Abstract
Objective:
The relationship between psychological stress and breast cancer risk is unclear. The present study sought to understand how stressfulness appraisal of salient Life Events (LEs) influences breast cancer risk.
Methods:
A case-control design was used and included 664 female cases identified through the Cancer Surveillance Program of Orange County, CA and 203 female population-based controls. A LE questionnaire determined if events occurred prior to breast cancer diagnosis and if these events were considered to be stressful or not. Multivariate unconditional logistic regression was used to calculate ORs while adjusting for known breast cancer covariates.
Results:
Cumulative adverse LEs perceived as stressful were associated with increased breast cancer risk in a dose response fashion (OR = 1.63, 95% CI = 1.00–2.66, Ptrend = 0.045). Conversely, events perceived as non-stressful did not have a significant impact on breast cancer risk. Previous personal illness was directly related to increased breast cancer risk, whether perceived as stressful (OR = 2.84, 95% CI = 1.96–4.11) or non-stressful (OR = 3.47, 95% CI = 1.34–8.94). Abortion and relocation were observed to have a protective effect on breast cancer risk only when reported as stressful (OR = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.32–0.92; OR = 0.63, 95% CI = 0.43–0.93, respectively). Pre/Peri-menopausal women who were nulliparous or who had their first child at ≥30 years of age were especially prone to the effects of appraised stress on increased breast cancer risk.
Conclusions:
This study underscores the importance of stressfulness appraisal when determining the effect of major LEs on breast cancer risk. Our results support incorporating assessments of perceived stressfulness in future epidemiological investigation of this topic.
Keywords: Breast cancer, Life events, Stress appraisal
1. Introduction
Breast cancer is the most common female malignancy in the United States and worldwide [1,2]. Breast cancer is the leading cause of cancer mortality among women in developing countries and the second leading cause of cancer mortality among women in the United States [3]. Known breast cancer risk factors include increased age, family history of breast cancer, post-menopausal obesity, hormone replacement therapy and reproductive characteristics mostly pertaining to increased estrogen exposure such as early age at menarche, nulliparity, and late menopause [4,5]. Genetic determinants of breast cancer have been increasingly the focus of breast cancer research. However, an estimated 85% of breast cancers are sporadic in nature, with no known familial or genetic contributions [6]. Further, an estimated 60–70% of breast cancer patients do not have known breast cancer risk factors [7]. Therefore, there is paramount importance to studying additional breast cancer risk factors.
1.1. Stress as a potential breast cancer risk factor
Psychosocial stress as a risk factor for cancer and breast cancer specifically has been subject of research investigation for centuries [8]. Nevertheless, the association between psychological stress and breast cancer risk in human populations remains unclear. Many breast cancer survivors attribute past experiences of stress as major contributors to their cancer development [9]. However, despite many studies examining the role of stress in breast carcinogenesis, firm conclusions have yet to be reached.
Studies examining whether LEs influence breast cancer risk have reached conflicting conclusions. Some epidemiological studies found that LEs are associated with increased breast cancer risk [10–13], while other studies did not supported this conclusion [14–16]. One of the main challenges in stress research is measuring the level of stress. When analyzing the effect of stress due to LEs on breast cancer risk, researchers have attempted to standardize measurement of stress exposure. The main measures of stressful LEs utilized in epidemiological studies thus far have been the Holmes and Rahe Social Readjustment Rating Scale [11–14] and the Brown and Harris LEs and Difficulties Schedule (LEDS) [15,17]. However, in both of these measures individual stress appraisal is not directly addressed [18,19].
2. Stress carcinogenesis
The carcinogenic properties of the stress hormones cortisol, epinephrine and norepinephrine have been demonstrated in cell culture and animal models [24–27]. Cortisol shifts the immune response from Th1 to Th2 dominated. Decreased IFN-gamma production and a decline in natural killer cells have also been observed in response to increased HPA axis signaling. These alterations impair the ability of the immune system to identify and neutralize cancer cells [28,29]. With prolonged stress, tissues become insensitive to cortisol regulation contributing to an inflammatory state [30]. This state is characterized by increased cell proliferation and elevated reactive nitrogen and oxygen species that contribute to DNA damage, dysplasia and resultant neoplasia [31]. Consistently, the pro-inflammatory cytokine Interleukin-6 (IL-6) is elevated in various cancers [32].
2.1. Stress appraisal
Psychological stress arises when a person appraises the environmental demands to overwhelm his/her ability to cope with the situation [33]. The stress researcher Selye, M.D. was the first to recognize that the individual is the one to determine based on their perception of the situation whether it is eustressful or distressful. When a stressor is appraised to be positive, beneficial or desired, it leads to eustress. In contrast, when a stressor is appraised to be negative, unwanted or exceeding coping abilities, it will cause a state of distress [34,35]. Personal perception of stress has been shown to be important in determining how stressors impact physiology [36].
2.2. Stress appraisal and breast cancer risk
The subjective reporting of daily stress and breast cancer incidence has been investigated previously [20]. In a large prospective cohort study investigating the influence of perceived daily stress on subsequent breast cancer risk during 24 years of follow-up, a 2-fold increase in breast cancer risk was observed for women who reported high levels of stress compared to women who reported no or minimal mental stress [20]. Conversely, another prospective study focusing on perceived daily stress found a protective relationship between higher stress levels and breast cancer risk [21]. However, these studies quantified stress based on reports of nervousness and anxiety in everyday life and did not address stressfulness of more salient LEs such as death of a close relative or serious illness. Within human studies focusing on stress and breast cancer etiology, there is more substantial evidence toward the role of stress in the form of salient LEs as opposed to daily stress [22]. Our recent work supports the role of negative valence LEs in promoting breast cancer development [23].
To our knowledge, the relationship between perceived stressfulness of salient LEs and breast cancer risk has not been directly addressed. Higher perceived stress has been linked to increased cortisol levels [37] and therefore can provide easily ascertained information regarding HPA perturbations resulting from LEs. Recent work has revealed that higher perceived stress is linked to higher resting amygdalar activity [38], a key component of the physiological stress system. The synergistic influence of a stressor and the perception that ‘stress influences health outcomes’ on increasing mortality, further supports the importance of stress perception [39].
2.3. Modulators of stress appraisal
Antoni et al. [27], developed a bio-behavioral model for the relationship between psycho-social factors and cancer incidence and progression. Based on this model, the cancer “macroenvironment” is comprised as a result of the interaction between environmental stress and an individual’s attitudes toward the stressor, perception of treat, and coping abilities [27]. When the state of appraised distress persists chronically, it is more likely to activate neuroendocrine stress systems leading to secretion of stress hormones and neuropeptides that contribute to cancer growth [40]. Recently, blocking the effects of physiological stress in mice models by antagonizing the ß-adrenergic receptor was shown to protect against breast tumor dissemination through lymphatic spread [41]. The individual’s internal working model and attachment style are important factors in modulating perceived stressfulness, which contributes to subsequent activation of the physiological stress response facilitating the tumor microenvironment [42]. A recent study found that an “optimal” relationship with at least one parent leading to a secure attachment style reduced the likelihood of LN involvement of breast malignancies by 62% [43].
2.4. Importance of life event stress appraisal in breast cancer risk
To better study the effects of stress in the form of LEs, we sought to understand if a woman’s personal stress appraisal influences the association between LEs and breast cancer risk. As discussed, perceived stress is associated with elevated cortisol levels [37], and higher resting amygdalar activity [38]. Considering our understating of the plausible role of cortisol in cancer initiation and progression [24–27], we hypothesize that the perception of stress resulting from major life events will increase cortisol signaling and hence impair immune surveillance [28,29] and contribute to breast cancer risk. The aim of the present analysis is to investigate whether LEs increase breast cancer risk depending on the individually reported experience of stress. Understanding how to better epidemiologically measure and quantify stress is expected to allow a more accurate assessment of whether stress influences breast cancer development.
3. Methods
3.1. Study population
Population-based primary invasive breast cancer cases and population-based controls were identified as part of the Hereditary Breast and Ovarian Cancer (HBOC) study of the University of California, Irvine (CA58860) [44,45]. Cases were identified via the population-based cancer registry of the Cancer Surveillance Program of Orange County (CSPOC). The Institutional Review Board of the University of California, Irvine approved the study protocol and written informed consent was obtained from participants of the study (IRB numbers HS91–137 and HS96–496) [45].
Data from incident breast cancer diagnoses among females aged 24–75 years, between March 1st, 1994 and February 28th, 1995 was used. Female controls of similar age and race/ethnicity that had no history of cancer were randomly selected from residents of Orange County using random digit dialing between December 1997 and February 1999. Cases and controls were asked to answer an epidemiological risk factor questionnaire (RFQ) that included extensive information regarding personal, social, medical and family history in addition to a section querying about LEs.
Seven hundred and nineteen breast cancer cases between 24 and 75 years of age completed a risk factors questionnaire (RFQ). Six hundred and sixty four of these women completed the life event (LE) section of the RFQ and were included in the present analyses. The majority of cases (94%) completed the questionnaire within 3 years of breast cancer diagnosis. Two hundred twenty six population-based controls in the appropriate age interval completed the RFQ, out of which 203 completed the LE section and were included in the analyses.
3.1.1. Measures
LE measures in the RFQ were based on a subset of the Holmes and Rahe scale [18]. LEs from the Holmes and Rahe scale that were inquired about here were: 1) marriage, 2) death of a spouse, 3) death of an offspring, 4) death of a close person: sister, brother, relative or friend, 5) death of a parent, 6) job loss, 7) relocation, 8) separation/divorce, 9) foreclosure of a mortgage loan or bankruptcy, 10) pregnancy, 11) pregnancy of a child, 12) illness and 13) illness in the family. We also asked participants about a history of abortion, marriage of an offspring, and buying a house. Participants were asked whether specific LEs occurred prior to their breast cancer diagnosis or corresponding reference age for controls, defined as age at questionnaire completion. We further assessed if events that occurred were individually perceived as “stressful” or “non-stressful”. The questionnaire prompted participants to identify their age at which each event occurred using the following age intervals: 10–19, 20–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59 and ≥60.
In order to analyze if perceived event stressfulness was impacted by event valence, we used the Paykel et al. [46]. LE Scale to dichotomize events based on whether they were more likely to be desirable or undesirable, i.e. of positive or negative valence. Events that were at the top half of the scale were determined to be of negative valence, while events at the bottom half of the scale determined to be of positive valence [46] (Fig. 1). LEs included in our study but not in the Paykel scale were not assigned valence.
Fig. 1.
Categorization of Life Events according to valence and perceived stress.
LE: Life Event, SLE: Stressful Life Event, NSLE: Non-Stressful Life Event.
(+): Positive valence, (−): Negative valence.
3.1.1.1. Negative valence LEs.
A subset of LEs that were identified as being more likely to be undesirable and unpredictable and therefore have negative valence were: 1) death of a spouse, 2) death of an offspring, 3) death of a sister, brother, close relative or friend, 4) death of a parent, 5) job loss, 6) foreclosure of a mortgage loan or bankruptcy, 7) illness and 8) illness in the family.
3.1.1.2. Positive valence LEs.
A subset of LEs that were identified as being more likely to be desirable and predictable and therefore have positive valence were identified: 1) marriage, 2) marriage of an offspring, 3) relocation, 4) buying a house, 5) pregnancy, 6) pregnancy of a child.
3.1.1.3. Stressful/non-stressful life events.
Participants were asked if the above life events, whether positive or negative, brought about a “moderate to severe” amount of stress. If “yes”, the life event was considered a ‘stressful life event’ (SLE), if “no”, the life event was considered a ‘non-stressful life event’ (NSLE) (Fig. 1). In a similar fashion described for LEs, age interval at SLE/NSLE was determined. Three different SLE/NSLE parameters were used to evaluate the effect of SLEs/NSLEs on breast cancer risk: (1) SLE/NSLE occurrence: (yes/no), (2) negative valence SLE/NSLE sum: 0, 1, 2, 3 and ≥4 events, and (3) positive valence SLE/NSLE sum: 0, 1, 2–3, 4–5 and ≥6 events.
For the SLE/NSLE occurrence variable, the LE category received a value of “0” if the event did not occur and “1” if any number of events happened in the same category (if someone was pregnant twice, the “pregnancy” occurrence variable obtained a value of “1”). The grouping of the sum of positive/negative valence SLEs/NSLEs was based on creating a zero event baseline and dividing the remaining distribution of events in the control group into quartiles. Here each event in a LE category contributed an unweightd value of “1” to the sum (if a women was pregnancy twice, “2” LEs would be added to the SLE/NSLE sum. For the SLE sum, LEs that were appraised as non-stressful (NSLEs) were included in the zero event baseline and for the NSLE sum, LEs that were appraised as stressful (SLEs) were included in the zero event baseline.
3.1.2. Data analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for cases and controls. Means and standard deviations were computed for continuous variables, whereas frequencies and percentages were computed for categorical variables. Additional descriptive statistics were generated in a stratified analysis according to occurrence of events appraised as stressful or non-stressful.
Candidate variables for model building were selected based on a literature review of breast cancer covariates. A standard stepwise model building process with statistical threshold for inclusion into the model of Pentry = 0.25 and Pstay = 0.3 was performed in selection of a baseline model for the multivariate analyses. The final multivariate logistic regression model adjusted for age (age at diagnosis for cases and age at RFQ completion for controls), smoking history (ever smoked/never smoked), race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic white/all other groups), education (less than college/some college or more), family history (yes/no) (determined based on family history of breast cancer in first degree relative), hormone replacement therapy (HRT) (ever use/never use), age at menarche (≤11, 12–13 and ≥14), age at first full term pregnancy (FFTP): (< 25, 25–29 and ≥30), menopausal status (pre-menopausal/peri-menopausal/post-menopausal) and physical activity (not active/moderately active/very active). To examine potential confounding, we examined the estimates for the covariates in the model when including negative life events and when not including negative life events. The estimates of the ORs associated with covariate coefficients did not differ by > 5% and therefore we conclude that there is no significant association between stress in the form of life events and the breast cancer covariates examined in the model.
SLE and NSLE parameters were evaluated in separate univariate and multivariate analyses. Univariate associations between breast cancer and SLE/NSLE occurrence parameters, negative valence SLE/NSLE sum, positive valence SLE/NSLE sum and total SLE/NSLE sum were determined. Frequency tables and χ2 statistics were used to compare SLE/NSLE variables among cases and controls. Likelihood of an event being “stressful” was determined based on the fraction of (SLEs)/(total LEs) reported for cases and controls.
The influence of SLE/NSLE parameters on breast cancer odds ratios (ORs) was determined using simple unconditional logistic regression where ORs and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were computed. Significant SLE/NSLE parameters were then included one at a time into the baseline multivariate regression model and adjusted ORs and CIs were determined using multivariate unconditional logistic regression. The negative valence SLE/NSLE sums were also included in the logistic regression model as continuous variables and Wald χ2 statistics were used to test for trend.
All analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).
4. Results
4.1. Demographics and characteristics among breast cancer cases and controls
Descriptive characteristics of cases and controls are presented in Table 1. Cases were slightly older than controls with a mean age of 55.6 years for cases and 53.6 years for controls. Cases were more likely to have a positive family history of breast cancer (24.7%) compared to controls (15.3%). Both cases (88.9%) and controls (87.7%) were primarily of white ethnicity. Mean age at first full term pregnancy was significantly younger for controls (23.7) compared to cases (25.2). The majority of controls had children at the age of ≤24 years (60.8%) in comparison to 49.8% in the case group. No significant differences between cases and controls were observed for the following parameters: BMI, age at menarche, parity, number of children, menopausal status, hormone replacement therapy, education level, smoking history and alcohol history.
Table 1.
Distribution of demographics and other characteristics among 664 cases and 203 controls.
Characteristic | Cases (n = 664) | Controls (n = 203) | P value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reference age, years: mean, s.d. | 55.6 | 10.9 | 53.6 | 12.3 | 0.039 |
BMI: no. (%) | |||||
Underweight | 15 | 2.26 | 4 | 2 | 0.348 |
Normal weight | 95 | 47.5 | 335 | 50.5 | |
Overweight | 54 | 27 | 196 | 29.5 | |
Obese | 47 | 23.5 | 118 | 17.8 | |
Race/ethnicity: no. (%) | |||||
White | 590 | 88.9 | 178 | 87.7 | 0.124 |
Hispanic | 38 | 5.7 | 19 | 9.4 | |
Black | 2 | 0.3 | 1 | 0.5 | |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 34 | 5.1 | 5 | 2.5 | |
Age at menarche: no. (%) | |||||
≤10 years | 142 | 22.15 | 44 | 22.2 | 0.966 |
11–13years | 349 | 54.45 | 106 | 53.5 | |
≥14 years | 150 | 23.4 | 48 | 24.2 | |
Age at first full-term pregnancy: no. (%) | |||||
≤24 years | 277 | 49.8 | 104 | 60.8 | 0.042 |
25–29 years | 184 | 33.1 | 44 | 25.7 | |
≥30 years | 95 | 17.1 | 23 | 13.5 | |
Parity: no. (%) | |||||
Nulliparous | 107 | 16.1 | 32 | 15.8 | 0.865 |
Parous | 556 | 83.7 | 171 | 84.2 | |
Number of children: mean., s.d | 2.1 | 1.5 | 2.2 | 1.4 | 0.386 |
Menopausal status: no. (%) | |||||
Pre/peri-menopausal | 239 | 36.0 | 74 | 36.5 | 0.832 |
Post-menopausal | 425 | 64.0 | 127 | 62.6 | |
Hormone replacement therapy: no. (%) | |||||
Never | 311 | 46.8 | 80 | 39.4 | 0.063 |
Ever | 347 | 52.3 | 121 | 59.6 | |
Family history of breast cancer in first-degree relative: no. (%) | |||||
No | 499 | 75.2 | 172 | 84.7 | 0.005 |
Yes | 164 | 24.7 | 31 | 15.3 | |
Education: no. (%) | |||||
< College | 419 | 63.1 | 121 | 59.6 | 0.357 |
Some college or more | 241 | 36.3 | 81 | 39.9 | |
Smoking: no. (%) | |||||
Never | 332 | 50.0 | 112 | 55.2 | 0.240 |
Ever | 326 | 49.1 | 91 | 44.8 | |
Alcohol use in year prior: no. (%) | |||||
None | 243 | 36.6 | 69 | 34.0 | 0.533 |
Any | 399 | 60.1 | 126 | 62.1 | |
Physical activity in previous year: no. (%) | |||||
Not active | 262 | 39.5 | 103 | 50.7 | 0.011 |
Moderately active | 209 | 31.5 | 60 | 29.6 | |
Very active | 193 | 29.1 | 40 | 19.7 |
Due to unknown values, subcategories may not sum to total numbers of cases and controls.
4.2. Descriptives of cases and controls who perceived events as “stressful”
Pre-menopausal or peri-menopausal women who appraised at least one event as stressful were more likely to be cases (P = 0.002). Similarly, nulliparous women (P = 0.043) and women who had their first child at age ≥ 30 and appraised at least one event as stressful were more likely to have had breast cancer (P = 0.024). There were no significant differences observed among cases and controls on the following parameters: age, BMI, race/ethnicity, age at menarche, number of children, HRT, family history of breast cancer, education level, smoking history and alcohol history (Table 2).
Table 2.
Demographics and characteristics of women who appraised events as stressful.a
Characteristic | Casesb | Controls | cP value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reference age, years: mean, s.d. | 54.5 | 11.9 | 55.6 | 10.6 | 0.296 |
BMI: no. (%) | |||||
Underweight | 10 | 71.4 | 3 | 75.0 | 0.467 |
Normal weight | 262 | 80.6 | 69 | 72.6 | 0.094 |
Overweight | 163 | 84.9 | 42 | 77.8 | 0.215 |
Obese | 95 | 83.3 | 42 | 89.4 | 0.466 |
Race/ethnicity: no. (%) | |||||
Non-Hispanic White | 479 | 83.5 | 139 | 78.1 | 0.103 |
Other | 51 | 76.0 | 19 | 71.8 | 0.687 |
Age at menarche: no. (%) | |||||
≤10 years | 28 | 77.8 | 9 | 75.0 | 1 |
11–13 years | 367 | 83.4 | 107 | 77.5 | 0.117 |
≥14 years | 121 | 81.8 | 38 | 79.2 | 0.690 |
Age at first full-term pregnancy: no. (%) | |||||
≤24 years | 226 | 84.0 | 85 | 81.7 | 0.595 |
25–29 years | 137 | 76.1 | 36 | 81.8 | 0.418 |
≥ 30 years | 78 | 85.7 | 15 | 65.2 | 0.024 |
Parity: no. (%) | |||||
Nulliparous | 89 | 84.8 | 22 | 68.8 | 0.043 |
Parous | 441 | 81.7 | 139 | 79.5 | 0.501 |
Number of children: mean., s.d | 2.2 | 1.4 | 2.1 | 1.5 | 0.386 |
Menopausal status: no. (%) | |||||
Pre/peri-menopausal | 194 | 84.4 | 50 | 67.6 | 0.002 |
Post-menopausal | 336 | 81.0 | 106 | 83.5 | 0.525 |
Hormone replacement therapy: no. (%) | |||||
Never | 244 | 80.8 | 61 | 76.3 | 0.368 |
Ever | 282 | 83.4 | 97 | 80.2 | 0.416 |
Family history of breast cancer in first degree relative: no. (%) | |||||
No | 397 | 82.2 | 133 | 77.3 | 0.163 |
Yes | 133 | 82.6 | 25 | 80.7 | 0.793 |
Education: no. (%) | |||||
< College | 331 | 80.7 | 95 | 78.5 | 0.590 |
Some college or more | 197 | 84.9 | 62 | 76.5 | 0.086 |
Smoking: no. (%) | |||||
Never | 261 | 81.1 | 85 | 75.9 | 0.242 |
Ever | 264 | 83.3 | 73 | 80.2 | 0.497 |
Alcohol use in year prior: no. (%) | |||||
None | 192 | 81.4 | 56 | 81.2 | 0.971 |
Any | 324 | 83.7 | 97 | 77.0 | 0.087 |
Physical activity in previous year: no. (%) | |||||
Not active | 207 | 81.5 | 78 | 75.7 | 0.219 |
Moderately active | 172 | 84.7 | 46 | 76.7 | 0.145 |
Very active | 151 | 80.3 | 34 | 85.0 | 0.492 |
(−) Test statistic could not be computed because of insufficient data.
Appraised stressful events: had at least one event that was appraised as stressful.
Table total values could be discrepant from the total cases and controls since some participants reported only NSLEs.
P values were calculated based on χ2 statistics unless cell counts were below 5 where fisher’s exact test was used.
4.2.1. Likelihood of events being “stressful”
The pattern of stressfulness of individual LE occurrence variables did not differ among cases and controls (Fig. 2). As expected, negative valence LEs were more likely to be perceived as stressful compared to positive valence LEs. On average, 85% of the time negative LEs were categorized as “stressful” among cases, as compared to 89% of the time among controls. On the other hand, positive LEs were categorized as “stressful” only 61% of the time among cases and 64% of the time among controls. Job loss and foreclosure were the negative valence LEs most likely to be reported as “stressful” among cases (89%) and controls (100%).
Fig. 2.
Likelihood of LEs being Perceived as “Stressful” among Breast Cancer Cases and Controls LEs (LEs) were categorized into “positive” or “negative” valence based on the Paykel (1971) LE scale (see text for details). LEs are organized based on events least to most likely to be stressful among cases in both valence categories. The likelihood an event category to be classified as “stressful” was determined by the fraction: (# stressful LEs)/(total # of LEs).
4.2.2. Stressful/non-stressful LEs and breast cancer risk
In the univariate and multivariate analyses, relocation (OR = 0.65, 95% CI = 0.44–0.95) and abortion (OR = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.32–0.91) were associated with decreased breast cancer risk only when perceived as stressful. History of personal illness was associated with increased breast cancer risk both when appraised as stressful (OR = 2.80, 95% CI = 1.94–4.04) and non-stressful (OR = 3.40, 95% CI = 1.32–8.75) (Table 3).
Table 3.
Univariate and multivariate odds ratios for significant stressful and non-stressful Life Event occurrence parameters.
Cases (N = 664) | Controls (N = 203) | OR | 95% CI | Adj ORa | 95% CI | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % | N | % | |||||||
LEs perceived as stressful | ||||||||||
Abortion | 60 | 9.0 | 28 | 13.8 | 0.62 | 0.38 | 1.00 | 0.54 | 0.32 | 0.91 |
Illness | 331 | 49.9 | 54 | 26.6 | 2.74 | 1.94 | 3.88 | 2.80 | 1.94 | 4.04 |
Relocation | 141 | 21.2 | 57 | 28.1 | 0.69 | 0.48 | 0.99 | 0.65 | 0.44 | 0.95 |
LEs perceived as non-stressful | ||||||||||
Death of sibling | 59 | 8.89 | 9 | 4.43 | 2.10 | 1.02 | 4.32 | 1.75 | 0.83 | 3.68 |
Illness | 62 | 9.34 | 5 | 2.46 | 4.08 | 1.62 | 10.29 | 3.40 | 1.32 | 8.75 |
Illness in family | 66 | 9.94 | 11 | 5.42 | 1.93 | 1.00 | 3.72 | 1.53 | 0.77 | 3.03 |
N: number of exposed; %: percent exposed.
Adjusted for reference age, age at first full term pregnancy, menopausal status, family history of breast cancer, HRT use, smoking history, education level, race/ethnicity and physical activity.
Negative valence LEs were associated with a significant dose-response increase in breast cancer risk only when perceived as stressful (Ptrend = 0.049). The highest category of negative valence SLEs was associated with a 62% increase in breast cancer risk after adjusting for breast cancer covariates (OR = 1.62, 95% CI = 0.99–2.63) (Table 4). Conversely, positive valence LEs were cumulatively associated with a non-significant trend toward decreased breast cancer risk only when events were perceived as stressful (data not shown).
Table 4.
Univariate and multivariate odds ratios for sum negative Life Events.
Cases (N = 664) | Controls (N = 203) | OR | 95% CI | Adj. ORa | 95% CI | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % | N | % | |||||||
Stressful negative valence LEs | ||||||||||
0 events | 123 | 18.5 | 45 | 22.2 | ||||||
1 event | 111 | 16.7 | 29 | 14.3 | 1.40 | 0.82 | 2.39 | 1.43 | 0.81 | 2.53 |
2 events | 84 | 12.7 | 46 | 22.7 | 0.67 | 0.41 | 1.10 | 0.81 | 0.47 | 1.38 |
3 events | 109 | 16.4 | 29 | 14.3 | 1.38 | 0.81 | 2.34 | 1.57 | 0.89 | 2.77 |
≥4 events | 237 | 35.7 | 54 | 26.6 | 1.61 | 1.02 | 2.52 | 1.62 | 0.99 | 2.63 |
P trendb | 0.042 | 0.049 | ||||||||
Non-stressful negative valence LEs | ||||||||||
0 events | 526 | 79.2 | 161 | 79.3 | ||||||
1 event | 46 | 6.9 | 18 | 8.9 | 0.78 | 0.44 | 1.39 | 0.77 | 0.42 | 1.42 |
2 events | 30 | 4.5 | 13 | 6.4 | 0.71 | 0.36 | 1.39 | 0.58 | 0.28 | 1.19 |
3 events | 21 | 3.2 | 6 | 3.0 | 1.07 | 0.43 | 2.70 | 0.80 | 0.31 | 2.10 |
≥4 events | 41 | 6.2 | 5 | 2.5 | 2.51 | 0.98 | 6.46 | 2.04 | 0.77 | 5.41 |
P trendb | 0.227 | 0.760 |
N: number of exposed; %: percent exposed.
Adjusted for reference age, age at first full term pregnancy, menopausal status, family history of breast cancer, HRT use, smoking history, education level, race/ethnicity and physical activity.
P trend computed by incorporating negative valence LE (NSLE) sum in the logistic regression model as a continuous variable.
5. Discussion
The results presented here suggest that LEs differentially influence breast cancer risk according to stress perception. This result is consistent with previous research indicating the importance of stress appraisal in altering the physiological stress system [37]. Prior research supports this finding given that LEs were overall perceived as more impactful among breast cancer patients as compared to controls [47].
Our results are in line with those reported by Helgesson et al. [20] where perceived stress was associated with an approximately 2-fold increase in breast cancer risk. However, in this study stress was determined based on experiencing daily stress “occasionally or more” during the past 5-years and did not address LE stress [20]. Likewise, a recent study that focused on perceived stress in daily life and increased breast cancer risk in a cohort of 29,098 Japanese women showed that women who reported “high perceived stress”, had a 1.71 (95% CI = 1.02–2.85) increased risk of developing breast cancer prospectively [48]. It is possible that women who reported higher perceived stress also had more negative valence LEs that they perceived as stressful since these measures are significantly correlated [49].
Our findings support the need to examine salient LEs in combination with other factors influencing perception of stress such as coping style and social support [50]. We found that repeated exposure to negative valence LEs perceived as “stressful” increased the risk of breast cancer. Therefore, it is likely that negative LEs cumulatively contribute to appraised overload of environmental demands compared to available coping mechanisms leading to a state of distress [34,35,40]. Consistently, a significant interaction was observed between LEs perceived as highly stressful and social support in predicting breast cancer risk, with the highest risk observed for those with highly stressful events and low social support [50].
As expected, we found that negative valence LEs were those to have the greatest likelihood of being perceived as “stressful” (Fig. 2). Therefore, negative valence, cumulative LEs perceived as stressful seem to be most influential in increasing breast cancer risk. Interestingly, “relocation” was the positive valence event to be reported as “stressful” most often and was the only positive LE to be significantly associated with decreased breast cancer risk. Therefore, it seems that to experience the benefits of positive stress, an event needs to be salient enough to induce significant eustress. Consistent with this hypothesis, the cumulative benefit of positive valence LEs on decreasing breast cancer risk was only observed when events were perceived as stressful.
History of personal illness, defined as ‘serious illness or injury of oneself’ was significantly associated with increased breast cancer risk both when perceived as stressful and when perceived as non-stressful. In line with our findings, a higher incidence of major personal illness has been reported in the past among breast cancer patients than among controls [12,47]. In a separate validation analysis, fibrocystic breast disease and gallstones were more commonly reported among breast cancer cases than among controls (P < 0.05). This may indicate the possibility of physical illness contributing to immune system breakdown and facilitation of a pro-cancer microenvironment, regardless of individual psychological stress perception. Consistent with this finding, previous research has shown that physical and psychological stressors activate the HPA stress axis in a similar fashion and independently contribute to immune system breakdown [51]. Pre-existing immune system function and HPA reactivity from transgenerational [52] or early-childhood [43] experiences may predis-pose women to both physical illness and breast cancer.
Younger women seem to be more impacted by the effects of perceived stress. Our results suggest that stressful LEs have a stronger influence on pre/peri-menopausal breast cancer risk than on post-menopausal breast cancer risk. In the only study identified focusing on breast cancer in young women, LEs were cumulatively associated with a 62% increase in breast cancer risk [53]. It is possible that LE stress has a stronger impact on basal estrogen levels during reproductive years.
Estrogen causes proliferation of breast tissue and may promote the progression from normal cellular proliferation to hyperplasia and neoplasia [54]. Further, estrogen metabolites are directly genotoxic [55,56]. Premenopausal women have higher average levels of estradiol [57,58]. In a mouse model of LE stress, tail shock and forced swim tests resulted in increased estrogen levels and persistently thereafter [59]. Consequently, decreased estrogen metabolism resulting from LE stress [60] likely more substantially contributes to increased premenopausal estrogen exposure.
Further, LE stress seems to influence breast cancer risk specifically among nulliparous women and women who delayed childbirth. Many studies have shown that the younger a woman is at her first full term pregnancy, the lower her risk of breast cancer [61]. It is hypothesized that the mammary gland undergoes complete differentiation during pregnancy, leading breast tissue to increased resistance against carcinogenic initiation [62]. Therefore, young women who are nulliparous or had an older age at first full-term pregnancy, not only have the highest endogenous estrogen levels, but their breast tissue is also the most susceptible to mutagenic changes. Hence this population is hypothesized to be the most vulnerable to the effects of LE stress on breast cancer risk. Future studies examining the interaction between parity/age at first full term pregnancy and negative valence LEs would deepen our understanding of this observation and the interplay between reproductive and psychological factors in breast tumorigenesis.
Premenopausal breast cancer tends to be more aggressive with estrogen-independent tumors being more common [63]. Further, approximately 30% of female breast malignancies are diagnosed among women younger than 50 years of age [64]. We are limited in our understanding of etiologic and mechanistic contributors to this disease. A study from 2016 achieved significance for the population attributable fraction when considering known risk factors for post-menopausal breast cancer but not premenopausal breast cancer [65]. Therefore, this research has important implications toward understanding contributing factors to development of breast malignancy in young women.
Breast cancer cases and controls did not exhibit a different pattern of LE stress perception. Stressfulness perception followed a predictable trend among cases and controls. The likelihood of an event being reported as “stressful” followed our valence categorization based on the Paykel scale (1971) [46]. Negative valence LEs were more often perceived as stressful as compared to positive valence LEs. These findings implicate that cases and controls do not differ in what events they perceive as stressful. However, given the cumulative effect of stressful events on breast cancer risk, it seems that cases are experiencing more stress through negative valence LEs and possibly are not as well equipped to cope with stress compared to the control group.
6. Future directions
Additional research focusing on the effect of perceived LE stress on breast cancer risk among premenopausal women is needed. Incorporating assessments of cortisol, catecholamines and estradiol would allow researchers to better understand the mechanism behind the proposed alterations in neuroendocrinological processes accelerating breast carcinogenesis. Further investigating the contributing factors that distinguish cases and controls in their response to stressful situations should be considered. Controls may be more likely to find meaning in their stressful situations and as a result gain resiliency, while cases are perhaps more debilitated in the face of stress.
Epidemiological studies examining the relationship between LEs and breast cancer risk thus far do not directly incorporate individualized stress appraisal measures. Questionnaires and structured interviews are currently widely used in an attempt to standardize stress exposure [11–13,15,17]. However, this approach ignores the importance of an individual’s interaction with their environment along with their coping mechanisms and resulting appraised stress [66,67]. Higher scores on the most commonly used measure of stress appraisal, the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), have been associated with prior occurrence of LEs and the presence of somatic and depressive symptomology [49]. However, this measure addresses perceived stress in the preceding 30 days. Our results support the utilization of an adapted PSS in future research focusing on LEs and breast cancer risk. Consistent with this recommendation, studies incorporating self-ratings of event stressfulness were superior predictors of health outcomes compared to studies that did not incorporate this information [68,69].
7. Limitations and strengths
When interpreting our findings, it is important to consider study limitations. Firstly, this is a case-control study, where information was ascertained retrospectively and hence the possibility of recall bias. However, since LEs are reported with high reproducibility [70], it is unlikely that cases were over-reporting the occurrence of LEs. Reflective of the population of Orange County, the majority of participants were of White ethnicity. Therefore, generalizability to other racial/ethnic groups is questionable. Additional studies examining a more diverse population are warranted. Because we asked about an additional 3 events not included in the validated Holmes and Rahe scale [71], we did not sum events according to assigned weights. A further limitation to our study pertains to the use of a dichotomized assessment of stressfulness and assignment of positive/negative valence to LE categories.
To gain a better understanding of the effect of perceived stressfulness on breast cancer risk, it would be more accurate to quantify stress appraisal on a gradient from non-stressful to extremely stressful. Further, it would be advised to include a positive-negative valence gradient for each event to better quantify the interplay of valence and stressfulness in breast cancer risk. Another limitation pertains to valence categorization of some events that did not perfectly match with the Paykel (1971) LE Scale categories [46]. For example, we could not differentiate the valence of our grouped category “separation/divorce” since based on our dichotomization “divorce” would be assigned negative valence and ‘separation not due to argument’ positive valence. Similarly, abortion could be desirable (terminating an undesirable pregnancy) or undesirable (spontaneous abortion) and therefore was not assigned valence.
This study provides novel insight into the relationship between LE stress and breast cancer risk. To our knowledge, this is the first study of its kind to investigate the influence of LE appraised stress on breast cancer risk. This assessment was facilitated by the information we had available to us on the personal categorization of events as “stressful”/’not-stressful’ by participants in the study. Further, our results shed light onto the effect of stress in the form of LEs on breast cancer risk in young women specifically. The use of population-based controls strengthens the external validity of our findings. Our rich dataset allowed for the adjustment of breast cancer covariates in our multivariate models, enabling a more accurate understanding of the influence of LEs on breast cancer risk while limiting the possibility of confounding.
8. Conclusion
This study demonstrates that stress appraisal should be addressed in future epidemiologic investigation of LEs and breast cancer risk. Negative valence LEs seem to increase breast cancer risk in a dose-response fashion only when perceived as stressful. Younger women, women who are nulliparous and women who delayed age at childbirth seem to be particularly sensitive to the effects of LE stress. Additional research should be performed specifically examining the effect of LE stress appraisal on premenopausal breast cancer risk according to breast cancer molecular subtypes.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute, CA58860, NIH MSTP Training Grant, 3T32GM008620.
Footnotes
Conflicts of interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- [1].American Cancer Society, Cancer facts & figures 2016, Cancer Facts Fig. 2016. (2016) 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- [2].Breast cancer statistics | World Cancer Research Fund International. (http://www.wcrf.org/int/cancer-facts-figures/data-specific-cancers/breast-cancer-statistics). (Accessed December 14, 2016).
- [3].Breast Cancer Statistics | Breast Cancer Research Foundation. (https://www.bcrfcure.org/breast-cancer-statistics). (Accessed December 15, 2016).
- [4].Goljan EF, Female reproductive disorders and breast disorders, Rapid Review Pathology, Mosby/Elsevier, Philadelphia, 2010, pp. 583–584. [Google Scholar]
- [5].Anderson KN, Schwab RB, Martinez ME, Reproductive risk factors and breast cancer subtypes: a review of the literature, Breast Cancer Res. Treat 144 (1) (2014) 1–10 electronic article http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=4026199&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].U.S. Breast Cancer Statistics, http://www.breastcancer.org/symptoms/understand_bc/statistics, Accessed date: 24 July 2016.
- [7].Risk Factors - National Breast Cancer Foundation, http://www.nationalbreastcancer.org/breast-cancer-risk-factors, Accessed date: 1 January 2016.
- [8].Chida Y, Hamer M, Wardle J, et al. , Do stress-related psychosocial factors contribute to cancer incidence and survival? Nat Clin Pr. Oncol 5 (8) (2008) 466–475 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18493231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Dumalaon-Canaria JA, Hutchinson AD, Prichard I, et al. , What causes breast cancer? A systematic review of causal attributions among breast cancer survivors and how these compare to expert-endorsed risk factors, Cancer Causes Control 25 (7) (2014) 771–785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Chen CC, David AS, Nunnerley H, et al. , Adverse life events and breast cancer: case-control study, BMJ 311 (7019) (1995) 1527–1530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Lillberg K, Stressful life events and risk of breast cancer in 10,808 women: a cohort study, Am. J. Epidemiol 157 (5) (2003) 415–423 electronic article]. http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/157/5/415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Kruk J, Self-reported psychological stress and the risk of breast cancer: a case-control study, Stress 15 (2) (2012) 162–171 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21875303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Michael YL, Carlson NE, Chlebowski RT, et al. , Influence of stressors on breast cancer incidence in the Women’s Health Initiative, Health Psychol. 28 (2) (2009) 137–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Roberts FD, Newcomb PA, Trentham-Dietz A, et al. , Self-reported stress and risk of breast cancer, Cancer 77 (6) (1996) 1089–1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Protheroe D, Turvey K, Horgan K, et al. , Stressful life events and difficulties and onset of breast cancer: case-control study, BMJ 319 (1999) 1027–1030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Schoemaker MJ, Jones ME, Wright LB, et al. , Psychological stress, adverse life events and breast cancer incidence: a cohort investigation in 106,000 women in the United Kingdom, Breast Cancer Res. 18 (1) (2016) 72 electronic article 10.1186/s13058-016-0733-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Chen CC, David AS, Nunnerley H, et al. , Adverse life events and breast cancer: case-control study, BMJ 311 (7019) (1995) 1527–1530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Holmes T, Rale R, The Social Readjustment Rating Scale, Medicine (Baltimore) 11 (5) (1967) 213–218 electronic article http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&btnG=Search&q=intitle:The+social+readjustment+rating+scale#0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Brown GW, Harris T, Social origins of depression: a study of psychiatric disorder in women, Wiley; (1978) 63–117. [Google Scholar]
- [20].Helgesson O, Cabrera C, Lapidus L, et al. , Self-reported stress levels predict subsequent breast cancer in a cohort of Swedish women, Eur. J. Cancer Prev 12 (5) (2003) 377–381 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14512802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Nielsen NR, Zhang Z-F, Kristensen TS, et al. , Self reported stress and risk of breast cancer: prospective cohort study, BMJ 331 (7516) (2005) 548 electronic article http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1200588&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Antonova L, Aronson K, Mueller CR, Stress and breast cancer: from epidemiology to molecular biology, Breast Cancer Res. 13 (2) (2011) 208 electronic article http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3219182&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Fischer A, Ziogas A, Anton-Culver H, Negative valence life events promote breast cancer development, Clin. Breast Cancer (2017) electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29170032, Accessed date: 7 December 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Strange KS, Kerr LR, Andrews HN, et al. , Psychosocial stressors and mammary tumor growth: an animal model, Neurotoxicol. Teratol 22 (1) (2016) 89–102 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10642118 , Accessed date: 16 February 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Fischman HK, Pero RW, Kelly DD, Psychogenic stress induces chromosomal and DNA damage, Int. J. Neurosci 84 (1-4) (1996) 219–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Reiche EMV, Nunes SOV, Morimoto HK, Stress, depression, the immune system, and cancer, Lancet Oncol. 5 (10) (2004) 617–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Antoni MH, Lutgendorf SK, Cole SW, et al. , The influence of bio-behavioural factors on tumour biology: pathways and mechanisms, Nat. Rev. Cancer 6 (3) (2006) 240–248 electronic article http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3146042&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Yang EV, Glaser R, Stressed-induced immunomodulation: implications for tumorigenesis, Brain Behav. Immun 17 (1) (2003) 37–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Glaser R, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Science and society: stress-induced immune dys-function: implications for health, Nat. Rev. Immunol 5 (3) (2005) 243–251 electronic article 10.1038/nri1571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Doyle WJ, et al. , Chronic stress, glucocorticoid receptor resistance, inflammation, and disease risk, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 109 (16) (2012) 5995–5999 electronic article 10.1073/pnas.1118355109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Lu H, Inflammation, a key event in cancer development, Mol. Cancer Res 4 (4) (2006) 221–233 electronic article 10.1158/1541-7786.MCR-05-0261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Heikkilä K, Ebrahim S, Lawlor DA, Systematic review of the association between circulating interleukin-6 (IL-6) and cancer, Eur. J. Cancer 44 (7) (2008) 937–945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Lazarus RS, Folkman S, Cognitive theories of stress and the issue of circularity, Dynamics of Stress, Springer US, New York, NY, 1986, pp. 63–80 http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4684-5122-1_4, Accessed date: 16 October 2016. [Google Scholar]
- [34].Selye H, Stress without distress, Psychopathology of Human Adaptation, Springer US, Boston, MA, 1976, pp. 137–146 http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4684-2238-2_9, Accessed date: 17 October 2017. [Google Scholar]
- [35].Le Fevre M, Matheny J, Kolt GS. Eustress, distress, and interpretation in occupational stress. J. Manag. Psychol [electronic article]. 2003;18(7):726–744. ( 10.1108/02683940310502412). (Accessed October 17, 2017). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- [36].McEwen BS, Protective and Damaging Effects of Stress Mediators, (1998), pp. 171–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Faresjö Å, Jullander M, Götmalm S, et al. , Higher perceived stress and poorer health reflected in elevated cortisol concentrations measured in extracts of hair from middle-aged healthy women, BMC Psychol. 2 (1) (2014) 30 electronic article http://www.biomedcentral.com/2050-7283/2/30. [Google Scholar]
- [38].Tawakol A, Ishai A, Takx RA, et al. , Relation between resting amygdalar activity and cardiovascular events: a longitudinal and cohort study, Lancet 6736 (16) (2017) electronic article http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0140673616317147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Keller A, Litzelman K, Wisk LE, et al. , Association with health and mortality, Health Psychol. 31 (5) (2013) 677–684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Lutgendorf SK, Sood AK, Biobehavioral factors and cancer progression: physiological pathways and mechanisms, 73 (9) (2011) 724–730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Le CP, Nowell CJ, Kim-Fuchs C, et al. , Chronic stress in mice remodels lymph vasculature to promote tumour cell dissemination, Nat. Commun 7 (2016) 10634 electronic article 10.1038/ncomms10634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Pierrehumbert B, Torrisi R, Ansermet F, et al. , Adult attachment representations predict cortisol and oxytocin responses to stress, Attach Hum. Dev 14 (5) (2012) 453–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Renzi C, Vadilonga V, Gandini S, et al. , Stress exposure in significant relationships is associated with lymph node status in breast cancer, PLoS One 11 (2) (2016) 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Anton-Culver H, Cohen PF, Gildea ME, et al. , Characteristics of BRCA1 mutations in a population-based case series of breast and ovarian cancer, Eur. J. Cancer 36 (10) (2000) 1200–1208 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10882857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Largent JA, McEligot AJ, Ziogas A, et al. , Hypertension, diuretics and breast cancer risk, J. Hum. Hypertens 20 (10) (2006) 727–732 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16885996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Paykel ES, Prusoff BA, Uhlenhuth EH, Scaling of life events, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 25 (4) (1971) 340–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Cooper CL, Cooper R, Faragher EB, Incidence and perception of psychosocial stress: the relationship with breast cancer, Psychol. Med 19 (2) (1989) 415–422 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2762443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Sawada T, Nishiyama T, Kikuchi N, et al. , The influence of personality and perceived stress on the development of breast cancer: 20-year follow-up of 29,098 Japanese women, Sci. Rep 6 (April) (2016) 4–10 electronic article 10.1038/srep32559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Hoberman HM, Cohen S, Positive Events and Social Supports as Buffers of Life Change Stress, (1983), pp. 99–125. [Google Scholar]
- [50].Price MA, Tennant CC, Butow PN, et al. , The role of psychosocial factors in the development of breast carcinoma: part II. Life event stressors, social support, defense style, and emotional control and their interactions, Cancer 91 (4) (2001) 686–697 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11241235. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Raison CL, Miller AH. When not enough is too much: the rold of insufficient glucocorticoid signaling in the pathophysiology of stress-related disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry [electronic article]. 2003;20(1):374–81. ( 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.001\n; http://jbr.sagepub.com/content/early/2015/03/25/0748730415576192.abstract%5Cn; http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2670633&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract%5Cn; http://www.ncbi.nlm.ni). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Nephew BC, Carini LM, Sallah S, et al. , Intergenerational accumulation of impairments in maternal behavior following postnatal social stress, http://www.psyneuen-journal.com/article/S0306-4530(17)30213-5/pdf), (2017) , Accessed date: 25 October 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Peled R, Carmil D, Siboni-Samocha O, et al. , Breast cancer, psychological distress and life events among young women, BMC Cancer 8 (2008) 245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Pike MC, Spicer DV, Dahmoush L, et al. , Estrogens, progestogens, normal breast cell proliferation, and breast cancer risk, Epidemiol. Rev 15 (1) (1993) 17–35 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8405201, Accessed date: 2 January 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Lupulescu A, Estrogen use and cancer incidence: a review, Cancer Investig. 13 (3) (1995) 287–295 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7743382, Accessed date: 2 January 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].Yue W, Yager JD, Wang JP, et al. , Estrogen receptor-dependent and independent mechanisms of breast cancer carcinogenesis, Steroids 78 (2) (2013) 161–170 electronic article 10.1016/j.steroids.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Mayo Clinic, Test Definition: ESTF Test Definition:ESTF, (2016), pp. 8–10 http://www.mayomedicallaboratories.com/test-catalog/Clinical+and+Interpretive/84230, Accessed date: 2 June 2017. [Google Scholar]
- [58].Casper RF, Clinical Manifestations and Diagnosis of Menopause - UpToDate, https://www.uptodate.com/contents/clinical-manifestations-and-diagnosis-of-menopause?source=search_result&search=estrogenlevels&selectedTitle=2~150, Accessed date: 6 February 2017.
- [59].Shors TJ, Pickett J, Wood G, et al. , Acute stress persistently enhances estrogen levels in the female rat, Stress 3 (2) (1999) 163–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Justenhoven C, Winter S, Dünnebier T, et al. , Combined UGT1A1 and UGT1A6 genotypes together with a stressful life event increase breast cancer risk, Breast Cancer Res. Treat 124 (1) (2010) 289–292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Kelsey JL, Gammon MD, John EM, Reproductive factors and breast cancer, Epidemiol. Rev 15 (0193-936X (Print)) (1993) 36–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Russo J, Russo IH, Role of differentiation in the pathogenesis and prevention of breast cancer, Endocr. Relat. Cancer 4 (1) (1997) 7–21 electronic article http://erc.endocrinology-journals.org/content/4/1/7.extract, Accessed date: 2 January 2016. [Google Scholar]
- [63].Rose DP, Haffner SM, Baillargeon J. Adiposity, the metabolic syndrome, and breast cancer in African-American and White American women. Endocr. Rev [electronic article]. 2007;28(7):763–777. ( 10.1210/er.2006-0019). (Accessed February 22, 2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].a. Amadou P. Ferrari R. Muwonge, et al. , Overweight, obesity and risk of premenopausal breast cancer according to ethnicity: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis, Obes. Rev 14 (8) (2013) 665–678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Dartois L, Fagherazzi G, Baglietto L, et al. , Proportion of premenopausal and postmenopausal breast cancers attributable to known risk factors: estimates from the E3N-EPIC cohort, Int. J. Cancer 138 (10) (2016) 2415–2427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Lazarus RS, Psychological Stress and the Coping Process, McGraw-Hill, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- [67].Lazarus RS, Psychological stress and coping in adaptation and illness, Psychiatry Med. 5 (4) (1974) 321–333 electronic article http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-0016308131&partnerID=40&md5=e0a6b854622a13080fad9622e9c29bdb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Sarason IG, Johnson JH, Siegel JM, Assesseing the impact of life changes: development of the Life Experiences Survey, J Consult Clin Psychol. 46 (5) (1978) 932–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Vinokur A, Selzer ML, Desirable versus undesirable life events: their relationship to stress and mental distress, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 32 (2) (1975) 329–337 electronic article http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1239500, Accessed date: 24 July 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Funch D, Marshall JR, Measuring life stress:factors affecting fall-off in the reporting of life events, J. Health Soc. Behav 25 (4) (1984) 453–464 electronic article http://www.jstor.org/stable/2136382. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Holmes TH, Rahe RH, The social readjustment rating scale, J. Psychosom. Res 11 (2) (1967) 213–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]