Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2020 May 16;172:107234. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107234

The effects of adolescent alcohol exposure on learning and related neurobiology in humans and rodents

Laurel R Seemiller 1, Thomas J Gould 1,*
PMCID: PMC7797082  NIHMSID: NIHMS1597317  PMID: 32428585

Abstract

Adolescent alcohol use is a widespread problem in the United States. In both humans and rodents, alcohol can impair learning and memory processes mediated by forebrain areas such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus (HC). Adolescence is a period in which alcohol use often begins, and it is also a time that can be uniquely sensitive to the detrimental effects of alcohol. Exposure to alcohol during adolescence can cause persisting alterations in PFC and HC neurobiology that are linked to cognitive impairments, including changes in neurogenesis, inflammation, and various neurotransmitter systems in rodent models. Consistent with this, chronic adolescent alcohol exposure can cause PFC-dependent learning impairments that persist into adulthood. Deficits in adult HC-dependent learning after adolescent alcohol exposure have also been reported, but these findings are less consistent. Overall, evidence summarized in this review indicates that adolescent exposure to alcohol can produce long-term detrimental effects on forebrain-dependent cognitive processes.

Keywords: Adolescence, alcohol, learning, memory, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus

1. United States adolescent drug use and cognition

Alcohol is the most commonly used drug of abuse among adolescents (Johnston et al., 2018). Twenty-four percent of adolescents use alcohol by the eighth grade, and around sixty percent use alcohol before graduating high school (Johnston et al. 2018). Additionally, adolescents display different patterns of alcohol use than adults. Adolescents generally consume alcohol less frequently, but they usually consume larger amounts of alcohol per occasion than adults (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA], 2011). These episodes of high alcohol intake are referred to as binge drinking episodes. Approximately 1 in 4 people over the age of 11 report binge alcohol use in the past month (SAMHSA, 2018). In 2018, fourteen percent of 12th graders admitted to binge drinking, which the study defined as having five or more drinks in a row on one or more occasions in the previous two weeks (Johnston et al., 2018). Binge drinking can have serious consequences, including increases in traffic accidents, violent behavior, suicide, and increased risk to develop alcohol use disorders (Stolle et al., 2009; Addolorato et al., 2018). Because of the prevalence and extent of adolescent alcohol use, it is critical to study its immediate and long-term consequences.

Adolescence can be defined as a transitional period between childhood and adulthood. It is accompanied by changes in numerous behaviors, some that may be responsible for the transition to independence associated with adulthood. For example, time spent interacting with peers and risk-taking behaviors drastically increase during adolescence (Spear, 2000). Examples of typical adolescent risk-taking behaviors include disobeying parents, school misconduct, antisocial behaviors, and substance use (Maggs et al., 1995). While all of these behaviors can have long-term consequences, adolescent substance use is especially dangerous as it predicts adult addictive disorders (Rohde et al., 2001; Brook et al., 2002). Exposure to drugs of abuse during adolescence can cause other long-lasting behavioral changes. For example, persistent learning and memory deficits can be seen after adolescent drug exposure (Hanson et al., 2011; Portugal et al., 2012; Mooney-Leber & Gould 2018). Drug-induced changes in cognition can have a wide range of consequences, including an increased propensity for drug addiction (for review, see Gould, 2010).

Adolescent risk-taking can be attributed to differential development of subcortical limbic regions and the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Subcortical limbic regions are more developed than the PFC during adolescence (Casey et al., 2008). Limbic development, specifically in the nucleus accumbens, can drive the seeking of novel experiences. The PFC circuitry that normally controls these impulses is underdeveloped at this point and cannot adequately regulate risk-taking in adolescence. As the PFC develops, so does executive control, and this development is thought to gradually reduce risk-taking behavior as adolescents reach adulthood (Casey et al., 2008). Additionally, the PFC and the hippocampus (HC) are important substrates of learning and memory processes (Dias & Aggleton, 2000; Smith & Milner, 1981), and they develop at different rates. The HC and related cognitive processes seem to be mostly developed by mid-childhood or adolescence (Ofen et al., 2007; Ghetti et al., 2010; Ofen et al., 2012; Shing et al., 2016), while the PFC continues developing through late adolescence into adulthood (Giedd et al., 1999; Sowell et al., 1999; Gogtay et al., 2004). Because the PFC and HC are still developing during adolescence, learning processes modulated by these regions may be more vulnerable to detrimental effects of adolescent risk-taking and drug use (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Spear, 2015).

2. Alcohol’s behavioral effects and pharmacology

Alcohol is the most commonly abused recreational drug (SAMHSA, 2015). Its behavioral impacts are wide-ranging, including both rewarding and aversive effects that influence its abuse liability. Alcohol can produce acute stimulating effects such as euphoria and social facilitation (King et al., 2011). Its negative acute effects include nausea, sedation, motor incoordination, and cognitive impairments (Weissenborn & Duka, 2003; Brumback et al., 2007; King et al., 2011; Acheson et al., 1998). Chronic alcohol use is associated with additional, often severe, consequences. Some may become addicted to alcohol and experience withdrawal symptoms such as headaches, nausea, tremors, depression, and irritability during periods of abstinence (Stewart & Brown, 1995). They may also experience lasting changes in mood and impaired learning after periods of alcohol use (Brière et al., 2014; Brandt et al., 1983). However, recovery of psychomotor skills and short-term memory has been seen after prolonged abstinence from long-term alcohol use (Brandt et al., 1983). This suggests that some lasting behavioral consequences of alcohol exposure are reversible.

Alcohol primarily acts as an agonist on γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)A receptors (see Kumar et al., 2009 for review). GABA is the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter of the central nervous system and plays a role in countless important brain processes (McCormick, 1989). Alcohol also antagonizes N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, a subtype of glutamate receptors, that contribute to excitatory neurotransmission (Lovinger et al., 1990). These receptors are involved in synaptic plasticity and learning and memory (Artola & Singer, 1987). In addition to modulating GABAA and NMDA receptor function, alcohol also influences AMPA, kainate, glycine, and nicotinic acetylcholine receptor function (Lovinger, 1993; Weiner et al., 1999; Mihic et al., 1997; Cardoso et al., 1999). AMPA and kainate receptors are important components of glutamatergic signaling (for review, see Bettler & Mulle, 1995), and glycine is important in inhibitory neurotransmission (Werman et al., 1968). Alcohol therefore suppresses the activity of the central nervous system while simultaneously affecting a number of other signaling molecules that can have diverse effects on cognition.

3. Human cognitive deficits after adolescent alcohol exposure

Acute and repeated alcohol exposure during adolescence or young adulthood can impair learning and memory during these periods (Acheson et al., 1998; Brown et al., 2000; Hanson et al., 2011; Thoma et al., 2012). In humans, 21- to 24-year-olds are more susceptible than 25- to 29-year-olds to acute alcohol-induced disruptions in semantic and figural memory (Acheson et al., 1998). Furthermore, after three weeks of abstinence from alcohol, alcohol-dependent adolescents show impaired performance in the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) Vocabulary, Information, Similarity, and Coding subtests. California Verbal Learning Test for Children (CVLT-C) retention rates and visual reproduction retention rates are also impaired in alcohol-dependent adolescents after a three-week abstinence period (Brown et al., 2000). Heavy alcohol use in adolescence has been linked to poorer memory in the California verbal learning test during adolescence (Hanson et al., 2011). These findings suggest that verbal learning, verbal retention, and nonverbal retention are affected by heavy alcohol use and/or alcohol dependence in adolescence. Adolescent alcohol use has also been linked to adolescent deficits in executive function and attention after two days of abstinence from alcohol use (Thoma et al., 2012).

Executive function deficits have also been reported in 18-year-old subjects after adolescent alcohol exposure, as measured by working memory in the N-back task (Mahedy et al., 2018). However, young adult working memory deficits have not been observed consistently. Others have reported no adult working memory deficits measured by the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Task after adolescent alcohol exposure (Boelema et al., 2015). These inconsistent findings may be due to the differences in tasks used to assess working memory. Ages of alcohol exposure and cognitive testing also differed between these studies. N-back task performance was assessed in subjects that were 18 years of age and exposed to alcohol around 15 years of age, and Amsterdam Neuropsychological Task performance was measured in adolescents that were 11–19 years old and exposed to alcohol at variable adolescent ages (Mahedy et al., 2018; Boelema et al., 2015). Because learning circuitry is still developing throughout adolescence, it is likely that alcohol exposure can affect learning processes differently in different stages of adolescence (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Spear, 2015). Therefore, differences in timing of alcohol exposure and in learning assessment tasks may contribute to the different effects of adolescent alcohol exposure on adult learning. The cognitive consequences of adolescent alcohol exposure may be better studied in rodent models, in which the timing of alcohol exposure and later testing can be carefully controlled.

4. Rodent PFC-mediated learning after adolescent alcohol exposure

The PFC is primarily responsible for executive function, which includes selfregulation to align actions with goals (Miller & Cohen, 2001). The PFC is an important component of cognitive flexibility and is known to be impacted by alcohol exposure during adolescence (Dias & Aggleton, 2000; De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008). The following sections of this review will describe short-term (present during adolescence) and long-term (present during adulthood) consequences of adolescent alcohol exposure on PFC-dependent cognitive behaviors.

