Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Jan 7;16(1):e0245057. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245057

Anxiety and depression symptoms, the recovery from symptoms, and loneliness before and after the COVID-19 outbreak among the general population: Findings from a Dutch population-based longitudinal study

Peter G van der Velden 1,2,*, Philip Hyland 3, Carlo Contino 4, Hans-Martin von Gaudecker 5, Ruud Muffels 6, Marcel Das 1,7
Editor: Fuschia M Sirois8
PMCID: PMC7790276  PMID: 33411843

Abstract

Objectives

Examine the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the mental health and loneliness in the general population. More specifically, the study focused on prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms, the extent to which individuals with existing symptoms recovered or not, the prevalence of subtypes of loneliness, and the extent to which loneliness before and during this pandemic was associated with anxiety and depression symptoms.

Methods

Data was extracted from the longitudinal LISS panel, based on a probability sample of the Dutch population, with assessments on loneliness in October 2019 (T1) and June 2020 (T4), and anxiety and depression symptoms in November 2019 (T2), March 2020 (T3) and June 2020 (T4; Ntotal = 4,084). Loneliness was examined with the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale and anxiety and depression symptoms with the Mental Health Inventory (MHI-5).

Results

Repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses (RMMLRA) showed a statistical significant lower prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms after the outbreak (T4 = 15.3%) than before (T2 = 16.8%) and during the COVID-19 outbreak (T3 = 17.2%). According to the Reliable Change Index, the distribution of recovery categories (remission, improvement, unchanged and worsening symptoms) after the outbreak did not differ significantly from the distribution of these categories before the outbreak. RMMLRA revealed that the prevalence of emotional loneliness increased significantly after the outbreak (T1 = 18.4%, T4 = 24.8%). Among individuals who were not lonely before and after the outbreak the prevalence of symptoms decreased significantly (T2 = 7.0%, T4 = 4.4%) and, likewise, among those who were not lonely anymore after the outbreak (T2 = 21.5%, T4 = 14.5%). However, the prevalence of symptoms increased significantly among those who became lonely during the pandemic (T2 = 17.9%, T4 = 26.3%).

Conclusions

Findings suggest that this pandemic did not negatively affect the prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms nor the normal recovery of symptoms among the general population during the first four months, but that emotional loneliness increased.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic [1] negatively affects countries on a micro-, meso- and macro- level. At the micro-level it varies from the threat of people becoming infected or when infected, becoming very ill or even die, loss of significant others, unemployment, and social isolation because of social distancing rules, to reduced health care use among non-COVID-19 patients [2]. At meso and macro-levels it leads to understaffing and reduced revenues of firms, increasing governmental deficits, increasing economic insecurity, changing demand patterns of consumers [3], and collapsing growth levels in the global economy [4, 5]. A key question is to what extent this pandemic and the uncertainties about the future will negatively affect the mental health of the general population.

Several studies suggest that this pandemic increases the risk of mental health problems among the general population [6, 7]. However, many of these studies used cross-sectional study designs and are based on convenience samples instead of probability samples of the general population. A recent review by Xiong et al. [7] showed that all 19 identified and included COVID-19 studies used cross-sectional study designs and that 18 out of 19 studies used (potentially biased) convenience samples. These convenience samples used, for instance, snowball techniques and messages on social media as strategies to recruit respondents.

Mental health problems

To the best of our knowledge, to date five prospective population-based studies with pre- and post-COVID-19 data have been published using probability samples of the general population to assess the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the mental health of the general population. Twenge and Joiner [8] assessed the effects in the United States (US) by comparing anxiety and depression symptomatology (according to the PHQ-2 and GAD-2) of adults participating in the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS; January-June 2019) with adults participating in the Household Pulse Survey (HPS; April-May 2020). They found that American adults participating in the HPS were three to four times as likely to screen positive for anxiety or depression disorders, or screen positive for both in April–May 2020, compared to American adults who were assessed in the first half of 2019. During the pandemic about 30% screened positive for these disorders. Importantly, the NHIS participants were asked about symptoms in the last two weeks and HPS participants were asked about symptoms in the last seven days. The study by McGinty et al. [9] found that of the adult Americans who participated in the Johns Hopkins COVID-19 Civic Life and Public Health Survey (April 2020) 13.6% reported symptoms of serious psychological distress compared to 3.9% of the adults who participated in the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) in 2018 according to the Kessler 6 scale. Daly et al. [10] compared the prevalence of depression symptoms according to the PHQ-2 among American adults participating in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHNE) of 2017–2018, and among American adults who participated in the Understanding America Study (UAS) in March and April 2020. Results showed a significant higher prevalence in March (10.6%) and April (14.4%) 2020 UAS samples compared to the 2017–2018 (8.7%) NHNE sample. Pierce et al. [11] focused on UK residents of 16 years and older who participated in the UK Household Longitudinal Study and were asked to complete a COVID-19 web survey. Their findings showed that mean scores on the GHQ-12 increased significantly from 11.5 in 2018–2019 to 12.6 in April 2020. The increase was not simply a continuation of previous upwards trends from 2014 to 2019. The prevalence of clinically significant mental distress according to the GHQ-12, which should be distinguished from a mental disorder, among the total study sample increased from 18.9% in 2018–2019 to 27.3% in April 2020. The study by Van der Velden et al. [12] focused on Dutch adults using the data of surveys conducted with the population-based longitudinal LISS panel. Contrary to the previous population-based studies, no increase was found in anxiety and depression symptoms according to the MHI-5 during the COVID-19 outbreak (March 2020) compared to pre-outbreak levels (November 2019) among the general population. To account for possible seasonal effects, they also assessed the course of symptoms in the same period one year earlier (November 2018- March 2019), showing a similar pattern ruling out the possibility that findings can be attributed to a seasonal effect.

The differences in results between the studies in US and UK on the one side and in the Netherlands on the other side may partly be a result of (slightly) different study periods, besides differences in instruments, use of different samples to compare pre- and post-outbreak prevalence [810], governmental responses, and health care and social welfare systems. Perhaps the negative effects of the pandemic on the mental health of the general population are especially prevalent in the months after the COVID-19 outbreak.

Recovery of symptoms

The sudden societal changes and related stressors after the COVID-19 outbreak may also have worsened symptoms or have hindered the recovery of symptoms among individuals with existing mental health problems. In addition, the pandemic may have reduced access to or use of mental health care services, or forced mental health care workers to continue the treatment using online facilities instead of usual vis-à-vis contacts contributing to a deteriorating mental health [13, 14].

However, the review by Sheridan Rains et al. [14] revealed that most studies on this important topic documented observations and experiences rather than reporting on research based on empirical data. To the best of our knowledge, to date only one empirical study focused on this issue. Based on a cross-sectional case-control study conducted at the end of February 2020, Hao et al. [15] revealed that non-psychotic psychiatric patients had higher COVID-19 related PTSD symptoms, and higher anxiety, depression, and stress symptom levels than the healthy control group. However, it is unknown if differences in anxiety, depression, and stress symptom levels were larger than under ‘normal’ circumstances. No empirical study has been conducted comparing pre-outbreak recovery-rates with post-outbreak recovery rates among those with existing mental health problems.

Loneliness

Lockdowns and social distancing (including stay-at-home orders) are important preventive measures that may help to reduce the spread of COVID-19, but social isolation could lead to a rise of loneliness [16, 17] creating the so-called social connectivity paradox [16]. Non-COVID-19 studies have shown that loneliness, e.g. the subjective feeling referring to the perceived inadequacy of one’s social relationships, is very prevalent and may be considered a major public concern. Loneliness is negatively associated with all-cause mortality, worse mental health and negative cardiovascular outcomes [1820].

The limited number of COVID-19 related longitudinal studies on loneliness among the general population showed somewhat mixed results. In the earlier mentioned study by McGinty et al. [9], 13.8% always or often felt lonely in April 2020. Based on a comparison with another national survey conducted in April-May 2018, the authors concluded that loneliness increased only slightly (from 11% to 13.8%). Bu et al. [21] compared loneliness data of the large UK Household Longitudinal Study (wave 9; January 2017- June 2019) with loneliness data of the COVID-19 Social Study (March-May 2020), and found that loneliness levels were higher in the COVID-19 Social Study. In the longitudinal study by Killgore et al. [22], using the US Mechanical Turk (MTurk) crowdsourcing platform, loneliness increased significantly from March to June 2020, particularly from April to May 2020. Only one longitudinal study among the adult general population was identified using pre-outbreak data on loneliness of the study sample. Luchetti et al. [23] assessed trajectories of loneliness among a nationwide sample of American adults in response to COVID-19, using data of assessments in January/early February 2020 (before the outbreak), late March (during the President’s initial "15 Days to Slow the Spread" campaign), and late April (during the "stay-at-home" policies of most states). In this study [23] no significant changes in loneliness were found among the total study sample, in contrast to an increase in social support.

In addition, current population wide studies treated loneliness as a unidimensional construct reflected by sum scores of items. The study by Hyland et al. [24] provided support for the presence of four exclusive subtypes of loneliness, e.g. a low loneliness, a social loneliness, a emotional loneliness, and a social loneliness and emotional loneliness subtype. To gain further insight in the effects of this pandemic on loneliness, we therefore formulated the following third research question:

Mental health problems and loneliness

In line with the results of non-COVID-19 studies, COVID-19 related studies among the general adult population found positive relationships between loneliness and mental health problems. For example, the large cross-sectional study by González-Sanguino et al. [25] among a sample of Spanish adults found that loneliness was significantly correlated with depression, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress symptoms at the end of March 2020. The large cross-sectional study by Li and Wang [26], conducted at the end of April 2020 among adult UK residents, showed that loneliness was positively associated with having COVID-19 related symptoms. In the cross-sectional study by Palgi et al. [27], conducted between March 15 and April 2020, loneliness among a large sample of Israeli was positively related with depression and general anxiety symptoms. The study by Killgore et al. [22] among American adults showed that loneliness was significantly correlated with depression and suicidal ideation at the three time points during the pandemic.

The findings above suggest that individuals who were lonely before or during the pandemic more often suffered from mental health problems during this period (from pre-, peri- to post-outbreak) compared to individuals who were not lonely. However, it is unknown to what extent mental health problems increased (or decreased) among individuals who were lonely before and after the outbreak, among individuals who were lonely before or after the outbreak, and individuals who were not lonely in the pre-outbreak period.

Research questions

Aim of the present prospective study is to gain further insight in the effects of the COVID-19 epidemic on the mental health, the recovery of symptoms, loneliness and the associations between loneliness and mental health problem among the general population. More specifically:

  1. To what extent did the post-outbreak prevalence of anxiety and depression symptom levels (June 2020) differ from the pre-outbreak (November 2019) and peri-outbreak (March 2020) prevalence?

  2. To what extent did individuals with anxiety and depression symptom levels during the outbreak (March 2020) differ in recovery of symptoms about three months later (June 2020), from the recovery of individuals with pre-outbreak symptoms (November-December 2019) about three months later (March 2020)?

  3. To what extent did the post-outbreak prevalence of emotional loneliness, social loneliness, and social and emotional loneliness (June 2020) differ from the pre-outbreak prevalence (October 2019)?

  4. To what extent were individuals with pre-outbreak (October 2019) and post-outbreak (June 2020) emotional and/or social loneliness more at risk for pre- (November 2019), peri- (March 2020) and/or post-outbreak (June 2020) anxiety and depression symptoms?

The objective of this prospective study is to answer these research questions using a Dutch population-based probability sample. Respondents participated in surveys in October 2019 (T1), November 2019 (T2), March 2020 (T3), and June 2020 (T4). With respect to anxiety and depression symptoms we focused on the prevalence of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels, and with respect to recovery we made a distinction between the recovery-categories ‘remission’, ‘improved’, ‘unchanged’ and ‘worsened’. Concerning loneliness, we focused on the following subtypes of loneliness: ‘low loneliness’, ‘emotional loneliness’, ‘social loneliness’, and ‘social and emotional’ loneliness.

Materials and methods

Procedures and participants

We extracted and merged data from the Longitudinal Internet studies for the Social Sciences (LISS) panel. This panel is administered by CentERdata and the set-up was funded by the Dutch Research Council (NWO). The LISS panel is based on a traditional probability sample drawn from the Dutch population register by Statistics Netherlands [28]. Participants receive an incentive of 15 euros per hour. Panel members who do not have a computer and/or internet access are provided with the necessary equipment at home (further information about the LISS panel, conducted studies since 2007, and open access data see: https://www.dataarchive.lissdata.nl/; in English). Data of the LISS panel (until March 2020) was also used in the COVID-19 study by van der Velden et al. [12].

Data on loneliness were extracted from two surveys: the (longitudinal) Social Integration and Leisure module of the LISS panel, wave October 2019 (T1: Ninvited = 5,929, response = 84.2%), and the ‘Effects of the Outbreak of Covid-19’-survey conducted in June 2020 (T4: Ninvited = 7,022, response = 79.7%). Data on anxiety and depression symptoms were extracted from three surveys: from the longitudinal Health module of the LISS panel, wave November 2019 (T2: Ninvited = 5,954, response = 86.4%), the third wave of the Victims in Modern Society survey [11] survey conducted in March 2020 (T3: Ninvited = 6,568, response = 83.6%), and the above mentioned ‘Effects of the Outbreak of Covid-19 survey’ conducted in June 2020 (T4). In total 4,084 respondents participated in all four surveys. To optimize the representativeness of the study sample, we weighted the data using 16 exclusive demographic profiles among the total adult Dutch population 2019 (N2019 = 13,926,066), based on the data of Statistics Netherlands (see: https://opendata.cbs.nl/#/CBS/en/; in English). The 16 profiles were constructed using the following demographic characteristics at the end of 2019: sex (male, female), age (18–34, 35–49, 50–64, 65 years and older) and marital status (married and unmarried), yielding 2*4*2 = 16 exclusive demographic profiles. All results are based on the total weighted sample.

Ethical approval and informed consent

According to the Dutch Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act (WMO) the present study did not require approval from a Medical Ethical Testing Committee (METC). Nevertheless, the longitudinal Social Integration and Leisure module and Health module (as part of the Longitudinal Core Study in LISS, starting in 2007) were evaluated and approved by the Board of Overseers, an Internal Review Board (IRB) until 2014. The VICTIMS survey (starting in 2018) was approved by an Institutional Review Board, consisting of external and internal reviewers of CentERdata. The ‘Effects of the Outbreak of Covid-19’ survey was conducted by the Institute for Applied Microeconomics of Universität Bonn in Germany, and in Germany no medical ethical approval is required for this type of research. The ‘Effects of the Outbreak of COVID-19’ survey was evaluated and approved by internal reviewers of CentERdata not involved in designing this study, and used the same questions on mental health and loneliness as the surveys approved by the IRB. In accordance with the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR), participants gave explicit written consent for the use of the collected data for scientific and policy relevant research.

Measures

Anxiety and depression symptoms

Anxiety and depression symptoms were assessed at T2, T3 and T4 using the Mental Health Index or Inventory (5-item subscale of the MOS 36-item short-form health survey) [29, 30]. The MHI-5 asks respondents to rate their mental health during the past month on 6-point Likert scales, such as ‘This past month I felt very anxious’ and ‘I felt depressed and gloomy’ (0 = never, 1 = seldom, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, 4 = mostly, 5 = continuously). After recoding the three negative formulated items, the total scores were computed and multiplied by four (to arrive at a 0–100 scale) where lower scores indicate more anxiety and depression symptoms levels (all Cronbach’s Alpha’s ≥ 0.87). A cut-off score of < 60 [31] was used to identify respondents with moderate to high anxiety and depression symptom levels.

Loneliness

Loneliness was assessed at T1 and T4, using the 6-item De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale [32, 33]. Respondents are asked to rate items such as ‘I often feel deserted’ and ‘there are enough people I can count on in case of a misfortune’ on three-point Likert scales (1 = yes, 2 = more or less, 3 = no). According to the scoring guidelines the items were dichotomized to reflect the ‘presence’ or ‘absence’ of an indicator (item) of loneliness. For the emotional loneliness items, agreement responses were taken to indicate item endorsement, while for the social loneliness items, disagreement responses were taken to indicate item endorsement. Given the analytic strategy (see below) we did not compute Cronbach’s Alpha’s.

Physical illness

Physical illness was assessed at T2. The Health module assessed several Physician-diagnosed Diseases (PD) in the past year (1 = yes, 2 = no) and Health Problems (HP) respondents regularly suffer from (1 = yes, 0 = no). For the present study we focused on reported: 1.) lung problems ((PD = chronic lung disease such as chronic bronchitis or emphysema or asthma) or (HP = short of breath, problems with breathing, or coughing, a stuffy nose or flu-related complaints)); 2.) heart problems ((PD = angina, pain in the chest, a heart attack including infarction or coronary thrombosis or another heart problem including heart failure) or (HP = heart complaints or angina, pain in the chest due to exertion)); and 3.) diabetes (PD = diabetes or a too high blood sugar level).

Data analyses

Repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses were conducted to answer the first research question, while controlling for sex and pre-outbreak age, education level, employment status, domestic situation, employment status, ethnicity, lung problems, heart problems, and diabetes at the end of 2019 (November-December).

To answer the second research question, we applied the Reliable Change Index (RCI) and clinical cut-off (CO)-score [34] to gain insight in how many respondents with moderate-high symptom levels at T2 recovered (remission), improved, faced similar symptom levels (unchanged) or suffered from higher symptom levels at T3 (worsened). The psychometric data required to compute both RCI and CO was based on the data of the total weighted sample at T2 (general population; N = 5.157). Computation was done as follows:

  1. RCI = 1.96*√ 2 (SEM)2, with SEM = SD*(1-Cronbach’s α); and

  2. CO = ((Mmod.-high symptom subgroup*SDgeneral population) + (Mgen. population*SDmod.high symptom subgroup))/ (SDgeneral *SDmod.high symptom subgroup)].

The prevalence of four categories of recovery (change) at T3 among respondents with moderate-high anxiety and depression scores at T2 were computed as follows [35]:

  1. Remission ((Mdifference T2-T3 ≥ RCI) and (MT2 ≥ MT3) and (MT3 < CO));

  2. Improved ((Mdifference T2-T3 ≥ RCI) and (MT2 ≥ MT3) and (MT3 ≥ CO));

  3. Unchanged (Mdifference T2-T3 < RCI); and

  4. Worsened ((Mdifference T2-T3 < RCI) and (MT2 < MT3) nd (MT3 ≥ CO)).

The same procedure was used to assess recovery at T4 among those with moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels at T3. For these analyses MHI-scores were reversed (100-score), where higher scores reflect more symptoms.

The subgroup of respondents with moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels at T3 partly overlapped with the subgroup with high symptom levels at T2. Of the 961 respondents with high symptom levels at T2 or T3, 431 had high symptoms levels at T2 and T3. Because of this overlap, differences in the distribution of these four recovery categories at T3 and T4 could not directly be assessed using a chi-squared test. To solve this non-independence problem and to be able to compare the distribution of recovery categories at T3 and T4 we used the following analytic strategy to obtain exclusive subsamples. Firstly, we randomly split the total study sample into two almost equal groups of respondents (A and B). This resulted in two independent subgroups with high symptom levels at T2 (subgroup A1 and B1) and two independent subgroups with high symptoms levels at T3 (subgroup A2 and B2). Secondly, we assessed the differences in the distribution of recovery categories between the independent subgroups A1 and B2, and between the independent subgroups B1 and A2 using a chi-squared test.

To answer the third research question, we first performed latent class analyses (LCA) to identify subtypes of loneliness with the dichotomized items of the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness scale as indicators. The analyses were performed for T1 and T4 separately. Given the results of Hyland et al. [24] that identified four classes using the instrument, we limit the comparisons of one to seven classes by assessing the relative fit based on reductions in the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), the sample size adjusted BIC, as well as bivariate residuals (BVR’s) [36, 37]. The most parsimonious model with the lowest BIC and sample size adjusted BIC was chosen, while examining the corresponding BVR’s of the model that seemed to be the most parsimonious (BVR’s larger than 3.84 indicate that the estimated models failed to account for the pairwise association of the specific indicators or covariates). In addition, we looked at entropy values that are measures of the quality of classification (approaching one indicates clear delineation of classes) [38]. LCA was conducted using LatentGold 5.1. Subsequently, class membership was added to the respondents’ data. Possible changes in loneliness, e.g. changes in the prevalence of identified classes, were assessed using repeated measures multivariate logistic regression while controlling for the same variables. All non-LCA analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS version 26.

For the fourth research question, multivariate logistic regression analyses and repeated measures multivariate logistic regression were performed with pre-, peri- and post-outbreak anxiety and depression symptoms as dependent variables, while controlling for the same variables as in the previous analyses. Similar to Hyland et al. [24] the LCA revealed four classes at T1 and T4. The four pre-outbreak (T1) and four post-outbreak (T4) classes were used to construct four exclusive subgroups consisting of respondents who were:

  1. not lonely at T1 and not lonely at T4;

  2. not lonely at T1, but social and/or emotional lonely at T4;

  3. social and/or emotional lonely at T1, but not lonely at T4; and

  4. social and/or emotional lonely at both T1 and T4.

Although 4*4 subgroups can be distinguished using the four loneliness classes at T1 and T4, we limited the number of subgroups because of low cell counts in several subgroups (see S1 Appendix), but also to improve the power of the statistical tests. Four repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses were performed to examine the course of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels (T2 to T4) within each subgroup, while series of multivariate logistic regression were performed to examine differences between those four subgroups in the prevalence of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels at T2, T3 and T4.

Results

Characteristics respondents

The characteristics of the respondents are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics study sample (N = 4,084).

November—December 2019
n (%)
Sex
 • male 2,012 (49.3)
 • female 2,072 (50.7)
Education level
 • low 991 (24.3)
medium 1,474 (36.1)
 • high 1,620 (39.7)
Domestic situation
 • (un)married cohabitation without child(ren) 1,399 (34.3)
 • (un)married cohabitation with child(ren) 1,382 (33.8)
 • single with child(ren) 237 (5.8)
 • single 947 (23.2)
 • other 118 (2.9)
Employment status
 • paid employment 2,005 (49.1)
 • self-employed 223 (5.5)
 • unemployed/job seeker 96 (2.4)
 • student 345 (8.4)
 • takes care of housekeeping 317 (7.8)
 • pensioner 775 (19.0)
 • has (partial) work disability 185 (4.5)
 • other 138 (3.4)
Age categories (in years)
 • 18–34 1,089 (26.7)
 • 35–49 966 (23.7)
 • 50–64 1,057 (25.9)
 • 65 and older 972 (23.8)
Ethnicity
 • native 3,203 (78.4)
 • non-native 881 (21.6)
Lung problems
 • no 3,258 (79.8)
 • yes 826 (20.2)
Heart problems
 • no 3,825 (93.7)
 • yes 259 (6.3)
Diabetes
 • no 3,908 (95.7)
 • yes 176 (4.3)

self-employed = autonomous professional, freelancer, self-employed, or works or assists in family business. Educational level: Low = primary education, preparatory intermediate vocational education, or other, medium = higher general secondary/pre-university education, intermediate professional education, high = higher professional education/university.

Changes in prevalence anxiety and depression symptoms

The prevalence of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels was 16.8%, 17.2% and 15.3% at T2, T3 and T4 respectively. Repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses showed that the prevalence decreased significantly at T4 compared to T2 (adjusted OR (AOR) = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.79–0.99, p = 0.026), and compared to T3 (AOR = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.81–0.97, p = 0.007), whereas it did not change significantly between T2 and T3 (AOR = 1.02, 95% CI = 0.94–1.12, p = 0.611).

Recovery anxiety and depression symptoms in months after T2 and T3

In Table 2 the prevalence of the four recovery categories at T3 and T4 of the total study sample and the two split-half samples are presented. A Chi-squared test revealed no significant differences in the distribution of recovery categories at T3 and T4 between subgroup A1 and B2 (χ2 = 3.726, df = 3, p = 0.293), nor between subgroup A2 and B1 (χ2 = 2.58, df = 3, p = 0.461) respectively. These analyses were repeated among those with high anxiety and depression symptom levels (using cut-off < 45 of original scale). No differences in recovery were found (see S2 Appendix). Because of the random sampling to obtain the two half-split samples (see Method section), we repeated the random sampling to rule out the possibility that findings can be attributed to one specific sampling. These control analyses also showed no differences.

Table 2. Symptoms, recovery, loneliness before and during the COVID-19 among general population.

Anxiety and depression symptoms at T3/T4
split-half groups1
Total group Subgroup A1 Subgroup B1
n % n % N %
Anxiety and depression symptoms at T2
 • remission at T3 110 16.1 56 17.0 58 16.2
 • improved at T3 37 5.4 16 4.9 17 4.8
 • unchanged at T3 373 54.3 179 54.4 194 54.3
 • worsened at T3 166 24.2 78 23.7 88 24.6
Total group Subgroup A2 Subgroup B2
n % n % n %
Anxiety and depression symptoms at T3
 • remission at T4 127 18.0 69 20.2 57 15.7
 • improved at T4 42 6.0 18 5.3 25 6.9
 • unchanged at T4 395 56.1 183 53.5 213 58.5
 • worsened at T4 140 19.9 72 21.1 69 19.0

1The total study sample (N = 4084) was randomly split in two exclusive subgroups (subgroup A = 2038 and subgroup B = 2046). Due to weighting the distribution is not exactly 50–50% and therefore the numbers of the subgroups sometimes differ from the total numbers. For all four subgroups recovery was computed like the computations of recovery at T3 and T4 among the total group with moderate-high symptom levels at T2 and T3, using the M’s and SD’s of the four subgroups.

Loneliness subtypes

The results of the LCA showed, similar to Hyland et al. (2019), that the 4-class solution offered the best parsimonious fit with the lowest BIC and SABIC values, relatively low classification errors (≤ 5%) and high Entropy R’s (≥ 0.91) at both T1 and T4 (see S3 Appendix for all details). Given the probabilities of endorsing loneliness items on the four identified classes at T1 and T4 (see S4 Appendix for all details), in parallel with Hyland et al. (2019), class 1 was labelled “low loneliness”, class 2 “emotional loneliness”, class 3 “social and emotional loneliness” and class 4 was labelled “social loneliness”. The distribution of classes at T1 and T4 is presented in Table 3 and indicates that the large majority was not social and/or emotional lonely at T1 or T4. The prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms at T2 among the low loneliness, emotional loneliness, social and emotional loneliness, and social loneliness class at T1 (one month before T2) differed significantly (χ2 = 612, 29, df = 3, p < .001). The prevalence was 8.9%, 40.5%, 54.2% and 25.4% respectively. The same pattern was found at T4 (χ2 = 786.32, df = 3, p < .001): the prevalence was 5.5%, 36.3%, 60.6%, and 23.9% respectively.

Table 3. Course of loneliness classes.

October 2019 (T1) June 2020 (T4)
Loneliness n % n % AOR (95% CI)
 • low loneliness (class 1) 3,095 75.8 2,862 70.1 0.68 (0.63–0.74)***
 • emotional loneliness (class 2) 753 18.4 1014 24.8 1.61 (1.48–1.76)***
 • social and emotional loneliness (class 3) 168 4.1 142 3.5 0.84 (0.69–1.01)
 • social loneliness (class 4) 68 1.7 67 1.6 0.97 (0.72–1.30)1

AOR = Odd Ratios adjusted for age, sex, education, employment status, domestic situation, employment status, ethnicity, lung problems, heart problems, diabetes. 95% CI = 95% Confidence interval. Due to weighting, numbers may slightly differ between Tables.

*** p < .001.

1 AOR without employment status as covariate because of computation problems due to low cell counts.

Changes in loneliness

Repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses (see Table 3) showed that the prevalence of respondents with low loneliness decreased significantly between T1 and T4 (75.8% and 70.1% respectively), while the prevalence of emotional loneliness increased in this period (18.4% and 24.4% respectively). The prevalence of ‘social and emotional loneliness’ and ‘social loneliness’ did not differ significantly between T1 and T4.

The associations between changes in loneliness, and anxiety and depression symptoms

Table 4 provides an overview of the prevalence of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels at T2, T3 and T4 of the subgroups with and without changes in loneliness [Nnot lonely at T1, not lonely at T4 = 2559 (62.7%); Nnot lonely at T1, lonely at T4 = 536 (13.1%); Nlonely at T1, not lonely T4 = 303 (7.4%); Nlonely at T1, lonely at T4 = 686 (16.8%)]. The results of the repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses show no changes in the prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms between T2 and T3 across the four subgroups. However, in the ‘not lonely at T1, not lonely T4’ subgroup’ and ‘lonely at T1, not lonely at T4’ subgroup, the prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms decreased significantly to 4.4% and 14.5% respectively. In subgroup ‘not lonely at T1, lonely at T4’ the prevalence rose significantly from 17.9% at T1 to 26.3% at T4.

Table 4. Course of loneliness and anxiety and depression symptoms.

Anxiety and depression symptoms
Total November 2019 March 2020 June T2 vs. T3 T2 vs. T4 T2 vs. T4
2020
(T2) (T3) (T4)
Loneliness n % n % n % n % AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
not lonely at T1, not lonely at T4 2559 62.7 178 7.0a 205 8.0a 112 4.4a 1.17 (0.99–1.39) 0.62 (0.49–0.77)*** 0.52 (0.43–0.64)***
not lonely at T1, lonely at T4 536 13.1 96 17.9b 104 19.4b 141 26.3b 1.10 (0.86–1.39) 1.63 (1.24–2.14)*** 1.49 (1.18–1.87)***
lonely at T1, not lonely T4 303 7.4 65 21.5b 64 21.1b 44 14.5c 0.91 (0.67–1.24) 0.60 (0.41–0.90)* 0.66 (0.47–0.92)*
lonely at T1, lonely at T4 686 16.8 348 50.7c 332 48.4c 329 48.0d 0.88 (0.75–1.04) 0.88 (0.72–1.07) 0.99 (0.84–1.17)

AOR = Odd Ratios adjusted for age, sex, education, employment status, domestic situation, employment status, ethnicity, lung problems, heart problems, diabetes. Percentages in a column with the same letter in superscript (a, b, c, d) do not differ significantly according to adjusted Odd Ratios. 95% CI = 95% Confidence interval. Due to weighting, numbers may slightly differ between Tables.

* p < 0.05,

** p < 0.01,

*** p < .001. Lonely = social loneliness, emotional loneliness, or social and emotional loneliness.

Despite the absence of changes in the prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms between T2 and T3 across the four subgroups based on the loneliness classes at T1 and T4, the multivariate logistic regression clearly show significant differences in the prevalence between the four groups at T2, T3, and T4. For example, at T4, the prevalence of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptoms levels differed significantly between all subgroups (4.4%, 26.3%, 14.5% and 48% respectively). As expected, respondents who were not lonely at pre- and post-outbreak had the lowest prevalence of moderate-high anxiety and depression symptom levels at T2, T3 and T4.

Discussion

To date, the present study on the effects of COVID-19 on mental health and loneliness among the general population is one of the very few prospective studies based on probability-based samples of the general population, using data on mental health and loneliness of the same study sample just before the COVID-19 outbreak (October-November 2019). The results showed the following clear patterns. Compared to the pre-outbreak (T2) and outbreak (T3) prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms, the prevalence of post-outbreak (T4) symptoms did not increase but decreased slightly but significantly. Interestingly, the study by Wang et al. [39] also showed a significant reduction in depression and anxiety symptoms during the stressful first four weeks after the COVID-19 outbreak in China.

These results seem to differ strongly from the studies by McGinty et al. [22] and Daly et al. [9], and especially Twenge and Joiner [7], but we cannot rule out the possibility that differences in study designs, among other things, play a role in these differences. For instance, these studies compared different pre- and post-COVID outbreak study samples instead of pre- and post-COVID-19 outbreak assessments within the same sample. However, the study by Pierce et al. [11], similar to our study, prospectively assessed mental distress among the same sample showing an increase from 2018–2019 to April 2020. On the other hand, in their population-based study Hyland et al. [40] concluded that the rates of screening positive for generalized anxiety disorder and depression in Ireland around the beginning of April 2020, did not differ markedly from those reported in previous non-COVID-19 national prevalence studies in the UK.

