Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jan 12.
Published in final edited form as: Sleep Health. 2020 Jul 2;6(6):767–777. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2020.05.007

Sociodemographic and environmental factors associated with childhood sleep duration

Tiffany Yip a,*, Yuen Mi Cheon b, Yijie Wang c, Wen Qin Deng d, Amber Levanon Seligson d
PMCID: PMC7803411  NIHMSID: NIHMS1658568  PMID: 32624439

Abstract

Objectives:

This study investigates sociodemographic and environmental correlates of sleep duration among school-aged children.

Design & Setting:

The New York City 2009 Child Community Health Survey was analyzed using weighted regression analyses.

Participants:

1293 Asian, Black, Latino and White children ages 6–12 years, 999 children in Pre-K – 5th grade and 294 children in the 6th-8th grades.

Measurements:

Parents/guardians completed a survey about the target child’s sleep duration on a typical school night/day, and sociodemographic and household characteristics.

Results:

Most children (89.3%) met the National Sleep Foundation’s (NSF) recommendation of 9–11 h of sleep per night. Pre-K-5th grade children who were born in the United States were less likely than children born outside of the United States to sleep 9–11 h. When sleep duration was examined continuously, children slept an average of 9 h 44 min. On average, with each additional year of age, children slept 7.2 min less than children who were one year younger. Although there were no differences among ethnic/racial groups in sleeping the recommended 9–11 h/night, when sleep duration was measured as a continuous variable, Asian, Latino, and Black children slept an average of 23, 14, and 17 min fewer, respectively, than White children, adjusting for sociodemographic and environmental factors.

Conclusions:

Sleep duration varied across sociodemographic groups of children in New York City in 2009. Future studies should determine causal influences and whether these differences persist.

Keywords: Children, Ethnicity/race, Disparities, Sleep duration


Sleep is a complex biological necessity influenced by sociodemographic and environmental factors, and it is fundamental for the health and functioning of all individuals. For young children and adolescents, sleep is essential for optimal socioemotional and academic development1,2. This study investigates sociodemographic (e.g., age, sex/gender, ethnicity/race, nativity, family socioeconomic status) and environmental (e.g., crowding, having a smoker in the home) factors associated with childhood sleep duration among children in New York City in 2009. Factors of interest were informed by prior work linked to sleep duration. Among sociodemographic variables, age-related decreases in sleep duration have been attributed to biological changes, increased academic demands, and increased autonomy3,4. While some research has observed sex/gender differences in sleep5, there is also research which has not observed differences3. Nativity has been found to be related to sleep, with United States-born individuals having shorter sleep6,7. In addition, family socioeconomic status (SES) has also been implicated to be associated with sleep duration810. Black and Latino children and adults report shorter sleep duration and poorer quality sleep relative to Whites.4,11,12 There is also research to suggest sleep disparities between Asian and White communities13,14. This study also investigates environmental correlates that have been observed to compromise sleep, such as crowding in the home which leads to stress15 and has been suggested to disturb childhood sleep duration16. The presence of a smoker in the home has also been observed to impact childhood sleep1720. Because we focus on sleep duration, we adjust for sleep-related covariates such as sleep quality21 and sleep hygiene (i.e., having a regular bedtime13 and media use22). Finally, we also control for health-related covariates such as physical activity, health history, asthma, early intervention status, and adult respondent health in the analyses23.

This study contributes to the literature on sleep health in several important ways. First, it contributes to health disparities research by considering the relative, independent association between sociodemographic and environmental factors with childhood sleep duration in a large, diverse, and representative sample of children ages 6–12 years in New York City. While research has explored the links between each of these variables and sleep duration, few have explored their relative associations in a large and representative dataset. Therefore, the current study contributes to research on sleep duration by investigating if previously observed effects may be attributed to shared variance with other factors (e.g., residential crowding effects explained by SES). Informed by prior work, we hypothesize age-related declines in sleep duration24,25. In addition, we hypothesize ethnic/racial differences in children’s sleep, with Asian, Black, and Latino children sleeping less than their White counterparts3,13,14. We also hypothesize shorter sleep duration for children from lower SES families810, and shorter duration for United-States-born youth than foreign born youth6,7. Finally, based on the existing research16,18, we expect that residential crowding and living with a smoker are negatively associated with sleep duration. The second contribution is that sleep duration is modeled as both a continuous and a dichotomous variable (i.e., whether children are sleeping the National Sleep Foundation (NSF)-recommended 9–11 h per night for this age group)26. Including both indicators of sleep duration maximizes the relevance of the research, recognizing that some members of the sleep science community may be interested in sleep duration as a continuous variable situating the current work in a larger scientific framework, while applied scientists and practitioners may be more interested in sleep duration as a dichotomous variable for the policy relevance of this work.

Methods

Participants

The study is a secondary analysis of a representative sample of 3002 children ages 0–12 years residing in New York City whose parent/guardian participated in the Child Community Health Survey conducted by the New York City Health Department and Mental Hygiene (NYC DOHMH) between April and December 200927. The data are weighted to represent the New York City population of children based on the 2006–2008 American Community Survey data27. Survey data were collected for two age groups: 0–5 years and 6–12 years. The current study focuses on the 6–12-year-olds since sleep questions were only administered to this group. Because school start and dismissal schedules become more standardized starting in kindergarten, children between ages 6 – 12 who were enrolled in school (including 79 children who were in Pre-K and kindergarten) were retained for analysis (n = 1459). Although the National Sleep Foundation recommends 9–11 h of sleep per night for this age group, sleeping up to 12 h at this age is also considered normative33. In the current sample, 53 youth were reported as sleeping >11 h (4% of the analytic sample). In the unweighted data, most of the children who were reported to sleep >11 h were young children (average age = 7.53, SD = 1.69; 6 years old = 39.62%, 7 years old = 18.87%, 8 years old = 15.09%, 9 years old = 11.32%, 10 years old = 9.43%, 11 years old = 1.89%, 12 years old = 3.77%). Among these children, approximately 47.2% were boys and 52.8% were girls. All ethnic/racial groups were represented (37.74% White, non-Hispanic, 22.64% Black, non-Hispanic, 33.96% Hispanic, 1.89% Asian/PI, non-Hispanic, and 3.77% other, non-Hispanic) and most were born in the United States (92.45%). To keep the results interpretable within the context of the NSF’s 9–11 h guideline, we opted to exclude these children from the primary analyses. However, to investigate how the exclusion of these youth influence the results, we also conduct sensitivity analyses to compare results with the inclusion of these youth. Since ethnicity/race was a sociodemographic variable of interest, children who were identified as “other, non-Hispanic race (n = 43)” were excluded due to lack of information regarding ethnicity/race. Additionally, children in high school (n = 2), with missing school grade information (n = 13), home-schooled/not enrolled in school (n = 16) or not living with at least one parent (n = 52) were excluded. The final analytic sample included 1293 children: 999 children in pre-K-5th grade and 294 children in 6th–8th grade.

Procedure

Random-digit dialing and a computer-assisted telephone interviewing system were employed for data collection, and only families with landline phones were eligible. Eligible households were contacted via the random digit dial (RDD) landline sample from the 2009 Community Health Survey (CHS)28 and a supplemental RDD landline sample. The CHS is an annual health survey conducted by NYC DOHMH to better understand the health and risk behaviors among New York City adults. Details of the CHS methodology have been described elsewhere28. In 2009 CHS, respondents who reported having children ages 0–12 in the household were invited to participate in the child health survey27. A knowledgeable adult (i.e., parent or guardian) living with a target child age 12 or below responded to a telephone survey (average completion = 23 min, 27% response rate for interviews recruited from the Community Health Survey; 48% response rate for interviews recruited from a supplemental Random Digit Dial sample). Ninety-one percent of respondents were parents of the target child (69% mothers, 22% fathers, 9% grandparent or other). Families completed the survey in English, Spanish, Russian or Chinese, and were offered $30 for participating27.

Although the data are publicly available, access is granted with an approved Data Use Agreement submitted to the Bureau of Epidemiology Services, NYC DOHMH, detailing the planned analyses, specific variables, authors, and instructions to destroy the data. Since the data are de-identified, this research was deemed exempt by Fordham University Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Recommended sleep duration.

National Sleep Foundation (NSF) recommends 9–11 h for children ages 6–1326. A dummy variable was constructed (0 = sleeping < 9 h; 1 = sleeping 9–11 h).

Sleep duration.

Sleep duration was measured continuously in hours and minutes based on adult respondent reports of when the target child goes to bed and wakes up on a typical school night/day.

Sociodemographic characteristics.

Child’s ethnicity/race was dummy coded as Asian, Black, Latino, with White children as the reference group. Child’s age was measured in years. Child’s sex/gender was coded as 0 = female or 1 = male, and child nativity was coded as 0 = foreign born or 1 = United States-born. Parent nativity was categorized as 0 = at least one parent born outside of the United States or 1 = both parents are United States-born. Family SES combined the highest level of education completed in the household (0 = less than high school, 1 = high school grad, 2 = some college, 3 = college grad) with the annual household income relative to the Federal Poverty Level (0 =< 100% FPL, 1 = 100 to 199% FPL, 2 = 200 to 399% FPL, 3 = 400 to 599% FPL, 4 = 600%+ FPL). Scores ranged from 0 to 7 with higher scores reflecting higher SES. School type was another indicator of SES and coded as 0 = private school or 1 = public.

Environmental correlates.

Crowding was computed as a people-to-room ratio with higher scores indicating more crowding (i.e., more people to fewer rooms). Respondents were asked to indicate “how many rooms are in your home or apartment? Include kitchens, but exclude bathrooms, porches, balconies and foyers.” Having a smoker in the home (i.e., “How often does anyone smoke inside your home/apartment?” 1 = all or most of the time, 2 = only occasionally, and 3 = never) was dichotomized as 0 = no smoker and 1 = smoker.

Covariates.

Variables associated with sleep duration were covaried in the analysis. First, we included the adult respondent’s report of the target child sleeping through the night (“Does [the target child] usually sleep through the night without waking?” 0=no, 1=sleep through night without waking) and having a regular bedtime (“During the past school year, did [the target child] have a regular bedtime?” 0 = no, 1 = has a regular bedtime). Child media use reflects the sum of leisure hours on an average weekday watching TV and on a computer (recoded 1 = none or less than 1 h; 2 = 1 h; 3 = 2–3 h; and 4 = 4 or more hours). Dummy variables for media use were created for 2 (1 h), 3 (2–3 h), and 4 (4 or more hours), with “1 = none or less than 1 h” as the reference group. Physical activity is the mean of the number of days that the target child was physically active in the past 7 days (e.g., practiced team sports, martial arts, dance). Child’s health history (“Has a doctor or other health care provider ever told you that [the target child] had [health condition]”) was dummy coded as 0 = none, 1 = ever having been diagnosed with at least one health condition, including ADHD, depression, anxiety, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, autism or other autism spectrum disorder, any developmental delays and learning disabilities. Given the impact of asthma on sleep, asthma status was dummy coded as 0 = no, 1 = ever having asthma as a separate variable, based on the adults’ responses on “Has a doctor or other health care provider ever told you that [the target child] had asthma?” Child’s need for early intervention (“Does [the target child] need or use more medical care or mental health services than is usual for most children of the same age?” “Does [the target child] have any developmental problem for which [he/she] needs or gets treatment or counseling?”) was dummy coded as 0=none, 1=needs or receives early intervention. Adult health—parent/respondent adult health— (“Would you say that in general your health is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?”) was categorized as 0 = fair/poor, good, 1 = good, 2 = very good, and 3 = excellent (the reference group).

Analyses

Given age-related changes in sleep29 and structural differences between elementary and middle school students (e.g., start times), analyses were conducted first for the full sample (i.e., 6–12 years), and separately for elementary-aged children (pre-K-5th). Separate analyses were not conducted for the 6th-8th grade children due to the smaller sample size. All analyses were conducted in Stata/SE 12.1. Prevalence estimates, logistic regression analyses of meeting the NSF 9–11 h recommendation, and ordinary least squares regression analyses of the amount of sleep as a continuous variable, were weighted to represent the population of children in New York City across the 5 boroughs, child age, gender, and ethnicity/race. The weights account for sampling bias resulting from differential nonresponses and/or under-coverage of sample frame including those who were not in the random-digit dial sample frame because they could only be reached by cellular phone or did not have telephone service27. Missing values were below 10% for all variables. To test if missing values were associated with sociodemographic variables, missingness for the dependent variable (i.e., sleep duration) was coded “1” while non-missing values were coded “0.” Results suggested that missingness was not related to any sociodemographic variables (i.e., child’s age, sex/gender, nativity, ethnicity/race, or family SES) when the data were unweighted, which implied missing completely at random. Even when the data were weighted, missingness remained unrelated to sociodemographic variables with only one exception, child’s sex. Under these conditions, it was deemed appropriate to employ list-wise deletion30.

Results

Sample characteristics

Weighted and unweighted descriptive statistics for all measures are included in Table 1. Weighted descriptive statistics are representative of a total of approximately 620,000 children in New York City (Table 1): approximately 500,000 children between pre-K and 5th grade (6–12 years old; 80.65%), and 120,000 children between 6th and 8th grade (9–12 years old; 19.35%). Of the sample, 45.7% were girls and 54.3% were boys. The average sleep duration was 9 h, 44 min for the full sample (pre-K-5th: 9 hours, 50 minutes; 6th-8th grade: 9 hours, 21 minutes).

Table 1.

Weighted descriptive statistics of study variables among Pre-K-8th grade school children, 6–12 years old, New York City, 2009a,b

Variable n (Weighted N) % [95% CI] Mean (SD) [95% CI] Min Max
Sleep duration (h, minutes) 1285 (617,000) 9 h 44 min (44 min) 6 h 25 min 11 h
[9h41 min~9 h 48 min]
White 377 (162,000) 26.15% [22.96–29.34%] 9 h 56 min (46 min) 7 h 11 h
[9 h 52 min~10h 1 min]
Asian 100 (75,000) 12.22% [9.26–15.18%] 9 h 34 min (43 min) 7 h 40 min 11 h
[9 h 26 min~9 h 43 min]
Black 336 (164,000) 26.55% [23.20–29.90%] 9 h 43 min (44 min) 6 h 25 min 11 h
[9 h 38 min~9 h 47 min]
Latino 472 (216,000) 35.08% [31.53–38.64%] 9 h 40 min (44 min) 7 h 30 min 11 h
[9 h 36 min~9 h 44 min]
Sleep duration (9–11 h) 1136 (616,000) 89.29% [86.90–91.70%]
Sleep through the night 1176 (620,000) 91.54% [89.59–93.48%]
Child’s age (years) 1279 (613,000) 9.12 (1.96)
[9.01~9.22]
Child’s sex/gender (female) 611 (283,000) 45.70% [41.95–49.45%]
Child - US-born 1183 (557,000) 90.28% [88.14–92.43%]
Child - foreign-born 109 (60,000) 9.72% [7.57–11.86%]
Both parent US-born 473 (201,000) 33.2% [29.66–36.74%]
At least one parent foreign-born 789 (406,000) 66.80% [63.26–70.34%]
Family SESc 1173 (559,000) 3.30 (2.06) 0 7
[3.18~3.41]
Highest level of education by anyone in the household
 Less than high school 128 (78,000) 12.77% [10.04–15.49%]
 High school grad 304 (158,000) 25.75% [22.42–29.08%]
 Some college 278 (166,000) 27.01% [23.41–30.62%]
 College grad 574 (212,000) 34.47% [31.06–37.87%]
Income
 <100%FPL 293 (181,000) 32.41% (28.45–36.37%)
 100 to 199% 262 (140,000) 25.10% (21.52–28.67%)
 200 to 399% 238 (106,000) 19.01% (16.07–21.94%)
 400 to 599% 209 (76,000) 13.58% (11.14–16.02%)
 600%+ 174 (55,000) 9.90% (8.15–11.65%)
School type - public 996 (488,000) 78.70% (75.56–81.84%)
School type - private 297 (132,000) 21.30% (18.16–24.44%)
Crowding (ratio people: room) 1292 (620,000) 1.08 (0.60) 0.17 6.00
[1.05~1.11]
Smoker in the home (yes) 141 (66,000) 10.75% (8.35–13.15%)
Regular bedtime (yes) 1221 (619,000) 95.28% (93.93–96.62%)
Media use (0 or < 1 hr) 114 (60,000) 9.88% (7.54–12.23%)
Media use (1 h) 170 (79,000) 12.89% (10.47–15.31%)
Media use (2–3 h) 520 (246,000) 40.33% (36.57–44.09%)
Media use (4 h or more) 473 (225,000) 36.89% (33.23–40.56%)
Physical activity (days/week) 1288 (617,000) 2.69 (1.86) 0 7
(2.58~2.79)
History of health conditions (yes) 214 (99,000) 16.12% (13.43–18.81%)
History of asthma (yes) 234 (106,000) 17.12% (14.43–19.80%)
History of early intervention (yes) 345 (169,000) 27.56% (24.15–30.96%)
Adult health (fair/poor) 213 (117,000) 18.91% (15.81–22.00%)
Adult health (good) 432 (213,000) 34.45% (30.76–38.15%)
Adult health (very good) 376 (163,000) 26.32% (23.19–29.45%)
Adult health (excellent) 269 (126,000) 20.32% (17.31–23.32%)
a

The estimates were obtained from Stata/SE 12.1 software. The 95% CI’s may be slightly different from those obtained from SUDAAN or other statistical software programs due to differences in estimation methods.40

b

Family SES was calculated by combining the highest level of education completed in the household (0=less than high school, 1=high school grad, 2=some college, 3=college grad) with the annual household income relative to the Federal Poverty Level (0=<100% FPL, 1 = 100 to 199%, 2 = 200 to 399%, 3 = 400 to 599%, 4 = 600%+).

c

Population estimates were rounded to the nearest thousand.

Bivariate correlation analyses (Table 2) showed that younger age, attending a private school, having a regular bedtime, less media use, more physical activity, and better adult health were correlated with longer sleep duration in hours. Similarly, younger age, having no smokers in the home, having a regular bedtime, less media use, and more physical activity were positively correlated with sleeping 9–11 hours per night.

Table 2.

Weighted correlation table of study variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
1. Sleep duration (h) -
2. Sleep duration (9–11 h) 0.65*** -
3.Asian - - -
4. Black - - - -
5. Latino - - - - -
6. Child age −0.31*** −0.19*** −0.02 −0.03 −0.01 -
7. Child sex/gender −0.01 −0.02 0.04 0.02 −0.07* 0.03 -
8. Child nativity 0.03 −0.03 −0.02 0.06* −0.10*** −0.08** 0.01 -
9. Parent nativity 0.03 −0.03 −0.26 0.1*** −0.14*** 0.05 0.08** 0.23*** -
10. Family SES −0.00 0.01 0.06* −0.11*** −0.30*** 0.01 −0.01 0.04 0.16*** -
11.School type −0.08** −0.02 0.08** 0.05 0.24*** −0.03 −0.03 −0.10*** −0.15*** −0.29*** -
12. Crowding 0.01 −0.04 0.05 −0.09** 0.23*** −0.16*** −0.01 −0.12*** −0.23*** −0.37*** 0.16*** -
13. Smoker in the home −0.04 −0.07* −0.08** 0.11*** 0.02 0.09** 0.06* 0.04 0.12*** −0.12*** 0.10*** −0.04 -
14. Sleep through night 0.03 −0.05 −0.07* −0.00 0.00 0.04 −0.03 0.03 0 0.03 −0.03 −0.01 −0.06* -
15. Bedtime 0.17*** 0.15*** 0.03 0.03 0.00 0.01 0.03 0.01 −0.04 −0.05 0.00 0.02 −0.06* 0.03 -
16. Media use (1~4) −0.22*** −0.10*** −0.01 0.10*** 0.10*** 0.14*** 0.02 −0.06* −0.02 −0.12*** 0.21*** −0.01 0.10*** −0.04 0.02 -
17. Physical activity 0.08** 0.06* −0.16*** 0.13*** −0.14*** −0.07* 0.05 0.12*** 0.19*** 0.16*** −0.10*** −0.14*** 0.07* 0.04 −0.01 0.01 -
18. Health history −0.05 −0.03 −0.11*** 0.01 0.08** 0.05 0.10*** −0.01 0.02 −0.06* 0.08** −0.08** 0.15*** −0.11*** −0.01 0.07** −0.05 -
19. Asthma −0.04 −0.01 −0.03 0.05 0.11*** 0.02 0.03 0.06* 0.04 −0.07* 0.10*** −0.06* 0.06* −0.15*** −0.01 0.04 −0.00 0.12*** -
20. Early intervention −0.02 0.01 −0.07** 0.09** 0.03 −0.01 0.11*** 0.02 0 −0.08** 0.06* −0.03 0.18*** −0.09** −0.03 0.03 −0.02 0.53*** 0.13*** -
21. Adult health (0~3) 0.06* 0.04 −0.03 −0.01 −0.19*** 0.04 −0.03 0.05 0.07** 0.30*** −0.16*** −0.18*** −0.09** 0.06* 0.02 −0.02 0.11*** −0.10*** −0.14*** −0.13***

Notes.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

***

p < 0.001.

Sleep duration (911 h): 0=below 9 h, 1=between 9 and 11 h; Child ethnicity/race: 0=White, 1=Asian, 1=Black, 1=Latino; Child sex/gender: 0=female, 1=male; Child nativity: 0=foreign-born, 1=US-born; Parent nativity: 0= child has at least one foreign-born parent, 1= US-born; Family SES: Combines the highest level of education completed in the household (0=less than high school, 1=high school grad, 2=some college, 3=college grad) with the annual household income relative to the Federal Poverty Level (0=<100% FPL, 1 = 100 to 199%, 2 = 200 to 399%, 3 = 400 to 599%, 4 = 600%+), 0 = lowest, 7=highest; School type: 0=private, 1=public; Crowding: Number of people/number of room ratio; Smoker in home: 0=none, 1=at least 1 smoker; Sleep through night: 0=no, 1=sleep through night without waking; Bedtime: 0=no, 1=has a regular bed time; Media use: Average hours spent watching or using TV and/or computer, 1=None/<1 h, 2 = 1 h, 3 = 2–3hours, 4 = 4 or more hours; Physical activity: 0~7 days of the past 7 days; Health history: 0=none, 1=at least 1 diagnosis of health conditions such as ADHD, depression, developmental delays, etc.; Asthma: 0=no, 1=has asthma; Early intervention: 0=none, 1=at least 1 case of needing or receiving early medical care and/or mental health services intervention; Adults health: 0=fair/poor, 1=good, 2=very good, 3=excellent * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

The percentage of children sleeping 9–11 hours per night was 89.29% [95% CI: 86.90–91.70%]. Weighted multivariable logistic regressions considered sociodemographic and environmental correlates and covariates to predict whether or not children were meeting the NSF’s recommendation of 9–11 hours of sleep per night (Table 3)26. Then, weighted multivariable linear regression analyses estimated sociodemographic and environmental correlates of sleep duration in minutes (Table 4). The following section summarizes the results by sociodemographic characteristics, environmental correlates, and covariates) for the 6–12 year-olds and for the Pre-K-5th grade children. The results are presented first for the dichotomous variable indicating whether children are sleeping 9–11 hours, and second for a continuous sleep variable.

Table 3.

Logistic regression predicting probability of sleeping 9–11 h

Variables OR (95% CI)
All (Pre-K-8th) 1st-8th Pre-K-5th 1st-5th
Sociodemographic Characteristics Asian (ref: White) 0.59 (0.14–2.48) 0.54 (0.13–2.28) 0.51 (0.08–3.18) 0.48 (0.08–2.88)
Black (ref: White) 0.77 (0.35–1.68) 0.82 (0.38–1.78) 0.56 (0.21–1.53) 0.60 (0.22–1.62)
Latino (ref: White) 0.66 (0.31–1.41) 0.67 (0.32–1.41) 0.64 (0.24–1.67) 0.65 (0.25–1.67)
Child age 0.71 (0.61–0.83)*** 0.73 (0.62–0.86)*** 0.71 (0.58–0.88)** 0.73 (0.59–0.92)**
Child sex/gender 0.63 (0.33–1.18) 0.61 (0.33–1.15) 0.86 (0.39–1.90) 0.82 (0.37–1.83)
Child nativity 0.40 (0.15–1.08) 0.39 (0.14–1.06) 0.12 (0.02–0.85)* 0.11 (0.02–0.81)*
Parent nativity 0.63 (0.34–1.17) 1.57 (0.84–2.93) 0.57 (0.26–1.25) 1.74 (0.77–3.91)
Family SES 0.93 (0.79–1.08) 0.93 (0.79–1.08) 0.92 (0.74–1.15) 0.92 (0.74–1.15)
School type 1.06 (0.54–2.08) 1.06 (0.54–2.07) 0.72 (0.29–1.80) 0.73 (0.29–1.81)
Environmental Correlates Crowding 0.64 (0.30–1.35) 0.62 (0.29–1.33) 0.82 (0.21–3.26) 0.78 (0.19–3.18)
Smoker in the home 1.54 (0.69–3.45) 1.64 (0.72 – 3.74) 2.46 (0.81–7.43) 2.88 (0.90–9.18)
Covariates Sleep through the night 0.45 (0.15–1.38) 0.37 (0.11–1.26) 0.55 (0.15–1.96) 0.43 (0.10–1.80)
Bedtime 8.06 (3.42–19.02)*** 7.18 (3.05–16.87)*** 8.26 (2.53–28.91)*** 6.81 (2.12–21.95)**
Media use (1 hr; ref: 0 or < 1 hr) 0.24 (0.05–1.19) 0.25 (0.05–1.22) 0.10 (0.01–0.82)* 0.11 (0.01–0.81)*
Media use (2–3 h) 0.27 (0.06–1.24) 0.29 (0.06–1.29) 0.11 (0.01–0.78)* 0.11 (0.02–0.79)*
Media use (4 h or more) 0.14 (0.03–0.64)* 0.14 (0.03–0.62)* 0.07 (0.01–0.47)** 0.06 (0.01–0.43)**
Physical activity 1.22 (1.03–1.45)* 1.21 (1.02–1.43)* 1.29 (1.03–1.61)* 1.27 (1.01–1.59)*
Health history 0.95 (0.43–2.12) 1.06 (0.47–2.39) 0.52 (0.19–1.43) 0.60 (0.21–1.68)
Asthma 0.78 (0.38–1.58) 0.72 (0.35–1.47) 0.71 (0.28–1.83) 0.63 (0.24–1.64)
Early intervention 1.63 (0.80–3.30) 1.56 (0.77–3.15) 2.53 (0.98–6.53) 2.35 (0.91–6.06)
Adult health (fair/poor; ref: excellent) 0.69 (0.23–2.03) 0.66 (0.23–1.89) 0.88 (0.25–3.05) 0.81 (0.24–2.72)
Adult health (good) 2.06 (0.82–5.18) 2.12 (0.86–5.27) 4.28 (1.42–12.84)** 4.45 (1.50–13.14)**
Adult health (very good) 1.87 (0.73–4.81) 1.83 (0.72–4.62) 3.45 (1.06–11.22)* 3.24 (1.02–10.30)*
# of observations 1101 1037 858 794

Notes.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

***

p < 0.001.

Table 4.

Linear regression predicting sleep duration in hours

Variables B(SE)
All (Pre-K-8th) All (1st-8th) Pre-K-5th 1st-5th
Intercept 10.68 (0.30)*** 10.75 (0.32)*** 10.52 (0.34)*** 10.58 (0.37)***
Sociodemographic Characteristics Asian (ref: White) −0.38 (0.12)** −0.41 (0.12)** −0.39 (0.13)** −0.43 (0.13)**
Black (ref: White) −0.28 (0.08)*** −0.29 (0.08)*** −0.30 (0.08)*** −0.32 (0.08)***
Latino (ref: White) −0.24 (0.08)** −0.26 (0.08)** −0.21 (0.08)** −0.23 (0.08)**
Child age −0.12 (0.01)*** −0.12 (0.02)*** −0.09 (0.02)*** −0.09 (0.02)***
Child sex/gender −0.03 (0.06) −0.05 (0.06) −0.01 (0.06) −0.03 (0.06)
Child nativity −0.08 (0.09) −0.09 (0.09) −0.08 (0.09) −0.10 (0.09)
Parent nativity 0.01 (0.06) −0.00 (0.07) −0.01 (0.07) 0.02 (0.07)
Family SES −0.03 (0.02) −0.03 (0.02) −0.02 (0.02) −0.02 (0.02)
School type −0.02 (0.07) −0.02 (0.07) −0.06 (0.07) −0.06 (0.07)
Environmental Correlates Crowding −0.05 (0.07) −0.02 (0.08) 0.00 (0.08) 0.04 (0.09)
Smoker in the home 0.14 (0.09) 0.20 (0.09)* 0.13 (0.10) 0.21 (0.10)*
Covariates Sleep through the night 0.06 (0.07) 0.02 (0.07) 0.05 (0.08) 0.01 (0.08)
Regular bedtime 0.68 (0.17)*** 0.65 (0.17)*** 0.57 (0.19)** 0.52 (0.19)**
Media use (1 hr; ref: 0 or < 1 hr) −0.10 (0.12) −0.08 (0.12) −0.10 (0.12) −0.08 (0.12)
Media use (2–3 h) −0.23 (0.10)* −0.23 (0.10)* −0.25 (0.11)* −0.26 (0.11)*
Media use (4 h or more) −0.39 (0.10)*** −0.38 (0.11)*** −0.38 (0.11)*** −0.38 (0.11)**
Physical activity 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02)
Health history −0.07 (0.09) −0.08 (0.09) −0.16 (0.09) −0.18 (0.09)*
Asthma −0.08 (0.07) −0.06 (0.07) −0.10 (0.08) −0.09 (0.08)
Early intervention 0.05 (0.07) 0.08 (0.07) 0.05 (0.07) 0.07 (0.07)
Adult health (fair/poor; ref: excellent) −0.05 (0.12) −0.06 (0.12) 0.01 (0.13) 0.01 (0.13)
Adult health (good) 0.11 (0.09) 0.11 (0.09) 0.15 (0.09) 0.16 (0.09)
Adult health (very good) 0.12 (0.09) 0.12 (0.09) 0.21 (0.09)* 0.20 (0.10)*
# of observations 1101 1037 858 794
R-squared 0.22 0.21 0.19 0.19

Notes.

*

p<0.05.

**

p<0.01.

***

p<0.001.

Sociodemographic characteristics and sleep duration

In the full sample of Pre-K-8th grade children (ages 6–12), there were no ethnic/racial differences in whether children were sleeping 9–11 hours per night. Older children were less likely to sleep 9–11 hours compared to younger children (OR = 0.71; 95% CI 0.61; 0.83; p < .001; Table 3). As a continuous variable, Asian (b = −0.38, SE = 0.12, p < .01; Table 4), Black (b = −0.28, SE = 0.08, p < .001; Table 4), and Latino (b = −0.24, SE = 0.08, p < .01; Table 4) children had shorter sleep duration than White children (Fig. 1). Sleep duration declined with age such that children slept an average of 7.2 fewer minutes with each additional year of age (b = 0.12, SE = 0.01, p < .001; Table 4). Having a regular bedtime was associated with longer sleep duration (b = 0.68, SE = 0.17, p < .001, Table 4).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Ethnic/racial differences in sleep duration.

Adjusted and unadjusted sleep duration by ethnic/racial group.

Notes. Models are adjusted for sleeping through the night, having a regular bedtime, physical activity, media use, child health history, asthma, age, gender, child nativity, parent nativity, family SES, crowding, smoker in the home. Non-Hispanic Asian/PI, non-Hispanic Black, and Hispanic/Latino children sleep significantly less than non-Hispanic White children.

Bonferroni post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed no significant differences between Asian, Black or Latino children (adjusted p = 0.05/6 comparisons = 0.008).

Among Pre-K-5th grade children, the odds of sleeping 9–11 h decreased with age (OR = 0.71, 95% CI = 0.58; 0.88, p < .01; Table 3). Children born in the United States were less likely than children born outside of the United States to sleep 9–11 hours (OR = 0.12; 95% CI = 0.02; 0.85, p < .05; Table 3). When investigating sleep duration as a continuous variable, Asian (b = −0.39, SE = 0.13, p < .01), Black (b = −0.30, SE = 0.08, p < .001), and Latino (b = −0.21, SE = 0.08, p < .01) children had shorter sleep duration than White children (Table 4). White Pre-K-5th grade children’s average sleep duration was 10 hours, 31 minutes, and Asian children slept 10 hours, 7 minutes (23 minutes less than White, SE = 8 min), Black children slept 10 hours, 13 minutes (18 minutes less than White, SE = 5 minutes), and Latino children slept 10 hours, 18 minutes (13 minutes less than White, SE = 5 minutes). When exploring differences among the ethnic/racial minority youth, Bonferroni post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed no differences between Asian, Black and Latino children (adjusted p = .05/6 comparisons = .008).

Environmental correlates and sleep duration

Neither crowding nor presence of a smoker in the home were identified as significant predictors of sleeping 9–11 hours (Table 3). There were also no associations between environmental correlates such as crowding or presence of a smoker in the household and sleep measured as a continuous variable for the full sample (Pre-K-8th grade; Table 4).

Other covariates and sleep duration

However, having a regular bedtime was conducive to sleeping 9–11 hours compared to not having a regular bedtime (OR = 8.06; 95% CI = 3.42 to 19.02; p < .001; Table 3). In the full sample (Pre-K-8th grade), children who spent 4+ hours/day on media use were less likely to sleep 9–11 hours compared to those who spent 0- <1 hours/day on media use (OR = 0.14, 95% CI = 0.03 to 0.64; p < .05; Table 3). Physical activity was conducive to sleep; with an additional day of physical activity per week increasing the odds of sleeping 9–11 hours by 22% (OR = 1.22, 95% CI 1.03 to 1.45; p < .05; Table 3). In hours, using media for 4+ or 2–3 hours/day was associated with shorter sleep duration compared to using media for 0- <1 hours/day (~10 hours 18 minutes, ~10 hours 28 minutes, ~10 h 41 minutes, respectively; Table 4).

For the younger Pre-K-5th grade children, compared to those without a regular bedtime, the odds of sleeping 9–11 hours for children with a regular bedtime was 8.26 (95% CI 2.53; 28.91; p < .001; Table 3). In addition, compared to those who spent 0- <1 hours/day on media, children who spent 1 hours/day (OR = 0.10, 95% CI = 0.01; 0.82, p < .05; Table 3), 2–3 hours/day (OR = 0.11, 95% CI = 0.01; 0.78, p < .05; Table 3), or 4+ hours/day (OR = 0.07, 95% CI = 0.01; 0.47, p < .01; Table 3) had lower odds of sleeping 9–11 hours. With each one day increase of physical activity per week, the odds of sleeping 9–11 hours increased by 29% (OR = 1.29 ,95% CI = 1.03; 1.61, p < .05; Table 3). For sleep duration measured continuously, children who spent 4+ or 2–3 h/day on media use slept less than those who spent 0- <1 hours/day (~10 hours 8 minutes, ~10 hours and 16 minutes, ~10 hours 31 minutes; Table 4).

Sensitivity analyses

Recommended hours of sleep as 9–12 hours

The National Sleep Foundation recommends that 7–8 or 12 hours of sleep “may be appropriate” for school-aged children.18 As such, we examined sleep duration using 9–12-hour categories (n = 1176) versus less-than-9-hour (n = 149) categories, and excluded children with more than 12 h of sleep (n = 7, Table 5). The results of the sensitivity analysis were similar to the analysis of sleeping within the 9–11 hours range (Table 3) with the exception of the adult health variable in the Pre-K-5th grade sample, which was significant at p<.05 for the 9–11 hours range (OR = 3.45, 95% CI 1.06, 11.22, p < .05, Table 3), but was not significant for the 9–12 hours range (OR = 3.16, 95% CI 0.99, 10.05, Table 5). However, because the odds ratios and confidence intervals are similar between the 9–11 hours and 9–12 hours range, the difference in significance could be attributed to the sample size included in each group.

Table 5.

Sensitivity analysis 1: Logistic regression predicting probability of sleeping 9–12 h

Variables OR (95% CI)
All (Pre-K-8th) All (1st-8th) Pre-K-5th 1st-5th
Sociodemographic Characteristics Asian (ref: White) 0.54 (0.14–2.16) 0.50 (0.131.97) 0.50 (0.08–3.08) 0.47 (0.08–2.78)
Black (ref: White) 0.83 (0.39–1.76) 0.87 (0.41–1.85) 0.61 (0.23–1.61) 0.65 (0.25–1.71)
Latino (ref: White) 0.69 (0.33–1.44) 0.69 (0.33–1.43) 0.63 (0.25–1.62) 0.64 (0.25–1.61)
Child age 0.71 (0.61–0.82)*** 0.72 (0.62–0.85)*** 0.71 (0.58–0.87)** 0.73 (0.59–0.92)**
Child sex/gender 0.66 (0.36–1.23) 0.65 (0.35–1.21) 0.90 (0.41–1.98) 0.87 (0.39–1.92)
Child nativity 0.43 (0.16–1.14) 0.42 (0.16–1.11) 0.12 (0.02–0.88)* 0.12 (0.02–1.83)*
Parent nativity 0.58 (0.32–1.07) 0.59 (0.32–1.08) 0.54 (0.25–1.17) 0.55 (0.25–1.20)
Family SES 0.93 (0.80–1.08) 0.92 (0.79–1.07) 0.93 (0.75–1.15) 0.93 (0.75–1.15)
School type 1.10 (0.57–2.12) 1.10 (0.58–2.10) 0.67 (0.27–1.67) 0.68 (0.28–1.68)
Environmental Correlates Crowding 0.62 (0.30–1.30) 0.61 (0.29–1.28) 0.84 (0.21–3.34) 0.80 (0.20–3.25)
Smoker in the home 1.20 (0.55–2.60) 1.27 (0.58–2.80) 2.06 (0.70–6.05) 2.36 (0.77–7.25)
Covariates Sleep through the night 0.35 (0.11–1.15) 0.29 (0.08–1.04) 0.51 (0.14–1.84) 0.40 (0.09–1.69)
Bedtime 7.05 (2.98–16.71)*** 6.36 (2.70–14.96)*** 8.17 (2.51–26.58)*** 6.77 (2.11–21.74)**
Media use (1 hr; ref: 0 or < 1 hr) 0.26 (0.05–1.28) 0.27 (0.05–1.32) 0.11 (0.01–0.80)* 0.11 (0.02–0.82)*
Media use (2–3 h) 0.24 (0.05–1.08) 0.26 (0.06–1.13) 0.10 (0.01–0.80)* 0.11 (0.02–0.71)*
Media use (4 h or more) 0.13 (0.03–0.60)** 0.13 (0.03–0.58)** 0.06 (0.01–0.44)** 0.06 (0.01–0.42)**
Physical activity 1.21 (1.02–1.42)* 1.191.01–1.41)* 1.27 (1.02–1.59)* 1.25 (1.00–1.57)
Health history 0.96 (0.44–2.12) 1.07 (0.48–2.38) 0.53 (0.19–1.46) 0.61 (0.22–1.71)
Asthma 0.74 (0.37–1.48) 0.68 (0.34–1.37) 0.64 (0.26–1.60) 0.57 (0.23–1.44)
Early intervention 1.62 (0.80–3.29) 1.56 (0.77–3.16) 2.55 (0.98–6.61) 2.36 (0.91–6.12)
Adult health (fair/poor; ref: excellent) 0.67 (0.23–1.97) 0.65 (0.23–1.83) 0.86 (0.25–3.02) 0.80 (0.24–2.70)
Adult health (good) 1.94 (0.78–4.84) 2.00 (0.81–4.92) (1.02–5.52) 4.20 (1.41–12.58)* 4.36 (1.48–12.84)**
Adult health (very good) 1.83 (0.72–4.61) 1.79 (0.72–4.44) 3.16 (0.99–10.05) 2.96 (0.95–9.22)
# of observations 1144 1074 895 825

Notes.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

***

p < 0.001.

Exclusion of pre-k

It is possible that napping behaviors among the Pre-K children may represent a meaningful subsample in the data. To test this possibility, we re-ran all of the analyses excluding the Pre-K youth (Table 35). For the logistic regressions (probability of sleeping 9–11 hours and 9–12 hours), the results were similar with no significant differences in the coefficients or confidence intervals. However, the association between physical activity and the probability of sleeping 9–12 hours, was no longer significant after excluding the Pre-K children (Table 5). For multivariable regression analysis predicting sleep duration in hours, differences were observed for having a smoker in the home and the child’s health history. After excluding the Pre-K children, living with a smoker was associated with longer sleep duration (1st-8th grade: b = 0.20, SE = 0.09, p < .05; 1st-5th grade: b = 0.21, SE = 0.10, p < .05; Table 4). Similarly, after removing the Pre-K children, having a poor health history was significantly associated with shorter sleep duration (b = 0.18, SE = 0.09, p < .05; Table 4) among the 1st-5th grade children.

Sleeping through the night as an exploratory proxy for sleep quality

Although the dataset did not include a valid and reliable indicator of sleep quality, we conducted exploratory analyses and considered sleeping through the night without waking to be a proxy for sleep quality (Table 6). Having a poor health history (6–12 year-olds: OR = 0.31, 95% CI 0.16, 0.62, p < .01; Pre-K-5th grade: OR = 0.33, 95% CI 0.16, 0.69, p < .01) and having had asthma (6–12 year-olds: OR =0.29, CI 0.16, 0.54, p < .001; Pre-K-5th grade: OR = 0.24, 95% CI 0.13, 0.47, p < .001) were significantly associated with being less likely to sleep through the night for both the entire sample and for the younger sample, after controlling for sleep duration in minutes, sociodemographic characteristics, environmental correlates and other covariates.

Table 6.

Sensitivity analysis 2: Logistic regression predicting sleep quality (probability of sleeping through night without waking)

Variables OR (95% CI)
All Pre-K-5th
Sociodemographic Characteristics Asian (ref: White) 0.56 (0.21–1.47) 0.54 (0.18–1.61)
Black (ref: White) 1.08 (0.49–2.37) 1.02 (0.43–2.41)
Latino (ref: White) 1.70 (0.77–3.72) 1.55 (0.65–3.70)
Child age 1.13 (0.99–1.29) 1.05 (0.87–1.26)
Child sex/gender 0.82 (0.46–1.47) 0.77 (0.40–1.47)
Child nativity 1.36 (0.55–3.38) 1.22 (0.43–3.44)
Parent nativity 1.04 (0.57–1.91) 1.20 (0.60–2.38)
Family SES 1.07 (0.89–1.28) 1.07 (0.87–1.30)
School type 1.16 (0.59–2.28) 1.22 (0.60–2.49)
Environmental Correlates Crowding 1.08 (0.57–2.05) 0.93 (0.49–1.77)
Smoker in the home 0.51 (0.25–1.02) 0.70 (0.32–1.53)
Covariates Sleep duration in minutes 1.14 (0.83–1.57) 1.18 (0.80–1.74)
Bedtime 0.86 (0.32–2.34) 0.52 (0.17–1.58)
Media use (1 hr; ref: 0 or < 1 hr) 1.62 (0.50–5.26) 1.88 (0.50–7.01)
Media use (2–3 h) 1.23 (0.46–3.30) 1.30 (0.46–3.70)
Media use (4 h or more) 1.32 (0.72–2.41) 1.31 (0.68–2.50)
Physical activity 1.07 (0.93–1.24) 1.07 (0.92–1.25)
Health history 0.31 (0.16–0.62)** 0.33 (0.16–0.69)**
Asthma 0.29 (0.16–0.54)*** 0.24 (0.13–0.47)***
Early intervention 1.27 (0.66–2.43) 1.12 (0.55–2.26)
Adult health (fair/poor; ref: excellent) 0.96 (0.38–2.43) 1.05 (0.37–2.96)
Adult health (good) 0.87 (0.39–1.96) 0.90 (0.36–2.26)
Adult health (very good) 0.97 (0.42–2.26) 0.85 (0.33–2.16)
# of observations 1101 858

Notes.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

***

p < 0.001.

Size of the odds ratios

The size of the odds ratios for predicting sleeping 9–11 hours were unusually high for having a bedtime (Table 3). Therefore, we conducted the analyses without any covariates, and the odds ratios were still statistically significant although they were smaller (OR = 4.48, 95% CI = 2.21–9.08). The odds ratios were similar when ethnicity/race was included in the model (OR = 4.76, 95% CI = 2.34–9.70).

Discussion

Sleep is fundamental to health and well-being. For children and adolescents, adequate sleep duration plays an important role in optimal development1,2. Comparing the relative impact of sociodemographic and environmental correlates of childhood sleep duration, the results identified several sociodemographic factors associated with childhood sleep duration, including ethnicity/race, age, having a regular bedtime, attending a private school, physical activity, and media use in a representative and diverse sample of children between ages 6–12 in New York City. While the vast majority of children met the NSF-recommended 9–11 hours of sleep per night, consistent with existing research, ethnic/racial differences in sleep duration were observed in our study1214. Specifically, Asian, Black, and Latino children slept less than White children; a pattern that was observed for the full sample of 6–12-year-olds, as well as the pre-K-5th grade children. There was little evidence that the environmental features under investigation in this study were associated with getting enough sleep.

Sociodemographic characteristics and sleep duration

First, multivariable logistic regressions investigated whether children were sleeping the recommended 9–11 hours on a typical school night. Younger children in both the full sample and in the pre-K-5th grade subsample were more likely to sleep 9–11 hours. For the pre-K-5th grade children, child’s nativity was associated with whether the child slept the NSF recommended 9–11 hours. Consistent with research on adult populations in the United States7,12, pre-K through 5th-grade children born in the United States were less likely to get the recommended 9–11 hours of sleep. United States culture may shape abbreviated sleep duration9 due to the prevailing narrative that Americans are more productive when they sleep less.

In addition to investigating children’s sleep with the recommended 9–11 hours, sleep duration in hours was also examined. Sleep duration is of particular importance to childhood development and has been linked to various health outcomes, such as obesity, cognitive functioning, and emotion regulation31,32. In particular, sleep has been implicated in children’s cognitive functioning and academic achievement34. Our findings on sleep duration in hours contribute to childhood sleep research more broadly. For example, the study observed normative age-related declines in sleep duration29. Age-related decreases in sleep duration may in part be attributable to structural changes such as earlier school start times, increased academic demands, and increased autonomy13 as well as biological changes such as puberty53,54.

Consistent with research on English children across a similar age span3, differences in ethnicity/race-based sleep duration were evident in both the full sample and the pre-K-5th grade subsample (although it is important to recognize that sociocultural dynamics may differ between the United States and the United Kingdom). Another study found that Black children in the United States were more likely to nap and have later bedtimes than their White counterparts, possibly explaining differences in nocturnal sleep patterns34. Although sleep is a biological necessity, its expression is grounded in sociocultural contexts35 and cultural beliefs about the importance of sleep for child development and health, yet these differences remain largely unexplored. The Race-Based Disparities in Stress and Sleep in Context Model (RDSSC) implicates differential exposure to stress as a source of ethnic/racial sleep disparities.36 Indeed, recent meta-analyses and research have observed effects of discrimination on sleep among adults37 and adolescents.3840 Moreover, a meta-analysis on the health effects of discrimination found that younger children are more affected than older children,41 possibly explaining the consistent disparities for the younger cohort. While the current study underscores ethnic/racial differences in sleep duration measured in hours, and the study includes several documented correlates of childhood sleep, it is still possible that there are other unmeasured “third” variables (e.g., undiagnosed sleep apnea) that underlie the associations between ethnicity/race and sleep,4244 The current findings extend discussions on ethnic/racial differences in sleep beyond Black-White comparisons and call for continuing research to unpack related correlates for these disparities. For example, research on the impact of delaying school start times on adolescent outcomes has found more pronounced effects on improving absences and first-period tardies for predominantly ethnic/racial minority schools,13 raising the question of whether interventions should consider the demographics of the target population.

In addition, future research should consider how the magnitude of these differences is linked to other known disparities in the domains of health and academic outcomes. The magnitude of observed differences (i.e., 13–23 min shorter sleep duration) is in the range of existing research observing correlates of childhood sleep duration.25 For example, research on academic outcomes finds that an hour of delayed wake time is associated with a 0.13 increased in first-year college grade point average.45 Research on media use finds that each hour youth spend on media use is associated with a 3.6-minute decrease in sleep duration.46 Similarly, having a TV in a child’s bedroom was associated with a 31-minute decline in sleep duration, an effect that was even more pronounced for ethnic/racial minority youth.25 Unfortunately, it was beyond the scope of this paper to consider how ethnicity/race or SES intersects with sociocultural and environmental predictors of sleep duration, however, existing research has focused on disentangling these related influences.810 Although the analyses did not support the influence of family SES on sleep, the bivariate associations suggested a link between attending a private school and longer sleep duration, an observation consistent with research linking SES to sleep.10

Of note, contrary to existing research and our hypotheses, there were no significant associations between either of the SES indicators (family SES, public vs. private school) and getting 9–11 hours of sleep, or sleep measured in minutes. However, a consideration of the bivariate associations in the sample suggest that the SES indicators may share variance with other variables of interest. For example, children attending private schools had longer sleep duration and private school attendance was higher for Asian and Latino families, and lower among non-US born children and adults. Similarly, family SES is higher among Asian and lower among the Black and Latino families in our sample, and children of two US-born parents also had higher family SES. These bivariate correlations suggest that the SES indicators share significant variance with other sociodemographic indicators, possibly resulting in that lack of significant effects for family SES alone. It is also possible that significant effects observed for child nativity and ethnicity/race could be explained by shared variance between these variables. However, 84–95% of the target youth were born in the United States (unweighted: Asians = 84%, Black = 95%, Latino = 90%, White = 93%; weighted: Asians = 89%, Black = 93%, Latino = 86%, White = 93%). Future research that includes a more diverse sample in terms of nativity may be better at investigating this possibility.

Environmental correlates and sleep duration

Despite research pointing to the importance of neighborhood and environmental conditions on sleep duration,8,47 there was little evidence for the influence of the two environmental correlates (i.e., crowding, presence of a smoker) under investigation in these analyses. The current measures of environmental factors were proximal and did not extend outside of the target child’s home; as such, the shared urban context may have limited variability in environmental correlates. Also, relative to existing research that has found that home conditions account for SES-differences in sleep,810 the current sample relies on adult-respondent reports (as compared to observer or child-reported conditions). On the one hand, it is possible that since adult respondents shared the same home environment as the target child, they may be more accurate reporters of the two relatively objective variables under investigation; it is also possible that they would be less able to provide accurate accounts of their living conditions due to a lack of a comparison frame of reference. Because the current study also considers the relative impact of sociodemographic and environmental correlates of sleep duration in the same model, it is also possible that the observed sociodemographic correlates share significant variance with environmental features (e.g., residential crowding and SES). As such, existing research that points to the importance of environmental correlates may be explained by how contextual features are intertwined with sociodemographic characteristics of children and their families.

Covariates and sleep duration

Having a regular bedtime promoted, while excessive media use (i.e., greater than 4 hours) interfered with, sleeping 9–11 hours. Physical activity was also associated with sleeping 9–11 hours for the full sample and the pre-K-5th grade subsample. 13,48 Because information about children’s sleep was provided by adult respondents, this association may reflect the symbiotic association between adult and children who sleep in the same household where parents with poor sleep quality are more likely to report that their children also suffer from poor sleep quality,49 or it is also possible that associations reflect reporting biases. It is also worth mentioning that the NSF recommendation of 9–11 hours of sleep per night should be interpreted in the context of a more general recommendation of 7–12 hours of sleep as “appropriate.” To address this possibility, sensitivity analyses were conducted and reported; however, the results of this larger window yielded comparable results to the 9–11 hours recommendation. Sleep scientists have already acknowledged the clinical importance of having a regular bedtime, excessive media use, and physical activity in sleep hygiene interventions.50 As such, the current research lends further support to focusing on these modifiable behaviors as health levers, and focal areas of intervention.

Covariates related to sleep hygiene such as the importance of having a regular bedtime19 and physical activity,48 and the detrimental effects of media use22 were also evident in analyses investigating sleep duration in hours. The importance of a regular bedtime was present in the entire sample including the pre-K-5th grade children. Although children gain autonomy as they age, to promote sufficient sleep duration, it is important for households to maintain good sleep hygiene and bedtime routines. These data are consistent with the protective effects of family rules and structures, including regular bedtimes for childhood sleep.13 Echoing existing research,22 media use was incrementally associated with shorter sleep duration for the full sample.

Limitations

Results need to be considered with limitations. This sample was large, ethnically/racially diverse, and representative of the New York City population; however, the results may not generalize to other populations. In addition, data were collected in 2009, and cohort effects are possible. Also, the sample included only families with a landline phone, and the response rates were relatively low (27% for the CHS sample frame and 48% for the supplemental RDD sample frame), which limits generalizability. However, the NYC DOHMH evaluated their Community Health Survey data in 2008 and found very few differences in the main health measures comparing the landline survey and the combined landline and cell phone survey.51 Compared to Pre-K-5th grade, the sample of 6th-8th grade children was smaller, precluding separate analysis for this older age group. Children’s sleep was based on adult respondent reports, which may be influenced by adults’ own sleep problems.49 Combining adult and/or child self-reports with objective indicators of sleep actigraphy is becoming increasingly common10 and the inclusion of each addresses the limitations of the other. For example, adult reports of childhood sleep may have limited accuracy, and this accuracy may even decline as children move through adolescence and gain more autonomy. Although data on sleep duration across adult reports and actigraphy have been found to be comparable,52 and actigraphy may be cost-prohibitive, future research should consider including both self-report and actigraphy based measures of sleep. Furthermore, other sleep indicators such as sleep quality and variability provide more nuanced assessments of children’s sleep. Of note, the crowding variable was constructed as a ratio of people to rooms in the home, but detailed information about the distribution of people to rooms (and bedrooms in particular) was not available. Future research would benefit from focusing on bedroom crowding as a more proximal influence on sleep. Data on pubertal development were not available in the dataset, precluding investigation of this important developmental process on sleep.53,54 Finally, adult respondents were queried about sleep on a “typical school night”, precluding investigation of known differences in weeknight versus weekend sleep.13

Conclusions

Despite these limitations, the current study evidences the influence of sociodemographic characteristics on child sleep duration, including ethnic/racial differences in sleep duration among a representative sample of New York City households. Sleep health is a gateway to a host of socioemotional, physical, and academic outcomes.1,2 Sleep duration varied across sociodemographic groups among children in New York City. Future research should continue to investigate the causes and consequences of sociodemographic disparities in childhood sleep and whether these disparities are linked to disparities in health and academic outcomes.

Acknowledgments

The first, second, and third authors were supported by NSF BCS - 1354134 and NIH R21MD011388 awarded to the first author. The New York City 2009 Child Community Health Survey was conducted by the New York City Department of Health and Mental Hygiene with support from the Children’s Health Fund. The authors thank L. Hannah Gould, PhD, Charon Gwynn, PhD, and James Hadler, MD for their critical reviews on the manuscript.

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

References

  • 1.El-Sheikh M, Bub KL, Kelly RJ, Buckhalt JA. Children’s sleep and adjustment: a residualized change analysis. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(8):1591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Owens J, Adolescent Sleep Working Group. Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: an update on causes and consequences. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3) e921–e932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Blair PS, Humphreys JS, Gringras P, et al. Childhood sleep duration and associated demographic characteristics in an English cohort. Sleep. 2012;35(3):353–360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Guglielmo D, Gazmararian JA, Chung J, Rogers AE, Hale L. Racial/ethnic sleep disparities in US school-aged children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Sleep Health. 2018;4(1):68–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Friedman EM. Sleep quality, social well-being, gender, and inflammation: an integrative analysis in a national sample. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1231:23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hale L, Rivero-Fuentes E. Negative acculturation in sleep duration among mexican immigrants and mexican americans. J Immigr Minor Health. 2011;13(2):402–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cunningham TJ, Wheaton AG, Ford ES, Croft JB. Racial/ethnic disparities in self-reported short sleep duration among US-born and foreign-born adults. Ethn Health. 2016;21(6):628–638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Doane LD, Breitenstein RS, Beekman C, Clifford S, Smith TJ, Lemery-Chalfant K. Early life socioeconomic disparities in children’s sleep: the mediating role of the current home environment. J Youth Adolesc. 2019;48(1):56–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bagley EJ, Kelly RJ, Buckhalt JA, El-Sheikh M. What keeps low-SES children from sleeping well: the role of presleep worries and sleep environment. Sleep Med. 2015;16(4):496–502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.El-Sheikh M, Bagley EJ, Keiley M, Elmore-Staton L, Chen E, Buckhalt JA. Economic adversity and children’s sleep problems: multiple indicators and moderation of effects. Health Psychol. 2013;32(8):849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Thomas KS, Bardwell WA, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. The toll of ethnic discrimination on sleep architecture and fatigue. Health Psychol. 2006;25(5):635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tomfohr L, Pung MA, Edwards KM, Dimsdale JE. Racial differences in sleep architecture: the role of ethnic discrimination. Biol Psychol. 2012;89(1):34–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Adam EK, Snell EK, Pendry P. Sleep timing and quantity in ecological and family context: a nationally representative time-diary study. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(1):4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chen X, Wang R, Zee P, et al. Racial/ethnic differences in sleep disturbances: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (MESA). Sleep. 2015;38(6):877–888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Solari CD, Mare RD. Housing crowding effects on children’s wellbeing. Soc Sci Res. 2012;41(2):464–476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Brown ED, Low CM. Chaotic living conditions and sleep problems associated with children’s responses to academic challenge. J Fam Psychol. 2008;22(6):920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Yolton K, Xu Y, Khoury J, et al. Associations between secondhand smoke exposure and sleep patterns in children. Pediatrics. 2010;125(2):e261–e268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Redline S, Tishler PV, Schluchter M, Aylor J, Clark K, Graham G. Risk factors for sleep-disordered breathing in children: associations with obesity, race, and respiratory problems. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1999;159(5):1527–1532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ali NJ, Pitson DJ, Stradling JR. Snoring, sleep disturbance, and behaviour in 4–5 year olds. Arch Dis Child. 1993;68(3):360–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bonham GS, Wilson RW. Children’s health in families with cigarette smokers. Am J Public Health. 1981;71(3):290–293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Galambos NL, Howard AL, Maggs JL. Rise and fall of sleep quantity and quality with student experiences across the first year of university. J Res Adolesc. 2011;21(2):342–349. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: a review. Sleep Med. 2010;11(8):735–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bakour C, O’Rourke K, Schwartz S, Wang W, Sappenfield W, Couluris M. Sleep duration, obesity, and asthma, in Florida adolescents: analysis of data from the Florida youth risk behavior survey (2009–2013). Sleep Breath. 2017;21(4):1039–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Healthy people. 2020 https://www.healthypeople.gov/.
  • 25.Cespedes EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rifas-Shiman SL, Redline S, Taveras EM. Television viewing, bedroom television, and sleep duration from infancy to mid-childhood. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):e1163–e1171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, et al. National sleep foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations. Sleep Health. 2015;1(4):233–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.New York City Department of Health and Mental Hygiene. Child community health survey methods summary. https://www1.nyc.gov/assets/doh/downloads/pdf/episrv/cchs2009methods.pdf. Accessed Apr 25, 2018.
  • 28.Norton JM, Sanderson M, Gupta L, Holder-Hayes E, Immerwahr S, Konty K. Methodology Updates to the New York City Community Health Survey. New York: City Department of Health and Mental Hygiene; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics. 2003;111(2):302–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nakai M, Ke W. Review of the methods for handling missing data in longitudinal data analysis. Int J Math Anal. 2011;5(1):1–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Nixon GM, Thompson JM, Han DY, et al. Short sleep duration in middle childhood: risk factors and consequences. Sleep. 2008;31(1):71–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hasler G, Buysse DJ, Klaghofer R, et al. The association between short sleep duration and obesity in young adults: a 13-year prospective study. Sleep. 2004;27(4):661–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.American Academy of Pediatrics Supports Childhood Sleep Guidelines. Published 2016. Accessed.
  • 34.Buckhalt JA, Staton LE. Children’s sleep, cognition, and academic performance in the context of socioeconomic status and ethnicity In: El-Sheikh M, ed. Sleep and development: Familial and Socio-Cultural Considerations. New York, NY US: Oxford University Press; 2011:245–264. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Spilsbury JC, Storfer-Isser A, Drotar D, et al. Sleep behavior in an urban US sample of school-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158(10):988–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Owens JA. Introduction: culture and sleep in children. Pediatrics. 2005;115(Supplement 1):201–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Levy DJ, Heissel JA, Richeson JA, Adam EK. Psychological and biological responses to race-based social stress as pathways to disparities in educational outcomes. Am Psychol. 2016;71(6):455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Slopen N, Lewis TT, Williams DR. Discrimination and sleep: a systematic review. Sleep Med. 2016;18:88–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Goosby BJ, Cheadle JE, Strong-Bak W, Roth TC, Nelson TD. Perceived discrimination and adolescent sleep in a community sample. J Soc Sci. 2018;4(4):43–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Yip T. The effects of ethnic/racial discrimination and sleep quality on depressive symptoms and self-esteem trajectories among diverse adolescents. J Youth Adolesc. 2015;44(2):419–430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zeiders KH, Updegraff KA, Sally I, Kuo C, Umaña-Taylor AJ, McHale SM. Perceived discrimination and mexican-origin young adults’ sleep duration and variability: the moderating role of cultural orientations. J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46(8):1851–1861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schmitt MT, Branscombe NR, Postmes T, Garcia A. The consequences of perceived discrimination for psychological well-being: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(4):921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Boss EF, Smith DF, Ishman SL. Racial/ethnic and socioeconomic disparities in the diagnosis and treatment of sleep-disordered breathing in children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2011;75(3):299–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Daniel LC, Boergers J, Kopel SJ, Koinis-Mitchell D. Missed sleep and asthma morbidity in urban children. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2012;109(1):41–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Koinis-Mitchell D, Kopel SJ, Boergers J, et al. Good sleep health in urban children with asthma: a risk and resilience approach. J Pediatr Psychol. 2015;40(9):888–903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Trockel MT, Barnes MD, Egget DL. Health-related variables and academic performance among first-year college students: implications for sleep and other behaviors. J Am Coll Health. 2000;49(3):125–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Magee CA, Lee JK, Vella SA. Bidirectional relationships between sleep duration and screen time in early childhood. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(5):465–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nam S, Whittemore R, Jung S, Latkin C, Kershaw T, Redeker NS. Physical neighborhood and social environment, beliefs about sleep, sleep hygiene behaviors, and sleep quality among african americans. Sleep Health. 2018;4(3):258–264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Garaulet M, Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, et al. Short sleep duration is associated with increased obesity markers in European adolescents: effect of physical activity and dietary habits. the HELENA study. Int J Obes. 2011;35(10):1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rönnlund H, Elovainio M, Virtanen I, Matomäki J, Lapinleimu H. Poor parental sleep and the reported sleep quality of their children. Pediatrics. 2016;137:(4) e20153425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tan E, Healey D, Gray AR, Galland BC. Sleep hygiene intervention for youth aged 10 to 18 years with problematic sleep: a before-after pilot study. BMC Pediatr. 2012;12(1):189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Corey C, Eisenhower D, Immerwahr S, Konty K, Norton J, Sanderson M. Including New Yorkers Who Can Only Be Reached By Cell Phones in the Community Health survey: Results from the 2008 Cell Phone Pilot Survey. New York City, NY: NYC DOHMH; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sadeh A. Commentary: comparing actigraphy and parental report as measures of children’s sleep. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;33(4):406–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hoyt LT, Deardorff J, Marceau K, et al. Girls’ sleep trajectories across the pubertal transition: emerging racial/ethnic differences. J Adolesc Health. 2018;62(4):496–503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES