Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2020 Jul 6;6(2):200–210. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.06.017

Neuroimaging markers of risk and pathways to resilience in autism spectrum disorders

Istvan Molnar-Szakacs 1,*, Lauren Kupis 2, Lucina Q Uddin 2,3,*
PMCID: PMC7785575  NIHMSID: NIHMS1610276  PMID: 32839155

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex, heterogeneous neurodevelopmental condition of largely unknown etiology. This heterogeneity of symptom presentation, combined with high rates of comorbidity with other developmental disorders and a lack of reliable biomarkers, makes diagnosing and evaluating life outcomes for individuals with ASD a challenge. We review the growing literature on neuroimaging-based biomarkers of risk for the development of autism and explore evidence for resilience in some autistic individuals. The current literature suggests that neuroimaging during early infancy, in combination with pre-birth and early genetic studies are promising tools for identifying biomarkers of risk, while studies of gene expression and DNA methylation have provided some key insights into mechanisms of resilience. With genetics and the environment contributing to both risk for the development of ASD and conditions for resilience, additional studies are needed to understand how risk and resilience interact mechanistically, whereby factors of risk may engender conditions for adaptation. Future studies should prioritize longitudinal designs in global cohorts, with the involvement of the autism community as partners in research to help identify domains of functioning that hold value and importance to the community.

Keywords: biomarker, early development, heterogeneity, epigenetic, OXTR, autism community

1. Heterogeneity, risk and resilience in autism

Standardized criteria for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) specify persistent deficits in social communication and interaction, and restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests (1,85). A diagnosis of autism is still determined based on clinician observation in combination with a detailed developmental history (2) rather than biomarker-based characterization (3). Contributors to heterogeneity in autism include diversity in phenotypic presentation, which overlaps with other conditions and the general population, and a broad spectrum of symptom severity leading to a range of prognoses (46).

Genetic and environmental studies have provided evidence that both genetic and nongenetic factors contribute to an increased susceptibility to developing autism (79). Nongenetic factors that increase the risk of autism include epigenetic and environmental factors (10), although the relative contribution of these factors is still not well understood (11,12). Furthermore, interactions between genetic and nongenetic factors may also contribute to autism risk in complex ways (13).

In this review, we will use the terms ‘autism’, ‘autistic’, ‘ASD’, and ‘persons on the autism spectrum’ interchangeably, out of respect for self-advocates (2,14,15) while maintaining consistency with existing terminology in the neuroimaging literature and the nomenclature in the DSM-V (1). Recently, many autism self-advocates have adopted the framework of neurodiversity, seeing autism as an example of diversity in a wide range of brains, none of which are considered ‘normal’ and all of which are different (14,16). The ‘disorder’ and ‘neurodiversity’ views both agree that autism is neurodevelopmental, in that it is characterized by variation in brain function and development (17). However, research has yet to uncover robust and clinically meaningful biomarkers of autism, including neuroimaging-based markers (18).

Resilience is a dynamic process of adaptability in the face of hardship or adversity, that enables adaptation to the context and a better-than-expected outcome. Resilient outcomes do not have to mean supernormal functioning, but rather that the individual is able to maintain or regain a good outcome despite significant adversity (17,1921). This positive adaptation arises from a combination of biological and psychosocial factors that may be specific to the individual (such as IQ, personality traits), the community (family, school), or the culture (customs and expectations) and includes anything that can favorably impact a person’s adaptational systems. Factors promoting resilience may also be specific to a life-stage, or present across the lifespan whereby the restorative effect is sometimes seen years after the initial stressor (22,23).

The construct of resilience in psychology emerged out of the adaptive outcomes observed among persons with schizophrenia beginning in the 1970’s, and was then expanded to include various adversities, such as adverse childhood experiences, catastrophic life events and chronic illness (for a review, see (24)). Decades of research have identified a range of protective factors, also called ‘assets’ or ‘resources’ that contribute to resilience across different contexts of adversity, including: i) higher cognitive abilities or lack of comorbid intellectual disability; ii) higher socioeconomic status; iii) the presence of supportive adults, and iv) access to intensive, high quality interventions (21,22,2426). There are also factors that impact a positive outcome more specifically in research on ASD, such as v) early diagnosis; vi) higher initial functioning prior to treatment, especially early language function; vii) being female; and viii) being an unaffected sibling (2730).

Researchers have proposed various markers of resilience, including the achievement of developmental milestones, cultural competencies, or the absence of psychopathology (21). However, it is important to consider that resilience is dynamic, and positive adaptation in one domain does not necessarily translate to another domain, and depending on the measurement criteria used, resilient functioning may not be captured by researchers (24). This highlights the importance for researchers to work with the autism community to help define which domains of resilience are relevant to the community and would have a valuable impact on quality of life (20). In fact, the Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee (IACC) has called for a ‘paradigm shift in how we approach autism’ towards research that will have a more immediate and direct impact on the daily lives of autistic people and their families (31).

In this review, we highlight how neuroimaging studies of infants and children have provided potential biomarkers of risk for the development of autism and recent genetic and epigenetic studies on the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR), as a possible biomarker of resilience. Considering the dynamic relationship between risk and protective factors, we discuss how multidisciplinary approaches can contribute to parsing heterogeneity in the autism phenotype and elucidating mechanisms of resilience. In order to improve diagnostic biomarkers for autism and trajectories of resilience throughout the lifecourse, we propose that longitudinal, dimensional research is needed, that considers both biological and psychosocial factors that may be common to mechanisms of both risk and resilience (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Risk factors for development of autism and factors affecting resilience across the lifespan.

Figure 1.

In the prenatal period, genetic and epigenetic factors influence risk for development of ASD. Infant sibling studies examining the first years after birth have identified features including brain structure, connectivity and function that may serve as candidate biomarkers of risk for autism. The identification of early genetic and neural risk factors is critical for ensuring accurate early diagnosis, which translates to the development of therapy and intervention strategies targeting the early life period. As children age, family involvement and the school environment become critical to resilience during childhood and adolescence. Community support and access to clinical resources become increasingly important for optimal outcomes for autistic individuals during the transition to adulthood and in later life.

2. Developmental neuroimaging studies reveal neural markers of risk

In the current system of diagnosis based on developmental history and clinician observation, children may not be diagnosed with ASD until the age of 18 to 36 months (32), and may face sociodemographic, geographic, cultural and other barriers delaying intervention, despite evidence of early interventions leading to better outcomes (33,34). While overt behavioral signs of autism are rarely observable in the first year, risk of autism appears to affect a range of systems in the developing brain within this early period (13,35,36). Non-invasive neuroimaging techniques, including magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) are useful tools that may be used to identify early biomarkers for the development of ASD, predict symptom severity, and parse heterogeneity within the ASD phenotype to guide intervention and treatment decisions (10,37).

Table 1 lists studies published in the last decade that have used MRI or EEG to investigate infants at risk for autism (3875). We draw on this literature to highlight key findings.

Table 1. Neuroimaging studies of infants at risk for autism.

Studies were identified through systematic searches in PubMed and Google Scholar with the key words ‘autism’ OR ‘ASD’ AND ‘infancy’ OR ‘infant’ AND ‘risk for ASD’ OR ‘sibling’ OR ‘risk’ AND ‘MRI’ OR ‘resting state’ OR ‘task’ OR ‘DTI’ OR ‘fMRI’ OR ‘connectivity’ OR ‘EEG’. As revealed by our literature review summarized here, no neuroimaging studies assessing risk or predicting outcomes for autism over the past decade have been conducted in countries other than the United States or the United Kingdom, and even in those studies, race or ethnicity of participants was not reported in most cases. Consequently, almost nothing is known about the influence of race and culture on the lifespan trajectories of autistic individuals (2,126128).

Study Number of subjects (HR, LR) Age range (months) Neuroimaging method Biomarker feature Outcomes predicted Country of data collection/Race or ethnicity of subjects (N) Longitudinal or cross-sectional
Blasi et al., 2015 (62) 33(15, 18) 4–7 Task-based
fMRI
Brain responses and sensitivity to human auditory stimuli Language impairments; social outcomes UK/not reported Cross-sectional
Bosl et al., 2011 (57) 79(46, 33) 6–24 EEG mMSE computed from resting state EEG signals Risk for ASD US/not reported Longitudinal* and Cross-sectional
Brito et al., 2019 (56) 129(not reported) 12 h-36 EEG Neonatal EEG Risk for atypical behaviors US/not reported Longitudinal**
Ciarrusta et al., 2019 (54) 36(18, 18) 1–52 d fMRI Synchronous neural activity in the right fusiform and left parietal cortex; anterior segment of the left insula and cingulate cortices Social processing UK/not reported Cross-sectional
Ciarrusta et al., 2020 (101) 40(20, 20) 39–44 PMA w fcMRI Short- and long- range functional connectivity ASD risk UK/not reported Cross-sectional
Elison etal., 2013 (44) 97(56, 41) 7–25 DTI Visual orienting and white matter organization in fiber tracts of the corticospinal pathways and splenium and genu of the CC LR infants US/not reported Longitudinal
Elsabbagh et al., 2011 (47) 16(16, 0) 9–40 EEG ERP responses to static faces with either a direct or averted gaze Social outcomes; stereotyped behaviors and restricted interests severity UK/not reported Longitudinal**
Elsabbagh et al., 2013 (63) 104(54, 50) 6–36 EEG ERP responses to dynamic eye gaze shifts ASD diagnosis UK/not reported Longitudinal**
Emerson et al., 2017 (91) 59(11, 48)a 6–24 fcMRI Functional connectivity ASD diagnosis; ASD symptom severity US/not reported Longitudinal**
Fingher et al., 2017 (42) 97(68, 29) 13–51 DTI Diffusion measures in the temporal CC tract ASD symptom severity US/not reported Longitudinal**
Garbard- Durnam et al., 2015 (54) 108 (57, 51) 6–18 EEG Frontal EEG alpha asymmetry ASD risk US/not reported Longitudinal*
Garbard- Durnam et al., 2019 (55) 171(102, 69) 3–36 EEG EEG power dynamics ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Hazlett et al., 2011 (48) Time point 1: 97(59b, 38); Time point 2: 59(38b, 21) 18–59 sMRI Cerebral gray and white matter volumes and cortical thickness ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Hazlett et al., 2012 (64) 134(98, 36) 6 sMRI Head circumference; brain volume No differences in head circumferen ce or brain volume in 6 month old HR or LR infants US/not reported Cross-sectional
Hazlett et al., 2017 (65) 435(318, 117) [Neuroima ged: 148(106, 42)] 6–24 sMRI Brain growth rate, total brain volume and surface area ASD diagnosis; social outcomes US/Non-white: [HR/LR: 53], White: [HR/LR: 376], Not reported: [HR/LR: 6]; [Neuroima ged: (Not reported)] Longitudinal*
Jones et al., 2016 (41) 88(43, 45) 6–24 EEG EEG responses to social stimuli Social outcomes US/not reported Longitudinal**
Kolesnik et al., 2019 (40) 143(116, 27) [EEG analyzed: 94(80, 14)] 8–36 EEG Cortical ‘hyperreactivity’ in response to a non- linguistic auditory oddball paradigm ASD diagnosis; language outcomes; social outcomes UK/not reported Longitudinal**
Levin et al., 2017 (39) 48(29,19) 3–36 EEG Frontal spectral power Expressive Language outcomes US/not reported Longitudinal*
Lewis et al., 2014 (49) 136(113, 23) 24 DTI Network efficiency ASD Symptom severity US/not reported Cross-sectional
Lewis et al., 2017 (66) 260(not reported); [Longitudinally neuroimag ed: 116(81, 35)] 6–24 DTI Network efficiency ASD symptom severity US/not reported Longitudinal*
Liu et al., 2019 (67) 34(19, 15) 6 w-36 DTI White matter pathways of the dorsal language network Language impairments; ASD symptom severity US/Non-white: [HR/LR: 24], White: [HR/LR: 10] Longitudinal**
Lombardo et al., 2015 (68) 103(60b, 24, 19)c 12–48 Task-based fMRI Superior temporal cortices Language Impairments US/not reported Longitudinal**
McKinnon et al., 2019 (69) 12 mos: 118(87, 31); 24 mos: 87(67, 20) 12–24 fcMRI Functional connectivity Restricted and repetitive behaviors US/not reported Longitudinal*
Orekhova et al., 2014 (56) 54(28, 26) 14–36 EEG Brain connectivity assessed using dbWPLI ASD diagnosis; restricted and repetitive behaviors UK/not reported Longitudinal**
Pote et al., 2019 (70) 50(24, 26) 4–36 sMRI Whole brain, Cerebellar and Subcortical volumes Repetitive behaviors UK/not reported Longitudinal**
Righi et al., 2014 (57) 54(28, 26) 6–12 EEG Functional connectivity in response to speech sounds ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Schumann et al., 2010 (58) 118(74b, 44) [Neuroima ged: 88(41b, 44)] 18–60 sMRI Brain volume growth (gray matter and white matter) ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Seery et al., 2013 (50) 108(62, 46) 6–36 ERP responses to native and non-native speech ASD diagnosis US/not reported Cross-sectional and longitudinal*
Seery et al., 2014 (52) 80(35, 45) 6–18 EEG ERP responses to repeated speech Risk for ASD; language outcomes US/[% Nonwhite (HR: 2.9, LR: 13.6)] Longitudinal**
Shen et al., 2013 (71) 64(41, 23) 6–36 sMRI Extra-axial CSF volume ASD diagnosis; ASD symptom severity US/not reported Longitudinal*
Shen et al., 2017 (72) 343(221, 122) 6–24 sMRI Extra-axial CSF volume ASD diagnosis; ASD symptom severity US/not reported Longitudinal*
Shen et al., 2018 (73) 236(132, 27, 77)d 24–42 sMRI Extra-axial CSF volume ASD diagnosis; sleep disturbances; non-verbal ability US/not reported Cross-sectional
Shephard et al., 2020 (59) 77(42, 35) 7 −7 y EEG ERP responses to faces and visual noise stimuli HR ASD; Social- communicati on UK/not reported Longitudinal**
Swanson et al., 2017 (60) 525(382, 143) [Neuroimaged: 368(264, 104)] 6–24 sMRI Subcortical volumes Language impairments US/White [82,80,83,8 1e]; African American: [1,5,2,5e]; one race: [14,10,10,1 1e]; not answered: [3,5,4,2e]Asian: [0,0,1,1e]; more than Longitudinal**
Tierney et al., 2012 (38) 122(65, 57) 6–24 EEG Frontal spectral power ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Wilkinson et al., 2019 (51) 101(58, 43) 24–36 EEG Resting frontal gamma power HR ASD; Language impairments US/not reported Longitudinal*
Wolff et al., 2012 (74) 92(28, 64)a 6–24 DTI White matter tract connectivity ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Wolff et al., 2015 (53) 378(270, 108) 6–24 DTI CC area and thickness ASD diagnosis US/not reported Longitudinal*
Wolff et al., 2017 (75) 217(44, 173)a 6–24 DTI Genu and cerebellar white matter connectivity Repetitive behaviors; sensory response US/not reported Longitudinal*
a

HR+, HR−

b

P-ASD

c

ASD, TD, LD/DD

d

Normal Risk, High Risk, TD

e

HR-ASD, HR-LD, HR-Neg, LR-Neg (%)

*

Included neuroimaging and behavior longitudinally

**

Included neuroimaging at a single time point, behavior longitudinally

ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; CC, corpus callosum; CSF, Cerebrospinal Fluid; d, days; dbWPLI, debiased weighted phase lag index; DD, developmentally delayed; DTI, diffusion tensor imaging; EEG, electroencephalogram; ERP, event-related potentials; fcMRI, functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; h, hours; HR, infants with an older sibling with ASD; HR+, infants with an older sibling with ASD and were later diagnosed with ASD; HR−, infants with an older sibling with ASD but were not later diagnosed with ASD; LD, language delay; mMSE, modified multiscale entropy; Normal Risk, came from a Simplex family in which they were the only child with ASD; p-ASD, provisionally diagnosed with ASD- at risk for ASD based on behavioral symptoms; PMA, post menstrual age; sMRI, structural magnetic resonance imaging; TD, typically developing; UK, United Kingdom; US, United States; w, weeks; y, years.

Brain Function

Neuroimaging studies of brain function have successfully differentiated high risk (HR) infants from those at low risk (LR). In the studies reviewed here, infants who have an older sibling diagnosed with autism are considered HR, and those who do not have a diagnosed older sibling are LR. As neuroimaging techniques are sensitive to motion artifacts, natural sleep paradigms permit researchers to conduct task-based fMRI studies in infants as young as 4–7 months and also to study more heterogeneous samples (62,76,77).

Deficits in communication and language are one of the hallmarks of ASD, thus, tasks designed to invoke language processing areas have been used as potential early markers for autism (68,76,78,79). In an fMRI study of brain responses to speech, autistic toddlers with subsequent poor language outcomes showed hypoactivation of the superior temporal cortex, an area known to be activated during language perception. Autistic toddlers who subsequently had good language outcomes showed similar brain responses to non-autistic comparison groups, robustly recruiting this brain area (68). While language trajectories in autism during the first years of life are highly unstable, identifying early language biomarkers represents a potentially meaningful way to parse phenotypic heterogeneity, and may improve early detection and intervention for those who are at risk of poor outcomes. Such findings also provide evidence of a possible neural correlate for resilience in language function. Interestingly, compensatory activity was found in left superior temporal cortex and other language areas in a study of language comprehension in 8 to 21 year olds diagnosed as high-functioning ASD in early childhood, who later achieved optimal outcomes (80).

In an event-related potential (ERP) study of the development of social brain networks in infants, neurotypical infants showed clear differentiation in brain responses to viewing faces with the eye gaze directed toward versus away from them. In contrast, infants who were later diagnosed with ASD showed little differentiation in their brain responses to these stimulus types (35,81). Furthermore, infants in the HR group who later exhibited typical behavioral outcomes showed a distinct difference in the processing of these stimuli relative to those who were later diagnosed with autism, as well as the control group. This ‘compensatory neural activity’ seen in the HR infants may be evidence of resilience, providing a neural correlate for the developmental shift into a ‘typical’ behavioral trajectory.

Further evidence of resilience was demonstrated in a group of children (49 – 77 months old) with ASD receiving 2 years of the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), a comprehensive developmental behavioral intervention (82). Children who received ESDM showed significant improvements in IQ, language, adaptive behavior and autism symptoms compared with the children who received a community intervention (CI) (83). In a follow-up study, brain responses were compared to pictures of faces and toys – social and non-social stimulus categories - using EEG. Children in both intervention groups and neurotypical controls showed similar N170 responses, reflecting typical early perceptual processing of the stimuli. While the ESDM group showed similar brain activity to TD children reflecting deeper attentional and cognitive processes when viewing the faces, the CI group showed an opposite pattern of activity (82). Both studies provide evidence of resilience in the social domain, as shown by the positive behavioral outcomes following ESDM intervention, as well as the ‘normalization’ of patterns of brain activity to social stimuli. These results also support the need for early, intensive and high-quality interventions as a way to foster resilience.

Brain Structure

Brain development depends on the balance between cellular and synaptic growth and appropriately timed pruning of neurons and synapses. In at least some children with ASD, this balance appears to be disrupted (10). Structural MRI provides evidence for potential risk biomarkers for autism in the form of increased brain volume detected as early as 6 months (8486). A longitudinal prospective study that used a deep-learning algorithm to examine hyperexpansion of the cortical surface area in children between 6 and 12 months of age was able to predict autism diagnosis at 24 months in HR children, with a positive predictive value of 81% and a sensitivity of 88% (65). One of the neuroendophenotypes identified as part of the Autism Phenome Project was boys with megalencephaly, a condition in which the brain is disproportionately large compared with body size (87). The estimated 15% of boys with autism with this phenotype were found to have more severe disabilities and had a poorer prognosis (88).

In addition to brain volume, measurements of extra-axial cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) have also been conducted in autism. Extra-axial CSF can be quantified by manual tracing of dura on successive coronal slices of the brain viewed with structural MRI that are summed to obtain intracranial volume. Semi-automated tissue segmentation is used to compute total cerebral volume, and the resulting space between the dura and cortical surface gives a measure of extra-axial CSF volume. From 6 months to 12 months, hyperexpansion of the cortical surface area and excessive CSF in the subarachnoid space (i.e., extra-axial CSF) precedes more global overgrowth and increased extra-axial fluid volume between 12 to 24 months (64,65,71,72). Recently, a large prospective study examined extra-axial fluid using MRI in subgroups of normal-risk (n=132) and HR (n=27) children with autism and neurotypical (n=77) controls, at age 3. Both autism groups had similar extra-axial fluid volumes, which were 15% greater than in neurotypical controls. Autism diagnosis was predicted at 83% (95% CI 76.2–88.3) by a machine learning algorithm (73). From the ages of 2 to 4 years, brain volumes in children with autism remain enlarged compared with those of their peers, particularly affecting the amygdala, frontal cortex, and temporal cortex, areas important for social cognition, emotion regulation, and language, respectively (89). Crucially, both early brain volume overgrowth and increased extra-axial fluid volume are associated with later autism symptoms - early brain volume overgrowth is associated with severity of social deficits and restricted and repetitive behaviors (65,70) and increased extra-axial fluid is predictive of communication deficits in ASD (71).

These results demonstrate the potential of using structural MRI, particularly to examine extra-axial fluid, as a risk marker in HR infants as young as 6 months, to and predict diagnosis and symptom severity for autism. Studies identifying early structural brain differences could also be used to investigate trajectories of resilience, if participants are followed longitudinally to assess symptom severity and adaptive outcomes through development and adulthood.

Brain Connectivity

Structural connectivity (SC) measured with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), has also been used to identify early risk markers and correlates of symptom severity in ASD (75). Fractional anisotropy (FA) quantifies white matter tract microstructure, with an increase in FA indicating an increase in myelination (90). In HR infants as young as 6 weeks to 6 months, FA and lateralization of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) were found to be related to later language development and autism symptoms (67,74).

Network efficiency, or the capacity of the brain to exchange information locally and globally, has also been examined using SC metrics. In a longitudinal study of 116 infants scanned at 6 months and 12 months of age, network inefficiencies in regions involved in low-level sensory processing were seen as young as 6 months in HR infants who were later diagnosed with ASD. This metric also predicted symptom severity at 24 months (66). This work suggests that early network inefficiencies contribute to a developmental cascade affecting brain organization and behavior.

Recent work examining functional connectivity (FC), derived from resting-state fMRI data, has also demonstrated potential in identifying risk markers for HR infants (69,91). Machine learning applied to FC measures derived from fMRI of children at 6 months of age has been used to predict autism diagnosis in HR infants, and correctly classify infants who were not diagnosed (91).

Alongside whole-brain approaches examining long-distance FC, local functional alterations can be assessed using regional homogeneity (ReHo), which computes the coherence between a given voxel and those surrounding it. In neonates with a family history of ASD, significantly elevated ReHo has recently been observed (61). In a cross-sectional investigation of children and adolescents with ASD, higher local connectivity as measured with ReHo was associated with poorer social communication skills (92).

Taken together, developmental neuroimaging studies show promise in establishing biomarkers for early diagnosis, for parsing phenotypic heterogeneity, and for predicting behavioral outcomes. However, as there is substantial variability during the early developmental period we have focused on, a multi-disciplinary approach to studying risk and resilience in autism may help move us closer to more precise risk markers and more adaptive models of care. One such approach is to combine neuroimaging with genetics to provide corroborative data on carriers of risk alleles for a more unequivocal assessment of risk (2,13,93) and to identify mechanisms of resilience. Jack and colleagues (2020) investigated female-specific protective factors in ASD, and found that females with ASD showed a different pattern of brain activation than males with ASD during the viewing of biological motion point-light stimuli. This was also true of neurotypical females, who showed greater recruitment of salience and executive control networks compared with neurotypical males, a possible correlate of the protective effect of being female (29). In the genetic data, the authors found female-specific gene expression patterns (larger size of rare copy number variants, CNVs) in striatal cortex of females with ASD that may be related to the sex-differential processes of risk and resilience (37).

There are limited conclusions we can draw about mechanisms of resilience in ASD from existing neuroimaging studies, as studying the dynamics of resilience as an outcome requires a longitudinal approach. These studies still need to be done, incorporating promising multi-disciplinary approaches (37). Most importantly, evidence already exists that early, intense, supportive intervention can promote factors that increase resilience in the face of genetic or environmental risks (82,94).

3. Towards genetic and epigenetic markers of resilience

Resilience is not simply a reversal of pathological mechanisms, but involves specific psychosocial and biological processes that buffer a person against the impact of a stressor. Oxytocin (OXT) is a neurohormone that has frequently been implicated in affiliative and social behaviors (95). One of the ways OXT may affect social behavior is by attenuating the stress response through its actions on the neuroendocrine system (hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal - HPA-axis) (96). OXT suppresses the HPA-axis, increasing interpersonal trust while reducing anxiety (97).

In recent years there has been enthusiasm about OXT as a potential pharmacological treatment for socio-emotional symptoms of autism (98,99). Functional neuroimaging studies of autistic children and adults have reported significant changes in activity after OXT administration in brain areas involved in social information processing and reward processing (100106). These studies suggest that variation in the OXT system contributes to social phenotypes, regardless of diagnosis (107).

To identify subgroups of individuals who are most likely to benefit from pharmacological treatment with OXT, Voinsky and colleagues looked at variations in expression of OXTR and other genes in children with ASD, their neurotypical siblings, and neurotypical controls aged 3 to 16 years. Across all groups, higher OXTR expression levels correlated with i) better outcome as measured by the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS), ii) less severe social impairment as measured by the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and iii) less severe behavior problems as measured by the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC). Based on their findings, the authors propose that relevant gene expression levels should be evaluated as prognostic biomarkers by clinical trials or before administration of other tentative autism treatments (108).

As a result of the dynamic balance between risk and protective factors, resilience is a process that arises out of the interaction of genetic, epigenetic and environmental influences. The control of gene expression may provide a possible genetic mechanism of resilience. One key mechanism of epigenetic control of gene expression is DNA methylation. DNA methylation is the process by which methyl groups are added to cytosine–phosphate–guanine (CpG) sites on the regulatory or promoter regions of genes to silence transcription (109). This process is under genetic control, but subject to environmental influences (110,111).

Andari and colleagues investigated epigenetic modification of the OXTR gene in adults with ASD. They found that participants showing hypermethylation in the intron 1 area of OXTR showed fewer deficits in social interaction and communication as measured by the Autism Diagnosis Interview (ADI-R) and hypo-connectivity between superior temporal and posterior cingulate cortices, brain areas involved in theory of mind (112). Thus, OXTR hypermethylation may be a potential epigenetic biomarker for resilient social phenotypes in autism, and DNA methylation may be an epigenetic mechanism that contributes to this phenotype.

Several common alleles of the OXTR gene may increase risk for ASD (113,114), and a recent study has found that increased OXTR risk-allele dosage was associated with symptom severity in a group of 41 high-functioning youth with ASD, as well as with reduced connectivity of the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) to other components of the reward system. On the other hand, youth in the neurotypical control group showed increased NAcc connectivity with frontal brain regions involved in mentalizing, which was correlated to better scores on the SRS. Thus, additive genetic effects as measured by risk-allele dosage, may be an important factor in conferring risk but also in revealing resilient endophenotypes (93).

Accumulating evidence suggests that genetic and epigenetic factors have significant impact on risk during developmental processes pre- and postnatally (13). Early life stressors may alter the epigenome at various sites, but the specific alterations and psychiatric outcomes depend on multiple factors specific to the individual. DNA methylation contributes to the regulation of OXTR gene expression, which is associated with both biological and behavioral outcomes. This may be a direct effect, through OXT influencing stress regulation via the HPA-axis, or an indirect effect whereby risk and resilience have a common cause. In either case, risk-allele dosage and gene expression may serve as potential biomarkers for resilience.

4. Future Directions and Conclusions

Researchers have primarily worked towards identifying biomarkers that categorize autistic persons as a separate group from neurotypical individuals. However, the research domain criteria (RDoC), a novel framework for investigating mental disorders that integrates information at multiple levels, presents practicable alternatives (115). This is important for both the study of risk for autism, a heterogeneous disorder of largely unknown etiology, and for the study of resilience, a dynamic process of adaptation similarly influenced by genetic and environmental factors. RDoC highlights the importance of understanding mental health on a comparative scale that crosses boundaries set by the DSM categories (116).

While substantial progress has been made by infant studies which typically track children until diagnosis of ASD (117), longitudinal studies with at least three lifecourse time points are needed, that concurrently track changes in the clinical, neurocognitive, functional, and anatomical trajectories to ascertain the prognostic value of biomarkers (118). Furthermore, multi-modal biomarkers – such as those combining genetics and neuroimaging (93,119) likely have improved prognostic as well predictive value relative to markers based on one modality (120,121), and will help to develop more precise interventions and effective treatments targeted specifically to individuals who stand to benefit from them.

While key protective factors in autism have been defined (21,2528), the long-term goal is for research to describe the mediating mechanisms for risks and protective factors (22,24). As resilience is a dynamic process, there is inter-individual variability in adaptation through time and by domain. To investigate mechanisms of this multi-faceted process, longitudinal studies are needed to establish causation from risk to resilience, as well as to capture individual differences throughout the lifecourse (122).

Conclusion

Current understanding of the etiology of autism is based on the interaction of multiple genes with each other and with environmental factors, leading to neurodevelopmental processes that result in the expression of autism. Similarly, the resilience of each individual in the face of adversity is the result of the interaction between vulnerability and protective factors. In this review, we have highlighted important early markers of risk, and explored potential mechanisms of resilience. Taken together, this work suggests that neuroimaging in early development can provide evidence of risk biomarkers, which can be refined through multi-modal approaches. Further, it may be possible to strengthen resilience through psychosocial and biological means at different timepoints during development to improve outcomes across the lifespan for autistic individuals.

The meaningful impact of research in autism depends on the integration of information from genetic, neural, and behavioral approaches over the lifecourse. Most importantly, in looking towards the future, views about autism are changing, driven by autistic self-advocates and their allies (123,124). Research has the potential to transform the lives of autistic people and their families, but only when it is relevant, valued and effectively implemented (125). The relevance and influence of autism research will depend on researchers getting involved with the community they are researching. The next chapter of autism research will be improved by multidisciplinary global collaboration between researchers and clinicians, the involvement of families, and the autism community itself.

Acknowledgments:

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health [R01MH107549], the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research, and a University of Miami Gabelli Senior Scholar Award to LQU.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Disclosures: IMS, LK and LQU report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association (2013): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lord C, Brugha TS, Charman T, Cusack J, Dumas G, Frazier T, et al. (2020): Autism spectrum disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers 6: 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hawks ZW, Constantino JN (2020): Neuropsychiatric “Comorbidity” as Causal Influence in Autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 59: 229–235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Masi A, DeMayo MM, Glozier N, Guastella AJ (2017): An Overview of Autism Spectrum Disorder, Heterogeneity and Treatment Options. Neuroscience Bulletin, vol. 33 pp 183–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Georgiades S, Szatmari P, Zwaigenbaum L, Duku E, Bryson S, Roberts W, et al. (2007): Structure of the autism symptom phenotype: A proposed multidimensional model. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 46: 188–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Volkmar FR, State M, Klin A (2009): Autism and autism spectrum disorders: diagnostic issues for the coming decade. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50: 108–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.de Zeeuw EL, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Boomsma DI (2017): The etiology of autistic traits in preschoolers: a population-based twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 58: 893–901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Abrahams BS, Geschwind DH (2008): Advances in autism genetics: on the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet 9: 341–355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jeste SS, Geschwind DH (2014): Disentangling the heterogeneity of autism spectrum disorder through genetic findings. Nat Rev Neurol 10: 74–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Muhle RA, Reed HE, Stratigos KA, Veenstra-VanderWeele J (2018): The Emerging Clinical Neuroscience of Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Review. JAMA Psychiatry 75: 514–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bai D, Yip BHK, Windham GC, Sourander A, Francis R, Yoffe R, et al. (2019): Association of Genetic and Environmental Factors With Autism in a 5-Country Cohort. JAMA Psychiatry. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Modabbernia A, Velthorst E, Reichenberg A (2017): Environmental Risk Factors for Autism: An Evidence-Based Review of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses. Molecular Autism, 8, 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Courchesne E, Gazestani VH, Lewis NE (2020): Prenatal Origins of ASD: The When, What, and How of ASD Development. Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 43 pp 326–342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sinclair J (1993): Don’t mourn for us. Our voice. Autism Network International 1. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Language - National Autistic Society (n.d.): Retrieved March 15, 2020, from https://www.autism.org.uk/get-involved/media-centre/how-to-talk.aspx
  • 16.Baron-Cohen S (2017): Editorial Perspective: Neurodiversity - a revolutionary concept for autism and psychiatry. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 58 pp 744–747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Georgiades S, Kasari C (2018): Reframing Optimal Outcomes in Autism. JAMA Pediatr 172: 716–717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Uddin LQ, Dajani DR, Voorhies W, Bednarz H, Kana RK (2017): Progress and roadblocks in the search for brain-based biomarkers of autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Transl Psychiatry 7: e1218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Herrman H, Stewart DE, Diaz-Granados N, Berger EL, Jackson B, Yuen T (2011): What is Resilience? The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 56 pp 258–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McConachie H, Mason D, Parr JR, Garland D, Wilson C, Rodgers J (2018): Enhancing the Validity of a Quality of Life Measure for Autistic People. J Autism Dev Disord 48: 1596–1611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Masten AS (2001): Ordinary magic. Resilience processes in development. Am Psychol 56: 227–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rutter M (2013): Annual research review: Resilience–clinical implications. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. Retrieved from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02615.x?casa_token=D2XBhLR3UTIAAAAA:86WxPDu-cOnY4ZBNZfAtbaJmpkd3AdWc3sL6b8fSo5ykriYKKQpjJaZx5H6v_EQ1r3GjosqrVKUdXew [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Werner EE, Smith RS (1992): Overcoming the Odds: High Risk Children from Birth to Adulthood. Cornell University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B (2000): The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev 71: 543–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bourgeron T (2018): Explaining “resilience”in autism may seed new therapies. Retrieved from https://www.spectrumnews.org/opinion/viewpoint/explaining-resilience-autism-may-seed-new-therapies/?format=pdf
  • 26.Kaboski J, McDonnell CG, Valentino K (2017): Resilience and Autism Spectrum Disorder: Applying Developmental Psychopathology to Optimal Outcome. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 4: 175–189. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Szatmari P (2018): Risk and resilience in autism spectrum disorder: a missed translational opportunity? Dev Med Child Neurol 60: 225–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lai M-C, Szatmari P (2019): Resilience in autism: Research and practice prospects. Autism 23: 539–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Robinson EB, Lichtenstein P, Anckarsater H, Happe F, Ronald A (2013): Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110 pp 5258–5262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ben-Itzchak E, Zachor DA (2020): Toddlers to teenagers: Long-term follow-up study of outcomes in autism spectrum disorder. Autism, vol. 24 pp 41–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.2017. IACC Strategic Plan for Autism Spectrum Disorder Research | IACC (n.d.): Retrieved March 20, 2020, from https://iacc.hhs.gov/publications/strategic-plan/2017/
  • 32.Dean DC 3rd, Freeman A, Lainhart J (2020): The development of the social brain in baby siblings of children with autism. Curr Opin Psychiatry 33: 110–116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gordon-Lipkin E, Foster J, Peacock G (2016): Whittling Down the Wait Time: Exploring Models to Minimize the Delay from Initial Concern to Diagnosis and Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Pediatr Clin North Am 63: 851–859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Antezana L, Scarpa A, Valdespino A, Albright J, Richey JA (2017): Rural Trends in Diagnosis and Services for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Psychol 8: 590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, Chandler S, Pasco G, Charman T, et al. (2012): Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Curr Biol 22: 338–342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McCleery JP, Akshoomoff N, Dobkins KR, Carver LJ (2009): Atypical face versus object processing and hemispheric asymmetries in 10-month-old infants at risk for autism. Biol Psychiatry 66: 950–957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jack A, Sullivan CAW, Aylward E, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M, Gaab N, et al. (n.d.): Neurogenetic Profiles of Risk and Resilience in Female Autism. SSRN Electronic Journal. 10.2139/ssrn.3532039 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Tierney AL, Gabard-Durnam L, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2012): Developmental Trajectories of Resting EEG Power: An Endophenotype of Autism Spectrum Disorder. PLoS ONE, vol. 7 p e39127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Levin AR, Varcin KJ, O’Leary HM, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2017): EEG power at 3 months in infants at high familial risk for autism. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, vol. 9 10.1186/s11689-017-9214-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kolesnik A, The BASIS Team, Ali JB, Gliga, Guiraud, Charman, et al. (2019): Increased cortical reactivity to repeated tones at 8 months in infants with later ASD. Translational Psychiatry, vol. 9 10.1038/s41398-019-0393-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jones EJH, Venema K, Earl R, Lowy R, Barnes K, Estes A, et al. (2016): Reduced engagement with social stimuli in 6-month-old infants with later autism spectrum disorder: a longitudinal prospective study of infants at high familial risk. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, vol. 8 10.1186/s11689-016-9139-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fingher N, Dinstein I, Ben-Shachar M, Haar S, Dale AM, Eyler L, et al. (2017): Toddlers later diagnosed with autism exhibit multiple structural abnormalities in temporal corpus callosum fibers. Cortex 97: 291–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ciarrusta J, O’Muircheartaigh J, Dimitrova R, Batalle D, Cordero-Grande L, Price A, et al. (2019): Social Brain Functional Maturation in Newborn Infants With and Without a Family History of Autism Spectrum Disorder. JAMA Network Open, vol. 2 p e191868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Elison JT, Paterson SJ, Wolff JJ, Reznick JS, Sasson NJ, Gu H, et al. (2013): White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. Am J Psychiatry 170: 899–908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Brito NH, Elliott AJ, Isler JR, Rodriguez C, Friedrich C, Shuffrey LC, Fifer WP (2019): Neonatal EEG linked to individual differences in socioemotional outcomes and autism risk in toddlers. Dev Psychobiol 61: 1110–1119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bosl W, Tierney A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson C (2011): EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Medicine, vol. 9 10.1186/1741-7015-9-18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Elsabbagh M, Holmboe K, Gliga T, Mercure E, Hudry K, Charman T, et al. (2011): Social and attention factors during infancy and the later emergence of autism characteristics. Prog Brain Res 189: 195–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Gerig G, Styner M, Chappell C, Smith RG, et al. (2011): Early brain overgrowth in autism associated with an increase in cortical surface area before age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 467–476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lewis JD, for the IBIS network, Evans AC, Pruett JR, Botteron K, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. (2014): Network inefficiencies in autism spectrum disorder at 24 months. Translational Psychiatry, vol. 4 pp e388–e388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Seery AM, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2013): Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 5 pp 10–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wilkinson CL, Levin AR, Gabard Durnam LJ, Tager Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2019): Reduced frontal gamma power at 24 months is associated with better expressive language in toddlers at risk for autism. Autism Research, vol. 12 pp 1211–1224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Seery A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2014): Event-related potentials to repeated speech in 9-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord 6: 43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wolff JJ, Gerig G, Lewis JD, Soda T, Styner MA, Vachet C, et al. (2015): Altered corpus callosum morphology associated with autism over the first 2 years of life. Brain 138: 2046–2058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Gabard-Durnam L, Tierney AL, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2015): Alpha asymmetry in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 45: 473–480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Gabard-Durnam LJ, Wilkinson C, Kapur K, Tager-Flusberg H, Levin AR, Nelson CA (2019): Longitudinal EEG power in the first postnatal year differentiates autism outcomes. Nat Commun 10: 4188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Orekhova EV, Elsabbagh M, Jones EJ, Dawson G, Charman T, Johnson MH, BASIS Team (2014): EEG hyper-connectivity in high-risk infants is associated with later autism. J Neurodev Disord 6: 40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Righi G, Tierney AL, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA (2014): Functional connectivity in the first year of life in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: an EEG study. PLoS One 9: e105176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Schumann CM, Bloss CS, Barnes CC, Wideman GM, Carper RA, Akshoomoff N, et al. (2010): Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical development through early childhood in autism. J Neurosci 30: 4419–4427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Shephard E, Milosavljevic B, Mason L, Elsabbagh M, Tye C, Gliga T, et al. (2020): Neural and behavioural indices of face processing in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A longitudinal study from infancy to mid-childhood. Cortex 127: 162–179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Swanson MR, Shen MD, Wolff JJ, Elison JT, Emerson RW, Styner MA, et al. (2017): Subcortical Brain and Behavior Phenotypes Differentiate Infants With Autism Versus Language Delay. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, vol. 2 pp 664–672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Ciarrusta J, Dimitrova R, Batalle D, O’Muircheartaigh J, Cordero-Grande L, Price A, et al. (2020): Emerging functional connectivity differences in newborn infants vulnerable to autism spectrum disorders. Transl Psychiatry 10: 131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Blasi A, Lloyd-Fox S, Sethna V, Brammer MJ, Mercure E, Murray L, et al. (2015): Atypical processing of voice sounds in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Cortex 71: 122–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Elsabbagh M, Gliga T, Pickles A, Hudry K, Charman T, Johnson MH, Team, Basis (2013): The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behav Brain Res 251: 147–154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hazlett HC, Gu H, McKinstry RC, Shaw DWW, Botteron KN, Dager SR, et al. (2012): Brain volume findings in 6-month-old infants at high familial risk for autism. Am J Psychiatry 169: 601–608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hazlett HC, Gu H, Munsell BC, Kim SH, Styner M, Wolff JJ, et al. (2017): Early brain development in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder. Nature 542: 348–351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lewis JD, Evans AC, Pruett JR Jr, Botteron KN, McKinstry RC, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. (2017): The Emergence of Network Inefficiencies in Infants With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psychiatry 82: 176–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Liu J, Tsang T, Jackson L, Ponting C, Jeste SS, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M (2019): Altered lateralization of dorsal language tracts in 6-week-old infants at risk for autism. Dev Sci 22: e12768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Lombardo MV, Pierce K, Eyler LT, Carter Barnes C, Ahrens-Barbeau C, Solso S, et al. (2015): Different functional neural substrates for good and poor language outcome in autism. Neuron 86: 567–577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.McKinnon CJ, Eggebrecht AT, Todorov A, Wolff JJ, Elison JT, Adams CM, et al. (2019): Restricted and Repetitive Behavior and Brain Functional Connectivity in Infants at Risk for Developing Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 4: 50–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Pote I, Wang S, Sethna V, Blasi A, Daly E, Kuklisova-Murgasova M, et al. (2019): Familial risk of autism alters subcortical and cerebellar brain anatomy in infants and predicts the emergence of repetitive behaviors in early childhood. Autism Res 12: 614–627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Shen MD, Nordahl CW, Young GS, Wootton-Gorges SL, Lee A, Liston SE, et al. (2013): Early brain enlargement and elevated extra-axial fluid in infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Brain 136: 2825–2835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Shen MD, Kim SH, McKinstry RC, Gu H, Hazlett HC, Nordahl CW, et al. (2017): Increased Extra-axial Cerebrospinal Fluid in High-Risk Infants Who Later Develop Autism. Biol Psychiatry 82: 186–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Shen MD, Nordahl CW, Li DD, Lee A, Angkustsiri K, Emerson RW, et al. (2018): Extra-axial cerebrospinal fluid in high-risk and normal-risk children with autism aged 2–4 years: a case-control study. Lancet Psychiatry 5: 895–904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wolff JJ, Gu H, Gerig G, Elison JT, Styner M, Gouttard S, et al. (2012): Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am J Psychiatry 169: 589–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wolff JJ, Swanson MR, Elison JT, Gerig G, Pruett JR Jr, Styner MA, et al. (2017): Neural circuitry at age 6 months associated with later repetitive behavior and sensory responsiveness in autism. Mol Autism 8: 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Eyler LT, Pierce K, Courchesne E (2012): A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain 135: 949–960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Pierce K (2011): Early functional brain development in autism and the promise of sleep fMRI. Brain Res 1380: 162–174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Dinstein I, Pierce K, Eyler L, Solso S, Malach R, Behrmann M, Courchesne E (2011): Disrupted neural synchronization in toddlers with autism. Neuron 70: 1218–1225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Redcay E, Courchesne E (2008): Deviant functional magnetic resonance imaging patterns of brain activity to speech in 2–3-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry 64: 589–598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Eigsti I-M, Stevens MC, Schultz RT, Barton M, Kelley E, Naigles L, et al. (2016): Language comprehension and brain function in individuals with an optimal outcome from autism. NeuroImage: Clinical, vol. 10 pp 182–191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Elsabbagh M (2012): The emerging autistic brain: processes of risk and resilience. Neuropsychiatry 2: 181. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Dawson G, Jones EJH, Merkle K, Venema K, Lowy R, Faja S, et al. (2012): Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity in young children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51: 1150–1159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J, Greenson J, et al. (2010): Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics 125: e17–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Havercamp S, Andreasen NC, Palmer P (1995): An MRI study of brain size in autism. Am J Psychiatry. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1996-92631-001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD, et al. (2001): Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology 57: 245–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Sparks BF, Friedman SD, Shaw DW, Aylward EH, Echelard D, Artru AA, et al. (2002): Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology 59: 184–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Nordahl CW, Lange N, Li DD, Barnett LA, Lee A, Buonocore MH, et al. (2011): Brain enlargement is associated with regression in preschool-age boys with autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 108 pp 20195–20200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Amaral DG, Li D, Libero L, Solomon M, Van de Water J, Mastergeorge A, et al. (2017): In pursuit of neurophenotypes: The consequences of having autism and a big brain. Autism Res 10: 711–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Courchesne E, Campbell K, Solso S (2011): Brain growth across the life span in autism: age-specific changes in anatomical pathology. Brain Res 1380: 138–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Basser PJ, Pierpaoli C (1996): Microstructural and physiological features of tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. J Magn Reson B 111: 209–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Emerson RW, Adams C, Nishino T, Hazlett HC, Wolff JJ, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. (2017): Functional neuroimaging of high-risk 6-month-old infants predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age. Sci Transl Med 9. 10.1126/scitranslmed.aag2882 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Dajani DR, Uddin LQ (2016): Local brain connectivity across development in autism spectrum disorder: A cross-sectional investigation. Autism Res 9: 43–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Hernandez LM, Krasileva K, Green SA, Sherman LE, Ponting C, McCarron R, et al. (2017): Additive effects of oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms on reward circuitry in youth with autism. Molecular Psychiatry, vol. 22 pp 1134–1139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Gunnar MR, Fisher PA, Early Experience, Stress, and Prevention Network (2006): Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol 18: 651–677. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A (2016): The Social Salience Hypothesis of Oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry 79: 194–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Gunnar M, Quevedo K (2007): The Neurobiology of Stress and Development. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 58 pp 145–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Carter CS (2005, December 20): The chemistry of child neglect: do oxytocin and vasopressin mediate the effects of early experience? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 102 pp 18247–18248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M (2011): Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 524–538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Nomi JS, Molnar-Szakacs I, Uddin LQ (2019): Insular function in autism: Update and future directions in neuroimaging and interventions. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 89: 412–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Aoki Y, Watanabe T, Abe O, Kuwabara H, Yahata N, Takano Y, et al. (2015): Oxytocin’s neurochemical effects in the medial prefrontal cortex underlie recovery of task-specific brain activity in autism: a randomized controlled trial. Mol Psychiatry 20: 447–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Aoki Y, Yahata N, Watanabe T, Takano Y, Kawakubo Y, Kuwabara H, et al. (2014): Oxytocin improves behavioural and neural deficits in inferring others’ social emotions in autism. Brain 137: 3073–3086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Domes G, Heinrichs M, Kumbier E, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Herpertz SC (2013): Effects of intranasal oxytocin on the neural basis of face processing in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry 74: 164–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Gordon I, Vander Wyk BC, Bennett RH, Cordeaux C, Lucas MV, Eilbott JA, et al. (2013): Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110 pp 20953–20958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Watanabe T, Abe O, Kuwabara H, Yahata N, Takano Y, Iwashiro N, et al. (2014): Mitigation of Sociocommunicational Deficits of Autism Through Oxytocin-Induced Recovery of Medial Prefrontal Activity. JAMA Psychiatry, vol. 71 p 166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Wigton R, Radua J, Allen P, Averbeck B, Meyer-Lindenberg A, McGuire P, et al. (2015): Neurophysiological effects of acute oxytocin administration: systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci 40: E1–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Johnson ZV, Young LJ (2017): Oxytocin and vasopressin neural networks: Implications for social behavioral diversity and translational neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 76: 87–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Young LJ, Barrett CE (2015): Neuroscience. Can oxytocin treat autism? Science 347: 825–826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Voinsky I, Bennuri SC, Svigals J, Frye RE, Rose S, Gurwitz D (2019): Peripheral Blood Mononuclear Cell Oxytocin and Vasopressin Receptor Expression Positively Correlates with Social and Behavioral Function in Children with Autism. Sci Rep 9: 13443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Bick J, Naumova O, Hunter S, Barbot B, Lee M, Luthar SS, et al. (2012): Childhood adversity and DNA methylation of genes involved in the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and immune system: whole-genome and candidate-gene associations. Dev Psychopathol 24: 1417–1425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.McGowan PO, Roth TL (2015): Epigenetic pathways through which experiences become linked with biology. Dev Psychopathol 27: 637–648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Jones MJ, Fejes AP, Kobor MS (2013, July 29): DNA methylation, genotype and gene expression: who is driving and who is along for the ride? Genome Biology, vol. 14 p 126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Andari E, Nishitani S, Kaundinya G, Caceres GA, Morrier MJ, Ousley O, et al. (2020): Epigenetic modification of the oxytocin receptor gene: implications for autism symptom severity and brain functional connectivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 10.1038/s41386-020-0610-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.LoParo D, Waldman ID (2015): The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 20: 640–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Wermte A-K, Kamp-Becker I, Hesse P, Schulte-Körne G, Strauch K, Remschmidt H (2010): Evidence for the involvement of genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in the etiology of autistic disorders on high-functioning level. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 153: 629–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, et al. (2010): Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 167: 748–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Casey BJ, Craddock N, Cuthbert BN, Hyman SE, Lee FS, Ressler KJ (2013): DSM-5 and RDoC: progress in psychiatry research? Nat Rev Neurosci 14: 810–814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Szatmari P, Chawarska K, Dawson G, Georgiades S, Landa R, Lord C, et al. (2016): Prospective Longitudinal Studies of Infant Siblings of Children With Autism: Lessons Learned and Future Directions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 55: 179–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Loth E, Murphy DG, Spooren W (2016): Defining Precision Medicine Approaches to Autism Spectrum Disorders: Concepts and Challenges. Front Psychiatry 7: 188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Uzefovsky F, Bethlehem RAI, Shamay-Tsoory S, Ruigrok A, Holt R, Spencer M, et al. (2019): The oxytocin receptor gene predicts brain activity during an emotion recognition task in autism. Mol Autism 10: 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Isaksson J, Tammimies K, Neufeld J, Cauvet É, Lundin K, Buitelaar JK, et al. (2018): EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP): the autism twin cohort. Mol Autism 9: 26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Lawrence KE, Hernandez LM, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M (2019): Atypical longitudinal development of functional connectivity in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, vol. 12 pp 53–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Werner EE (1995): Resilience in Development. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 4: 81–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Pellicano L, Mandy W, Bölte S, Stahmer A, Lounds Taylor J, Mandell DS (2018): A new era for autism research, and for our journal. Autism 22: 82–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Bagatell N (2010): From Cure to Community: Transforming Notions of Autism. Ethos 38: 33–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Fletcher-Watson S, Adams J, Brook K, Charman T, Crane L, Cusack J, et al. (2019): Making the future together: Shaping autism research through meaningful participation. Autism 23: 943–953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.de Leeuw A, Happé F, Hoekstra RA (2020): A Conceptual Framework for Understanding the Cultural and Contextual Factors on Autism Across the Globe. Autism Res. 10.1002/aur.2276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Kang-Yi CD, Grinker RR, Beidas R, Agha A, Russell R, Shah SB, et al. (2018): Influence of Community-Level Cultural Beliefs about Autism on Families’ and Professionals’ Care for Children. Transcult Psychiatry 55: 623–647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Pickard K, Reyes N, Reaven J (2019): Examining the inclusion of diverse participants in cognitive behavior therapy research for youth with autism spectrum disorder and anxiety. Autism 23: 1057–1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES