Abstract
Background
Neurocognitive impairment is a common complication of congenital heart disease (CHD) as well as acquired cardiovascular disease. Data are limited on neurocognitive function in adults with CHD (ACHD).
Methods and Results
A total of 1020 individuals with mild‐to‐moderate ACHD and 497 987 individuals without ACHD from the volunteer‐based UK Biobank study underwent neurocognitive tests for fluid intelligence, reaction time, numeric memory, symbol‐digit substitution, and trail making at enrollment and follow‐up. Performance scores were compared before and after exclusion of preexisting stroke or coronary artery disease as measures of cerebro‐ and cardiovascular disease. Individuals with ACHD had significantly poorer performance on alpha‐numeric trail making, a measure of visual attention and cognitive flexibility, spending 6.4 seconds longer on alpha‐numeric trail making (95% CI, 3.0–9.9 seconds, P=0.002) and 2.5 seconds longer on numeric trail making (95% CI, 0.5–4.6 seconds, P=0.034), a measure of visual attention and processing speed. The ACHD cohort had modestly lower performance on symbol‐digit substitution, a measure of processing speed, with 0.9 fewer correct substitutions (95% CI, − 1.5 to − 0.2 substitutions, P=0.021). After excluding preexisting stroke or coronary artery disease, individuals with ACHD continued to show poorer performance in all 6 domains (P=NS).
Conclusions
Individuals with mild‐to‐moderate ACHD had poorer neurocognitive performance, most significantly in tests of cognitive flexibility, analogous to deficits in children with CHD. These differences appear to be driven by increased burden of cerebro‐ and cardiovascular disease among individuals with ACHD.
Keywords: adult congenital heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, cognitive deficits, cognitive dysfunction, cognitive impairment, coronary artery disease, neurocognitive
Subject Categories: Pediatrics, Quality and Outcomes
Neurocognitive differences and developmental delay are recognized complications of congenital heart disease (CHD), detectable in school‐aged children with persistence into adolescence. 1 , 2 , 3 A diverse set of risk factors for neurocognitive deficits in children are well described, including perinatal risk factors, cyanosis, hypoperfusion, and perioperative complications that may manifest as impaired myelination and white‐matter brain injury in up to 50% of neonates with repaired CHD. 4 , 5 The severity of neurological impairment is directly correlated with the complexity of CHD, with the most severe deficits observed in individuals with single ventricle defects. 6
With increasing survivorship to adulthood, individuals with adult congenital heart disease (ACHD) have been observed to have an increased risk for depression, post‐traumatic stress disorder, decreased quality of life, and neurodegenerative disease. 1 , 7 Previously, we have shown a substantially increased burden of cardiovascular risk and disease in a small subset of individuals with ACHD among a large British cohort study of volunteers. 8 While the deleterious effects of acquired cardiovascular disease on neurocognitive functioning in normal adults have previously been reported, 9 , 10 data on the neurocognitive effects in the ACHD population are minimal. Here we report the performance of individuals with mild‐to‐moderate ACHD on 6 neurocognitive tests and specifically examine the impact of acquired cardiovascular disease upon these outcomes.
Methods
Study Population and Data Access
The study population was derived from the UK Biobank (UKB), which includes >502 000 middle‐ and older‐aged volunteers from the general UK population who have undergone detailed phenotypic and functional assessments. All data from the UKB used in this study are available to qualified researchers from the UKB organization (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/using-the‐resource/) for a nominal application fee and all methods are available upon request from the authors. CHD was defined as any structural cardiac abnormality present in the heart or adjoining great vessels from birth in the absence of syndromic illness (ie, syndromes characterized by extracardiac or neurocognitive manifestations in addition to cardiac malformations). As previously described, we used a multistep classification algorithm derived from hospital encounter statistics and self‐reported medical history at the time of enrollment in the UKB, to identify 1020 individuals with ACHD (excluding isolated aortic valve defects) and 497 983 non‐ACHD comparison individuals. 8 A minority of individuals with ACHD (n = 96) were determined as having “complex” lesions associated with cyanosis (ie, single ventricle lesions). The remaining 924 individuals with ACHD represent mild‐to‐moderate lesions, most predominantly left‐to‐right shunt lesions.
Institutional Review Board Approval
The study of de‐identified patient data was determined as exempt from oversight by the Stanford University Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was obtained at the time of enrollment by the UKB research organization. The study was performed using data from UKB applications 15860 and 13721.
Neurocognitive Domains
All UKB participants were invited to complete a set of novelly designed computer‐based neurocognitive tests of fluid intelligence, reaction time, pairs matching, and numeric memory at enrollment. These tests have been previously described with demonstration of sufficient stability in all but pairs matching, which was excluded from this study. 11 Standardized tests of symbol‐digit substitution (adapted from the Symbol Digit Modalities Test) and trail making (adapted from the Halstead‐Reitan Trail Making Test [TMT]) were completed at a mean follow‐up time of 5.82 years via an online platform. 12 The difference in scores between the numeric TMT (TMT‐A) and alpha‐numeric TMT (TMT‐B) was computed to assess for cognitive flexibility. 13 Table 1 lists the definitions, names, and descriptions of domains tested, assessment instructions, scoring, and examples of real‐life applications of neurocognitive deficits within each domain.
Table 1.
Test Name | Neurocognitive Domain Tested | Domain Characteristics | Test Description | Real‐life Application | Scoring |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fluid Intelligence | Reasoning | Problem‐solving using logic and reasoning ability, independent of acquired knowledge | Multiple choice questions | Problem‐solving, reasoning, abstraction, decision making | Number answered correctly in 2 min (range 0‐13) |
Reaction Time | Simple Processing Speed | Processing speed, sustained attention, and inhibition | Match pairs of symbols | Attention, starting and stopping tasks, driving a car, inhibiting actions/verbalizations | Time in ms |
Numeric Memory | Working Memory | Attention, working memory (maintenance) | Recall strings of numbers of increasing lengths | Attention, remembering directions/to‐do lists, efficient learning | Number recalled correctly (range 0‐12) |
Symbol‐Digit Substitution | Complex Processing Speed | Processing speed, sustained attention, visual scanning, and incidental memory | Match symbols to numbers using a key | Attention, learning and applying information to memory in daily life (unconscious memory processes), efficiency in routine tasks | Number correct |
Numeric Trail Making (TMT‐A) | Visual Attention | Visual scanning, attention, processing speed | Touch 25 circles in ascending numerical order | Efficient searching, numeric sequencing, brief attention for tasks | Time in seconds |
Alpha‐Numeric Trail Making (TMT‐B) | Visual Attention | Visual scanning and attention, processing speed, set‐shifting, multitasking | Touch 25 circles in ascending alternating alpha‐numeric order | Multitasking, prioritizing, maintenance of information in working memory, planning, organizing | Time in seconds |
Trail Making Difference | Visual Attention | Cognitive flexibility, intelligence | Difference in TMT‐A and TMT‐B scores | NA | Time in seconds |
NA indicates not applicable.
Demographics
Demographic information obtained through self‐report and objective measurement at time of enrollment include age, sex, ethnicity (White, non‐White), Townsend deprivation index (a validated measure of socioeconomic status in the United Kingdom), 14 smoking status (current, previous, never), alcohol consumption (seldom, daily, weekly), weight (obese, nonobese), systolic and diastolic blood pressure, family history of cognitive dysfunction, educational attainment, employment status, and whether the individual lives alone.
Clinical Definitions
Neurocognitive, cerebrovascular, and cardiovascular diseases were defined with the use of International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision (ICD‐9), International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision (ICD‐10), and Operating Procedure Codes‐4 codes from hospital encounter statistics data, as well as self‐reported survey responses (see Table S1 for details). Mood disorders include depression, anxiety, panic attacks, suicidality, and/or bipolar depression. Dementia and degenerative diseases include Alzheimer disease, vascular dementia, and neurodegenerative diseases other than Parkinson disease (defined separately). Intellectual delays include cognitive impairments from organic brain damage, developmental delays, and mental retardation. Seizure disorders include any history of epilepsy. Stroke was defined by both ischemic and hemorrhagic pathogenesis. Coronary artery disease was defined as history of initial or recurrent unstable angina, non–ST‐segment–elevation myocardial infarction, ST‐segment–elevation myocardial infarction, chronic ischemic heart disease, prior percutaneous coronary interventions, and/or prior coronary artery bypass graft procedures.
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using the “tableone,” “stats,” and “lsmeans” packages in R software for statistical computing version 3.4.3. In preliminary analyses of the data, many variable distributions appeared to be non‐normal 8 ; therefore, sociodemographic and comorbid illness variables were compared across groups using χ2 tests for categorical variables and the Wilcoxon rank‐sum test for continuous variables.
Neurocognitive test performance scores were compared across ACHD and non‐ACHD cohorts using multivariable linear regression adjusted for age at enrollment, sex (categorical), Townsend deprivation index, alcohol consumption (categorical), and systolic and diastolic blood pressure measured at time of enrollment. Neurocognitive function was then compared using the same models after exclusion of all individuals with history of prior ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke and/or coronary artery disease. The Benjamini‐Hochberg method was used to correct P values for multiple hypothesis testing while preserving power with an alpha threshold of 0.05 after correction. For each model, we performed standard diagnostics for assumptions of linearity, equal variance, and normality.
Results
Demographics and Clinical Characteristics
Of the 1020 participants with ACHD, the majority had mild‐to‐moderate ACHD (N = 924, 91%) while the remaining 96 individuals (9%) had a history of cyanotic ACHD. More than 50% of individuals with mild‐to‐moderate ACHD had left‐to‐right shunt type lesions (Table S2).
Demographic factors, quality of life measures, and prevalence of comorbid illnesses among ACHD and non‐ACHD participants are displayed in Table 2. Compared with the non‐ACHD group, the ACHD group was not significantly different in age, genetic sex, ethnicity, obesity, or smoking status from the non‐ACHD group. The ACHD group had slightly more socioeconomic deprivation (median Townsend Deprivation Index −1.89 versus −2.14, P<0.001), decreased alcohol consumption (37.6% versus 30.8% reported “seldom” drinking, P<0.001), and lower blood pressure (median systolic blood pressure 134 mm Hg versus 137, P<0.001; median diastolic blood pressure 79 versus 82 mm Hg, P<0.001) than the non‐ACHD group. The unemployment rate in the ACHD group was nearly twice that of the non‐ACHD group (12.2% versus 5.8%, P<0.001). There was no significant difference between the ACHD and non‐ACHD groups in the attainment of a college degree, median hours worked per week, or percentage of individuals living with a partner or spouse. The sociodemographics of individuals with mild‐to‐moderate ACHD were not significantly different from individuals with cyanotic ACHD with the exception of a higher prevalence of obesity among those with mild‐to‐moderate disease (26% versus 13%, P=0.010) (Table S3).
Table 2.
No ACHD | ACHD | P Value | |
---|---|---|---|
N | 497 983 | 1020 | |
Demographic factors | |||
Age (y) at enrollment (median, [IQR]) | 58 [50, 63] | 57 [49, 63] | 0.032 |
Male sex (%) | 226 574 (45.5) | 482 (47.3) | 0.274 |
Year of birth (median [IQR]) | 1950 [1945, 1958] | 1951 [1945, 1959] | 0.064 |
White ethnicity (%) | 468 429 (94.2) | 964 (94.5) | 0.755 |
Townsend (median [IQR]) | −2.14 [−3.65, 0.54] | −1.89 [−3.40, 1.28] | <0.001 * |
Smoking (%) | 0.840 | ||
Current | 52 473 (10.6) | 102 (9.9) | |
Previous | 171 277 (34.6) | 351 (34.7) | |
Never | 271 321 (54.8) | 562 (55.3) | |
Alcohol (%) | <0.001 * | ||
Daily | 100 874 (20.3) | 190 (18.7) | |
Weekly | 242 782 (48.9) | 444 (43.7) | |
Seldom | 152 844 (30.8) | 382 (37.6) | |
Obese (%) | 120 854 (24.4) | 254 (25.2) | 0.602 |
Systolic blood pressure (median, IQR]) | 137 [125, 150] | 134 [121, 147] | <0.001 * |
Diastolic blood pressure (median [IQR]) | 82 [76, 89] | 79 [72, 87] | <0.001 * |
Family history of cognitive disorder (%) | 109 286 (22.6) | 237 (24.0) | 0.304 |
Quality of life measures | |||
Achieved college graduation (%) | 161 115 (32.6) | 308 (30.6) | 0.173 |
Unemployed (%) | 28 857 (5.8) | 124 (12.2) | <0.001 * |
Hours worked per week (median [IQR]) | 37 [28, 42] | 37 [25, 42] | 0.155 |
Lives with a partner/spouse (%) | 359 958 (89.1) | 726 (88.4) | 0.555 |
Comorbid illness | |||
Dementia (%) | 139 (0) | 0 (0) | 0.973 |
Parkinson (%) | 821 (0.2) | 1 (0.1) | 0.604 |
Intellectual delay (%) | 24 (0) | 0 (0) | 0.985 |
Mood disorder (%) | 34 202 (6.9) | 77 (7.7) | 0.330 |
Seizure disorder (%) | 4354 (0.9) | 16 (1.6) | 0.019 |
Ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke (%) | 8304 (1.7) | 89 (8.9) | <0.001 * |
Coronary artery disease (%) | 24 047 (4.9) | 185 (18.7) | <0.001 * |
Indicates significance at P<0.001.
The overall prevalence of neurologic and neuropsychiatric disorders was low (Table 2). Less than 1% of UKB participants were found to have a history of dementia, Parkinson disease, or intellectual delay. There was no significant difference between the ACHD and non‐ACHD groups in the prevalence of mood disorders (7.7% versus 6.9%, P=0.330) or seizure disorders (1.6% versus 0.9%, P=0.019, nonsignificant at P>0.002 with Bonferroni correction). The prevalence of cerebro‐ and cardiovascular disease was vastly greater among the ACHD participants compared with the non‐ACHD group, as previously reported. The ACHD group had 5 times greater prevalence of stroke (8.9% versus 1.7%, P<0.001) and nearly 4 times greater incidence of coronary artery disease (18.7% versus 4.9%, P<0.001). There were no significant differences in the prevalence of aforementioned comorbid illnesses between individuals with mild‐to‐moderate and cyanotic ACHD (Table S3).
Neurocognitive Outcomes
The ACHD group showed worse performance than the non‐ACHD group in all 6 neurocognitive assessments examined (Table 3), with performance differences meeting significance threshold for 3 of 6 tests. After adjusting for standard risk factors, the greatest difference was observed for TMT‐B, a measure of visual attention and cognitive flexibility, in which the ACHD group spent 6.47 seconds longer on average (95% CI, 3.01–9.94 seconds, P=0.002) to complete the test than their non‐ACHD peers (Table 3). On TMT‐A, the simpler numbers‐only trail‐making task, the ACHD group spent 2.51 seconds longer on average (95% CI, 0.45–4.58 seconds, P 0.034) than the non‐ACHD group for completion (Table 3). The score difference between TMT‐A and TMT‐B, a measure of cognitive flexibility that is highly correlated with intelligence and impairment severity, 15 was also greater in the ACHD group compared with non‐ACHD (Table 3). In symbol‐digit substitution, the ACHD group achieved 19 correct substitutions on average compared with 20 correct substitutions in the non‐ACHD group (95% CI, −1.5 to −0.22 correct substitutions, P=0.021), representing a modestly lower performance another measure of cognitive flexibility (Table 3). In a sensitivity analysis, these differences did not appear to be driven by the subgroup of 96 individuals with complex CHD (Table S4). After exclusion of individuals with history of stroke and/or coronary artery disease, the ACHD group continued to show worse performance in all domains on average, though without statistically significant difference (Table 4).
Table 3.
n | Raw Mean (SD) | Least‐Squares Mean (95% CI) | Beta (95% CI)* | P Value* | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Non‐ACHD/ACHD | Non‐ACHD | ACHD | Non‐ACHD | ACHD | |||
Neurocognitive testing | |||||||
Completed at enrollment | |||||||
Reaction time, ms | 492 245/1007 |
559 (118) |
564 (123) |
558 (558, 559) |
561 (554, 568) |
3.0 (−3.9, 10.0) | 0.393 |
Fluid Intelligence, n correct | 164 041/305 |
6.0 (2.1) |
5.7 (2.1) |
6.0 (6.0, 6.0) |
5.9 (5.6, 6.1) |
‐0.2 (−0.4, 0.1) | 0.233 |
Numeric memory, n digits remembered | 51 324/93 |
6.5 (1.8) |
6.1 (1.9) |
6.5 (6.5, 6.5) |
6.2 (5.9, 6.6) |
‐0.3 (−0.6, 0.1) | 0.228 |
Completed at follow‐up | |||||||
SymbolDigit Substitution, n correct | 116 903/202 |
19.8 (5.1) |
19.1 (5.4) |
19.8 (19.7, 19.8) |
18.9 (18.3, 19.5) |
‐0.9 (−1.5, −0.22)† | 0.021† |
TMT‐A, seconds | 103 270/188 |
39.17 (14.99) |
41.23 (18.31) |
39.04 (38.95, 39.13) |
41.55 (39.49, 43.62) |
2.51 (0.45, 4.58) † |
0.034† |
TMT‐B, seconds | 103 268/188 |
66.76 (25.73) |
73.35 (30.35) |
66.66 (66.50, 66.82) |
73.14 (69.67, 76.60) |
6.47 (3.01, 9.94)† | 0.002† |
Trail Making Difference, seconds | 103 268/188 |
27.59 (20.49) |
31.12 (21.54) |
27.62 (27.49, 27.75) |
31.58 (28.72, 34.44) |
3.96 (1.10, 6.82)† | 0.021† |
ACHD indicates adult congenital heart disease; TMT‐A, Numeric Halstead‐Reitan Trail Making Test; and TMT‐B, Alphanumeric Halstead‐Reitan Trail Making Test.
Linear model included age at enrollment, sex, Townsend deprivation index, alcohol consumption, and blood pressure.
indicates significance at P<0.05. P values adjusted using Benjamini‐Hochberg correction with significance threshold of 0.05.
Table 4.
n | Raw Mean (SD) | Least‐Squares Mean (95% CI) | Beta (95% CI) * | P Value * | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Non‐ACHD/ACHD | Non‐ACHD | ACHD | Non‐ACHD | ACHD | |||
Neurocognitive testing | |||||||
Completed at enrollment | |||||||
Reaction time, ms | 456 607/720 | 557 (116) | 555 (113) | 555 (555, 556) | 555 (547, 563) | −0.3 (−8.4, 7.8) | 0.943 |
Fluid Intelligence, n correct | 152 804/219 | 6.0 (2.2) | 5.8 (2.1) | 6.1 (6.1, 6.1) | 5.9 (5.6, 6.2) | −0.2 (−0.4, 0.1) | 0.397 |
Numeric memory, n digits remembered | 47 672/68 | 6.5 (1.8) | 6.3 (1.7) | 6.5 (6.5, 6.6) | 6.4 (5.9, 6.8) | −0.2 (−0.6, 0.3) | 0.558 |
Completed at follow‐up | |||||||
Symbol Digit Substitution, n correct | 111 124/154 | 19.9 (5.1) | 19.3 (5.4) | 19.9 (19.9, 19.9) | 19.0 (18.3, 19.7) | −0.9 (−1.6, −0.1) | 0.072 |
TMT‐A, seconds | 98 281/140 | 38.99 (14.87) | 40.45 (18.87) | 38.84 (38.74, 38.93) | 40.97 (38.60, 43.34) | 2.14 (−0.23, 4.51) | 0.206 |
TMT‐B, seconds | 98 279/140 | 66.30 (25.39) | 69.69 (29.86) | 66.16 (66.00, 66.32) | 70.93 (66.98, 74.88) | 4.47 (0.82, 8.73) | 0.072 |
Trail Making Difference, seconds | 98 279/140 | 27.31 (20.21) | 29.24 (20.78) | 27.32 (27.19, 27.45) | 29.96 (26.69, 33.22) | 2.64 (−0.63, 5.90) | 0.228 |
ACHD indicates adult congenital heart disease; TMT‐A, Numeric Halstead‐Reitan Trail Making Test; and TMT‐B, Alphanumeric Halstead‐Reitan Trail Making Test.
Linear model included age at enrollment, sex, Townsend deprivation index, alcohol consumption, and blood pressure. P values adjusted using Benjamini‐Hochberg correction with significance threshold of 0.05.
DISCUSSION
Here we use a cross‐sectional approach to show that older adults with mostly mild‐to‐moderate complexity ACHD have worse performance in tests of neurocognition compared with individuals without ACHD. The most significant deficits were found in symbol‐digit substitution and numeric/alpha‐numeric trail making, indicative of deficits in processing speed, attention, and cognitive flexibility, which fall under the broad category of executive functioning. The TMT has previously been shown to correlate strongly with overall severity of neurological impairment, with predictable age‐related decline in performance. 15 Deficits in alpha‐numeric trail making have been described in school‐aged children and adolescents with CHD following cardiac surgery, 3 , 16 demonstrating that differences in cognitive flexibility among individuals with CHD can be observed much earlier than adulthood. While similar studies among ACHD are sparse, 2 recent studies of 48 adults with ACHD detected deficits in visuospatial skills, working memory, and processing speed. 1 , 17 Notably, both of these studies were of individuals with more severe ACHD and did not have sufficient power to detect differences in those with milder ACHD. Our study recapitulates neurocognitive differences in an older population of primarily low‐to‐moderate‐complexity CHD.
The substantially increased burden of cerebro‐ and cardiovascular disease in the ACHD cohort may be a significant contributor to poor neurocognitive outcomes in this cohort, as demonstrated by the attenuation in neurocognitive differences between the ACHD and non‐ACHD cohorts when excluding individuals with a history of stroke and coronary artery disease. Atherosclerosis of the carotid arteries has been shown to be a strong risk factor for vascular dementia and Alzheimer disease, likely mediated by thromboembolic events and hypoperfusion. 18 Thromboembolic cerebrovascular events are common sequelae of atrial fibrillation and coronary artery bypass grafting, indications of poor cardiovascular health, both of which have been strongly associated with cognitive decline in prospective studies. 19 , 20 Heart failure, 1 of several common long‐term complications of cardiac dysfunction in ACHD, has also been widely associated with cognitive deficits in executive function attributable to hypoperfusion of the brain. 21 The strong connection of vascular disease with neurocognitive dysfunction combined with the substantial burden of vascular disease in the ACHD population highlights the importance of early preventive care and early intervention for this population.
Important outcomes in congenital heart disease extend beyond just survival alone, with heightened focus on outcomes related to quality of life and neuropsychological functional status. Employment and social connectedness remain challenging for many individuals with ACHD. 22 Similar to the study by Ilardi et al, we found that while there was no perceivable difference in educational attainment, unemployment was more common among individuals with ACHD. 17 , 22 In adults with multiple sclerosis, an example of chronic disease, differences in cognitive flexibility measured by the symbol‐digit substitution and trail making tests display a negative impact upon employment and ability to perform activities of daily living. 23 While the differences in neurocognitive function observed in the ACHD group may be of nominal clinical significance (eg, 0.9 fewer correct answers in symbol‐digit substitution), these findings could suggest the persistence of neurocognitive differences observable in childhood survivors of CHD into adult life. Furthermore, differences in education‐matched employment rates may be explained by subtle neurocognitive differences such as the ones described in this study that cannot be distinguished by gross measures of cognitive function such as intelligence quotient scores.
These findings are not without limitations. The conclusions are derived from observational data of middle‐aged and older volunteers from the UKB, which are different than the general population of Great Britain. 24 There is likely a strong survivorship bias in this ACHD population, which predates the modern era of pediatric CHD care by several decades. 25 Furthermore, there is likely a strong healthy volunteer bias affecting the rate of completed neurocognitive tests. This is evidenced by the substantially lower than expected prevalence of dementia and other neurocognitive dysfunction and lower rate of test completion among the ACHD cohort. Another limitation lies in the fact that neurocognitive tests were asynchronous, with half the tests being done at enrollment and the other half at follow‐up separated by several years. The observed differences in symbol‐digit substitution and trail making test performance between the experimental groups at follow‐up may be accentuated by this time lapse between enrollment and testing as well as by survivorship bias. Lastly, though this study represents the largest ACHD population examined for neurocognitive outcomes to date, the modest sample sizes for select neurocognitive tests were still relatively underpowered.
CONCLUSIONS
In a study population of middle‐ and older‐aged volunteers in the UKB, we find that individuals with mild‐to‐moderate ACHD have decreased neurocognitive function, most significantly in cognitive flexibility, analogous to deficits observed in children with CHD. These differences appear to be driven by increased burden of cerebro‐ and cardiovascular disease among individuals with ACHD. Given the impact of neurocognition on quality of life measures such as employability, further longitudinal studies on neurocognitive function in the ACHD population are paramount for directing better preventive clinical care.
Sources of Funding
This work was funded by the National Institutes of Health (K99HL130523 to Dr Priest) and the Sarnoff Cardiovascular Research Foundation (Dr Saha).
Disclosures
None.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
All persons who have made substantial contributions to the research and/or manuscript and fulfilled authorship criteria are named as authors.
(J Am Heart Assoc. 2020;9:e015379 DOI: 10.1161/JAHA.120.015379.)
Supplementary Materials for this article are available at https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/suppl/10.1161/JAHA.119.015379.
For Sources of Funding and Disclosures, see page 7.
References
- 1. Klouda L, Franklin WJ, Saraf A, Parekh DR, Schwartz DD. Neurocognitive and executive functioning in adult survivors of congenital heart disease. Congenit Heart Dis. 2017;12:91–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Newburger JW, Sleeper LA, Bellinger DC, Goldberg CS, Tabbutt S, Lu M, Mussatto KA, Williams IA, Gustafson KE, Mital S, et al. Early developmental outcome in children with hypoplastic left heart syndrome and related anomalies. Circulation. 2012;125:2081–2091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Bergemann A, Hansen JH, Rotermann I, Voges I, Scheewe J, Otto‐Morris C, Geiger F, Kramer HH. Neuropsychological performance of school‐aged children after staged surgical palliation of hypoplastic left heart syndrome. Eur J Cardiothorac Surg. 2015;47:803–811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Marelli A, Miller SP, Marino BS, Jefferson AL, Newburger JW. Brain in congenital heart disease across the lifespan: the cumulative burden of injury. Circulation. 2016;133:1951–1962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Goldberg CS, Newburger JW. Chapter 74: Neurodevelopmental outcomes after heart surgery in children In: Allen HD, Shaddy RE, Penny DJ, Feltes TF, Cetta F, eds. Moss & Adams’ Heart Disease in Infants, Children, and Adolescents: Including the Fetus and Young Adult. 9th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2012:1708–1713. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Karsdorp PA, Everaerd W, Kindt M, Mulder BJM. Psychological and cognitive functioning in children and adolescents with congenital heart disease: a meta‐analysis. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32:527–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Gurvitz M, Burns KM, Brindis R, Broberg CS, Daniels CJ, Fuller SMPN, Honein MA, Khairy P, Kuehl KS, Landzberg MJ, et al. Emerging research directions in adult congenital heart disease: A report from an NHLBI/ACHA working group. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2016;67:1956–1964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Saha P, Potiny P, Rigdon J, Morello M, Tcheandjieu C, Romfh A, Fernandes SM, McElhinney DB, Bernstein D, Lui GK, et al. Substantial cardiovascular morbidity in adults with lower‐complexity congenital heart disease. Circulation. 2019;139:1889–1899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Eggermont LHP, de Boer K, Muller M, Jaschke AC, Kamp O, Scherder EJA. Cardiac disease and cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Heart. 2012;98:1334–1340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE, Broberg CS, Daniels CJ, Fuller SMPN, Honein MA, Khairy P, Kuehl KS, Landzberg MJ, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia. Stroke. 2011;42:2672–2713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lyall DM, Cullen B, Allerhand M, Smith DJ, Mackay D, Evans J, Anderson J, Fawns‐Ritchie C, McIntosh AM, Deary IJ, et al. Cognitive test scores in UK Biobank: Data reduction in 480,416 participants and longitudinal stability in 20,346 participants. PLoS One. 2016;11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Lyall LM, Cullen B, Lyall DM, Leighton SP, Siebert S, Smith DJ, Cavanagh J. The associations between self‐reported depression, self‐reported chronic inflammatory conditions and cognitive abilities in UK Biobank. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;60:63–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Hagenaars SP, Cox SR, Hill WD, Davies G, Liewald DCM, charge consortium Cognitive Working Group ; Harris SE, McIntosh AM, Gale CR, Deary IJ. Genetic contributions to trail making test performance in UK Biobank. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23:1575–1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Townsend P, Phillimore P, Beattie A. Health and Deprivation : Inequality and the North. London; New York: Croom Helm; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:2277–2281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Matos SM, Sarmento S, Moreira S, Pereira MM, Quintas J, Peixoto B, Areias JC, Areias MEG. Impact of fetal development on neurocognitive performance of adolescents with cyanotic and acyanotic congenital heart disease. Congenit Heart Dis. 2014;9:373–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ilardi D, Ono KE, McCartney R, Book W, Stringer AY. Neurocognitive functioning in adults with congenital heart disease. Congenit Heart Dis. 2017;12:166–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Van Oijen M, de Jong FJ, Witteman JCM, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MMB. Atherosclerosis and risk for dementia. Ann Neurol. 2007;61:403–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Kwok CS, Loke YK, Hale R, Potter JF, Myint PK. Atrial fibrillation and incidence of dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Neurology. 2011;76:914–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Newman MF, Kirchner JL, Phillips‐Bute B. Neurological Outcome Research Group and the Cardiothoracic Anesthesiology Research Endeavors Investigators. Longitudinal assessment of neurocognitive function after coronary‐artery bypass surgery. N Engl J Med. 2001;344:395–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Trojano L, Antonelli Incalzi R, Acanfora D, Picone C, Mecocci P, Rengo F. Congestive heart failure italian study investigators. Cognitive impairment: a key feature of congestive heart failure in the elderly. J Neurol. 2003;250:1456–1463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Zomer AC, Vaartjes I, Uiterwaal CSP, van der Velde ET, Sieswerda GJT, Wajon EMC, Plomp K, van Bergen PJM, Verheugt CL, Krivka E, et al. Social burden and lifestyle in adults with congenital heart disease. Am J Cardiol. 2012;109:1657–1663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Benedict RH, DeLuca J, Phillips G, LaRocca N, Hudson LD, Rudick R. Validity of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler Houndmills Basingstoke Engl. 2017;23:721–733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Fry A, Littlejohns TJ, Sudlow C, Doherty N, Adamska L, Sprosen T, Collins R, Allen NE. Comparison of sociodemographic and health‐related characteristics of uk biobank participants with those of the general population. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Freedom RM, Lock J, Bricker JT. Pediatric cardiology and cardiovascular surgery: 1950–2000. Circulation. 2000;102:IV58-IV68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.