As described earlier [12] the absence of differences in anxiety and depression symptoms within the Dutch study sample may also be related to how the Dutch governments reacted and intervened, and related to the Dutch social welfare and health care systems in the Netherlands. Unemployed adults (or adults who lost their job due to this pandemic) can invoke for unemployment benefits and, in principle, each Dutch citizen has a health care insurance regardless of being employed or not. During the study period the Dutch government very quickly implemented financial support to companies who lost revenues significantly because of the COVID-19 pandemic to allow them to keep people at work. The governmental acts, interventions and management of this pandemic were, until now, not politized in the Netherlands, meaning that the compensation for lost salaries and profits was rather generous, preventing potential sources of additional financial worries and stress to the people concerned.

A relevant research topic not covered in previous longitudinal COVID-19 studies is the recovery of existing mental health problems during this pandemic [14]. We examined, using the Reliable Change Index (RCI) of Jacobson and Truax [34] the extent to which individuals with anxiety and depression symptoms at the beginning of the COVID-19 outbreak (T3) recovered a few months later (T4). We compared the distribution of the four recovery-categories (remission, improved, unchanged and worsened) with the distribution of these categories during the outbreak (T3) among those with anxiety and depression symptoms before the outbreak (T2) and found no significant differences. Moreover, additional analyses among individuals with very high existing MHI-5 scores showed similar results. In other words, we found no support in the data that the COVID-19 pandemic hindered recovery of anxiety and depression symptoms during the first four months. This suggest that we have to be careful to attribute worsening symptoms solely to this pandemic. In addition, our findings do not deny or neglect that people with existing anxiety and depression symptoms may have concerns about their mental illness worsening because of the COVID-19 pandemic [41].

In line with the study of Hyland et al. [24], the latent class analyses revealed four exclusive classes of loneliness: the low loneliness, emotional loneliness, social loneliness, and emotional and social loneliness class. Findings with respect to the course of loneliness during the pandemic differed partly from the findings with respect to anxiety and depression symptoms. The analyses showed that the prevalence of emotional loneliness increased after the outbreak (18.4% versus 24.8%), while the prevalence of individuals who were social lonely or social and emotional lonely did not differ from the pre-outbreak prevalence. The prevalence of individuals with low loneliness decreased slightly from 75.8% to 70.1%, suggesting that the lockdowns and social distancing (including stay-at-home orders) negatively affected loneliness for a relatively small group [9, 11, 22]. The absence of differences between pre- and post-outbreak social loneliness and social and emotional loneliness suggest that people have found ways to keep social contacts, such as via (mobile) telephone, social media (e.g. Whatsapp, Facebook) and video calls (e.g. Skype, ZOOM), but that these communication facilities may not always provide emotional connectedness. Although we controlled for demographics and physical illness, it was outside the aim and scope of the present study to further discuss which groups were more at risk to become emotional lonely. Our results seem to differ from the outcomes of the prospective study of Luchetti et al. [23], the only other prospective study on loneliness among the general population using a longitudinal sample. In their study, however, loneliness was treated as a unidimensional concept.

As expected, and in line with previous research, individuals with persistent loneliness (lonely before and after the COVID-19 outbreak) much more often suffered from anxiety and depression symptoms before, during and after the outbreak (prevalence ≥ 48%) than individuals who were not lonely before and after the outbreak (prevalence ≤ 8%). Importantly, results furthermore showed that the prevalence of symptoms among the four subgroups (not lonely at T1, not lonely at T4; not lonely at T1, lonely at T4; lonely at T1, not lonely at T4; lonely at T1, lonely at T4) did not change between November 2019 and March 2020 suggesting that the effects of all social distancing measures took place after the outbreak. Interestingly, among those who became lonely, the prevalence of symptoms increased, while among those who were not lonely anymore after the outbreak or not lonely before and after the outbreak, the prevalence of symptoms decreased after the outbreak (June 2020). These findings indicate that loneliness plays a key role in the presence of anxiety and depression symptoms during this pandemic. The results also suggest that COVID-19 related mental health policies should target individuals who became lonely after the COVID-19 outbreak (13.1% of the total study sample) and especially target individuals who were and are lonely after the outbreak (16.8% of the total study sample). Unfortunately, due to the low cell counts we could not examine the prevalence of symptoms among all sixteen combinations of the pre- and post-outbreak loneliness classes. Future studies are warranted to examine loneliness on the longer term to assess the extent to which patterns in loneliness remain stable or change.

Strengths and limitations

This study has its strengths and limitations. The prospective study design with pre-COVID-19 data on loneliness, anxiety and depression symptoms, use of a longitudinal population-based probability sample with high response-rates, and use of well-validated questionnaires are major strengths of the current study. We used the data of a large group of respondents (N = 4,084) who participated in all four waves. However, we did not conduct clinical interviews to assess mental disorders such as generalized anxiety disorder and schizophrenia nor examined other relevant mental health problems such as sleeping problems and fatigue, which would have enriched our study. For instance, we cannot rule out the possibility that the recovery of depression, fatigue or sleeping problems after the COVID-19 outbreak is, due to all (practical) changes, lower than before the outbreak. In addition, we were not able to assess mental health consequences among people specific mental disorders such as schizophrenia [42]. The most recent assessment in this study took place in June 2020 and future follow-ups are needed to gain insight in the effects of the pandemic on the longer term given the uncertainties about how this pandemic will develop and when effective vaccines will become available. In addition, we did not include children and adolescents of the Dutch general population who might be affected differently by the COVID-19 pandemic. We have no data on anxiety and depression symptoms, and loneliness covering an exact similar study period (October-November to June) to rule out possible seasonal effects: it is basically possible that for instance the decrease of the prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms is caused by this effect.

Results in this paper are based on data that were representative for the Dutch population. It is unclear to what extent the results can be generalized to other developed countries who were hit harder by the COVID-19 pandemic or where the individual-level employment consequences were more severe, let alone the global South. The Dutch economy is projected to recover quickly from the COVID-19 health crisis in 2021 with a reduction in economic growth of -4% in 2020 and a positive forecasted growth of almost 3% in 2021 [43], and countries differ on these topics [44].

Final conclusions

Taken together, these results suggest that the Dutch population was capable to cope with and to adjust successfully to the drastic changes of their lives as a result of the COVID-19 pandemic outbreak during the first four months of 2020 (March 2020 to June 2020; cf. [12, 45, 46]. We did not find an increase in the prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms during the first four months of the outbreak. And among those with symptoms we found similar recovery-rates during the first four months compared to before the pandemic. Nevertheless, the prevalence of emotional loneliness increased to some extent, and an increase in loneliness, especially persistent loneliness, was very strongly associated with the presence of anxiety and depression symptoms.

About 100 years ago (1918–1919) the world was confronted with another global devastating pandemic; the Spanish flu. It is estimated that about one-third of the world’s population was infected and at least 50 million people were killed by the Spanish flu [47]. To gain further insight in the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic it might be of interest, besides future empirical research following the COVID-19 outbreak, to include historical research on the effects of the Spanish flu [48] on for instance (mental) health, family life and local communities. It may further help to formulate future research questions based on the lessons learned from the Spanish flu.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Tabel S1.

Cross-tabulation loneliness classes at T1 and T4.

(DOCX)

S2 Appendix. Table S2.

Recovery in the months following at T2 and T3.

(DOCX)

S3 Appendix. Table S3.

Results LCA loneliness.

(DOCX)

S4 Appendix. Table S4.

Latent class profiles of loneliness at T1 and T4.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

The study was conducted using the Dutch Longitudinal Internet studies for the Social Sciences (LISS) panel [27]. The LISS panel started in 2007 and is based on a large traditional probability sample drawn from the Dutch population. The Dutch Research Counsel (NWO) funded the set-up of LISS. Panel members receive an incentive of €15 per hour for their participation and those who do not have a computer and/or Internet access are provided with the necessary equipment at home. Further information about all conducted surveys and regulations for free access to the data, as well as the data files can be found at www.lissdata.nl (in English). The LISS panel has received the international Data Seal of Approval (see https://www.datasealofapproval.org/en/). All data of studies conducted with the LISS panel are anonymized.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by Fonds Slachtofferhulp, The Haque, The Netherlands (50006/VICTIMS). One of the authors (CC) is deputy director at Fonds Slachtofferhulp. The funder had no further role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Pascarella G, Strumia A, Piliego C, Bruno F, Del Buono R, Costa F, et al. COVID-19 diagnosis and management: a comprehensive review. J Intern Med. 2020; 288:192–206. 10.1111/joim.13091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.de Rosa S, Spaccarotella C, Basso C, Calabrò MP, Curcio A, Perrone Filardi P, et al. Reduction of hospitalizations for myocardial infarction in Italy in the COVID-19 era. Eur Heart J. 2020; ehaa409 10.1093/eurheartj/ehaa409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sheridan A, Andersen AL, Hansen ET, Johannesen N. Social distancing laws cause only small losses of economic activity during the COVID-19 pandemic in Scandinavia PNAS. 2020; 117: 20468–20473. 10.1073/pnas.2010068117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.IMF. The Great Lockdown: Worst Economic Downturn Since the Great Depression. International Monetary Fund, 2020. https://blogs.imf.org/2020/04/14/the-great-lockdown-worst-economic-downturn-since-the-great-depression/ (accessed April 14, 2020).
  • 5.IMF. COVID-19 Crisis Poses Threat to Financial Stability. International Monetary Fund, 2020. https://blogs.imf.org/2020/04/14/covid-19-crisis-poses-threat-to-financial-stability/ (accessed April 14, 2020).
  • 6.Vindegaard N, Benros ME. COVID-19 pandemic and mental health consequences: Systematic review of the current evidence. Brain Behav Immun. 2020; S0889-1591(20)30954-5. 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Xiong J, Lipsitz O, Nasri F, Lui LMW, Gill H, Phan L, et al. , Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2020; 277: 55–64. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Twenge JM, Joiner TE. U.S. Census Bureau-assessed prevalence of anxiety and depressive symptoms in 2019 and during the 2020 COVID‐19 pandemic. Depress. Anxiety 2020; 1–3. 10.1002/da.23077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.McGinty EE, Presskreischer R, Han H, Barry CL. Psychological distress and loneliness reported by US adults in 2018 and April 2020. JAMA. 2020; 324: 93–94. 10.1001/jama.2020.9740 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Daly M, Sutin A, Robinson E. Depression reported by US adults in 2017–2018 and March and April 2020. J Affect Disord. 2020. In press. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Pierce M, Hope H, Ford T, Hatch S, Hotopf M, John A, et al. Mental health before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: a longitudinal probability sample survey of the UK population. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020; 21 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30308-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.van der Velden PG, Contino C, Das M, van Loon P, Bosmans MWG. Anxiety and depression symptoms, and lack of emotional support among the general population before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. A prospective national study on prevalence and risk factors. J Affective Disord. 2020a. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schlegl S, Maier J, Meule A, Voderholzer U, 2020. Eating disorders in times of the COVID-19 pandemic-Results from an online survey of patients with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2020; 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sheridan Rains L, Johnson S, Barnett P, Steare T, Needle JJ, Carr S, et al. , COVID-19 Mental Health Policy Research Unit Group, 2020. Early impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health care and on people with mental health conditions: framework synthesis of international experiences and responses. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2020; 1–12. 10.1007/s00127-020-1924-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hao F, Tan W, Jiang L, Zhang L, Zhao X, Zou Y, et al. Do psychiatric patients experience more psychiatric symptoms during COVID-19 pandemic and lockdown? A case-control study with service and research implications for immunopsychiatry. Brain Behav Immun. 2020; 87: 100–106. 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Smith BJ, Lim MH, 2020. How the COVID-19 pandemic is focusing attention on loneliness and social isolation. Public Health Res Pract. 2020; 30; 3022008 10.17061/phrp3022008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Philip KEJ, Polkey MI, Hopkinson NS, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Social isolation, loneliness and physical performance in older-adults: fixed effects analyses of a cohort study. Sci Rep. 2020; 10: 13908 10.1038/s41598-020-70483-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann Behav Med. 2020; 40: 218–227. 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Heinrich LM, Gullone E. The clinical significance of loneliness: a literature review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006; 26: 695–718. 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Leigh-Hunt N, Bagguley D, Bash K, Turner V, Turnbull S, Valtorta N, et al. An overview of systematic reviews on the public health consequences of social isolation and loneliness. Public Health, 2017; 152: 157–171. 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.07.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bu F, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Who is lonely in lockdown? Cross-cohort analyses of predictors of loneliness before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Public Health. 2020; 5: 31–34. 10.1016/j.puhe.2020.06.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Killgore WDS, Cloonan SA, Taylor EC, Miller MA, Dailey NS. Three months of loneliness during the COVID-19 lockdown. Psychiatry Res. 2020; 293: 113392 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Luchetti M, Lee JH, Aschwanden D, Sesker A, Strickhouser JE, Terracciano A, et al. The trajectory of loneliness in response to COVID-19. Am. Psychol. 2020. 10.1037/amp0000690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hyland P, Shevlin M, Cloitre M, Karatzias T, Vallières F, McGinty G, et al. Quality not quantity: loneliness subtypes, psychological trauma, and mental health in the US adult population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2019; 5: 1089–1099. 10.1007/s00127-018-1597-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.González-Sanguino C, Ausín B, Castellanos MÁ, Saiz J, López-Gómez A, Ugidos C, et al. Mental health consequences during the initial stage of the 2020 Coronavirus pandemic (COVID-19) in Spain. Brain Behav Immun. 2020; 87: 172–176. 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Li LZ, Wang S. Prevalence and predictors of general psychiatric disorders and loneliness during COVID-19 in the United Kingdom. Psychiatry Res. 2020; 291: 113267 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Palgi Y, Shrira A, Ring L, Bodner E, Avidor S, Bergman Y, et al. The loneliness pandemic: Loneliness and other concomitants of depression, anxiety and their comorbidity during the COVID-19 outbreak. J Affect Disord. 2020; 275: 109–111. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Scherpenzeel A, Das M. True longitudinal and probability based internet panels: evidence from The Netherlands In: Das M, Ester P, Kaczmirek L, editors. Social and behavioral esearch and the internet: advances in applied methods and research strategies. Taylor & Francis, New York, 2011, 77–104. 10.4324/9780203844922 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey, SF-36: I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med. Care 1992; 30; 473–483. 10.2307/3765916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Means-Christensen AJ, Arnau RC, Tonidandel AM, Bramson R, Meagher MW. An efficient method of identifying major depression and panic disorder in primary care. J Behav Med. 2005; 28: 565–572. 10.1007/s10865-005-9023-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Driessen M. Een beschrijving van de MHI-5 in de gezondheidsmodule van het Permanent Onderzoek Leefsituatie [A Description of the MHI-5 in the Health Module of Permanent Research of Living Conditions, POLS]. Den Haag, Statistics Netherlands, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.de Jong Gierveld J, van Tilburg TG. A 6-item scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness: confirmatory tests on survey data. Res Aging. 2006; 28: 582–598. 10.1177/0164027506289723 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.de Jong Gierveld J, van Tilburg T. The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: tested on data from 7 countries in the UN generations and gender surveys. Eur J Ageing. 2010; 7: 121–130. 10.1007/s10433-010-0144-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991; 59: 12–19. 10.1037//0022-006x.59.1.12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dingemans AE, van Son GE, Aardoom JJ, Bruidegom K, Slof-Op’t Landt MC, van Furth EF. Predictors of psychological outcome in patients with eating disorders: A routine outcome monitoring study. Int J Eat Disord. 2016; 49: 863–873. 10.1002/eat.22560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Magidson J, Vermunt JK. Latent class models In Kaplan D (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative methodology for the social sciences (pp. 175–198). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vermunt, J.K, Magidson J. Latent GOLD 5.0 Upgrade Manual. Belmont MA, Statistical Innovations Inc, 2013.
  • 38.Celeux G, Soromenho G. An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. J. Classification 1996; 13: 195–212. 10.1007/BF01246098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, McIntyre RS, et al. A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav Immun. 2020; 87: 40–48. 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hyland P, Shevlin M, McBride O, Murphy J, Karatzias T, Bentall RP, et al. Anxiety and depression in the Republic of Ireland during the COVID-19 pandemic. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2020; 142: 249–256. 10.1111/acps.13219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Costa M, Pavlo A, Reis G, Ponte K, Davidson L. COVID-19 Concerns Among Persons With Mental Illness. Psychiatr Serv. 2020; 71: 1188–1190. 10.1176/appi.ps.202000245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kozloff N, Mulsant BH, Stergiopoulos V, Voineskos AN. The COVID-19 global pandemic: implications for people with schizophrenia and related disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2020; 46: 752–757. 10.1093/schbul/sbaa051Advance [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.CPB (Centraal Planbureau). Novemberraming: Economische vooruitzichten 2021[In Dutch: November estimates: Economic Forecasts]. Centraal Planbureau (CPB), The Hague, The Netherlands, 2020.
  • 44.OECD (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development). OECD Economic outlook December 2020. http://www.oecd.org/economic-outlook/december-2020/. Accessed 8-12-2020.
  • 45.Chen S, Bonanno GA. Psychological adjustment during the global outbreak of COVID-19: A resilience perspective. Psychol Trauma. 2020; 12: s51–s54. 10.1037/tra000068510.1037/tra0000685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wessely S. Don’t panic! Short and long term psychological reactions to the new terrorism: the role of information and the authorities. J Mental Health. 2005; 14: 1–6. 10.1080/09638230500048099 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.CDC. Pandemic (H1N1 virus). March 20 2019. https://www.cdc.gov/flu/pandemic-resources/1918-pandemic-h1n1.html. Accessed 7-12-2020
  • 48.Spinney L. Pale rider: The Spanish flu of 1918 and how it changed the world. London: Jonathan Cape; 2017. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Fuschia M Sirois

4 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-31580

Anxiety and depression symptoms, the recovery from symptoms and loneliness before and after the COVID-19 outbreak among the general population. Findings from a Dutch population-based longitudinal study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. van der Velden,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Both reviewers were positive about your work and believe with some minor revisions, it could be suitable for publication. I agree with their assessment. Please see their detailed and useful comments and  be sure to address each in your revision.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 18 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Fuschia M. Sirois, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

3.We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This research article aims to gain an insight into the effects of the Covid-19 pandemic on mental health and loneliness. In the current paper, the authors have extracted data from a sample of the Dutch population and examined loneliness and mental health using well-established validated measures. This is a timely piece of research conducted during the pandemic, and one of the few prospective studies I have seen that have studied the impact of covid-19 on mental health and loneliness, and so I welcome this piece of research to the field. I do have a few minor comments.

1. In the abstract it would be useful to have some p values alongside percentages if possible.

2. The 4 research questions are scattered throughout the introduction and I wonder if these could be either summarised at the end of this section or moved to the Methods section.

3. The authors may wish to make reference to whether these results can be generalised (perhaps in the limitations section).

4. Minor suggestions for re-wording and few errors on the attached pdf.

Reviewer #2: This is a really interesting and well conducted study on an extremely important topic, and the results are important, both now and for the future. I don't have any major suggestions for improvements because the research questions you selected and the methods you used to investigate them were sound, the analysis was detailed and the presentation of results and discussion very carefully done.

One area that interested me was in the interpretation of results. Given your method, one element that was naturally missing was the self-reported in depth perspectives of participants, and a qualitative component would have been needed to pursue that. I appreciate this was not what you set out to do, and your approach is absolutely valid. However, I was left wondering, as perhaps you were, how to interpret some of the results. Given how multi-factorial mental health and loneliness are, and how unusual the global pandemic has been, were there were some aspects of the common experience which might have impacted on individuals' experience? For example, did the seriousness of the situation, and our common experience across the country and indeed around the world, provide a unifying experience for people, who may otherwise not have felt that they had much in common with others, and thus counteract the tendency for lockdown to engender loneliness? What has really been going on when so many of us have been connecting through online meetings and video calls - how has this effected levels of isolation? Did the amount of expressed emotion during 2020, which we know has connections to psychological well-being, have an impact on anxiety and depression as we faced the pandemic and discussed it widely? Could the experience of existential or ontological anxiety which many of us have faced have a deeper impact on anxiety levels, including paradoxically to decrease everyday or ontic anxiety? I mention these ideas not to criticise your work, which is excellent and very thought-provoking, but to suggest that there could be other processes which your study was not unreasonably not designed to detect, but which could have had a protective effect on participants and showed up in the data.

I don't think you necessarily need to add anything to your discussion, which was a very good interpretation of your results. These thoughts just occurred to me, and I therefore wanted to pass them on for interest.

Finally, there was a typo error, I believe, on page 21. You state:

"Firstly, we randomly split the total study sample into two almost equal groups of respondents (A and B), This resulted in two

independent subgroups with high symptom levels at T2 (subgroup A1 and B1) and two independent subgroups with high symptoms levels at T3 (subgroup A1 and B2)."

I believe this should have been (subgroup A2 and B2)...?

Congratulations on producing an excellent paper :-)

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Chris Blackmore

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Manuscript with comments.pdf

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 7;16(1):e0245057. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245057.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


9 Dec 2020

We would like to thank the reviewers very much for their time and effort, and very clear suggestions to improve our manuscript. We really appreciated the kind words and compliments of the reviewers.

---------------------------------------------------

REVIEWER #1:

1. This research article aims to gain an insight into the effects of the Covid-19 pandemic on mental health and loneliness. In the current paper, the authors have extracted data from a sample of the Dutch population and examined loneliness and mental health using well-established validated measures. This is a timely piece of research conducted during the pandemic, and one of the few prospective studies I have seen that have studied the impact of covid-19 on mental health and loneliness, and so I welcome this piece of research to the field. I do have a few minor comments.

Response:

Thank you very much for your kind words.

2. In the abstract it would be useful to have some p values alongside percentages if possible.

Response:

Based on this comment we added the p-values, but after adding the p-values we crossed the word-count limit of 300 words (even after we tried to shorten other sections). However, we agree with the reviewer that information about statistical significance was missing, and we therefore revised the results sections as follows (and shortened the Objectives part of the Abstract):

“Results. Repeated measures multivariate logistic regression analyses (RMMLRA) showed a statistical significant lower prevalence of anxiety and depression symptoms after the outbreak (T4=15.3%) than before (T2=16.8%) and during the COVID-19 outbreak (T3=17.2%). According to the Reliable Change Index, the distribution of recovery categories (remission, improvement, unchanged and worsening symptoms) after the outbreak did not differ significantly from the distribution of these categories before the outbreak. RMMLRA revealed that the prevalence of emotional loneliness increased significantly after the outbreak (T1=18.4%, T4=24.8%). Among individuals who were not lonely before and after the outbreak the prevalence of symptoms decreased significantly (T2=7.0%, T4=4.4%) and, likewise, among those who were not lonely anymore after the outbreak (T2=21.5%, T4=14.5%). However, the prevalence of symptoms increased significantly among those who became lonely during the pandemic (T2=17.9%, T4=26.3%).”

3. The 4 research questions are scattered throughout the introduction and I wonder if these could be either summarised at the end of this section or moved to the Methods section.

Response:

We agree with the reviewer and have summarized the research questions at the end of the introduction section. The intro sections now ends with:

Aim of the present prospective study is to gain further insight in the effects of the COVID-19 epidemic on the mental health, the recovery of symptoms, loneliness and the associations between loneliness and mental health problem among the general population. More specifically:

1. To what extent did the post-outbreak ………”

Because we moved the research questions, we added sub-headings in the introduction section to improve the readability of the introduction (new sub-headings are Mental health problems, Recovery of symptoms, Loneliness, Mental health problems and loneliness, Research questions).

4. The authors may wish to make reference to whether these results can be generalised (perhaps in the limitations section).

Response:

Based on this comment, in the limitations section we added:

“Results in this paper are based on data that were representative for the Dutch population. It is unclear to what extent the results can be generalized to other developed countries who were hit harder by the COVID-19 pandemic or where the individual-level employment consequences were more severe, let alone the global South. The Dutch economy is projected to recover quickly from the COVID-19 health crisis in 2021 with a reduction in economic growth of -4% in 2020 and a positive forecasted growth of almost 3% in 2021 [43], and countries differ on these topics [44]..”.

[43] CPB (Centraal Planbureau). Novemberraming: Economische vooruitzichten 2021[In Dutch: November estimates: Economic Forecasts]. Centraal Planbureau (CPB), The Hague, The Netherlands, 2020.

[44] OECD (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development). OECD Economic outlook December 2020. Available from: http://www.oecd.org/economic-outlook/december-2020/. Accessed 8-12-2020.

5. Minor suggestions for re-wording and few errors on the attached pdf.

Response:

Thank you very much for your detailed comments.

5.1. Lots of percentages given here, could some p-values be added from the results section?

Response:

See our response to point 2

5.2. …may benefit from adding a reference for what COVID-19 is (e.g. WHO 2020 reference/report) to give context of the disease and symptoms

Response:

We have revised the first sentence in (and re-numbered all references)

“The COVID-19 pandemic [1] negatively…….

[1] Pascarella G, Strumia A, Piliego C, Bruno F, Del Buono R, Costa F, Scarlata S, Agrò FE. COVID-19 diagnosis and management: a comprehensive review. J Intern Med. 2020; 288:192-206. https://doi.org/10.1111/joim.13091

5.3. re-word sentence perhaps?

Response:

We have deleted this sentence.

5.4. "male" and "female" might work better here

Response:

We have replaced “man” and “women” with “male” and “female”.

5.5. delete extra comma

Response:

We have deleted the comma

5.6. reword - "unemployed adults"

Response:

We have revised this sentence into:

“Unemployed adults (or adults who lost their job due to this pandemic) can invoke for…… “

5.7. "may" not?

Response:

We added “may”.

-------------------------------------------------- 

REVIEWER #2:

6. This is a really interesting and well conducted study on an extremely important topic, and the results are important, both now and for the future. I don't have any major suggestions for improvements because the research questions you selected and the methods you used to investigate them were sound, the analysis was detailed and the presentation of results and discussion very carefully done.

Response:

Thank you very much for your compliments.

7. One area that interested me was in the interpretation of results. Given your method, one element that was naturally missing was the self-reported in depth perspectives of participants, and a qualitative component would have been needed to pursue that. I appreciate this was not what you set out to do, and your approach is absolutely valid. However, I was left wondering, as perhaps you were, how to interpret some of the results. Given how multi-factorial mental health and loneliness are, and how unusual the global pandemic has been, were there were some aspects of the common experience which might have impacted on individuals' experience? For example, did the seriousness of the situation, and our common experience across the country and indeed around the world, provide a unifying experience for people, who may otherwise not have felt that they had much in common with others, and thus counteract the tendency for lockdown to engender loneliness?

What has really been going on when so many of us have been connecting through online meetings and video calls - how has this effected levels of isolation? Did the amount of expressed emotion during 2020, which we know has connections to psychological well-being, have an impact on anxiety and depression as we faced the pandemic and discussed it widely? Could the experience of existential or ontological anxiety which many of us have faced have a deeper impact on anxiety levels, including paradoxically to decrease everyday or ontic anxiety? I mention these ideas not to criticise your work, which is excellent and very thought-provoking, but to suggest that there could be other processes which your study was not unreasonably not designed to detect, but which could have had a protective effect on participants and showed up in the data.

I don't think you necessarily need to add anything to your discussion, which was a very good interpretation of your results. These thoughts just occurred to me, and I therefore wanted to pass them on for interest.

Response:

Thank you for sharing your thoughts on these issues. Your thoughts automatically reminded us of of Spanish flue 100 years ago and how people dealt with all consequences (estimates of global death rates are between 50 and 100 million): what can we learn from this previous devastating pandemic in this perspective? We think historical research on effects of the Spanish flu of interest for empirical researchers too. It may help to focus and formulate history-informed research questions and hypotheses. Based on your thoughts we therefore added at the end of the discussion section:

About 100 years ago (1918-1919) the world was confronted with another global devastating pandemic; the Spanish flu. It is estimated that about one-third of the world’s population was infected and at least 50 million people were killed by the Spanish flu [47]. To gain further insight in the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic it might be of interest, besides future empirical research following the COVID-19 outbreak, to include historical research on the effects of the Spanish flu [48] on for instance (mental) health, family life and local communities. It may further help to formulate future research questions based on the lessons learned from the Spanish flu.”

[47] CDC. 1918 Pandemic (H1N1 virus). March 20 2019. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/flu/pandemic-resources/1918-pandemic-h1n1.html. Accessed 7-12-2020

[48] Spinney L. Pale rider: The Spanish flu of 1918 and how it changed the world. London: Jonathan Cape; 2017.

8. Finally, there was a typo error, I believe, on page 21. You state:

"Firstly, we randomly split the total study sample into two almost equal groups of respondents (A and B), This resulted in two independent subgroups with high symptom levels at T2 (subgroup A1 and B1) and two independent subgroups with high symptoms levels at T3 (subgroup A1 and B2)." I believe this should have been (subgroup A2 and B2)...?

Response:

The reviewer is correct, and we have revised the sentence into:

This resulted in two independent subgroups with high symptom levels at T2 (subgroup A1 and B1) and two independent subgroups with high symptoms levels at T3 (subgroup A2 and B2).

9. Congratulations on producing an excellent paper :-)

Response:

Thanks a lot. 

-----------------------------------------------------------

Comments of PLOS ONE

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.

Response:

We have checked and revised our manuscript to meet PLOS ONE's style requirements.

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

Response:

In the original manuscript we wrote: “In accordance with the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR), participants gave explicit written consent for the use of the collected data for scientific and policy relevant research.”

We have revised this sentence in:

“In accordance with the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR), participants gave explicit written consent for the use of the collected data for scientific and policy relevant research”.

3. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

Response:

We are not sure if we understand this comment correctly because all requested information was given (“…….Further information about all conducted surveys and regulations for free access to the data, as well as the data files can be found at www.lissdata.nl (in English)….. All data of studies conducted with the LISS panel are anonymized. Data of surveys conducted in 2020 will be added to the open access data archive soon”.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

Response

We have included the requested captions.

Attachment

Submitted filename: letter to reviewers 8-12-2020.docx

Decision Letter 1

Fuschia M Sirois

22 Dec 2020

Anxiety and depression symptoms, the recovery from symptoms and loneliness before and after the COVID-19 outbreak among the general population. Findings from a Dutch population-based longitudinal study

PONE-D-20-31580R1

Dear Dr. van der Velden,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Fuschia M. Sirois, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Fuschia M Sirois

28 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-31580R1

Anxiety and depression symptoms, the recovery from symptoms, and loneliness before and after the COVID-19 outbreak among the general population. Findings from a Dutch population-based longitudinal study

Dear Dr. van der Velden:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Fuschia M. Sirois

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Appendix. Tabel S1.

    Cross-tabulation loneliness classes at T1 and T4.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Appendix. Table S2.

    Recovery in the months following at T2 and T3.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Appendix. Table S3.

    Results LCA loneliness.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Appendix. Table S4.

    Latent class profiles of loneliness at T1 and T4.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Manuscript with comments.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: letter to reviewers 8-12-2020.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The study was conducted using the Dutch Longitudinal Internet studies for the Social Sciences (LISS) panel [27]. The LISS panel started in 2007 and is based on a large traditional probability sample drawn from the Dutch population. The Dutch Research Counsel (NWO) funded the set-up of LISS. Panel members receive an incentive of €15 per hour for their participation and those who do not have a computer and/or Internet access are provided with the necessary equipment at home. Further information about all conducted surveys and regulations for free access to the data, as well as the data files can be found at www.lissdata.nl (in English). The LISS panel has received the international Data Seal of Approval (see https://www.datasealofapproval.org/en/). All data of studies conducted with the LISS panel are anonymized.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES