Abstract
The paracingulate sulcus -PCGS- has been considered for a long time to be specific to the human brain. Its presence/absence has been discussed in relation to interindividual variability of personality traits and cognitive abilities. Recently, a putative PCGS has been observed in chimpanzee brains. To demonstrate that this newly discovered sulcus is the homologue of the PCGS in the human brain, we analyzed cytoarchitectonic and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data in chimpanzee brains which did or did not display a PCGS. The results show that the organization of the mid-cingulate cortex of the chimpanzee brain is comparable to that of the human brain, both cytoarchitectonically and in terms of functional connectivity with the lateral frontal cortex. These results demonstrate that the PCGS is not human-specific but is a shared feature of the primate brain since at least the last common ancestor to humans and great apes ~6 mya.
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Evolution
The paracingulate sulcus (PCGS) is a brain structure long thought to be specific to humans, and variation in this structure has been linked to personality traits and cognitive abilities. In this study, Céline Amiez and Jérôme Sallet et al. analyze brain imaging data from humans and chimpanzees to demonstrate that the PCGS is in fact present in our closest relative and its functional connectivity in chimpanzees is comparable to that in humans.
Introduction
Understanding the mechanisms underlying brain evolution, and more specifically of the human brain, is still the topic of intense debates1–4. Comparative neuroanatomical studies have demonstrated that ecological and social pressures are key factors that have driven the expansion of the neocortex in primates. But this expansion has differentially impacted brain circuits5. With the development of neuroimaging tools, one could address comparative neuroanatomy questions in vivo at different levels of analysis, from gross morphology (e.g., sulcal pattern analysis) to brain connectivity (e.g., resting-state functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging analysis). Comparative neuroimaging studies are principally relying on a comparison between human brains and a limited number of non-human primate models, namely macaques and marmosets6,7, whose ancestors diverged from human ancestors 25 and 35 million years ago, respectively8. With a common evolutionary history until 7 million years ago, the chimpanzee is a key model for better understanding the evolution of brain regions that have largely expanded in the human brain, such as the medial prefrontal cortex9. Among the sulci that characterize the human medial frontal cortex, the paracingulate sulcus (PCGS) is a secondary sulcus running dorsal and parallel to the cingulate sulcus (CGS) in a rostro-caudal direction10,11 in the medial frontal cortex. The PCGS is observed in about 70% of subjects at least in one hemisphere10–13 and most often starts at the intersection with the sus-orbitalis and the supra-rostral sulcus, in front and at the level of the anterior limit of the genu of the corpus callosum, where the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) lies (Fig. 1a, b)13,14. We are referring here to the cingulate subdivisions proposed by Vogt et al.15 (Fig. 1c). From the ACC, the PCGS runs caudally where the anterior mid-cingulate cortex (aMCC) lies, but it can also run as far posterior as the level of the anterior commissure (where the posterior mid-cingulate cortex (pMCC) lies) (Fig. 1). In the human brain, the impact of the presence of a PCGS on the cytoarchitectonic organization (i.e., the cellular organization of the cerebral cortex) of the aMCC is known10: when the PCGS is absent, areas 24c′ and 32′ occupy, respectively, the ventral and the dorsal banks of the CGS; however, when a PCGS is present, area 24c′ occupies both banks of the CGS and area 32′ occupies the PCGS (Fig. 1).
In the human brain, the morphological variability of the PCGS has been linked to personality traits16–18, and pathologies19–23, and has also been associated with higher-order cognitive processing, i.e., several so-called human-specific processes12,24–27. These observations have led some investigators to suggest that cortical area 32′ which occupies the PCGS when present and the dorsal bank of the CGS when the PCGS is absent, might be unique to the human brain28,29. However, a recent study has shown, based on morphological observations of the sulcal organization on structural MRI scans, the presence of a putative homologue of the human PCGS in 33.8% of chimpanzees, at least in one hemisphere13. Furthermore, opposing the view of a lack of area 32′ in non-human primates, this transition area (i.e., area 32′) between the cingulate cortex and cortex of the medial frontal gyrus has been shown in macaques30. However, the statement that the PCGS is not human-specific and can be observed in chimpanzee brains must be supported by cytoarchitectonic evidence showing that the organization of this region in the chimpanzee brain is comparable to that in the human brain.
In the chimpanzee brain, it is not known whether, as in the human brain, the PCGS starts in the ACC and runs caudally to the midcingulate cortex (MCC)13. In the present study, we therefore first assessed the extent of the PCGS in both human and chimpanzee brains and hypothesized that, if the sulcus that we identified as a PCGS in the chimpanzee13 is homologous to the human PCGS, the mapping of the cytoarchitectonic organization of the aMCC in the chimpanzee should be comparable to that in the human brain.
It should be noted that, in the human brain, the functional connectivity within the MCC of seeds located in the PCGS and in the CGS, when the PCGS is absent, is similar. Specifically, Loh et al.31 have assessed the functional connectivity of the anterior rostral cingulate zone (RCZa) which is located within the anterior part of the MCC. Within the RCZa, there are limb and face motor representations with the limb motor representations lying within the CGS even when a PCGS is present; the face motor representations lie in the PCGS if present and in the CGS if the PCGS is absent32. Loh et al.31 have shown that the functional connectivity of the face motor representation of RCZa with lateral prefrontal and lateral motor regions of interest is similarly organized, regardless of whether it is located in the CGS in hemispheres without a PCGS or in the PCGS in hemispheres with a PCGS. The functional connectivity is stronger with anterior prefrontal regions and weaker with posterior motor regions31.
In the present study, we examined (1) the extent of the PCGS in both chimpanzee and human brains, (2) the cytoarchitectonic organization of the aMCC with a specific emphasis on the distribution of areas 24c′ and 32′ in three post-mortem chimpanzee brains which did or did not display a PCGS, and (3) in vivo functional connectivity of this region in four anesthetized chimpanzees based on the availability of resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data. The results demonstrate that, in the chimpanzee brain, the impact the PCGS has on the cytoarchitectonic organization of the aMCC is comparable to that observed in the human brain. The results also show that the functional connectivity of the CGS and the PCGS is comparable to that observed in the human brain31. Altogether, these results demonstrate that the PCGS in chimpanzee brains is comparable in terms of cytoarchitecture and functional connectivity with the PCGS in human brains. These observations demonstrate that the PCGS is not human-specific and had already emerged in the brains of the last common ancestor with chimpanzees.
Results
Morphological study
We first re-analyzed data from Amiez et al.13 to assess the occurrence of a PCGS in the ACC versus the MCC in 197 human and 225 chimpanzee brains. Note that the ACC/MCC limit was identified using the probabilistic cytoarchitectonic map of the ACC from the JuBrain atlas (see “Methods”)33. This new analysis demonstrates that, in hemispheres displaying a PCGS (i.e., n = 183 human brain hemispheres, n = 91 chimpanzee brain hemispheres), the probability of observing a PCGS in the ACC is higher (89.6%) in human hemispheres than in chimpanzee (50.5%) hemispheres (Fig. 2a, dependent variable: PCGS present (0/1), main effect species: χ2 = 49.7, df = 1,p-value = 1.79e−12, logistic regression; source data are provided as Supplementary data 1).
By contrast, the probability of observing a PCGS in the MCC is comparable in human (78.7%) and chimpanzee (69.2%), i.e., it does not statistically differ between these two species (Fig. 2b) (dependent variable: PCGS present in MCC (0/1), main effect species: χ2 = 2.9, df = 1, p-value = 0.09, logistic regression). Finally, the probability of observing a PCGS in both the ACC and the MCC is significantly higher in human (in 68.3% of hemispheres displaying a PCGS) compared to chimpanzees (in 19.8% of hemispheres displaying a PCGS, Fig. 2c) (dependent variable: PCGS location (ACC/MCC), main effect species: χ2 = 60.2, df = 1, p-value = 8.48e−15, logistic regression).
Cytoarchitectonic study
Note that our analysis is specifically focused on the distribution of area 24c′ and area 32′ and the impact of the presence/absence of a PCGS on it. Previous studies had already investigated the dorsal-ventral or rostro-caudal organization of the cingulate cortex and adjacent areas of the medial frontal region7,33–36.
From the morphological inspection of the three chimpanzee brains included in the following cytoarchitectonic analysis, we selected four hemispheres. We analyzed the left hemisphere of CHIMP_1 and the right hemisphere of CHIMP_3 in which the PCGS was absent. We also selected the right hemisphere of CHIMP_1 and the left hemisphere of CHIMP_2 which displayed a PCGS. In CHIMP_2, the PCGS was present in the anteriormost part of the aMCC, but absent in the posterior part of the aMCC. Note that the remaining hemispheres (left hemisphere of CHIMP_3 and right hemisphere of CHIMP_2) displayed no PCGS.
Hemispheres without a PCGS
We examined first the MCC within the left hemisphere of CHIMP_1, which did not display a PCGS. As shown in Fig. 3a, proceeding from the corpus callosum dorsally, we observed successively areas 24a′ and 24b′, respectively on the ventral and dorsal part of the gyrus of the cingulate cortex, and areas 24c′ and 32′, respectively in the ventral and dorsal bank of the CGS (for the cytoarchitectonic characteristics, see “Methods” section). Note that a transition zone was observed between each area (i.e. between areas 24a′ and 24b′, between areas 24b′ and 24c′, and between areas 24c′ and 32′). Proceeding dorsally along the cingulate gyrus, the cytoarchitecture does not change abruptly, but rather a smooth reorganization is observed. We also examined the posterior part of the aMCC of CHIMP_2 (Fig. 4, slice 81) which does not display a PCGS (although the anterior part of the aMCC does possess a PCGS). The results demonstrated exactly the same cytoarchitectonic organization as in the left MCC of CHIMP_1.
Hemispheres with a PCGS
We examined the right hemisphere of CHIMP_1 and CHIMP_3, both of which display a PCGS in the MCC. In both chimpanzees, we observed successively from the corpus callosum in a dorsal direction towards the lateral cortical surface, area 24a′ and area 24b′, respectively on the ventral and dorsal parts of the cingulate gyrus, area 24c′ in the ventral bank and in part of the dorsal bank of the CGS, and area 32′ which extends from a part of the dorsal bank of the CGS to the ventral bank of the PCGS (for the cytoarchitectonic characteristics, see the “Methods” section). As in hemispheres in which the PCGS is absent, we observed small transition zones between adjacent areas (Fig. 3b). We also examined the anterior part of the aMCC in the left hemisphere of CHIMP_2 (Fig. 4, slices 141 and 701) which displays a PCGS. The results demonstrated exactly the same cytoarchitectonic organization as in the right MCC of CHIMP_1 and CHIMP_3.
Rs-fMRI study
From the morphological inspection of the four chimpanzee brains included in the following rs-fMRI analysis, we observed a PCGS in the left hemisphere of CHIMP_A, and in both the left and right hemispheres of CHIMP_C. The right hemisphere of CHIMP_A, and both hemispheres of CHIMP_B and CHIMP_D did not display a PCGS.
We first assessed, in hemispheres displaying a PCGS, the intra-hemispheric functional connectivity profiles of areas 24c′ and 32′ with Regions Of Interest (ROIs) located in the lateral frontal cortex (Fig. 5a). To be able to conduct a comparison of connectivity fingerprints between species, ROIs were chosen for their known homologies between chimpanzee and human brains. The location of seeds and ROIs are displayed on the medial (left diagram) and the lateral cortical surface (right diagram) of a typical example (left hemisphere of CHIMP_C). The heat-maps reflecting the average correlation strength between each pair of seed-ROI clusters in the three hemispheres displaying a PCGS (see “Methods”) are shown in Fig. 5a. The Boxplots further depict the average Z values of correlations between the two seeds and ROIs across the three hemispheres displaying a PCGS (see “Methods”). The results demonstrate how the activity of each seed is differentially correlated with the activity of lateral prefrontal/motor ROIs (see “Methods”). For each seed, we tested these differences in connectivity z values with a generalized linear model with ROI zones (prefrontal zones: Area 10, DLPFC (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), Area 45, Area 44, and Fo (Frontal operculum), and motor zones: FEF (Frontal Eye Field), M1Face and M1Hand) as a fixed effect. To account for the variability observed across individuals, the chimpanzee ID was used as a random effect. The results indicated that, as in the human brain, the correlation strength between Area 24c′ and Area 32′ with the prefrontal cortex is significantly higher than with the motor zones (Area 24c′: df = 7, F = 154.8, p < 2.2e−16; Area 32’: df = 7, F = 157, p < 2.2e−16, ANOVA). We then assessed the linearity of correlation trends with lateral frontal areas along the rostro-caudal axis (Fig. 5c). To test and quantify these linear trends from anterior prefrontal to motor areas, we recoded the various lateral frontal ROIs into a numeric axis variable (ROIline) that corresponded to their relative posterior-to-anterior positions (see “Methods”). Based on this coding, the lowest value (1) corresponds to Area 10 (the most anterior ROI) and the highest value (8) corresponds to the M1-Hand region (the most posterior ROI).
We performed the same analysis in hemispheres without a PCGS. The location of seeds and ROIs are displayed on the medial (left diagram) and the lateral cortical surface (right diagram) of a typical example (left hemisphere of CHIMP_B). The results indicated that, as in hemispheres displaying a PCGS, the correlation strength between Area 24c′ and Area 32′ with the prefrontal areas is significantly higher than with the motor zones as demonstrated by heat-maps and boxplots (Fig. 5b, Area 24c′: df = 7, F = 123.5, p < 2.2e−16; Area 32′: df = 7, F = 110.1, p < 2.2e−16, ANOVA; source data are provided as Supplementary data 2).
We then performed multiple linear regressions on the correlation values with seed identity, seed location, and ROIline as predictors. A significant negative linear trend (slope) was observed for both seeds (stronger correlation with rostral prefrontal areas) and in both morphology types (presence or absence of a PCGS) (Fig. 5Cc. These negative slopes were statistically similar for both seeds (Area 24c′ and Area 32′) and for both morphologies (presence or absence of a PCGS) (interaction between seed identity, seed location, and ROIline, F = 3.03, p = 0.08, ns, 3-ways ANOVA).
Thus, the connectivity profiles of areas 24c′ and 32′ with the lateral frontal cortex regions follow the same pattern in hemispheres with a PCGS and those hemispheres that do not display a PCGS: both areas 24c′ and 32′ display equally stronger functional coupling with the lateral prefrontal cortex and weaker with the motor cortex.
Discussion
The present study demonstrates that the PCGS in the common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), as previously identified by morphological examination13, does correspond to the PCGS observed in the human (Homo sapiens sapiens)10,11, both cytoarchitectonically and in term of functional connectivity.
When a PCGS is absent, the ventral and dorsal banks of the CGS are, respectively, occupied by areas 24c′ and 32′; when a PCGS is present, we observed an expansion of area 32′ on the medial wall above the CGS up to the fundus of the PCGS. Importantly, the functional connectivity of both areas 24c′ and 32′ with the lateral frontal cortex is similarly organized to that in the human brain31: both areas display equally stronger connectivity with rostral prefrontal areas than with caudal motor areas. Importantly, this gradient of functional connectivity can be observed despite the positioning of each ROI on the basis of the sulcal morphological organization in each chimpanzee brain (see “Methods” and Supplementary Fig. 1). Specifically, the positioning of each ROI was based on (1) the fMRI literature on the human brain concerning the precise relationship between the local sulcal organization and functional activity in the regions of interest37–41, and (2) studies revealing that the sulcal organization between human and chimpanzee is well preserved13,37,42,43. We chose this methodology because, for both ethical and methodological considerations, there is no study assessing the direct relationships between local sulcal morphology and functional activity in behaving chimpanzees. The present study strongly suggests that, in addition to preservation of the sulcal and cytoarchitectonic organization from the chimpanzee to the human brain, the sulcal-functional organization is also preserved. This is of importance because it indicates that the understanding of the sulcal organization in great apes may allow us to infer the functional organization of the brain in chimpanzees.
Based on the morphological sulcal organization of the medial prefrontal cortex13 and on the present study, three differences can be identified between the human and the chimpanzee cingulate organization: (1) the PCGS is present in fewer hemispheres in the chimpanzee (33.8% of chimpanzees display a PCGS at least in one hemisphere, compared to about 70% of humans); (2) the PCGS is more frequently observed in the left hemisphere than in the right hemisphere in the human but not in the chimpanzee brains; (3) the PCGS has a more caudal distribution in chimpanzee than in human brains, implying that the PCGS is more commonly found in the MCC compared to the ACC in chimpanzees.
The anatomo-functional organization of the cingulate cortex has received considerable attention. Along the rostral-caudal axis, several anatomical and functional subdivisions have been identified15,44–50. Similarly, differences between the cingulate gyrus and the CGS have been demonstrated44,51–53. One could identify similar functional properties of neurons in the ventral and dorsal banks of the CGS. For instance, neurons sensitive to distance to reward in a reward-guided sequential task have been recorded in both banks of the CGS54,55. However, very few studies have directly investigated what might be the respective roles of areas 24c′ and 32′ that occupy the banks of the CGS. In a rare study investigating the functional properties of the dorsal and ventral banks of the CGS in macaques, major differences were reported56. Only neurons in the dorsal bank were modulated by the oculomotor saccade direction and, in addition, neurons in the dorsal bank were most active prior to the choice, while neurons in the ventral bank were most active at the outcome phase.
Sensorimotor properties of the human cingulate cortex have been well characterized57. The aMCC contains a cingulate motor area (the anterior Rostral Cingulate Zone, RCZa) that is somatotopically organized. Whereas the face motor representations (mouth and eye) are located in the PCGS when present and in the CGS when the PCGS is absent (and, therefore, putatively in area 32′), the limb motor representations (hand and foot) are located in the CGS regardless of the presence or absence of the PCGS (and thus putatively in area 24c′)31,32,37,50. In exploratory situations in which the learning is driven by behavioral feedback, the analysis of visual, gustatory, and auditory feedback recruits a region located in the PCGS when present and in the CGS when the PCGS is absent37,38,40, a region that is co-localized with the face motor area of RCZa37,50. Altogether, these data led us to hypothesize that the role of the aMCC may be to perform an embodied analysis of feedback in exploratory situations, i.e. juice/visual/voice feedback recruit the face motor area of RCZa, whereas somatosensory feedback on the hand recruits the hand motor area of RCZa37,50. Within this framework, areas 32′ and 24c′ might support effector specific comparable feedback-related functional processes in exploratory situations. In rhesus macaques, recordings from the ventral and dorsal banks of the CGS have highlighted the role of both structures in reward processing and behavioral adaptation54–56,58. Given that both the cytoarchitectonic and the functional connectivity organization of the aMCC is comparable in macaque, chimpanzee and human brains, it is reasonable to hypothesize that the role of the aMCC in higher cognitive processing may also be preserved. However, in hemispheres displaying a PCGS, area 24c′ occupies about half of the dorsal bank of the CGS in human brains10,44, but it occupies only the fundus of the CGS and the most lateral part of the dorsal bank of the CGS in chimpanzee. One should be cautious in interpreting this difference as the boundaries between cingulate cortical areas 24c′ and 32′ in human brains are not consistent in the literature59.
Unlike the human brain11,12,24,60, we did not observe at the population level a left/right PCGS asymmetry in the chimpanzee. Although brain asymmetry is not a specifically human trait, its origins in a population have yet to be determined61. One hypothesis is that behavioral and brain asymmetry observed in primates might be related to the gradual evolution of language62–64. At the individual subject level, future studies should aim at investigating a putative link between PCGS morphology and cognitive abilities in chimpanzees, as has been done in humans12,65. Finally, another difference between human and chimpanzee brains is the prevalence of the PCGS in the ACC. While the PCGS is present in MCC in both chimpanzees and humans, the PCGS is more often observed in ACC in human brains. This finding suggests that differential evolutionary pressures impacted the ACC. This is a surprising result as some studies showed that the hotspot of cortical expansion in the primate medial frontal cortex may be located in the MCC rather than in the ACC66,67. Other studies, however, showed that the ACC presents high structural variability across subjects in several primate species, contrasting with the MCC which presents less variability13,14,68. The latter studies suggest that the ACC underwent greater expansion than the MCC during primate evolution. This expansion is however associated with a preserved cytoarchitectonic organization of the areas composing the ACC and adjacent ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)30,35,47.
Within the ACC, resting-state analyses have shown that area 24c has stronger coupling with the anterior insula, the striatum and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, while perigenual area 32 has stronger coupling with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the amygdala, and the hippocampus33. The ACC in the human brain has been associated with value-based computations in economic and social domains69–74. Similar properties have been identified in the non-human primate brain52,75,76. Implicit mentalizing abilities have been described in chimpanzees and macaques77,78, and are affected by reversible lesions of the ACC in macaques78. Recursive thinking and counterfactual manipulation of information to guide behavior have also been observed in macaques75,79. Counterfactual reasoning was also impacted by reversible lesions of the ACC in macaques75. However, human subjects have been shown to understand more complex relationships between social agents and their intentions than non-human primates70,80. The more complex the information about intentionality of social agents a human subject can comprehend, the larger the gray matter volume in the ACC and vmPFC81. Altogether, these results suggest that the building blocks of a human ACC have been present since the last common ancestor to human and macaques, but its evolution might reflect the development of recursive thinking in hominids82.
To conclude, using multimodal data, the present study demonstrates that chimpanzee brains do possess a PCGS in the MCC that is comparable to that in the human brain in terms of cytoarchitecture and functional connectivity. The similarities between ACC and MCC in primates and rodents are still a matter of debate7,83.
Methods
Subjects/specimens
Human subjects
The first step in this investigation was a reanalysis of the morphological organization of the PCGS in 197 human brains13 to refine our previous analysis by assessing the location and extent of the PCGS in the ACC and/or the MCC. High-resolution anatomical scans of these brains were obtained from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) database [http://www.humanconnectome.org/]. Only data from subjects with no family relationships were analyzed. The participants in the HCP study were recruited from the Missouri Family and Twin Registry that includes individuals born in Missouri84. Acquisition parameters of T1 anatomical scans are the following: whole head, 0.7 mm3 isotropic resolution, TR = 2.4 s, TE = 2.14 ms, flip angle = 8° (more details can be found at [https://humanconnectome.org/storage/app/media/documentation/s1200/HCP_S1200_Release_Appendix_I.pdf]). The full set of inclusion and exclusion criteria is detailed elsewhere. Briefly, the HCP subjects are healthy individuals free from major psychiatric or neurological illnesses. They are drawn from ongoing longitudinal studies84, in which they had received extensive assessments, including the history of drug use, and emotional and behavioral problems. The experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations and all experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) (IRB #201204036; Title: ‘Mapping the Human Connectome: Structure, Function, and Heritability’). All subjects provided written informed consent on forms approved by the Institutional Review Board of Washington University in St Louis. In addition, the present study received approval (n°15-213) from the Ethics Committee of Inserm (IORG0003254, FWA00005831) and from the Institutional Review Board (IRB00003888) of the French Institute of Medical Research and Health.
Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)
Three chimpanzee groups were examined in (1) the morphological sulcal organization of the medial frontal cortex (Group 1, N = 225), (2) in the cytoarchitectonic analysis of the MCC (Group 2, N = 3), and (3) in the functional connectivity analysis of the MCC (Group 3, N = 4, note that these four chimpanzees were also part of group 1).
Specifically, the morphological analysis aimed at refining our previous analysis on 225 chimpanzees13 by assessing the location and extent of the PCGS in the ACC and/or MCC. In the cytoarchitectonic analysis, we examined three post-mortem male chimpanzee brains that died from natural causes (CHIMP_1, CHIMP_2, and CHIMP_3; ages at death 38, 33, and 37 years, respectively). Within 14 h of each subject’s death, the brain was removed and immersed in 10% formalin at necropsy. In the in vivo resting-state fMRI study, we analyzed the data obtained in four chimpanzees: three females (CHIMP_A, CHIMP_B, CHIMP_C, respectively 16, 17, and 27 years of age at the time of the experiment) and one male (CHIMP_D, 15 years of age). All four chimpanzees were captive born and were members of the colony of apes housed at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center of Emory University.
The chimpanzees were all born in captivity and had all lived in social groups ranging from 2 to 13 individuals at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center and were housed according to institutional guidelines. Chimpanzee data collection was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at YNPRC and UTMDACC and followed the guidelines of the Institute of Medicine in the use of chimpanzees in research. We note here that all in vivo MRI scans were obtained prior to changes in NIH policy on the acquisition of neuroimaging data from chimpanzees (Nov 2015).
MRI data acquisition
Structural MRI of post-mortem chimpanzee brains
Each post-mortem chimpanzee brain was scanned overnight in a 3 T Siemens Prisma MRI scanner to obtain structural T1 volumetric images (repetition time = 23 ms, echo time = 5.65 ms, voxel resolution = 0.4 × 0.4 × 0.4 mm). Each brain was scanned in a container filled with 10% formalin and supported with padding to prevent scanning artifacts from occurring near the edges of the container. 24, 4, and 36 repetitions of T1 scans of respectively CHIMP_1, CHIMP_2, and CHIMP_3 were obtained and averaged.
In vivo rs-fMRI acquisition in chimpanzee
In vivo resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data came from Dr Hopkins’ laboratory. These data were acquired in early 2015 at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC) on the four adult chimpanzees at the time they were being surveyed for their annual physical examinations. Subjects were first immobilized by ketamine (10 mg/kg) or telazol (3–5 mg/kg) and subsequently anaesthetized with propofol (40–60 mg/[kg/h]) following standard procedures at the YNPRC. The subjects remained anaesthetized for the duration of the scans as well as the time needed to transport them between their home cage and the imaging facility (between 5 and 10 min). Chimpanzees were placed in the scanner chamber in a supine position with their head fitted inside the human-head coil.
They were scanned using a 3.0-T scanner (Siemens Trio; Siemens Medical Solutions USA, Inc., Malvern, PA, USA). T1-weighted images were collected using a three-dimensional gradient-echo sequence (repetition time = 2300 ms, echo time = 4.4 ms, number of signals averaged = 2, voxel resolution = 0.625 × 0.625 × 0.60 mm. In addition, 2 runs of 350 measurements each (16 min/session) of rs-fMRI scans were collected using a three-dimensional gradient-echo sequence (repetition time = 2683 ms, echo time = 25 ms, voxel resolution = 1.9 × 1.9 × 1.9 mm, right-left phase-encoding direction). An additional short run was performed with the same characteristics except that the phase-encoding was in the opposite direction (left-right). Scan duration was about 90 min.
After completing MRI procedures, the subjects were temporarily housed in a single enclosure for 6–12 h to allow the effects of the anesthesia to wear off before being returned to their social group.
Sulcal morphology of the medial frontal cortex
On the basis of structural T1 MRI scans of each hemisphere of all chimpanzee and human brains, we established the presence or absence of a PCGS in each cerebral hemisphere. A PCGS was marked as present if running parallel and dorsal to the CGS12,13. We then examined whether this PCGS was located within the ACC, within the MCC, or extending along both regions. Note that the ACC-MCC limit was based on Vogt’s four-region model (Fig. 1)15,45,85,86.
Morphological analysis: occurrence and location of PCGS in human and chimpanzee
To establish the probability of occurrence of a PCGS in the MCC and the ACC in human and chimpanzee brains, we first reanalyzed the neuroimaging T1 anatomical data of 197 human brains and 225 chimpanzee brains from our previous study13. From this inspection, we identified 76 chimpanzees displaying a PCGS at least in one hemisphere (15, 29, and 32 displaying a PCGS in both hemispheres, only in the left hemisphere, and only in the right hemispheres, respectively), for a total of 91 hemispheres displaying a PCGS. We also identified 139 human brains displaying a PCGS at least in one hemisphere (45, 69, and 25 displaying a PCGS in both hemispheres, only in the left hemisphere, and only in the right hemispheres, respectively), for a total of 184 hemispheres displaying a PCGS. In these hemispheres, we then identified whether the PCGS was present in the ACC, the MCC, or in both the ACC and MCC. The limit between the ACC and the MCC was identified using the probabilistic cytoarchitectonic map of the ACC from the JuBrain atlas (https://jubrain.fz-juelich.de/apps/cytoviewer2/cytoviewer-main.php#)33.
Cytoarchitectonic analysis: occurrence and location of PCGS in chimpanzee
From the morphological inspection of the three chimpanzee brains included in this analysis, we selected four hemispheres for the cytoarchitectonic analysis:
The left hemisphere of CHIMP_1, in which the PCGS is absent.
The right hemisphere of CHIMP_1, which displays a PCGS.
The left hemisphere of CHIMP_2, where a PCGS is present in the anterior-most part of the aMCC, but absent in the posterior part of the aMCC.
The right hemisphere of CHIMP_3, in which the PCGS is absent.
Note that the remaining hemispheres (left hemisphere of CHIMP_3 and right hemisphere of CHIMP_2) displayed no PCGS.
Rs-fMRI analysis: occurrence and location of PCGS in chimpanzee
We identified three hemispheres with a PCGS in the four chimpanzees included in this analysis:
Both the left and right hemispheres of CHIMP_C, and the left hemisphere of CHIMP_A, displayed a PCGS.
The right hemisphere of CHIMP_A, and both hemispheres of CHIMP_D and CHIMP_B did not display a PCGS.
Blocking and histological processing
Based on the averaged structural MRI data of the chimpanzee brains and using a neuronavigation system (Brainsight), each selected brain was cut in several blocks to obtain histological sections optimal for the study of the architecture of the regions of interest (see Novek et al.87 for the method used). The blocks including the MCC were optimized to allow for histological sections that were perpendicular to the orientation of both the cingulate and/or paracingulate sulci. Note that the remaining blocks were processed for ongoing studies aiming to assess cytoarchitectonic areas of various regions of the chimpanzee cortex. All blocks were cryoprotected by immersion in buffered sucrose solutions from 10 to 30% until they sank, frozen to −60 ˚C in a bath of 2-methylbutane chilled by a surrounding mixture of dry ice and ETOH, then stored at −80 ˚C until use, where the block was kept surrounded by dry ice on a frozen microtome stage during sectioning. The histological sections were cut at a thickness of 30 μm, three out of every ten sections were kept, and a photograph was taken before each set of the kept sections throughout the entire blocks to aid with 3D reconstructions.
Cytoarchitectonic analysis
The detailed cytoarchitectonic analysis of the mid-cingulate cortex of the selected blocks was carried out from the sections that were cut in a coronal orientation. Every tenth section was mounted on 2” × 3” coated slides and stained with cresyl violet, a Nissl cell body stain, for cytoarchitectonic analysis; the remaining sections are being used in ongoing studies.
The architectonic organization of the MCC and, more specifically, the boundaries between the cytoarchitectonic areas composing the MCC, were identified on high-resolution tilted images of the entire region of interest from the cresyl violet sections, obtained under bright field with a Zeiss Axio Scan.Z1 at ×10 magnification.
The assessment of the architectonic organization of the MCC was based on prior studies of this region in both the human10 and in the macaque monkey30,88 brains. Proceeding from the corpus callosum in a dorsal direction towards the lateral surface of the frontal lobe, the following areas were identified in the cingulate region: area 24a′, area 24b′, area 24c′, and area 32′. Note that, dorsal to these cingulate cortical areas, the medial frontal areas 6. 8, and 9 are located. Area 24a′ is an agranular area located on the gyrus just dorsal to the corpus callosum. It is characterized by a clear layer II, a thick layer III, a dense layer V, and a poorly defined layer VI. Area 24b’ is an agranular area located dorsal to area 24a′ and ends where the CGS starts. It is characterized by a broad layer III containing distinct layers IIIa, b, and c, as well as a highly prominent layer V composed of large pyramidal neurons. Area 24c′ is an agranular area located in the ventral bank of the CGS and is characterized by thin layers II and III, as well as the presence of more densely packed large pyramidal neurons in layer V in comparison with area 24b’. Finally, area 32′ is a dysgranular cingulo-frontal transitional area located dorsal to area 24c′ in the dorsal bank of the CGS when there is no PCGS in the human brain and located within the PCGS if present10. In the macaque, area 32′ (labeled as 32/6 or 32/8 in macaque) is located in the dorsal bank of the CGS 30,88. It displays a wide layer IIIc containing large pyramidal neurons, a dysgranular layer IV, and a thinner and less dense layer V than in area 24c′.
Rs-fMRI data analysis
Data were collected using 2 phase-encoding directions (2 full runs in Right-Left, and a shorter 3d run in Left-Right directions). It resulted in two pairs of images with distortions going in opposite directions (pair 1: 1st run in right-left and 3d run in left-right direction; pair 2: 2d run in right-left and 3d run in left-right direction). Distortions were corrected using TOPUP’s FSL tool. First, TOPUP estimated from these pairs the susceptibility-induced off-resonance field using a method similar to that described by Andersson et al.89 as implemented in FSL90. Once estimated, the images were then combined into corrected ones. Two runs, corrected for distortions, resulted from this analysis and were further preprocessed.
The preprocessing of resting-state scans was then performed with SPM 12. The first 5 volumes of each run were removed to allow for T1 equilibrium effects. The head motion correction was applied using rigid body realignment and we then applied a slice timing correction using the time center of the volume as reference. Then, using the AFNI software91, the segmentation of each brain was performed on skull-stripped brains. A temporal filtering was then applied to extract the spontaneous slowly fluctuating brain activity (0.01–0.1 Hz). Finally, linear regression was used to remove nuisance variables (the cerebrospinal fluid and white matter signals from the segmentation) and spatial smoothing with a 4-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel was applied to the output of the regression.
Seed selection in the MCC
The seeds consisted of 2.5-mm radius spheres and were positioned in the aMCC at the same antero-posterior level as in the cytoarchitectonic study as follows:
In hemispheres displaying no PCGS (N = 5): a seed assigned to area 24c′ was positioned in the ventral bank of the CGS and a seed assigned to area 32′ was positioned in the dorsal bank of the CGS.
In hemispheres displaying a PCGS (N = 3): a seed assigned to area 24c′ was positioned in the CGS and a seed for area 32′ was positioned in the ventral bank of the PCGS.
In both cases, the two seeds were positioned on an imaginary line going through the posterior limit of the genu of the corpus callosum and perpendicular to the axis on which the CGS and PCGS are running (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for positioning of seeds in all chimpanzees and hemispheres). In hemispheres displaying no PCGS, the two seeds (area 24c′ and area 32′) displayed a small overlap. This overlap was removed before performing the ROI-based resting-state data analysis.
Selection of regions of interest (ROIs)
For a stricter comparison of the present results with results obtained in our previous study which assessed the functional connectivity of the CGS and PCGS in the anterior part of the human MCC31, we used the same ROIs (see below and Supplementary Fig. 1 for positioning of ROIs in all chimpanzees and hemispheres). Each ROI consisted of a sphere with a 5-mm radius.
ROIs selection in motor cortical areas
For each subject, 3 ROIs within the motor cortex of both hemispheres were identified based on sulcal morphology. These included the hand motor region (the precentral knob) within the central sulcus –M1Hand–43 and the primary face motor region within the ventral part of the posterior part of the precentral gyrus –M1Face–92. We also included the frontal eye field –FEF–. In the human brain, this region is located within the ventral branch of the superior precentral sulcus39. As the chimpanzee presents the same sulcal pattern in this region, including a ventral branch of the superior precentral sulcus, we tentatively included this ROI in our analysis.
Selection of ROIs in the prefrontal cortex
For each subject, 6 ROI locations within the left prefrontal cortex were identified based on the local anatomy. On a rostro-caudal axis:
The frontopolar cortex –Area 10–. In the human brain, this region is located at the intersection between the vertical segment of the intermediate frontal sulcus, the lateral and the medial frontomarginal sulcus, see41. Because chimpanzee brains display a similar sulcal pattern, we hypothesized that Area 10 most likely lies at the same location.
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex -DLPFC-. In the human brain, area 9/46 of the DLPFC lies at the rostral level of the genu of the corpus callosum, above the inferior frontal sulcus. Because the chimpanzee displays also an inferior frontal sulcus that is also present at the level of the rostral level of the genu of the corpus callosum, we hypothesized that area 9/46 lies at the same location.
Broca’s region: The two cytoarchitectonic areas that comprise Broca’s region, namely area 44 and area 45, have been shown to be located in the chimpanzee brain between the inferior precentral sulcus and the fronto-orbital sulcus, and anterior to the fronto-orbital sulcus42,93.
The frontal operculum –Fo– (intersection between the frontal operculum and the circular sulcus, see ref. 38.
Statistics and reproducibility
The mean signal from Seeds and ROI regions was then extracted using AFNI software. For each chimpanzee brain, correlation coefficients between the two seeds with the various ROIs in the prefrontal cortex and the motor cortex were computed and normalized using the Fisher’s r-to-z transform formula. The significant threshold at the individual subject level was Z = 0.1 (p < 0.05). These normalized correlation coefficients, which corresponded to the functional connectivity strength between each seed and each ROI in individual chimpanzee brains, were subsequently processed with R statistical software (https://www.r-project.org/) for all the following analyses.
To compare the connectivity profile of each seed with the various lateral frontal ROIs, we constructed boxplots corresponding to the correlation strength of each seed location with each of the ROIs. Based on these boxplots, it can be discerned that both Area 24c′ and Area 32′ seeds have stronger connectivity with prefrontal regions and weaker connectivity with premotor and motor areas (see “Results”). We then characterized this rostro-caudal functional axis based on the correlation profiles of Area 24c′ and Area 32′, when a PCGS is present and also when it is not present, with the lateral frontal cortex by estimating linear trends in the correlation strength for each seed with the rostro-caudal lateral frontal ROIs (for details, see “Methods“ in Loh et al.31). The 8 ROIs were first ranked along a rostro-caudal axis based on their average Y coordinate values across chimpanzee brains and recoded into a numeric axis variable (ROIline): Area 10 (most anterior)-1, DLPFC-2, Area 45-3, Area 44-4, Fo-5, FEF-6, M1Face-7, M1Hand (most posterior)-8. We then performed multiple linear regressions on the correlation z values with seed identity (area 24c′ and area 32′), sulcal morphology (PCGS absent or present), and the linear axis variable (ROIline) as predictors. We assessed whether the linear trends (slopes) observed for each seed were identical or not in each sulcal morphology using a 3-ways ANOVA.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Human Frontier Science Program (RGP0044/2014). C.A. received funding from the French National Research Agency (ANR-18-CE37-0012-01). J.S. was supported by a Sir Henry Dale Wellcome Trust Fellowship (105651/Z/14/Z) and IDEXLYON “IMPULSION” 2020 grant (IDEX/IMP/2020/14). The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z). C.A. and J.S. were also supported by Laboratoire d’excellence (LabEx) CORTEX ANR-11-LABX-0042 of Université de Lyon. W.D.H. is supported by NIH grants NS-042867, NS-073134, and NS-092988. M.P. was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Foundation grant FDN-143212. C.A. is employed by the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique. We thank Sarah Lefebvre, Kristina Drudik, and Ilana Ruth Leppert for technical assistance in histology and scanning the tissue in the MRI. We also thank Franck Lamberton for help with analysis of rs-fMRI data obtained in chimpanzee.
Author contributions
C.A. organized the project and coordinated the consortium to gather the dataset. C.A., J.S., and M.P. interpreted data and wrote the article. J.N. and M.P. acquired post-mortem structural MRI scans in chimpanzees. C.A., J.S., J.N., and M.P. performed the blocking and histological processing of post-mortem chimpanzee brains. C.A., J.S., F.H.-B., C.G., and J.A. performed the analysis of chimpanzee rs-fMRI data. W.D.H. provided in vivo chimpanzee T1 and rs-fMRI data.
Data availability
The source data underlying Figs. 1 and 5 are provided as Source Data file (respectively Supplementary Data 1 and Supplementary Data 2). Human anatomical MRI scans are available from the Human Connectome Project database [http://www.humanconnectome.org/]94, and Chimpanzee anatomical MRI and rs-fMRI scans are available from Dr. W. Hopkins [http://www.chimpanzeebrain.org/]95. A reporting summary for this Article is available as a Supplementary Information file.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Céline Amiez, Jérôme Sallet.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s42003-020-01571-3.
References
- 1.Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. Why are there so many explanations for primate brain evolution? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2017;372:20160244. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Barton RA, Montgomery SH. Proportional versus relative size as metrics in human brain evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2019;116:3–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1817200116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.DeCasien AR, Williams SA, Higham JP. Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2017;1:112. doi: 10.1038/s41559-017-0112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mars RB, et al. Primate comparative neuroscience using magnetic resonance imaging: promises and challenges. Front. Neurosci. 2014;8:298. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.DeCasien AR, Higham JP. Primate mosaic brain evolution reflects selection on sensory and cognitive specialization. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2019;3:1483–1493. doi: 10.1038/s41559-019-0969-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sallet J, et al. The organization of dorsal frontal cortex in humans and macaques. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:12255–12274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5108-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Schaeffer DJ, et al. Divergence of rodent and primate medial frontal cortex functional connectivity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:21681–21689. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2003181117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schrago CG, Russo CAM. Timing the origin of New World monkeys. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2003;20:1620–1625. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msg172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Passingham R. How good is the macaque monkey model of the human brain? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2009;19:6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Vogt BA, Nimchinsky EA, Vogt LJ, Hof PR. Human cingulate cortex: surface features, flat maps, and cytoarchitecture. J. Comp. Neurol. 1995;359:490–506. doi: 10.1002/cne.903590310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Paus T, et al. Human cingulate and paracingulate sulci: pattern, variability, asymmetry, and probabilistic map. Cereb. Cortex. 1996;6:207–214. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Amiez C, Wilson CRE, Procyk E. Variations of cingulate sulcal organization and link with cognitive performance. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:13988. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32088-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Amiez C, et al. Sulcal organization in the medial frontal cortex provides insights into primate brain evolution. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:3437. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11347-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lopez-Persem A, Verhagen L, Amiez C, Petrides M, Sallet J. The human ventromedial prefrontal cortex: sulcal morphology and its influence on functional organization. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:3627–3639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2060-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Vogt BA, Berger GR, Derbyshire SWG. Structural and functional dichotomy of human midcingulate cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003;18:3134–3144. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03034.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mériau K, et al. A neural network reflecting individual differences in cognitive processing of emotions during perceptual decision making. Neuroimage. 2006;33:1016–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Whittle S, et al. Variations in cortical folding patterns are related to individual differences in temperament. Psychiatry Res. 2009;172:68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kanai R, Rees G. The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;12:231–242. doi: 10.1038/nrn3000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Yücel M, et al. Paracingulate morphologic differences in males with established schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging morphometric study. Biol. Psychiatry. 2002;52:15–23. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01312-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Yücel M, et al. Anterior cingulate dysfunction: implications for psychiatric disorders? J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2003;28:350–354. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Le Provost J-B, et al. Paracingulate sulcus morphology in men with early-onset schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2003;182:228–232. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.3.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shim G, et al. Reduced cortical folding of the anterior cingulate cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:443–449. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Meredith SM, et al. Anterior cingulate morphology in people at genetic high-risk of schizophrenia. Eur. Psychiatry. 2012;27:377–385. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Fornito A, et al. Individual differences in anterior cingulate/paracingulate morphology are related to executive functions in healthy males. Cereb. Cortex. 2004;14:424–431. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Buda M, Fornito A, Bergström ZM, Simons JS. A specific brain structural basis for individual differences in reality monitoring. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:14308–14313. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3595-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Borst G, et al. Folding of the anterior cingulate cortex partially explains inhibitory control during childhood: a longitudinal study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;9:126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cachia A, et al. Longitudinal stability of the folding pattern of the anterior cingulate cortex during development. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;19:122–127. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cole MW, Yeung N, Freiwald WA, Botvinick M. Cingulate cortex: diverging data from humans and monkeys. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:566–574. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Vogt, B. A. Architecture, Neurocytology and Comparative Organization of Monkey and Human Cingulate Cortices. In Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. (ed. Vogt, B. A.) 65–93 (Oxford University Press, 2009).
- 30.Petrides, M. & Pandya, D. N. Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, (eds. Boller, F. & Grafman, J.), 9, 17–58 (Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1994).
- 31.Loh, K. K., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Petrides, M., Procyk, E. & Amiez, C. Rostro-caudal organization of motor areas and lateral frontal regions. Front. Neurosci. 11, 753 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 32.Amiez C, Petrides M. Neuroimaging evidence of the anatomo-functional organization of the human cingulate motor areas. Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:563–578. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Palomero-Gallagher N, et al. Human pregenual anterior cingulate cortex: structural, functional, and connectional heterogeneity. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:2552–2574. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mackey S, Petrides M. Architecture and morphology of the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2014;40:2777–2796. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mackey S, Petrides M. Quantitative demonstration of comparable architectonic areas within the ventromedial and lateral orbital frontal cortex in the human and the macaque monkey brains. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2010;32:1940–1950. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07465.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Vogt, B. A. Regions and Subregions of the Cingulate Cortex. In Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. (ed. Vogt, B. A.) 3–30 (Oxford University Press, 2009).
- 37.Loh KK, et al. Cognitive control of orofacial motor and vocal responses in the ventrolateral and dorsomedial human frontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:4994–5005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1916459117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Amiez C, et al. The location of feedback-related activity in the midcingulate cortex is predicted by local morphology. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:2217–2228. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2779-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Amiez C, Kostopoulos P, Champod A-S, Petrides M. Local morphology predicts functional organization of the dorsal premotor region in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:2724–2731. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4739-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Amiez C, et al. Single subject analyses reveal consistent recruitment of frontal operculum in performance monitoring. Neuroimage. 2016;133:266–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Petrides, M. The Human Cerebral Cortex.An MRI Atlas of the Sulci and Gyri in MNI Stereotaxic Space. (Academic Press, New York, 2012).
- 42.Schenker NM, et al. Broca’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): probabilistic mapping, asymmetry, and comparison to humans. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:730–742. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hopkins WD, et al. Evolution of the central sulcus morphology in primates. Brain Behav. Evol. 2014;84:19–30. doi: 10.1159/000362431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Palomero-Gallagher N, Mohlberg H, Zilles K, Vogt B. Cytology and receptor architecture of human anterior cingulate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 2008;508:906–926. doi: 10.1002/cne.21684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Palomero-Gallagher N, Vogt BA, Schleicher A, Mayberg HS, Zilles K. Receptor architecture of human cingulate cortex: evaluation of the four-region neurobiological model. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2336–2355. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Alexander WH, Brown JW. Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1338–1344. doi: 10.1038/nn.2921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Neubert F-X, Mars RB, Sallet J, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity reveals relationship of brain areas for reward-guided learning and decision making in human and monkey frontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E2695–E2704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1410767112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Heilbronner SR, Hayden BY. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a bottom-up view. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;39:149–170. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kolling N, et al. Value, search, persistence and model updating in anterior cingulate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1280–1285. doi: 10.1038/nn.4382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Procyk E, et al. Midcingulate motor map and feedback detection: converging data from humans and monkeys. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26:467–476. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rudebeck PH, Buckley MJ, Walton ME, Rushworth MFS. A role for the macaque anterior cingulate gyrus in social valuation. Science. 2006;313:1310–1312. doi: 10.1126/science.1128197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Chang SWC, Gariépy J-F, Platt ML. Neuronal reference frames for social decisions in primate frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:243–250. doi: 10.1038/nn.3287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Apps MAJ, Rushworth MFS, Chang SWC. The anterior cingulate gyrus and social cognition: tracking the motivation of others. Neuron. 2016;90:692–707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Procyk E, Tanaka YL, Joseph JP. Anterior cingulate activity during routine and non-routine sequential behaviors in macaques. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:502–508. doi: 10.1038/74880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Shidara M, Richmond BJ. Anterior cingulate: single neuronal signals related to degree of reward expectancy. Science. 2002;296:1709–1711. doi: 10.1126/science.1069504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cai X, Padoa-Schioppa C. Neuronal encoding of subjective value in dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate cortex. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:3791–3808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3864-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Picard N, Strick PL. Imaging the premotor areas. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2001;11:663–672. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(01)00266-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Amiez C, Joseph JP, Procyk E. Reward encoding in the monkey anterior cingulate cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2006;16:1040–1055. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ding S-L, et al. Comprehensive cellular-resolution atlas of the adult human brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 2016;524:3127–3481. doi: 10.1002/cne.24080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wei X, et al. Paracingulate sulcus asymmetry in the human brain: effects of sex, handedness, and race. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:42033. doi: 10.1038/srep42033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Vallortigara G, Chiandetti C, Sovrano VA. Brain asymmetry (animal) Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2011;2:146–157. doi: 10.1002/wcs.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Gentilucci M, Corballis MC. From manual gesture to speech: a gradual transition. Neurosci. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30:949–960. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Iturria-Medina Y, et al. Brain hemispheric structural efficiency and interconnectivity rightward asymmetry in human and nonhuman primates. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21:56–67. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Meguerditchian A, Vauclair J. Baboons communicate with their right hand. Behav. Brain Res. 2006;171:170–174. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Del Maschio N, et al. ACC Sulcal patterns and their modulation on cognitive control efficiency across lifespan: a neuroanatomical study on bilinguals and monolinguals. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:3091–3101. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sneve MH, et al. High-expanding regions in primate cortical brain evolution support supramodal cognitive flexibility. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:3891–3901. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Chaplin TA, Yu H-H, Soares JGM, Gattass R, Rosa MGP. A conserved pattern of differential expansion of cortical areas in simian primates. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:15120–15125. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2909-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Croxson PL, Forkel SJ, Cerliani L, Thiebaut de Schotten M. Structural variability across the primate brain: a cross-species comparison. Cereb. Cortex. 2018;28:3829–3841. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Coricelli G, Nagel R. Neural correlates of depth of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:9163–9168. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807721106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Nicolle A, et al. An agent independent axis for executed and modeled choice in medial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2012;75:1114–1121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Bartra O, McGuire JT, Kable JW. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage. 2013;76:412–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Vassena E, Krebs RM, Silvetti M, Fias W, Verguts T. Dissociating contributions of ACC and vmPFC in reward prediction, outcome, and choice. Neuropsychologia. 2014;59:112–123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Juechems K, et al. A network for computing value equilibrium in the human medial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2019;101:977–987.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.12.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Fouragnan EF, et al. The macaque anterior cingulate cortex translates counterfactual choice value into actual behavioral change. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;22:797–808. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0375-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hunt LT, et al. Triple dissociation of attention and decision computations across prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:1471–1481. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0239-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Krupenye C, Kano F, Hirata S, Call J, Tomasello M. Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science. 2016;354:110–114. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf8110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Hayashi T, et al. Macaques exhibit implicit gaze bias anticipating others’ false-belief-driven actions via medial prefrontal cortex. Cell. Rep. 2020;30:4433–4444.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ferrigno S, Cheyette SJ, Piantadosi ST, Cantlon JF. Recursive sequence generation in monkeys, children, U.S. adults, and native Amazonians. Sci. Adv. 2020;6:eaaz1002. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz1002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Powell JL, Lewis PA, Dunbar RIM, García-Fiñana M, Roberts N. Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3554–3562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Lewis PA, Rezaie R, Brown R, Roberts N, Dunbar RIM. Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size. NeuroImage. 2011;57:1624–1629. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Corballis, M. C. The Recursive Mind: the origins of human language, thought, and civilization. (Princeton University Press, 2011).
- 83.van Heukelum S, et al. Where is cingulate cortex? A cross-species view. Trends Neurosci. 2020;43:285–299. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Van Essen DC, et al. The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage. 2012;62:2222–2231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Vogt BA, Vogt L. Cytology of human dorsal midcingulate and supplementary motor cortices. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2003;26:301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Vogt BA, Vogt L, Laureys S. Cytology and functionally correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas. Neuroimage. 2006;29:452–466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Novek, J., Sprung-Much, T., Nolan, E., Frey, S. & Petrides, M. Optimal cutting of the cortex for cytoarchitectonic visualization: a neuronavigation-based approach. in vol. Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM), Abstract M854 (2019).
- 88.Morecraft RJ, et al. Cytoarchitecture and cortical connections of the anterior cingulate and adjacent somatomotor fields in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. Bull. 2012;87:457–497. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Andersson JLR, Skare S, Ashburner J. How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage. 2003;20:870–888. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Smith SM, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Bailey, P., Von Bonin, G. & McCulloch, W. S. The Isocortex of the Chimpanzee. (University of Illinois Press, 1950).
- 93.Keller SS, Roberts N, Hopkins W. A comparative magnetic resonance imaging study of the anatomy, variability, and asymmetry of Broca’s area in the human and chimpanzee brain. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:14607–14616. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2892-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Human Connectome Project database, http://www.humanconnectome.org/.
- 95.Chimpanzee MRI database, http://www.chimpanzeebrain.org/.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The source data underlying Figs. 1 and 5 are provided as Source Data file (respectively Supplementary Data 1 and Supplementary Data 2). Human anatomical MRI scans are available from the Human Connectome Project database [http://www.humanconnectome.org/]94, and Chimpanzee anatomical MRI and rs-fMRI scans are available from Dr. W. Hopkins [http://www.chimpanzeebrain.org/]95. A reporting summary for this Article is available as a Supplementary Information file.