4.1. Trace fear conditioning and contextual fear extinction

Adolescent alcohol exposure affects numerous PFC-dependent memory tasks, including both contextual fear extinction and trace fear conditioning. Fear conditioning is a common learning paradigm in which subjects are trained to associate an initially neutral conditioned stimulus, such as a context or tone, with a naturally aversive unconditioned stimulus, like a mild footshock. This conditioning paradigm relies on similar neural regions in rodents and humans (Delgado et al., 2006). In classical cued fear conditioning, the shock is presented during the auditory cue so that there is no gap between stimuli. Trace fear conditioning is a form of fear conditioning in which there is a temporal gap, or a trace interval, between the offset of the tone and the onset of the shock. This paradigm recruits PFC-dependent working memory processes in addition to HC-dependent declarative memory processes (Connor & Gould, 2016). Adolescent rats show more severe deficits in trace fear conditioning than adults when given acute alcohol before training in this task (Hunt & Barnet 2016). This contrasts what has been reported for contextual fear conditioning, in which subjects are trained to associate an aversive stimulus with the context in which they experienced that stimulus. Adolescent rats show less severe acute alcohol-induced contextual fear learning deficits than adults (Land & Spear, 2004; Broadwater & Spear 2013; Hunt & Barnet, 2016). Because trace fear conditioning is PFC-dependent and contextual fear conditioning is primarily HC-dependent (Connor and Gould 2016; Logue et al. 1997), increased susceptibility to ethanol deficits in trace fear learning may suggest that the adolescent PFC is more susceptible than the adolescent HC to the effects of acute alcohol.

Fear extinction is a reduction in fear response after repeated re-exposure to the conditioning context or cue without co-exposure to the aversive stimulus. Contextual fear extinction depends on the PFC in addition to the HC and amygdala (Morgan et al., 1993; Quirk & Mueller, 2008). Contextual fear extinction in adult rats has been shown to be impaired after intermittent alcohol exposure during adolescence (Broadwater & Spear, 2013). This demonstrates that contextual fear extinction is another PFC-dependent learning process that is impaired by adolescent alcohol exposure. Importantly, in this same study, cued fear extinction was not impaired by adolescent alcohol exposure. This could suggest that a region, such as the HC, that is engaged by contextual and not cued fear learning processes is responsible for observed contextual fear extinction deficits. It is possible that damage to the PFC acts in combination with damage to the HC to cause contextual fear extinction deficits. Because cued fear extinction is HC-independent (Phillips & LeDoux, 1992), it may be that damage to the PFC after adolescent alcohol exposure is insufficient to impair cued fear extinction. Contextual fear extinction can be used to understand the persistence of fear memories in anxiety disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder (Milad et al., 2006). If adolescent alcohol exposure reduces adult fear extinction (Broadwater & Spear, 2013), this could imply an increased susceptibility to anxiety disorders (Milad et al., 2006). This is consistent with other pre-clinical findings suggesting that adolescent alcohol exposure can increase anxiety-like behaviors in adults (Pandey et al., 2015; Kyzar et al., 2016).

4.2. Object Recognition

Deficits in novel object recognition (NOR) after repeated adolescent alcohol treatment have been documented in numerous studies (Pascual et al., 2007; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017). The NOR test measures time exploring novel and familiar objects to determine memory of the familiar; rodents will spend more time exploring novel objects. This is a valuable way to measure learning in a task that is neither aversive nor appetitive (Ennaceur & Delacour, 1988; Antunes & Biala, 2012). Variation in the timing between training and testing, herein referred to as the delay, can affect the neural circuitry involved in this task. It has been suggested that shorter delays require the PFC, while longer delays may depend more on the perirhinal cortex and HC (Warburton & Brown, 2015). Studies in rats have shown that lesions to the perirhinal cortex impairs NOR performance with delays longer than 5 minutes, but have little effect with a shorter NOR delay (Norman & Eacott, 2004). Other areas modulate related aspects of object recognition such as the entorhinal cortex, which may be involved in object-context associations (Wilson et al., 2013). Alcohol can produce neurodegeneration of the entorhinal cortex (Obernier et al., 2002), which suggests other parts of the circuitry involved in NOR could be disrupted. One study in mice tested both short (5 minute) and long (1 hour) delay NOR and found that only short-delay NOR showed a deficit in adulthood after adolescent alcohol exposure, suggesting that NOR deficits may be PFC-related (Wolstenholme et al., 2017). Consistent with this finding, a study in rats using a short 1-minute delay also observed adult NOR deficits after adolescent alcohol treatment (Pascual et al., 2007). While the neurocircuitry mediating NOR may not be fully characterized (Warburton & Brown, 2015), converging evidence of deficits in PFC-mediated tasks after adolescent alcohol exposure (Coleman et al., 2011; Vetreno et al., 2012; Broadwater & Spear, 2013) suggests that alterations in PFC function may play a role in NOR deficits after adolescent alcohol exposure. These deficits can persist into adulthood (Wolstenholme et al., 2017; Pascual et al., 2007).

4.3. Cognitive flexibility and serial reaction time tasks

Rodent studies demonstrate that exposure to alcohol during adolescence leads to long-term deficits in PFC-dependent reversal learning (Coleman et al., 2011; Vetreno & Crews, 2012). When tested in the Morris Water Maze, adult mice that were treated with alcohol during adolescence had deficits in reversal learning, or learning new escape conditions from a water maze (Coleman et al., 2011). Deficits in adult mice and rats exposed to alcohol during adolescence have also been seen in Barnes maze reversal learning, where subjects re-learn escape conditions in a dry spatial maze (Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Coleman et al., 2014). Set-shifting tasks, in which rodents are trained and then retrained to respond to different cues for food rewards, also serve as a measure of cognitive flexibility. Impairments in this task have been observed in adult rats treated with alcohol in adolescence (Gass et al., 2014). The 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) can be used to assess impulsivity and cognitive flexibility in rodents (Robbins, 2002). Subjects are trained to nose-poke the location of light stimuli and are subsequently rewarded with food pellets, and their ability to sustain attention between different locations over numerous trials is assessed. The serial reaction time task is thought to be dependent on the PFC, cingulate cortex, and striatum (Robbins, 2002). Adult impairments in cognitive flexibility indicated by more timeout responses in the 5-CSRTT have also been observed after adolescent alcohol exposure in rats (Semenova, 2012). Reports of such deficits in maze reversal learning, set-shifting tasks, and the 5-CSRTT suggest that adolescent alcohol exposure can produce long-lasting deficits in reversal learning and cognitive flexibility.

Notably, impairments in cognitive flexibility have not been consistent across learning paradigms. Studies in rats using the 5-CSRTT have found that adult cognitive flexibility during this task is impaired (Semenova, 2012) or unaffected (Boutros et al., 2017) after adolescent intermittent alcohol. Adult performance in the 2-choice reaction time task is actually improved after adolescent alcohol exposure (Slawecki, 2006). It has been suggested that the different performance seen between 5-choice and 2-choice reaction time tasks is related to task difficulty, as changing stimulus durations can change alcohol’s effects on serial reaction time tasks (Semenova, 2012). The following section will discuss how changing task difficulty by introducing an acute alcohol challenge can influence performance in a serial reaction time task.

4.4. PFC-dependent cognition after adult acute alcohol challenge and prior adolescent alcohol exposure - Serial Reaction Time Task

Exposure to alcohol during adolescence can change susceptibility to acute alcohol-induced changes in adult cognitive faculties. Acute alcohol treatment in adult rats can impair attentional performance, increase impulsive responding, and decrease cognitive flexibility in the 5-CSRTT (Semenova, 2012). However, adults exposed to alcohol during adolescence have been reported to be less susceptible to acute alcohol-induced attentional impairments in 5-CSRTT. An acute alcohol exposure decreased correct responses and increased omissions in subjects with no history of prior alcohol exposure. However, in subjects that were treated intermittently with alcohol during adolescence, an acute alcohol exposure did not impair these measures of attentional performance. Without an alcohol challenge, subjects exposed to alcohol during adolescence demonstrated comparable numbers of omissions and correct responses to water-treated subjects (Semenova, 2012). This finding suggests that exposure to intermittent alcohol during adolescence can alter adult susceptibility to acute alcohol-induced cognitive deficits.

4.5. Summary of PFC-dependent learning

A converging body of evidence in rodent models suggests that repeated alcohol exposure during adolescence can impair PFC-dependent learning tasks in adulthood (Hunt & Barnet, 2016; Barnet & Spear, 2013; Pascual et al., 2007; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017; Coleman et al., 2011; Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Gass et al., 2014; Semenova, 2012). Adult PFC-dependent cognitive behaviors after adolescent alcohol exposure are summarized in Table 1. These impairments have been observed primarily in assessments of novel object recognition (Pascual et al., 2007; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017) or cognitive flexibility (Coleman et al., 2011; Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Coleman et al., 2014; Gass et al., 2014; Semenova, 2012). These findings in rodents recapitulate the deficits in executive function seen in young adult humans after alcohol exposure during adolescence (Mahedy et al., 2018), providing strong evidence that adolescent alcohol exposure has long-term effects on PFC function.

Table 1:

Summary of adult PFC-dependent learning after adolescent alcohol exposure.

Species Sex Strain Behavior Dose Treatment pattern Treatment Duration Adolescent age of treatment Abstinence duration before testing Age of testing Effect on Learning Paper
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Context extinction 4 g/kg Every 48 hours 21 days PND 35–55 22 days PND 70–90 Impaired Broadwater and Spear 2013
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Cued extinction 4 g/kg Every 48 hours 21 days PND 28–48 22 days PND 70–90 No effect Broadwater and Spear 2013
Rat not listed Wistar NOR (1 min delay) 3 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 14 days PND 25–38 2 days and 22 days PND 41 and 61 Impaired Pascual et al. 2007
Mouse M+F DBA/2J NOR (5 min delay) 4 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 14 days PND 29–42 24 days PND 66+ Impaired Wolstenholme et al. 2017
Mouse M C57BL/6 (Charles River) MWM reversal learning 5 g/kg Daily 10 days PND 28–37 23 days PND 60–72 Impaired Coleman et al. 2011
Mouse M C57BL/6 (Charles River) Barnes maze reversal 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 10 days PND 28–37 31 days PND 68–91 Impaired Coleman et al. 2014
Rat M Wistar Barnes maze reversal 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 31 days PND 25–55 9 days PND 64–75 Impaired Vetreno and Crews 2012
Rat M Long-Evans Set shifting N/A, vapor 2-day on/2-day off 13 days PND 28–41 49 days PND 90 Impaired Gass et al. 2014
Rat M Wistar 5-CSRTT 5 g/kg Every 8 hours 4 days PND 33–36 24 days PND 60+ Impaired Semenova 2012
Rat M Wistar 5-CSRTT 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 30 days PND 28–57 8 days PND 65+ No effect Boutros et al.2017
Rat M Sprague-Dawley 2-CRTT N/A, vapor Daily 14 days PND 30–43 7 days PND 50 Improved Slawecki 2006
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Set shifting 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 30 days PND 25–54 ~21 days PND 70–120 Impaired Fernandez and Savage 2017

5. Adolescent alcohol effects on PFC structure and neurobiology

In both adult and adolescent humans, alcohol use has been associated with changes in PFC volume (Pfefferbaum et al., 1997; De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008; Luciana et al., 2013; Squeglia et al., 2015). Studies in adult males and females after alcohol exposure have shown that alcohol use is associated with smaller PFC volume (Pfefferbaum et al., 1997; Kubota et al., 2001). Studies in adolescents have shown that alcohol can disrupt frontal cortical maturation (De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008; Luciana et al., 2013; Squeglia et al. 2015; Pfefferbaum et al., 2016; Pfefferbaum et al., 2018). Frontal lobe maturation has been linked to adolescent performance in learning and memory tasks (Sowell et al., 2001). Normal PFC development in adolescence consists of reductions in gray matter, due to pruning processes, and increases in white matter, due to increases in myelination (Giedd et al., 1999; Pfefferbaum et al., 2013; Pfefferbaum et al., 2016; Pfefferbaum et al., 2018). However, some report different patterns of white matter development, suggesting that PFC white matter volume remains consistent or decreases in a sex-dependent manner (Nagel et al., 2006). Most studies report that adolescent alcohol users have reduced PFC volume (De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008; Luciana et al., 2013), with reduced PFC white matter volume (De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008; Pfefferbaum et al., 2018) and accelerated PFC gray matter reduction (Squeglia et al., 2015; Pfefferbaum et al., 2018). Medina and colleagues (2008) observe a sex difference in alcohol’s impact on PFC volume, with adolescent females experiencing decreases and adolescent males experiencing increases in PFC volume after alcohol exposure. This sex difference is not observed in the longitudinal study by Squeglia and colleagues (2015). Still, this potential sex difference calls for a more thorough investigation of the effects of adolescent alcohol exposure on learning in female human and rodent subjects. Converging reports of impaired adolescent PFC maturation after alcohol exposure suggest that altered PFC neurobiology likely contributes to altered cognitive performance observed after adolescent alcohol exposure (Brown et al., 2000; Hanson et al., 2011; Thoma et al., 2012; Mahedy et al., 2018).

Rodent studies have given some insight into the long-term neurobiological changes that occur after adolescent alcohol exposure. Functionally, studies have demonstrated that PFC synaptic transmission and intrinsic excitability are altered in adult subjects exposed to alcohol during adolescence (Kroener et al., 2012; Trantham-Davidson et al., 2017; Salling et al., 2018). The medial PFC (mPFC) has important dopaminergic and GABAergic connections that undergo pruning during adolescence (Teicher et al., 1995; Tseng & O’Donnell, 2006). Adolescent alcohol has been shown to interrupt this development, causing reduced dopamine D1 receptor modulation of intrinsic firing and D1 receptor modulation of evoked NMDA currents in prelimbic pyramidal cells. Fast-spiking interneurons also exhibited reduced intrinsic excitability and modulation by D1 receptors. D2 receptor function appeared to be unaffected by alcohol exposure (Trantham-Davidson et al., 2017). Another recent study expanded upon these findings and demonstrated that adult mPFC pyramidal neurons also display reduced intrinsic excitability after adolescent alcohol exposure (Salling et al., 2018). It is suggested that alcohol exposure may arrest normal development of intrinsic excitability during adolescence (Salling et al., 2018).

Some evidence suggests that adult neuroimmune markers are dysregulated after adolescent alcohol exposure (Vetreno & Crews, 2012). Specifically, adult levels of high-mobility group box 1 and Toll-like receptors 3 and 4 were increased in adolescent alcohol-exposed adult rats (Vetreno & Crews, 2012). These findings are consistent with the known pro-inflammatory effects of alcohol treatment in adult rodents (Qin et al., 2008). Inflammatory pathways are known to influence abundance of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), an important contributor to neuroplasticity (Lapchak et al., 1993). Importantly, persistent reductions in PFC BDNF have been reported in adult rodents after adolescent alcohol exposure (Fernandez et al., 2016). However, another study reported that BDNF levels returned to normal after protracted abstinence (Fernandez et al., 2017). While the duration of alcohol-induced BDNF alterations is unclear, it is possible that temporary reductions in BDNF levels could produce lasting downstream consequences. Both reductions in BDNF and altered inflammatory processes can impair learning and memory (Linnarsson et al., 1997; Boitard et al., 2014), making it likely that these neurobiological changes are linked to cognitive deficits seen after adolescent alcohol exposure.

In summary, in humans, adolescent alcohol exposure has been found to disrupt PFC maturation (De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008; Luciana et al., 2013; Squeglia et al. 2015; Pfefferbaum et al., 2016; Pfefferbaum et al., 2018). Exposure to alcohol during adulthood has also been linked to reduced PFC volume (Pfefferbaum et al., 1997; Kubota et al., 2001). Rodent studies have shown that synaptic transmission and intrinsic excitability within the PFC are altered after adolescent alcohol exposure (Kroener et al., 2012; Trantham-Davidson et al., 2017; Salling et al. 2018). These findings suggest that adolescent alcohol may halt or slow different aspects of PFC development, which could contribute to the persistent adolescent-like behavior, such as increased impulsivity, that is typically observed in adulthood after adolescent alcohol exposure (Spear & Swartzwelder, 2014). Increases in inflammatory markers and decreases in BDNF have also been reported in the PFC after adolescent alcohol exposure (Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Fernandez et al., 2016; Fernandez et al., 2017). Many or all of these neurobiological changes are likely to contribute to deficits in cognitive flexibility and working memory after adolescent alcohol exposure.

6. Rodent HC-mediated learning after adolescent alcohol exposure

The HC is an important component of spatial memory and memory consolidation processes (Smith & Milner, 1981). The HC and learning behaviors dependent upon this region are also affected by adolescent alcohol exposure (De Bellis et al., 2000; Nagel et al., 2005; Broadwater & Spear, 2013). The following sections examine the immediate and long-term effects of adolescent alcohol exposure on HC-related cognitive function.

6.1. Contextual fear conditioning

As described previously, contextual fear learning involves the association of an aversive stimulus with a context. It is known to be HC-dependent and impaired by alcohol in rodents (Logue et al., 1997; Wehner & Radcliffe, 2004; Gould, 2003; Tipps et al., 2015). Adolescent rats are less susceptible to HC-dependent contextual fear learning deficits after an acute alcohol challenge than adults (Land & Spear 2004; Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Hunt & Barnet, 2016). Cued fear learning, in which subjects associate an aversive stimulus with an auditory cue, is HC-independent (Kim & Fanselow, 1992; Logue et al., 1997) and more resistant to alcohol-induced deficits in both adolescent and adult mice and rats, suggesting a HC-specific effect of alcohol on contextual fear conditioning (Gould, 2003; Broadwater & Spear, 2013). Adult contextual fear learning in rats has been shown to be impaired after early adolescent, and not adult, intermittent alcohol exposure (Broadwater & Spear, 2013). These findings suggest that, while adolescents may be less susceptible to acute alcohol-induced contextual learning deficits, they are more susceptible to long-term intermittent alcohol-induced contextual learning deficits in adulthood (Broadwater & Spear, 2013).

6.2. Object Recognition

As discussed in relation to PFC-dependent learning, chronic adolescent alcohol exposure can result in adult deficits in NOR (Pascual et al., 2007; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017). One study found that these deficits were present after a short (5 minute) delay but not a long (1 hour) delay between NOR training and testing, suggesting that deficits in NOR can be attributed to the PFC and not the perirhinal cortex and HC (Wolstenholme et al., 2017). In support of this, another study using long delays between training and testing (24 hours and 7 days) did not report adult rat NOR impairments after adolescent alcohol exposure (Fernandez & Savage, 2017). However, a different study using a long 24 hour delay reported adult rat NOR deficits after adolescent alcohol exposure (Vetreno & Crews, 2015). Notably, NOR performance in this study positively correlated to HC neurogenesis markers after adolescent alcohol exposure (Vetreno & Crews, 2015). This finding suggests a role for the HC in novel object recognition deficits caused by exposure to alcohol during adolescence. Differences in alcohol treatments or NOR paradigms, such as the delay duration between training and testing, may contribute to differing reports of NOR performance after adolescent alcohol exposure.

Other object recognition tasks have been shown to be impaired in adulthood by adolescent alcohol exposure in rats (Swartzwelder et al., 2015). Adolescent alcohol exposure caused adult deficits in a modified spatial-temporal object recognition task (stOR). In this task, training involves the introduction of two sets of objects one hour apart. One hour after the second set of objects is introduced, familiar objects from both sessions are reintroduced in old and new locations. Adult mice treated with alcohol during adolescence spent more time exploring the older objects than saline controls. Deficits were not seen in spatial task components, which could suggest that hippocampal learning was not affected by adolescent alcohol exposure. However, Swartzwelder and colleagues (2015) suggest that the complexity of temporal task components may have been required to expose alcohol-induced impairments. The idea that complex learning challenges are required to reveal deficits caused by adolescent alcohol exposure will be explored further in a later section.

6.3. Maze-learning

Other tasks have been used to assess HC-dependent learning after adolescent alcohol exposure. Conditional discrimination learning in a Y maze is a HC-dependent task in which rodents are trained to choose visually distinctive arms for food rewards (Murray & Ridley, 1999; Pascual et al., 2007). Intermittent alcohol treatment during adolescence in rats has been shown to impair adolescent and adult performance in this task (Pascual et al., 2007). Other studies have suggested that some HC-dependent learning is not affected by adolescent alcohol exposure. Radial arm maze learning, for example, assesses working memory by testing a rodent’s ability to collect rewards from arms of a maze quickly without repeating entry into those arms. While many brain regions can contribute to this task, it is thought to be associated with HC function (Jarrard, 1993; Floresco et al., 1997). In adult rats, radial arm maze learning has been reported to be unaffected by adolescent alcohol exposure using standard measures of learning (Risher et al., 2013). Spontaneous alternation is another HC-dependent task in which rodents explore a maze and revisits into previous arms are recorded as a measure of memory (Johnson et al., 1977). Spontaneous alternation in a plus maze during adulthood is not impaired by adolescent alcohol exposure in rats (Fernandez et al., 2017). These findings suggest that adolescent alcohol may have task-specific effects on adult maze-learning, impairing some (Pascual et al., 2007) but not other (Risher et al., 2013; Fernandez et al., 2017) maze-learning tasks.

The acquisition phase of the Morris Water Maze, in which rodents learn escape conditions, is also HC-dependent (Morris et al., 1982). In rats, an acute alcohol exposure causes more severe Morris Water Maze acquisition deficits in adolescents than in adults (Markwiese et al., 1998). However, inconsistent effects of intermittent adolescent alcohol exposure on water maze acquisition have been reported (Sircar et al., 2009; Sircar & Sircar, 2005; Coleman et al., 2011; Silvers et al., 2006). While some studies in rats have reported adolescent and adult deficits in Morris Water Maze acquisition after adolescent intermittent alcohol treatment (Sircar et al., 2009; Sircar & Sircar, 2005), other studies in rats and mice have reported that intermittent alcohol does not affect this task in adolescents or adults (Coleman et al., 2011; Silvers et al., 2006). Similarly, learning spatial escape conditions of a dry Barnes maze in adulthood has also been reported to be unaffected after adolescent alcohol exposure in rats (Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Fernandez et al., 2017). Differences in maze protocols or alcohol administration paradigms between laboratories may be responsible for these different effects.

6.4. HC-dependent cognition after adult acute alcohol challenge and prior adolescent alcohol exposure

Findings from studies in rats using acute alcohol challenges support the idea that adult learning deficits after adolescent alcohol may only be revealed during more complex learning tasks and challenges (Swartzwelder et al., 2015; White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013). Some adult HC-dependent learning tasks are unaffected by adolescent alcohol exposure during normal conditions, but more sensitive to alcohol challenges during adulthood (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013). Adult learning in a radial arm maze, for example, is typically unaffected by adolescent alcohol exposure (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013), but an acute alcohol treatment prior to this task can impair learning (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013). While adolescent alcohol exposure did not alter adult learning by itself in this task, it did increase adult vulnerability to an acute alcohol challenge in this same task (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013). Contrary to these findings, it has also been reported that adolescent alcohol exposure protects adults from acute alcohol-induced learning impairments in the Morris Water Maze acquisition test. In rats that were treated with water during adolescence, an acute alcohol treatment in adulthood impaired memory acquisition in the Morris Water Maze. Subjects that were treated with alcohol throughout adolescence did not experience acute alcohol-induced acquisition impairments when tested one day after cessation of chronic alcohol treatment (postnatal day 50). However, this effect was reduced over time. When these subjects were challenged 12 days after cessation of intermittent alcohol treatment (postnatal day 62), they experienced acute alcohol-induced deficits similar to the intermittent water-treated group, suggesting that protection from acute alcohol-induced acquisition deficits caused by adolescent alcohol exposure may not be long-lasting (Silvers et al., 2006).

It is unclear why adolescent alcohol exposure could decrease alcohol sensitivity in Morris Water Maze acquisition (Silvers et al., 2006) and increase alcohol sensitivity in radial arm maze learning (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013) when alcohol exposures occurred during similar age ranges between studies. One possible explanation is that intermittent alcohol exposure has different effects on acute alcohol sensitivity after brief or extended periods of abstinence from alcohol. Consistent with this idea, decreased adult sensitivity to an alcohol challenge on HC-dependent learning was reported 1 day after cessation of chronic treatment (Silvers et al., 2006), no effect on learning was reported 12 days after chronic treatment (Silvers et al., 2006), and increased sensitivity was reported 20 days after chronic treatment (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013). This would suggest that a history of intermittent alcohol exposure in adolescence can make adults differentially susceptible to acute alcohol-induced cognitive deficits, depending on the time elapsed since adolescent alcohol treatment.

6.5. Summary of HC-dependent learning

Studies consistently report that adolescents are less susceptible to acute alcohol-induced contextual learning deficits than adults (Land & Spear, 2004; Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Hunt & Barnet, 2016). Intermittent alcohol exposure during adolescence can also produce long-term contextual learning deficits (Broadwater & Spear, 2013). While the role of the HC in NOR is debated (Warburton & Brown, 2015), it is clear that adult performance in this learning task can also be affected by adolescent alcohol exposure (Pascual et al., 2007; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017). It has been reported that exposure to alcohol during adolescence can alter sensitivity to acute alcohol challenges on HC-dependent learning tasks in adulthood (White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013; Silvers et al., 2006), with sensitivity increasing over time. Effects of adolescent alcohol exposure on other HC-dependent tasks are inconsistent (Pascual et al., 2007; Risher et al., 2013; Fernandez et al., 2017; Sircar et al., 2009; Sircar & Sircar, 2005; Coleman et al., 2011; Silvers et al., 2006). Adult HC-dependent learning after adolescent alcohol exposure is summarized in Table 2. These findings suggest that alcohol’s long-term effects on the HC may be more nuanced or less robust than its previously discussed effects on the PFC.

Table 2:

Summary of adult HC-dependent learning after adolescent alcohol exposure.

Species Sex Strain Behavior Dose Treatment pattern Treatment Duration Adolescent age of treatment Abstinence duration before testing Age of testing Effect on Learning Paper
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Contextual FC 4 g/kg Every 48 hrs 21 days PND 28–48 22 days PND 70–90 Impaired Broadwater and Spear 2013
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Contextual FC 4 g/kg Every 48 hrs 21 days PND 35–55 22 days PND 70–90 No effect Broadwater and Spear 2013
Rat M Wistar NOR (24 hr delay) 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 31 days PND 25–55 108 days PND 163–165 Impaired Vetreno and Crews 2015
Mouse M+F DBA/2J NOR (1 hr delay) 4 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 14 days PND 29–42 24 days PND 66+ No effect Wolstenholme et al. 2017
Rat M Sprague-Dawley NOR (24 hr and 7 day delay) 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 30 days PND 25–54 ~21 days PND 70–120 No effect Fernandez and Savage 2017
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Spatial-temporal object recognition task (stOR) 5 g/kg 2-day on/1 to 2-day off 16 days PND 30–45 25 days PND 70 Impaired Swartzwelder et al. 2015
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Radial arm maze 5 g/kg Every 48 hrs 20 days PND 30–49 20 days PND 59 No effect White et al. 2006
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Radial arm maze 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 20 days PND 30–49 20 days PND 71+ No effect Risheret al. 2013
Rat not listed Wistar Conditional discrimination (Y-maze) 3 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 14 days PND 25–38 2 days and 22 days PND 41 and 61 Impaired Pascual et al. 2007
Rat M Sprague-Dawley MWM acquisition 5 g/kg Every 48 hrs 20 days PND 30–48 No abstinence PND 29–43 No effect Silvers et al. 2006
Rat F Sprague-Dawley MWM acquisition 2 g/kg Daily 5 days PND 30–34 No abstinence PND 30– 34 Impaired Sircar et al. 2009
Rat M Sprague-Dawley MWM acquisition 2 g/kg Daily 5 days PND 30–34 No abstinence PND 30– 34 Impaired Sircar and Sircar 2005
Mouse M C57BL/6J MWM acquisition 5 g/kg Daily 10 days PND 28–37 23 days PND 60–72 No effect Coleman et al. 2011
Rat M Wistar Barnes maze acquisition 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 31 days PND 25–55 9 days PND 64–75 No effect Vetreno and Crews 2012
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Barnes maze acquisition 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 30 days PND 25–54 ~21 days PND 70–120 No effect Fernandez and Savage 2017
Rat M Sprague-Dawley Spontaneous alternation 5 g/kg 2-day on/2-day off 30 days PND 25–54 ~21 days PND 70–120 No effect Fernandez and Savage 2017

7. Adolescent alcohol effects on HC structure and neurobiology

In humans, alcohol use during adulthood and adolescence has been associated with smaller HC volumes (De Bellis et al., 2000; Nagel et al., 2005). A recent longitudinal study in college students found that a greater alcohol use index was associated with an accelerated decline of HC and para-hippocampal gray matter volume. This decline was associated with poor performance in the California Verbal Learning Test (Meda et al., 2018). These findings importantly suggest a causal link between alcohol use and decreased HC volume. It is important to note that some studies report no association between HC volume and alcohol dependence (Lee et al., 2007; Fein et al., 2013). One recent meta-analysis suggests that the inconsistency between adult studies may be due to variability in sample sizes and a small effect size (Wilson et al., 2018). The study also found that the association of problematic alcohol use with reduced HC volume increases with age. As older subjects have generally been using alcohol for longer periods of time, this likely suggests that longer duration of alcohol use leads to more severe HC injury. Alternatively, an increased susceptibility to alcohol damage with age could suggest that adolescents are less sensitive than adults to alcohol-induced HC damage (Wilson et al., 2018).

Rodent studies have suggested mechanisms responsible for HC damage after alcohol exposure. HC-related learning deficits are often attributed to reductions in HC neurogenesis or increased HC cell death (Vetreno & Crews, 2015) and alterations in cholinergic signaling (Mohapel et al., 2005). Cholinergic signaling from the basal forebrain is critical for learning and memory processes (Blokland, 1995; Mohapel et al., 2005). It has been shown that intermittent alcohol exposure during adolescence can lead to a reduction of cholinergic markers in the basal forebrain (Ehlers et al., 2011; Vetreno et al., 2014; Vetreno et al., 2018). This reduction may indicate reduced cholinergic signaling to the HC after exposure to alcohol during adolescence, which could underlie some alcohol-induced learning deficits.

Reductions in neurogenesis have also been linked to alcohol-induced learning deficits (Vetreno & Crews, 2015). Numerous reports have shown that adolescent or adult alcohol exposure can alter adult HC neurogenesis (Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Sakharkar et al., 2016; He et al., 2005). The adolescent brain is particularly vulnerable to alcohol-induced reductions in neurogenesis (Crews et al., 2006) and therefore likely to be more vulnerable to behavioral consequences associated with changes in neurogenesis. Consistent with this idea, it has been shown that adult HC neurogenesis after adolescent alcohol exposure positively correlates with performance in an object recognition memory task (Vetreno & Crews, 2015). However, findings have been inconsistent as to whether adolescent alcohol affects adult neurogenesis. Some studies show decreases in adult neurogenesis and cell proliferation (Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Sakharkar et al., 2016), while some show no effects of adolescent alcohol on adult cell proliferation (Broadwater et al., 2014). Broadwater and colleagues found decreases in doublecortin, a marker of neurogenesis, that they attributed to increases in death of immature neurons and not decreases in neurogenesis itself (2014). Differences in reports of cell proliferation may be attributed to differences in alcohol administration paradigms. Those that reported decreases in cell proliferation used two-day on/two-day off treatment patterns (Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Sakharkar et al., 2016), while Broadwater and colleagues (2014) administered alcohol every 48 hours. Different findings of altered cell proliferation after unique alcohol treatment paradigms could imply that the patterns of alcohol exposure may differentially influence adult cell proliferation in the HC.

HC neuronal loss is seen in adult rodents treated with alcohol during adolescence, and this cell death is thought to play a role in adult cognitive deficits after adolescent alcohol (Risher et al., 2015). Glutamate toxicity could contribute to observed cell death (Olney, 1969; Choi, 1992). In support of this idea, rodent electrophysiological studies have shown that adolescent intermittent alcohol can increase NMDA receptor-mediated currents in HC CA1 pyramidal cells (Swartzwelder et al., 2017). Adolescent intermittent alcohol can also lower the threshold for the induction of HC long-term potentiation (Risher et al., 2015). These changes could drive neuronal loss after adolescent alcohol through increased glutamate signaling at potentially toxic levels.

Human studies have demonstrated that adolescent alcohol exposure can lead to reductions in adolescent and adult HC volume (De Bellis et al., 2000; Nagel et al., 2005). Rodent studies have suggested that impaired cholinergic input (Ehlers et al., 2011; Vetreno et al., 2014; Vetreno et al., 2018), reduced neurogenesis and cell proliferation (Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Sakharkar et al., 2016; He et al., 2005), and cell death (Risher et al., 2015) may contribute to damaged adult HC after adolescent alcohol exposure. These biological changes in adulthood likely contribute to impairments in HC-dependent learning. Reduced HC volume after alcohol in adolescence has been linked to poor performance in verbal learning tasks (Meda et al., 2018), and neurogenesis after adolescent alcohol in rodents has been positively correlated to performance in object recognition memory task (Vetreno & Crews, 2015). These findings suggest that adolescent alcohol exposure can cause long-lasting HC damage, and this is likely related to reported deficits in HC-dependent cognitive behaviors.

8. Important mediating factors of adolescent alcohol’s long-term effects

As outlined within this paper, adolescent alcohol exposure has the potential to affect a range of learning phenotypes. Converging evidence points to the PFC and HC as potential mediators of these effects. Broadly, deficits in adult PFC-related cognitive flexibility have been reported after adolescent intermittent alcohol exposure (Coleman et al., 2011; Vetreno et al., 2012; Broadwater & Spear, 2013). Adult HC-dependent cognitive impairments after adolescent alcohol exposure have also been reported, but these findings are inconsistent between studies (Pascual et al., 2007; Risher et al., 2013; Fernandez et al., 2017; Sircar et al., 2009; Sircar & Sircar, 2005; Coleman et al., 2011; Silvers et al., 2006). Detection of persistent deficits in HC-dependent learning after adolescent alcohol exposure seems to be largely dependent upon task and treatment paradigms. Variation in abstinence periods prior to learning assessment, administration patterns, and age of treatment likely contribute to inconsistent reports of HC-related learning deficits.

Studies in human subjects have shown that alcohol abstinence in alcohol-dependent individuals can improve working memory impairments caused by alcohol use (Petit et al., 2017). Adolescents and young adults may be more likely to recover cognitive abilities after abstinence than older alcohol-dependent subjects, suggesting that age and alcohol abstinence duration may interact to mediate long-term consequences of alcohol use (Munro et al., 2000; Tapert et al., 2002). Rodent models also demonstrate that alcohol-induced cognitive impairments can be attenuated over time. A study in alcohol-dependent rats found that binge-like alcohol exposure caused deficits in spatial and recognition memory one week after treatment. Ten weeks after treatment, spatial memory impairments persisted, while recognition memory normalized to that of water-treated rats (Cippitelli et al., 2010). These findings suggest that the impact of an alcohol abstinence period on the recovery of cognitive function can vary depending on the learning task and neural circuitry employed in the task. This may explain some inconsistencies between studies that find persistent cognitive impairments (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Pascual et al., 2007; Coleman et al., 2011; Coleman et al., 2014; Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Gass et al., 2014; Vetreno &Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017) or lack thereof (Slawecki, 2006; Boutros et al., 2017) after adolescent alcohol exposure.

The term “intermittent” has been used to describe a variety of alcohol treatment paradigms. Some common alcohol treatment patterns include treatments once every 24 hours (Sircar et al., 2009; Coleman et al., 2011), once every 48 hours (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; White et al., 2006; Silvers et al., 2006), and once daily for two consecutive days followed by two abstinent days (Pascual et al., 2007; Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Coleman et al., 2014; Fernandez & Savage, 2017). However, the period of abstinence between alcohol exposures has been shown to be critical in determining the severity of adult phenotypes (Tapert et al., 2002). For example, it has been reported that intermittent and chronic alcohol exposure paradigms can differentially affect later alcohol-related behaviors. Specifically, in adult and adolescent rodents, intermittent alcohol access patterns produce greater elevations in alcohol intake than chronic alcohol access patterns (Wise, 1973; Tomie et al., 2006; Melendez, 2011; Kimbrough et al., 2017). This effect is greater in adolescent subjects than in adult subjects (Melendez, 2011). While differential escalation effects may be attributed to factors specific to voluntary alcohol drinking, such as conditioned taste aversion, other potential mediators such as acute withdrawal effects could be broadly relevant to behavioral changes reported after other intermittent alcohol exposure paradigms (for review, see Spear, 2020). These periods of acute alcohol withdrawal, and not just the periods of alcohol administration by themselves, are major causes of altered neurobiology that is linked to learning impairments. Decreases in neurogenesis, for example, can be caused by acute alcohol withdrawal (Nixon & Crews, 2002). While alcohol can be damaging in many ways, variation in acute alcohol withdrawal duration during alcohol administration paradigms undoubtedly produces variation in the severity of these phenotypes. This finding highlights the importance of alcohol administration patterns in determining alcohol’s long-term effects.

Another explanation for inconsistent reports of learning deficits after adolescent alcohol exposure is the variable timing of adolescent alcohol exposure. It has been posited that adolescence consists of separable periods of vulnerability based on the brain regions that are differentially developing during those periods (Spear, 2015). Effects of alcohol on contextual fear conditioning, for example, can be variable depending on the stage of adolescence in which rats are exposed to alcohol. Intermittent alcohol exposure during early adolescence can impair adult contextual fear retention. Exposure during mid-late adolescence, however, leaves contextual fear retention unaffected while impairing contextual fear extinction (Broadwater & Spear, 2013). This may suggest that the HC is more vulnerable during early adolescence, while other regions that mediate fear extinction, such as the PFC, are vulnerable during late adolescence (Spear, 2015). This is consistent with current understanding of adolescent brain development, as the PFC is more plastic during late adolescence.

9. Conclusion

Overall, adolescent alcohol exposure has detrimental effects on brain development that translate into potential later life psychological challenges. These effects are not equal across behaviors and brain regions. Long-term cognitive deficits are associated with adolescent alcohol exposure, and PFC-dependent tasks are particularly sensitive to this effect. This most likely relates to the late developmental maturation of the PFC (Giedd et al., 1999; Sowell et al., 1999; Gogtay et al., 2004). Because the PFC is involved in executive function, this also suggests increased risk for mental health problems and antisocial behavior associated with executive function deficits (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Ogilvie et al., 2011; Brower & Price, 2011; Willcutt et al., 2005). Both adult mental health problems and antisocial behaviors are associated with adolescent alcohol use (Rohde et al., 2001; Brière et al., 2014).

Adolescent alcohol exposure can lead to impaired PFC morphology (Pfefferbaum et al., 1997; De Bellis et al., 2005; Medina et al., 2008), PFC neurobiology (Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Fernandez et al., 2017; Fernandez et al., 2018), and PFC-related learning and cognition (Coleman et al., 2011; Vetreno & Crews, 2012; Coleman et al., 2014; Broadwater & Spear, 2013). Adolescent alcohol exposure can also produce impaired HC morphology (De Bellis et al., 2000; Nagel et al., 2005), HC neurobiology (Broadwater et al., 2014; Risher et al., 2015; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Sakharkar et al., 2016), and HC-related learning (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Swartzwelder et al., 2015; White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013). Impaired adult learning is sometimes reported as being more severe after adolescent alcohol exposure than after adult alcohol exposure (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Markweise et al., 1998; White et al., 2006). Lasting HC-dependent learning impairments after adolescent alcohol exposure are not reported in all studies (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Vetreno & Crews, 2015; Wolstenholme et al., 2017; Fernandez & Savage, 2017; Swartzwelder et al., 2015; White et al., 2006; Risher et al., 2013; Pascual et al., 2007; Markwiese et al., 1998; Silvers et al., 2006; Sircar et al., 2009; Sircar & Sircar, 2005; Coleman et al., 2011). Some cognitive consequences of adolescent alcohol exposure have been shown to improve over prolonged abstinence (Cippitelli et al., 2010; Munro et al., 2000; Tapert et al., 2002; Petit et al., 2017), suggesting that duration of abstinence period before behavioral testing may account for variable findings between studies. Intermittent alcohol exposure patterns also vary between studies (Sircar et al., 2009; Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Fernandez & Savage, 2017), creating differences in the amount of alcohol, duration of exposure, and number of alcohol withdrawal periods to which subjects are exposed. Additionally, behaviors dependent upon brain regions that develop at different rates throughout adolescence may be differentially susceptible to alcohol’s harmful effects (Broadwater & Spear, 2013; Spear, 2015). Therefore, timing of exposure may play a critical role in determining the nature and severity of alcohol’s cognitive consequences. These relevant mediating factors highlight the importance of continued study in this field. Overall, alcohol exposure during adolescence is problematic because it has been shown to impair adolescent and adult cognitive functioning and associated neural substrates (Brown et al., 2000; Hanson et al., 2011; Thoma et al., 2012; Mahedy et al. 2018). Further exploration of how abstinence, adolescent age of exposure, and intermittence patterns mediate alcohol’s effects will determine risk factors for long-term consequences of drug use. This will ultimately inform prevention and treatment plans for affected individuals.

Highlights.

  • Alcohol use during adolescence can have negative behavioral consequences

  • Adolescent alcohol exposure impairs short- and long-term cognitive function

  • Prefrontal biology and cognition are consistently impaired by adolescent alcohol

  • Hippocampal learning and biology are also damaged by adolescent alcohol exposure

  • Relevant human and rodent studies are summarized in this review article

Acknowledgements

This publication was supported, in part, by the National Institutes of Health (T32GM108563) and the Jean Phillips Shibley Endowment (T.J.G.). We would like to thank Dr. Janine Kwapis for her detailed review of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Acheson SK, Stein RM, & Swartzwelder HS (1998). Impairment of semantic and figural memory by acute ethanol: age-dependent effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 22(7), 1437–1442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Addolorato G, Vassallo GA, Antonelli G, Antonelli M, Tarli C, Mirijello A, Agyei-Nkansah A, Mentella MC, Ferrarese D, Mora V, Barbàra M, Maida M, Cammà C, Gasbarrini A, & Alcohol Related Disease Consortium. (2018). Binge Drinking among adolescents is related to the development of Alcohol Use Disorders: results from a Cross-Sectional Study. Scientific reports, 8(1), 12624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antunes M, & Biala G (2012). The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cognitive processing, 13(2), 93–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blokland A (1995). Acetylcholine: a neurotransmitter for learning and memory?. Brain Research Reviews, 21(3), 285–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Artola A, & Singer W (1987). Long-term potentiation and NMDA receptors in rat visual cortex. Nature, 330(6149), 649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bettler B, & Mulle C (1995). AMPA and kainate receptors. Neuropharmacology, 34(2), 123–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boelema SR, Harakeh Z, van Zandvoort MJ, Reijneveld SA, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, & Vollebergh WA (2015). Adolescent heavy drinking does not affect maturation of basic executive functioning: longitudinal findings from the TRAILS study. PLoS One, 10(10), e0139186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boitard C, Cavaroc A, Sauvant J, Aubert A, Castanon N, Layé S, & Ferreira G (2014). Impairment of hippocampal-dependent memory induced by juvenile high-fat diet intake is associated with enhanced hippocampal inflammation in rats. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 40, 9–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boutros N, Der-Avakian A, Markou A, & Semenova S (2017). Effects of early life stress and adolescent ethanol exposure on adult cognitive performance in the 5-choice serial reaction time task in Wistar male rats. Psychopharmacology, 234(9–10), 1549–1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brandt J, Butters N, Ryan C, & Bayog R (1983). Cognitive loss and recovery in long-term alcohol abusers. Archives of general psychiatry, 40(4), 435–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brière FN, Rohde P, Seeley JR, Klein D, & Lewinsohn PM (2014). Comorbidity between major depression and alcohol use disorder from adolescence to adulthood. Comprehensive psychiatry, 55(3), 526–533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Broadwater M, & Spear LP (2013). Consequences of ethanol exposure on cued and contextual fear conditioning and extinction differ depending on timing of exposure during adolescence or adulthood. Behavioural brain research, 256, 10–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Broadwater MA, Liu W, Crews FT, & Spear LP (2014). Persistent loss of hippocampal neurogenesis and increased cell death following adolescent, but not adult, chronic ethanol exposure. Developmental neuroscience, 36(3–4), 297–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brook DW, Brook JS, Zhang C, Cohen P, & Whiteman M (2002). Drug use and the risk of major depressive disorder, alcohol dependence, and substance use disorders. Archives of general psychiatry, 59(11), 1039–1044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brower MC, & Price BH (2001). Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: a critical review. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71(6), 720–726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown SA, Tapert SF, Granholm E, & Delis DC (2000). Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: Effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcoholism: clinical and experimental research, 24(2), 164–171. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brumback T, Cao D, & King A (2007). Effects of alcohol on psychomotor performance and perceived impairment in heavy binge social drinkers. Drug and alcohol dependence, 91(1), 10–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cardoso RA, Brozowski SJ, Chavez-Noriega LE, Harpold M, Valenzuela CF, & Harris RA (1999). Effects of Ethanol on Recombinant Human Neuronal Nicotinic Acetylcholine Receptors Expressed in XenopusOocytes. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 289(2), 774–780. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Casey BJ, Getz S, & Galvan A (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental review, 28(1), 62–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Choi DW (1992). Excitotoxic cell death. Journal of neurobiology, 23(9), 1261–1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cippitelli A, Zook M, Bell L, Damadzic R, Eskay RL, Schwandt M, & Heilig M (2010). Reversibility of object recognition but not spatial memory impairment following binge-like alcohol exposure in rats. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 94(4), 538–546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Coleman LG Jr, He J, Lee J, Styner M, & Crews FT (2011). Adolescent binge drinking alters adult brain neurotransmitter gene expression, behavior, brain regional volumes, and neurochemistry in mice. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(4), 671–688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Coleman LG Jr, Liu W, Oguz I, Styner M, & Crews FT (2014). Adolescent binge ethanol treatment alters adult brain regional volumes, cortical extracellular matrix protein and behavioral flexibility. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 116, 142–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Crews FT, Mdzinarishvili A, Kim D, He J, & Nixon K (2006). Neurogenesis in adolescent brain is potently inhibited by ethanol. Neuroscience, 137(2), 437–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, Soloff PH, Boring AM, Hall J, Kersh BA, & Keshavan MS (2000). Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(5), 737–744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Delgado MR, Olsson A, & Phelps EA (2006). Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans. Biological psychology, 73(1), 39–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dias R, & Aggleton JP (2000). Effects of selective excitotoxic prefrontal lesions on acquisition of nonmatching-and matching-to-place in the T-maze in the rat: Differential involvement of the prelimbic-infralimbic and anterior cingulate cortices in providing behavioural flexibility. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12(12), 4457–4466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ehlers CL, Criado JR, Wills DN, Liu W, & Crews FT (2011). Periadolescent ethanol exposure reduces adult forebrain ChAT+ IR neurons: correlation with behavioral pathology. Neuroscience, 199, 333–345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ennaceur A, & Delacour J (1988). A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behavioural brain research, 31(1), 47–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fernandez GM, Stewart WN, & Savage LM (2016). Chronic drinking during adolescence predisposes the adult rat for continued heavy drinking: neurotrophin and behavioral adaptation after long-term, continuous ethanol exposure. PloS one, 11(3), e0149987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fernandez GM, Lew BJ, Vedder LC, & Savage LM (2017). Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure leads to alterations in brain-derived neurotrophic factor within the frontal cortex and impaired behavioral flexibility in both adolescent and adult rats. Neuroscience, 348, 324–334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fein G, Greenstein D, Cardenas VA, Cuzen NL, Fouche JP, Ferrett H, Thomas K, & Stein DJ (2013). Cortical and subcortical volumes in adolescents with alcohol dependence but without substance or psychiatric comorbidities. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 214(1), 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Floresco SB, Seamans JK, & Phillips AG (1997). Selective roles for hippocampal, prefrontal cortical, and ventral striatal circuits in radial-arm maze tasks with or without a delay. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(5), 1880–1890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gass JT, Glen WB Jr, McGonigal JT, Trantham-Davidson H, Lopez MF, Randall PK, Yaxley R, Floresco SB, & Chandler LJ (2014). Adolescent alcohol exposure reduces behavioral flexibility, promotes disinhibition, and increases resistance to extinction of ethanol self-administration in adulthood. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(11), 2570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ghetti S, DeMaster DM, Yonelinas AP, & Bunge SA (2010). Developmental differences in medial temporal lobe function during memory encoding. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(28), 9548–9556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Giedd JN, Vaituzis AC, Hamburger SD, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Kaysen D, Vauss YC, & Rapoport JL (1996). Quantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: ages 4–18 years. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 366(2), 223–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, & Rapoport JL (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga AW, Rapoport JL, & Thompson PM (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(21), 8174–8179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gould TJ (2003). Ethanol disrupts fear conditioning in C57BL/6 mice. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17(1), 77–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gould TJ (2010). Addiction and cognition. Addiction science & clinical practice, 5(2), 4. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hanson KL, Cummins K, Tapert SF, & Brown SA (2011). Changes in neuropsychological functioning over 10 years following adolescent substance abuse treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25(1), 127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. He J, Nixon K, Shetty AK, & Crews FT (2005). Chronic alcohol exposure reduces hippocampal neurogenesis and dendritic growth of newborn neurons. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(10), 2711–2720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hunt PS, & Barnet RC (2016). Adolescent and adult rats differ in the amnesic effects of acute ethanol in two hippocampus-dependent tasks: Trace and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioural brain research, 298, 78–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jarrard LE (1993). On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behavioral and neural biology, 60(1), 9–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Johnson CT, Olton DS, Gage FH, & Jenko PG (1977). Damage to hippocampus and hippocampal connections: effects on DRL and spontaneous alternation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91(3), 508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Johnston LD, Miech RA, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, & Patrick ME (2018). Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2017: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. [Google Scholar]
  47. Kim JJ, & Fanselow MS (1992). Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science, 256(5057), 675–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kimbrough A, Kim S, Cole M, Brennan M, & George O (2017). Intermittent access to ethanol drinking facilitates the transition to excessive drinking after chronic intermittent ethanol vapor exposure. Alcoholism: clinical and experimental research, 41(8), 1502–1509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. King AC, de Wit H, McNamara PJ, & Cao D (2011). Rewarding, stimulant, and sedative alcohol responses and relationship to future binge drinking. Archives of general psychiatry, 68(4), 389–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kroener S, Mulholland PJ, New NN, Gass JT, Becker HC, & Chandler LJ (2012). Chronic alcohol exposure alters behavioral and synaptic plasticity of the rodent prefrontal cortex. PloS one, 7(5), e37541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kubota M, Nakazaki S, Hirai S, Saeki N, Yamaura A, & Kusaka T (2001). Alcohol consumption and frontal lobe shrinkage: study of 1432 non-alcoholic subjects. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71(1), 104–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kumar S, Porcu P, Werner DF, Matthews DB, Diaz-Granados JL, Helfand RS, & Morrow AL (2009). The role of GABA A receptors in the acute and chronic effects of ethanol: a decade of progress. Psychopharmacology, 205(4), 529–564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kyzar EJ, Zhang H, Sakharkar AJ, & Pandey SC (2017). Adolescent alcohol exposure alters lysine demethylase 1 (LSD1) expression and histone methylation in the amygdala during adulthood. Addiction biology, 22(5), 1191–1204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Land C, & Spear NE (2004). Fear conditioning is impaired in adult rats by ethanol doses that do not affect periadolescents. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 22(5–6), 355–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lapchak PA, Araujo DM, & Hefti F (1993). Systemic interleukin-1 beta decreases brain-derived neurotrophic factor messenger RNA expression in the rat hippocampal formation. Neuroscience, 53(2), 297–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lee E, Jang DP, Kim JJ, An SK, Park S, Kim IY, Kim SI, Yoon KJ, & Namkoong K (2007). Alteration of brain metabolites in young alcoholics without structural changes. Neuroreport, 18(14), 1511–1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Morgan AB, & Lilienfeld SO (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical psychology review, 20(1), 113–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Linnarsson S, Björklund A, & Ernfors P (1997). Learning deficit in BDNF mutant mice. European Journal of Neuroscience, 9(12), 2581–2587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Logue SF, Paylor R, & Wehner JM (1997). Hippocampal lesions cause learning deficits in inbred mice in the Morris water maze and conditioned-fear task. Behavioral neuroscience, 111(1), 104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lovinger DM, White G, & Weight FF (1990). NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic excitation selectively inhibited by ethanol in hippocampal slice from adult rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 10(4), 1372–1379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lovinger DM (1993). High ethanol sensitivity of recombinant AMPA-type glutamate receptors expressed in mammalian cells. Neuroscience letters, 159(1–2), 83–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Luciana M, Collins PF, Muetzel RL, & Lim KO (2013). Effects of alcohol use initiation on brain structure in typically developing adolescents. The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse, 39(6), 345–355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mahedy L, Field M, Gage S, Hammerton G, Heron J, Hickman M, & Munafò MR (2018). Alcohol use in adolescence and later working memory: findings from a large population-based birth cohort. Alcohol and alcoholism, 53(3), 251–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Markwiese BJ, Acheson SK, Levin ED, Wilson WA, & Swartzwelder HS (1998). Differential effects of ethanol on memory in adolescent and adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 22(2), 416–421. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. McCormick DA (1989). GABA as an inhibitory neurotransmitter in human cerebral cortex. Journal of neurophysiology, 62(5), 1018–1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Meda SA, Hawkins KA, Dager AD, Tennen H, Khadka S, Austad CS, Wood RM, Raskin S, Fallahi CR, & Pearlson GD (2018). Longitudinal effects of alcohol consumption on the hippocampus and parahippocampus in college students. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(7), 610–617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Medina KL, McQueeny T, Nagel BJ, Hanson KL, Schweinsburg AD, & Tapert SF (2008). Prefrontal cortex volumes in adolescents with alcohol use disorders: unique gender effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(3), 386–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Melendez RI (2011). Intermittent (every-other-day) drinking induces rapid escalation of ethanol intake and preference in adolescent and adult C57BL/6J mice. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(4), 652–658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mihic SJ, Ye Q, Wick MJ, Koltchine VV, Krasowski MD, Finn SE, Mascia MP, Valenzuela CF, Hanson KK, Greenblatt EP, Harris RA, & Harrison NL (1997). Sites of alcohol and volatile anaesthetic action on GABA A and glycine receptors. Nature, 389(6649), 385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Milad MR, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, & Quirk GJ (2006). Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biological psychology, 73(1), 61–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Miller EK, & Cohen JD (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual review of neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mohapel P, Leanza G, Kokaia M, & Lindvall O (2005). Forebrain acetylcholine regulates adult hippocampal neurogenesis and learning. Neurobiology of aging, 26(6), 939–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mooney-Leber SM, & Gould TJ (2018). The long-term cognitive consequences of adolescent exposure to recreational drugs of abuse. Learning & Memory, 25(9), 481–491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Morgan MA, Romanski LM, & LeDoux JE (1993). Extinction of emotional learning: contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience letters, 163(1), 109–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JA, & O’Keefe J (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297(5868), 681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Munro CA, Saxton J, & Butters MA (2000). The neuropsychological consequences of abstinence among older alcoholics: a cross-sectional study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(10), 1510–1516. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Murray TK, & Ridley RM (1999). The effect of excitotoxic hippocampal lesions on simple and conditional discrimination learning in the rat. Behavioural brain research, 99(1), 103–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Nagel BJ, Medina KL, Yoshii J, Schweinsburg AD, Moadab I, & Tapert SF (2006). Age-related changes in prefrontal white matter volume across adolescence. Neuroreport, 17(13), 1427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Nixon K, & Crews FT (2002). Binge ethanol exposure decreases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. Journal of neurochemistry, 83(5), 1087–1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Norman G, & Eacott MJ (2004). Impaired object recognition with increasing levels of feature ambiguity in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions. Behavioural brain research, 148(1–2), 79–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Obernier JA, White AM, Swartzwelder HS, & Crews FT (2002). Cognitive deficits and CNS damage after a 4-day binge ethanol exposure in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 72(3), 521–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Ofen N, Kao YC, Sokol-Hessner P, Kim H, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, & Gabrieli JD (2007). Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nature neuroscience, 10(9), 1198–1205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Ofen N, Chai XJ, Schuil KD, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, & Gabrieli JD (2012). The development of brain systems associated with successful memory retrieval of scenes. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(29), 10012–10020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Ogilvie JM, Stewart AL, Chan RC, & Shum DH (2011). Neuropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 49(4), 1063–1107. [Google Scholar]
  85. Olney JW (1969). Brain lesions, obesity, and other disturbances in mice treated with monosodium glutamate. Science, 164(3880), 719–721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Pandey SC, Sakharkar AJ, Tang L, & Zhang H (2015). Potential role of adolescent alcohol exposure-induced amygdaloid histone modifications in anxiety and alcohol intake during adulthood. Neurobiology of disease, 82, 607–619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pascual M, Blanco AM, Cauli O, Miñarro J, & Guerri C (2007). Intermittent ethanol exposure induces inflammatory brain damage and causes long-term behavioural alterations in adolescent rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(2), 541–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Petit G, Luminet O, de Sousa Uva MC, Zorbas A, Maurage P, & de Timary P (2017). Differential spontaneous recovery across cognitive abilities during detoxification period in alcohol-dependence. PloS one, 12(8), e0176638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Mathalon DH, & Lim KO (1997). Frontal lobe volume loss observed with magnetic resonance imaging in older chronic alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 21(3), 521–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Pfefferbaum A, Rohlfing T, Rosenbloom MJ, Chu W, Colrain IM, & Sullivan EV (2013). Variation in longitudinal trajectories of regional brain volumes of healthy men and women (ages 10 to 85 years) measured with atlas-based parcellation of MRI. Neuroimage, 65, 176–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Pfefferbaum A, Rohlfing T, Pohl KM, Lane B, Chu W, Kwon D, Nichols N, Brown S, Tapert S, Cummins K, Thompson WK, Brumback T, Meloy MJ, Jernigan TL, Dale A, Colrain IM, Baker FC, Prouty D, De Bellis MD, Voyvodic JT, Clark DB, Luna B, Chung T, Nagel B, & Sullivan EV (2016). Adolescent development of cortical and white matter structure in the NCANDA sample: role of sex, ethnicity, puberty, and alcohol drinking. Cerebral cortex, 26(10), 4101–4121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pfefferbaum A, Kwon D, Brumback T, Thompson WK, Cummins K, Tapert SF, Brown SA, Colrain IM, Baker FC, Prouty D, De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Nagel BJ, Chu W, Park SH, Pohl KM, & Sullivan EV (2018). Altered brain developmental trajectories in adolescents after initiating drinking. American journal of psychiatry, 175(4), 370–380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Phillips RG, & LeDoux JE (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioral neuroscience, 106(2), 274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Portugal GS, Wilkinson DS, Turner JR, Blendy JA, & Gould TJ (2012). Developmental effects of acute, chronic, and withdrawal from chronic nicotine on fear conditioning. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 97(4), 482–494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Qin L, He J, Hanes RN, Pluzarev O, Hong JS, & Crews FT (2008). Increased systemic and brain cytokine production and neuroinflammation by endotoxin following ethanol treatment. Journal of neuroinflammation, 5(1), 10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Quirk GJ, & Mueller D (2008). Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(1), 56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Risher ML, Fleming RL, Boutros N, Semenova S, Wilson WA, Levin ED, Markou A, Swartzwelder HS, & Acheson SK (2013). Long-term effects of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure in adolescent and adult rats: radial-arm maze performance and operant food reinforced responding. PloS one, 8(5), e62940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Risher ML, Sexton HG, Risher WC, Wilson WA, Fleming RL, Madison RD, Moore SD, Eroglu C, & Swartzwelder HS (2015). Adolescent intermittent alcohol exposure: dysregulation of thrombospondins and synapse formation are associated with decreased neuronal density in the adult hippocampus. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(12), 2403–2413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Robbins T (2002). The 5-choice serial reaction time task: behavioural pharmacology and functional neurochemistry. Psychopharmacology, 163(3–4), 362–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rohde P, Lewinsohn PM, Kahler CW, Seeley JR, & Brown RA (2001). Natural course of alcohol use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(1), 83–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sakharkar AJ, Vetreno RP, Zhang H, Kokare DM, Crews FT, & Pandey SC (2016). A role for histone acetylation mechanisms in adolescent alcohol exposure-induced deficits in hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression and neurogenesis markers in adulthood. Brain Structure and Function, 221(9), 4691–4703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Salling MC, Skelly MJ, Avegno E, Regan S, Zeric T, Nichols E, & Harrison NL (2018). Alcohol consumption during adolescence in a mouse model of binge drinking alters the intrinsic excitability and function of the prefrontal cortex through a reduction in the hyperpolarization-activated cation current. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(27), 6207–6222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Semenova S (2012). Attention, impulsivity, and cognitive flexibility in adult male rats exposed to ethanol binge during adolescence as measured in the five-choice serial reaction time task: the effects of task and ethanol challenges. Psychopharmacology, 219(2), 433–442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Shing YL, Brehmer Y, Heekeren HR, Bäckman L, & Lindenberger U (2016). Neural activation patterns of successful episodic encoding: Reorganization during childhood, maintenance in old age. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 20, 59–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Silvers JM, Tokunaga S, Mittleman G, O’Buckley T, Morrow AL, & Matthews DB (2006). Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence reduces the effect of ethanol challenge on hippocampal allopregnanolone levels and Morris water maze task performance. Alcohol, 39(3), 151–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Sircar R, & Sircar D (2005). Adolescent rats exposed to repeated ethanol treatment show lingering behavioral impairments. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(8), 1402–1410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Sircar R, Basak AK, & Sircar D (2009). Repeated ethanol exposure affects the acquisition of spatial memory in adolescent female rats. Behavioural brain research, 202(2), 225–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Slawecki CJ (2006). Two-choice reaction time performance in Sprague–Dawley rats exposed to alcohol during adolescence or adulthood. Behavioural pharmacology, 17(7), 605–614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Smith ML, & Milner B (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia, 19(6), 781–793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Jernigan TL, & Toga AW (1999). In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nature neuroscience, 2(10), 859–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sowell ER, Delis D, Stiles J, & Jernigan TL (2001). Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(3), 312–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Spear LP (2015). Adolescent alcohol exposure: are there separable vulnerable periods within adolescence?. Physiology & behavior, 148, 122–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Spear LP (2020). Timing eclipses amount: The critical importance of intermittency in alcohol exposure effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Spear LP, & Swartzwelder HS (2014). Adolescent alcohol exposure and persistence of adolescenttypical phenotypes into adulthood: a mini-review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 45, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Squeglia LM, Tapert SF, Sullivan EV, Jacobus J, Meloy MJ, Rohlfing T, & Pfefferbaum A (2015). Brain development in heavy-drinking adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(6), 531–542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Stewart DG, & Brown SA (1995). Withdrawal and dependency symptoms among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers. Addiction, 90(5), 627–635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Stolle M, Sack PM, & Thomasius R (2009). Binge drinking in childhood and adolescence: epidemiology, consequences, and interventions. Deutsches Ärzteblatt International, 106(19), 323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Admin (SAMHSA), US Dept of Health and Human Services, & United States of America. (2011). Report to Congress on the Prevention and Reduction of Underage Drinking. [Google Scholar]
  119. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), US Dept of Health and Human Services, & United States of America. (2015) National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). [Google Scholar]
  120. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), US Dept of Health and Human Services, & United States of America. (2018) National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). [Google Scholar]
  121. Swartzwelder HS, Acheson SK, Miller KM, Sexton HG, Liu W, Crews FT, & Risher ML (2015). Adolescent intermittent alcohol exposure: deficits in object recognition memory and forebrain cholinergic markers. PloS one, 10(11), e0140042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Swartzwelder HS, Park MH, & Acheson S (2017). Adolescent ethanol exposure enhances NMDA receptor-mediated currents in hippocampal neurons: reversal by gabapentin. Scientific reports, 7(1), 13133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Tapert SF, Granholm E, Leedy NG, & Brown SA (2002). Substance use and withdrawal: Neuropsychological functioning over 8 years in youth. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(7), 873–883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Teicher MH, Andersen SL, & Hostetter JC Jr (1995). Evidence for dopamine receptor pruning between adolescence and adulthood in striatum but not nucleus accumbens. Developmental Brain Research, 89(2), 167–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Thoma RJ, Monnig MA, Lysne PA, Ruhl DA, Pommy JA, Bogenschutz M, Tonigan JS, & Yeo RA (2011). Adolescent substance abuse: the effects of alcohol and marijuana on neuropsychological performance. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(1), 39–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Tipps ME, Raybuck JD, Buck KJ, & Lattal KM (2015). Acute ethanol withdrawal impairs contextual learning and enhances cued learning. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(2), 282–290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Tomie A, Miller WC, Dranoff E, & Pohorecky LA (2006). Intermittent presentations of ethanol sipper tube induce ethanol drinking in rats. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 41(3), 225–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Trantham-Davidson H, Centanni SW, Garr SC, New NN, Mulholland PJ, Gass JT, Glover EJ, Floresco SB, Crews FT, Krishnan HR, Pandey SC, & Chandler LJ (2017). Binge-like alcohol exposure during adolescence disrupts dopaminergic neurotransmission in the adult prelimbic cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(5), 1024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Tseng KY, & O’Donnell P (2006). Dopamine modulation of prefrontal cortical interneurons changes during adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 17(5), 1235–1240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Vetreno RP, & Crews FT (2012). Adolescent binge drinking increases expression of the danger signal receptor agonist HMGB1 and Toll-like receptors in the adult prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience, 226, 475–488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Vetreno RP, Broadwater M, Liu W, Spear LP, & Crews FT (2014). Adolescent, but not adult, binge ethanol exposure leads to persistent global reductions of choline acetyltransferase expressing neurons in brain. PLoS One, 9(11), e113421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Vetreno RP, & Crews FT (2015). Binge ethanol exposure during adolescence leads to a persistent loss of neurogenesis in the dorsal and ventral hippocampus that is associated with impaired adult cognitive functioning. Frontiers in neuroscience, 9, 35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Vetreno RP, & Crews FT (2018). Adolescent binge ethanol-induced loss of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons and neuroimmune activation are prevented by exercise and indomethacin. PloS one, 13(10), e0204500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Warburton EC, & Brown MW (2015). Neural circuitry for rat recognition memory. Behavioural brain research, 285, 131–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Wehner JM, & Radcliffe RA (2004). Cued and contextual fear conditioning in mice. Current Protocols in Neuroscience, 27(1), 8–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Weiner JL, Dunwiddie TV, & Valenzuela CF (1999). Ethanol inhibition of synaptically evoked kainate responses in rat hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. Molecular pharmacology, 56(1), 85–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Weissenborn R, & Duka T (2003). Acute alcohol effects on cognitive function in social drinkers: their relationship to drinking habits. Psychopharmacology, 165(3), 306–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Werman R, Davidoff RA, & Aprison MH (1968). Inhibitory of glycine on spinal neurons in the cat. Journal of neurophysiology, 31(1), 81–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. White AM, Ghia AJ, Levin ED, & Swartzwelder HS (2006). Binge pattern ethanol exposure in adolescent and adult rats: differential impact on subsequent responsiveness to ethanol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(8), 1251–1256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, & Pennington BF (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Wilson DI, Langston RF, Schlesiger MI, Wagner M, Watanabe S, & Ainge JA (2013). Lateral entorhinal cortex is critical for novel object‐context recognition. Hippocampus, 23(5), 352–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Wilson S, Bair JL, Thomas KM, & Iacono WG (2017). Problematic alcohol use and reduced hippocampal volume: a meta-analytic review. Psychological medicine, 47(13), 2288–2301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Wise RA (1973). Voluntary ethanol intake in rats following exposure to ethanol on various schedules. Psychopharmacologia, 29(3), 203–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Wolstenholme JT, Mahmood T, Harris GM, Abbas S, & Miles MF (2017). Intermittent ethanol during adolescence leads to lasting behavioral changes in adulthood and alters gene expression and histone methylation in the PFC. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 10, 307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES