Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2020 Dec 21;16(12):e1008418. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008418

Differential effects of propofol and ketamine on critical brain dynamics

Thomas F Varley 1,2,¤,*, Olaf Sporns 1, Aina Puce 1, John Beggs 3
Editor: Saad Jbabdi4
PMCID: PMC7785236  PMID: 33347455

Abstract

Whether the brain operates at a critical “tipping” point is a long standing scientific question, with evidence from both cellular and systems-scale studies suggesting that the brain does sit in, or near, a critical regime. Neuroimaging studies of humans in altered states of consciousness have prompted the suggestion that maintenance of critical dynamics is necessary for the emergence of consciousness and complex cognition, and that reduced or disorganized consciousness may be associated with deviations from criticality. Unfortunately, many of the cellular-level studies reporting signs of criticality were performed in non-conscious systems (in vitro neuronal cultures) or unconscious animals (e.g. anaesthetized rats). Here we attempted to address this knowledge gap by exploring critical brain dynamics in invasive ECoG recordings from multiple sessions with a single macaque as the animal transitioned from consciousness to unconsciousness under different anaesthetics (ketamine and propofol). We use a previously-validated test of criticality: avalanche dynamics to assess the differences in brain dynamics between normal consciousness and both drug-states. Propofol and ketamine were selected due to their differential effects on consciousness (ketamine, but not propofol, is known to induce an unusual state known as “dissociative anaesthesia”). Our analyses indicate that propofol dramatically restricted the size and duration of avalanches, while ketamine allowed for more awake-like dynamics to persist. In addition, propofol, but not ketamine, triggered a large reduction in the complexity of brain dynamics. All states, however, showed some signs of persistent criticality when testing for exponent relations and universal shape-collapse. Further, maintenance of critical brain dynamics may be important for regulation and control of conscious awareness.

Author summary

Here we explore how different anaesthetic drugs change the nature of brain dynamics, using neural activity recorded from sub-dural electrophysiological arrays implanted in a macaque brain. Previous research has suggested that loss of consciousness under anaesthesia is associated with a movement away from critical brain dynamics, towards a less flexible regime. When comparing ketamine and propofol, two anaesthetics with largely different effects on consciousness, we find that propofol, but not ketamine, produces a dramatic reduction in the complexity of brain activity and restricts the range of scales where critical dynamics are plausible. These results suggest that maintenance of critical dynamics may be important for regulation and control of conscious awareness.

Introduction

The hypothesis that the brain operates in a critical regime near a “tipping” point between different states (often, but not always, described as low and high entropy, respectively) is growing in popularity, both on neurophysiological evidence and due to appealing properties of critical, or near-critical, systems [1], that are thought to be key elements of optimal nervous system functioning. In both in vivo recordings and simulations, critical systems show the widest dynamic range [25], which indicates that critical systems can respond to, and amplify, a broad range of signals. For a system embedded in a complex environment, where salient signals may have a variety of intensities, a broad dynamic sensory range is a necessary adaptation. Critical systems also have optimized memory storage capacity from which complex information about states and patterns can be retrieved [69]. A related series of findings suggest that, in addition to optimal dynamic range and memory capacities, critical systems have optimized information transmission capabilities [1012], and that the ability to integrate information is locally maximal near the critical zone as well [13]. Signs of critical dynamics have been found in the brains of a large number of different animals with different levels of CNS complexity, including humans [14], zebrafish [15], turtles [16], rat brain cultures [17], leeches [18], freely-behaving rodents [19], and non-human primates [20]. This suggests that the evolutionary advantage conferred by critical neural dynamics is highly conserved between species. The “critical brain” hypothesis has not been universally accepted however [21], and disagreement remains within the field as to when it is acceptable to conclude data was produced by a critical or near-critical system [17].

Despite the considerable work that has been performed on identifying indicators of criticality at the level of neuronal circuits, the relationship between critical dynamics (or lack thereof) at the micro-scale and macro-scale phenomena such as cognition, sensation, and awareness is less clear. In human neuroimaging studies, deviations from criticality have been found to be associated with altered or abnormal states of consciousness. Long-term sleep deprivation reduces signatures of critical dynamics in human MEG activity [14], and the pathophysiology of epilepsy has been modelled as a failure to maintain healthy critical dynamics [22, 23]. Psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, obsessive-compulsive disorder and major depressive disorders have also been discussed as possible deviaitons away from “healthy” critical dynamics. Human neuroimaging studies have suggested that “classical” serotonergic psychedelic drugs (eg. psilocybin, lysergic acid diethylamide, dimethyltryptamine, etc) increase markers of criticality, both in fMRI and MEG studies (for review see [24]). These findings have prompted some to hypothesize that normal consciousness emerges when brain activity is tuned near a critical regime, and that alterations in consciousness are reflective of transitions towards, or away from, the critical regime [25, 26].

However, signs of criticality in neural tissue are observed in animals (or cultured tissues) that could not be plausibly considered conscious. This includes dissociated cortical cultures [17], excised turtle brains [16], and animals anesthetized with a number of different anaesthetics [27]. Clearly criticality alone is not sufficient for conscious awareness, although the question of necessity remains open. Cortical recordings from anaesthetized rats found loss of critical dynamics during the period of anaesthesia that re-emerged over the course of waking [28]. A similar study using cellular imaging found that anaesthesia reversibly altered signs of critical dynamics, as well as reducing the complexity of brain activity [29]. Computational models of brain dynamics informed by the pharmacology of anaesthetic agents have suggested that loss of consciousness induced by anaesthesia may be associated with a loss of critical dynamics [30] at a macro scale as well, although this remains an under-explored area of research.

To assess the relative differences in criticality between states of consciousness, we used publicly available invasive electrocorticography (ECoG) recordings from a Macaca fuscata monkey to compare the normal, awake resting-state (the monkey is awake, with eyes open, restrained in a primate chair) to two distinct states of anesthesia induced by two different drugs: propofol and ketamine (recordings were begun only after loss of consciousness had been diagnosed). These recordings are available as part of the NeuroTycho project’s open data initiative [31, 32]. While both drugs induce surgical anaesthesia in high doses, and are used in clinical settings, they display markedly different pharmacologies and trigger different subjective experiences at low to moderate doses. Propofol is a commonly-used anaesthetic administered either by inhalation or intravenously. While its exact mechanism of action remains unknown, it is believed that it’s primary action is through potentiating inhibitory GABAA receptors resulting in wide-spread decreases in neuronal activity [33]. Behaviourally, propofol induces sedation, atonia, and at high doses, cardiac arrest, respiratory depression, and hypotension [34]. Ketamine acts as an antagonist of glutamatergic NMDA receptors and, in contrast to propofol, causes mild nervous system stimulation and has little effect on respiration [34, 35]. Furthermore, while propofol induces a state reminiscent of deep coma, at sub-anaesthetic doses, ketamine induces an atypical state known as “dissociative anaesthesia” [36, 37], in which a person appears unresponsive to sensory or physical stimuli, but will often experience dream-like states, including hallucinations, out-of-body experiences, and immersive visions [35]. In this way, ketamine models other altered states of consciousness such as NREM sleep and locked-in syndrome, where phenomenological awareness can persist, despite an external appearance of unresponsive unconsciousness.

We chose electrophysiological recordings for this analysis for several reasons. Many previous studies that have found signs of criticality in anaesthetized animals have been performed while recording at the single-neuron micro-scale, while studies reporting alterations to critical dynamics following changes to consciousness have been performed using macro-scale neuroimaging methods (fMRI, MEG, etc). Intracranial ECoG provides a type of meso-scale signal: with coarse-graining of electrical activity in large numbers of neurons (≈ 105), but with finer localization than scalp EEG/MEG [38]. Consequently, these data are ideal for the study of criticality as the likely highly local preservation of critical dynamics in neuronal-level studies may interact with alterations to criticality seen in more macro-scale neuroimaging studies. Human ECoG studies have found indirect evidence of dynamical criticality [39, 40]—not using avalanche-based analyses such as we used here. Alonso et al (2014) reported changes high-frequency ECoG signal stability during the onset of clinical anaesthesia although signs of metastability persisted through all states.

Here we test two hypotheses concerning the relationship between consciousness and critical dynamics. Specifically, we test if:

  1. during the awake state, activity would express several hallmarks of criticality, including a power-law distribution of avalanche sizes and durations, exponent relations [17], as well as signs of data collapse when subsampling channels (known as finite size scaling).

  2. propofol would dramatically reduce signs of critical dynamics, but that criticality would persist under the influence of ketamine. This is based on the phenomena of “ketamine dreams” discussed above: based on the persistence of phenomenological consciousness under ketamine, we would signs of consciousness-like dynamics to persist under ketamine but not necessarilly under propofol. We also included a related measure, complexity [41], which has been hypothesized to relate to consciousness and found to be associated with criticality in cortical cultures [13].

For a glossary giving definitions for the various technical terms used in this paper, see Glossary.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

All data used in this experiment was taken from an open-source repository (http://www.neurotycho.org/). The original data was collected “in accordance with the experimental protocols (No. H24-2-203(4)) approved by the RIKEN ethics committee and the recommendations of the Weatherall report, “The use of non- human primates in research” [32]. Readers are referred to the quoted paper, as well as [31] and the Neurotycho repository website.

Data set and preprocessing

Data set

We used the NeuroTycho dataset, an open-source set of 128-channel invasive ECoG recordings in two Macaca fuscata monkeys [31]. For this study, we analysed the resting-state scans from one monkey (Chibi), as the scans from the other monkey contained intractable artifacts that could not be eliminated during preprocessing. Arrays were placed on the left hemisphere only, recording from all major areas including the medial wall (for a map, see Fig 1).

Fig 1. Recording array map.

Fig 1

The placement of recording arrays for Chibi. Image taken from the NeuroTycho website, http://neurotycho.org/spatial-map-ecog-array-task.

In both the propofol and ketamine conditions, the monkey was restrained in a primate chair and recorded during normal consciousness for 10 minutes, with a sampling frequency of 1 KHz. In the propofol condition, the monkey was given a single bolus of intra-venous propofol (5.2 mg/kg), until loss of consciousness was observed (defined as unresponsiveness to having their forepaws touched and/or unresponsiveness to having their nose tickled with a cotton swab). For the ketamine condition, a single bolus of intramuscular ketamine (5.1 mg/kg) was administered until loss of consciousness was observed using the above criteria. No maintenance anaesthesia was given following the initial induction. Following diagnosis of anaesthesia, 10-minute recordings were taken, sampled at 1000 Hz. We did not include any recordings from the transition period between wakefullness and anaesthesia. More detailed discussion of the drug administration protocols can be found in [32] and the Neurotycho data wiki (http://wiki.neurotycho.org/Anesthesia_and_Sleep_Task_Details).

There were a total of four scans in the awake condition, and two each in the propofol and ketamine conditions (each anaesthesia condition having it’s own associated awake scan).

Preprocessing

The data were initially examined visually in EEGLAB (v.14.1.2) [42] run in a MATLAB (v. 2018b, The Mathworks, Natick, MA) environment for major non-reoccurring artefacts. Some sample five-second plots of the selected ECoG channels can be seen in Fig 2. After inspection (no data were removed), we performed Independent Component Analysis (ICA) using the runica function in EEGLAB to extract minor artefacts [38, 42]. We ran the ICA with the full 128 possible components and, across all scans, removed an average of 4 components, corresponding to brief bursts of high-frequency noise. After the ICA and removal of components, the data were filtered in MNE Python (v. 0.19.2) with a low-pass filter at 120Hz, high-pass filtered at 1Hz, and notch filtered at 50Hz and all subsequent harmonics up to 250 Hz, to account for electrical line-noise in Japan. We did not downsample the data, operating on the 1 KHz sample rate. All filters were of the FIR Overlap type (the default in MNE Python), [43])and all filters were run twice, forwards and backwards to eliminate phase-shift artefacts. Finally, we normalized the data by removing the mean of each channel and dividing it by its standard deviation. Due to the need for long time-series in this analysis, we operated on the whole 10 minute time-series (there was no epoching). MNE Python (v. 0.19.2) was run in Python 3.7.4 using the Anaconda (v. 3.7) environment.

Fig 2. Examples of raw EEG timeseries visualized.

Fig 2

Visualization of five seconds of all 128 channels for each condition. Top: awake, middle: ketamine, bottom: propofol. Images taken from EEGLAB (v.14.1.2) [42]. Note that the time-series are un-normalized, with scales ranging from 464-515 microvolts.

Point process

For each condition, we followed the method for point-processing data described by [44] and [14]. Briefly: after filtering, the probability distribution of signal amplitudes in terms of their standard deviation from the mean, was plotted, along with the best-fit Gaussian distribution, and the point at which the actual distribution and the best-fit curve diverged were chosen as the threshold (σ). This ensured that diversions above the threshold are unlikely to be due to noise inherent in the signal. While there were differences in the point of divergence between conditions, we chose a threshold of 4, as the most conservative likely value. We sampled a range of thresholds around 4 (3 to 4.25) and found that this did not substantively alter the results, although a too-low threshold (< 3) allowed excessive noise through, while a too-high threshold (> 4.5) did not allow enough events for analysis of avalanches. For visualization of the various distributions, see Fig 3.

Fig 3. Best-fit normal distributions between conditions.

Fig 3

Representative examples of the histograms of instantaneous amplitudes across the three conditions. A: the awake condition. B: ketamine, and C: propofol. Note that while ketamine allows for a heavy-tail to persist, the propofol condition collapses to a tight Gaussian distribution.

Once the threshold had been chosen, all instances where the absolute value of a signal was less than the threshold were set to zero:

t|X|:X(t)=0if|X(t)|<σ

For all excursions above σ, the global maxima of the period was set to 1 and all other moments set to 0. We calculated the Pearson correlation coefficient ρ from the moment the series crossed the σ threshold (tmin) to the end point, the moment it dropped below σ (tmax) against the same range in every other channel, and if ρ ≥ 0.75, we also set the local maximum of the interval in the associated channel to 1 (even if it did not cross our threshold σ) as well (sending all other values to 0). For a more in-depth discussion of this, see [14].

The resulting calculation produced a binary raster plot (channel × samples, for an example see Fig 4) where every event corresponds to the moment of maximal excursion from the mean. This data structure matches the type used when analysing spiking activity from cultured neurons [10], making it amenable to many of the tools already developed for criticality analysis. We chose not to re-bin the data, instead maintaining the 1000 Hz sampling rate across all scans due to the relative shortness of the recordings: significant re-binning would reduce the available data to the point that not enough individual avalanches could be identified.

Fig 4. Coherence cascades in the awake condition.

Fig 4

An example of a section of the raster plot. Notice the banding effect of coherence cascades (a few examples are highlighted by blue ovals). This banding effect shows the cascades of events that include many distinct channels that “fire” in synchrony.

Avalanches and critical exponents

“Avalanches”, defined as transient periods of coordinated activity between elements of a system, are a feature common to many complex, dynamic systems [45]. In the context of the nervous system, avalanches typically refer to synchronized action potentials in a neural network [10]. Coordinated avalanches of activity have also been found in human EEG [44], MEG [14, 46], and fMRI [47] data. Avalanches are described by two values: the avalanche size (S, the number of elements that participate in the avalanche) and the avalanche duration (T, the lifetime of the avalanche from start to finish). In critical systems, these two values are expected to follow power laws with exponents τ, and α:

P(S)S-τP(T)T-α

S and T should be distributed such that, over all avalanches, the average size for a given duration also follows a power law:

S(T)T1/σνz

τ, α and 1/σνz are collectively known as the critical exponents of the system [17]. We note that in this context, 1/σνz is treated as a single variable and should not be considered a function of three distinct variables. Furthermore, all three exponents should be related to each other such that:

α-1τ-1-1σνz=0

It is generally considered to be insufficient evidence of criticality if only the distributions of avalanche sizes and durations follow power laws: these exponents must be related to each other through an exponent relation as given above [17].

To calculate the scaling exponents τ and α, we used the NCC Toolbox (v. 1.0) [48] to extract avalanches from our binary time series, and perform a maximum likelihood estimate (MLE) of the power-law exponent [48, 49]. The NCC Toolbox returns several values associated with each power-law inference: the MLE value of the exponent, the minimum and maximum values of x for which the power law estimate holds (xmin and xmax, where x can refer either to avalanche sizes or durations), and the p-value. It is crucial to note that xmin and xmax do not refer to the smallest and largest values of x in the empirical distribution, but rather, the minimum and maximum values between which a power-law fit plausibly holds. Following the convention of Timme et al., (2016), we set our significance threshold such that we would only accept the power law hypothesis at p ≥ 0.2 (this is the reverse of how significance estimation is usually performed, for discussion, see [49]. For the estimate of 1/σνz, we plotted 〈S〉(T) against T and extracted an estimate of the exponent by linear regression in log-log space. While this is a much cruder method than the MLE power law fit described above, unfortunately the values of 〈S〉(T) vs. T do not describe a probability distribution and so the usual methods of inference do not work.

When plotting the distribution of avalanche sizes and avalanche durations, we use complementary cumulative distribution functions (CCDFs) (defined as 1-CDF) instead of probability density functions (PDFs), following [50]. When dealing with power law (or plausibly power law) distributions with defined upper and lower bounds (xmin, xmax), the CDF and CCDFs may not display the characteristic straight line when plotted in log-log space [51, 52]. Consequently, distributions may look curved while still being plausibly drawn from a doubly-truncated power law. For a visualization of this, see Fig 5.

Fig 5. Synthetic and empirical doubly-truncated power law distributions for the data.

Fig 5

A: an empirical, doubly-truncated power-law distribution taken from this dataset with an xmin of 16 and an xmax of 196. Within this range, the exponent has been estimated to be 2.7. B: a synthetic doubly-truncated power-law distribution with the same minimum and maximum values as the empirical distribution as well as the same exponent. Note that the synthetic model has significant curves which begin well before the upper-bound cut-off. When modelling a system that produces data within a fixed range, even power-law distributed values may not follow the canonical straight line when plotted in log-log space [51, 52].

Avalanche shape collapse

In addition to having sizes and durations, avalanches also have profiles, which describe the number of channels active at each moment over the course of the avalanche’s lifetime. Near the critical regime, the profiles of all avalanches should be self-similar, that is, it should be possible to find some scaling exponent γ such that all avalanches can be rescaled to lie on top of one another [13, 17]. This is referred to as “shape collapse” and is a commonly used indicator of operating near the critical point in dynamical systems.

This scaling parameter γ can be defined as:

γ=1σνz-1

where 1/σνz is the same exponent that describes the distribution of average avalanche sizes for a given duration.

To calculate shape collapse, we first averaged together all avalanches of a given duration to create “average profiles”. From there, we used the NCC Toolbox [48] to find the optimal rescaling value and extract an estimate of 1/σνz.

Data collapse and finite size scaling

A common issue with analysis of critical dynamics in complex systems is one of sub-sampling [53, 54]. When a very large system is sub-sampled, the characteristic power-law behaviour can be lost as large events are fragmented and perceived as separate, smaller events. This is a particularly salient issue in electrophysiological recordings, as the number of channels in an array is orders of magnitude less than the number of functional units in the cortex that could be participating in critical avalanche dynamics.

In a system near the critical point, while subsampling destroys the power-law, it should not destroy the scale-free nature of the distributions—that is, when appropriately renormalized, the distributions (despite not following power laws), should display data collapse. This property is known as finite size scaling. The logic here is the same as when performing data collapse on individual avalanches, as above.

We tested for distribution shape-collapse following a similar approach to a previously procedure [54]. For each scan, we randomly sub-sampled half, a quarter, and an eighth of the channels, ten times each, and from each, calculated the associated probability distribution of avalanche events and sizes. In addition to plotting them to visualize the differences between the four distributions (the original plus the three subsampled distributions), we calculated the average pair-wise Wasserstein metric [55] to quantify the similarity between all distributions.

We then performed the same sub-sampling again, this time rebinning the binary timeseries to reflect the sampling of elements: when taking half the channels, we rebinned by a factor of two, when taking a quarter of the channels, we rebinned by a factor of four, etc. If the system is poised near the critical point, after rebinning, the distributions of avalanche activities should collapse onto one-another. When recalculating the Wasserstein metric, the average pairwise distance should be significantly reduced.

Complexity

Here, “complexity” of an N-dimensional system X (CN(X)) can be intuitively understood as “the degree to which the whole is greater than the sum of it’s parts” [41]. To calculate the complexity, the entropy of the entire system is compared to the joint entropies of all possible subsets of the system. Each state i of an N-dimensional system can be described as an N-dimensional vector xi, and the probability of state i (pi) is the number of occurrences of i divided by the number of samples. The Shannon entropy (H(X)) of the system is given by:

H(X)=-ip(xi)log2(p(xi))

We can measure the level of integration (or coordination) of a set of elements of the system by comparing the joint entropies of all the elements in the set to the sum of their individual entropies:

I(X)jk=(jkH(Xj1))-H(X)jk

Here, k is the number of neurons in a subset, and j is the index of a given subset of k neurons in the set of all sets of k neurons. This has also been called “multinformation” or “total correlation” [56]. For instance, Xj1 refers to the jth element from the set of N -choose-1 elements alone, while Xj4 refers to the jth set of 4 elements from the set of N-choose-4 sets of 4 elements.

The complexity is then found by comparing the integration of the entire system (X1N) to the average integration (I(k)1jj) over every possible subset, for every possible subset size of k.

CN(X)=1Nk=2N[(k-1N-1)I(X)-I(Xjk)j]

This value of CN(X) is a more intuitively meaningful representation of “complexity” than other commonly used measures, such as Lempel-Ziv Complexity [57], in that it is low for systems that are both perfectly ordered and systems that are perfectly random [13], while peaking in systems that combine elements of both. Calculating the complexity of a system is a non-trivial task, exploding to intractable levels as N gets even modestly large (for context, for a 128-channel system, it would take longer than the expected lifetime of the universe of exhaustively search all partitions). To avoid interminable run-times, the NCC Toolbox [48] includes several corrections for sub-sampling and heuristics for estimating integration in large systems. One correction is to only consider those bins where at least one “event” occurs, consequently calculating the complexity of the avalanches themselves, which controls for variable numbers and distances between avalanches. Furthermore, the NCC Toolbox corrects for sub-sampling biases in the joint probability distribution by comparing the empirical integrations to an ensemble of time-randomized null models and subtracting the expected value of the distribution of null integrations and optimizing on the subsets of size k that are most informative about the structure of the system. By implementing these corrections, the toolbox is able to infer the multi-scale integration/segregation structure without having to brute-force all possible bipartitions of a sparse multi-dimensional time-series.

As with the avalanche size and duration distributions, we used the same 30-second jitter null-model to explore the effect of randomizing the data. We hypothesized that jittering the data should significantly reduce the nonlinearity and total integrated information.

Results

Channel activity

We found large differences between the channel rates following the administration of both ketamine and propofol. In the awake condition, the average channel-wise firing rate was 9 × 10−4 spikes per millisecond (s/m), which dropped to 5 × 10−4 (s/m) in the ketamine condition and 8 × 10−5 (s/m) under propofol. We found that, within conditions, there was a high correlation between channel activity rates during different scans. Within the awake condition (across all six possible pairwise comparisons), we found an average correlation of 0.89 ± 0.03. In the ketamine condition (which only allows a single comparison), the correlation was 0.76 and in the propofol condition, the correlation was 0.62). Interestingly, we did not find strong correlations when comparing channel activity rates within the same scan before and after induction of anaesthesia. The correlation between channel activity rates before and after ketamine induction was 0.225 (p-value > 0.05) and the correlation between channel activity before and after propofol induction was 0.06 (p-value > 0.05). These results suggest a much higher degree of dynamical similarity within conditions scanned on different days than between conditions induced during a single scan. For visualization of these results, see Fig 6.

Fig 6. Changes in channel activity levels between conditions.

Fig 6

A: For each condition, the correlation between channel activity (firing rate × 100) between two scans. In all conditions, it is apparent that those channels that are more activity in one scan are similarly active during different scans within the same condition. This suggests that brain dynamics are consistent within conditions. B: histograms of the the channel rate × 100 for all three conditions. It is apparent that propofol decreases the overall channel firing rate more that ketamine, although both have a lower firing rate than the awake condition. C-E: normalized channel activity rates projected onto the recording array. It is clear that propofol and ketamine disrupt the activity-rate structure that exists in the awake condition. Original images taken from the NeuroTycho wiki: http://neurotycho.org/spatial-map-ecog-array-task.

Using the NeuroTycho channel labelling (see Fig 1), we found that, in the awake condition, high levels of activity were seen occipito-temporal and parietal regions of the brain. This pattern was disrupted by both propofol and ketamine. In the ketamine condition, there was a decrease in the range of channel activities, and in both anaesthetic conditions, there was a shift in activity towards the occipital lobe. This significance of this is unclear, as the monkeys were wearing eyeshades throughout the anaesthetic experience and consequently were not receiving visual input.

Avalanche distributions

There are clear differences between the awake, ketamine, and propofol distributions of avalanche sizes and durations (Fig 7). The Wasserstein distances between the conditions reveal that, in all cases, the awake condition was far more similar to the ketamine condition than the propofol condition. The distance between the avalanche size distribution in the awake condition and the ketamine condition was 0.46, while between the awake condition and the propofol condition it was 1.77. The same pattern was apparent between the avalanche duration distributions. The distance between the awake and ketamine conditions was 0.05, while for the awake and propofol conditions it was 0.43 (for the raw values see Table 1).

Fig 7. CCDFs of Avalanche Size and Duration by Condition.

Fig 7

Comparison CCDFs for awake (purple) vs. ketamine (grey) and propofol (orange). Each plot includes two recordings: the anaesthesia condition (propofol or ketamine) and the associated pre-anaesthesia awake condition from the same monkey during the same session. Right panels: awake vs. ketamine. Left panels: awake vs. propofol. Avalanches sizes (top four panels) and durations (bottom four panels). Visual inspection shows that the ketamine condition tracks the awake condition much more closely than the propofol condition does: note how the propofol condition begins to drop below it’s associated awake distribution almost immediately, while the ketamine distribution tracks the awake distribution for a considerable range. This indicates that ketamine supports larger, longer-lived avalanches than propofol.

Table 1. Table of results.

A table giving all the raw values of each of the measures reported here, for each monkey, in each condition.

Subject Condition τ xmin S xmax S α xmin D xmax D Exp. Rel. Avg. Size / Dur 1/σνz Complexity Rebin KS D No Rebin KS D Rebin KS S No Rebin KS S
813KT awake 2.709 16 196 4.422 8 30 2.00234055 1.2529880263 1.144 0.0473942748 0.0444209518 0.1290998009 0.0623528147 0.1503449156
813KT Ketam 2.225 6 60 3.007 4 17 1.6383673469 1.2472912656 1.221 0.0316139101 0.0259019459 0.1375173692 0.0508161703 0.1653117455
802PF awake 2.484 13 368 4.783 9 41 2.5491913747 1.2654549163 1.262 0.0469241472 0.0395054996 0.1244217247 0.0677982829 0.1588397862
802PF Propo 2.295 2 40 3.407 3 16 1.8586872587 1.2407987895 1.271 0.0036012452 0.0297453155 0.1134300394 0.0788062334 0.1661862264
730PF awake 2.834 14 316 3.679 7 32 1.4607415485 1.3887947163 1.377 0.0470132706 0.0378105651 0.1299924942 0.0556711293 0.1639426327
730PF Propo 2.299 2 56 2.446 1 8 1.1131639723 1.4430822801 1.508 0.0051650643 0.0252161665 0.0926269857 0.0729665868 0.1340633845
719KT awake 2.93 22 634 4.029 8 67 1.5694300518 1.405535723 -1.1 0.0477691165 0.0346876135 0.12823451 0.063741846 0.1690890631
719KT Ketam 2.93 22 634 4.029 8 67 1.5694300518 1.2537008081 1.25 0.029773869 0.0261199899 0.1324190428 0.0550677446 0.1445344283

Scaling exponents

For every distribution, there was a range between some xmin and xmax for which the power-law hypothesis held with p ≥ 0.2. However, in the propofol condition, this range was dramatically restricted. In the awake condition, the average value xmin for the avalanche size distribution was 16.25 channels and the average value for xmax was 378.5 channels. In the ketamine condition, the average value xmin for the avalanche size distribution was 14 channels, and the average value for xmax was 347 channels. Finally, in the propofol condition, the average value xmin for the avalanche size duration was 2 channels and the average value xmax was 48 channels. The same pattern was observed in the distributions of avalanche durations. The average xmin of the distribution of avalanche durations in the awake condition is 8 ms, while the average xmax is 42.5 ms. In the ketamine condition, the average xmin was 6 ms and the average xmax is 42 ms. In the propofol condition, the range was much more constricted: the average xmin was 2 ms and the average xmax was 12 ms. These results indicate that, while all distributions had regions that could be plausibly modelled as following a power-law, in the propofol condition, this region was extremely narrow and typically restricted to very small values (excluding the tail, where the power-law distribution is most relevant). In contrast, ketamine maintained a range comparable to the awake condition, suggesting that, unlike propofol, ketamine anaesthesia does not suppress the propagation of large bursts of coordinated activity.

For each range where the power-law fit held, we calculated the associated exponents, τ and α. The sample size is too small for significance testing, although on average, the awake condition had the largest exponents for avalanche sizes (2.74) and durations (4.23), followed by ketamine, for both sizes (2.58) and durations (3.52) as well. Propofol had the smallest scaling exponents for both avalanche parameters (size: 2.3, durations: 2.93). The average calculated values of 1/σνz were quite similar between all three (awake: 1.89, ketamine: 1.6, propofol: 1.5).

In all conditions, within the relevant ranges of xmin and xmax, we found a strong relationship between the average size for a given duration (see Fig 8). The calculated value of 1/σνz was similar across all conditions (awake: 1.33, ketamine: 1.25, propofol 1.34). Having two separate estimations of 1/σνz (one from the exponent relation, one from the average size for a given duration) allows us to see how well the scaling exponents relate. Surprisingly, the awake condition had the greatest percentage difference between the two values, at 35.19%, followed by ketamine at 24.76% and finally propofol with 10.83%. We had hypothesized that the awake and/or ketamine conditions would show the highest degree of concurrence between the measures, but instead, the awake condition has the lowest degree of concurrence. The significance of this is difficult to explain. One possibility is that the awake condition is noisier than either anaesthesia condition, which would reduce the critical fitness, as well as driving down the xmin value below which a power-law holds (see Discussion for more on this issue). Finally, we note that, while concurrence is highest in the propofol condition, it is over a much narrower range of x values for both avalanche sizes and durations, as opposed to the awake and ketamine conditions and so the higher concurrence may be reflective of the more restricted range with fewer degrees of freedom (for further discussion of this issue, see the Discussion).

Fig 8. The average avalanche size for a given duration by condition.

Fig 8

The color mapping is the same as in Fig 7: purple corresponds to the awake condition (A, B, C, D), grey to ketamine (E, F), and orange to propofol (G, H). Each plot includes two scans: the anaesthesia condition (propofol or ketamine) and the associated pre-anesthesia awake condition. In all cases there is a clear linear relationship between the average avalanche size for a given duration. Such a relationship is considered necessary, but not sufficient, to conclude a system is displaying critical dynamics [17].

Avalanche shape collapse

The avalanche collapse results are largely consistent with the analysis of average size for a given duration. The estimated values of 1/σνz calculated from the shape collapse between conditions were largely similar: awake: 1.261, ketamine: 1.24, propofol 1.39. Once again, the percentage difference between these values and the exponent relationship was highest in the awake condition (40.2%), followed by the ketamine condition (25.95%), and with the greatest agreement in the propofol condition (6.7%). We also calculated the percentage difference between values of 1/σνz derived from the shape collapse and the average size/given duration analysis. These showed a much higher degree of concurrence. The awake condition again had the greatest percentage difference (5.19%), followed by propofol (3.48%), and ketamine had the lowest percentage difference (1.2%).

Finite-size scaling and distribution collapse

In addition to power-law and exponent-relation based indicators of critical dynamics, we also tested whether the probability distributions of avalanche sizes and durations exhibited data collapse when subsampled (finite size scaling) [54]. We initially sub-sampled the system by randomly selecting subsets of channels (half the channels, a quarter, an eighth, etc) and comparing the distributions of avalanche sizes and durations by computing the average pairwise KS-distance between all four distributions. We then rebinned the binary time series with the inverse of the fraction of the sample selected (eg: when subsampling half the nodes, we rebinned the timeseries by two, when sampling a quarter, rebinned by four, etc).

When compared, all conditions showed visual indicators of finite size scaling collapse (see Fig 9 for an illustration with the distributions of avalanche sizes). To quantify this, we calculated the fold-change between the average pairwise KS-distance before and after re-binning. For the distributions of avalanche sizes, ketamine showed the highest average fold change (−0.66), indicating the greatest collapse. The awake condition came second with a fold-change of −0.61, and the propofol condition showed the weakest shape-collapse, with a fold change of −0.49. For the avalanche duration distributions, the pattern was similar, with ketamine showing the greatest fold change (−8.1), but in this case, propofol was in the middle with a fold change of −0.73, and the awake condition (−0.69) at the end.

Fig 9. Data collapse under rescaling.

Fig 9

Shape collapse for the various subsampled avalanche sizes before (left column) and after (right column) renormalizing (rebinning) the binary timeseries. The color indicators are consistent with other figures: purple is awake (A, B), grey is ketamine (C, D), yellow is propofol (E, F). In all conditions, renormalization resulted in noticeable shape collapse, which can be quantified by doing pairwise calculations of the Wasserstein distance metric. Unlike Figs 7 and 8, this does not show all scans, but rather, representative examples of shape collapse for each of the three conditions.

This suggests that all the conditions display some measurable collapse when renormalized, and in both avalanche shape and durations, the effect was most dramatic in the ketamine condition, compared to awake and propofol.

Complexity

There were dramatic differences between the degree to which ketamine and propofol reduced the complexity of brain activity (Fig 10). Both anaesthetics reduced the total CN(X), however, propofol had a much stronger effect. On average, the fold-change between the awake condition and the ketamine condition was −0.35, compared to the awake vs. propofol condition, which showed an average fold-change of −0.91. This is a strong indicator that, even at surgical doses, ketamine supports significantly more complex brain dynamics than propofol.

Fig 10. Differences in multi-scale complexity between conditions.

Fig 10

Integration of information across scales for awake (purple) vs. ketamine (grey) (A, B) and awake vs. propofol (orange) (C, D). Complexity was calculated using the NCC Toolbox [48], using the measure first proposed by Tononi, Sporns & Edelman [41]. While the ketamine condition (grey) was associated with a noticable decrease in complexity compared the awake condition (purple), it was a much smaller decrease than what was observed in the propofol condition, which was an order of magnitude less complex. Note that the total value of complexity is not the greatest value, but rather, the difference between the area under the curves for the linear fit and the true non-linear integration.

Discussion

In this manuscript we present several converging lines of evidence that propofol and ketamine produce different effects on markers of critical brain dynamics. Furthermore, ketamine, but not propofol, appears to supports dynamics similar to those observed in normal waking consciousness in ECoG activity recorded from a single macaque in different states of consciousness. Despite inducing states that appear similar to external observers (unresponsiveness to pain or sensory stimuli, decreased voluntary motor output, anaesthesia), propofol produces a near-total extinction of consciousness, while ketamine frequently induces states of “dissociative” anaesthesia, which can include dream-like or out-of-body experiences [36, 37]. To test for signs of criticality we explored the dynamics of avalanches (coordinated bursts of high-intensity activity) across the cortex, testing for scale-free distributions and exponent relations between them. We also examined universality under renormalization and an information-theoretic measure of multi-dimensional complexity to further characterize the effects of these drugs on brain dynamics.

We found that ketamine slightly reduced the rate of events across the cortex when compared the awake condition, while the propofol condition reduced activity rates by an entire order of magnitude. Within conditions, the distributions of activities across channels were consistent between scans, suggesting that these measures are stable across multiple drug experiences, at least within one individual.

Avalanche distributions

We found that, when compared to normal wakefulness, propofol dramatically attenuated signs of power-law distributions in both the distributions of avalanche sizes and durations. Heavy tailed distributions imply that avalanche dynamics are playing out over a range of temporal and spatial scales; consequently, the finding of multiscale dynamics in the awake and ketamine conditions, but not the propofol condition suggests that this kind of multiscale coordination is significant for the maintenance of consciousness. This is consistent with previous work suggesting that loss of multiscale structure and decreases in the fractal dimension of EEG signals under sedation [5862]. The spectral exponent of the power spectrum density function, which typically follows a power-law decay has been found to be strongly indicative of conscious states [62, 63], providing evidence that multi-scale, or scale-free processes in the brain are related to the maintenance of normal awareness.

The differences in the distributions of avalanche sizes and durations may be indicative of changes to the ability of the brain to coordinate activity in different states. In the awake condition, we observed avalanches that included many distinct channels, as well as being comparatively longer lived, while those long, large avalanches were significantly impaired by propofol, but not by ketamine. The propofol result is consistent with previous work which found that propofol inhibited the ability of the brain to form integrated higher-level networks, while leaving local activity in tact [64, 65]. If avalanches indicate when signals are able to propagate across the cortex, we might expect to see alterations in avalanche activity being associated with fragmentation or disintegration of functional connectivity networks. Previous research using point-processing methods on fMRI time-series has found that rare, high-amplitude events can capture a significant information relevant to critical brain dynamics and states of consciousness [47, 66]. Using techniques from information theory such as mutual information and transfer entropy [67] it would be informative to construct functional connectivity networks from the binarized time-series to directly relate changes in critical dynamics to alterations to network topologies and computational capabilities [68, 69].

The relative differences in the frequency of large avalanches between the three conditions may also be relevant to work with the Global Workspace Theory (GWT). Previous work has proposed that large avalanches represent the “ignition” of information percolating through the neuronal global workspace [70, 71]. In the context of the GWT, this percolation is thought to define the difference between information processing that is “conscious”, from processing that is “unconscious” [72], and so ketamine may allow consciousness to persist (in contrast to propofol) by failing to disrupt the ability of information to “ignite” and propagate into the Global Workspace.

Indicators of critical dynamics

The other indicators of criticality are harder to interpret. All conditions showed similar patterns of critical exponent relations, with the value of 1/σνz calculated from the avalanche shape collapse showing a high degree of agreement when calculated from the regression of the average avalanche size for a given avalanche duration. In contrast, both values of 1/σνz showed less consistency when compared to the exponent relation α − 1/τ − 1. Finally, when individual channels were subsampled and rescaled, all conditions showed reasonable signs of collapse, with ketamine showing the most significant signs of operating near the critical point.

None of the states showed consistently tighter exponent relations, although we should stress that all exponents were calculated within the range of xmin and xmax for which the power-law hypothesis fit with p-values0.2. The propofol condition had a dramatically reduced range of xmin and xmax values compared to awake and ketamine, so whatever inferences about the plausibility of critical dynamics we make from these data is only relevant within these ranges. Consequently, although all three conditions showed reasonably similar behaviour in terms of exponent relations, overall, the awake and ketamine conditions showed this behaviour over 2 orders of magnitude, while propofol showed it over one tenth of that range. Based on these, we propose that the brain is able to support critical dynamics in all three states, but that propofol (but not ketamine) reduces the scale over which critical dynamics can occur. Critical dynamics are often described as being ‘scale-free’, so the notion of restricting critical dynamics to a range of scales may seen contradictory. However perfect scale-freeness of critical systems only occurs in infinite systems. In all finite systems, criticality can emerge in a restricted range. It is worth unpacking how this restriction might play out in the brain. Two dynamical changes might restrict the range over which the power-law held. An increase in high-frequency noise will have the effect of driving a deviation from power-law scaling at the upper end of the distribution, while limiting the diverging correlation length will drive a deviation from power-laws at the lower end of the tail. By examining how the xmin and xmax values change between conditions, we can better understand the changing dynamics. In the propofol condition, for both avalanche sizes and durations, the values of xmin and xmax are shifted up the distribution, so the power-law fit begins and ends with smaller, shorter lived avalanches. This could be consistent with both a reduction in high-frequency noise, as well as a decreasing correlation length, both of which are consistent with other, well-established elements of propofol anaesthesia. As previously mentioned, a leading hypothesis is that anaesthetics like propofol “fragment” brain networks [64, 73, 74], or alternately “mute” the flow of information [75] between regions. In the context of functional connectivity analysis (a core element of many of these analyses), decreased connectivity can be directly related to a decrease in correlation length, which is consistent with the loss of power-law behaviour in the tails of the distributions. The decrease in high-frequency noise is likely associated with the increase in high-power, low-frequency oscillations that characterize the state induced by propofol [76, 77].

This shrinking of the critical zone may provide an explanation for the original motivating question for this study: why do critical dynamics seem preserved in unconscious systems at the micro-scale [16, 17, 27], but are altered at the macroscale [26, 62]? It may be that at the local level, neural networks are able to maintain critical dynamics, but the ability of these local ensembles to coordinate is impaired [64, 65]. Relevant to this hypothesis is the finding that anaesthetics alter the functioning of pyramidal neurons in Layer IV and V of the cortex [78, 79]. As Layer V pyramidal neurons are thought to serve as “outputs” from one brain region to another [80] and so changes to pyramidal neural functioning may explain how local critical dynamics are able to propagate to macro-scale critical dynamics. Layer V pyramidal neurons expressing 5-HT2A receptors have already been implicated in macro-scale critical dynamics [25] in the context of serotonergic drugs, so a possible involvement in anaesthesia is not totally out of the question.

The question about relative ranges is significant for the exponent relationship results, but not necessarily for the universality and sub-sampling results, which are probably the hardest to interpret. All three conditions showed roughly equivalent degrees of shape collapse upon time-series re-binning, suggesting that all three display self-similar dynamics across channels. One possible interpretation is that, despite the alteration in consciousness induced by propofol and ketamine, the brain is able to adaptively maintain at least some qualities of critical dynamics. Previous work has found that the brain appears to adapt to perturbations and return to the critical regime despite alterations to incoming sensory inputs [16, 81]. One possible explanation for the persistence of signs of criticality is that the brains are rapidly adapting to the drug state. If this is the case, it presents an intriguing window of possibility: might it be possible to break “criticality” writ-large down into separate phenomena and explore which ones are necessary (or sufficient) for complex consciousness and cognition, and which may be irrelevant?

Complexity

We also found that, using a multi-scale measure of complexity [41], the propofol condition was associated with dramatic decreases in the complexity of brain activity, while ketamine preserved higher levels of complexity.

The most dramatic difference is the effect of the anaesthetics on multi-scale complexity. This measure has been suggested to be relevant to the emergence of phenomenological consciousness [82]. Our findings are consistent with tendency of ketamine to preserve consciousness in states of dreamlike “dissociative anaesthesia” in contrast to propofol. It is unsurprising that highly complex behavioural states should be underpinned by complex dynamics, and evidence from studies of anaesthesia [8388], disorders of consciousness [8991], sleep [92], and psychedelic states [25, 26] bears this out. Our results are consistent with, and extend these previous findings using a more principled measure of “complexity” than randomness-based measures.

Limitations

One of the most significant limitations of this work is that it rests on the assumption that, when in the ketamine condition, the monkey was experiencing dissociative anaesthesia, as opposed to true anaesthesia (as it presumably experienced under propofol). The dosage used in the original study (5.1mg/kg) were consistent with doses used for pre-surgical anaesthesia in primates [32] and it is unclear at what dose dissociative anaesthesia transitions into “total” anaesthesia (with no dream-like content at all). Presumably even in cases of “true” ketamine anaesthesia, the subject passes through hallucinatory states during induction and emergence [3537]. It is also not entirely clear that, even if the dose was appropriate to induce hallucinatory states of dissociative anaesthesia in humans, whether macaques experience such a state at all. Despite this limitation, the fact that ketamine maintained wakefulness-like dynamics on multiple measures implies that it is not inconceivable that whatever processes allow consciousness to persist under ketamine in humans are also playing out in the macaque brain.

There is also a question about how the difference between the open eyes in the awake condition, compared to the closed eyes in the anaesthesia conditions, may be affecting neural dynamics. It could be argued that the awake state may be less stationary than either anaesthesia states, as the monkey is still able to engage with it’s environment despite the restraints. The documentation does not make it clear how much potentially stimulating activity was taking place during the period of awake recording, although the monkey is described as “calmly sitting for periods up to 20 minutes” (http://wiki.neurotycho.org/Anesthesia_and_Sleep_Task_Details). While this may be considered a limitation, we argue that alertness and responsiveness to the environment is actually a key component to understanding “normal” waking consciousness. Awareness of, and responsiveness to, the environment is a key element of what it means to be conscious, and so while the effects of incoming stimuli on neural data remain a fascinating outstanding question, we do not think that their presence is incompatible with our goal of understanding the differences between these three states of consciousness.

Having only a single viable monkey to analyse is another significant limitation, although given the rarity of this dataset, we believe the results are still informative. This work should, however, be replicated in a larger cohort of humans or animals undergoing similar anaesthesia and recording procedures. Concerns about the low power are at least somewhat ameliorated by the fact that, for our monkey, we have 2× scans in both the propofol and ketamine conditions, and 4× scans in the awake condition. In general, the results were quite consistent within conditions, suggesting that, at least for this individual, the effects observed here are reasonably robust.

Finally, the analyses described here require binarizing continuous time-series data, which throws out a considerable amount of information (although point-processes like this have been found retain surprising amounts of information, see [47, 66]). For this study, the point process was necessary as the indicators of critical dynamics we explored here were derived from models of binary branching processes [17]. Future analyses that incorporate the full, continuous time series may provide insights missing from this current body of work. The method that we used to define an “event” (an above-threshold excursion in one channel or a sequence in another channel that is significantly correlated with the excursion) partially addresses this concern by including temporal similarity as a criteria by which an event could be identified, but further improvements are no doubt possible.

Conclusions

These results show that, despite their shared status as anaesthetics and the similarity of the external appearance of effects, propofol and ketamine cause different brain states typified by dramatically different neural dynamics. Unlike propofol, ketamine allows for large, long-lasting avalanches of coordinated activity to persist in the cortex in a manner similar to normal consciousness, as well as maintaining a much higher degree of multi-scale complexity. These results may explain why the state induced by ketamine can often include complex, phenomenological consciousness to persist in a dissociated, dream-like state, while propofol extinguishes awareness completely. Understanding which brain dynamics are necessary or sufficient to support consciousness is of interest to both for those interested in the theoretical basis of conscious awareness as well as addressing clinical concerns related to identifying covert consciousness in patients who may not be able to directly communicate.

Glossary

  • Anaesthetic: A drug that reversibly disrupts normal consciousness, inducing a state of coma-like unconsciousness.

  • Criticality: Refers to a system on the boundary between two qualitatively distinct phases. A canonical example from physics is boiling water, which is at the critical boundary between liquid and gas.

  • Metastability: Refers to a dynamical regime where the system orbits a set of competing attractors without any one “winning.” Associated with the maintenance of complex, adaptive behaviour.

  • Power Law: A statistical distribution where P(X = x) ∝ xα. Critical systems are expected to produce power law distributions of events, although non-critical systems can produce them as well.

  • Universality: When a complex system has the same critical exponents even when smaller scale details are varied, it is said to belong to a universality class.

Supporting information

S1 Table. All of the results referred in Table 1 as a.csv file.

(CSV)

S1 Scripts. Scripts required to recreate data analysis and figures.

(TAR.GZ)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Yang-Yeol Ahn for advice on power-law inference and visualization, Dr. Filippo Radicci for insights and discussion, as well as Joshua Faskowitz and Dr. Alice Patania for support, friendship, and advice.

Data Availability

All of the data is available for free download from the NeuroTycho website: http://neurotycho.org/anesthesia-and-sleep-task.

Funding Statement

TFV is supported by the NSF-NRT grant 1735095, Interdisciplinary Training in Complex Networks and Systems. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Cocchi Luca, Gollo Leonardo L., Zalesky Andrew, and Breakspear Michael. Criticality in the brain: A synthesis of neurobiology, models and cognition. Progress in Neurobiology, 158:132–152, November 2017. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kinouchi Osame and Copelli Mauro. Optimal dynamical range of excitable networks at criticality. Nature Physics, 2(5):348–351, May 2006. 10.1038/nphys289 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Shew Woodrow L., Yang Hongdian, Petermann Thomas, Roy Rajarshi, and Plenz Dietmar. Neuronal Avalanches Imply Maximum Dynamic Range in Cortical Networks at Criticality. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(49):15595–15600, December 2009. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3864-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Larremore Daniel B., Shew Woodrow L., and Restrepo Juan G. Predicting Criticality and Dynamic Range in Complex Networks: Effects of Topology. Physical Review Letters, 106(5):058101, January 2011. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.106.058101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Gautam Shree Hari, Hoang Thanh T., Kylie McClanahan, Grady Stephen K., and Shew Woodrow L. Maximizing Sensory Dynamic Range by Tuning the Cortical State to Criticality. PLOS Computational Biology, 11(12):e1004576, December 2015. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004576 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Breakspear M. Perception of odors by a nonlinear model of the olfactory bulb. International Journal of Neural Systems, 11(2):101–124, April 2001. 10.1142/S0129065701000564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Stewart Craig V. and Plenz Dietmar. Inverted-U profile of dopamine-NMDA-mediated spontaneous avalanche recurrence in superficial layers of rat prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26(31):8148–8159, August 2006. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0723-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Deco Gustavo and Jirsa Viktor K. Ongoing cortical activity at rest: criticality, multistability, and ghost attractors. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32(10):3366–3375, March 2012. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2523-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Yang Dong-Ping, Zhou Hai-Jun, and Zhou Changsong. Co-emergence of multi-scale cortical activities of irregular firing, oscillations and avalanches achieves cost-efficient information capacity. PLOS Computational Biology, 13(2):e1005384, February 2017. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005384 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Beggs John M. and Plenz Dietmar. Neuronal Avalanches in Neocortical Circuits. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(35):11167–11177, December 2003. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11167.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Shew Woodrow L., Yang Hongdian, Yu Shan, Roy Rajarshi, and Plenz Dietmar. Information Capacity and Transmission Are Maximized in Balanced Cortical Networks with Neuronal Avalanches. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(1):55–63, January 2011. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4637-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Fagerholm Erik D., Dinov Martin, Thomas Knöpfel, and Leech Robert. The characteristic patterns of neuronal avalanches in mice under anaesthesia and at rest: An investigation using constrained artificial neural networks. PloS One, 13(5):e0197893, 2018. 10.1371/journal.pone.0197893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Timme Nicholas M., Marshall Najja J., Bennett Nicholas, Ripp Monica, Lautzenhiser Edward, and Beggs John M. Criticality Maximizes Complexity in Neural Tissue. Frontiers in Physiology, 7, 2016. 10.3389/fphys.2016.00425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Shriki Oren, Alstott Jeff, Carver Frederick, Holroyd Tom, Henson Richard N.A., Smith Marie L., Coppola Richard, Bullmore Edward, and Plenz Dietmar. Neuronal Avalanches in the Resting MEG of the Human Brain. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(16):7079–7090, April 2013. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Adrián Ponce-Alvarez, Jouary Adrien, Privat Martin, Deco Gustavo, and Sumbre Germán. Whole-Brain Neuronal Activity Displays Crackling Noise Dynamics. Neuron, 100(6):1446–1459.e6, December 2018. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Shew Woodrow L., Clawson Wesley P., Pobst Jeff, Karimipanah Yahya, Wright Nathaniel C., and Wessel Ralf. Adaptation to sensory input tunes visual cortex to criticality. Nature Physics, 11(8):659–663, August 2015. 10.1038/nphys3370 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Friedman Nir, Ito Shinya, Brinkman Braden A. W., Shimono Masanori, Lee DeVille R. E., Dahmen Karin A., Beggs John M., and Butler Thomas C. Universal critical dynamics in high resolution neuronal avalanche data. Physical Review Letters, 108(20):208102, May 2012. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.108.208102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Mazzoni Alberto, Broccard Frédéric D., Elizabeth Garcia-Perez, Bonifazi Paolo, Ruaro Maria Elisabetta, and Torre Vincent. On the Dynamics of the Spontaneous Activity in Neuronal Networks. PLoS ONE, 2(5), May 2007. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000439 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Senzai Yuta, Fernandez-Ruiz Antonio, and Buzsáki György. Layer-Specific Physiological Features and Interlaminar Interactions in the Primary Visual Cortex of the Mouse. Neuron, 101(3):500–513.e5, February 2019. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Petermann Thomas, Thiagarajan Tara C., Lebedev Mikhail A., Nicolelis Miguel A. L., Chialvo Dante R., and Plenz Dietmar. Spontaneous cortical activity in awake monkeys composed of neuronal avalanches. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(37):15921–15926, September 2009. 10.1073/pnas.0904089106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Beggs J. M. and Timme N Being Critical of Criticality in the Brain Frontiers in Physiology, 3 2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Hsu David, Chen Wei, Hsu Murielle, and Beggs John M. An open hypothesis: is epilepsy learned, and can it be unlearned? Epilepsy & behavior: E&B, 13(3):511–522, October 2008. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Meisel Christian, Storch Alexander, Hallmeyer-Elgner Susanne, Bullmore Ed, and Gross Thilo. Failure of Adaptive Self-Organized Criticality during Epileptic Seizure Attacks. PLoS Computational Biology, 8(1), January 2012. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Carhart-Harris, R. L. and Friston, K. J. REBUS and the Anarchic Brain: Toward a Unified Model of the Brain Action of Psychedelics, Pharmacological Reviews, 71, 2019-07-10 10.1124/pr.118.017160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 25. Carhart-Harris Robin Lester, Leech Robert, Hellyer Peter John, Shanahan Murray, Feilding Amanda, Tagliazucchi Enzo, Chialvo Dante R., and Nutt David. The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 2014. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Carhart-Harris Robin L. The entropic brain—revisited. Neuropharmacology, 142:167–178, November 2018. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Fontenele Antonio J., de Vasconcelos Nivaldo A.P., Feliciano Thaís, Aguiar Leandro A.A., Carina Soares-Cunha, Bárbara Coimbra, Porta Leonardo Dalla, Ribeiro Sidarta, Rodrigues Ana João, Sousa Nuno, Carelli Pedro V., and Copelli Mauro. Criticality between Cortical States. Physical Review Letters, 122(20):208101, May 2019. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.122.208101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Scott Gregory, Fagerholm Erik D., Mutoh Hiroki, Leech Robert, Sharp David J., Shew Woodrow L., and Thomas Knöpfel. Voltage Imaging of Waking Mouse Cortex Reveals Emergence of Critical Neuronal Dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(50):16611–16620, December 2014. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3474-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Fekete Tomer, Omer David B., Kazunori O’Hashi, Grinvald Amiram, van Leeuwen Cees, and Shriki Oren. Critical dynamics, anesthesia and information integration: Lessons from multi-scale criticality analysis of voltage imaging data. NeuroImage, 183:919–933, December 2018. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Lee Heonsoo, Golkowski Daniel, Jordan Denis, Berger Sebastian, Ilg Rüdiger, Lee Joseph, Mashour George A., Lee UnCheol, Avidan Michael S., Stefanie Blain-Moraes, Golmirzaie Goodarz, Hardie Randall, Hogg Rosemary, Janke Ellen, Kelz Max B., Maier Kaitlyn, Mashour George A., Maybrier Hannah, Andrew McKinstry-Wu, Muench Maxwell, Ochroch Andrew, Palanca Ben J. A., Picton Paul, Marlon Schwarz E., Tarnal Vijay, Vanini Giancarlo, and Vlisides Phillip E. Relationship of critical dynamics, functional connectivity, and states of consciousness in large-scale human brain networks. NeuroImage, December 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Yanagawa Toru, Chao Zenas C., Hasegawa Naomi, and Fujii Naotaka. Large-Scale Information Flow in Conscious and Unconscious States: an ECoG Study in Monkeys. PLoS ONE, 8(11), November 2013. 10.1371/journal.pone.0080845 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Nagasaka Yasuo, Shimoda Kentaro, and Fujii Naotaka. Multidimensional recording (MDR) and data sharing: an ecological open research and educational platform for neuroscience. PloS One, 6(7):e22561, 2011. 10.1371/journal.pone.0022561 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Hemmings Hugh C., Riegelhaupt Paul M., Kelz Max B., Solt Ken, Eckenhoff Roderic G., Orser Beverley A., and Goldstein Peter A. Towards a Comprehensive Understanding of Anesthetic Mechanisms of Action: A Decade of Discovery. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 40(7):464–481, July 2019. 10.1016/j.tips.2019.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Khan Khurram Saleem, Hayes Ivan, and Buggy Donal J. Pharmacology of anaesthetic agents I: intravenous anaesthetic agents. Continuing Education in Anaesthesia Critical Care & Pain, 14(3):100–105, June 2014. 10.1093/bjaceaccp/mkt039 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Zanos Panos, Moaddel Ruin, Morris Patrick J., Riggs Lace M., Highland Jaclyn N., Georgiou Polymnia, Pereira Edna F. R., Albuquerque Edson X., Thomas Craig J., Zarate Carlos A., and Gould Todd D. ketamine and ketamine Metabolite Pharmacology: Insights into Therapeutic Mechanisms. Pharmacological Reviews, 70(3):621–660, July 2018. 10.1124/pr.117.015198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Domino E. F., Chodoff P., and Corssen G. Pharmacologic effects of CI-581, a new dissociative anesthetic, in man. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 6:279–291, June 1965. 10.1002/cpt196563279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Krystal J. H., Karper L. P., Seibyl J. P., Freeman G. K., Delaney R., Bremner J. D., Heninger G. R., Bowers M. B., and Charney D. S. Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. Psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51(3):199–214, March 1994. 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950030035004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Hari Ritta and Puce Aina. MEG-EEG Primer. Oxford University Press, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Solovey Guillermo, Miller Kai J., Ojemann Jeffrey G., Magnasco Marcelo O., and Cecchi Guillermo A. Self-Regulated Dynamical Criticality in Human ECoG. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, July 2012. 10.3389/fnint.2012.00044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Alonso Leandro M., Proekt Alex, Schwartz Theodore H., Pryor Kane O., Cecchi Guillermo A., and Magnasco Marcelo O. Dynamical criticality during induction of anesthesia in human ECoG recordings. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 8, 2014. 10.3389/fncir.2014.00020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Tononi G., Sporns O., and Edelman G. M. A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 91(11):5033–5037, May 1994. 10.1073/pnas.91.11.5033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Delorme Arnaud and Makeig Scott. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1):9–21, March 2004. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Gramfort Alexandre, Luessi Martin, Larson Eric, Engemann Denis A., Strohmeier Daniel, Brodbeck Christian, Goj Roman, Jas Mainak, Brooks Teon, Parkkonen Lauri, and Matti Hämäläinen. MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 2013. 10.3389/fnins.2013.00267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Meisel Christian, Olbrich Eckehard, Shriki Oren, and Achermann Peter. Fading Signatures of Critical Brain Dynamics during Sustained Wakefulness in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(44):17363–17372, October 2013. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1516-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Bak Per and Chen Kan. Self-Organized Criticality. Scientific American, 264(1):46–53, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Matias Palva J., Zhigalov Alexander, Hirvonen Jonni, Korhonen Onerva, Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen, and Palva Satu. Neuronal long-range temporal correlations and avalanche dynamics are correlated with behavioral scaling laws. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(9):3585–3590, February 2013. 10.1073/pnas.1216855110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Tagliazucchi Enzo, Balenzuela Pablo, Fraiman Daniel, and Chialvo Dante R. Criticality in Large-Scale Brain fMRI Dynamics Unveiled by a Novel Point Process Analysis. Frontiers in Physiology, 3, 2012. 10.3389/fphys.2012.00015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Marshall Najja, Timme Nicholas M., Bennett Nicholas, Ripp Monica, Lautzenhiser Edward, and Beggs John M. Analysis of Power Laws, Shape Collapses, and Neural Complexity: New Techniques and MATLAB Support via the NCC Toolbox. Frontiers in Physiology, 7, 2016. 10.3389/fphys.2016.00250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Clauset A., Shalizi C., and Newman M. Power-Law Distributions in Empirical Data. SIAM Review, 51(4):661–703, November 2009. 10.1137/070710111 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Newman M. E. J. Power laws, Pareto distributions and Zipf’s law. Contemporary Physics, 46(5):323–351, September 2005. arXiv: cond-mat/0412004. 10.1080/00107510500052444 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Yu Shan, Klaus Andreas, Yang Hongdian, and Plenz Dietmar. Scale-Invariant Neuronal Avalanche Dynamics and the Cut-Off in Size Distributions. PLOS ONE, 9(6):e99761, June 2014. 10.1371/journal.pone.0099761 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Burroughs S.M. and Tebbens S.F. Upper-truncated Power Laws in Natural Systems. pure and applied geophysics, 158(4):741–757, April 2001. 10.1007/PL00001202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Priesemann Viola, Munk Matthias HJ, and Wibral Michael. Subsampling effects in neuronal avalanche distributions recorded in vivo. BMC Neuroscience, 10(1):40, April 2009. 10.1186/1471-2202-10-40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Ribeiro Tiago L., Ribeiro Sidarta, Belchior Hindiael, Fábio Caixeta, and Copelli Mauro. Undersampled Critical Branching Processes on Small-World and Random Networks Fail to Reproduce the Statistics of Spike Avalanches. PLOS ONE, 9(4):e94992, April 2014. 10.1371/journal.pone.0094992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Villani C. (2009). Optimal Transport: Old and New. Springer-Verlag; 10.1007/978-3-540-71050-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Watanabe S. Information Theoretical Analysis of Multivariate Correlation. IBM Journal of Research and Development, 4(1):66–82, January 1960. 10.1147/rd.41.0066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Ziv J. and Lempel A. Compression of individual sequences via variable-rate coding. IEEE Transactions on Information Theory, 24(5):530–536, September 1978. 10.1109/TIT.1978.1055934 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Ferenets R., Lipping Tarmo, Anier A., Jantti V., Melto S., and Hovilehto S. Comparison of entropy and complexity measures for the assessment of depth of sedation. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 53(6):1067–1077, June 2006. 10.1109/TBME.2006.873543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Kekovic Goran, Stojadinovic Gordana, Martac Ljiljana, Podgorac Jelena, Sekulic Slobodan, and Culic Milka. Spectral and fractal measures of cerebellar and cerebral activity in various types of anesthesia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 70(1):67–75, 2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Kesić Srdjan and Spasić Sladjana Z. Application of Higuchi’s fractal dimension from basic to clinical neurophysiology: A review. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 133:55–70, September 2016. 10.1016/j.cmpb.2016.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Ruiz de Miras J., Soler F., Iglesias-Parro S., Ibáñez-Molina A. J., Casali A. G., Laureys S., Massimini M., Esteban F. J., Navas J., and Langa J. A. Fractal dimension analysis of states of consciousness and unconsciousness using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 175:129–137, July 2019. 10.1016/j.cmpb.2019.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Colombo M. A., Napolitani M., Boly M., Gosseries O., Casarotto S., Rosanova M., Brichant J.-F., Boveroux P., Rex S., Laureys S., Massimini M., Chieregato A., & Sarasso S. (2019). The spectral exponent of the resting EEG indexes the presence of consciousness during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine. NeuroImage, 189, 631–644. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Muthukumaraswamy Suresh D. and Liley David TJ. 1/f electrophysiological spectra in resting and drug-induced states can be explained by the dynamics of multiple oscillatory relaxation processes. NeuroImage, 179:582–595, October 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Lewis Laura D., Weiner Veronica S., Mukamel Eran A., Donoghue Jacob A., Eskandar Emad N., Madsen Joseph R., Anderson William S., Hochberg Leigh R., Cash Sydney S., Brown Emery N., and Purdon Patrick L. Rapid fragmentation of neuronal networks at the onset of propofol-induced unconsciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(49):E3377–3386, December 2012. 10.1073/pnas.1210907109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Mashour George A. and Hudetz Anthony G. Neural Correlates of Unconsciousness in Large-Scale Brain Networks. Trends in Neurosciences, 41(3):150–160, 2018. 10.1016/j.tins.2018.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Tagliazucchi Enzo, Carhart-Harris Robin, Leech Robert, Nutt David, and Chialvo Dante R. Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience. Human Brain Mapping, 35(11):5442–5456, November 2014. arXiv: 1405.6466. 10.1002/hbm.22562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Timme Nicholas, Alford Wesley, Flecker Benjamin, and Beggs John M. Synergy, redundancy, and multivariate information measures: an experimentalist’s perspective. Journal of Computational Neuroscience, 36(2):119–140, April 2014. 10.1007/s10827-013-0458-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Timme Nicholas M., Ito Shinya, Myroshnychenko Maxym, Nigam Sunny, Shimono Masanori, Yeh Fang-Chin, Hottowy Pawel, Litke Alan M., and Beggs John M. High-Degree Neurons Feed Cortical Computations. PLoS computational biology, 12(5):e1004858, 2016. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004858 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Faber Samantha P., Timme Nicholas M., Beggs John M., and Newman Ehren L. Computation is concentrated in rich clubs of local cortical networks. Network Neuroscience, pages 1–21, September 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Stanislas Dehaene. Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts. Penguin, January 2014. Google-Books-ID: CWw2AAAAQBAJ.
  • 71. Mashour G. A., Roelfsema P., Changeux J.-P., & Dehaene S. (2020). Conscious Processing and the Global Neuronal Workspace Hypothesis. Neuron, 105(5), 776–798. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Robinson Richard. Exploring the “Global Workspace” of Consciousness. PLOS Biology, 7(3):e1000066, March 2009. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Hudetz A. G. and Mashour G. A. (2016). Disconnecting consciousness: Is there a common anesthetic end-point? Anesthesia and analgesia, 123(5):1228 10.1213/ANE.0000000000001353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Lee U., Kim S., Noh G.-J., Choi B.-M., Hwang E., & Mashour G. A. (2009). The directionality and functional organization of frontoparietal connectivity during consciousness and anesthesia in humans. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 18(4), 1069–1078. 10.1016/j.concog.2009.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Areshenkoff C. N., Nashed J. Y., Hutchison R. M., Hutchison M., Levy R., Cook D. J., Menon R. S., Everling S., & Gallivan J. P. (2020). Muting, not fragmentation, of functional brain networks under general anesthesia. BioRxiv, 2020.07.08.188011. 10.1101/2020.07.08.188011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Hutt A. (2013). The anesthetic propofol shifts the frequency of maximum spectral power in EEG during general anesthesia: Analytical insights from a linear model. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 7 10.3389/fncom.2013.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Purdon P. L., Pierce E. T., Mukamel E. A., Prerau M. J., Walsh J. L., Wong K. F. K., Salazar-Gomez A. F., Harrell P. G., Sampson A. L., Cimenser A., Ching S., Kopell N. J., Tavares-Stoeckel C., Habeeb K., Merhar R., & Brown E. N. (2013). Electroencephalogram signatures of loss and recovery of consciousness from propofol. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(12), E1142–E1151. 10.1073/pnas.1221180110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Potez Sarah and Larkum Matthew E. Effect of common anesthetics on dendritic properties in layer 5 neocortical pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99(3):1394–1407, March 2008. 10.1152/jn.01126.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Suzuki Mototaka and Larkum Matthew E. General Anesthesia Decouples Cortical Pyramidal Neurons. Cell, 180(4):666–676.e13, February 2020. 10.1016/j.cell.2020.01.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Spruston Nelson. Pyramidal neurons: dendritic structure and synaptic integration. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(3):206–221, March 2008. 10.1038/nrn2286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Ma, Zhengyu and Turrigiano, Gina G. and Wessel, Ralf and Hengen, Keith B. Cortical Circuit Dynamics Are Homeostatically Tuned to Criticality In Vivo Neuron, 2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 82. Tononi Giulio. Consciousness as Integrated Information: a Provisional Manifesto. The Biological Bulletin, 215(3):216–242, December 2008. 10.2307/25470707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Sarasso Simone, Boly Melanie, Napolitani Martino, Gosseries Olivia, Charland-Verville Vanessa, Casarotto Silvia, Rosanova Mario, Casali Adenauer Girardi, Brichant Jean-Francois, Boveroux Pierre, Rex Steffen, Tononi Giulio, Laureys Steven, and Massimini Marcello. Consciousness and Complexity during Unresponsiveness Induced by propofol, Xenon, and ketamine. Current Biology, 25(23):3099–3105, December 2015. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Schartner Michael, Seth Anil, Noirhomme Quentin, Boly Melanie, Bruno Marie-Aurelie, Laureys Steven, and Barrett Adam. Complexity of Multi-Dimensional Spontaneous EEG Decreases during propofol Induced General Anaesthesia. PLOS ONE, 10(8):e0133532, August 2015. 10.1371/journal.pone.0133532 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Wang Jisung, Noh Gyu-Jeong, Choi Byung-Moon, Ku Seung-Woo, Joo Pangyu, Jung Woo-Sung, Kim Seunghwan, and Lee Heonsoo. Suppressed neural complexity during ketamine- and propofol-induced unconsciousness. Neuroscience Letters, 653:320–325, July 2017. 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.05.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Wenzel M., Han S., Smith E. H., Hoel E., Greger B., House P. A., & Yuste R. (2019). Reduced Repertoire of Cortical Microstates and Neuronal Ensembles in Medically Induced Loss of Consciousness. Cell Systems. 10.1016/j.cels.2019.03.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Varley T. F., Luppi A. I., Pappas I., Naci L., Adapa R., Owen A. M., Menon D. K., Stamatakis E. A. Consciousness & Brain Functional Complexity in propofol Anaesthesia. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–13. 10.1038/s41598-020-57695-3 (2020) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Brito Michael A. and Li Duan and Mashour George A. and Pal Dinesh (2020) State-Dependent and Bandwidth-Specific Effects of Ketamine and Propofol on Electroencephalographic Complexity in Rats Frontiers https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnsys.2020.00050/full [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Bodart Olivier and Gosseries Olivia and Wannez Sarah and Thibaut Aurore and Annen Jitka and Boly Melanie and Rosanova Mario and Casali Adenauer G. and Casarotto Silvia and Tononi Giulio and Massimini Marcello and Laureys Steven Measures of metabolism and complexity in the brain of patients with disorders of consciousness Neuroimage: Clinical, 14, 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Thul Alexander and Lechinger Julia and Donis Johann and Michitsch Gabriele and Pichler Gerald and Kochs Eberhard F. and Jordan Denis and Ilg Rüdiger and Schabus Manuel EEG entropy measures indicate decrease of cortical information processing in Disorders of Consciousness Clinical Neurophysiology, 127 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.07.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Casali Adenauer G., Gosseries Olivia, Rosanova Mario, Mélanie Boly, Sarasso Simone, Casali Karina R., Casarotto Silvia, Bruno Marie-Aurélie, Laureys Steven, Tononi Giulio, and Massimini Marcello. A Theoretically Based Index of Consciousness Independent of Sensory Processing and Behavior. Science Translational Medicine, 5(198):198ra105–198ra105, August 2013. 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Schartner Michael M., Pigorini Andrea, Gibbs Steve A., Arnulfo Gabriele, Sarasso Simone, Barnett Lionel, Nobili Lino, Massimini Marcello, Seth Anil K., and Barrett Adam B. Global and local complexity of intracranial EEG decreases during NREM sleep. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), January 2017. 10.1093/nc/niw022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008418.r001

Decision Letter 0

Saad Jbabdi, Kim T Blackwell

24 Jun 2020

Hi

The three reviewers raise a number of concerns, but two of them are particularly salient:

(1) is the work sufficiently novel.

(2) can conclusions be drawn from such a small sample size?

I would like to invite the authors to convincingly address these concerns alongside the other points raised by the reviewers.

Best wishes

Saad

---------------

Dear Mr Varley,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Differential Effects of Propofol and Ketamine on Critical Brain Dynamics" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology.

As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. In light of the reviews (below this email), we would like to invite the resubmission of a significantly-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments.

We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent to reviewers for further evaluation.

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to the review comments and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 60 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. Please note that revised manuscripts received after the 60-day due date may require evaluation and peer review similar to newly submitted manuscripts.

Thank you again for your submission. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Saad Jbabdi

Associate Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Kim Blackwell

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: Using ECoG recordings in a macaque brain, this study investigated the effect of different anesthetics (propofol and ketamine) on critical brain dynamics. Consistent with different mechanisms of the two anesthetics, they found propofol, but not ketamine, dramatically restricted the size and duration of avalanches, as well as a large reduction in the complexity of brain dynamics. Overall, I think this is a well-conducted study. The methodology is sound, the conclusion helps to improve our understanding of altered states of consciousness and brain dynamics.

My main concern is about the small sample size. I agree with the authors about the rarity of the dataset, but with only N=4 for wake sessions and N=2 for propofol and ketamine, I think the results with mean+/-SD is inappropriate and somewhat misleading. I would suggest changing the descriptive statistics in the main text (perhaps only mean, or the range like minimum-maximum?), and moving the individual session results in supplemental table 1 to main text for clarity.

Line 122. Please clearly specify the number of scans for each condition (wake, propofol, ketamine).

Line 132. Please specify the sampling frequency of the data.

Line 156-157. The description of the method is not clear. For example, which correlation function was used? Is ρ correlation coefficient? What do t_min and t_max mean?

Line 192. So, x denotes avalanche size or duration, and minimum and maximum values of x, which were named as x_min and x_max in line 234? Please clarify.

Line 319. For avalanche size distribution, with only 128 channels of ECoG, how to get the maximum values of 378? Was this derived from fitted data or empirical data?

Figures 7, 8 and 10 showed the results from two scans? Please appropriately describe this in the legend.

Line 516. I would suggest a brief discussion on the effect of eyes open on awake results, and on the observed difference between awake and unresponsive conditions (propofol/ketamine).

Reviewer #2: The authors studied the differential effects of propofol and ketamine on the critical brain dynamics of a single macaque. A previously validated test of criticality, avalanche dynamics, was applied to analyze ECoG data of propofol and ketamine that induce differentiable effects on consciousness. Many previous studies with human and animal subjects suggested that maintenance of critical dynamics is necessary for the emergence of consciousness. However, controversially, some studies also demonstrated criticality in unconscious states. Thus, the authors tried to fill the knowledge gap and showed that propofol dramatically restricted the size and duration of avalanches, while ketamine allowed for more awake-like dynamics to persist. And propofol produces a dramatic reduction in the complexity but all states show some signs of persistent criticality. The author concluded that maintenance of critical brain dynamics may be important for regulation and control of conscious awareness.

The paper was well written and published timely. The authors provided proper background and relevant knowledge. The novelty and the need for this study appear clearly. I think this study may provide many insights to the researchers in this research field especially on the controversial findings of criticality in conscious and unconscious states.

Despite this paper was well written, I feel this study has a serious limitation that has already been mentioned by the authors. That is, all the results were drawn only from a single monkey. Even though several ECoG recordings were analyzed for two different anesthetics, still I doubt whether the results could be reproducible with other subjects. Considering the ambiguity of determining consciousness which mainly depends on the subject’s responsiveness, in particular, with monkey it is worse than human subject. The authors cannot completely get rid of the possibility that the macaque was in covert consciousness. The subject could have covert consciousness if the dose of propofol was not enough to induce deep anesthesia or the anesthetic concentration was not maintained during the ECoG recording (in this study, the concentration was not maintained), or if the level of consciousness fluctuated after the inintal induction, the subject could have woken up for a moment. Because of these possibilities, the authors did not remove, it seems difficult the authors can conclude that the signs of criticality were observed in the unconscious state. Without gatering more subjects, it would be difficult to improve this critical limitation.

Reviewer #3: Varley and colleagues investigated the properties of non-human primate ECoG data from the perspective of critical dynamics, with the hypothesis that consciousness (perhaps more adequately, responsiveness) would correlate with critical behaviour. They found some evidence supporting that propofol (but not ketamine) disrupts critical dynamics.

While this direction of research is interesting, the results are hardly novel at this stage. The authors cited several papers showing a departure from critical dynamics induced by general anesthetics, including propofol. This extends to other states of reduced consciousness, such as slow wave sleep or epileptic seizures. Perhaps the most interesting finding is that ketamine resembled wakefulness; although this could also be expected from previous work, I'm not aware it has been shown from the perspective of criticality.

Besides this potential novelty issue, I have the following comments for the authors:

- The use of the word "exotic" to refer to non-ordinary/altered states of consciousness is somewhat strange... is there a reason not to stick to the common nomenclature?

- I checked ref. 25 and it does not include analysis of ECoG recordings acquired under the effect of psychedelics - this would have been somewhat strange, I think, since invasive recordings tend to be acquired in neurological patients only.

- The NeuroTycho dataset has ECoG data acquired during other potentially interesting states of reduced awareness, which, if analyzed by the authors, could contribute to broaden the scope of their conclusions. I'm thinking of the sleep and ketamine plus medetomidine datasets. Is there a reason to exclude these datasets and to focus on the ketamine and propofol recordings only?

- The following hypothesis "propofol would dramatically reduce signs of critical dynamics, but that criticality would persist under the influence of ketamine" is reasonable for sub-anesthetic doses of ketamine, but that is not so clear for anesthetic doses (at least not from the previous paragraphs of the introduction)

- Perhaps the authors should invest additional efforts in model comparison, for instance, how do log normal, exponential, and exponential cutoff models reproduce the avalanche distributions, and how do the goodness of fit compare to those seen for power laws?

- "To avoid interminable run-times, the NCC Toolbox [49] includes several corrections for sub-sampling and heuristics for estimating integration in large systems" -> Maybe some information concerning those heuristics, to make the manuscript more self-contained?

- "Visual inspection shows that theketamine condition tracks the Awake condition much more closely than the propofol condition does" -> Sorry, I really can't see this by visual inspection. I know the authors checked the stats on the manuscript text, but perhaps they should make one or more new figures where these are visualized. For example, the values of the exponents are scattered throughout the text and this makes it difficult to draw quick comparisons. Here a figure could help.

- "We had hypothesized that the Awake and/or ketamine conditions would show the highest degree of concurrence between the measures (reflecting a greater degree of criticality), but instead, the Awake condition has the lowest degree of concurrence" -> I think this deserves more discussion.

- "Based on these, we propose that the brain is able to support critical dynamics in all three states, but that propofol (but not ketamine) reduces the scale over which critical dynamics can occur." -> At this point, I think you need to delve deeper into how this "partial" scale-free dynamics relate to known results from statistical physics. What kind of system would show critical dynamics over a restricted range, and is the brain such a system?

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: None

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Duan Li

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, PLOS recommends that you deposit laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions, please see http://journals.plos.org/compbiol/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008418.r003

Decision Letter 1

Saad Jbabdi, Kim T Blackwell

5 Oct 2020

Dear Mr Varley,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript 'Differential Effects of Propofol and Ketamine on Critical Brain Dynamics' has been provisionally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology.

Before your manuscript can be formally accepted you will need to complete some formatting changes, which you will receive in a follow up email. A member of our team will be in touch with a set of requests.

Please note that your manuscript will not be scheduled for publication until you have made the required changes, so a swift response is appreciated.

IMPORTANT: The editorial review process is now complete. PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling, formatting or significant scientific errors from this point onwards. Requests for major changes, or any which affect the scientific understanding of your work, will cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us now if you or your institution is planning to press release the article. All press must be co-ordinated with PLOS.

Thank you again for supporting Open Access publishing; we are looking forward to publishing your work in PLOS Computational Biology. 

Best regards,

Saad Jbabdi

Associate Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Kim Blackwell

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************************************************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: The authors have addressed my comments appropriately.

Reviewer #2: I think the authors well explained and applied my concerns to the revised manuscript.

Reviewer #3: The authors have addressed all my concerns extensively and adequately, and because of this I recommend this manuscript for publication.

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: None

Reviewer #2: None

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Duan Li

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes: Enzo Tagliazucchi

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008418.r004

Acceptance letter

Saad Jbabdi, Kim T Blackwell

24 Nov 2020

PCOMPBIOL-D-20-00547R1

Differential Effects of Propofol and Ketamine on Critical Brain Dynamics

Dear Dr Varley,

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been formally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript is now with our production department and you will be notified of the publication date in due course.

The corresponding author will soon be receiving a typeset proof for review, to ensure errors have not been introduced during production. Please review the PDF proof of your manuscript carefully, as this is the last chance to correct any errors. Please note that major changes, or those which affect the scientific understanding of the work, will likely cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Soon after your final files are uploaded, unless you have opted out, the early version of your manuscript will be published online. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

Thank you again for supporting PLOS Computational Biology and open-access publishing. We are looking forward to publishing your work!

With kind regards,

Nicola Davies

PLOS Computational Biology | Carlyle House, Carlyle Road, Cambridge CB4 3DN | United Kingdom ploscompbiol@plos.org | Phone +44 (0) 1223-442824 | ploscompbiol.org | @PLOSCompBiol

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. All of the results referred in Table 1 as a.csv file.

    (CSV)

    S1 Scripts. Scripts required to recreate data analysis and figures.

    (TAR.GZ)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: response_to_reviewers_v3.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    All of the data is available for free download from the NeuroTycho website: http://neurotycho.org/anesthesia-and-sleep-task.


    Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES