituation in the key limbic regions to the emotional faces in ASD compared with controls at baseline (i.e., placebo condition), possibly due to differences in sample characteristics and the task adopted. For example, some previous studies investigated children and adolescents [25], whereas we recruited adults [63]. Previous pharmacological studies have found children are less likely to tolerate and/or respond to SSRIs with increased risk of adverse effects [64, 65], suggesting that there might be significant differences in brain 5-HT function in pre- and post-pubertal individuals with ASD [66]. We also used an adapted face-matching paradigm presenting a mix of angry and fearful faces [5] whereas previous studies used paradigms with fearful faces only [26], angry faces only [27] or included sad faces [25]. Thus, the studies differed in some key methodological aspects. Furthermore, we examined habituation across multiple blocks in the adapted face-matching paradigm, whereas previous studies were comparing the change in activations between two runs of their paradigms over a longer duration [25, 26]. Therefore, the habituation patterns captured in our study are broadly equivalent to the initial run of previous studies. Thus, the longer time-scale of previous studies precludes direct comparison of our approaches. It should also be emphasised that, due to the nature of the null hypothesis testing, the interpretation of our insignificant baseline differences should be considered with caution.
In our study, we found that citalopram (which increases levels of 5-HT) caused sustained activation in key limbic regions during processing of negative facial emotions in adults with ASD—but not in neurotypical adults. Although, the adults with ASD have higher anxiety and depression levels than the neurotypical adults in our study sample, the scores were significantly lower than the clinical threshold that a neurotypical individual with anxiety or depressive disorder would have. Nevertheless, given the group difference we also controlled for anxiety and depression scores in our statistical analyses to minimise any impact on our results and their interpretation. We did not observe effects of citalopram on task accuracy or response reaction time. We also did not observe any associations between task accuracy or response reaction time and habituation. Furthermore, we did not observe any associations between anxiety and depression scores and the effects of citalopram on habituation. Therefore, the effects of citalopram in ASD that we detected were not a consequence of more general drug effects on performance and/or presence of common co-occurring conditions. 5-HT has been strongly implicated in emotional behaviours [67] and manipulation of brain 5-HT levels has been shown to alter the processing and recognition of facial emotions [39–42]. Consequently, when abnormalities in the 5-HT system in individuals with ASD were identified [32, 33, 43] this was considered to be consistent with social behavioural difficulties. For example, reducing brain 5-HT using a tryptophan depletion paradigm ‘restored’ an fMRI pattern of case-control differences in ASD during facial-emotion processing to the baseline activation levels observed in neurotypical controls. This was a BOLD level response and we cannot be certain of the cellular mechanisms underlying it. It does however suggest that the brain activations generating the BOLD response in adults with and without ASD responded differently to the availability of 5-HT [17]. The present work takes this evidence for differential effects of 5-HT in ASD further and suggests that 5-HT has a differential impact on the brain temporal dynamics in people with and without ASD. This may reflect group differences in functioning of the 5-HT system and its relation to responsivity to modulation of 5-HT in individuals with ASD.
The cellular basis of this difference in responsivity to 5-HT is not known but may at least partly be genetic [35–37]. For example, reduced habituation of the amygdala response to facia emotion has been observed in individuals with ASD with low expression of the 5-HT transporter-linked promoter region, indicating that the altered facial-emotion processing in ASD could be genetically influenced in relation to the serotonergic system [68]. This fits with the suggestion that the 5-HT reuptake transporter protein, the major regulator of synaptic levels of 5-HT, is ‘overwhelmed’ or even impaired in individuals with ASD [69]. The end result is long-term impairment of serotonin transporter function which leads to permanently up- or down-regulated ancillary clearance uptake mechanisms for 5-HT and hence, altered homoeostasis of 5-HT in ASD [70]. We postulate that such a mechanism may contribute to the altered homeostasis of brain activity at the macro-scale level observed here.
A corollary of these findings is that we cannot assume that individuals with and without ASD respond to 5-HT drugs in the same way. While there is some evidence supporting the use of SSRIs for various indications in adults with ASD, the benefits are modest and especially in young people there appears to be an increased risk of adverse effects [64, 71]. Consensus guidelines for SSRI use in individuals with ASD currently state that they should be used in low doses and titrated up gradually with careful monitoring of side effects [71]. Whether the differential effect of SSRI on brain function observed here might contribute to a greater sensitivity and/or different clinical response to this family of medicines in ASD requires further investigation.
Limitations
We acknowledge several limitations of this study. First, we did not have a large study sample size because we adhered to strict recruitment criteria with repeated testing and drug administration. However, we adopted a conservative statistical thresholding in controlling for multiple comparisons and because each participant provided both placebo and drug data, inter-individual variability was reduced. Future studies may include a larger sample. Second, the inclusion of only male participants limits generalisability of findings. However, it also limits potential sex differences in clinical profiles, pharmacology and functional activation in a modest sample size. Third, we used citalopram to investigate the modulatory effect of brain 5-HT levels and how brain activations might respond to a 5-HT challenge. Future studies could explore the receptor mechanisms underlying this finding with more selective drug probes of the 5-HT system. Fourth, we measured baseline anxiety and depression levels but could not measure again at the time of peak blood levels of citalopram when participants were in the scanner. Previous studies have shown that using a tryptophan depletion protocol to lower 5-HT levels in individuals with ASD would increase their anxiety levels [72]. Thus, the role of the 5-HT system in ASD is likely to be complex with effects depending on acute or chronic changes to 5-HT and the age of participants in any given study.
Conclusions
We found that increasing levels of 5-HT using an SSRI caused a sustained activation in key limbic regions during processing of negative facial emotions in adults with ASD. In contrast the neurotypical response to emotion reverted to baseline. Thus, the homoeostatic control of serotonin pathways which regulate limbic system habituation is distinct in adults with ASD. Our results encourage a shift away from examining average differences in brain activity towards a focus on brain temporal dynamics. Our findings also indicate that we cannot assume that 5-HT drugs act in the same way in people with and without ASD. This study provides a fresh perspective on the biology of ASD and may help inform approaches to pharmacological intervention. It also encourages further examination of the clinical utility of altering habituation pharmacologically in ASD.
Funding and disclosure
The authors acknowledge support from the Clinical Biochemistry Department at King’s College Hospital, the Maudsley Pharmacy Department and the Neurodevelopmental Clinic at the Maudsley Hospital.
The authors acknowledge support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience King’s College London. This article presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The results leading to this publication has received funding from the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking under grant agreement No 777394 for the project AIMS-2-TRIALS. This Joint Undertaking receives support from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme and EFPIA and AUTISM SPEAKS, Autistica, SFARI. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the IMI, NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all the volunteers for their participation.
Author contributions
DGM, GMM and ED conceived of the original idea. JLF, RHW, DGM, GMM and ED designed the study. JLF and RHW carried out the study. VG, VS, CMM, SB, and CE provided administrative, technical and material support. NMLW analysed the data. NMLW and GMM contributed to the interpretation of the results. NMLW drafted the manuscript in consultation with DGM, GMM and ED. All authors provided critical feedback. DGM, GMM and ED supervised the whole study.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: James L. Findon, Robert H. Wichers, Declan G. Murphy, Grainne M. McAlonan, Eileen Daly
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41386-020-0693-0).
References
- 1.Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. 2014;383:896–910. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Deutsch SI, Raffaele CT. Understanding facial expressivity in autism spectrum disorder: an inside out review of the biological basis and clinical implications. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2019;88:401–17. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 4.Jackson ME, Moghaddam B. Amygdala regulation of nucleus accumbens dopamine output is governed by the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2018;21:676–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-02-00676.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Mattay VS, Fera F, Weinberger DR. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage. 2002;17:317–23. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Whalen PJ, Raila H, Bennett R, Mattek A, Brown A, Taylor J, et al. Neuroscience and facial expressions of emotion: the role of amygdala-prefrontal interactions. Emot Rev. 2013;5:78–83. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sergerie K, Chochol C, Armony JL. The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:811–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dricu M, Frühholz S. Perceiving emotional expressions in others: Activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses of explicit evaluation, passive perception and incidental perception of emotions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:810–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Freese JL, Amaral DG. Neuroanatomy of the primate amygdala. In: Whalen PJ, Phelps EA, editors. The Human Amygdala. New York: Guilford; 2009. p. 3–42.
- 10.Pennartz CMA, Groenewegen HJ, Lopes, da Silva FH. The nucleus accumbens as a complex of functionally distinct neuronal ensembles: an integration of behavioural, electrophysiological and anatomical data. Prog Neurobiol. 1994;42:719–61. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(94)90025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Motzkin JC, Philippi CL, Wolf RC, Baskaya MK, Koenigs M. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:276–84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, et al. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:519–26. doi: 10.1038/nn1421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Critchley HD, Daly EM, Bullmore ET, Williams SCR, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson DM, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour. Brain. 2000;123:2203–12. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ashwin C, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, O’Riordan M, Bullmore ET. Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pinkham AE, Hopfinger JB, Pelphrey KA, Piven J, Penn DL. Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res. 2008;99:164–75. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bullmore ET, Williams SCR, Ring HA, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin C, Wheelwright S. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;24:355–64. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00011-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Daly EM, Deeley Q, Ecker C, Craig M, Hallahan B, Murphy C, et al. Serotonin and the neural processing of facial emotions in adults with autism: an fMRI study using acute tryptophan depletion. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:1003–13. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Daly E, Deeley Q, Hallahan B, Craig M, Brammer M, Lamar M, et al. Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on neural processing of facial expressions of emotion in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2010;210:499–510. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1850-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wicker B, Fonlupt P, Hubert B, Tardif C, Gepner B, Deruelle C. Abnormal cerebral effective connectivity during explicit emotional processing in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3:135–43. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Breakspear M. Dynamic models of large-scale brain activity. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:340–52. doi: 10.1038/nn.4497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Baker AP, Brookes MJ, Rezek IA, Smith SM, Behrens T, Smith PJP, et al. Fast transient networks in spontaneous human brain activity. Elife. 2014;3:e01867. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Watanabe T, Rees G. Brain network dynamics in high functioning individuals with autism. Nat Commun. 2017;8:16048. doi: 10.1038/ncomms16048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Plichta MM, Grimm O, Morgen K, Mier D, Sauer C, Haddad L, et al. Amygdala habituation: a reliable fMRI phenotype. Neuroimage. 2014;103:383–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, Kennedy WA, Rauch SL, Hyman SE, et al. Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron. 1996;17:875–87. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Swartz JR, Wiggins JL, Carrasco M, Lord C, Monk CS. Amygdala habituation and prefrontal functional connectivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:84–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kleinhans NM, Richards T, Greenson J. Altered dynamics of the fMRI response to faces in individuals with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:232–41. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2565-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tam FI, King JA, Geisler D, Korb FM, Sareng J, Ritschel F, et al. Altered behavioral and amygdala habituation in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder: an fMRI study. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14097-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hellyer PJ, Jachs B, Clopath C, Leech R. Local inhibitory plasticity tunes macroscopic brain dynamics and allows the emergence of functional brain networks. Neuroimage. 2016;124:85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Deco G, Ponce-Alvarez A, Hagmann P, Romani GL, Mantini D, Corbetta M. How local excitation-inhibition ratio impacts the whole brain dynamics. J Neurosci. 2014;34:7886–98. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5068-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lord L-D, Expert P, Atasoy S, Roseman L, Rapuano K, Lambiotte R, et al. Dynamical exploration of the repertoire of brain networks at rest is modulated by psilocybin. Neuroimage. 2019;199:127–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Carhart-Harris RL. How do psychedelics work? Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2019;32:16–21. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wang J, Meili D, Philippe P, Grünblatt E, Van Maldergem L, Nava C, et al. Autism spectrum disorder associated with low serotonin in CSF and mutations in the SLC29A4 plasma membrane monoamine transporter (PMAT) gene. Mol Autism. 2014;5:43. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Muller CL, Anacker AMJ, Veenstra-VanderWeele J. The serotonin system in autism spectrum disorder: from biomarker to animal models. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;321:24–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hranilovic D, Bujas-Petkovic Z, Vragovic R, Vuk T, Hock K, Jernej B. Hyperserotonemia in adults with autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1934–40. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0324-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Anderson BM, Schnetz-Boutaud NC, Bartlett J, Wotawa AM, Wright HH, Abramson RK, et al. Examination of association of genes in the serotonin system to autism. Neurogenetics. 2009;10:209–16. doi: 10.1007/s10048-009-0171-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nabi R, Serajee FJ, Chugani DC, Zhong H, Huq AHMM. Association of tryptophan 2,3 dioxygenase gene polymorphism with autism. Am J Med Genet. 2004;125B:63–68. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Devlin B, Cook EH, Coon H, Dawson G, Grigorenko EL, McMahon W, et al. Autism and the serotonin transporter: the long and short of it. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:1110–6. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Murphy DGM, Daly E, Schmitz N, Toal F, Murphy K, Curran S, et al. Cortical serotonin 5-HT2A receptor binding and social communication in adults with Asperger’s syndrome: an in vivo SPECT study. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:934–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Murphy SE, Longhitano C, Ayres RE, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. Tryptophan supplementation induces a positive bias in the processing of emotional material in healthy female volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2006;187:121–30. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0401-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Harmer CJ, Rogers RD, Tunbridge E, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Tryptophan depletion decreases the recognition of fear in female volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2003;167:411–7. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1401-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Selvaraj S, Walker C, Arnone D, Cao B, Faulkner P, Cowen PJ, et al. Effect of citalopram on emotion processing in humans: a combined 5-HT1A [11C]CUMI-101 PET and Functional MRI Study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;43:655–64. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Niederhofer H, Staffen W, Mair A. Tianeptine: a novel strategy of psychopharmacological treatment of children with autistic disorder. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2003;18:389–93. doi: 10.1002/hup.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cook EH, Leventhal BL. The serotonin system in autism. Curr Opin Pediatr. 1996;8:348–54. doi: 10.1097/00008480-199608000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Calhoun VD, Miller R, Pearlson G, Adali T. The chronnectome: time-varying connectivity networks as the next frontier in fMRI data discovery. Neuron. 2014;84:262–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.King JB, Prigge MBD, King CK, Morgan J, Dean DC, Freeman A, et al. Evaluation of differences in temporal synchrony between brain regions in individuals with autism and typical development. JAMA Netw Open. 2018;1:e184777. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.4777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Watanabe T, Rees G, Masuda N. Atypical intrinsic neural timescale in autism. Elife. 2019;8:1–18. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kleinhans NM, Johnson LC, Richards T, Mahurin R, Greenson J, Dawson G, et al. Reduced neural habituation in the amygdala and social impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:467–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.WHO. ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioral disorders. Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1993.
- 49.Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, Heemsbergen J, Jordan H, Mawhood L, et al. Austism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. J Autism Dev Disord. 1989;19:185–212. doi: 10.1007/BF02211841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wechsler D. The psychometric tradition: developing the wechsler adult intelligence scale. Contemp Educ Psychol. 1981;6:82–5. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17. doi: 10.1023/a:1005653411471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol. 1959;32:50–5. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–63. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sangkuhl K, Klein TE, Altman RB. PharmGKB summary: citalopram pharmacokinetics pathway. Pharmacogenet Genom. 2011;21:769–92. doi: 10.1097/FPC.0b013e328346063f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Milne RJ, Goa KL. Citalopram. A review of its pharmacodynamic and pharmacokinetic properties, and therapeutic potential in depressive illness. Drugs. 1991;41:450–77. doi: 10.2165/00003495-199141030-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage. 2005;25:1325–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Odriozola P, Dajani DR, Burrows CA, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Goodman E, Baez AC, et al. Atypical frontoamygdala functional connectivity in youth with autism. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2019;100603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 60.Mackey S, Petrides M. Architecture and morphology of the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;40:2777–96. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Montagu JD. Habituation of the psycho-galvanic reflex during serial tests. J Psychosom Res. 1963;7:199–214. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(63)90004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Whalen PJ, Eccard CH, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, et al. Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:309–16. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01066-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.King BH, Hollander E, Sikich L, McCracken JT, Scahill L, Bregman JD, et al. Lack of efficacy of citalopram in children with autism spectrum disorders and high levels of repetitive behavior: citalopram ineffective in children with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:583–90. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mcdougle CJ, Naylor ST, Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, Heninger GR, Price LH. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of fluvoxamine in adults with autistic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:1001–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830110037005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Mcdougle CJ, Kresch LE, Posey DJ. Repetitive thoughts and behavior in pervasive developmental disorders: Treatment with serotonin reuptake inhibitors. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:427–35. doi: 10.1023/a:1005551523657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Elliott R, Zahn R, Deakin JFW, Anderson IM. Affective cognition and its disruption in mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:153–82. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Wiggins JL, Swartz JR, Martin DM, Lord C, Monk CS. Serotonin transporter genotype impacts amygdala habituation in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;9:832–8. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Anderson GM, Gutknecht L, Cohen DJ, Brailly-Tabard S, Cohen JHM, Ferrari P, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter variants in autism: functional effects and relationship to platelet hyperserotonemia. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7:831–6. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Garbarino VR, Gilman TL, Daws LC, Gould GG. Extreme enhancement or depletion of serotonin transporter function and serotonin availability in autism spectrum disorder. Pharm Res. 2019;140:85–99. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2018.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Howes OD, Rogdaki M, Findon JL, Wichers RH, Charman T, King BH, et al. Autism spectrum disorder: Consensus guidelines on assessment, treatment and research from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol. 2018;32:3–29. doi: 10.1177/0269881117741766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Mcdougle CJ, Naylor ST, Cohen DJ, Aghajanian GK, Heninger GR, Price LH. Effects of tryptophan depletion in drug-free adults with autistic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:993–1000. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830110029004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.