Abstract
Extinction memory destabilized by recall is restabilized through mTOR-dependent reconsolidation in the hippocampus, but the upstream pathways controlling these processes remain unknown. Hippocampal NMDARs drive local protein synthesis via mTOR signaling and may control active memory maintenance. We found that in adult male Wistar rats, intra dorsal-CA1 administration of the non-subunit selective NMDAR antagonist AP5 or of the GluN2A subunit-containing NMDAR antagonist TCN201 after step down inhibitory avoidance (SDIA) extinction memory recall impaired extinction memory retention and caused SDIA memory recovery. On the contrary, pre-recall administration of AP5 or of the GluN2B subunit-containing NMDAR antagonist RO25-6981 had no effect on extinction memory recall or retention per se but hindered the recovery of the avoidance response induced by post-recall intra-CA1 infusion of the mTOR inhibitor rapamycin. Our results indicate that GluN2B-containing NMDARs are necessary for extinction memory destabilization whereas GluN2A-containing NMDARs are involved in its restabilization, and suggest that pharmacological modulation of the relative activation state of these receptor subtypes around the moment of extinction memory recall may regulate the dominance of extinction memory over the original memory trace.
Subject terms: Learning and memory, Extinction
Introduction
Recall reactivates memories that lay dormant and may affect their strength and endurance. When triggered by a single brief re-presentation of the conditioned stimulus in the absence of the unconditioned stimulus, recall can destabilize well-consolidated memories, which must then undergo protein synthesis-dependent reconsolidation to persist. Conversely, repetitive non-reinforced recall events may induce extinction, a protein synthesis-dependent process that generates a new memory that prevents the original one from continuing to control behavior. Notably, extinction memory may also re-enter an instability phase upon recall and must be reconsolidated to maintain its dominance over the extinguished original trace1,2. In the case of fear-motivated avoidance, the reconsolidation of extinction memory requires mTOR-dependent BDNF expression in the dorsal hippocampus3,4, but the upstream pathways controlling this process remain largely unknown.
N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptors (NMDARs) are heterotetrameric ionotropic receptors formed by co-assembly of seven subunits (GluN1, GluN2A-D, and GluN3A-B) that mediate a Ca2+-permeable component of glutamatergic neurotransmission. Most native NMDARs contain two obligatory GluN1 subunits and two GluN2 subunits, which confer distinctive channel, ligand-binding and signaling properties to NMDAR subtypes and enable them to fulfill specific physiological functions. In particular, GluN2A- and GluN2B-containing NMDARs control bidirectional synaptic plasticity5, play key roles in memory consolidation and extinction6,7, and underlie the destabilization and restabilization of different memory types during reconsolidation8–13. Here, we analyzed whether hippocampal NMDARs are necessary for extinction memory reconsolidation by assessing the effect of the intra-dorsal CA1 administration of non-subunit specific and subunit-specific NMDAR antagonists at different time points around the moment of step-down inhibitory avoidance (SDIA) extinction memory recall.
Results
Post-recall intra-CA1 administration of the non-subunit specific NMDAR antagonist AP5 hinders SDIA extinction memory and induces SDIA memory renewal
To study the role of hippocampal NMDARs in the reconsolidation of extinction memory, we first trained adult male Wistar rats in one-trial step-down inhibitory avoidance (SDIA; 0.4 mA/2 s foot shock), a learning task that induces a long-lasting hippocampus dependent fear-motivated avoidance memory14,15 and then, beginning one day post-training, we re-exposed the animals to the SDIA training apparatus in the absence of the foot shock once daily for 5 consecutive days. This procedure generates a hippocampus dependent SDIA extinction memory16–18 resistant to spontaneous recovery, renewal and reinstatement1,3. Twenty four hours after the last extinction training trial, we submitted the animals to an extinction memory reactivation session (RA), and 5 min or 6 h later they received bilateral intra-dorsal CA1 infusions of vehicle (VEH; 1% DMSO in saline) or the non-subunit specific NMDAR antagonist D(-)-2-Amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (AP5; 5 µg/side). Retention was evaluated twice, 1 day and 7 days after RA. We found that AP5 impaired SDIA extinction memory retention and recovered the SDIA response when injected 5 min but not 6 h after RA (Fig. 1a,b, 1 day after RA: U = 1, p < 0.0001, VEH vs AP5; 7 days after RA: U = 0, p < 0.0001, VEH vs AP5 5 min after RA in Mann–Whitney test). AP5 did not affect SDIA extinction memory when given 24 h after the last extinction training trial in the absence of RA (Fig. 1c) or when administered 5 min after a pseudo-RA session carried out in a non-aversive training box (Fig. 1d, RA: U = 0, p < 0.0001, VEH vs AP5; pRA: U = 42.50, p = 0.5875, VEH vs AP5 in Mann–Whitney test).
Pre-recall intra-CA1 administration of the non-subunit specific NMDAR antagonist AP5 does not affect SDIA extinction memory expression or retention but hampers the amnesic effect of reconsolidation blockers
To evaluate the effect of pre-recall hippocampal NMDAR inhibition on SDIA extinction memory, we submitted SDIA-trained animals to the extinction protocol described above. Twenty-four hours after the last extinction training trial the animals received bilateral intra-dorsal CA1 infusions of VEH or AP5 and 20 min later were submitted to RA. We found that pre-RA AP5 did not affect SDIA extinction memory recall or retention (Fig. 2a) but impeded the recovery of avoidance induced by post-RA intra-CA1 administration of rapamycin (RAP; 0.02 µg/side), an inhibitor of mammalian target of RAP (mTOR), a kinase that regulates synaptic protein synthesis through the phosphorylation of eukaryotic initiation factor 4E-binding protein 1 and p70 ribosomal S6 kinase19 and is required for SDIA extinction memory reconsolidation in the hippocampus4 (Fig. 2b, 1 day after RA: U = 0, p < 0.0001, VEH vs RAP; 7 days after RA: U = 0, p < 0.0001, VEH vs RAP in Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 2c, 1 day after RA: H = 17.73, p = 0.0005; p < 0.01 for VEH + VEH vs VEH + RAP, p < 0.01 for VEH + RAP vs AP5 + VEH, p < 0.05 for VEH + RAP vs AP5 + RAP; 7 days after RA: H = 17.74, p = 0.0005; p < 0.01 for VEH + VEH vs VEH + RAP, p < 0.01 for VEH + RAP vs AP5 + VEH, p < 0.05 for VEH + RAP vs AP5 + RAP in Dunn's multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test). At the dose used in our experiments, AP5 did not affect SDIA memory recall (Fig. 2d).
Hippocampal GluN2B-containing NMDARs mediate SDIA extinction memory destabilization during recall whereas GluN2A-containing NMDARs are necessary for SDIA extinction memory reconsolidation
GluN2A-containing and GluN2B-containing NMDAR regulate different cellular events in the amygdala during fear memory reconsolidation, controlling restabilization and destabilization phases, respectively13,20. Therefore, because our results with AP5 are consistent with a dual role of hippocampal NMDAR in extinction memory reconsolidation, we analyzed whether GluN2A-containing NMDAR and GluN2B-containing NMDAR also mediate SDIA extinction memory restabilization and destabilization differentially. To evaluate the involvement of these two NMDAR subtypes in extinction memory restabilization, we submitted SDIA-trained animals to the SDIA extinction protocol, as described above. One day after the last extinction training session, SDIA extinction memory was reactivated and 5 min later the animals received bilateral intra-dorsal CA1 infusions of VEH, the GluN2B-containing NMDAR antagonist RO25-6981 (RO; 2.5 µg/side), or the GluN2A-containing NMDAR antagonist TCN201 (TCN; 0.05 µg/side). As can be seen in Fig. 3, TCN, but not RO, impaired extinction memory retention and induced SDIA memory recovery 1 day and 7 days post-RA (Fig. 3a, 1 day after RA: H = 20.10, p < 0.0001; p < 0.001 for VEH vs TCN, p < 0.01 for RO vs TCN; 7 days after RA: H = 21.51, p < 0.0001; p < 0.001 for VEH vs TCN, p < 0.001 for RO vs TCN in Dunn's multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test). Neither RO nor TCN affected extinction memory retention when administered 24 h after the last extinction training trial in the absence of RA (Fig. 3b) or when given 5 min after a pseudo-RA session carried out in a non-aversive training box (Fig. 3c, RA: H = 19.30, p < 0.0001; p < 0.001 for VEH vs TCN, p < 0.01 for RO vs TCN; pRA: H = 2.006, p = 0.3667 in Dunn's multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test). We next evaluated whether GluN2B and GluN2A-containing NMDAR mediate SDIA extinction memory destabilization. To that end, SDIA-trained animals were submitted to the SDIA extinction protocol and 24 h after the last extinction training trial received bilateral intra-dorsal CA1 infusions of VEH, RO or TCN. SDIA extinction memory was reactivated 20 min after the injections and 5 min thereafter the animals received VEH or RAP in dorsal CA1. Retention was assessed 24 h post-RA. TCN did not affect extinction memory recall 20 min post-injection but TCN, RAP and TCN + RAP impaired SDIA extinction memory retention 1 day and 7 days after RA, causing the reappearance of the SDIA response (Fig. 4a, 1 day after RA: H = 20.65, p = 0.0001; p < 0.001 for VEH + VEH vs VEH + RAP, p < 0.001 for VEH + VEH vs TCN + VEH, p < 0.05 for VEH + VEH vs TCN + RAP; 7 days after RA: H = 19.96, p = 0.0002; p < 0.001 for VEH + VEH vs VEH + RAP, p < 0.01 for VEH + VEH vs TCN + VEH, p < 0.01 for VEH + VEH vs TCN + RAP in Dunn's multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test). RO did not affect extinction memory recall 20 min post-injection either but had no effect per se on retention and blocked the recovery of avoidance induced by post-RA RAP administration (Fig. 4b, 1 day after RA: H = 21.18, p < 0.0001; p < 0.001 for VEH + VEH vs VEH + RAP, p < 0.01 for VEH + RAP vs RO + VEH, p < 0.05 for VEH + RAP vs RO + RAP; 7 days after RA: H = 22.77, p < 0.0001; p < 0.001 for VEH + VEH vs VEH + RAP, p < 0.01 for VEH + RAP vs RO + VEH, p < 0.05 for VEH + RAP vs RO + RAP in Dunn's multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test). Neither RO nor TCN affected SDIA memory recall or locomotor activity when given in dorsal CA1 20 min before a SDIA memory retention test (Fig. 4c) or a 5 min-long free-exploration session in an open-field arena, respectively (Fig. 4d; F(8, 92) = 1.710, p = 0.1065 for interaction; F(2, 23) = 0.02170, p = 0.9786 for treatment effect; F(4, 92) = 50.11, p < 0.0001 for time effect in two-way RM ANOVA).
Discussion
Our findings confirm that extinction learning does not erase SDIA memory but creates a new trace that competes with it for controlling behavior, and corroborate that SDIA extinction memory enters an instability phase when recalled and must be restabilized through reconsolidation to maintain the learned avoidance response inhibited. Importantly, our data demonstrate that SDIA extinction memory destabilization and restabilization necessitate hippocampal NMDARs activation and indicate that GluN2B- and GluN2A-containing NMDAR subtypes, respectively, are differentially involved in these processes. This assertion is based on experiments showing that post-recall infusion of AP5 and TCN, but not of RO, impaired extinction memory retention lastingly and induced the recovery of avoidance in a time-dependent manner whereas pre-recall administration of AP5 and RO, but not of TCN, rendered the reactivated extinction memory trace resistant to the amnesia caused by mTOR inhibition. Our conclusions are further supported by the fact that neither AP5 nor TCN affected extinction memory when its reactivation was omitted or when they were given after exploration of a non-aversive environment, as well as by results showing that pre-recall infusion of AP5, TCN and RO had no effect on SDIA memory expression. Moreover, our data agree with reports that pre-recall administration of non-subunit selective or GluN2B-selective NMDAR antagonists prevents contextual fear memory updating10,21 and others showing that destabilized memories require GluN2A-containing NMDAR upregulation to regain stability and persist13,22,23. Milton and coworkers13 attributed the differential involvement of GluN2B and GluN2A-containing NMDARs in the destabilization and restabilization of reconsolidating memories to the fact that GluN2B-NMDARs regulate the activation state of the protein degradation system that modulates memory lability24 whereas GluN2A-NMDARs promote CREB phosphorylation25 and long-term potentiation26, which are associated with memory re-encoding and maintenance27–29.
Psychotherapeutic interventions based on memory extinction are initially effective in reducing the exacerbated recall of traumatic events that afflicts post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients. However, PTSD symptoms usually return spontaneously, or due to unexpected triggers, after the end of psychotherapy. Therefore, most research on the psychopharmacology of extinction has revolved around the search for tools to improve extinction learning, based on the premise that this could prevent fear recovery. Because it has been repeatedly shown that NMDAR blockade impairs extinction retention whereas NMDAR function enhancement facilitates fear extinction in experimental animals30–33, NMDARs have taken center stage in this enterprise. In this regard, it has been reported that the NMDAR partial agonist d-cycloserine (DCS) increases exposure therapy effects34–36, although the available data are inconclusive37,38 and experimental animals as well as humans treated with DCS during extinction learning show substantial recovery of learned fear, suggesting that DCS may not prevent PTSD relapse39–42. These studies, together with several others, suggest that extinction can indeed be modulated by drugs, but not lastingly43–45. Our findings that SDIA memory is still able to assume control of behavior after undergoing an extinction procedure that generates an extinction memory resistant to spontaneous recovery, renewal and reinstatement but sensitive to recall-induced GluN2B-containing NMDAR-dependent destabilization indicate that it is this destabilization what enables the reappearance of avoidance, and lead us to propose that blockers of these receptors might be suitable tools to prevent PTSD relapse. It is crucial to emphasize here that memory destabilization can result not only from explicit recall, but also from exposure to subtle signals and cues unable to elicit discernible behavioral responses46. In addition, it is important to consider that because of local animal housing regulations and facility constraints our experiments were carried out exclusively on adult male rats although sex and age related differences in NMDAR endogenous modulation and subunit composition have been reported47–49, women are twice as likely to develop PTSD than men50,51, and PTSD management in children might require a differential therapeutic approach52,53.
Methods
Animals
We used 3-month-old male Wistar rats, weighing 300–350 g at the start of the experiments. They were housed in groups of 5 with free access to water and food in a holding room maintained at 22–23 °C on a normal light cycle (12 h light:12 h dark; lights on at 6.00 A.M.). Experiments were performed during the light phase of the cycle. All procedures were performed in accordance with the USA National Institutes of Health Guidelines and Regulations for Animal Care and were approved by the local institutional ethics committee (Comissão de Ética no Uso de Animais—CEUA, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte).
Surgery and drug infusion procedures
Animals were submitted to stereotaxic surgery and implanted with 22-gauge guides aimed at the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus (coordinates in mm: anteroposterior, − 4.2; laterolateral, ± 3.0; dorsoventral, − 3.0). Experiments began 1 week after surgery. At the time of drug delivery, infusion cannulas were fitted into the guides and injections (1 µl/side) performed during 60 s with a microinjection pump. Cannulas were left in place for 60 additional seconds to minimize backflow. Cannula placement was verified postmortem. To do that, 2–4 h after the last behavioral test we infused 1 μl of 4% methylene-blue as described above; the extension of the dye 30 min thereafter was taken as an indicator of drug diffusion. Only data from animals with correct implants were analyzed (see Supplementary Fig. S1).
Step-down inhibitory avoidance (SDIA) training
The SDIA training box (50 × 25 × 25 cm) consisted of a Plexiglas chamber with a grid floor through which a scrambled electric shock could be delivered to the rat's feet and a wooden platform (5 × 8 × 25 cm) at the left end of the grid floor. For training (TR), each animal was placed on the platform and when it stepped down and placed its four paws on the grid, received a mild foot shock (0.4 mA/2 s). Immediately thereafter the rat was withdrawn from the training box.
SDIA memory extinction protocol
To extinguish SDIA memory, animals were submitted to five daily extinction sessions during which they were placed on the training box platform and allowed to freely explore the apparatus for 30 s after stepping down to the grid. To reactivate the SDIA extinction memory (RA), the animals were placed on the platform and after stepping down from it were immediately removed from the training box.
Drugs
D( −)-2-Amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (AP5; 5 µg/side), RO 25–6981 (RO; 2.5 µg/side), TCN 201 (TCN; 0.05 µg/side) and rapamycin (RAP; 0.02 µg/side) were dissolved according to the manufacturer's instructions and stored protected from light at − 20 °C until use. Aliquots were thawed and diluted to working concentration in 1% DMSO in sterile saline (pH 7.2) on the day of the experiment. The doses used were determined based on pilot experiments and previous studies showing the behavioral and biochemical effects of each compound3,4,54,55. An equal volume of 1% DMSO in sterile saline served as vehicle control.
Data analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism 8 software. Significance was set at p < 0.05. A 300 s ceiling was imposed on retention test session latency. Data were analyzed using two-tailed Mann–Whitney U test, Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn's post hoc comparisons, or two-way RM ANOVA followed by Bonferroni's multiple-comparisons test.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq, Brazil) and Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES, Brazil). A.R. is a postdoctoral research fellow supported by CAPES. D.A.N. holds a CAPES doctoral research fellowship through Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicobiologia at Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN, Brazil).
Author contributions
A.R., M.C.G., D.A.N. and J.I.R. performed research; A.R., M.C.G., D.A.N., J.I.R., L.R.M.B. and M.C. analyzed data; L.R.M.B. and M.C. designed and supervised research. A.R., M.C.G. and M.C. wrote the paper.
Data availability
Data are available upon request by contacting the corresponding author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-020-80674-7.
References
- 1.Rossato JI, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Medina JH, Cammarota M. Retrieval induces reconsolidation of fear extinction memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:21801–21805. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016254107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Garcia-Delatorre P, Rodríguez-Ortiz CJ, Balderas I, Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Differential participation of temporal structures in the consolidation and reconsolidation of taste aversion extinction. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2010;32:1018–1023. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07365.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Radiske A, et al. Requirement for BDNF in the reconsolidation of fear extinction. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:6570–6574. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4093-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Radiske A, et al. mTOR inhibition impairs extinction memory reconsolidation. Learn. Mem. 2021;28:1–6. doi: 10.1101/lm.052068.120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rebola N, Srikumar BN, Mulle C. Activity-dependent synaptic plasticity of NMDA receptors. J. Physiol. 2010;588:93–99. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.179382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Morris RG. NMDA receptors and memory encoding. Neuropharmacology. 2013;74:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Radulovic J, Ren LY, Gao C. N-methyl D-aspartate receptor subunit signaling in fear extinction. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:239–250. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5022-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Przybyslawski J, Sara SJ. Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behav. Brain Res. 1997;84:241–246. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(96)00153-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sadler R, Herzig V, Schmidt WJ. Repeated treatment with the NMDA antagonist MK-801 disrupts reconsolidation of memory for amphetamine-conditioned place preference. Behav. Pharmacol. 2007;18:699–703. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282effb81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ben Mamou C, Gamache K, Nader K. NMDA receptors are critical for unleashing consolidated auditory fear memories. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:1237–1239. doi: 10.1038/nn1778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lopez J, Gamache K, Schneider R, Nader K. Memory retrieval requires ongoing protein synthesis and NMDA receptor activity-mediated AMPA receptor trafficking. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:2465–2475. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0735-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Finnie PS, Nader K. The role of metaplasticity mechanisms in regulating memory destabilization and reconsolidation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012;36:1667–1707. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Milton AL, et al. Double dissociation of the requirement for GluN2B- and GluN2A-containing NMDA receptors in the destabilization and restabilization of a reconsolidating memory. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1109–1115. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3273-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bernabeu R, et al. Learning-specific, time-dependent increase in [3H]phorbol dibutyrate binding to protein kinase C in selected regions of the rat brain. Brain Res. 1995;685:163–168. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00310-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Paratcha G, et al. Involvement of hippocampal PKCbetaI isoform in the early phase of memory formation of an inhibitory avoidance learning. Brain Res. 2000;855:199–205. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)02323-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cammarota M, et al. Relationship between short- and long-term memory and short- and long-term extinction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2005;84:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rossato JI, et al. On the participation of hippocampal p38 mitogen-activated protein kinase in extinction and reacquisition of inhibitory avoidance memory. Neuroscience. 2006;143:15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.07.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bonini JS, et al. Histamine facilitates consolidation of fear extinction. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;14:1209–1217. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hay N, Sonenberg N. Upstream and downstream of mTOR. Genes Dev. 2004;18:1926–1945. doi: 10.1101/gad.1212704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Holehonnur R, et al. Increasing the GluN2A/GluN2B ratio in neurons of the mouse basal and lateral amygdala inhibits the modification of an existing fear memory trace. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:9490–9504. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1743-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Haubrich J, et al. Reconsolidation allows fear memory to be updated to a less aversive level through the incorporation of appetitive information. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:315–326. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wells AM, et al. Contribution of an SFK-mediated signaling pathway in the dorsal hippocampus to cocaine-memory reconsolidation in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:675–685. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hafenbreidel M, Rafa Todd C, Mueller D. Infralimbic GluN2A-containing NMDA receptors modulate reconsolidation of cocaine self-administration memory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:1113–1125. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lee JL. Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:1264–1266. doi: 10.1038/nn.2205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hardingham GE, Bading H. Coupling of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors to a CREB shut-off pathway is developmentally regulated. Biochim. Biophys. Acta. 2002;1600:148–153. doi: 10.1016/S1570-9639(02)00455-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Liu L, et al. Role of NMDA receptor subtypes in governing the direction of hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Science. 2004;304:1021–1024. doi: 10.1126/science.1096615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mamiya N, et al. Brain region-specific gene expression activation required for reconsolidation and extinction of contextual fear memory. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:402–413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4639-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Clarke JR, Cammarota M, Gruart A, Izquierdo I, Delgado-Garcia JM. Plastic modifications induced by object recognition memory processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:2652–2657. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0915059107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Okubo-Suzuki R, et al. Frequency-specific stimulations induce reconsolidation of long-term potentiation in freely moving rats. Mol. Brain. 2016;9:36. doi: 10.1186/s13041-016-0216-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Falls WA, Miserendino MJ, Davis M. Extinction of fear-potentiated startle: blockade by infusion of an NMDA antagonist into the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 1992;12:854–863. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-03-00854.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Santini E, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires NMDA receptor-dependent bursting in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2007;53:871–880. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lee JL, Milton AL, Everitt BJ. Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear: inhibition and potentiation. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:10051–10056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2466-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Walker DL, Ressler KJ, Lu KT, Davis M. Facilitation of conditioned fear extinction by systemic administration or intra-amygdala infusions of D-cycloserine as assessed with fear-potentiated startle in rats. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:2343–2351. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02343.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Heresco-Levy U, et al. Placebo-controlled trial of D-cycloserine added to conventional neuroleptics, olanzapine, or risperidone in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psych. 2002;159:480–482. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Difede J, et al. D-cycloserine augmentation of exposure therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: a pilot randomized clinical trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:1052–1058. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rodrigues H, et al. Does D-cycloserine enhance exposure therapy for anxiety disorders in humans? A meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e93519. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Davis M. NMDA receptors and fear extinction: implications for cognitive behavioral therapy. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2011;13:463–474. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2011.13.4/mdavis. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Tart CD, et al. Augmentation of exposure therapy with post-session administration of D-cycloserine. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013;47:168–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.09.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Woods AM, Bouton ME. D-cycloserine facilitates extinction but does not eliminate renewal of the conditioned emotional response. Behav. Neurosci. 2006;120:1159–1162. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Guastella AJ, Lovibond PF, Dadds MR, Mitchell P, Richardson RA. randomized controlled trial of the effect of D-cycloserine on extinction and fear conditioning in humans. Behav. Res. Ther. 2007;45:663–672. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mickley GA, et al. Acute, but not chronic, exposure to d-cycloserine facilitates extinction and modulates spontaneous recovery of a conditioned taste aversion. Physiol. Behav. 2012;105:417–427. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hofmann SG, et al. Effect of d-cycloserine on fear extinction training in adults with social anxiety disorder. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0223729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fitzgerald PJ, Seemann JR, Maren S. Can fear extinction be enhanced? A review of pharmacological and behavioral findings. Brain Res. Bull. 2014;105:46–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Goode TD, Holloway-Erickson CM, Maren S. Extinction after fear memory reactivation fails to eliminate renewal in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017;142:41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chalkia A, et al. No persistent attenuation of fear memories in humans: A registered replication of the reactivation-extinction effect. Cortex. 2020;129:496–509. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gisquet-Verrier P, Riccio DC. Memory reactivation effects independent of reconsolidation. Learn. Mem. 2012;19:401–409. doi: 10.1101/lm.026054.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hönack D, Löscher W. Sex differences in NMDA receptor mediated responses in rats. Brain Res. 1993;620:167–170. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90287-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zhao X, et al. The effects of aging on N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunits in the synaptic membrane and relationships to long-term spatial memory. Neuroscience. 2009;162:933–945. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.05.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Magnusson KR. Declines in mRNA expression of different subunits may account for differential effects of aging on agonist and antagonist binding to the NMDA receptor. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:1666–1674. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-05-01666.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Tolin DF, Foa EB. Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 2006;132:959–992. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Velasco ER, Florido A, Milad MR, Andero R. Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019;103:81–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Najjar F, Weller RA, Weisbrot J, Weller EB. Post-traumatic stress disorder and its treatment in children and adolescents. Curr. Psych. Rep. 2008;10:104–108. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0019-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kaminer D, Seedat S, Stein DJ. Post-traumatic stress disorder in children. World Psych. 2005;4:121–125. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Go BS, Barry SM, McGinty JF. Glutamatergic neurotransmission in the prefrontal cortex mediates the suppressive effect of intra-prelimbic cortical infusion of BDNF on cocaine-seeking. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26:1989–1999. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gipson CD, et al. Reinstatement of nicotine seeking is mediated by glutamatergic plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:9124–9129. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220591110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data are available upon request by contacting the corresponding author.